45
The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault NSC 371 Evidence and Inquiry and Polymathic Scholars Program The University of Texas at Austin December 2013 ____________________________________________ Dr. Ann Repp Department of Psychology Faculty Mentor

The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories

Jamie L. Archambault  

NSC 371 Evidence and Inquiry and Polymathic Scholars Program The University of Texas at Austin

December 2013

____________________________________________ Dr. Ann Repp

Department of Psychology Faculty Mentor

Page 2: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

ABSTRACT

Throughout life, the prevalence of seizures is highest during the neonatal period, a

critical time for synaptic development. While neonatal seizures can be attributed to a

wide array of predisposed risk factors and underlying medical complications, the majority

of seizures result from hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy, a condition that occurs when

the brain is deprived of adequate oxygen supply. Early life neonatal seizures often result

in cognitive deficits such as long-term impairments in learning and memory.

Additionally, the occurrence of seizure activity during the this developmental period

increases susceptibility to epilepsy far into adulthood. In this study, we propose two

distinct sets of hypotheses to explain the origin of cognitive dysfunction resulting from

neonatal seizures. From a psychological perspective, the extensive and far-reaching

effects of such seizures can be rationalized using components from existing memory

models. Furthermore, these deficits can be explained on a synaptic level through the

physiological association between long-term potentiation and the ability to encode, store,

and retrieve memories.

Page 3: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

TABLE OF CONTENTS

INTRODUCTION 1 COGNITIVE EFFECTS OF NEONATAL SEIZURES 4 Long-Term Complications 4 Methods of Treatment 5 Etiology of Cognitive Dysfunction 6 MEMORY PROCESSES 7 Multi-Store Memory Model 7 Working Memory Model 10 Memory During Development 13 PSYCHOLOGICAL IMPACT ON MEMORY 15 Hypothesis 1.1 - Breakdown of Executive Functions 15 Hypothesis 1.2 - Developmental Amnesia 16 Hypothesis 1.3 - Verbal and Visual Deficits 16 BIOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF NEONATAL SEIZURES 18 Altered Electrical Activity 18 Calcification from Atrophy 19 Decreased Synaptic Plasticity 19 NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF MEMORY 20 Anatomical Structures 20 LTP and AMPA Receptor Cycling 24 PHYSIOLOGICAL IMPACT ON MEMORY 28 Hypothesis 2.1 - Induction and Saturation of LTP 28 Hypothesis 2.2 - Increased Phosphorylation of AMPA Receptors 29 Hypothesis 2.3 - Reduction of Silent Synapses 30 CONCLUSION 32 BIBLIOGRAPHY 34

Page 4: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

1

INTRODUCTION

Neonatal seizures, which can be attributed to a variety of etiologies, occur in 1.8

to 3.5 per 1,000 live births (Cowan, 2002; Cowan et al., 2003; Silverstein & Jensen,

2007). Throughout life, the prevalence of seizures is highest during the neonatal period, a

critical time for both psychological and physiological development. Neurological

disruption during this period often has detrimental and far-reaching effects on the

cognitive function of affected infants. They are a problem in both term and pre-term

infants and are secondary to risk factors such as shortened gestational period, low birth

weight, and male sex (Kohelet, Shochat, Lusky, & Reichman, 2006a; Kohelet, Shochat,

Lusky, & Reichman 2006b; Kurabe, Sorimachi, Sasaki, Koike, & Fujii, 2009).

Additionally, these seizures can be associated with a host of underlying conditions, such

as transient metabolic disturbance, systemic or central nervous system infection,

cerebrovascular abnormalities, and hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy (Hallberg &

Blennow, 2013). While an array of factors contributes to the onset of neonatal seizures,

neonatal encephalopathy due to hypoxia-ischemia is the most common cause of seizures

in neonates (Hallberg & Blennow, 2013; Silverstein & Jensen, 2007). During hypoxic-

ischemic encephalopathy (HIE), brain injury to both gray and white matter structures

often occurs as a result of asphyxia, most specifically, damage to the basal ganglia and

thalamus (Gano et al., 2013).

Unlike seizures that occur during adulthood, neonatal seizures are difficult to

detect and treat because they are most commonly associated with subtle behavior and a

lack of coordinated EEG pattern (Jensen, 2009). Given the range of variability in

neonatal seizures, it is important to distinguish the difference between these seizures and

Page 5: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

2

general epilepsy. Epilepsy in children includes a body of disorders that is characterized

by recurrent, unprovoked seizure activity. Neonatal seizures, while not considered an

epileptic disorder, are a specific type of seizure that occur within the first 28 days of life

and often contribute to the future development of epilepsy. In addition, febrile seizures,

congenital malformations, metabolic disorders, head trauma, CNS infection, and family

history are specific risk factors that are indicative of epilepsy later in life (Cowan, 2002).

Interestingly, many of these risk factors for epilepsy independently contribute to the onset

of neonatal seizures. To fully understand how seizures affect memory, the pathology of

these seizures must first be understood.

The clinical expression of neonatal seizures often inhibits a clear diagnosis, as

both inward and outward symptoms tend to vary greatly depending on the etiology and

type of seizure. A review by Silverstein and Jensen (2007) detailing the clinical and

electrical differences in the types of seizures seen in neonates suggests that each of these

factors is associated with a specific outcome. Subtle seizures, which account for

approximately 50% of all neonatal seizures, are characterized by motions that imitate

normal behaviors in neonates and are not consistently associated with EEG seizure

activity. Tonic seizures, when focal, exhibit sustained muscle contractions and

coordinated EEG activity. Similarly, focal clonic seizures display evident EEG seizure

activity, but are outwardly associated with rhythmic jerks. Finally, myoclonic seizures are

characterized by rapid, single, arrhythmic jerks. Though the initial diagnosis is often

based on clinical observation, EEG seizure activity is the most definitive indicator of

short-term outcome. Since neonatal seizures commonly have a debilitating effect on

Page 6: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

3

quality of life, early diagnosis using these indicators are critical in managing and

minimizing the risk to long-term cognitive function (Silverstein & Jensen, 2007).

Unfortunately, the spectrum of disorders that provoke the onset of neonatal

seizures is simply too broad for the scope of this study. For this reason, our analysis of

the effects of neonatal seizures on memory deficits will be limited to those resulting from

hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy, the most prevalent cause of neonatal seizures. In an

effort to understand both the psychological and physiological impact of these seizures

and assess how cognition is affected, a thorough analysis of the neonatal stage of

development will be performed. The intent of this paper is to integrate a working

knowledge of memory formation with a physiological understanding of neonatal seizures

to fully explain how these seizures affect cognitive functioning. In doing this, we will

present two sets of original hypotheses: one set that relates the cognitive deficits from

neonatal seizures to existing memory models and another that explains the connection

between the neural substrates of memory and the neural substrates of seizures. We will

first discuss what is known regarding the cognitive deficits associated with neonatal

seizures, specifically those associated with learning and memory. To explain these

deficits from a psychological perspective, we will examine both early and revised models

that have been proposed to explain the process of memory formation. Following this, we

will explore the neural basis of memory formation and relate it to the molecular processes

that occur during seizures.

Page 7: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

4

COGNITIVE EFFECTS OF NEONATAL SEIZURES Long-Term Complications The risk for seizures is highest in the neonatal period, a critical period for both

psychological and physiological development (Jensen, 2009). Seizures during this period

are predominantly caused by HIE, a pathology that contributes to motor and cognitive

deficits in up to 40% of surviving neonates (Silverstein & Jensen, 2007; Shankaran

2009). MRI findings indicate that the extent and severity of brain injury is commonly

associated with the duration of the seizure (Glass et al., 2009; van Rooij, van den Broek,

Rademaker, & de Vries, 2013).

The adverse outcomes associated with seizures resulting from HIE are most

severe in the absence of other disabilities, such as cerebral palsy. For instance, a

“normal” child is more likely to experience severe adverse outcomes from HIE-induced

neonatal seizures than a child who has already been diagnosed with a cognitive disorder.

These outcomes include intellectual impairments, memory and verbal problems,

difficulty in executive functions, and motor problems (Perez et al., 2013). Issues with

short-term memory and social interaction often extend far past childhood and into the

adult years (Lindstrom et al., 2008; Odd, Dunnell, Lewis, & Rasmussen 2011). A study

by van Handel, de Sonneville, de Vries, Jongmans, and Swaab (2012) found that seizures

from moderate HIE result in the compromised maintenance and retrieval of verbal,

visuospatial, and associative memories, while also decreasing the speed and efficiency of

working memory. In cases resulting from only mild HIE, patients showed normal short-

term and working memory with only a slightly weakened verbal learning capacity.

Additionally, neonatal HIE can contribute to later-life developmental amnesia.

Page 8: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

5

Developmental amnesia is a selective disorder characterized by a patient’s frequent

failure to remember events from their everyday life. In these cases, deficits are observed

in episodic memory while the function of semantic memory and working memory remain

largely intact (Sans, 2011).

Methods of Treatment

In neonates that exhibit pharmacoreistant seizures, epileptic surgeries have been

effective in reducing the severity of cognitive deficits. Two and ten-year follow up

studies show improvement in social conduct, verbal learning capacity, figurative learning

capacity, and working memory (Viggedal, Kristjansdottir, Olsson, Rydenhag, &

Uvebrant, 2012). Interestingly, these patients exhibited signs of memory reorganization

both prior to and immediately following the procedure (Viggedal et al., 2012). Because

seizure disorders cause an inability to properly form memories, the brain attempts to

preemptively compensate for the lack of function by reorganizing the process for memory

consolidation. For instance, the posterior remnant of the hippocampus ipsilateral to the

damaged hemisphere appeared to take over the function of verbal memory after the

resection of an affected anterior temporal lobe (Bonelli et al., 2013). Despite the evident

success with surgical intervention for recurrent seizures, recent advances have employed

the use of therapeutic hypothermia to noninvasively treat and prevent future seizures

(Harbert et al., 2011). Clinical trials indicate that post-seizure hypothermia reduces the

extent of brain injury contributing to social dysfunction and impaired memory by

preventing the molecular cascade that leads to secondary energy failure and eventual cell

death (Dehaes et al., 2013).

Page 9: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

6

Etiology of Cognitive Dysfunction

In understanding the foundation of developmental impairments from neonatal

seizures, as well as the success of treatment methods, we can begin to hypothesize

precisely how memory is affected. Memory is a complex concept that depends widely

upon the type of information to be stored and the current stage of processing in which it

resides. It is unknown whether seizure activity affects the capability to encode, store, or

retrieve information that is intended for memory.

Three separate models, as detailed in Figure 1, have been suggested as possible

explanations for reduced cognitive functioning in infants exhibiting seizure activity

(Martinos et al., 2012). The first explanation proposes that neonatal seizures themselves

directly cause the brain damage responsible for cognitive deficits. The second model

suggests that the relative risk factors that predispose neonates to seizures, such as

gestational length, birth weight and sex, act independently from seizures to hinder

cognition and contribute to memory deficits. The third possible explanation integrates the

first two models by suggesting that the overarching effect of a predisposition to brain

injury along with actual seizure activity causes the severe memory impairments

commonly seen in affected patients.

Figure 1. Models proposed by Martinos et al. (2012) to explain the relationship between neonatal seizures and memory deficits

Page 10: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

7

MEMORY PROCESSES

Multi-Store Memory Model

As a topic of long-time psychological interest, various abstract models have been

proposed to provide representations of how memory is believed to work. One of the

earliest models for memory, the multi-store model proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin

(1968), divided memory into short-term and long-term components (Figure 2). Unlike

other theories of the time, the multi-store model supported the idea of a dichotomized

memory. Early studies demonstrated that patients with hippocampal lesions retained both

their previous long-term memories and the ability to create new short-term memories

(Milner, 1959; Milner, 1967). However, they lacked the ability to move these new short-

term memories into long-term storage (LTS), thus supporting the notion that there is a

distinct separation between short-term storage (STS) and LTS.

The multi-store model proposed that sensory stimuli are immediately registered

and used to create trace memories. Memory traces may vary in strength depending on the

Figure 2. Atkinson-Shiffrin model of multi-store memory

Page 11: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

8

intensity of their associated meanings, a property that later affects the success of retrieval.

Unless these traces are committed to STS, and ultimately LTS, they will quickly decay.

Recent studies have noted that there is a maximum capacity, or buffer, for sensory

information during the initial encoding process. Often, the failure to filter input within

this buffer contributes to the loss of information from STS, rather than a failure of

memory consolidation itself (Buschman, Siegel, Roy, & Miller, 2011). Though other

forms of STS exist, Atkinson and Shiffrin relied heavily on auditory-verbal-linguistic

(AVL) short-term store to explain their model. AVL, most widely studied by Peterson

and Peterson (1959), demonstrated that stimuli that appeal to either the sense of sight or

sound could be registered by the other sense before being transferred to STS for coding.

They suggested that both task difficulty and the rate at which information is presented

directly decreases available rehearsal time, which causes a decay of STS and ultimately

affects the ability to accurately recall information. If coding is interrupted by additional

tasks or stimuli, the transfer is terminated and the memory trace, along with its associated

meaning, will decay before reaching STS.

The transfer of traces from STS to LTS is regulated by a variety of control

processes, which will be discussed shortly. However, it is important to note that this

transfer could be either probabilistic or continuous in manner depending upon the type of

stimuli and the order in which they are presented. Probabilistic transfer occurs in cases of

incidental learning where the subject does not make a conscious effort to store material,

yet retains it anyways. On the other hand, continuous transfer is most often employed

when material is repeatedly presented and gradually learned.

Page 12: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

9

Once traces have been committed to LTS, they can be retrieved and brought

forward to STS by either recognition or recall. The difference between these is best

exemplified by the “tip of the tongue” phenomenon. Oftentimes, individuals find it

difficult to recall a memory from LTS without further prompting. The recall of

information is occasionally hindered by interferences from other stimuli, which can occur

during coding or retrieval. When given a cue, relevant information becomes available

through the process of recognition. The recognition of one trace brings forward others

through associative processes. The explanation for this phenomenon is rooted in the

belief that many partial traces are coded in LTS through their association with existing

complete traces. The idea that traces do not have to be stored “all or none” fashion is

supported by the observation that a second guess is often correct and not based

completely on chance when attempting to recall specific fragments of information

(Binford & Gettys, 1965).

The control processes utilized when coding, as previously mentioned, are

transient events managed by the subject and influenced by either the subject’s personal

history or the nature of the task at hand. These processes consist of schemes, coding

techniques, and mnemonics that increase the efficacy of encoding and retrieving

memories. At the level of the sensory register, individuals typically employ a scanning

process to determine which senses they will devote attention to. The rehearsal of

information regenerates STS traces and allows time for coding processes that assist in the

transfer to LTS to operate. However, there is a trade off between the buffer size of STS

and the ability to encode this information for LTS. While a large buffer size enables the

individual to retain more information in STS, it hinders the ability to accurately transfer

Page 13: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

10

this information to LTS. Put simply, the mind is so focused on maintaining a large

quantity of information that there is a reduction in the quality of encoded information.

Conversely, a small buffer size limits the amount of information in STS but allows for a

more accurate encoding process.

When retrieving information from STS, it is more efficient for individuals to

employ a quick search along ordered dimensions, or to list facts in an organized manner

rather than at random. If partial decay has already occurred, it is often necessary to

retrieve associated traces from LTS to generate a complete idea. Information from the

sensory register can only enter STS indirectly, a process that is most effective if traces

have been accurately encoded in LTS. When a stimulus is presented, the brain must

perform a LTS search for previously encoded associations to this stimulus. The need to

create new associations can often be eliminated by relying on existing associations.

Encoding information through existing associations decreases the area of memory that

must be searched when attempting to retrieve information and protects against

interference from succeeding sensory inputs. The length of the search and the control

processes utilized when recalling information from LTS are indicative of the efficiency of

that search. As with searches in STS, those in LTS must be performed along ordered

dimensions and it is the nature of the task that typically determines the procedure that is

utilized. Individuals must know what information to search for, where to search for it,

and when to terminate unsuccessful searches.

Working Memory Model

Shortly after the proposal of the multi-store model of dichotomized memory,

Baddeley and Hitch (1974) published the working memory model (Figure 3). In short,

Page 14: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

11

working memory replaced simplistic STS by suggesting that there is an active

maintenance of short-term information, which further organizes a trace according to the

particular sense that it appeals to. The original model for working memory, which was

proposed in 1974, consisted of three major components: a central executive, a

phonological loop, and a visuo-spatial sketchpad. Baddeley and Hitch suggested that the

phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad passively hold traces of sensory

information that can cycle back and forth to the central executive. Many years later,

Baddeley revised the model to include an episodic buffer, which provided a link to long-

term memory (Baddeley, 2000).

The phonological loop, which is controlled by the left hemisphere of the brain,

maintains speech and acoustic information. When auditory stimuli are presented, memory

traces can be held in the phonological store for up to two seconds. Individuals employ

Figure 2. Baddeley-Hitch working memory model

Page 15: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

12

articulatory rehearsal processes to recite and prevent the decay of this information.

Covert verbalization, as opposed to speaking out loud, is typically used to carry out this

process internally. Unlike the information stored in the phonological loop, the right

hemisphere of the brain maintains the visual, spatial, and kinaesthetic information stored

in the sketchpad. Because visual information is typically more difficult to remember than

auditory information, it is often converted to speech using preexisting associations. For

example, an individual that views a word on a page will typically recite this word to

himself or herself in an attempt to memorize it, rather than simply relying on their

memory of the image of the word.

Information from these subcomponents of the working memory model is actively

directed to the central executive, which was initially proposed to support storage, provide

an interface with long-term memory, and allocate attention towards different resources.

Subsequently, Baddeley and Hitch adopted the model of a supervisory attention system

(SAS), to explain how the central executive directs changes in behavior when adjusted

planning is required. This revision to the working model removed storage from the

central executive’s list of functions and designated the function of an interface with long-

term memory to a component called the episodic buffer. Consequently, the central

executive was left with the primary task of focusing and dividing attention towards

executive processes. The episodic buffer acts as a multimodal temporary store that links

traces from the phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad to long-term memory in an

effort to create meaningful ideas. The capacity of this buffer is limited to the amount of

information that can be maintained simultaneously without decay. Through the flexible

Page 16: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

13

integration of these traces, the episodic buffer is ultimately responsible for a state of

conscious awareness.

Memory During Development

Long-term memory is subdivided into categories depending on if a memory is

implicit or explicit in nature. Implicit memory is the unconscious retention of procedural

information. Simply stated, these memories instruct us how to perform learned activities

such as brushing our teeth, eating a meal, or driving a car. On the other hand, explicit

memory is the intentional remembrance of declarative and factual information.

Declarative memories are further divided into semantic memories and episodic

memories. Semantic memories are made up of concept-based knowledge, ideas, and facts

that are not drawn from personal experiences. For example, the knowledge that fish live

in water or that the United States Capitol is located in Washington, D.C. is a semantic

memory that can be explicitly recalled. Episodic memories are autobiographical events

made up from one’s personal collection of experiences. Such memories could include the

birth of a sibling or the first day of school. Together, these two subcategories make up

long-term explicit memory.

Memory storage is a complex mechanism that is largely influenced by

developmental maturation. Implicit memory, specifically priming, matures at a fairly

young age and is often fully developed by the age of four (Russo, Nichelli, Gibertoni, &

Corina, 1995). Priming, which is a unique and unconscious aspect of implicit memory,

occurs when prior exposure to a stimulus influences later exposure to that stimulus.

Children who have undergone some degree of priming typically perform better on picture

fragmentation tasks, during which prior exposure to an object would help them to better

Page 17: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

14

identify a partial picture of that object. Even so, the accuracy of implicit material

recognition is limited in early years and increases with age (Parkin & Streete, 1988)

Further, studies have shown that the processes utilized by children to encode and recall

these memories differ from those employed by fully matured adults (Carroll, Byrne, &

Kirsner, 1985).

Explicit memory develops significantly later than implicit memory. Despite this,

the immature recollection of explicit memories often contributes to implicit memory

performance (Cycowicz, Friedman, Snodgrass, & Rothstein, 2000). This is most

commonly exhibited in priming through early autobiographical memories, which are

established during childhood between the ages of three and eight (Nelson, 1989). For

instance, a young child who possesses an autobiographical memory of their most recent

birthday party could more accurately identify a fragmented picture of a birthday party.

However, it is not until adolescence that the ability to accurately recollect personal

experiences is fully functional. At this stage, individuals have cultivated personalized

rehearsal strategies, possess a larger knowledge base, and have developed a self-

awareness of their own capabilities and limitations with memory. These properties,

amongst others, allow for the fine tuning of encoding and retrieval processes that become

more efficient with age (Ceci & Howe, 1978).

Page 18: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

15

PSYCHOLOGICAL IMPACT ON MEMORY

Neonatal seizures resulting from HIE often have long-term negative effects on

cognitive development. In particular, patients commonly present with deficits in memory,

verbal abilities, visual abilities, and executive functions. While animal models of these

disorders thoroughly explain the physiological effects of seizures, they often fail to

suggest exactly how these effects contribute to observable memory deficits later in life.

To address this, the focus of this paper will now turn to our first set of hypotheses, in

which we will address three main memory deficits and their relationship to existing

psychological models for memory formation.

Hypothesis 1.1 – Breakdown of Executive Functions

Neonatal seizures frequently result in poor social conduct resulting from the

breakdown of executive functions, a set of skills that regulates behavior. When executive

functions are impaired, patients experience an inability to integrate past experiences with

present actions. For example, a child who is scolded for inappropriate behavior lacks the

ability to learn from his or her mistake. Because these integration deficits also correlate

with the ability to self-monitor behavior, we can conclude that the shortcomings

experienced in executive functions result from a failure within the central executive

component of memory. As discussed, the central executive functions in the allocation of

attention resources towards working memory. An impairment of the central executive’s

ability to direct attention would presumably present as a lack of focus in the individual. In

this case, the inability to retrieve, reflect upon, and manipulate information would most

certainly result in the impairment of executive functions.

Page 19: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

16

Hypothesis 1.2 - Developmental Amnesia

When patients present with later-life developmental amnesia, explicit and

episodic memories of personal facts are affected. Despite this, semantic memories and

working memory remain intact. Because these amnesic effects appear later on in life, we

can conclude that this deficit does not result from difficulty encoding memories for

storage, but perhaps from ineffective maintenance and retrieval. In this case, we could

postulate that dysfunction occurs within the central executive, episodic buffer, or both.

The central executive accesses the episodic buffer through the medium of conscious

awareness. Thus, attention is managed. If attention is not efficiently regulated due to

failure of the central executive, retrieval and reflection on information cannot be directed

appropriately. Similarly, if developmental amnesia is due to a failure within the episodic

buffer, episodic memories cannot be retrieved from long-term memory. Therefore, it is

reasonable to construe that developmental amnesia resulting from HIE affects the

maintenance and retrieval of memories through defects in the functions of both the

central executive and the episodic buffer.

Hypothesis 1.3 - Verbal and Visual Deficits

Verbal and visual deficits are an additional concern for HIE-induced neonatal

seizures. In these cases, it is semantic memory, not episodic memory, which is affected.

As in the case of developmental amnesia, individuals experience no challenge in learning

new material but struggle to successfully carry out the transfer phase. When familiar

information is presented with new features or in novel combinations, patients fail to

associate these modified tasks to the previous ones. The deficit in recall stems from the

inability to utilize information residing in the phonological loop and visuospatial

Page 20: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

17

sketchpad when conducting LTS searches for previously encoded associations. Impaired

control processes prevent patients from performing efficient searches and relating new

information to existing memories. In short, these individuals must create entirely new

memories without utilizing valuable associations with previously encoded memories.

Page 21: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

18

BIOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF NEONATAL SEIZURES

With any abstract psychological phenomena, there are underlying neural

substrates that provide explanation for clinical observations. To fully understand the

origin of the cognitive deficits that result from neonatal seizures, we must explore how

seizure activity affects brain function at a molecular level. We will now present a second

set of theories that explain how synaptic plasticity at the neuronal level influences the

behavioral plasticity that is necessary for memory formation. However, before we can

delve into the neural substrates of memory we must first review the physiological effects

of seizures.

Altered Electrical Activity

During seizures, neurons abandon normal function and begin to fire in sporadic

bursts of activity, inhibiting normal brain function. Depending on the area of the brain

involved, patients may exhibit varied states of consciousness (Cavanna & Monaco,

2009). If only an isolated portion of the brain is affected, then some degree of

consciousness may be preserved, indicating a more positive long-term outcome. In the

case of HIE-induced seizures, a chronic period of ischemia and consequent hypoxia

continually deprives neurons of the substrates needed for cellular metabolism. When this

occurs during the neonatal stage, the immature brain responds by inducing a complete

and rapid necrosis of the affected tissue (Shin, Hong, Taem, & Kim, 2002). If these

effects are progressive over time, cerebral blood flow in the affected region of the brain

may be significantly altered, including changes in venous pressure, arterial pressure, etc

(Shin et al., 2002).

Page 22: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

19

Calcification from Atrophy

After recurrent exposure to hypoxic conditions, the affected region of the brain

may begin to atrophy, resulting in an overall reduction of tissue volume (Jensen, 2009).

Atrophy from neonatal seizures is characterized by a decrease in neuronal size as well as

a loss of synaptic connections between neurons, contributing to a decrease in functional

capabilities. Additionally, atrophy from HIE often results in the calcification of ischemic

lesions. As detailed in a review by Ansar, Chincanchan, and Armstrong (1990), HIE

causes a rapid decay of the extracellular Ca2+ gradient, which plays a significant role in

the propagation of normal action potentials. Hypoxic conditions cause large quantities of

extracellular Ca2+ to rapidly flow into the cell, where it is taken up by mitochondira. This

drastic increase in mitochondrial Ca2+ inhibits normal function, ultimately resulting in

necrosis and irreversible cell death.

Decreased Synaptic Plasticity

Overall the increased propensity to seizures during the neonatal period results

from heightened neuroexcitability and augmented potential for synaptic plasticity. When

hypoxic conditions contribute to a general reduction in both the size and function of

affected regions, the developing brain exhibits a decreased potential for synaptic

plasticity. Long term potentiation (LTP), a process that is widely accepted to play a role

in learning and memory, is associated with a change in synaptic strength in response to

varied levels of transmission. Seizure-induced alterations in LTP during critical

developmental stages commonly result in the aforementioned cognitive, behavioral, and

motor deficits that plague patients far beyond childhood.

Page 23: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

20

NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF MEMORY

The potential for plasticity, both at a synaptic and behavioral level, is highest

during early developmental periods, particularly the perinatal period that lasts from the

22nd week of gestation until seven weeks after birth (Nagy, 2011). Synaptogenesis begins

during the sixth gestational week and undergoes a prominent increase during the two

months before and after birth (Silverstein & Jensen, 2007). Initial encounters with

sensory stimuli induce fine-tuning of the neural circuit, and these connections are

strengthened by subsequent encounters (Hebb, 1949). The variation of responses to both

novel and previously encountered stimuli are influenced by trace memories from prior

experiences. These associations between stimuli are built into memory and are the basis

for the capacity to learn. The following sections will examine the various structures

associated with memory formation, as well as the molecular processes that occur within

these structures.

Anatomical Structures

The process of encoding information as a memory is complex and involves a

large number of anatomical structures. Because the first step in memory storage,

regardless of the model employed, involves the registration of sensory input, virtually

every lobe of the cortex is involved. The parietal lobe assists in the internal mediation of

attention when filtering input (Berryhill, Chein, & Olson, 2011). Further, it can facilitate

recall but cannot incorporate external cues into recognition (Dobbins, Jaeger, Studer, &

Simons, 2012). The frontal lobe functions in coordination of behaviors, working memory,

and non-task based long-term memories. Studies have found that epileptics with frontal

Page 24: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

21

lobe damage must recruit wider areas of the brain when encoding memories for storage

(Centeno et al., 2012). In the temporal lobe, domain specificity, which suggests there are

specialized learning devices for different aspects of cognition, plays a significant role in

the recognition of information from cues. This function is critically important to the

ability to draw on associations between memories (Staresina, Cooper, & Henson, 2013).

While the perirhinal cortex is devoted specifically to object-processing, the

parahippocampal cortices are specific to scene-processing. One of the most striking

associations between an anatomical structure and memory formation occurs within the

cerebellum, which functions in the acquisition and storage of procedural memory. When

cerebellar degeneration occurs, the degree of cellular atrophy directly correlates with the

reduced capacity for motor learning (Thieme et al., 2013)

The limbic system is perhaps the greatest contributor to both the formation of

memories and the emotions associated with them (Prada-Alcala, Medina, Lopez, &

Quirarte, 2012). It encompasses a collection of structures that lie beneath the cerebrum

and on both sides of the thalamus. Two major components of the limbic system are the

amygdala and the hippocampus. The amygdala contributes primarily to emotion and fear

learning (Prada-Alcala et al., 2012; Ross & Slotnick, 2008). Conditioned fear learning

leaves a trace in the basolateral amygdala. In storing these traces, individuals either

associate the stimuli with fear or acclimate themselves to suppress fear in future

encounters. Interestingly, reactivation of this trace followed by suppressed fear can

abolish this memory altogether, in essence, attenuating the connectivity of the fear circuit

(Agren et al., 2012). Through the discovery of its participation in fear learning, the

amygdala has been recognized as a structure essential for the aversion to emotionally

Page 25: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

22

unpleasant experiences. The hippocampus, like the amygdala, is capable of associating an

emotional response to a memory. A recent study by Bellace, Williams, Mohamed, and

Faro (2013) found that the type of stimuli associated with emotional responses dictates

the geographical location of where the memory is stored. While the right hippocampus

generates emotional responses to visual stimuli, the left hippocampus constructs

associations with auditory stimuli (Wimmer & Shohamy, 2012). In addition, the

hippocampus functions in the consolidation of memories from STS to LTS, specifically

those associated with spatial learning. When attempting to retrieve memories, fMRI

studies have shown hippocampal activation is most significant during recall rather than

during recognition (Rugg et al., 2012)

Despite the amygdala’s significant contribution to memory, the synaptic basis of

memory provided in this paper will focus specifically on studies performed in the

hippocampus. The hippocampus is unique in that it has the capacity to store large

quantities of unrelated information as well as self-generate temporally evolving

memories, those that continue to be modified over time (Buzsáki & Moser, 2013). Early

hippocampal activation indicates that there is a rapid recruitment of memory processes

within this structure that enables memory retrieval (Horner et al., 2012). During

hippocampal activity, theta waves and slow oscillations drive the preferential firing of

hippocampal cell assemblies (Battaglia, Benchenane, Sirota, Pennartz, & Wiener, 2011).

Through the propagation of these waves to other structures, the hippocampus regulates

the exchange of information with nearby structures.

Memory consolidation, the process of preparing traces for LTS, has been most

extensively studied in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, one of four subdivisions in the

Page 26: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

23

cornu ammonis. The hippocampus receives the majority of its input from the entorhinal

cortex and is believed to be largely responsible for memory formation and consolidation,

processes that are frequently studied by examining spatial memory in rats. Information

from the entorhinal cortex travels via the perforant pathway to the dentate gyrus, CA3,

CA1, or the subiculum. Hippocampal studies often focus most closely on a pathway

known as the trisynaptic circuit (Figure 3). In this circuit, information passes from

granule cells of the dentate gyrus to CA3 pyramidal neurons via mossy fibers. Neural

transmission runs through CA3 axons to CA1 pyramidal neurons via Schaffer collaterals.

Axons from CA1 synapse in the deep layers of the subiculum, where information can

loop back around to the entorhinal cortex or pass to the fornix. Because the trisynaptic

circuit has been widely recognized for its role in memory formation, it is important to

understand the physiologic changes that take place in these neurons.

Figure 3. Schematic of the neural circuitry within the trisynaptic circuit of the hippocampus

Page 27: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

24

LTP and AMPA Receptor Cycling

As mentioned, LTP is an imperative process in the maintenance of synaptic

plasticity, during which synapses undergo a change in strength in response to varied

levels of transmission. These changes often directly correlate with the size of the

postsynaptic density (PSD), number of receptors, and amount of neurotransmitter

released. Since both learning and memory formation are believed to result from synaptic

plasticity, it is widely accepted that LTP plays a major role in these processes.

Neurotransmission begins when a depolarizing action potential propagates to the

terminal of a presynaptic axon. As depicted in Figure 4, presynaptic vesicles are

exocytosed from the axon terminal and their contents diffuse across the synaptic cleft.

The concentration of neurotransmitter in the synaptic cleft remains high for only a short

time, resulting in brief synaptic currents (Luscher & Malenka, 2012). Whereas receptors

on the presynaptic side participate in a feedback mechanism that modulates

neurotransmitter release, those on the postsynaptic dendrites bind neurotransmitters to

elicit a variety of cellular responses.

Figure 4. Depiction of synaptic neurotransmission that occurs during LTP (Sadava, 2008)

Page 28: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

25

During LTP, glutamate, an important neurotransmitter, is released from the

presynaptic axon and binds to two types of ionotropic receptors: N-methyl-D-aspartate

(NMDA) receptors and α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA)

receptors. These receptors directly form ion channel pores, unlike metabotropic receptors

that indirectly link with ion channels on the plasma membrane through signal

transduction mechanisms. Both NMDARs and AMPARs are permeable to a strong

inward current of Na+ and a weak outward current of K+, in addition to a Ca2+ current.

AMPARs carry a net inward current at negative potentials and a net outward current at

positive potentials.

AMPARs are either a homomeric or heteromeric mixture of four possible

subunits: GluA1, GluA2, GluA3, and GluA4. While homomeric AMPARs contain only

one type of subunit, heteromeric AMPARs consist of two sets of symmetric subunits. The

hippocampus, which is the primary site of LTP, is characterized by heteromeric

AMPARs containing GluA2, along with either GluA1, GluA3, or GluA4 (Kauer &

Malenka, 2006). The GluA2 subunit is unique in that it undergoes RNA editing after

transcription, during which the codon for a glutamine residue in the ion channel pore is

replaced by a codon for arginine (Luscher & Malenka, 2012). This exchange of codons

enables the subunit to move out of the endoplasmic reticulum where it can be

incorporated into the AMPAR. The size of the arginine residue in modified GluA2

subunits prevents the passage of divalent ions such as Ca2+. As a result, AMPARs lacking

GluA2 are permeable to Ca2+ and have an inward-rectifying current.

Synapses that contain NMDARs with few to no AMPARs are considered “silent,”

as their ability to transmit depolarizing action potentials is significantly impaired. The

Page 29: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

26

silent synapse hypothesis, which states that AMPARs are recruited to inactive synapses

by the induction of LTP, is based on the belief that NMDARs are capable of detecting

twice the quanta of glutamate that can be detected by AMPARs (Kullmann, Asztely, &

Walker, 2000). In theory, LTP can be modulated by NMDARs alone if the amount of

glutamate released from the presynaptic axon terminal is insufficient to open the

AMPARs located on the membrane. However, NMDARs are occluded by an Mg2+ block

that requires depolarization to be expelled. As a result, the flow of Na+, K+, and Ca2+

through the cell is a voltage-dependent process. Thus, accentuating the need for

AMPARs, which drive synaptic signaling.

It has been mentioned that the absence of AMPARs renders the synapse silent.

Therefore, it is reasonable to conclude that AMPARs are recruited to the membrane

during LTP. During cycling, AMPARs come from a reserve pool of receptors that are

stored within early endosomes (Park, Penick, Edwards, Kauer, & Ehlers, 2004). AMPAR

degradation within neurons is regulated in an activity-dependent manner that is specific

to both the type of subunit receptor and the nature of its internalization, the process of

endocytosing AMPARs from the synaptic membrane. AMPA-induced internalization

causes GluA2 subunits to enter the recycling pathway. However, NMDA-induced

internalization causes GluA2 subunits to be sent to late endosomes and lysosomes for

degradation. On the other hand, this same type of internalization causes GluA1 to remain

active in the recycling pathway (Lee, Simonetta, & Sheng, 2004). Further, the source of

AMPARs during cycling induced by LTP has been confirmed by studies using mutants

that prevent endocytotic translocation to the plasma membrane. In these cases, the overall

expression of AMPARs was significantly decreased (Park et al., 2004). AMPAR cycling

Page 30: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

27

is regulated by the Ca2+-induced activation of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase

II (CaMKII) and protein kinase A (PKA), which phosphorylate serine residues on the

AMPAR subunits. The phosphorylation of serine 831 (S831) and serine 845 (S845) sites

on the GluA1 subunit during the induction of LTP causes these subunits to be inserted to

the plasma membrane (Santos, Carvalho, Caldeira, & Duarte, 2009). Interestingly, this

process depends largely on the history of the synapse itself. Immature or previously

depressed synapses can experience an intense upregulation of serine residue

phorphorylation upon the administration of high frequency stimulation (Lee et al., 2004).

However, these subunits are not delivered directly to the PSD and require NMDA

activation for their localization (Santos et al., 2009). Lipid membrane and proteins

accompany the delivery of GluA1 subunits, which explains why AMPAR cycling plays a

vital role in the increase of both synaptic strength and size.

Page 31: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

28

PHYSIOLOGICAL IMPACT ON MEMORY

Because LTP is viewed as a long-lasting enhancement of synaptic transmission, it

is no surprise that seizure activity, which is characterized by sporadic and uncoordinated

synaptic excitation, has an effect on this process. The risk for seizures is highest during

the neonatal period, a critical time for synaptic development, during which dendritic

processes are constantly expanding and retracting, sampling their environment for

potential synaptic partners (Cline & Haas, 2008). During this time, the expression of

ionotropic glutamate receptors is at its peak and contributes to the neonate’s increased

susceptibility to seizures. In the hippocampus beginning in the first postnatal week, the

synapses gradually become functional as purely NMDAR-based synapses, silent

synapses, are converted to functional AMPAR/NMDAR synapses. This transformation is

input specific and dependent upon the occurrence of LTP (Durand, Kovalchuk, &

Konnerth, 1996). The increase in the number of activated receptors modulates the

increase in amplitude of excitatory post-synaptic currents (EPSCs) (Stubblefield &

Benke, 2010).

Hypothesis 2.1 - Induction and Saturation of LTP

The excitability observed during seizure activity induces LTP by stimulating

AMPAR cycling. Phosphorylation of the GluA1 and GluA2 subunits is evident shortly

after seizures, resulting in hyperexcitability that increases sensitivity to future seizures.

This excitability may reduce the number of silent synapses present in the neonatal brain

by recruiting AMPARs to the plasma membrane.

Page 32: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

29

Early studies investigating the connection between seizures and synaptic plasticity

examined the effects of electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) on cognitive function. ECT, an

early form of treatment for severe affective disorder, was administered via electrical

seizure induction. Patients undergoing ECT reported difficulty in learning new material

for up to six months after treatment, especially when multiple seizures were induced

(Reid et al., 1997). Additional studies performed using adult male hooded Lister rats

aimed to emulate these effects through the induction of electroconvulsive seizures (ECS)

(Reid et al.). The study showed that the amplitude and frequency of excitatory

postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) associated with LTP increased with each application of

ECS, stabilizing around 4-5 seizures. However, when the researchers attempted to induce

LTP following the seizures, the rats showed impaired performance in their ability to form

memories regarding spatial bias in a water maze. Overall, the over expression of EPSPs

resulted in decreased LTP. From this, it can be concluded that ECS itself induces LTP

and saturates its mechanism of action in a widespread and indiscriminate manner,

reducing overall synaptic potential.

Hypothesis 2.2 - Increased Phosphorylation of AMPA Receptors

More recent studies have focused on the effects of seizures on synaptic plasticity

in neonates, who are undergoing a period of critical synaptic development and have the

potential to experience long-term effects. In subjecting postnatal day 10 (P10) rat pups to

hypoxia-induced seizures (HS), Rakhade et al. (2008) demonstrated an increase in the

amplitude of AMPAR-mediated excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) from CA1

pyramidal neurons. The observed changes in EPSCs correlated with increased

phosphorylation of the S831 and S845 sites on GluA1. Therefore, it is reasonable to

Page 33: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

30

conclude that increased AMPAR-mediated currents were a result of increased GluA1

phosphorylation.

In addition to the alterations experienced by GluA1, HS appeared to cause a

significant increase in the phosphorylation of GluA2. P10 rat pups naturally have Ca2+

permeable AMPARs due to decreased GluA2 expression. HS further decreased the

expression of GluA2 on the synaptic membrane 48 hours after stimulation via

phosphorylation of the serine 880 (S880) site, resulting in GluA2-lacking AMPARs that

produced an inward rectifying current. PKA and CaMKII are responsible for the

phosphorylation of S845, while protein kinase C (PKC) phosphorylates both S831 and

S880 (Santos et al., 2009). Since increased stimulation of LTP correlates with an increase

in the phosphorylation of S831, S845, and S880, it is reasonable to suspect that increased

activity of PKA, CaMKII, and PKC is also observed. Further, it was found that the

GluA2-lacking AMPAR antagonist philanthotoxin was capable of blocking LTP if

applied before and during stimulation (Plant et al., 2006). Combined, these results

provide further evidence that the Ca2+ flow permitted by GluA2-lacking AMPARs is

necessary for the induction of LTP during development.

Hypothesis 2.3 - Reduction of Silent Synapses

Because studies have established that the HS-stimulated increase in

phosphorylation of AMPAR subunits caused a profound effect on LTP, it can be

hypothesized that the phosphorylation induced by HS may alter the fraction of silent

synapses present on the postsynaptic membrane. Silent synapses, which lack AMPARs

altogether, are characterized by a large failure rate when attempting to evoke EPSCs at

negative membrane potentials of -55mV to -65mV (Rakhade et al., 2008). A study by

Page 34: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

31

Zhou, Lippman, Sun, and Jensen (2011) noted that as predicted, the ratio of silent to

functional synapses in P10 rat pups was reduced during HS. The possibility of Mg2+

insensitivity was ruled out as a factor, indicating that NMDARs did not contribute to the

failure rate of EPSCs used to calculate these ratios (Zhou et al., 2011). Additionally, the

effects of hypoxia alone were found to be negligible due to the lack of difference between

hypoxic pups that did not experience seizures and the control group. Together, these

results suggest that HS diminished silent synapses by simply introducing AMPARs to the

membrane.

Further, HS appeared to increase the colocalization of AMPARs and NMDARs

across the membrane. Synaptophysin, GLUR1, and NR1, functional subunits from each

receptor, were triple immunolabeled to detect the presence of NR1 puncta that were in

contact with both synaptophysin puncta and GLUR1 puncta (Zhou et al., 2011). HS

decreased the percentage of synapses containing only contacts between NR1 puncta and

synaptophysin puncta, indicating that HS decreased the number of silent synapses (Zhou

et al., 2011). The reduction in silent synapses by increased colocalization can be inferred

to be a result of AMPAR subunit phosphorylation, which ultimately moves AMPARs to

the membrane. It is important to note that these processes occur during normal

stimulation of LTP. However, the occurrence of a seizure magnifies these effects and

unnecessarily induces LTP in an unregulated manner. In effect, seizures saturate the

mechanism and inhibit the ability to induce LTP during actual learning and memory

formation.

Page 35: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

32

CONCLUSION

Neonatal seizures, particularly those due to HIE, are a significant problem in both

term and pre-term infants and are influenced by a variety of inherent risk factors.

Neonates exhibit the highest proclivity to seizure activity due to their heightened

potential for neuroexcitation, a property that has a profound impact on the potential for

synaptic plasticity. Early life neonatal seizures are commonly associated with long-term

cognitive impairments, such as those seen in learning and memory. While not classified

as an epileptic disorder, the occurrence of seizures during the neonatal period increases

susceptibility to epilepsy far into adulthood.

Through a comprehensive understanding of existing memory paradigms, such as

Atkinson and Schiffrin’s multi-store model and Baddeley and Hitch’s working memory

model, we can begin to explain how neonatal seizures result in impaired executive

function, verbal and visual function, and memory storage processes. During this critical

time for development, seizure activity alters the function of memory components that are

critical to both STS and LTS. Additionally, seizures disrupt the control processes used

when attempting to process sensory stimuli, to prevent the decay of traces from STS, and

to retrieve memories from LTS. From this, we can deduce that the apparent memory

damage resulting from neonatal seizures is attributed to the third model proposed by

Martinos et al. (2012) in an effort to explain the source of cognitive dysfunction. In this

model, it is suggested that both an inherent predisposition to neonatal seizures, as well as

the seizure itself, contribute to the cognitive deficits seen in these individuals.

On a physiologic level, the impairments seen in these individuals can be

explained through the relationship between memory formation and LTP. LTP, a process

Page 36: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

33

that is widely accepted to play a role in learning and memory, is associated with a change

in synaptic strength in response to varied levels of transmission. Alterations in LTP

during critical developmental stages commonly result in cognitive, behavioral, and motor

deficits that plague patients far beyond childhood. Rat studies have found that HIE-

induced seizures induce the activation of various kinases that phosphorylate serine

residues on AMPAR subunits. This process triggers the mobilization of AMPARs to the

synaptic membrane, thus decreasing the ratio of silent to active synapses and allowing for

heightened synaptic potential. Overall, these studies have found that seizure activity

overstimulates LTP, thus saturating its mechanism and reducing the potential for

effective memory storage. Thus, resulting in many forms of memory impairment and

cognitive setbacks.

In this paper, we have presented two related, yet distinctly separate sets of

hypotheses that explain the cognitive deficits from seizures by means of both

psychological and physiological perspectives. Using the various STS and LTS

components from existing memory models, we have explained the theoretical basis of

seizure-induced cognitive deficits. Additionally, we have used findings from studies at a

synaptic level to propose an association between the neural substrates of memory and the

neural substrates of seizures. However, the direct relationship between these neural

substrates and the memory components that they ultimately affect is beyond the scope of

this paper and future studies are needed to more clearly elucidate this relationship.

Page 37: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

34

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Agren, T., Engman, J., Frick, A., Björkstrand, J., Larsson, E. M., Furmark, T., &

Fredrikson, M. (2012) Disruption of reconsolidation erases a fear memory trace in

the human amygdala. Science, 337(6101), 1550-1552.

Ansari, M. Q., Chincanchan, C. A., & Armstrong, D. L. (1990) Brain calcifications in

hypoxic-ischemic lesions: an autopsy review. Pediatric Neurology, 6(2), 94-101.

Baddeley, A. D. (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?

Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(11), 417-423.

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974) Working memory. New York: Academic Press.

Battaglia, F. P., Benchenane, K., Sirota, A., Pennartz, C. M. A., & Wiener, S. I. (2011)

The hippocampus: hub of brain network communication for memory. Trends in

Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 310-318.

Bellace, M., Williams, J. M., Mohamed, F. B., & Faro, S. H. (2013) An fMRI study of

the activation of the hippocampus by emotional memory. International Journal of

Neuroscience, 123(2), 121-127.

Berryhill, M. E., Chein, J., & Olson, I. R. (2011) At the intersection of attention and

memory: the mechanistic role of the posterior parietal lobe in working memory.

Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1306-1315.

Binford, J. R., & Gettys C. (1965) Nonstationarity in paired-associate learning as

indicated by a second guess procedure. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 2,

190-195.

Bonelli, S. B., Thompson, P. J., Yogarajah, M., Powell, R. H. W., Samson, R. S.,

McEvoy, A. W., . . . Duncan, J. S. (2013) Memory reorganization following

Page 38: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

35

anterior temporal lobe resection: a longitudinal functional MRI study. Brain: A

Journal of Neurology, 136(6), 1889-1900.

Buschman, T. J., Siegel, M., Roy, J. E., & Miller, E. K. (2011) Neural substrates of

cognitive capacity limitations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

of the United States of America, 108(27), 11252-11255.

Buzsáki, G., & Moser, E. I. (2013) Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the

hippocampal-entorhinal system. Nature Neuroscience, 16(2), 130-138.

Carroll, M. V., Byrne, B., & Kirsner, K. (1985) Autobiographical memory and perceptual

learning: a developmental study using picture recognition, naming latency, and

perceptual identification. Memory and Cognition, 13(3), 273-279.

Cavanna, A. E., & Monaco, F. (2009) Brain mechanisms of altered conscious states

during epileptic seizures. Nature Reviews Neurology, 5, 267-276.

Ceci, S. J., & Howe, M. J. A. (1978) Semantic knowledge as determinant of

developmental differences in recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,

26, 230-245.

Centeno, M., Vollmar, C., O'Muircheartaigh, J., Stretton, J., Bonelli, S. B., Symms, M.

R., . . . Koepp, M. J. (2012) Memory in frontal lobe epilepsy: an fMRI study.

Epilepsia, 53(10), 1756-1764.

Cline, H., & Haas, K. (2008) The regulation of dendritic arbor development and plasticity

by glutamatergic synaptic input: a review of the synaptotrophic hypothesis.

Journal of Physiology, 586(6), 1509-1517.

Cowan, L. D. (2002) The epidemiology of the epilepsies in children. Mental Retardation

and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8(3), 171-181.

Page 39: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

36

Cowan, L. D., Rutherford, M., Groenendaal, F., Eken, P., Mercuri, E., Bydder, G. M., . . .

de Vries, L. S. (2003) Origin and timing of brain lesions in term infants with

neonatal encephalopathy. Lancet, 361(9359), 736-742.

Cycowicz, Y. M., Friedman, D., Snodgrass, J. G., & Rothstein, M. (2000) A

developmental trajectory in implicit memory is revealed by picture fragment

completion. Memory, 8(1), 19-35.

Dehaes, M., Aggarwal, A., Lin, P.-Y., Rosa Fortuno, C., Fenoglio, A., Roche-Labarbe,

N., . . . Grant, P. E. (2013) Cerebral oxygen metabolism in neonatal hypoxic

ischemic encephalopathy during and after therapeutic hypothermia. Journal of

Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, August, 1-8.

Dobbins, I. G., Jaeger, A., Studer, B., & Simons, J. S. (2012) Use of explicit memory

cues following parietal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 50(13), 2992-3003.

Durand, G. M., Kocalchuk, Y., & Konnerth, A. (1996) Long-term potentiation and

functional synapse induction in developing hippocampus. Nature, 381, 71-75.

Gano, D., Sargent, M. A., Miller, S. P., Connolly, M. B., Wong, P., Glass, H. C., . . .

Chau, V. (2013) MRI findings in infants with infantile spasms after neonatal

hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Pediatric Neurology, 49(6), 401-405.

Glass, H. C., Glidden, D., Jeremy, R. J., Barkovich, A. J., Ferriero, D. M., & Miller, S. P.

(2009) Clinical neonatal seizures are independently associated with outcome in

infants at risk for hypoxic-ischemic brain injury. The Journal of Pediatrics,

155(3), 318-323.

Hallberg, B., & Blennow, M. (2013) Investigations for neonatal seizures. Seminars in

Fetal & Neonatal Medicine, 18(4), 196-201.

Page 40: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

37

Harbert, M. J., Tam, E. W. Y., Glass, H. C., Bonifacio, S. L., Haeusslein, L. A.,

Barkovich, a. J., . . . Ferriero, D. M. (2011) Hypothermia is correlated with

seizure absence in perinatal stroke. Journal of Child Neurology, 26(9), 1126-

1130.

Hebb, D. O. (1949) The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New

York: Wiley and Sons.

Horner, A. J., Gadian, D. G., Fuentemilla, L., Jentschke, S., Vargha-Khadem, F., &

Duzel, E. (2012) A rapid, hippocampus-dependent, item-memory signal that

initiates context memory in humans. Current Biology, 22(24), 2369-2374.

Jensen, F. E. (2009) Developmental factors in the pathogenesis of neonatal seizures.

Journal of Pediatric Neurology, 7(1), 5-12.

Kauer, J. A., & Malenka, R. C. (2006) News and views: LTP and AMPA receptors

trading places. Nature Neuroscience, 9(5), 593-594.

Kohelet, D., Shochat, R., Lusky, A., & Reichman, B. (2006a) Risk factors for neonatal

seizures in very low birthweight infants: population-based survey. Journal of

Child Neurology, 19(2), 123-128.

Kohelet, D., Shochat, R., Lusky, A., & Reichman, B. (2006b) Risk factors for seizures in

very low birthweight infants with periventricular leukomalacia. Journal of Child

Neurology, 21(11), 965-970.

Kullmann, D. M., Asztely, F., & Walker, M. C. (2000) The role of mammalian ionotropic

receptors in synaptic plasticity: LTP, LTD and epilepsy. Cellular and Molecular

Life Sciences, 57(11), 1551-1561.

Page 41: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

38

Kurabe, S., Sorimachi, T., Sasaki, O., Koike, T., & Fujii, Y. (2009) Low birth weight as a

risk factor for seizure following acute subdural hematoma. Child's Nervous

System, 25(9), 1101-1104.

Lee, S. H., Simonetta, A., & Sheng, M. (2004) Subunit rules governing the sorting of

internalized AMPA receptors in hippocampal neurons. Neuron, 43, 221-236.

Lindstrom, K., Hallberg, B., Blennow, M., Wolff, K., Fernell, E., & Westgren, M. (2008)

Moderate neonatal encephalopathy: pre- and perinatal risk factors and long-term

otcome. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 87, 503-509.

Luscher, C., & Malenka, R. C. (2012) NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation

and long-term depression (LTP/LTD). Cold Spring Harbour Perspectives in

Biology, 4, 1-15.

Martinos, M. M., Yoong, M., Patil, S., Chin, R. F., Neville, B. G., Scott, R. C., & de

Haan, M. (2012) Recognition memory is impaired in children after prolonged

febrile seizures. Brain, 135(10), 3153-3164.

Milner, B. (1959) The memory defect in bilateral hippocampal lesions. Psychiatric

Research Reports, 11, 43-58.

Milner, B. (1967) Amnesia following operation on the temporal lobes. In O. L. Zangwill

and C. W. M. Whitty (Eds.), Amnesia (109-133). London and Washington, D.C.:

Butterworth.

Nagy, E. (2011) The newborn infant: a missing stage in developmental psychology.

Infant and Child Development, 20, 3-19.

Nelson, K. (1989) Remembering: A Functional Developmental Perspective. New York:

Springer.

Page 42: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

39

Odd, D. E., Gunnell, D., Lewis, G., & Rasmussen, F. (2011) Long-term impact of poor

birth condition on social and exonomic outcomes in early adulthood. Pediatrics,

127, 1498-1504.

Park, M., Penick, E. C., Edwards, J. G., Kauer, J. A., & Ehlers, M. D. (2004) Recycling

endosomes supply AMPA receptors for LTP. Science, 305(5692), 1972-1975.

Parkin, A. J., & Streete, S. (1988) Implicit and explicit memory in young children and

adults. British Journal of Psychology, 79(3), 361-369.

Perez, A., Ritter, S., Brotschi, B., Werner, H., Caflisch, J., Martin, E., & Latal, B. (2013)

Long-term neurodevelopmental outcome with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy.

The Journal of Pediatrics, 163(2), 454-459.

Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. (1959) Short-term retenion of individual verbal items.

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193-198.

Plant, K., Pelkey, K. A., Bortolotto, Z. A., Morita, D., Terashima, A., McBain, C. J., . . .

Isaac, J. T. R. (2006) Transient incorporation of native GluR2-lacking AMPA

receptors during hippocampal long-term potentiation. Nature Neuroscience, 9(5),

602-604.

Prada-Alcala, R. A., Medina, A. C., Lopez, N. S., & Quirarte, G. L. (2012) Intense

emotional experiences and enhanced training prevent memory loss induced by

post-training amnesic treatments administered to the striatum, amygdala,

hippocampus or substantia nigra. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23(5-6), 501-

508.

Page 43: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

40

Rakhade, S. N., Zhou, C., Aujla, P. K., Fishman, R., Sucher, N. J., & Jensen, F. E. (2008)

Early alterations of AMPA receptors mediate synaptic potentiation induced by

neonatal seizures. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(32), 7979-7990.

Reid, I. A. N. C., Stewart, A., & Stewart, C. A. (1997) Seizures, memory and synaptic

plasticity. Seizure: European Journal of Epilepsy, 6(5), 351-359.

Ross, R. S., & Slotnick, S. D. (2008) The hippocampus is preferentially associated with

memory for spatial context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 432-446.

Rugg, M. D., Vilberg, K. L., Mattson, J. T., Yu, S. S., Johnson, J. D., & Suzuki, M.

(2012) Item memory, context memory and the hippocampus: fMRI evidence.

Neuropsychologia, 50(13), 3070-3079.

Russo, R., Nichelli, P., Gibertoni, M., & Corina, C. (1995) Developmental trends in

implicit and explicit memory: a picture completion study. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 59, 566-578.

Sadava, D. E. (2008) Life: The Sciences of Biology. Maryland: Sinauer Associates.

Sans, A. (2011) Developmental amnesia as a focal cognitive sequela of a neonatal

pathology. Revista de Neurologiá, 52(1), S29-38.

Santos, S. D., Carvalho, A. L., Caldeira, M. V., & Duarte, C. B. (2009) Regulation of

AMPA receptors and synaptic plasticity. Neuroscience, 158(1), 105-125.

Shankaran, S. (2009) Neonatal encephalopathy: treatment with hypothermia. Journal of

Neurotrama, 26(3), 437-443.

Shin, W. C., Hong, S. B., Tae, W. S., & Kim, S. E. (2002) Ictal hyperfusion patterns

according to the progression of temporal lobe seizures. Neurology, 58, 373-380.

Page 44: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

41

Silverstein, F. S., & Jensen, F. E. (2007) Neonatal seizures. Annals of Neurology, 62(2),

112-120.

Staresina, B. P., Cooper, E., & Henson, R. N. (2013) Reversible information flow across

the medial temporal lobe: the hippocampus links cortical modules during memory

retrieval. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(35), 14184-14192.

Stubblefield, E. A., & Benke, T. A. (2010) Distinct AMPA-type glutamatergic synapses

in developing rat CA1 hippocampus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104, 1899-

1912.

Thieme, A., Thürling, M., Galuba, J., Burciu, R. G., Göricke, S., Beck, A., . . . Timmann,

D. (2013) Storage of a naturally acquired conditioned response is impaired in

patients with cerebellar degeneration. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 136(7),

2063-2076.

van Handel, M., de Sonneville, L., de Vries, L. S., Jongmans, M. J., & Swaab, H. (2012)

Specific memory impairment following neonatal encephalopathy in term-born

children. Developmental Neuripsychology, 37, 30-50.

van Rooij, L. G. M., van den Broek, M. P. H., Rademaker, C. M. A., & de Vries, L. S.

(2013) Clinical management of seizures in newborns: diagnosis and treatment.

Paediatric Drugs, 15(1), 9-18.

Viggedal, G., Kristjansdottir, R., Olsson, I., Rydenhag, B., & Uvebrant, P. (2012)

Cognitive development from two to ten years after pediatric epilepsy surgery.

Epilepsy & Behavior, 25(1), 2-8.

Wimmer, G. E., & Shohamy, D. (2012) Preference by association: how memory

mechanisms in the hippocampus bias decisions. Science, 338(6104), 270-273.

Page 45: The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode ...The Impact of Neonatal Seizures on the Ability to Encode, Store, and Retrieve Memories Jamie L. Archambault ! NSC 371 Evidence

42

Zhou, C., Lippman, J. J. B., Sun, H., & Jensen, F. E. (2011) Hypoxia-induced neonatal

seizures diminish silent synapses and long-term potentiation in hippocampal CA1

neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(50), 18211-18222.