51
1 Journal of Marketing Research Article Postprint © 2011, American Marketing Association All rights reserved. Cannot be reprinted without the express permission of the American Marketing Association. The Impact of Candidate Appearance and Advertising Strategies on Election Results JoAndrea Hoegg Assistant Professor, Marketing Sauder School of Business University of British Columbia 2053 Main Mall Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z2 Canada Tel: (604) 827-4541 Fax: (604) 822-4697 [email protected] Michael V. Lewis Associate Professor, Marketing Goizueta Business School Emory University 1300 Clifton Road NE Atlanta, Georgia 30322 Tel: (404) 727-9438 Fax: (404) 727-3552 [email protected] Authors’ names appear in alphabetical order. The authors wish to acknowledge the helpful input of the editor, associate editor and reviewers, and to thank Darren W. Dahl, Richard J. Lutz, and Katherine White for their comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

1

Journal of Marketing Research Article Postprint © 2011, American Marketing Association All rights reserved. Cannot be reprinted without the express permission of the American Marketing Association. 

The Impact of Candidate Appearance and Advertising Strategies on Election Results

JoAndrea Hoegg Assistant Professor, Marketing

Sauder School of Business University of British Columbia

2053 Main Mall Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z2

Canada Tel: (604) 827-4541 Fax: (604) 822-4697

[email protected]

Michael V. Lewis Associate Professor, Marketing

Goizueta Business School Emory University

1300 Clifton Road NE Atlanta, Georgia 30322

Tel: (404) 727-9438 Fax: (404) 727-3552

[email protected]

Authors’ names appear in alphabetical order. The authors wish to acknowledge the helpful input of the editor, associate editor and reviewers, and to thank Darren W. Dahl, Richard J. Lutz, and Katherine White for their comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Page 2: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

2

The Impact of Candidate Appearance and Advertising Strategies on Election Results

Spending on political advertising has grown dramatically in recent years, and political campaigns

increasingly adopt the language and techniques of marketing. As such political marketing efforts

proliferate, the factors that drive electoral success warrant greater attention and investigation.

The present research employs a combination of laboratory studies and analysis of actual election

results to reveal influences of candidate appearance and spending strategies in campaigns. The

authors analyze how personality trait inferences based on candidate appearance interact with

political party brand image, advertising spending, and negative advertising. The results indicate

that appearance-based inferences about candidates influence election outcomes, but their impact

is driven partially by trait associations at the party brand level. This interaction between

appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of

negative advertising. The findings have implications for the marketing of political candidates in

terms of their party’s brand image.

Page 3: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

3

It is widely accepted that candidate appearance influences election outcomes. This view

is supported by anecdotal evidence (Stanton 2000) and academic study (e.g. Rosenberg and

McCafferty 1987; Todorov et al. 2005). It is also widely believed that elections are in many ways

marketing contests, with outcomes heavily influenced by the marketing efforts of candidates and

associated political parties (Faber et al. 1993; Homer and Batra 1994; Klein and Ahluwalia 2005;

Newman and Sheth 1985; Pinkleton 1997). This paper presents the results of an empirical study

of the intersection of candidate appearance and marketing tactics. We examine how personality

judgments based on candidate appearance interact with marketing variables including party

brand image, advertising spending, and negative advertising.

The issues of candidate appearance, campaign spending, and negative advertising have

received individual attention in the psychology and political science literatures (e.g. Erikson and

Palfrey 1998; Levitt 1994; Welch 1974), but a marketing-focused perspective offers additional

insights. Specifically, we investigate these variables in terms of how they interact with the

parties’ brand positions. Given that the two major U.S. political parties have distinct positions

and associations, appearance-based inferences about candidates may have asymmetrical effects

across parties. Our guiding premise is that the trait associations that voters have about each party

alter the effects of appearance-based trait judgments about individual candidates. We examine

this complex issue using a series of experiments and an analysis of U.S. congressional election

results.

This work builds on previous research showing that candidates with an appearance

conveying a high level of overall competence enjoy greater electoral success (Todorov et al.

2005). We extend this research by demonstrating that within the positive perceptual space related

to candidates’ perceived competence, there exist subtle but distinct traits that can be

communicated visually and have greater explanatory power than an overall measure. More

Page 4: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

4

importantly, we find that these distinct traits interact with party brand. Specifically, we show that

the physical appearance most beneficial to a Republican candidate is not the same appearance

that best benefits a Democratic candidate. We show that this asymmetry stems from differences

in the traits that voters associate with each party.

This paper is organized as follows: We begin with a review of relevant literature in

marketing, psychology, and political science and present our hypotheses. We then discuss a

series of experiments demonstrating the interaction between trait inferences and party affiliation.

Findings from those studies are used to develop specifications to examine outcomes for a subset

of congressional elections. A discussion of the implications and limitations of our work, along

with suggestions for future research, concludes the paper.

BACKGROUND

Candidate Traits and Political Branding

Among the most basic components of a candidate’s image are his or her perceived trait

qualities. Candidate personality trait impressions have been found to significantly influence voter

choice, in some cases to a greater degree than issues (Shanks and Miller 1990). Research has

attempted to identify the traits that exert the most influence on voters; however, agreement on the

exact traits remains elusive (Funk 1996). Funk (1996) highlights several distinct “dimensions”

that have been shown to impact voter preference, including competence, trustworthiness, and

warmth/likeability. Competence encompasses traits such as intelligence, reliability, competence,

and effectiveness, and it has been shown to be the most important dimension in predicting voter

choice (Markus 1982; Shanks and Miller 1990; Todorov et al. 2005). Trustworthiness involves

morality and honesty. Warmth or likeability suggests caring and sociability.

The identification of influential personality dimensions is useful for political marketing

Page 5: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

5

because they may be translatable to the perceptual space used by voters to assess candidates.

Each of these dimensions has a positive and negative side (Asch 1946; Judd et al. 2005;

Rosenberg et al. 1968). Obviously all candidates will attempt to convey images on the positive

side of each dimension, i.e., images of competence, trustworthiness, and likeability rather than

images involving ineptitude, dishonesty, or meanness. However, given that voters have

heterogeneous preferences, it is likely that more subtle positioning differences are important. For

example, beneath the umbrella of the positive dimension of competence may be specific skills or

distinct traits that appeal differentially to various segments of voters. Image management efforts

can focus on dissecting the broader dimensions and highlighting conceptually distinct constructs

to make subtle but critical distinctions in how candidates are perceived.

Competence vs. intelligence. Although prior research has found that competence and

intelligence are correlated, we propose that there are important differences between the

constructs in terms of judgments of a candidate’s ability. This is illustrated by Rosenberg et al.

(1968), who used multidimensional scaling to identify the structure of human personality

judgments. Although ability-related constructs such as intelligence, practicality, and

industriousness do tend to load on a single dimension, the authors found important differences

among the constructs. For example, a significant distance separates the constructs of practical

and reliable from intelligent. Consistent with this distinction, a thesaurus links intelligence to

being brainy, and links competence to being skilled or proficient. In other words, although the

traits are similar, there are differences. It is possible, for example, to imagine someone who is

highly intelligent but ineffective at accomplishing tasks, or a person who is a highly reliable

employee, but not brilliant. These types of distinctions are of particular importance in politics,

where perceptions of candidate personalities are influenced not only by candidates themselves

but also by opponents. An example of this occurred in the 1952 presidential election when

Page 6: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

6

Richard Nixon referred to Adlai Stevenson as an “egghead” (Hofstadter 1966), a term

acknowledging Stevenson’s intellect but simultaneously suggesting his lack of common sense.

To explore differences in the constructs of intelligence and competence we ran a pilot

study in which 41 undergraduate students rated the similarity of “competence” and “intelligence”

to a number of other traits, adapted from brand personality research (e.g. daring, charming,

reliable, cheerful, etc.) (Aaker 1997). On a 7-point scale where 1 = “not at all similar to

[competence]” and 7 = “extremely similar to [competence]” we found that, consistent with prior

literature, competence was rated quite similar to intelligence (M = 5.68). However, competence

was rated more similar to other traits such as good judgment (M = 6.22) and successful (M =

6.24) than to intelligence: t(40) = 2.11, p = .02 and t(40) = 1.91, p = .06, for good judgment and

successful, respectively. Similarly, competence and reliability were seen as more similar than

were intelligence and reliability (M = 5.85 vs. M = 4.17, t(40) = 7.3, p < .001).

Personality traits in party positioning. An added layer of complexity in the judgment of

candidate traits is the fact that each candidate is a member of a group (i.e., party) that likely has

its own trait associations. Although research on candidate traits has focused on how personality

traits are inferred for individual candidates (e.g. Funk 1996), it may be revealing to consider how

personality traits operate at the party brand level. Branding is a critical issue in political

marketing since the two major U.S. parties need to define distinct positions to differentiate

themselves, yet only a few academic papers have focused on understanding the brand images and

traits that voters associate with the parties (Downs 1957; Findlay 2008; Gelb and Sorescu 2000).

One area of research that has considered differences in party association is issue

ownership theory, which suggests that parties influence voters through their focus on key issues

(Petrocik 1996). For example, Brewer (2009) found that the Democratic Party has consistently

enjoyed a positive image regarding economic issues, while Republicans have tended to enjoy

Page 7: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

7

positive images in terms of government management. Although issue ownership theory

demonstrates important differences in the public perceptions of the parties, its focus is primarily

on the preferences of the electorate with regard to issues, rather than differences in the parties’

brand images or positioning. Considering party image at a more fundamental level is potentially

instructive because it can be viewed in terms of broad trait associations, rather than according to

narrow statements of policy on specific social or economic issues.

Brand personality research has identified a variety of traits used to describe product

brands (Aaker 1997). However, in the case of political parties, we suggest that it could be more

informative to distinguish them utilizing the same personality traits as those applied to individual

political candidates. Given that stereotypes at the group level often reflect traits at the individual

level (Fiske et al. 2002), it is useful to consider the extent to which these traits are associated

with each political brand. In particular, we focus on the dimension that has been previously

referred to as competence, but which can be construed as a broad dimension of ability (Fiske et

al. 2002). We propose that this ability dimension can be segmented into a trait of practical or

reliable competence and a separate trait of intelligence. We further propose that these traits are

differentially associated with the two major U.S. political parties. We argue that the Democratic

party currently tends to be more associated with intelligence than with competence, and the

Republican party is more associated with competence than with intelligence (Brooks 2008).

Certainly at the voter level there is a broad spectrum of Democrat, from union members

to new immigrants, but at the brand level the party in recent years has been linked in the media to

the intellectual elite, as represented by individuals like Bill Clinton, John Kerry, and Barack

Page 8: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

8

Obama (Brooks 2008; Douthat 2009).1 The Republican party, while also wide-ranging in its

representation at the voter level, has moved toward an ideology that supports “regular folk,”

decisive strength, and traditional values, with leaders such as George W. Bush, Ronald Reagan,

and John McCain (Dionne 2007).2 Unlike the Democrats, who are said to prize “deliberation and

self-examination,” Republicans are said to “govern from the gut” (Brooks 2008) and to be more

effective at getting things accomplished (Meyerson 2009; Toner and Hulse 2007).

Although portrayals in the popular press suggest a distinction between the two party

images on relative competence and relative intelligence, to our knowledge no empirical evidence

exists for this distinction. Thus, to confirm that voters perceive a relative difference between

competence and intelligence for the two major parties, we conducted an online survey of 130

individuals in the U.S. (58 female, 72 male, aged 20 to 60, 88% under 40).3 We provided

participants with a 5-point, zero-centered semantic differential scale from -2 = “higher on

intelligence than competence” to 2 = “higher on competence than intelligence.” We asked

participants to think about the Republican party brand and the Democratic party brand, and then

to place each party along the scale. They also provided their own political affiliation.

A party x affiliation mixed ANOVA revealed no interaction (F < 1) but did reveal the

expected main effect of party. As suggested in the popular press, regardless of affiliation of the

respondent, the Democratic party was viewed as higher on intelligence than on competence

relative to the Republican party (MDems = -.34 vs. MReps = .15), F(1, 126) = 5.61, p < .05.4 Thus,

1 The IQ hoax of 2004 exemplifies the distinction on the intelligence trait. Although the available evidence (e.g. test scores) suggests that George W. Bush has a higher IQ than John Kerry (Tierney 2004), various sources from significant internet buzz to a Doonesbury cartoon commented on Bush’s apparently low intelligence. 2 For example, the Republican party has often been described as anti intellectual (Brooks 2008). The famous statement by William Buckley that he would rather be ruled by the first two thousand people in the phonebook than by the Harvard faculty highlights this tendency. 3 These data were collected in 2009 and as such represent the images of the parties at that time. 4 We also examined the extent to which the dimensions of trust and likeability were associated with the two parties. Because these are more affective traits, we suspected that judgment of these traits would be driven primarily by individuals’ party affiliation. One hundred seventeen individuals from the same pool rated the parties along the

Page 9: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

9

people do seem to differentially associate the traits of competence and intelligence with the two

parties. The primary goal of our research, however, is to understand how these party trait

associations interact with a candidate’s visually inferred personality traits.

Visual trait inferences and party trait associations. Inferences about others’ personality

traits can occur quickly and can be long-lasting (Wyer et al. 1984). Inferences based on facial

characteristics have been shown to influence ratings of job suitability (Hassin and Trope 2000),

to alter perceived CEO responsibility for a crisis (Gorn et al. 2008), to correlate with promotions

in the military (Mueller and Mazur 1996), and even to correlate with company profits (CEO

appearance) (Rule and Ambady 2008). Most relevant to our research, a study by Todorov et al.

(2005) asked participants to make judgments of congressional candidates on a variety of

personality traits based solely on photographs. The results indicated that visually inferred traits

related to the dimension of competence were predictive of election success.

The findings by Todorov et al. (2005) are striking; however, their work examined trait

inferences in aggregate and did not consider the possibility that trait advantages may be

moderated by party brand. We suggest that trait inferences based on the physical appearance of a

candidate’s face interact with party affiliation such that candidates will benefit if their

appearance communicates traits that are associated with their respective party. Prior research on

the match-up hypothesis has shown the value of a fit between certain physical characteristics of a

representative (such as gender or attractiveness) and the characteristics of a product (Kahle and

Homer 1985; Koernig and Page 2002; Lynch and Schuler 1994). In the domain of political

advertising, Ansolabehere and Iyengar (1994) showed that candidate advertising is more

successful when it focuses on issues already associated with the candidate’s party. Although

dimensions of trustworthiness and likeability. We found a significant interaction of party and affiliation: F(1, 115) = 35.50, p < .001. In each case members of a party viewed their party as relatively more trustworthy than likeable and viewed the other party as equivalent on trustworthiness and likeability.

Page 10: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

10

centered on issues rather than physical appearance, their research supports the hypothesis that a

match between the candidate and the party is more beneficial than a mismatch. Formally,

H1: An appearance reflecting traits associated with a party will lead to higher preference and vote share than an appearance that reflects traits associated with the other party. Previously we provided some evidence that the traits of competence and intelligence,

although correlated, are distinct. We also demonstrated that, as suggested by the popular press,

competence tends to be relatively more associated with Republicans and intelligence more

associated with Democrats. Based on the construct differences in competence versus intelligence

and their differential associations with the two parties, we predict that, consistent with the match-

up hypothesis, an appearance suggesting greater competence than intelligence will be predictive

of success for Republican candidates, while an appearance suggesting greater intelligence than

competence will be beneficial for Democrats. More formally,

H2: A visual advantage in competence will lead to an increase in preference and vote share for Republican candidates relative to Democratic candidates.

H3: A visual advantage in intelligence will lead to an increase in preference and vote share for Democratic candidates relative to Republican candidates.

It should be emphasized that we are not theorizing that appearing intelligent or competent

will be problematic for any candidate. As noted previously, the broad ability dimension is

positively valenced, and all candidates would do well to signal, visually or otherwise, that they

have a high level of ability. In line with Todorov et al. (2005), we expect that perceptions of

some overarching measure of ability will be beneficial for all candidates. However, we suggest a

more subtle additional effect, whereby Democrats benefit from possessing an appearance

suggesting that their ability is more driven by intellect, while Republicans benefit from

portraying ability that is associated with practical skills and competence.

Page 11: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

11

Visual Stereotypes

We have proposed that for political candidates, an advantageous match will occur when

the personality trait inferences that voters make about candidates (based on their appearance)

match the trait associations that voters have about the party as a whole. However, an alternative

perspective holds that candidates would do well to match a visual stereotype for their party rather

than matching party-wide trait associations. In other words, it is plausible that there exists a

visual stereotype for the Republican party (i.e., a typical look or set of appearance characteristics

for Republican politicians) and a different visual stereotype for the Democratic party, and a

“match” would occur when a candidate looks typical of his/her party. If so, then all else being

equal, candidates who resemble the stereotypical member of their party would do better.

The stereotype explanation for candidate appearance is consistent with research on

groups demonstrating that people judge a person’s fit with a group by assessing the extent to

which the individual matches the group norm (Turner et al. 1994). Political candidates may be

viewed as a human extension of the political brand (Thomson 2006), and therefore judged on

their fit with the party stereotype. Formally, this alternative hypothesis is,

H4: An appearance that is consistent with a visual stereotype for candidates of the party will lead to higher preference and vote share than an appearance that is not. The distinction between visual stereotypes and visually inferred trait associations is a

subtle point that merits investigation. It is conceivable that visual party stereotypes are actually a

function of visually inferred traits associated with the party. If so, Hypothesis 4 will effectively

be the same as Hypothesis 1. For example, if the appearance of intelligence is the major

component of the Democratic visual stereotype, then a stereotypical-looking candidate will have

an intelligent appearance. However, visual stereotypes of successful candidates likely include a

variety of other traits that may not be directly associated with the party itself, and perhaps other

Page 12: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

12

visual information not related to trait inferences at all. For instance, certain visual characteristics

not necessarily suggestive of personality traits (e.g. a full head of hair) make differences in

elections (Leigh and Susilo 2009). As long as other elements beyond the identified trait

associations play a role in visual stereotypes, we will be able to distinguish between Hypotheses

1 and 4 and to draw conclusions regarding the value of candidate appearance characteristics.

Advertising Spending Effects

A huge determinant of the success of a political campaign is the money spent on it.

Spending on political advertising in the U.S. grew from $1.6 billion in 1998 to $5.3 billion in

2008 (opensecrets.org). However, a consistent finding is that while challenger spending has a

positive effect on vote share, spending by incumbents usually has a statistically insignificant

effect (Ansolabehere and Gerber 1994; Jacobson 1990). The main explanation is that like an

early market entrant, incumbents start with advantages in awareness, which results in lower

marginal effects of advertising investments. Moreover, since the goal is simply to win, managing

costs may not be a primary objective. An incumbent may spend more than necessary to secure

the win, and hence the actual benefit per dollar spent would be lower. In contrast, challengers

often start from a point of relative anonymity, so marginal spending has a greater effect.

Beyond general campaign spending, spending on negative advertising has been a subject

of much inquiry (Sorescu and Gelb 2000). Negative ads have been found to be more effective

than positive ads at influencing likeability of candidates (Homer and Batra 1994), although this

has been shown to vary across different groups of voters (Klein and Ahluwalia 2005). Assessing

the direct impact of negative advertising on elections, however, has proven to be difficult. For

example, based on an analysis of Senate campaigns from 1992 to 2002, Lau and Pomper (2004)

find no clear evidence regarding the effectiveness of negative advertising.

Page 13: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

13

Despite the difficulty in measuring its impact, negative advertising has been employed

for decades and has flourished since the 1970s. The surge in negative advertising is often

attributed to the increased role of television in political campaigns (Lau et al. 1999). Television

enables transmission of negative messages combined with an image of an opponent’s face,

creating a link that may be hard to undo. Because of the prevalence of the visual elements, it is

plausible that the effectiveness of negative advertising may be altered by candidate appearance.

As noted, research has demonstrated that a spontaneous reaction to physical appearance can alter

how negative information is processed (Gorn et al. 2008). If a candidate possesses an appearance

that is consistent with party-associated traits, a positive voter reaction to this face trait–party trait

match may reduce the impact of negative advertising. More formally,

H5: Negative advertising will be less effective against a candidate whose appearance conveys personality traits that match traits associated with his/her party than against a candidate whose appearance does not match traits associated with his/her party. As noted earlier, an alternative view is that a fit with a visual stereotype, rather than with

particular party trait associations, may be a benefit to candidates. Thus we also present the

alternative hypothesis that considers the possibility that a match with a visual stereotype has the

same effect as a match with party trait associations.

H6: Negative advertising will be less effective against a candidate whose appearance resembles the visual stereotype of his/her party than against a candidate whose appearance does not resemble the visual stereotype of his/her party.

OVERVIEW OF STUDIES

In the following sections we present the results of a series of laboratory studies and

analyses of election data. We begin with the development of a database of candidate photographs

that was utilized in both our laboratory and field studies. We then present the results of several

experiments conducted to test our hypotheses regarding the relationships among candidate

Page 14: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

14

appearance, party branding, and negative advertising. Study 1 tests whether appearance-based

trait inferences have different effects on candidate evaluation when candidates are labeled

Democrat or Republican (H1, H2, and H3). A follow-up study addresses the alternative

hypothesis that matching a visual brand stereotype rather than particular party-level trait

associations will also result in more favorable candidate evaluations (H4). Study 2 examines how

negative advertising, appearance, and party labels interact to impact candidate evaluation (H5).

As in Study 1, we also explore the effects of a match with a visual stereotype (H6).

We then investigate the extent to which these effects occur in actual elections. Study 3

presents an analysis of voting data for a subset of congressional districts from 2000 to 2004. This

analysis utilizes data on advertising spending, advertising tone, and the visual trait inferences.

The results provide confirmatory evidence regarding the effects of candidate appearance and the

interactions among appearance, party membership, and negative advertising. Finally, in Study 4,

we utilize the election data to further examine the issue of a match with a visual party stereotype.

Personality trait database. To facilitate our empirical investigations, we created a

database of personality trait ratings for a set of congressional candidate pairs. Personality

judgments were based on candidate photographs for 153 congressional races from 2000, 2002,

and 2004. Undergraduate participants were presented with photographs of the two main

candidates for a given district, which were obtained from CNN.com. For each pair of

photographs, participants were asked to provide relative evaluations on a variety of personality

traits, including competence, intelligence, trustworthiness, and likeability. Participants were also

asked to predict which candidate was the Republican and which was the Democrat, and to

provide their political affiliation and some demographic information. Full details of the data

collection procedure are provided in web appendix A.

The top half of Table 1 lists descriptive statistics for the entire set of races and separates

Page 15: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

15

races with Republican and Democratic incumbents. The data are coded such that if the

participant chose the middle of the scale, which represents equality, the incumbent advantage

would be zero. Given the 7-point scale, the maximum advantage is 3 and the maximum deficit is

-3. On average, incumbents are rated as more competent and intelligent than challengers. This

tendency is more pronounced for the Republican incumbents compared to Democratic

incumbents. In fact, Democratic incumbents are rated slightly less competent and intelligent-

appearing than their Republican challengers, suggesting that there may be systematic differences

between the appearances of individuals who run as Republicans and Democrats. See Appendix A

for examples of candidate pairs.

The personality trait judgments were consistent across participants, even despite different

political leanings. An ANOVA comparing the average trait ratings as a function of political

affiliations (Republican, Democrat, Independent, None) revealed no main effect of affiliation or

trait rating, and no interaction (all F’s < 1). Following Olney, Holbrook, and Batra (1991), we

assessed interjudge reliability for each set of ratings using coefficient alpha. Alpha ranged from

.87 to .94 across the different datasets. This judgment consistency was expected, since the

participants did not know the candidates or their affiliation. It parallels research in face

perception that reports high levels of inter-rater agreement on ratings of attractiveness and facial

emotion across gender and culture (Ekman 2004; Langlois et al. 2000).

Our database includes 48 cross-gender and eight cross-ethnicity pairs. Previous research

on how gender influences perceptions of candidate traits is mixed. Some have found that gender

stereotypes play a significant role in how voters perceive candidate traits (Huddy and Terkildsen

1993), but others report that gender is relatively irrelevant in candidate evaluation (Thompson

and Steckenrider 1997). These demographically diverse races were included for ecological

validity and to maximize sample size. To test for potential gender or ethnicity effects, we

Page 16: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

16

analyzed the results for each trait (competence, intelligence, trustworthiness, and likeability) as a

function of these two factors but found no significant main effects and no interaction.

STUDY 1: APPEARANCE AND PARTY

The primary goal of Study 1 was to examine how Republican versus Democratic

candidates would be evaluated when they presented an appearance that suggested competence

versus one that suggested intelligence. We showed participants a photograph of a political

candidate and a labeled political affiliation in order to elicit an evaluation. To manipulate the

appearance of competence versus intelligence, we selected two congressional candidate

photographs from our dataset for which the candidates had scored relatively high on perceived

competence and two photographs for which the candidates had scored relatively high on

perceived intelligence. We chose faces of the same gender, race, and approximate age. We also

pretested the selected faces on other factors including trustworthiness, likeability, and

babyfacedness to ensure there were no significant differences (p’s > .16). We then manipulated

the candidates’ political affiliation. Thus the study conformed to a 2 (Appearance: Competent,

Intelligent) x 2 (Party: Republican, Democrat) x 2 (replicate) between-subjects design.

Participants and procedure. Three hundred twenty-seven participants (59% women; 46%

under 30) from a national online pool completed the study for a cash payment.5 Participants were

told they would be evaluating congressional candidates based on very little information. They

were shown a photo of a candidate along with a fictitious name (the same name was used in all

conditions) and the candidate’s party (not necessarily his or her actual party). For example,

participants in the Competent/Republican condition saw a photo of one of the two relatively

5 The age of participants ranged from under 20 to over 60. 52% had some form of post-secondary education; 50% had an income less than $50,000. Since the same online pool was utilized for all studies and the demographic statistics were similar in each case, we do not report full demographic information in subsequent studies.

Page 17: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

17

competent-looking individuals along with the words “Eric Hammond, Republican.” Participants

indicated how much they liked the candidate on a 9-point scale (1 = “dislike very much” to 9 =

“like very much”) and their likelihood of voting for the candidate (1 = “very unlikely” to 9 =

“very likely”). Subsequently, participants provided some demographic information and their

level of political conservatism from 1 = “very liberal” to 7 = “very conservative.”

Results. An evaluation index was created from the two dependent measures, α = .84. We

regressed the index on political party, appearance, the interaction of party and appearance,

political conservatism, gender, age, the interaction of conservatism and party, the interaction of

gender and appearance, and the interaction of age and appearance.6 These last three interaction

terms were included as control measures. The analysis yielded no effect of political party, (β = -

.46, t < 1), or appearance, (β = -1.03, t = -1.50, p = .14), but importantly revealed an interaction

of political party and appearance, (β = .82, t = 2.94, p < .01). Democratic candidates were rated

higher than Republicans when they appeared intelligent, (MIntel_Rep = 3.22 vs. MIntel_Dem = 3.78,

t(323) = -3.20, p < .01); and Republican candidates were rated marginally significantly higher

than Democrats when they appeared competent, (MComp_Rep = 4.18 vs. MComp_Dem = 3.71, t (323)

= 1.80, p = .07).Table 2 lists full results of the analysis.

Discussion. The interaction of political party label and candidate appearance provides

some initial support for Hypotheses 1, 2, and 3. The appearance of intelligence was more

beneficial for Democrats, and the appearance of competence was directionally more beneficial

for Republicans. Given the notion in the popular press as well as the findings from our pilot

study that intelligence is a trait more associated with Democrats and competence a trait more

associated with Republicans, these findings confirm Hypothesis 1 that an appearance reflecting

6 We also included the replicate factor in the model; however, it did not interact with any other variables (all p’s > .10) and thus for simplicity we collapsed the results across the replicate and removed it from the analysis.

Page 18: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

18

the image people associate with the party can be beneficial in terms of voter preference.

Although not a central focus of our investigation, for completeness we also ran a study

that used the traits of trustworthiness and likeability. We followed the same design and

procedure as the competence versus intelligence study. One hundred fifty-seven subjects from

the same pool completed the experiment. The results revealed only main effects for appearance

(β = -1.74, t = -2.13, p < .05) and party (β = -2.42, t = -5.08, p < .001), and an interaction of party

and liberalism (β = .47, t = 4.29, p < .001) (see Table 2). There was no interaction of appearance

and party (p > .40). The results suggest that appearing trustworthy may be more important than

appearing likeable, but this is true regardless of party. We further address the traits of

trustworthiness and likeability in Study 3.

Study 1 tested whether possessing visual characteristics that communicate a personality

trait consistent with party trait associations would be advantageous. However, as noted

previously, an alternative possibility is that party and candidate consistency might also be

defined in terms of more general stereotypes. Hypothesis 4 proposes that rather than appearing

consistent with a brand trait association, it may be advantageous to have an appearance that

matches the stereotypical member of one’s party. To test the possibility of the visual advantage

stemming from stereotypes, we replicated Study 1 using photographs of candidates that the vast

majority of participants in our dataset agreed were Democrats or Republicans. When the

experiment was repeated using “stereotypical” candidates, the interaction between fit and party

label became insignificant (t < 1, p = .55)—see Table 2. Thus in contrast to H4, there does not

seem to be a positive effect for an appearance that is consistent with a party stereotype. Rather, it

seems that an appearance communicating party-associated traits is most effective.

STUDY 2: NEGATIVE ADVERTISING, APPEARANCE, AND PARTY MEMBERSHIP

Page 19: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

19

In Study 1 we demonstrated the positive effect of a match between visually inferred

personality traits and a party-associated trait. Our second study investigated the impact of these

visually inferred traits on a candidate’s ability to resist negative advertising attacks. If a match

between visually inferred traits and party brand associations leads to positive outcomes for a

candidate, this should subsequently mitigate the impact of a negative advertising campaign (H5).

Participants, design, and procedure. Two hundred ninety-one individuals from an online

pool completed the study for a cash payment. The experiment used a 2 (ad target party

affiliation: Republican vs. Democrat) x 2 (ad target appearance: Competent vs. Intelligent) x 2

(replicate) between-subjects design. The candidate photographs utilized in Study 1 were again

employed. All participants were presented with the same negative ad text (see Appendix B), with

the only change being whether the target of the ad was stated to be a Republican or a Democrat.

Inserted throughout the advertisement were pictures of the candidate who was ostensibly the

target. At the end of the advertisement was a picture of both the target of the ad and the candidate

sponsoring the ad, as well as a recommendation to vote for the ad’s sponsor.

Participants read the ad copy and indicated their relative preference between the two

candidates, the relative appeal of the candidates, and their likelihood of voting for either

candidate, each on 9-point scales where lower scores indicate support for the sponsor of the ad

and higher scores indicate support for the target of the ad. They also provided some demographic

information and their level of political conservatism.

Results. A relative evaluation index was created from the dependent measures, α = .86.

We regressed the index on the dummy variables of target appearance, target party affiliation, and

the interaction of the two variables.7 We also included political conservatism and the interaction

of conservatism and target party in the model to control for participants’ political leanings. The

7 Again the replicate factor did not interact with any of the variables. All p’s > .16; thus, we collapsed across it.

Page 20: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

20

analysis revealed main effects of target party (β = -2.42, t = -3.11, p < .01) and target appearance

(β = -2.43, t = 2.84, p < .01) and a significant interaction of party and appearance (β = 1.66, t =

3.46, p < .01). Providing support for Hypothesis 5, a competent-looking candidate was more

resistant to negative attacks when labeled a Republican rather than a Democrat, (MComp_Rep =

5.10 vs. MComp_Dem = 4.36, t = 2.15, p < .05). Similarly, an intelligent-looking candidate was

more resilient when labeled a Democrat rather than a Republican, (MIntel_Rep = 3.64 vs. MIntel_Dem

= 4.50, t = 2.71, p < .01. Table 3 presents the full results.

Discussion. Study 2 shows that appearance interacts with party label to mitigate the

influence of negative advertising. The significant interaction of ad target party affiliation and ad

target appearance suggests that negative advertising may be less effective against a candidate

whose appearance communicates a personality trait associated with the party brand. However,

we note that the effect for Republicans was far more pronounced than for Democrats. In Study 4

we present analyses of election data that may help to shed light on reasons for this difference.

To test Hypothesis 6, we repeated this investigation using candidates who looked

stereotypical of the political party. Three hundred fourteen individuals from the same online pool

completed the study. Consistent with Study 1, we again found no support for H6, because fit

with the visual brand stereotype did not yield a significant interaction with party label (t < 1).

STUDY 3: PERSONALITY INFERENCES AND ELECTION OUTCOMES

The preceding studies suggested several insights that were used to design specifications

for empirically studying actual U.S. congressional elections. To examine how personality

inferences based on candidate appearance might influence election outcomes, we utilized several

sources of data including the personality trait judgments, selected results from the 2000, 2002,

and 2004 congressional elections, and data on television advertising spending. (Definitions for

Page 21: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

21

all variables and the correlations among the variables are provided in web appendices B and C.)

Appearance traits. We first examined the relationship between candidate appearance and

vote share. Table 4 lists results for different specifications that predict incumbent vote share

based on all four inferred traits (competence, intelligence, trustworthiness, and likeability). These

specifications varied in terms of how the inferred personality traits entered the model. The first

model, labeled “Inferred Traits,” simply used the incumbent’s relative rating on the personality

traits to predict vote share. This model yielded an R-square of .041 and no significant estimates.

Since the correlations among the trait variables clearly suggested an overlap, we next performed

a factor analysis to investigate whether the data could be represented by a smaller number of

latent dimensions. This analysis suggested a two-factor solution with competence, intelligence,

and trustworthiness loading on the first factor, and likeability and trustworthiness loading on the

second factor. This first factor corresponds to the summary dimension of competence (Todorov

et al. 2005). This model, labeled “Factors,” yielded a positive significant effect for the

competence factor and a non-significant estimate for the second factor. The R-square for this

model is .040. This result is consistent with the results reported by Todorov et al. (2005).

The third model, labeled “Party-Specific Inferred Personality Traits,” estimates separate

parameter estimates for each personality trait based on whether the race involves a Democratic or

Republican incumbent. This model yielded an R-square of .13 and a striking pattern of results.

Democrats benefit by appearing relatively more intelligent and likeable, whereas Republicans

benefit from having an advantage in competence and trustworthiness. Despite correlations in the

traits, the variance inflation factors for this model are in the acceptable range. The maximum VIF

is 8.4 for the Republican competency measure, and the greatest condition index is 7.24.

The model labeled “Party-Specific Factors” provides estimates for the factor scores for

each party. This model yields limited insights, as only the competence–ability factor is

Page 22: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

22

marginally significant for Republican incumbents and the R-square is only .04.

Although the collinearity diagnostics suggest that the party-specific inferred personality

traits model is acceptable, the diagnostics are evaluated via rules of thumb rather than hard

criteria. The pattern of results whereby Republicans benefit from appearing competent and

trustworthy, and Democrats benefit from appearing intelligent and likeable, combined with the

results from the party-specific factor model, suggest that the traits merit further consideration.

Based on possible differences in the constructs and the divergent pattern of results

observed across the traits in our laboratory experiments, we created four new variables. The first

two are the differences between the competence and intelligence ratings separately for the

Democratic incumbent and for the Republican incumbent, and the other two represent the

differences between trustworthiness and likeability for incumbents of each party. This model is

labeled “Difference Scores” and is represented by equation 1 below:

(1)

LikeTrustRILikeTrustDI

IntelCompRIIntelCompDIVoteI

LikeTrustRILikeTrustDI

IntelCompRIIntelCompDIVoteI

__

__%_

__

__%_

where I_Vote% is incumbent vote share, DI_Comp-Intel is Democratic incumbent difference between competence and intelligence, RI_Comp-Intel is Republican incumbent difference between competence and intelligence, DI_Trust-Like is Democratic incumbent difference between trust and likeability, and RI_Trust-Like is Republican incumbent difference between trust and likeability. This specification provides a structure that allows for further testing of Hypotheses 2 and 3. This

model yields an R-square of .08. The difference-score model suggests that Republicans benefit

from appearances that are more competent than intelligent and more trustworthy than likeable.

Democrats benefit from the reverse. The VIFs for this model are all less than 1.6. Importantly,

the correlations between the traits and the difference scores are relatively low.

Because the party-specific effects of appearance seem to be symmetric, we collapsed the

four difference-score variables into two variables that encompass the trait advantages. We define

Page 23: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

23

an “advantage” variable related to the competence–intelligence difference trait as AdvCI =

IncComp – IncIntel if the incumbent is a Republican, and AdvCI = IncIntel – IncComp if the

incumbent candidate is a Democrat. Similarly, we defined AdvTL as the difference between

IncTrust and IncLike for Republican incumbents and the reverse for Democratic incumbents.

We use the advantage variables and the factor scores described above to estimate equation 2.

(2) 21%_ _2_1%_ FactorFactorAdvTLAdvCIVoteI WarmthFCompFAdvTLAdvCIVoteI

The results for the model with the factors and the advantage variables are illuminating. The

variables explain approximately the same amount of variance as the party-level inferred traits

model while using only half the number of covariates.

This model yields several crucial insights. First, the positive effects associated with the

competence-oriented factor and the insignificant effects for the trust–likeability factor are

consistent with previous literature (Todorov et al. 2005). However, the results also highlight the

fact that differences between competence and intelligence, and between trust and likeability, both

have larger impacts on voter decision making than the competence-oriented factor. The variance

inflation factors for this model are all below 1.4.

Advertising. We next expanded the analysis to add measures of advertising spending and

tone, and we considered voter heterogeneity across districts. Television advertising makes up the

majority of total spending in campaigns and serves as the way most voters come in visual contact

with candidates; thus, we utilized the dollars spent on television advertising. Campaign

fundraising and spending statistics were sourced from the FEC. Table 1 highlights the magnitude

and dispersion of spending. Republican incumbents spent on average $858,000 compared to

$528,000 for Democratic challengers, while Democratic incumbents spent $820,000 versus

$357,000 for Republican challengers.

Summary data are also provided for the ratios of incumbent spending relative to total

Page 24: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

24

spending in a district. The variable Ratio is computed in terms of incumbent spending and

challenger spending: SpendChSpendI

SpendIRatio

__

_

where I_Spend is incumbent spending and

Ch_Spend is challenger spending. Incumbents spent significantly more than challengers, with the

Ratio variable having an average value of .71. The Ratio measure ranged from .21 to .99.

In addition to overall spending, we also considered the proportion that was spent on

negative advertising. The Wisconsin Advertising Project (Goldstein, Franz, and Ridout 2002;

Goldstein and Rivlin 2005)8 has published data on television advertising for congressional races

in the 2000, 2002, and 2004 elections. The database contains information on races in the top 75

media markets in 2000 and the top 100 markets in 2002 and 2004. It also includes qualitative

judgments as to whether each advertisement had a negative or positive tone, as well as estimated

costs for each airing. We used the estimated costs and tone judgments to calculate the percentage

of negative advertising used by each candidate.

Overall, incumbents were found to spend 16% of advertising dollars on negative ads

versus 17% for challengers, although the use of negative advertising varied from 0% to 100%.

We calculated a ratio (NegRatio) of negative to total advertising spending as the difference

between the incumbents’ and challengers’ spending on negative advertising divided by total

spending. The negative advertising ratio has a mean of -.002 and ranges from .98 to -.55.

Finally, we employed the Cook Partisan Voting Index, which is a measure of the partisan

leaning of each congressional district compared to the nation as a whole. It is derived by

comparing the average of the results of the two prior presidential elections in a given district with

8 The data were obtained from a project of the Wisconsin Advertising Project, under Professor Kenneth Goldstein and Joel Rivlin of the University of Wisconsin-Madison, and includes media tracking data from the Campaign Media Analysis Group in Washington, D.C. The Wisconsin Advertising Project was sponsored by a grant from The Pew Charitable Trusts. The opinions expressed in this article are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Wisconsin Advertising Project, Professor Goldstein, Joel Rivlin, or The Pew Charitable Trusts.

Page 25: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

25

the national result. It indicates which party was ahead in the district and the magnitude of the

advantage relative to the national average (cookpolitical.com 2009).

Results. Table 5 provides estimation results for several models that include spending. The

model labeled “Spending” includes the ratio of incumbent to total spending and the ratio of

negative spending. The R-square of this model is .28, and only the overall spending ratio is

significant. The second model, “Spending and Appearance,” adds the difference-score variables

for each of the two parties (i.e., competence–intelligence and trust–likeability), and includes

interactions between appearance and the negative advertising spending measure (NegRatio). This

model allows us to examine whether the physical appearance of Democrats versus Republicans

alters the effectiveness of negative advertising. In addition, the model includes the Cook Partisan

Voting Index to account for district heterogeneity. This model is detailed in equation 3.

(3)

AdvTLNegRatioAdvCINegRatioYRSI

DICookRICookFactorFactorAdvTL

AdvCINegRatioRatioRatioVoteI

dvTLNegRatioXACINegRatXAdvYRSI

DICookRICookWarmthFCompFAdvTL

AdvCINegRatioRatioSQRatioVoteI

_

**21

%_

_

___2_1

2%_

This model yields an R-square of .56. The analysis reveals unsurprising positive effects

for higher spending and a negative effect for negative advertising. We also see significant effects

of the Cook Partisan Voting Index that are similar for both political parties. The index is centered

at zero, so the different signs across parties indicate the advantage for each party. The significant

result for both parties highlights the importance of district heterogeneity.

For the visual effects, the main effects of the difference score variables are consistent

with the trait models. The interaction between negative advertising and the trait-advantage

variables is useful for addressing Hypothesis 5. We observe a significant negative interaction

between the competence–intelligence difference score and the negative ad spending ratio for

Democratic incumbents, and a marginally significant (p = .07) positive interaction between the

Page 26: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

26

competence–intelligence difference score and the negative ad spending ratio for Republican

incumbents. This suggests that Democratic incumbents will benefit from using negative ads if

they appear more intelligent than competent relative to their opponents, whereas Republican

incumbents will benefit from negative advertising if they appear more competent than intelligent.

Discussion. The analysis of election data provides evidence that candidate appearance has

a significant effect on outcomes. Consistent with our experiments and in support of Hypotheses

1, 2, and 3, visually inferred personality traits can be advantageous to a candidate if they are

consistent with traits associated with the party as a whole. Specifically, Republicans benefit from

appearing more competent than intelligent, and Democrats benefit from appearing more

intelligent than competent. In evaluating the effects of trustworthiness and likeability, we found

that these variables have divergent main effects across the two parties, although they do not

interact with negative advertising.

STUDY 4: CANDIDATE FIT WITH VISUAL PARTY STEREOTYPE

Thus far we have shown that when personality traits inferred from a candidate’s

appearance match personality traits associated with the candidate’s party, it can promote success

in electoral outcomes and can mitigate the impact of a negative ad directed at the candidate. Our

laboratory results have also indicated that a match between a candidate’s appearance and a visual

party stereotype is not sufficient to produce these outcomes; however, our election data remain

mute with respect to the alternative hypothesis of a visual stereotype. In our final study we revisit

the issue of visual party stereotypes to confirm our lack of support for this alternative hypothesis

(H4). We utilize the same datasets described in Study 3. Our dependent measure is the party

prediction for each candidate pair in our trait inference database. As noted, as part of the

development of the personality-trait face database, we asked participants to make a binary

Page 27: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

27

prediction of the party affiliation of the candidates based on their photographs. We interpreted

these judgments to be an indication of how much a candidate looked stereotypical of his or her

party. A case with nearly unanimous agreement about which party a candidate was from

suggested strong adherence to a visual party stereotype. We used respondents’ binary judgments

to create a stereotype variable for each candidate pair, ranging from 0 to 1 (a score near 0 or 1

suggests rater agreement on candidate party affiliation). This measure was then used to analyze

the relationship between stereotypicality and personality-trait judgments.

Analysis. We regressed the stereotype variable on the four trait inferences (competence,

intelligence, trustworthiness, and likeability) to determine whether the traits would predict

political party stereotypicality. The analysis was conducted separately for Republican and

Democratic incumbents. As shown in Table 6, for Republicans stereotypicality was positively

related to competence and negatively related to likeability, suggesting that the appearance of

competence is part of the Republican stereotype. For Democratic incumbents, however, none of

the trait measures were significant predictors. We also tested the relationship between party

stereotypicality and electoral success using a regression that predicts incumbent vote share as a

function of the incumbent’s visual stereotypicality. Table 7 shows that the analysis revealed no

significant effects and an R-square of only .01.

Discussion. Study 4 provides further evidence that it is not consistency with a visual

brand stereotype that drives the appearance effects. Rather the effects seem to be driven by a

match between visually inferred personality traits and trait associations with the party brand.

Nonetheless, we do find some evidence that, at least for Republicans, certain appearance-based

traits are associated with a visual stereotype. We found that for Republican candidates, the extent

to which a candidate appeared stereotypical was related to having the same physical

characteristics that were advantageous to the candidate in the election results.

Page 28: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

28

The result for Republicans appears inconsistent with our laboratory studies, but we think

that it may actually help to clarify the laboratory findings. In the experiments we selected faces

that conformed to our experimental criteria, and we deliberately attempted to disentangle the trait

of competence from other traits. Stereotypes are not unidimensional, however, and while we

have evidence that competence is a component of the Republican stereotype, it is likely not the

only trait that makes up a typical Republican’s appearance. Overall our results suggest that the

appearance of competence is a critical trait for Republicans, since it is both a trait that voters

associate with the party and a component of the visual stereotype for candidates.

Interestingly, the personality-trait judgments did not predict Democratic stereotypicality.

This may be because the Republican brand image is more narrowly defined than the Democratic

brand, and this is represented visually as well. To test this possibility we asked a sample of 43

men and 32 women, 42% Democrat, aged between 18 and 60, which party had a narrower

position. Seventy-two percent reported that the Republican party had the narrower position.

In addition to the preceding survey data, further analysis of the stereotypicality variable

supports the possibility that voters have more distinct visual expectations of Republicans. Our

participants correctly predicted the parties of Republican incumbents at a significantly higher

rate than Democrats (74.7%, χ2 = 21.25, p <.001). In contrast, for the subset of Democrats,

judgments about party membership were not significantly better than random chance (44.8%).

While interpretation of these results is complicated by the fact that evaluations are made for pairs

of Republicans and Democrats (i.e., one incumbent and one challenger), the finding that survey

participants are more accurate when evaluating races with Republican incumbents is telling. The

key observation is that participant evaluations are more accurate when assessing races with

successful Republican candidates than races with successful Democratic candidates.

Page 29: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

29

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Previous literature on candidate appearance suggests that an appearance conveying traits

related to competence is beneficial (Todorov et al. 2005). Through experimental studies and

analysis of election results, we have shown that effects of appearance may be more nuanced than

previously reported. We find that effects of physical appearance at the candidate level are

dependent on trait associations at the party level. Candidates are at an advantage when there is a

match between personality traits inferred from their physical appearance and the traits associated

with their party. Democrats gain by appearing more intelligent than their opponents, whereas

Republicans benefit from appearing more competent. This advantage occurs not only in terms of

voter preference but also in terms of susceptibility to attacks from negative advertising.

In political campaigns candidate and party images are actively managed. Our findings

suggest that the two parties have become differentiated within the positive space associated with

broad trait dimensions. Therefore, while a factor analysis loads competence and intelligence on a

single dimension, the non-overlapping elements of the two constructs provide an opportunity for

candidates to be distinguished. This is an important finding for political image consultants

because it suggests that differentiation of candidates can occur within a small perceptual space.

Our studies demonstrate that a match between appearance-based traits and party-level

traits can benefit a candidate. The converse of this finding is that some candidates may struggle

due to their physical appearance. While a full analysis of the negative effects of visual inferences

is beyond the scope of our research, we address one way that such effects might be mitigated.

The influence of appearance on personality judgments tends to be spontaneous and not a

result of deliberation (Hassin and Trope 2000); thus, if voters are prompted to think consciously

about the extent to which candidates’ appearances convey particular traits, such appearance

effects should dissipate. To test this possibility, we ran an additional study in which we

Page 30: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

30

manipulated whether or not participants thought consciously about a candidate’s appearance. We

presented 294 participants with a picture and a short written profile about a candidate. The

picture and profile were matched or mismatched on traits. For example, participants viewed a

picture-profile set in which the candidate looked competent and the profile suggested he was

competent, or they viewed a picture-profile set in which the candidate looked competent but the

profile suggested he was incompetent. Prior to reading the profile, half the participants were

shown the picture only and were told to think about the extent to which the candidate looked

competent. The other participants received no instructions. Both groups then saw the picture with

the profile and were asked to indicate how likely they would be to vote for the candidate.

A 2 (picture profile: match vs. mismatch) x 2 (trait attention: yes vs. no) ANOVA

revealed a significant interaction: F(1, 292) = 4.17, p < .05. When prior attention was not drawn

to the trait, we saw a replication of our previous studies. Subjects judged the candidate more

favorably if the picture and profile traits were matched (MMatch = 4.25) than mismatched

(MMismatch = 3.76), t(292) = 1.98, p < .05. However, when participants were alerted to think

about the trait ahead of time, the appearance effect was mitigated: MMatch = 4.02 vs. MMismatch =

4.32), t(292) = 1.01, p = .31. The results imply that potential negative effects of appearance-

based judgments may be limited by calling explicit attention to the candidate’s appearance

deficiencies, presumably by inducing pause and avoiding snap judgments on the voter’s part.

In terms of limitations to our study, the most obvious point relates to the sample size and

time period. The advertising tone data from the Wisconsin Advertising Project are available only

for select media markets and cycles, and within that set we removed non-contested races. One

consequence is that we were unable to delve deeply into the role of voter or district

heterogeneity. The political science literature has devoted significant attention to voter

partisanship and party identification (Campbell et al. 1960). A common view is that voters rely

Page 31: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

31

on partisanship to guide their evaluations of politicians and policies (Goren 2005; Alvarez and

Brehm 2002; Feldman 1988; Baumer and Gold 2007). That is, political parties trigger emotional

connections or serve as cues that can influence heuristic decision making.

This notion of party identification is in many respects similar to notions of brand loyalty

in the marketing literature. However, rather than focus on how the parties (brands) create loyalty,

which would be the standard in marketing, the interest in political science is on how the strength

of segment membership changes over time. In other words, rather than focus on how parties craft

brand images, the literature focuses on the resulting segmentation structure. The implication is

that party loyalty may influence response to individual candidates. A less obvious but potentially

important issue is that partisanship may influence reaction to candidate appearance or marketing

tactics. In the context of our research an open question is how voter appearance, party image,

and voter ideology interact. For example, in the case of Republican candidates trying to appeal to

Democratic-leaning voters, it is unclear whether they would benefit from possessing visual traits

that match Republican or Democratic party-trait associations. The issue of voter heterogeneity

merits additional research.9

Our election and laboratory data were all collected during 2000–2009. Since political

parties and their personality-trait associations evolve over time as a function of the political

climate and party leaders, our results may not generalize to other decades.10 Although the effects

of these particular trait inferences may change as brand associations evolve over time, we

suspect that the more general notion will continue to hold that candidate appearance and brand

9 To illustrate the importance of district heterogeneity, we conducted further analysis of the results of Study 4. The analysis of incumbent vote share in Study 4 yielded insignificant effects for members of both parties as a function of incumbent stereotypicality. Our follow-up analysis used interactions between incumbent party stereotypicality and district partisanship. This analysis yields the equation I_Vote% = 59.63 + 0.10DIStereo X Cook – 1.26RIStereo X Cook. While the coefficient for the interaction between the fit of the Democratic incumbents and district partisanship is non-significant, the coefficient for the Republican term yields a t stat of -2.74 (see Table 7). This result suggests that Republican-leaning voters are more responsive to visual markers. 10 It should be noted that in Brewer’s (2009) work the parties have maintained similar images for the last 50 years.

Page 32: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

32

associations will interact to impact elections. Research in future years is needed to examine the

evolution of the party association and its impact on election outcomes.

The results of the negative advertising study have important managerial implications. As

noted, it is widely accepted that candidate appearance influences election results, and prior work

has considered many elements such as height, hair, and attractiveness (Leigh and Susilo 2009).

Our results suggest that appearance should be evaluated in concert with party affiliation, in terms

of candidate selection and even in terms of managing candidate appearance. These types of

practices have long been noted (Shapiro 2008), but our findings suggest that image management

at the candidate level should be done in conjunction with image at the party level.

The results of our research are consistent with a growing body of work demonstrating

dramatic effects of facial characteristics beyond physical attractiveness, and how relatively

automatic reactions to appearance can have significant consequences (Gorn et al. 2008; Mazur

and Mueller 1996). Although we focus on the domain of political marketing, we speculate that

such effects may occur in other marketing contexts in which competitors have distinct brand

positions. Specifically, personality traits inferred based on the appearance of a spokesperson or

sales representative could be altered depending on the personality traits associated with the

brand. Perhaps more importantly, visually inferred personality traits might alter the personality

traits normally associated with the brand. Given the role of a brand’s personality in developing

customer loyalty, the potential interaction between visually inferred personality traits and brand-

level personality traits is an important topic for future research.

Page 33: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

33

REFERENCES

Aaker, Jennifer (1997), “Dimensions of Brand Personality,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34

(August), 347–56.

Alvarez, Michael, and John Brehem (2002), Hard Choices, Easy Answers: Values, Information

and American Public Opinion Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Ansolabehere, Stephen and Alan Gerber (1994), “The Mismeasure of Campaign Spending:

Evidence from the 1990 U.S. House Elections,” Journal of Politics, 56(4), 1106–18.

Ansolabehere, Stephen and Shanto Iyengar (1994), “Riding the Wave and Claiming Ownership

Over Issues: The Joint Effects of Advertising and News Coverage in Campaigns,” The

Public Opinion Quarterly, 58 (Autumn), 335–57.

Asch, S. (1946), “Forming Impressions of Personality,” Journal of Abnormal and Social

Psychology, 41, 258–90.

Baumer, Donald and Howard Gold (2007), “Party Images and Partisan Resurgence,” The Social

Science Journal, 44, 465–79.

Brewer, Mark (2009), Party Image in the American Electorate, New York: Routledge.

Brooks, David (2008), “The Class War Before Palin,” The New York Times, Oct. 9.

www.nytimes.com/2008/10/10/opinion/10brooks.html

Campbell, Angus, Philip Converse, Warren Miller, and Donald Stokes (1960), The American

Voter. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Dionne, E. J. (2007), “Democrats Hone the Brand,” Washington Post, November 9,

washingtonpost.com.

Downs, Anthony (1957), An Economic Theory of Democracy, New York: Harper Collins.

Douthat, Ross (2009), “Can a Wonk Win?” The New York Times, November 24, nytimes.com.

Page 34: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

34

Ekman, Paul (2004), “Happy, Sad, Angry, Disgusted,” New Scientist, 184(2467), 4–5.

Erikson, Robert and Thomas Palfrey (1998), “Campaign Spending and Incumbency: An

Alternative Simultaneous Equations Approach,” The Journal of Politics, 60(2), 355–73.

Faber, Ronald A., Albert R. Tims, and Kay G. Schmitt (1993), “Negative Political Advertising

and Voting Intent: The Role of Involvement and Alternative Information Sources,”

Journal of Advertising, 22(4), 67–76.

Feldman, Stanley (1988), “Structure and Consistency in Public Opinion: The Role of Core

Beliefs and Values,” American Journal of Political Science, 32(2), 416–40.

Findlay, Christie (2008), “Can this Brand Be Saved?” Politics, September, 34–39.

Fiske, S., A. Cuddy, P. Glick, and J. Xu (2002), “A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Context:

Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow from Perceived Status and Competition,”

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878–02.

Funk, Carolyn L. (1996), “Understanding Trait Inferences in Candidate Images,” Research in

Micropolitics, 5, Eds. Michael X. Delli Carpini, Leonie Huddy, and Robert Y. Shapiro.

Greenwich: JAI Press.

Funk, Carolyn L. (1999), “Bringing the Candidate into Models of Candidate Evaluation,” The

Journal of Politics, 61(3), 700–20.

Gelb, Betsy D. and Alina B. Sorescu (2000), “Republican Brands, Democrat Brands,” Journal of

Advertising Research, 40(1/2), 95–102.

Goldstein, Kenneth, Michael Franz, and Travis Ridout (2002), “Political Advertising in 2000.”

Combined File [dataset]. Final release. Madison, WI: The Department of Political

Science at The University of Wisconsin-Madison and The Brennan Center for Justice at

New York University.

Page 35: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

35

Goldstein, Kenneth, and Joel Rivlin (2005), “Political Advertising in 2002.” Combined File

[dataset]. Final release. Madison, WI: The Wisconsin Advertising Project, The

Department of Political Science at The University of Wisconsin-Madison.

Goren, Paul (2005), “Party Identification and Core Political Values,” American Journal of

Political Science, 49(4), 881–96.

Gorn, Gerald J., Yuwei Jiang, and Gita Johar (2008), “Babyfaces, Trait Inferences, and Company

Evaluations in a PR Crisis,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35 (June), 36–49.

Hassin, Ran and Yaacov Trope (2000), “Facing Faces: Studies on the Cognitive Aspects of

Physiognomy,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 837–52.

Hofstadter, Richard (1966), Anti-intellectualism in American Life, New York: Knopf,

Homer, P. and Rajeev Batra (1994), “Attitudinal Effects of Character-Based versus Competence-

Based Negative Political Communications.” Journal of Consumer Psychology 3(2), 163–

85.

Huddy, Leonie and Nayda Terkildsen (1993), “Gender Stereotypes and the Perception of Male

and Female Candidates,” American Journal of Political Science, 37(1), 119–47.

Jacobson, Gary (1990), “The Effects of Campaign Spending in House Elections: New Evidence

for Old Arguments,” American Journal of Political Science, 34(2), 334–62.

Judd, Charles, Laurie James-Hawkins, Vincent Yzerbyt and Yoshihisa Kashima (2005),

“Fundamental Dimensions of Social Judgment: Understanding the Relations between

Judgment of Competence and Warmth,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

89 (December), 899–913.

Kahle, L. R. and Pamela Homer (1985), “Physical Attractiveness of the Celebrity Endorser: A

Social Adaptation Perspective,” Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (March), 954–61.

Klein, Jill and Rohini Ahluwalia (2005), “Negativity in the Evaluation of Political Candidates,”

Page 36: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

36

Journal of Marketing, 69(1), 131–42.

Koernig, Stephen K. and Albert L. Page (2001), “What if Your Dentist Looked Like Tom

Cruise? Applying the Match-Up Hypothesis to a Service Encounter,” Psychology and

Marketing, 19(1), 91–110.

Langlois, Judith H., Lisa E. Kalakanis, Adam J. Rubenstein, Andrea D. Larson, Monica J.

Hallam, and Monica T. Smoot (2000). “Maxims or Myths of Beauty: A Meta-Analytic

and Theoretical Review,” Psychological Bulletin, 126 (May), 390–423.

Lau, Richard and Gerald Pomper (2004), Negative Campaigning: An Analysis of U.S. Senate

Elections, Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Lau, Richard R., Lee Sigelman, Caroline Heldman, and Paul Babitt (1999), “The Effects of

Negative Political Advertisements: A Meta-Analytic Assessment,” American Political

Science Review, 93(4), 851–75.

Leigh, Andrew and T. Susilo (2009), “Is Voting Skin-Deep? Estimating the Effect of Candidate

Ballot Photographs on Election Outcomes,” Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(1), 61–

70.

Levitt, Steven (1994), “Using Repeat Challengers to Estimate the Effect of Campaign Spending

on Election Outcomes in the U.S. House,” Journal of Political Economy, 102 (4), 777–

98.

Lynch, James and Drue Schuler (1994), “The Matchup Effect of Spokesperson and Product

Category: A Schema Theory Interpretation,” Psychology and Marketing, 11(5), 417–45.

Mark, David (2006), Going Dirty: The Art of Negative Campaigning, Lanham, MD,: Rowman &

Littlefield.

Markus, Gregory (1982), “Political Attitudes During an Election Year: A Report on the 1980

NES Panel Study,” American Political Science Review, 76, 538–60.

Page 37: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

37

Meyerson, Harold (2009), “The Can’t-Do Blue Dogs,” Washington Post, July 22,

washingtonpost.com.

Mueller, Ulrich and Allan Mazur (1996), “Facial Dominance of West Point Cadets as a Predictor

of Later Military Rank,” Social Forces, 74(3), 823–50.

Newman, Bruce and Jagdish Sheth (1985), “A Model of Primary Voter Behavior,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 12(2), 178–87.

Olney, Thomas, Morris Holbrook, and Rajeev Batra (1991), “Consumer Responses to

Advertising: The Effects of Ad Content, Emotions, and Attitude Toward the Ad on

Viewing Time,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (4), 440–53.

Petrocik, John (1996), “Issue Ownership in Presidential Elections with a 1980 Case Study,”

American Journal of Political Science, 40(3), 825–50.

Pinkleton, Bruce (1997), “The Effects of Negative Comparative Political Advertising on

Candidate Evaluations and Advertising Evaluations,” Journal of Advertising, 26(1), 19–

29.

Rosenberg, Shawn W. and Patrick McCafferty (1987), “The Image and the Vote: Manipulating

Voters’ Preferences,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 31–47.

Rosenberg, Seymour, Carnot Nelson, and P.S. Vivekananthan (1968), “A Multidimensional

Approach to the Structure of Personality Impressions,” Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 9(4), 283–94.

Rule, Nicholas and Nalini Ambady (2008), “The Face of Success,” Psychological Science, 19(2),

109–11.

Shanks, J. Merill and Warren E. Miller (1991), “Partisanship, Policy, and Performance: The

Reagan Legacy in the 1988 Election,” British Journal of Political Science, 21, 129-97.

Shapiro, Ben (2008), Project President: Bad Hair and Botox on the Road to the White House,

Page 38: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

38

Nashville TN: Thomas Nelson.

Sorescu, Alina and Betsy Gelb (2000), “Negative Comparative Advertising: Evidence Favoring

Fine Tuning,” Journal of Advertising, 29(4), 25–40.

Stanton, Frank (2000), “The First Debate over Presidential Debates,” Newsweek, 136(13), 11.

Teinowitz, Ira (2004), “Election Ad Spending Hits Record 1.6 billion,” Advertising Age, October

25, 75(43), 1.

Thomson, Matthew (2006), “Human Brands: Investigating Antecedents to Consumers’ Strong

Attachments to Celebrities,” Journal of Marketing, 70 (July), 104–19.

Thompson, Seth and Janie Steckenrider (1997), “The Relative Irrelevance of Candidate Sex,”

Women and Politics, 17(4), 71–92.

Tierney, John (2004), “Secret Weapon for Bush?” The New York Times, October 24.

Todorov, Alexander, Anesu Mandisodza, Amir Goren, and Crystal Hall (2005), “Inferences of

Competence from Faces Predict Election Outcomes,” Science, 308 (June), 1623–26.

Toner, Robin and Carl Hulse (2007), “A Tough 5 Months, but Democrats Cite Successes,” The

New York Times, May 26, nytimes.com.

Turner, John, Penelope Oakes, S. Alexander Haslam, and Craig McGarty (1994), “Self and

Collective: Cognition and Social Context,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

20(5), 454–63.

Welch, William (1974), “The Economics of Campaign Funds,” Public Choice, 20(1), 83–97.

Wyer, Robert, T. Srull, and S. Gordon (1984), “The Effects of Predicting a Person’s Behavior on

Subsequent Trait Judgments,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 29–46.

Page 39: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

39

Table 1 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR INCUMBENTS IN DATABASE

All Races Republican Incumbents

Democrat Incumbents

Mean Std. Dev. Mean Mean Competence .14 .63 .36 -.13 Intelligence .16 .67 .32 -.04 Trust .02 .62 .14 -.14 Likeability -.04 .81 .13 -.26 Spending Ratio (Ratio) .714 0.20 0.69 0.74 Challenger Spending (Ch_Spend) $453,000 $55,000 $528,000 $357,000 Incumbent Spending (I_Spend) $841,000 $68,500 $858,000 $820,000 Challenger Negative % (Ch_Neg%) 17.0% 0.227 19.8% 13.4% Incumbent Negative % (I_Neg%) 16.0% .253 20.8% 9.7% Incumbent Vote Share (I_Vote%) 60.08% 6.84 60.64% 59.34%

Page 40: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

40

Table 2 STUDY 1 AND FOLLOW-UP STUDIES: IMPACT OF APPEARANCE ON CANDIDATE

EVALUATION

COMPETENCE VS. INTELLIGENCE

TRUST VS. LIKEABILITY

VISUAL STEREOTYPE

Variable Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Intercept 1.84 (1.28)

5.50*** (.50)

5.10*** (.35)

Appearance -1.03 (.69)

-1.75** (.82)

-.34 (.43)

Party .46 (.58)

-2.42*** (.51)

-1.76*** (.34)

Appearance*Party .82*** (.28)

.35 (.44)

-.14 (.29)

Conservatism .64*** (.11)

-.13* (.08)

-.24*** (.06)

Party*Conservatism -.41*** (.07)

.47*** (.11)

.45*** (.08)

Gender .94* (.48)

-.35 (.36)

.02 (.22)

Age .06 (.21)

-.17 (.12)

.02 (.08)

Gender*Appearance -.44 (.28)

.38 (.44)

.05 (.30)

Age*Appearance .03 (.12)

.27 (.16)

.05 (.12)

R-Square .18 .21 .15 F Statistic 7.82*** 4.44*** 4.96*** Observations 327 157 267

* p < .10, ** p < .05, *** p< .01

Page 41: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

41

Table 3 STUDY 2: IMPACT OF APPEARANCE ON NEGATIVE ADVERTISING

Variable Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Intercept 8.04*** (1.40)

Appearance -2.43*** (.86)

Party -2.42*** (.78)

Appearance*Party 1.66*** (.48)

Conservatism .25 (.21)

Party*Conservatism -.19 (.13)

R-Square .08 F Statistic 4.62*** Observations 291

* p < .10, ** p < .05, *** p< .01

Page 42: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

42

Table 4 STUDY 3: IMPACT OF APPEARANCE ON VOTE SHARE

Inferred Traits

Factors Party-Specific Inferred Traits

Party -Specific Factors

Difference Scores

Factors & Differences

Parameter (Std. Err.)

Parameter (Std. Err.)

Parameter (Std. Err.)

Parameter (Std. Err.)

Parameter (Std. Err.)

Parameter (Std. Err.)

Intercept 59.77*** (.52)

59.79*** (.49)

59.64*** (.54)

59.83*** (.52)

59.77*** (.51)

Incumbent Competence .12 (1.93)

Incumbent Intelligence .50 (1.82)

Incumbent Trustworthiness .56 (1.40)

Incumbent Likeability .97 (1.03)

Factor 1 (Competence) 1.17** (.50)

1.40*** (.49)

Factor 2 (Warmth) .51 (.50)

.36 (.48)

Dem Incumbent Competence -3.98 (2.50)

Rep Incumbent Competence 5.84** (2.84)

Dem Incumbent Intelligence 5.55** (2.40)

Rep Incumbent Intelligence -5.08** (2.53)

Dem Incumbent Trust -3.29* (2.02)

Rep Incumbent Trust 4.30** (1.87)

Dem Incumbent Likeability 4.53*** (1.46)

Rep Incumbent Likeability -2.64* (1.42)

Dem Incumbent Factor 1 1.34 (.82)

Rep Incumbent Factor 1 1.07* (.63)

Dem Incumbent Factor 2 .73 (.70)

Rep Incumbent Factor 2 .31 (.73)

Dem Incumbent (DI_Comp–Intel) -4.28* (2.35)

Dem Incumbent (DI_Trust–Like) -3.74*** (1.37)

Rep Incumbent (RI_Comp–Intel) 5.77** (2.66)

Rep Incumbent (RI_Trust–Like) 2.51* (1.43)

Advantage Comp vs Intel (AdvCI) 5.20*** (1.72)

Advantage Trust vs Like (AdvTL) 3.49*** (.97)

R-Square .041 .040 .130 .042 .081 .127 Observations 153

Page 43: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

43

Table 5

STUDY 3: IMPACT OF SPENDING AND APPEARANCE ON VOTE SHARE Spending Spending &

Appearance Parameter

(Std. Err.) Parameter (Std. Err.)

Intercept 62.51*** (4.72)

60.00*** (4.03)

Spending Ratio (Ratio) -29.46** (14.53)

-21.77* (12.29)

Spending Ratio Squared (Ratio2) 34.24*** (10.57)

25.76*** (8.99)

Negative Spending Ratio (NegRatio) -.92 (0.76)

-1.11 (.71)

Advantage Comp vs. Intel (AdvCI) 3.53*** (1.32)

Advantage Trust vs. Like (AdvTL) 2.53*** (.74)

Factor 1: Competence .74** (.37)

Factor 2: Warmth .47 (.36)

Cook X Rep Incumbent (Cook*RI) -.33*** (.07)

Cook X Dem Incumbent (Cook*DI) .30*** (.06)

Incumbent Years (I_YRS) .10* (.06)

AdvCI X NegRatio 5.18** (2.59)

AdvTL X NegRatio .52 (1.17)

Observations 153 153 R-Square .28 .56

* p < .10, ** p < .05, *** p< .01

Page 44: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

44

Table 6 STUDY 4: REGRESSIONS OF STEREOTYPICALITY ON INFERRED TRAITS

Republican Incumbents

Democratic Incumbents

Variable Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Intercept .051*** (.017)

-.003 (.02)

Competence .187*** (.66)

.009 (.068)

Intelligence -.069 (.060)

-.012 (.065)

Trustworthiness -.009 (.043)

-.033 (.054)

Likeability -.071** (.032)

.035 (.040)

R-Square .23 .024 F Statistic 6.15 .38 Observations 87 67

* p < .10, ** p < .05, *** p< .01

Table 7 REGRESSIONS OF VOTE SHARE ON STEREOTYPICALITY

Study 4 Follow-Up Study Variable Coefficient

(Std. Err.) Coefficient (Std. Err.)

Intercept 59.68*** (.54)

59.63*** (.50)

Dem Incumbent Stereotype (DIStereo) -4.29 (4.87)

Rep Incumbent Stereotype (RIStereo) 1.85 (4.18)

DIStereo X Cook .10 (.54)

RIStereo X Cook -1.25*** (.46)

R-Square .01 .05 Observations 153 153

* p < .10, ** p < .05, *** p< .01

Page 45: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

45

APPENDIX A

ILLUSTRATIVE CANDIDATE PAIRS

Pair 1: A is more competent than B Pair 2: A is more trustworthy than B

A: B: A: B: Pair 3: A is more intelligent than B. B is more competent than A.

Pair 4: A and B are similarly trustworthy. B is more likeable than A.

A: B: A: B:

Above are four pairs of candidates that produced relatively extreme ratings. These pairs are useful for illustrating the types of judgments made by subjects. For example, in Pair 1, Candidate A was perceived as more competent than Candidate B, and in Pair 2 Candidate A was perceived as more trustworthy than Candidate B. Furthermore, although prior research has noted that the personality traits are strongly correlated and we too observed high correlations, we also found important differences between the key measures. For example, Pair 3 illustrates the potential divergence between judgments of intelligence and competence with the candidate on the left being rated as more intelligent but less competent than the candidate on the right. Pair 4 highlights the distinction between trustworthiness and likeability, with the two candidates being perceived as equally trustworthy, but Candidate B being perceived as more likeable.

Page 46: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

46

APPENDIX B SAMPLE OF STIMULI USED IN STUDIES

Intelligent Appearance Competent Appearance

SAMPLE OF TEXT USED IN STUDY 2

Storyboard Page 1 (includes picture of ad target): Who will fight for working families? Not Stan Williams. Storyboard Page 2 (includes picture of ad target): Republican/Democratic candidate Stan Williams wants to remove all assistance to local farmers and opposes an income tax cut for hard‐working middle‐class Americans. Storyboard Page 3 (includes picture of ad target): And yet, Williams approves a plan to give congressmen a whopping 74% pay raise. Storyboard Page 4: Is this the kind of representation we want in Washington? Storyboard Page 5 (includes picture of ad target and ad sponsor): Say no to Stan Williams.  Elect Republican/Democrat Eric Hammond to Congress. 

Page 47: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

47

THE IMPACT OF CANDIDATE APPEARANCE AND ADVERTISING STRATEGIES ON ELECTION RESULTS

JoAndrea Hoegg Michael V. Lewis

WEB APPENDIX A

DATA COLLECTION METHOD FOR TRAIT DATABASE

Stimuli. Candidate photographs were obtained from the CNN.com election results pages, which provide information on candidates and race results. Because we used the actual photographs supplied by the candidates, some pictures were in color and in others were in black and white. Pairs in which one picture was in color and the other was black and white were discarded. Photographs pairs were adjusted to be uniform in size. All photographs were approximately two inches high and 1.6 inches wide. Ninety-eight races from 2000, 50 races from 2002 and 42 races from 2004 were selected. Each photograph was rated by between 58 and 87 undergraduate business students who participated in the study for partial course credit. Although undergraduate students are likely not representative of the U.S. population in terms of political views, their political knowledge and affiliation were immaterial to the task. Prior research has well documented that people’s perceptions of others based on physical attributes are universal (Ekman 2004).

Procedure. The ratings were collected in stages. The 2000 races were collected first, with the 2002 and 2004 sets collected in subsequent rounds. Because there were 98 pairs of photographs in the 2000 set, rater fatigue was a concern. The pairs of photographs were divided into three sets (two sets of 33 and one set of 32), and participants were randomly assigned to one of the sets. Ratings occurred on computer. Participants were provided with an introductory screen telling them that they would be viewing actual candidates from a congressional election and would be asked to rate the candidates on a variety of personality traits based only on candidate photographs. They were then presented with a training pair as a practice trial. Upon successful completion of the practice trial, participants viewed the pairs in random order. The two candidates for each race appeared on either side of the screen, position determined randomly. The photographs were labeled “Candidate A” and “Candidate B.” No information was provided regarding name, district, incumbency, or party affiliation. To address our research question about the divergence of competence and intelligence across the parties, as well as to adhere to prior research in political trait inferences, we not only collected trait ratings of competence and intelligence, but also included the traits of trustworthiness and likeability. All judgments were based on relative judgments of the candidates’ appearance. Participants provided responses to the personality trait questions, which also appeared in random order. Each personality trait was rated on a 7-point scale from “Candidate A looks much more [intelligent]” to “Candidate B looks much more [intelligent].” After rating all candidate pairs on the personality traits, participants viewed the photograph pairs a second time. This time they were asked to indicate which candidate they thought was the Democrat or Republican (question counterbalanced) using a binary choice measure. After the binary choice, participants were asked whether they recognized the politician shown in the picture. Results indicated that candidate

Page 48: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

48

familiarity was extremely low, with no candidate being recognized by more than three raters. The analysis was conducted with and without the inclusion of the few participants who recognized a candidate; no differences were found. At the end of the study participants were asked to provide their level of political involvement, party affiliation, and some demographic information. One hundred forty-seven undergraduate students from a large Midwest university participated in the study for course credit.

An examination of the election results from the 98 congressional races revealed that in 25 of the races the incumbent won with at least 85% of the vote. In the vast majority of these lopsided races, advertising spending by challengers was negligible. Because the inclusion of such data points could lead to disproportional influence on the spending variables, we elected to omit races in which the outcome was 85% or more of vote share. This exclusion criterion left only 73 usable candidate pairs. In the second round of ratings, using the 2002 data, we began with 50 races. The procedure was identical to the one described for the 2000 races, except that the picture set was broken into two sets of 25 pairs each. One hundred-sixteen undergraduate students completed the study. In this set, 11 races were not sufficiently close to be included and were removed from the analysis. The final data collection procedure involved the 2004 picture pairs. Again the procedure was identical but in this case we eliminated races that were not close prior to the data collection; thus we were able to use the entire set of 41 races for 2004. Eighty-one undergraduate students rated the 2004 face pairs. The three sets of face ratings resulted in a final total of 153 candidate pairs being entered into the analysis.

Page 49: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

49

WEB APPENDIX B

VARIABLES IN STUDY 3 VARIABLE NAME DEFINITION Incumbent Competence (Comp) Incumbents’ relative rating on competence.

Incumbent Intelligence (Intel) Incumbents’ relative rating on intelligence.

Incumbent Trustworthiness (Trust) Incumbents’ relative rating on trustworthiness.

Incumbent Likeability (Like) Incumbents’ relative rating on likeability.

Challenger Spending (Ch_Spend) Total amount the challenger spent on the campaign.

Incumbent Spending (I_Spend) Total amount the incumbent spent on the campaign.

Spending Ratio (Ratio) Incumbent spending divided by the sum of incumbent and challenger spending.

Challenger Negative % (Ch_Neg%) The proportion of total spending by the challenger that was allocated to negative advertising.

Incumbent Negative % (I_Neg%) The proportion of total spending by the incumbent that was allocated to negative advertising.

Negative Spending Ratio (NegRatio) The difference between incumbents’ and challengers’ spending on negative advertising divided by total spending.

Incumbent Vote Share (I_Vote%) Proportion of votes in a given district that went to the incumbent.

Factor 1 (Ability) Factor encompassing incumbents’ relative ratings on the traits of competence, intelligence and trustworthiness.

Factor 2 (Ability) Factor encompassing incumbents’ relative ratings on the traits of trustworthiness and likeability.

Dem Incumbent Competence Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on competence.

Rep Incumbent Competence Republican incumbents’ relative rating on competence.

Dem Incumbent Intelligence Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on intelligence.

Rep Incumbent Intelligence Republican incumbents’ relative rating on intelligence.

Dem Incumbent Trust Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on trustworthiness.

Rep Incumbent Trust Republican incumbents’ relative rating on trustworthiness.

Dem Incumbent Likeability Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on likeability.

Rep Incumbent Likeability Republican incumbents’ relative rating on likeability.

Dem Incumbent Factor 1 Factor encompassing Democratic incumbents’ relative ratings on the traits of competence, intelligence and trustworthiness.

Rep Incumbent Factor 1 Factor encompassing Republican incumbents’ relative ratings on the traits of competence, intelligence and trustworthiness.

Dem Incumbent Factor 2 Factor encompassing Democratic incumbents’ relative ratings on the traits of likeability and trustworthiness.

Rep Incumbent Factor 2 Factor encompassing Republican incumbents’ relative ratings on the traits of likeability and trustworthiness.

Dem Incumbent (Comp – Intel) Difference between Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on competence and relative rating on intelligence.

Rep Incumbent (Comp – Intel) Difference between Republican incumbents’ relative rating on competence and relative rating on intelligence.

Dem Incumbent (Trust – Like) Difference between Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on trustworthiness and relative rating on likeability.

Rep Incumbent (Trust – Like) Difference between Republican incumbents’ relative rating on trustworthiness and relative rating on likeability.

AdvantageCI (Comp vs. Intel) Difference between Republican incumbents’ relative rating on competence and relative rating on intelligence and the difference between Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on intelligence and relative rating on competence.

AdvantageTL (Trust vs. Like) Difference between Republican incumbents’ relative rating on trustworthiness and relative rating on likeability and the difference between Democratic incumbents’ relative rating on likeability and relative rating on trustworthiness.

Cook Cook Partisan Voting Index, a measure of the partisan leaning of a given district compared to the nation as a whole.

Page 50: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

50

Incumbent Years Number of years incumbent has been in office.

Page 51: The Impact of Candidate Appearance and …...appearance and party alters the effects of advertising spending, particularly the effects of negative advertising. The findings have implications

51

WEB APPENDIX C CORRELATIONS AMONG VARIABLES IN STUDY 3

All Incumbents Incumbent Vote Share

Spending Ratio

Competence Intelligence Trust Likeability

Incumbent Vote Share 1.0 Spending Ratio .60 1.0 Competence .16 .15 1.0 Intelligence .14 .18 .88 1.0 Trustworthiness .22 .19 .59 .47 1.0 Likeability .16 .16 .38 .016 .078 1.0 Republicans Incumbent Vote Share 1.0 Spending Ratio .64 1.0 Competence .21 .26 1.0 Intelligence .13 .23 .90 1.0 Trustworthiness .24 .19 .64 .54 1.0 Likeability .09 .16 .52 .33 .83 1.0 Democrats Incumbent Vote Share 1.0 Spending Ratio .57 1.0 Competence .04 .16 1.0 Intelligence .11 .21 .56 1.0 Trustworthiness .16 .27 .44 .32 1.0 Likeability .22 .25 .58 -.18 .68 1.0