93
University of Connecticut OpenCommons@UConn Doctoral Dissertations University of Connecticut Graduate School 8-20-2013 e Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions on Cardiovascular Reactions to Acute Stress Katherine O'Leary University of Connecticut, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: hps://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations Recommended Citation O'Leary, Katherine, "e Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions on Cardiovascular Reactions to Acute Stress" (2013). Doctoral Dissertations. 170. hps://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations/170

The Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions on

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Running head: BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

The Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions on Cardiovascular Reactions to Acute Stress

Katherine Elizabeth O’Leary

University of Connecticut, 2013

Stress is a major public health concern due to its’ harmful effects on physical and

mental health. An important goal for clinical health practitioners is to help patients

reduce the psychological and physiological burden of stress. The present study sought to

examine the impact brief clinical interventions have on cardiovascular reactions to acute

stress. Additionally, we explored how depressive and anxiety symptomatology influence

cardiovascular reactivity and recovery, and their moderating effects on treatment

response to interventions. To address these aims, subjects were randomized into one of

three conditions (Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, Autogenic Training, Attention-

only Control group) prior to undergoing an acute psychosocial stress paradigm, the Trier

Social Stress Test (TSST). Psychosocial measures were given at baseline, and heart rate

and blood pressure measurements were obtained at various time points throughout the

protocol. Our results indicated a time by group interaction for heart rate (HR) and

diastolic blood pressure (DBP), demonstrating that brief interventions differentially

affected heart rate and diastolic blood pressure over time. Within the Autogenic Training

group results showed that subjects with higher levels of depressive symptomatology had

lower systolic blood pressure reactivity compared to those with lower levels of depressive

symptoms. Additionally, within the Autogenic Training group, greater recovery in both

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS ii

systolic and diastolic blood pressure was found in subjects with higher levels of social

anxiety. Our results offer important implications for clinical assessment and intervention,

particularly within the field of behavioral medicine.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS iii

The Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions on Cardiovascular Reactions to Acute Stress

Katherine Elizabeth O’Leary

B.S., Georgetown University, 2005

M.A., Teachers College, Columbia University, 2008

M.A., University of Connecticut, 2010

A Dissertation

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

at the

University of Connecticut

2013

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS iv

APPROVAL PAGE

Doctor of Philosophy Dissertation

The Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions on Cardiovascular Reactions to Acute Stress

Presented by

Katherine Elizabeth O’Leary, M.A.

Major Advisor __________________________________________

Dean G. Cruess

Associate Advisor ________________________________________

Amy A. Gorin

Associate Advisor ________________________________________

John D. Salamone

Examiner 1 ________________________________________

Marianne L. Barton

Examiner 2 ________________________________________

Juliane R. Fenster

University of Connecticut

2013

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract……..……………………………………………………………………………...i

Introduction……………………………………………………………………………..…1

Stress……………………………………………..………………………………..1

Allostasis and Allostatic Load…………………………………………….………2

Stress, depression, anxiety, and cardiovascular disease………………......4

Acute Stress Testing………………………………………………………………6

Trier Social Stress Test…………………………………………………....8

Psychological Interventions……………………………………………………...11

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy…………………………………..12

Autogenic Training………………………………………………………15

Present Study…………………………………………………………………….18

Method.…………………………………………………………………………………..19

Participants……………………………………………………………………….19

Procedure………………………………………………………………………...20

Physiological Measures.........................................................................................21

Psychological Measures………………………………………………………….21

Anxiety Sensitivity Index………………………………………………..22

Beck Depression Inventory-II……………………………………………22

Demographic and Medical History Questionnaire………………………23

Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale………………………………………….23

Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Inventory- Trait………………………...24

Experimental and Control Conditions…………………………………………...24

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS vi

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy condition………………………..24

Autogenic Training condition……………………………………………25

Attention-only Activity condition………………………………………..25

Challenge Task…………………………………………………………………...25

Trier Social Stress Test protocol…………………………………………25

Data Analytic Plan……..…………………………………………………….......26

Results……………………………………………………………………………………28

Participant Characteristics………………………………….……………………28

Descriptive Statistics for Study Measures………………………...……………..29

Baseline physiological measures………………………………………...29

Psychological measures………………………………………………….30

Hypothesis 1: Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions……………………………30

Hypothesis 1a: Depressive and/or Anxiety Symptoms as Treatment

Moderators……………………………………………………………….31

Hypothesis 1b: Association of Depressive and Anxiety Symptoms with

Cardiovascular Reactivity and Recovery………………………………...36

Discussion…………………………………………………………………….………….36

Purpose of the Present Study…………………………………………………….36

Hypothesis 1……………………………………………………………………...38

Hypothesis 1a…………………………………………………………………….40

Hypothesis 1b…………………………………………………………………….43

Limitations……………………………………………………………………….45

Future Directions………………………………………………………………...45

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS vii

References…………………………………………………………………….………….47

Appendix ...………………………………………………………………………………62

Tables……………………………………………………..……………………………...63

Figures……………………………………………………………………………………69

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 1

Stress

Stress is defined as a real or implied threat to one’s psychological or physical

integrity. It evokes behavioral and physiological responses to psychosocial,

environmental, and physical challenges, and thus can be viewed and measured from a

variety of perspectives (Clark, Bond, & Hecker, 2007; Holmes & Rahe, 1967; Kiecolt-

Glaser, McGuire, Robles, & Glaser, 2002; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Logan &

Barksdale, 2008). Stress is a major public health concern. Over the past several decades,

there has been a growing awareness of stress’s detrimental health effects, thereby

necessitating a need for research into the pathways linking stress and health (Kudielka,

Buske-Kirschbaum, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2004a).

The most common form of stress is acute stress. In moderation acute stress is not

harmful, however, if experienced frequently it can cause psychological and physical

distress. Stress has long been linked to maladaptive functioning and various

psychopathologies, having been found to play a large causal and maintaining role in

psychological disorders (Kessler, 1997; Nugent, Tyrka, Carpenter, & Price, 2011). For

example, individuals prone to experience acute stress are more likely to be over-aroused,

irritable, and anxious. Additionally, acute stress has been found to trigger panic attacks

and psychotic episodes in those predisposed to thought disorders (Chrousos, 2009).

Several epidemiological studies have also emerged evidencing stress as a major

psychosocial risk factor for various medical illnesses, such as cardiovascular disease

(McEwen, 2000; Yusef et al., 2004). Individuals who experience frequent acute stress

are more prone to experience headaches, allergic reactions (e.g., asthma, eczema),

hypertension, pain, and gastrointestinal symptoms (Black & Garbutt, 2002; Chrousos,

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 2

2009). Currently, however, the exact mechanisms whereby stress exert its’ effects on

human physiological and behavioral systems are not well understood (Epel et al., 2006).

Allostasis and Allostatic Load

The concepts of allostasis and allostatic load are useful frameworks for

investigating the detrimental health effects of stress. Sterling and Eyer (1988) introduced

allostasis as a concept that represents the active, adaptive process of regulatory

physiological systems to physical, psychosocial, and environmental challenges.

Stemming from allostasis theory, allostatic load, according to McEwen and Stellar

(1993), is the wear and tear on the body caused by repeated activation of the stress

response. The concept of allostatic load is used to explain how frequent acute and

chronic stress causes repeated activation of the stress response, resulting in damage and

pathology (Clark et al., 2007; McEwen, 1998; Logan & Barksdale, 2008).

As McEwen (2000) explains there are four main conditions that lead to allostatic

load: repeated frequency of the stress response, failure to habituate to repeated stressors,

failure to turn off the stress response in a timely manner, and inadequate response leading

to hyperactivity of other mediators. Allostatic load is measured through a composite

index of indicators of cumulative strain. It is measured in physiological systems as

chemical imbalances in the autonomic nervous system, central nervous system, and

neuroendocrine immune system, and is thought to lead to disease through a number of

routes. For example, chemical messengers, such as cortisol and epinephrine are released

as part of the stress response. These chemicals can be affected by a variety of lifestyle

factors (e.g., sleep, diet) and are generally beneficial. However, when the body is

overexposed to them, cumulative changes occur. Repeated or dysregulated (i.e., over or

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 3

under-active) exposure to chemical mediators leads to tissue and organ specific effects,

such as elevated blood pressure and increased heart rate. These specific effects lead to

subsequent outcomes, which are the actual disorders that arise from allostatic load

(McEwen, 2003). Examples of chronic illnesses thought to be associated with allostatic

load are atherosclerosis, obesity, impaired physical capacity, anxiety, depression, and

cognitive decline (Clark et al., 2007; Logan & Barksdale, 2008; Schulkin, 2004; Seeman,

Singer, Rowe, Horwitz, & McEwen, 1997; McEwen, 2004).

Responses to acute stress may be mediated through a variety of behavioral

pathways, including lifestyle factors such as smoking, substance use, obesity, and

socioeconomic status (McEwen, 2000). As we know, the brain perceives stress (whether

real or imagined) and produces behavioral responses to cope with it. These responses are

determined by interpretations by the brain, which are greatly affected by psychosocial

and individual differences, such as anxiety, depression, and social support (Logan &

Barksdale, 2008; McEwen, 2002). For example, one person may perceive a stressful

event as threatening, while another may not. Additionally, one may respond to an event

to avoid a threat, where another may respond in such a way as to increase danger

(McEwen, 2000). In sum, how a person interprets and copes with an event determines

behaviors and habits that are highly influenced by psychosocial and individual

differences. These behaviors make life more or less dangerous in the short-run and

increase allostatic load over time.

In addition to behavioral pathways, direct physiological pathways also contribute

to associations between psychosocial factors and the stress response. Chronic, acute

physical symptoms common in anxiety disorders and depressive illnesses, suggest that

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 4

physiological stress-related processes, particularly those involving the autonomic nervous

system, greatly influence these conditions (American Psychiatric Association, 2000;

Chrousos, 2009; Chrousos & Gold, 1992; Sullivan, Coplan, Kent, & Gorman, 1999).

Currently, there is a discrepancy among the literature in how psychosocial factors affect

the stress response. For example, some studies have found positive psychological states

and traits, anxiety, neuroticism, and negative affect to be associated with reduced

hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and cardiovascular reactivity to stress and

poor recovery, while other studies have found hyperactive responses due to these factors

(Logan & Barksdale, 2008; McEwen, 1998; McEwen, 2007; Tsigos & Chrousos, 2002).

This inconsistency indicates that psychosocial factors may differentially influence

individual stress responses (Chida & Hamer, 2008).

Stress, depression, anxiety, and cardiovascular disease. Within the allostatic

load literature, the best-studied system in the human body is the cardiovascular system

(McEwen, 1998). Researchers have repeatedly linked stress to cardiovascular disease,

one of the most frequent causes of death in the United States (Armario et al. 2003,

Rosamond et al., 2007). In addition, other psychosocial factors, including depression and

anxiety, have been shown to increase one’s risk for cardiovascular disease. There have

been several large reviews about the links between depression, anxiety, and heart disease.

For example, Kubzansky, Kawachi, Weiss, and Sparrow (1998) found that chronic

anxiety was a major risk factor for coronary heart disease and Glassman and Shapiro

(1997) demonstrated that depression was associated with increased mortality from

coronary heart disease. However, the mechanisms by which these relationships exist are

not as well known (Logan & Barksdale, 2008).

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 5

Anxiety and depression are believed to affect the heart through three main

pathways: increasing ill-health behaviors, promoting atherogenesis via hypertension

arising from chronic sympathetic nervous system activation and/or dysregulation, and

triggering fatal coronary events. In regards to acute stress, researchers have hypothesized

that repeated episodes of acute psychological stress can result in elevated blood pressure,

heart rate, and inflammatory processes (i.e., markers of allostatic load), increasing

atherosclerotic plaques and stiffness of large arteries causing greater risk for

cardiovascular disease (McEwen, 2002). In fact, epidemiologic studies have found an

association between heightened cardiovascular reactivity in response to stress, extensive

progression of atherosclerosis, and incidence of ischemic heart disease and mortality

(Black & Garbutt, 2002). In sum, it is thought that anxiety and depression make

individuals more susceptible to stress, resulting in repeated exposure to the stress

response and an increase in allostatic load.

Currently, there is uncertainty in the literature over whether increased

cardiovascular reactivity, poor cardiovascular recovery, or both lead to disease states.

Additionally, research has shown mixed results on the effects depressive and anxiety

symptomatology have on reactivity and recovery. For example, studies investigating

cardiovascular responses to acute psychosocial stressors have suggested that depression

and anxiety are related to enhanced cardiovascular reactivity, as evidenced by increased

blood pressure and heart rate (de Rooij, Schene, Phillips, Roseboom, 2010; Kibler & Ma,

2004; Mausbach et al., 2005). Others, however, have found negative associations

implicating a hyporeactivity to acute psychosocial stress (Carroll, Phillips, Hunt, & Der,

2007; Jezova, Makatsori, Duncko, Moncek, & Jakubek, 2004; Salomon, Clift,

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 6

Karisdottir, & Rottenberg, 2009). For example, Heopniemi et al. (2007) found that

delayed heart rate recovery and lower cardiac autonomic reactivity were associated with

higher carotid atherosclerosis. Mixed results in the literature may be in part due to an

integrated stress response pattern, thereby supporting the concept of allostatic load that

psychosocial factors could lead to different acute stress responses (Chida & Hamer,

2008).

In sum, current explanations of stress have failed to adequately clarify its

association with health and chronic illness. The concept of allostatic load has

applicability to subclinical and clinical psychological symptoms and suggests ways in

which allostasis theory can be applied to the development of clinical interventions to

increase resilience and the likelihood of better health outcomes (Logan & Barksdale,

2008). Further, responses to acute psychological challenge in the laboratory may not be

considered of great clinical importance in and of themselves. However, they adequately

index the way individuals respond to ordinary psychological demands, thus offering a

way to investigate how disturbed stress responses have pathophysiological significance in

everyday life (Chida & Hamer, 2008; Salomon et al., 2009).

Acute Stress Testing

As previously mentioned, acute stress has long been thought to play a role in the

etiology of human psychopathologies, such as anxiety and depressive disorders, as well

as medical illnesses, like cardiovascular disease (Black & Garbutt, 2002; Chrousos, 2009;

Kessler, 1997; McEwen, 2000; Nugent et al., 2011; Yusef et al., 2004). Due to the

clinical significance of acute stress reactions, investigating the various ways stress

impacts pathological and psychopathological mechanisms is of great importance

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 7

(McEwen , 2007).

In order to investigate these mechanisms, standardized procedures to induce acute

psychosocial stress have been widely developed (Childs, Vicini, & De Wit, 2006). One

useful tool for this type of research has been laboratory-based mental stress testing. This

particular testing involves the measurement of acute physiological responses, such as

cardiac, neuroendocrine, and autonomic nervous system functions, to standardized

stimuli (e.g., public speaking, mental arithmetic tasks, emotionally demanding social

interaction) (Chida & Hamer, 2008; Hellhammer & Schubert, 2012; Kirschbaum, Pirke,

Hellhammer, 1993). Although psychosocial stress testing involves artificial stimuli, it

allows for the induction of consistent physiological responses with good test–retest

reliability and offers a way to eliminate confounds, thereby elucidating causal factors

(Brotman, Golden, & Wittstein, 2007; Chida & Hamer, 2008).

In contrast to protocols that involve physical exertion or pharmaceutical agents,

acute psychosocial stress stimuli exert physiological, behavioral, and subjective stress

responses (Kudielka, Wust, Kirschbaum, & Hellhammer, 2007). In a meta-analysis of

208 studies, Dickerson and Kemeny (2004) found that psychosocial stress tasks perceived

as uncontrollable and that involve social-evaluative threat elicit the largest physiological

response and longest recovery times. Tasks that are defined as uncontrollable are those

in which outcomes are perceived to be unaffected by individual behavioral responses.

Social-evaluative threat involves the perception that others could negatively judge one’s

task performance, thereby impacting social esteem, self-evaluation, and social goals

(Kudielka et al., 2007). The magnitude of changes in physiological parameters has been

shown to depend on intensity of threat, context, and vulnerability, as well as protective

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 8

factors (Dickerson & Kemeny 2004).

Trier Social Stress Test. A variety of standardized procedures have been

developed to induce acute psychosocial stress in a laboratory setting. One of the most

widely used protocols to study physiological responses to psychosocial stress is the Trier

Social Stress Test (TSST) (Kirschbaum et al., 1993). The TSST involves a public speech

and mental arithmetic task that is performed in front of an evaluative panel and therefore,

involves elements of uncontrollability and social evaluation. It has been used in research

with healthy study participants and clinical populations. For example, the TSST has been

successfully applied in studies with patients suffering from depressive illnesses, anxiety

disorders (e.g., social phobia, posttraumatic stress disorder), burnout, exhaustion,

functional somatic syndromes (e.g., chronic fatigue syndrome, fibromyalgia, irritable

bowel syndrome), pain, and breast cancer survivors (Kudielka et al., 2007). The TSST is

one of the few available protocols that reliably elicits cardiovascular parameters, having

been shown to produce moderate increases in both heart rate and blood pressure

(Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004; Nater et al., 2005). It has also been proven to produce

moderate increases in cortisol and subjective reports of anxiety (Childs et al., 2006;

Kudielka et al., 2004a; Williams, Hagerty, & Brooks, 2004).

The standardized TSST protocol lasts for a total of thirteen minutes. It involves a

brief preparation period (3 minutes) followed by a test period during which subjects are

asked to deliver a free speech (5 minutes). Lastly, subjects are given a time to perform a

mental arithmetic task (5 minutes). They are asked to take over the role of a job applicant

invited for an interview with the company’s selection committee. The selection

committee is comprised of 2-3 males or females unacquainted with the subject and has

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 9

been trained to respond in an unresponsive, neutral manner. Selection committee

members are introduced to subjects as being trained to monitor nonverbal behavior.

Subjects are informed that their speech will be taped and video recorded for later

analysis. During the speech period, the selection committee members are given

directions to respond to subjects in a standardized way. For example, if a subject ends the

speech in less than five minutes, members are given prepared questions (e.g., what are

your personal strengths? what are your major shortcomings?) The mental arithmetic task

involves asking the subject to serially subtract an uneven number from a larger number as

fast and as accurately as possible. If the subject makes an error, they are asked to restart

(Kirschbaum et al., 1993).

Over the past two decades, the TSST has shown to be a useful tool in basic,

applied, and clinical psychobiological research. Eliciting a range of psychobiological

outcome variables, the TSST protocol has been utilized in a multitude of studies with

children, adults, and the elderly to investigate the stress-disease connection (Kudielka et

al., 2004a). It has even recently been successfully applied in both group and virtual

reality formats (Child et al., 2006; Jonsson et al., 2010). Inducing endocrine and

cardiovascular responses in 70–80% of subjects tested, the TSST procedure has been

shown to significantly increase parameters such as free cortisol, total plasma cortisol,

adrenocorticotropic hormone, catecholamines, growth hormone, prolactin, testosterone,

blood pressure, heart rate, immune parameters, and measures of hemoconcentration and

blood coagulation (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Kudielka et al., 2007; Nater et al., 2005).

As previously mentioned, the TSST has been utilized in research investigating

cardiovascular reactions to acute psychosocial stress in nonclinical, healthy populations.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 10

For example, in a more recent study, Taylor et al. (2010) found a main effect of time

showing a strong increase in heart rate, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood

pressure from baseline to peak stress. Additionally, these researchers found that although

cardiovascular parameters decreased after the post-stress task, full recovery was

incomplete, as evidenced by significant baseline to recovery differences. In another

study conducted by Nater et al. (2005), researchers found a significant difference in heart

rate between the TSST and rest conditions and a positive relationship between salivary

alpha amylase and sympathetic tone during stress. In conclusion, the TSST has been

reliably shown to induce acute physiological reactivity and incomplete recovery from

psychosocial stress.

The TSST has also been successful utilized to investigate the impact psychosocial

factors have on cardiovascular reactions to acute psychosocial stress in clinical

populations and subjects with subthreshold psychological symptomatology. For

example, Chida and Hamer (2008) found that depressive mood or hopelessness exhibited

significant associations with increased cardiovascular reactivity. Regarding

cardiovascular recovery after stress, these researchers found that general life stress and

anxiety, neuroticism, and negative affect were associated with poorer cardiovascular

recovery. In contrast, a meta-analysis conducted by Kibler and Ma (2004), reported

small, nonsignificant effect sizes linking depression to blood pressure reactivity and

modest effect sizes for heart rate reactivity. Additionally, York et al. (2007), Carrol et al.

(2007), and Solomon et al., (2009) found a negative association between depressive

symptoms and cardiovascular reactivity to mental stress. In sum, and as previously

mentioned, current research has shown mixed results on the effects psychological

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 11

symptoms, particularly depression and anxiety, have on physiological stress reactions.

The TSST has been utilized for well over two decades as a tool to study the

mechanisms by which stress impacts physiological systems. Given its success, the next

important step is to investigate whether the TSST protocol can be utilized to investigate

the efficacy of psychosocial interventions (Kudielka et al., 2007).

Psychosocial Interventions

A key goal for clinical health practitioners is to help patients reduce the

psychological and physiological burden of stress. Over the past few decades, researchers

have been investigating whether social, behavioral, and psychotherapeutic interventions

can help practitioners achieve this goal (McEwen, 2007). Studies have focused on

psychosocial interventions aimed at managing stress, increasing resilience to stressful

experiences, and treating physiologic responses to stress to improve health outcomes,

such as cardiovascular function. These nonpharmacological interventions target lifestyle

factors (e.g., sleep, diet, physical activity), as well as interpersonal relationships, self-

efficacy, and self-esteem (Logan & Barksdale, 2008). Others teach relaxation techniques

to help one cognitively, behaviorally, and physiologically cope with, counteract, or alter

the way in which one experiences and recovers from stressful experiences. Psychosocial

interventions can be offered in both long-term and brief formats, and are thought to offer

a useful, nonpharmacological means to prevent the negative effects stress has in both

healthy and clinical populations. Two such psychosocial interventions are Autogenic

Training (Schultz, 1932) and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Hayes, Strosahl, &

Wilson, 1999).

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 12

(ACT) is a therapeutic approach developed by Hayes et al. (1999) that focuses on

increasing flexibility and willingness to experience stressful and anxiety provoking

events and feelings. It is a comprehensive psychotherapy treatment that can be delivered

in both individual and group formats over both short and long-term. ACT is based on

relational frame theory, a contextual theory of language and cognition (RFT; Barnes-

Holmes, Hayes, & Roche, 2001). Having grown out of the cognitive-behavioral

tradition, ACT is considered a third-wave behavioral therapy. However, unlike CBT,

ACT makes no attempts to change unwanted thoughts or feelings; these are not

necessarily viewed as problematic. As ACT teaches, acceptance and willingness, rather

than control, is what leads a person to desired goals and improvements in quality of life.

In order to attain goals and implement behavior change, ACT makes use of experiential

methods and a number of therapeutic strategies (Hayes, Masuda, Bissett, Luoma, &

Guerrero, 2004).

Many, including ACT followers, conceptualize forms of psychopathology as

unhealthy efforts to control emotions, memories, thoughts, and other internal experiences

(Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette & Strosahl, 1996). ACT teaches that attempts to

control unwanted subjective experiences are counterproductive, resulting in an increase

in psychological distress. ACT teaches positive psychological skills to counteract

attempts to control, such as acceptance and cognitive defusion. Acceptance is taught as

an alternative to experiential avoidance and involves an active embrace of private events

without attempts to change their frequency or form. Cognitive defusion attempts to alter

one’s sensitivity to undesirable thoughts (e.g., “I am being judged”) by reducing the

literal quality of the experienced thought (e.g., I am having the thought “I am being

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 13

judged”). Cognitive defusion techniques attempt to decrease the believability/reality of

thoughts (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis 2006).

Although a relatively new psychotherapy treatment, ACT has shown empirical

evidence for its effectiveness in treating a variety of psychopathologies (Forman, Herbert,

Moitra, Yeomans, & Geller, 2007; Hayes et al., 2004). In a recent review of the

literature, Hayes et al. (2006) found that the average post-treatment effect size for

randomized controlled clinical trials of ACT ranged from .55 to .99 and average follow-

up effect sizes ranged from .55 to .80. Studies have shown promising results for its

efficacy in treating depression (Forman et al., 2007; Zettle & Hayes, 1986; Zettle &

Rains, 1989), anxiety (Woods, Wetterneck, & Flessner, 2006; Zettle, 2003), thought

disorders (Bach & Hayes, 2002), substance abuse (Hayes et al., 2004), and workplace

stress (Bond & Bunce, 2003).

For example, in a published study relevant to ours, Block (2002) examined the

efficacy of ACT for social anxiety symptoms. In that study, 39 college students with

public speaking anxiety were semi-randomly assigned to six weeks of either ACT,

cognitive-behavioral group therapy, or a no-treatment, waitlist control. Results showed

that scores on social anxiety decreased for both treatment groups compared to the control

condition. In addition, willingness to engage in public speaking situations increased for

both treatment conditions but only the ACT group showed significant decreases in

behavioral avoidance (Block, 2002). Additionally, in another study conducted by

Forman et al. (2007), 101 university counseling center patients reporting moderate to

severe levels of anxiety or depression were randomly assigned to traditional cognitive

therapy or ACT (medium length of treatment = 15-16 sessions). Results indicated that

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 14

both cognitive therapy and ACT evidenced large, equivalent improvements in depression,

anxiety, functioning difficulties, quality of life, life satisfaction, and clinician-rated

functioning. In regards to mechanisms of action, researchers found that changes in

“observing” and “describing” one’s experiences appeared to mediate outcomes for

cognitive therapy, whereas “experiential avoidance,” “acting with awareness,” and

“acceptance” mediated outcomes for ACT (Forman et al., 2007).

ACT has also been shown to be a useful therapeutic approach in behavioral

medicine populations. ACT has been implemented with chronic pain (McCracken &

Eccleston, 2006), cigarette smoking cessation (Gifford et al., 2004), diabetes (Gregg,

Callaghan, Hayes, Glenn-Lawson, 2007), and epilepsy (Lundgren, Dahl, & Hayes, 2004)

patients. One hypothesized mechanisms by which ACT benefits medical patient

populations is through its emphasis on emotional acceptance rather than suppression.

Research has shown that certain emotion-regulation processes, such as rumination and

worry, increase distress and prolong physiological stress responses. In contrast,

acceptance-oriented coping strategies promote efficient cardiovascular habituation and

recovery from stress (Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, & Hofmann, 2006; Low, Stanton,

& Bower, 2008).

For example, in a recent study conducted by Low et al. (2008), 81 participants

were randomly assigned to write about ongoing stressful experience while either (1)

evaluating the appropriateness of their emotional response, (2) attending to their emotions

in accepting way or, (3) simply describing the objective details of their experience.

Results indicated that writing about emotions in an evaluative way led to less efficient

heart rate habituation and recovery than processing emotions in an accepting manner

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 15

(Low et al., 2008). In another study by Campbell-Sills et al. (2006), researchers

compared subjective and physiological effects of emotional suppression and acceptance

in 60 subjects diagnosed with anxiety and mood disorders. Subjects were randomly

assigned to either a group that listened to a rationale for suppressing emotions or a group

that listened to a rationale for accepting emotions. Subjective distress, heart rate, skin

conductance level, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia were measured before, during, and

after watching an emotion-provoking film during which the subjects were told to apply

the learned instructions. Results indicated that both groups reported similar levels of

subjective distress during the film but that the acceptance group displayed less negative

affect during the post-film recovery period. Additionally, the suppression group showed

an increased heart rate while the acceptance group showed a decreased heart rate in

response to the film (Cambell-Sills et al., 2006). In sum, there is solid and growing

evidence that ACT is a beneficial intervention for both nonclinical and clinical

populations.

Autogenic Training. Autogenic Training (AT) is a relaxation technique

performed through passive concentration aimed at promoting relaxation and stress relief.

Johannes Schultz, a German psychiatrist and neurologist, first presented AT in the 1930s.

Schultz theorized that mentally connecting with parts of one’s body produces a state

similar to that experienced under hypnosis and found that his technique of AT improved

health and reduced stress in many of his patients (Schultz, 1932). AT was further

developed by Wolfgang Luthe and in its’ current form involves a series of statements

aimed at reducing autonomic arousal (Linden, 1990; Luthe, 1969; Luthe & Schultz,

1959). AT teaches passive concentration on breathing, heartbeat, warmth, and heaviness

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 16

of body parts, and is normally taught and learned over a period of eight to ten weeks

(Kanji, 1997; Morgan & Jorm, 2008). AT is based on three main principles: reduction of

exteroceptive and proprioceptive afferent stimulation, mental repetition of

psychophysiologically adapted verbal statements, and mental activity conceived as

'passive concentration' (Schulz, 1932; Kanji, 1997). Some believe that AT may be

superior to other behavioral relaxation techniques because it teaches individuals how to

self-regulate physiological responses and, once learned, can be practiced without a

therapist (Cowings, Toscano, Timbers, Casey, & Hufnagel, 2005; Schlamann, Naglatzki,

de Greiff, Forsting, & Gizewski, 2010).

AT offers individuals a way in which to exert calming effects on their mind and

body. A meta-analysis by Stetter & Kupper (2002) on 60 randomized controlled trials

found main effects in the range of medium-to-large effect sizes for pre-post comparisons

of disease specific AT-effects. They also found that unspecific AT-effects, such as

mood, cognitive performance, physiological variables, and quality of life, were larger

than main effects. Analyzing study populations separately, they found positive effects in

the medium effect size range for migraines, hypertension, coronary heart disease, pain

disorder, anxiety disorders, mild-to-moderate depression, and functional sleep disorders.

However, they also observed that AT had not been compared with cognitive or exposure

therapy or psychopharmacological treatments for depression and anxiety, and that

comparison with medical treatments were rare. In sum, Stetter & Kupper (2002) stated

that AT results in medium-to-large clinical main effects that are stable at follow-up, but

further comparison to other efficacious treatments are needed.

In a randomized controlled trial conducted by Kanji, White, & Ernst (2004a)

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 17

researchers observed the effectiveness of AT in reducing anxiety in a group of nursing

students. The study investigated the differential effects interventions (e.g., AT, attention

control group, time control group) have on anxiety, burnout, and cardiovascular

parameters. They found a greater reduction of state and trait anxiety in the AT group

than in other groups immediately after treatment but no differences between groups for

burnout. They also found that the AT group had the greatest reduction immediately after

treatment in systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and heart rate. In regards to

depressive symptoms, Krampen (1999) compared AT with individual psychotherapy and

delayed treatment in 55 adults with depressive disorders. Subjects were randomized to

one of three groups: individual psychotherapy alone, AT and individual psychotherapy,

and waitlist control. In long-term follow-up (3 years), results showed that AT in

combination with individual psychotherapy resulted in more positive effects than

psychotherapy alone, such as lower rates of relapse and treatment reentry, and reductions

in depressive and somatic symptoms. In another study with 134 adults with minor,

subclinical depressive symptoms, Farne and Gnugnoli (2000) found that AT improved

depressive mood after three months significantly more than in the control group. These

results demonstrate that AT may be a beneficial intervention for patients suffering from

minor to moderate psychological pathology.

Studies have also found AT to have positive effects in medical patients with high

blood pressure, early stage cancer, irritable bowel syndrome, migraines, and acute

anxiety, (Aivazyan, Zaitsev, & Yurenev, 1988; Herbert & Gutman, 1983; Hidderley &

Holt, 2004; Kanji, White, & Ernst, 2004b; Kanji, White, & Ernst, 2006; Labbe, 1995;

Shinozaki et al., 2010; Ter Kuile, Spinhoven, Linssen, Zitman, & Van Dyck, 1994;

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 18

Zsombok, Juhasz, Buravari, Vitrai, & Bagdy, 2003). In regards to cardiovascular

function, Atkinson et al. (2000) found that AT showed a greater reduction immediately

after treatment in blood pressure and heart rate measures compared to a control group. In

59 patients undergoing coronary angioplasty, Kanji et al. (2004b) found significant

reductions in anxiety in AT group at two and five months post surgery. Additionally, in a

pilot study of early stage cancer patients, Hidderley and Holt (2004) found that AT

resulted in a decrease in anxiety and depression, and an increase in immune responses (as

measured by T and B cell markers). In sum, AT has shown to be a beneficial relaxation

intervention in both nonclinical and clinical populations.

As the literature shows, both Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and

Autogenic Training have been applied successfully in a variety of study populations.

However, to date, ACT and AT have yet to be applied in brief formats to investigate the

differential, and possible buffering, effects these interventions have on physiological

stress reactions to acute psychosocial stress.

Present Study

As previously mentioned, allostatic load provides a conceptual and

methodological basis for elucidating behavioral and physiological mechanisms of stress.

It offers a mechanistic framework for investigating how genes, early life experiences,

current environments, interpersonal relationships, and lifestyle factors join to affect the

structure and function of the human body. Most importantly, it offers a basis for

understanding the etiology of systemic illnesses. Allostatic load allows one to consider

the multiple pathways of cause and effect with consideration of interactions between

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 19

organ systems, for example, the link between cardiovascular disease and anxiety and

depressive symptoms (Juster et al., 2011; McEwen, 2000).

The primary aim of the current study was to investigate physiological stress

reactions to an acute psychosocial stressor and the possible buffering effects brief

interventions have on these reactions. To address this aim, participants were randomized

to one of three intervention conditions (Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT),

Autogenic Training (AT), or Attention-only Control condition) prior to undergoing an

acute psychosocial stress protocol (Trier Social Stress Test; TSST).

In addition, for clinical assessment and intervention purposes, we believed it was

highly warranted to investigate how certain psychological symptoms (e.g., anxiety and

depression) influence acute physiological stress reactions and if they moderate the

treatment response. To address these exploratory aims, participants were asked to

complete a variety of psychosocial measures, including measures of anxiety and

depression, prior to the TSST.

We hypothesized that brief interventions (ACT and AT) would have differential

effects on physiological stress reactions. We believed that the experimental conditions

(ACT and AT) would buffer the stress response and result in lower reactivity and greater

recovery following the TSST. More specifically, we thought Autogenic Training would

be superior to the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy condition, and that both would

be superior to the Control Condition. We also hypothesized that anxiety (social anxiety,

trait anxiety, anxiety sensitivity) and depressive symptoms would impact the

physiological stress response in that higher symptomatology would result in greater

reactivity and lower recovery following the TSST. Lastly, we hypothesized that anxiety

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 20

and depression would moderate the treatment response.

Method

Participants

Data for the present study was collected as a part of a larger study exploring the

effects brief interventions have on subjective and objective stress reactions. Participants

were drawn from the undergraduate population at the University of Connecticut, Storrs

from Fall 2009 to Spring 2011. Participants were recruited through the Psychology 1100

and 1103 participant pool for which they registered to gain experience regarding the

experimental method while earning credits as unpaid participants in psychological

research. For inclusion, participants were between 18-25 years of age and were able to

provide informed consent. There were no additional inclusion or exclusion criteria. A

total of 123 students consented and data was collected on all of them. The average age

was 19.08 (1.02) years; 66.67% were women, and 67.5% were White.

Procedure

The Institutional Review Board at the University of Connecticut approved the

study, which was a randomized controlled trial utilizing a repeated measures cohort

design. Participation involved one, three-hour visit to the lab. The study was broken

down into three main assessment points (see Appendix A):

Time 1 lasted approximately 75 minutes. Upon arrival to the lab, informed

consent was obtained. Following this process, participants completed a Demographic

and Medical History Questionnaire, along with baseline physiological (heart rate and

blood pressure) and psychological assessments (Anxiety Sensitivity Index, Beck

Depression Inventory-II, Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale, Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 21

Inventory- Trait Form). Next participants were randomized to one of three conditions.

The first condition involved an Acceptance and Commitment Therapy intervention, the

second condition involved an Autogenic Training intervention, and the third condition

was an Attention-only Control group (described in further detail below). All interventions

lasted twenty minutes.

Time 2 lasted approximately 45 minutes. It began immediately after the

intervention with a second physiological assessment (heart rate and blood pressure).

Next, participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; described in further

detail below).

Time 3 lasted approximately 60 minutes. It began immediately following the

conclusion of the TSST with a third physiological assessment (heart rate and blood

pressure). Participants were then debriefed on the aims of the study. They were informed

that the recording equipment was not turned on and that their performance was not

recorded using any audiovisual equipment. They were also introduced to the two

confederates who made up the selection committee. Subjects were told that these

individuals were undergraduate research assistants who were trained to play roles.

Following the debriefing, all participants engaged in a period of rest before a final

physiological assessment (heart rate and blood pressure) was conducted. At the

conclusion of the study, participants were given time to ask questions and thanked for

their participation.

Physiological Measures

Blood pressure and heart rate were collected non-invasively utilizing a

commercially available monitor (Omron Digital Blood Pressure Cuff; model number

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 22

HEM-907XL; White & Anwar, 2001). The device was used in accordance with the

directions provided with the equipment. Participants were asked to sit comfortably and

upright in a chair while the monitor’s cuff was placed around their upper arm to

simultaneously measure blood pressure and heart rate. One reading per time point was

made.

Psychological Measures

Anxiety Sensitivity Index. The Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI; Reiss, Peterson,

Gursky, & McNally, 1986) is a 16-item self-report questionnaire that assesses fear of

anxiety sensations. Subjects rate scale items on a 5-point scale (0 = very little; 4 = very

much) to indicate to what extent they agree with statements such as: ‘It scares me when I

feel faint,’ ‘It is important for me to stay in control of my emotions,’ and ‘Other people

notice when I feel shaky.’ The scale yields one total summary score ranging from 0 to

64. Various ASI reliability studies revealed a high degree of internal consistency with

Cronbach’s alpha statistics ranging from .80 to .90. The ASI has been shown to measure

a stable personality trait, as one study reported a three-year test-retest correlation of .71

(Maller & Reiss 1992; Reiss 1991). The ASI has also shown excellent diagnostic

criterion validity. Scores have been found to be strongly associated with fearfulness,

panic attacks, and subjective anxiety during biological challenge (Reiss et al., 1986;

Reiss, Peterson, & Gursky, 1988; Peterson & Sacks 1987; Maller & Reiss 1987). In the

present study, the ASI was shown to have good internal consistency (average α = .86).

Beck Depression Inventory-II. The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II;

Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) is a 21-item self-report measure that assesses depressive

symptoms. Subjects rate scale items on a 4-point scale (0-3) to indicate which statement

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 23

in each group best describes the way they have been feeling during the past two weeks.

Items measure both cognitive (e.g., self-dislike, guilt) affective (e.g., sadness, crying),

and neurovegetative (e.g., appetite, sleep) symptoms. The scale yields one total summary

score ranging from 0-63; higher scores indicate increasing intensity of depressive

symptoms. The BDI-II is one of the most widely used depression rating scales for both

clinical and research purposes. It has demonstrated excellent internal consistency with an

alpha coefficient of .92, excellent concurrent validity, construct validity, and test-retest

reliability across studies (Beck, Steer, Ball, & Ranieri, 1996). In the present study, the

BDI-II had an average α of .33.

Demographics and Medical History Questionnaire. The Demographic and

Medical History Questionnaire was created specifically for this study. It gathers basic

demographic data, such as date of birth, and race and ethnicity, as well as medical

information necessary for analyses of physiological data. Questions include diagnoses of

mental and/or physical illness, medication(s), weight and height, substance use, and

exercise.

Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale. The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS;

Liebowitz, 1987) is a 24-item self-report questionnaire that measures respondents’ fear

and avoidance of social situations, such as social interactions (e.g., going to a party,

meeting strangers) and public performances (e.g., giving a prepared oral talk, speaking up

at a meeting). Each item is rated on two 4-point scales, the first measures fear/anxiety

and the second measures avoidance. Scores range from 0 (no fear/never avoid) to 3

(severe fear/usually avoid). The scale yields one total summary score ranging from 0-

144; higher scores indicate increasing intensity of social anxiety. Total scores have been

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 24

shown to adequately differentiate between individuals with and without social anxiety

disorder (i.e., social phobia). The LSAS has demonstrated adequate reliability and

validity. The scale has shown excellent internal consistency with an alpha coefficient of

.95. It also has good test-retest reliability, as well as good discriminant and convergent

validity (Heimberg et al., 1999; Fresco et al., 2001; Baker et al., 2002). The LSAS was

shown to have good internal consistency in the present study (average α = .85).

Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. The Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety

Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970) is a self-report questionnaire

that consists of two 20-items subscales. The state subscale (“state anxiety”) assesses

momentary anxiety and includes statements such as ‘I feel secure,’ ‘I am jittery,’ and ‘I

feel confused.’ The trait subscale (“trait anxiety”) assesses the stable tendency to

experience anxiety and includes statements such as ‘I lack self-confidence,’ ‘I feel

nervous and restless,’ and ‘I feel rested.’ For the state subscale, subjects are asked to

answer based on how they feel at that present moment. For the trait subscale, subjects are

asked to answer based on how they generally feel. Scores for individual items range

from 1 (not at all/almost never) to 4 (very much so/almost always), with total scale scores

ranging from 20-80. Higher scores indicate higher degrees of anxiety. The STAI is one

of the most frequently used self-report measures of state and trait anxiety with adequate

reliability and validity. The trait subscale has shown good internal consistency (median

alpha coefficient .90) and adequate test-retest reliability (ranging from .73 to .86). The

state subscale has shown good internal consistency as well (median alpha coefficient .93)

and expected low test-retest reliability given it assesses momentary anxiety (scores

ranging from .16 to .62; Spielberger, 1983). In this study, we only utilized the trait

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 25

subscale (STAI- Trait), which was shown to have excellent internal consistency in the

present study (average α = .92).

Experimental and Control Conditions

ACT intervention. This experimental condition involved exercises drawn from

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). This

condition involved the researcher explaining and defining the main principles of ACT

(e.g., experiential avoidance and cognitive fusion). It also utilized time to teach strategies

for coping with anxiety (e.g., acceptance and defusion techniques) and involved

experiential exercises (e.g., envisioning emotions as waves and observing and labeling

thoughts).

AT intervention. This experimental condition involved Autogenic Training (AT;

Schultz, 1932), which is a self-relaxation procedure. This condition involved the

researcher offering a rationale for Autogenic Training. The researcher than lead

participants through a series of statements, which facilitated a passive concentration on

bodily perceptions (e.g., heaviness in the limbs, feelings of warmth) aimed at relieving

tension and stress.

Attention-only Control. This control condition involved an attention-only task.

The researcher read aloud from a fictional book called, “Journey Under the Sea,” which

is a book from “Choose Your Own Adventure” series (Published by Chooseco LLC).

These books are interactive and ask readers to make decisions throughout the story. The

participants were involved in giving their input in choosing the various story lines,

therefore ensuring engagement in the task.

Challenge Task

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 26

Trier Social Stress Test protocol. As previously described, the Trier Social

Stress Test (TSST; Nater et al., 2005) is a widely utilized standardized protocol for the

induction of psychological stress in a laboratory setting. In this study, the TSST began

with the researcher leading the participant to a different room, where they were

introduced to two confederates sitting at a table (trained undergraduate research assistants

posing as a selection committee). In the room, there was a podium where the participant

was instructed to stand, along with a video camera and a tape recorder. The researcher

explained the task they were about to perform which involved assuming the role of a job

applicant who is invited for an interview. Participants were told that after having a few

minutes to prepare, they would be asked to give a 5-minute speech about why they are

the best person for the job position. Participants were told that the selection committee

was specially trained to monitor nonverbal behavior, that a video recording of their

speech would be conducted, and that a voice frequency analysis of nonverbal behavior

would be performed at a later date. After the instructions were read, participants were led

back to room A, where they were given three minutes to prepare a speech. After their

time was up, they were led back into room B where they were asked to deliver their

speech. After five minutes, the committee interrupted the participant and asked him/her

to serially subtract the number 13 from 1,022 as fast and as accurately as possible. On

every failure the participant had to restart at 1,022 with one member of the committee

interfering with, “Stop – mistake – start over at 1,022, please.” The participant was given

five minutes to complete this task before being led back to room A for further assessment

and debriefing.

Data Analytic Plan

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 27

All data analyses were performed using SPSS version 19.0 (SPSS, Inc., 2010).

We first conducted exploratory data analyses to assess demographic composition of the

sample and to determine whether variables of interest met assumptions of normality.

Bivariate correlation matrixes were computed using data from the Demographic and

Medical History Questionnaire to determine whether potential covariates should be

controlled for in analyses. ANOVA’s were conducted to assess baseline differences

between measures. Where appropriate, Tukey’s HSD post hoc comparisons were used

for a posteriori comparisons among means. Three participants were excluded: one

participant was excluded based on age, and two others did not complete the study in its

entirety due to the severity of their baseline psychological symptoms and the moderately

stressful nature of the TSST.

To test whether brief interventions have differential effects on physiological stress

reactions over time, 3 (experimental condition) x 4 (time) repeated measures ANOVAs

and ANCOVAs were conducted for heart rate, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic

blood pressure. To test whether depression and/or anxiety moderate the treatment

response, ANCOVAs were conducted utilizing total scores on the ASI, BDI-II, LSAS,

and STAI-Trait as moderator variables, entered as covariates into analyses. To explain

significant three-way interactions, ANCOVAs were conducted to describe depression and

anxiety effects for each treatment group separately. ANCOVAs were also conducted to

demonstrate intervention effects for low and high anxiety and depression separately.

Median splits were utilized to dichotomize psychosocial variables in these analyses.

Reactivity scores were calculated as the change between time 2 and time 3 cardiovascular

measures. Recovery scores were calculated as change scores between time 3 and time 4.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 28

Parameter estimates were used to graph 3-way interactions with continuous variables.

This allowed us to best graphically illustrate psychological indices as moderator variables

in a nonclinical sample; high and low BDI-II, STAI-Trait, ASI, and LSAS variables were

defined as follows: high = 1 SD above the mean, low = 1 SD below the mean.

All analyses were first run with four time points including baseline cardiovascular

measures (time 1, time 2, time 3, time 4). Separate analyses were then run on only three

time points (time 2, time 3, time 4) with baseline cardiovascular values as covariates to

control for baseline effects on reactivity and recovery. Next, a series of analyses were

conducted to determine the influence of potentially confounding factors on baseline

cardiovascular measures. Any demographic variable associated with baseline HR, SBP,

or DBP at p ≤ .1 was considered a potential confound, and thus, entered as a covariate in

relevant analyses. In sum, bivariate correlations revealed that exercise was a potential

confound for baseline HR; drug use, alcohol consumption, gender, and BMI were

potential confounds for baseline SBP; and drug use, smoking, and BMI were potential

confounds for DBP. Experimenter and time of day were also used as covariates to

control for between group differences observed in baseline cardiovascular measures.

Analyses were finally run with baseline cardiovascular values and the above-mentioned

relevant physiological covariates. Reported results were corrected by Greenhouse-

Geisser procedure where appropriate (violation of sphericity assumption), indicated by

uneven degrees of freedom (Greenhouse and Geisser, 1959; Vasey and Thayer, 1987).

Throughout, partial eta2 is reported as a measure of effect size.

Lastly, in order to investigate whether depressive and/or anxiety symptoms are

associated with greater reactivity and lower recovery following the TSST, partial

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 29

correlations were conducted. Intervention group was entered as covariate in all analyses

to control for possible intervention effects. In our analyses, power ranged from 0.4 to 1.0.

Results

Participant Characteristics

Demographic characteristics by group and gender of those included in the

analyses are presented in Tables 1-2. Participants included 120 college undergraduate

students. The average age of participants was 19.08 years (SD = 1.02). Two-thirds were

female (n = 80). All participants reported race/ethnicity as follows: 67.5% White, Non-

Hispanic; 12.5% Black/African American; 11.7% Asian; 6.6% Hispanic/Latino;

Biracial/Multiracial 1.6%. Seventeen individuals (14.2%) reported a mental health

diagnosis (e.g., depression, anxiety) and nine individuals (7.5%) reported a chronic

medical condition (e.g., asthma, diabetes). Thirty-three individuals (27.5%) reported

daily medication use (e.g., birth control, psychiatric medications). Over half the sample

(57.5%) reported alcohol consumption, 5% reported cigarette smoking, and 13.3%

engaged in illicit drug use (e.g., marijuana). Average body mass index was 23.44 kg/m2

(SD = 4.02) and 55% reported engagement in regular physical activity.

Descriptive Statistics for Study Measures

Descriptive statistics for all physiological measures at each time point by group

are detailed in Table 3 and for each time point by gender in Table 4. Statistics for

psychological measures are also included. Baseline statistics are reported below.

Baseline physiological measures. Mean baseline heart rate (bpm) for all

participants (n = 120) was 76.47 bpm (SD = 14.72). ANOVAs were conducted to

determine significant between group differences in baseline heart rate. A significant

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 30

difference (p ≤ 0.05) was found, such that the AT intervention group (mean = 81.78 bpm)

had a higher mean baseline heart rate compared to the ACT (mean = 73.60 bpm) and

Control (mean = 74.03 bpm) groups.

Mean baseline systolic blood pressure (mmHg) for all participants (N = 120) was

126.11 mmHg (SD = 14.60). ANOVAs were conducted to determine significant between

group differences in baseline systolic blood pressure. No differences between

intervention groups were found.

Mean baseline diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) for all participants (n = 120) was

72.97 mmHg (SD = 8.90). ANOVAs were conducted to determine significant

differences between intervention groups in baseline systolic blood pressure. A significant

group difference (p ≤ 0.05) was found between the AT (mean DBP was 76.43 mmHg, SD

= 8.57) and Control (mean DBP was 69.45 mmHg, SD = 8.76) groups.

Psychological measures. All psychological measures were administered once at

baseline. ANOVAs were conducted and no group differences were found among any of

the psychological measures. Mean score for the BDI-II was 8.34 (SD = 6.87), for the

LSAS was 35.50 (SD = 21.60), for the STAI-Trait was 38.88 (SD = 10.67), and for the

ASI was 19.65 (SD = 9.98).

Hypothesis 1: Impact of Brief Clinical Interventions

For heart rate (HR), repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a main effect of time

(F (2.30, 269.55) = 59.58, p ≤ .001, η2

= .34) as well as a time by group interaction (F

(4.61, 269.55) = 3.82, p = .003, η2

= .06). Post hoc tests indicate a group differences in

HR at baseline among AT and ACT (p = .032), and AT and Control groups (p = .045),

but at no other time point. Thus, after controlling for baseline HR, repeated measures

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 31

ANCOVAs revealed no effects. However, once relevant covariates (exercise, time of

day, experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline HR, a main effect of time

reemerged (F (1.91, 216.08) = 3.25, p = .04, η2

= .03), demonstrating a significant change

in HR over time.

For systolic blood pressure (SBP), repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a main

effect of time (F (2.88, 337.22) = 53.08, p ≤ .001, η2

= .31). Repeated measures

ANCOVAs, with baseline SBP as a covariate, revealed no main or interaction effects.

Similarly, controlling for relevant covariates (drug use, alcohol, gender, BMI, time of

day, experimenter) along with baseline SBP did not reveal any significant effects.

For diastolic blood pressure (DBP), repeated measures ANOVAs revealed a main

effect of time (F (2.9, 338.72) = 43.86, p ≤ .001, η2

= .27) as well as a time by group

interaction (F (5.79, 338.72) = 2.39, p = .03, η2

= .04). Post hoc tests indicate a group

differences in DBP at baseline among the AT and Control groups (p = .001). Controlling

for baseline DBP revealed no main or interaction effects. Additionally, controlling for

relevant covariates (drug use, smoker, BMI, time of day, experimenter) along with

baseline DBP did not reveal any significant effects.

Hypothesis 1a: Depressive and/or Anxiety Symptoms as Treatment Moderators

Depression (Beck Depression Inventory-II). For HR, repeated measures

ANCOVAs, with total BDI-II score entered as a covariate to test for moderation, revealed

a main effect of time (F (2.27, 258.18) = 20.10, p ≤ 0.001, η2

= .15) as well as a time by

group interaction (F (4.53, 258.18) = 2.47, p = .04, η2

= .04). Post hoc tests indicate a

group differences in HR at baseline among AT and ACT (p = .032), and AT and Control

groups (p = .045), but no group differences at any other time point. Thus, controlling for

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 32

baseline HR revealed no main or interaction effects. However, once relevant covariates

(exercise, time of day, experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline HR, a main

effect of time reemerged (F (1.89, 208.20) = 3.60, p = .031, η2

= .03), demonstrating a

significant change in HR over time.

For SBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total BDI-II score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.82, 321.99) = 25.77,

p ≤ 0.001, η2

= .18) as well as a 3-way time by group by depression interaction (F (5.65,

321.99) = 2.24 p = .043, η2

= .04), which remained after controlling for baseline SBP (F

(3.8, 214.4) = 2.46, p = .05, η2

= .04); see Figures 1-5. The 3-way interaction

demonstrates that across time points, SBP is affected differently among intervention

groups depending on level of depression. In the AT group, subjects with lower levels of

depressive symptomatology displayed greater SBP reactivity to the TSST compared to

those with higher levels of symptomatology (F (1, 37) = 4.72, p = .036, η2

= .11); see

Figure 1. There was neither a significant difference in SBP reactivity or recovery

between levels of depression in either the ACT or Control conditions nor a significant

difference in SBP between groups at a given level of depression at any time point (p’s >

.1); See Figures 2-5. Once relevant covariates (drug use, alcohol, gender, BMI, time of

day, experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline SBP, the 3-way interaction

was no longer significant (p = .06).

For DBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total BDI-II score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.9, 330.07) = 19.56, p

≤ 0.001, η2

= .15) as well as a time by group interaction (F (5.79, 330.07) = 73.09, p =

.047, η2

= .02). Post hoc tests indicate a group difference in DBP at baseline among the

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 33

AT and Control groups (p = .001). Once baseline DBP was controlled for, no main or

interaction effects were observed. Similarly, controlling for relevant covariates (drug

use, smoker, BMI, time of day, experimenter), along with baseline DBP, did not result in

any significant effects.

Anxiety Sensitivity (Anxiety Sensitivity Index). For HR, repeated measures

ANCOVAs, with total ASI score entered as a covariate to test for moderation, revealed a

main effect of time (F (2.28, 259.3) = 5.16, p = .004, η2

= .04). Controlling for baseline

HR revealed no main or interaction effects. However, once relevant covariates (exercise,

time of day, experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline HR, a main effect of

time reemerged (F (1.9, 208.64) = 3.13, p = .048, η2

= .03), indicating a significant

change in HR over time.

For SBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total ASI score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.86, 326.06) = 16.96,

p ≤ 0.001, η2

= .13). Once baseline SBP was added as a covariate, however, no main or

interaction effects were observed. Similarly, controlling for relevant covariates (drug

use, alcohol, gender, BMI, time of day, experimenter), along with baseline SBP, did not

result in any significant effects.

For DBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total ASI score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.89, 329.12) = 7.54, p

≤ 0.001, η2

= .06). Once baseline DBP was controlled for, however, no main or

interaction effects were observed. Similarly, controlling for relevant covariates (drug

use, smoker, BMI, time of day, experimenter), along with baseline DBP, did not result in

any significant effects.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 34

Social Anxiety (Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale). For HR, repeated measures

ANCOVAs, with total LSAS score entered as a covariate to test for moderation, revealed

a main effect of time (F (2.3, 255.01) = 9.18, p ≤ 0.001, η2

= .08). Controlling for baseline

HR revealed no main or interaction effects. However, once relevant covariates (exercise,

time of day, experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline HR, a main effect of

time reemerged (F (1.92, 205.01) = 5.02, p = .008, η2

= .05), indicating a significant

change in HR over time.

For SBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total LSAS score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.9, 322.33) = 10.39, p

≤ 0.001, η2

= .09) as well as a 3-way time by group by social anxiety interaction (F (5.81,

322.33) = 2.68, p = .016, η2

= .05). This 3-way interaction remained after controlling for

baseline SBP (F (3.91, 214.86) = 3.46 p = .01, η2

= .06) and after controlling for relevant

covariates (drug use, alcohol, gender, BMI, time of day, experimenter) along with

baseline SBP (F (3.9, 200.77) = 3.8 p = .006, η2

= .07); See Figures 6-10. The 3-way

interaction demonstrates that across time points, SBP is affected differently among

intervention groups depending on level of social anxiety. In the AT group, individuals

with lower levels of social anxiety symptomatology recovered less after the TSST than

those with higher levels of symptomatology (F (1, 37) = 6.17, p = .018, η2

= .14); see

Figure 6. At time 4, there was a significant difference in SBP between levels of social

anxiety symptomatology in the AT group (F (1, 37) = 5.13, p = .029, η2

= .12); see Figure

6. There was neither a significant difference in SBP reactivity or recovery between levels

of social anxiety in either the ACT or Control conditions nor a significant difference in

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 35

SBP between groups at a given level of social anxiety at any time point (p’s > .1); see

Figures 7-10.

For DBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total LSAS score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.88, 320.10) = 10.0, p

≤ 0.001, η2

= .08) as well as a 3-way time by group by social anxiety interaction (F (5.77,

320.1) = 2.34, p = .034, η2

= .04). This 3-way interaction remained after controlling for

baseline DBP (F (3.92, 215.75) = 2.7, p= .032, η2

= .05); see Figures 11-15. The 3-way

interaction demonstrates that across time points, DBP is affected differently among

intervention groups depending on level of social anxiety. In the AT group, individuals

with lower levels of social anxiety symptomatology recovered less after the TSST than

those with higher levels of symptomatology (F (1, 37) = 4.38, p = .043, η2

= .11); see

Figure 11. There was neither a significant difference in DBP reactivity or recovery

between levels of social anxiety in either the ACT or Control conditions nor a significant

difference in DBP between groups at a given level of social anxiety at any time point (p’s

> .1); see Figures 12-15. Once relevant covariates (drug use, smoker, BMI, time of day,

experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline DBP, the 3-way interaction was no

longer significant (p = .056).

Trait Anxiety (Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Inventory- Trait). For HR,

repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total STAI-Trait score entered as a covariate to test

for moderation, revealed no main or interaction effects. Similarly, controlling for

baseline HR, no main or interaction effects were observed. However, once relevant

covariates (exercise, time of day, experimenter) were controlled for along with baseline

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 36

HR, a main effect of time was emerged (F (1.91, 209.54) = 3.34, p = .039, η2

= .03),

indicating a significant change in HR over time.

For SBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total STAI-Trait score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.85, 324.61) = 5.71, p

= .001, η2

= .05). Once baseline SBP was added as a covariate, however, no main or

interaction effects were observed. Similarly, controlling for relevant covariates (drug

use, alcohol, gender, BMI, time of day, experimenter), along with baseline SBP did not

result in any significant effects.

For DBP, repeated measures ANCOVAs, with total STAI-Trait score entered as a

covariate to test for moderation, revealed a main effect of time (F (2.88, 328.36) = 4.17, p

= .007, η2

= .04). Once baseline DBP was controlled for, however, no main or interaction

effects were observed. Similarly, controlling for relevant covariates (drug use, smoker,

BMI, time of day, experimenter), along with baseline DBP and STAI-Trait did not result

in any significant effects.

Hypothesis 1b: Association of Depressive and Anxiety Symptoms with

Cardiovascular Reactivity and Recovery

Partial correlations, with group as covariate, revealed no associations between

depression (as measured by total score on the BDI-II) and HR, SBP, or DBP reactivity or

recovery. Additionally, no associations were found between social anxiety (as measured

by total score on the LSAS) or trait anxiety (as measured by total score on the STAI-

Trait) and HR, SBP, or DBP reactivity or recovery. However, HR reactivity was found

to be positively associated with anxiety sensitivity (as measured by total score on the

ASI) (pr = .21, p = .027). No associations were observed between HR recovery and ASI.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 37

Discussion

Purpose of the Present Study

Stress is a psychosocial risk factor that has long been linked to medical illness and

various psychopathologies (Kessler, 1997; McEwen, 2003; Nugent et al., 2011). While it

has been well established that behavioral factors (e.g., diet, exercise, substance use) play

a critical role in the onset and maintenance of stress-related illness, the role of internal

biological mechanisms of stress are less understood (Luthar, 2003; McEwen, 2000). As

discussed throughout the introduction, the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load offer a

useful framework for investigating the psychobiological mechanisms affected by stress,

particularly those involving the cardiovascular system (McEwen & Stellar 1993; Sterling

& Eyer, 1988).

In our study we set out to examine cardiovascular stress reactions to an acute

psychosocial stressor and were particularly interested in investigating whether brief

psychosocial interventions would attenuate these reactions. As previously mentioned,

although responses to stressors in a laboratory setting may not be of great clinical

importance, they indicate how a person responds to stressful demands in their everyday

life and offer a useful way to examine disturbed stress responses that may have

pathological consequences (Chida & Hamer, 2008; Salomon et al., 2009). Additionally,

laboratory settings provide a controlled environment whereby direct physiological

outcomes to clinical interventions can be validly acquired. Another aim of our study was

to explore how certain psychological symptoms (e.g., anxiety and depressive

symptomatology) influence acute physiological stress reactions and whether these

symptoms moderate the treatment response to brief interventions. Given the current

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 38

discrepancy among the literature in regards to how psychosocial factors affect the stress

response, we believe this aim offers important implications for clinical assessment and

intervention.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the effects brief clinical

interventions have on cardiovascular reactions (e.g., heart rate, systolic blood pressure,

diastolic blood pressure) to an acute psychosocial stress paradigm, and whether common

psychosocial factors (e.g., anxiety and depression) moderate the treatment response.

Hypothesis 1

In our study, participants were randomized to one of three intervention conditions.

The first experimental condition was developed based on Acceptance and Commitment

Therapy (ACT), a therapeutic approach that focuses on increasing one’s flexibility and

willingness to accept stressful and anxiety provoking events and feelings (Hayes et al.,

1999). ACT has proven empirical evidence for its effectiveness in treating a variety of

psychopathologies, including depression and anxiety, and has been shown to affect

positive cardiovascular outcomes (Cambell-Sills et al., 2008; Forman et al., 2007; Hayes

et al., 2004; Low et al., 2008; Woods et al., 2006; Zettle, 2003; Zettle & Hayes, 1986;

Zettle & Rains, 1989).

The second experimental condition was Autogenic Training (AT), a self-

relaxation procedure that involves a series of statements aimed at reducing autonomic

arousal (Linden, 1990). AT has shown clinical main effects in the range of medium-to-

large effect sizes in both nonclinical and clinical populations. For example, a number of

studies have shown AT to be a beneficial for the reduction of anxiety and depressive

symptoms, as well as a variety of cardiovascular outcomes, such as heart rate and blood

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 39

pressure (Kanji, 2004a; Kanji, 2004b; Krampen, 1999, Farne & Gnugnoli, 2000; Stetter

& Kupper, 2002). The third condition, an Attention-only Control group, involved joining

with a research assistant to read an interactive adventure book.

Based on literature demonstrating the positive benefits relaxation and acceptance

techniques have on health outcomes, we hypothesized that brief interventions would have

differential effects on heart rate, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood pressure

following a psychosocial stress paradigm. Specifically, we thought the ACT and AT

experimental conditions would attenuate the stress response, resulting in positive

cardiovascular outcomes (e.g., lower reactivity and greater recovery following the TSST).

Further, because we were measuring acute physiological outcomes, we hypothesized that

the AT condition, being a somatic relaxation technique, would be superior to the ACT

condition, and that both experimental conditions would be superior to the Attention-only

Control condition. In support of our hypothesis, results showed a time by group

interaction for heart rate and diastolic blood pressure, indicating that brief interventions

differentially affected heart rate and diastolic blood pressure over time. However, after

controlling for relevant covariates, particularly baseline differences in heart rate and

diastolic blood pressure, these effects were no longer evident. Additionally, contrary to

our hypothesis, we did not observe any effects on systolic blood pressure.

One way of interpreting our results is to suggest that the original time by group

interactions for both heart rate and diastolic blood pressure were driven by baseline

differences between groups. However, another interpretation is that intervention effects,

which could no longer be measured after controlling for baseline time points, drove our

results. For example, in regards to heart rate outcomes, there was a greater reduction in

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 40

heart rate from baseline to pre-TSST in the AT group compared to the ACT and Control

groups. This suggests that there was a greater intervention effect for the AT group

compared to the other groups. However, because we had to control for baseline

differences in subsequent analyses, and therefore remove time 1, the intervention effect

could no longer be displayed. Therefore, it may be that the observed time by group

interaction was in fact due to the AT intervention effect rather than baseline differences

in heart rate.

In regards to diastolic blood pressure, the same interpretations must be

considered. Again, it may be that the group by time interaction was driven by baseline

differences in diastolic blood pressure. However, another possibility is that intervention

effects, which could no longer be measured after controlling for baseline time points,

drove our results. On further examination, the AT group again showed a significant

intervention effect compared to the ACT and Control groups, which was evidenced by a

lowering of diastolic blood pressure from baseline to pre-TSST. However, because we

had to control for baseline differences in subsequent analyses, and therefore remove time

1, this effect could no longer be demonstrated. Therefore, as with heart rate, it may be

that the intervention effects of the AT group rather than baseline differences in diastolic

blood pressure drove the observed time by group interaction.

Hypothesis 1a

Our second hypothesis was that anxiety and depressive symptomatology would

moderate the treatment response. This hypothesis was more exploratory in nature since

there is no literature, to our knowledge, investigating whether psychosocial factors

moderate cardiovascular outcomes to brief clinical interventions.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 41

We hypothesized that anxiety and depression levels would moderate the treatment

response to brief clinical interventions. In regards to depressive symptomatology and

systolic blood pressure, our hypotheses were supported. Results showed a significant

three-way interaction indicating that across time points, SBP was affected differently

among intervention groups depending on level of depression. Within the Autogenic

Training group, subjects with higher levels of depressive symptomatology had lower SBP

reactivity to the TSST than those with less symptomatology. There were no significant

differences between levels of depression in SBP reactivity, recovery, or across any time

points within either the ACT or Control conditions. This finding provides evidence that

AT may be most beneficial for individuals with higher levels of depression. Once

relevant covariates were controlled for, the three-way interaction was no longer

significant (p = .06).

In regards to social anxiety symptomatology and systolic and diastolic blood

pressure, our hypotheses were also supported. Results showed a significant three-way

interaction indicating that across time points, after controlling for all relevant covariates,

SBP was affected differently among intervention groups depending on level of social

anxiety. Within the AT group, subjects with higher levels of social anxiety

symptomatology had greater SBP recovery following the TSST than those with less

symptomatology. Additionally, within the AT group, subjects with higher levels of

social anxiety symptomatology had significantly lower SBP than those with less

symptomatology at time 4. There were no significant differences between levels of social

anxiety in SBP reactivity, recovery, or across any time points within either the

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy or Control conditions. Overall, this finding

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 42

provides evidence that AT may be most beneficial for individuals with higher levels of

social anxiety.

Additionally, results showed a significant three-way interaction indicating that

across time points, DBP is affected differently among intervention groups depending on

level of social anxiety. This finding held after controlling for baseline differences in

diastolic blood pressure. Within the AT group, subjects with higher levels of social

anxiety symptomatology had greater DBP recovery following the TSST than those with

less symptomatology. There were no significant differences between levels of social

anxiety in DBP reactivity, recovery, or across any time points within either the ACT or

Control conditions. This finding also provides evidence that AT may be beneficial for

individuals with higher levels of social anxiety. However, once other relevant covariates

were controlled for along with baseline DBP, the three-way interaction was no longer

significant (p = .056).

In sum, our results show that depressive and social anxiety symptomatology

moderated the effectiveness of our brief clinical interventions, as measured by

cardiovascular outcomes. Analyzing these findings together it is evident that subjects

with greater levels of social anxiety and those with greater levels of depressive symptoms

benefited more from the Autogenic Training intervention than those with lower levels. It

is an interesting finding that individuals with higher levels of psychopathology showed

greater cardiovascular benefits from AT. Because the intervention was brief, and

therefore less potent than if it had been practiced on several occasions, some may have

expected individuals with lower levels of psychopathology to benefit the most from it.

One interpretation for our findings is that nonclinical subjects with greater social

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 43

anxiety (and depression) were more motivated to participate in the intervention, and thus,

learned and benefited more from it, thereby affecting cardiovascular outcomes. There is

a vast literature on the importance of motivation for the acquisition and transfer of

techniques and skills taught in psychosocial interventions (see Ryan et al., 2011 for a

review). In fact, motivation has been found to be so important for learning and transfer

that an empirically supported directive therapeutic style, Motivational Interviewing, has

been developed to focus on helping patients increase it (Miller, 1983; Miller & Rollnick,

1991; Miller & Rollnick, 2002; Hettema, Steele, & Miller, 2005). Based on motivational

theories (e.g., Self-Determination Theory; Deci & Ryan, 1985), it is reasonable that

individuals with greater intrinsic motivation, perhaps those with more symptoms of

psychopathology, would be more likely to fully engage in a brief version of Autogenic

Training, which is directly aimed at reducing the symptoms they are experiencing.

Hypothesis 1b

As discussed in the introduction, current research has shown mixed results for the

effects depression and anxiety have on physiological stress reactions. For example,

studies investigating physiological responses to acute psychosocial stressors have

suggested that depression and anxiety are related to enhanced cardiovascular reactivity,

as evidenced by increased blood pressure and heart rate (de Rooij et al., 2010; Kibler &

Ma, 2004; Mausbach et al., 2005). However, other researchers have found negative

associations implicating a hyporeactivity to acute psychosocial stress (Carroll et al.,

2007; Jezova et al., 2004; Salomon et al., 2009). In addition, it is important to note that

there is a lack of research investigating the relationship between depressive and anxiety

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 44

symptomatology and cardiovascular recovery, rather most studies concentrate on

reactivity.

Our study design offered an opportunity for us to add to the literature regarding

the possible associations between certain key psychosocial factors and cardiovascular

reactivity and recovery. Based on prior research, we hypothesized that after controlling

for treatment group, anxiety and depressive symptomatology would be associated with

greater cardiovascular reactivity and lower recovery following the TSST. Our hypothesis

was partially supported in that heart rate reactivity was found to be positively associated

with anxiety sensitivity (as measured by total score on the ASI). However, no additional

measures were associated with any other cardiovascular parameters.

In our study, mean scores for all four measures (BDI-II, ASI, LSAS, STAI-Trait)

were all in the nonclinical range. Therefore, one interpretation of these results is that

certain associations were not observed because our study included a nonclinical

population that displayed subthreshold symptomatology. Our results imply that lower

levels of psychopathology (i.e., a few symptoms of anxiety or depression), although

meaningful, may not affect physiological parameters as drastically as more severe

pathology.

In further discussion of our results, we did not specifically study individual

differences in regards to psychopathology. For example, a biological predisposition to

certain disorders may engender more physiological effects. Additionally, certain

symptoms of anxiety and depression may elicit cardiovascular responses (e.g.,

somatic/physical symptoms) more than others (e.g., cognitive/affective symptoms). It is

also reasonable to ascertain that age of onset and length of time experiencing certain

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 45

pathology may have more or less of an effect on physiological parameters.

Unfortunately, because we did not directly study these types of individual differences, we

were not able to test these possibilities. Therefore, one important implication of our

study is that direct consideration of individual differences when investigating

physiological outcomes is imperative.

Limitations

Although there are many strengths of this study, such as its novelty, there are

some important limitations to mention. First, although our subjects were randomly

assigned to groups, there were still baseline differences in two of three cardiovascular

parameters. This indicates that our baseline data might represent anticipation stress

rather than a true baseline condition. Therefore, future research utilizing a similar study

design should take baseline anticipation stress into consideration by adequately

measuring and treating it as a confounding factor. Second, we did not measure

cardiovascular parameters during the TSST; rather we assessed reactivity immediately

following the cessation of the protocol. Although this method has been utilized in

various studies, assessing cardiovascular parameters during the TSST may provide a

more valid measure of reactivity. Third, because of our sample population and sample

size of men, we were not able to investigate possible age and gender differences in

treatment response. Several studies (e.g., Kelly, Tyrka, Anderson, Price, Carpenter,

2008; Kudielka et al, 2004a) have found age and gender differences in responses to social

stress challenges. Therefore, exploring age and gender differences in treatment response

involving objective outcome measures is warranted. Lastly, we did not directly measure

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 46

motivation and treatment engagement, which can both be considered as possible

confounds decreasing intervention effectiveness.

Future Directions

There is a growing awareness of the detrimental health effects stress has on

individuals. This awareness has led to research endeavors investigating the exact

pathways linking stress and health. Just as important as elucidating these mechanisms,

there is also a need for the development and study of specific interventions to counteract

the effects of stress. Our study has specifically added to this field by showing that brief

clinical interventions involving somatic relaxation, particularly Autogenic Training, may

be beneficial to individuals undergoing acute stress. Additionally, our study

demonstrated that individuals with higher levels of depression and/or anxiety might

benefit the most from these targeted interventions.

Particularly within the field of behavioral medicine, our findings offer important

implications for assessment and treatment. Acute physiological reactivity from stressful

medical and dental procedures is common and may lead to negative outcomes (Cohen,

Fiske, & Newton, 2000; Johnsen et al., 2003; Munafo & Stevenson, 2001; Mavros et al.,

2011; Pignay-Demaria, Lesperance, Demaria, Frasure-Smith, & Perrault, 2003).

Moreover, it is well evidenced that mild and severe forms of anxiety and depressive

disorders are high in medical and dental patient populations and are considered risk

factors for disease (Demertizis & Craske 2006; Moussavi et al., 2007; Sherbourne,

Jackson, Meredith, Camp, & Wells, 1996; Stoudemire, 1996; Wittchen et al., 2002).

Targeting certain individuals more prone to poor outcomes (e.g., those suffering from

mild or severe psychopathology) and administering brief clinical interventions prior to

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 47

stressful procedures may attenuate and/or prevent negative health outcomes. Future

research investigating the effectiveness brief clinical interventions have on these

populations is not only warranted, but highly feasible.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 48

References

Aivazyan, T., Zaitsev, V., & Yurenev, A. (1988). Autogenic training in the treatment and

secondary prevention of essential hypertension: Five-year follow-up. Health

Psychology, 7(Suppl.), 201-8.

al’Absi, M., & Arnett, D. (2000). Adrenocortical responses to psychological stress and

risk for hypertension. Biomedical Pharmacotherapy, 54, 234-44.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders, (Rev. 4th ed.). Washington, DC.

Armario, P., del Rey, R., Martin-Baranera, M., Almendros, M., Ceresuela, L., & Pardell,

H. (2003). Blood pressure reactivity to mental stress task as a determinant of

sustained hypertension after 5 years of follow-up. Journal of Human

Hypertension, 17, 181-6.

Bach, P., & Hayes, S. (2002). The use of acceptance and commitment therapy to prevent

the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: A randomized controlled trial. Journal

of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1129-39.

Baker, S., Heinrichs, N., Kim, H., & Hofman, S. (2002). The Liebowitz social anxiety

scale as a self-report instrument: A preliminary psychometric analysis. Behavior

Research and Therapy, 40, 701-15.

Beck, A., Steer, R., & Brown, G. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory (2nd

edition). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Beck, A., Steer, R., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. (1996). Comparison of Beck Depression

Inventories IA and II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 67, 588-97.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 49

Black, P., & Garbutt, L. (2002). Stress, inflammation and cardiovascular disease. Journal

of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 1-23.

Block, J. (2002). Acceptance or change of private experiences: A comparative analysis in

college students with public speaking anxiety. Doctoral dissertation. University at

Albany, State University of New York.

Bond, F., & Bunce, D. (2003). The role of acceptance and job control in mental health,

job satisfaction, and work performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 1057-

67.

Brosschot, J., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A

review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health.

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113–24.

Brotman, D., Golden, S., & Wittstein, I (2007). The cardiovascular toll of stress. Lancet,

370, 1089-100.

Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D., Brown, T., & Hofmann, S., (2006). Effects of

suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety

and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1251-63.

Carroll, D., Phillips, A., Hunt, K., & Der, G. (2007). Symptoms of depression and

cardiovascular reactions to acute psychological stress: Evidence from a

population study. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 68-74.

Chida, Y., & Hamer, M. (2008). Chronic psychosocial factors and acute physiological

responses to laboratory-induced stress in healthy populations: A quantitative

review of 30 years of investigations. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 829-85.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 50

Childs, E., Vicini, L., & De Wit, H. (2006). Responses to the Trier Social Stress Test

(TSST) in single and grouped participants. Psychophysiology, 43, 366-71.

Chrousos, G. (2009). Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews

Endocrinology, 5, 374-81.

Chrousos, G., Gold, P. (1992). The concepts of stress and stress system disorders:

Overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis. Journal of the American

Medical Association, 267, 1244-52.

Clark, M., Bond, M., & Hecker, J. (2007). Environmental stress, psychological stress and

allostatic load. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 12, 18-30.

Cohen, S., Fiske, J., & Newton, J. (2000). Behavioral Dentistry: The impact of dental

anxiety on daily living. British Dental Journal, 189, 385-90.

Cowings, P., Toscano, W., Timbers, A., Casey, C., & and Hufnagel, J. (2005). Autogenic

feedback training exercise: A treatment for airsickness in military pilots. The

International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 15, 395-412.

de Rooij, S., Schene, A., Phillips, D., & Roseboom, T. (2010). Depression and anxiety:

Associations with biological and perceived stress reactivity to a psychological

stress protocol in a middle-aged population. Psychoneurendocrinology, 35, 866-

77.

Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

behavior. New York: Plenum Publishing Co.

Demertizis, K., & Craske, M. (2006). Anxiety in primary care. Current Psychiatry

Reports, 8, 291-7.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 51

Dickerson, S., & Kemeny, M. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A

theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological

Bulletin, 130, 355-91.

Epel, E., Lin, J., Wilhelm, F., Wolkowitz, O., Cawthon, R., Adler, N., … Blackburn, E.

(2006). Cell aging in relation to stress arousal and cardiovascular disease risk

factors. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 277-87.

Farne, M., & Gnugnoli, D. (2000). Effects of autogenic training on emotional distress

symptoms. Stress Medicine, 16, 259-61.

Forman, E., Herbert, J., Moitra, E., Yeomans, P., & Geller, P. (2007). A randomized

controlled effectiveness trial of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and

Cognitive Therapy for anxiety and depression. Behavior Modification, 31, 772-

99.

Fresco, D., Coles, M., Heimberg, R., Liebowitz, M., Hami, S., Stein, M., & Goetz, D.

(2001). The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale: A comparison of the psychometric

properties of self-report and clinician-administered formats. Psychological

Medicine, 31, 1025-35.

Gifford, E., Kohlenberg, B., Hayes, S., Antonuccio, D., Piasecki, M., Rasmussen-Hall,

M., & Palm, K. (2004). Acceptance theory-based treatment for smoking

cessation: An initial trial of acceptance and commitment therapy. Behavior

Therapy, 35, 689-706.

Glassman, A., & Shapiro, P. (1997). Depression and the course of coronary artery

disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 4-11.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 52

Greenhouse, S., & Geisser, S. 1959. On methods in the analysis for profile data.

Psychometrica, 24, 95–112.

Gregg, J., Callaghan, G., Hayes, S., Glenn-Lawson, J. (2007). Improving diabetes self-

management through acceptance, mindfulness, and values: A randomized

controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 336-43.

Hayes, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A Post-

Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition. New York, NY: Kluwer.

Hayes, S., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and

Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 44, 1-25.

Hayes, S., Masuda, A., Bissett, R., Luoma, J., & Guerrero, J. (2004). DBT, FAP, and

ACT: How empirically oriented are the new behavior therapy technologies?

Behavior Therapy, 35, 35-54.

Hayes, S., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An

experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press.

Hayes, S., Wilson, K., Gifford, E., Follette, V., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential

avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to

diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64,

1152-68.

Hayes, S., Wilson, K., Gifford, E., Bissett, R., Piasecki, M., Batten, S., … Gregg, J.

(2004). A preliminary trial of twelve-step facilitation and acceptance and

commitment therapy with polysubstance-abusing methadone-maintained opiate

addicts. Behavior Therapy, 35,667-88.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 53

Hellhammer, J., & Schubert, M. (2012). The physiological response to Trier Social Stress

Test relates to subjective measures of stress during but not before or after the test.

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 119-24.

Heopniemi, T., Elovainio, M., Pulkki, L., Puttonen, S., Raitakari, O., & Keltikangas-

Jarvinen, L. (2007). Cardiac autonomic reactivity and recovery in predicting

carotid atherosclerosis: The cardiovascular risk in young Finns study. Health

Psychology, 26, 13–21.

Herbert, C., & Gutman, G. (1983). Practical group autogenic training for management of

stress- related disorders in family practice. Canadian Family Physician, 29, 109-

17.

Hettema, J., Steele, J., & Miller, W. (2005). Motivational Interviewing. Annual Review of

Clinical Psychology, 1, 91-111.

Hidderley, M., & Holt, M. (2004). A pilot randomized trial assessing the effects of

autogenic training in early stage cancer patients in relation to psychological status

and immune system responses. European Journal of Oncology Nursing, 8, 61-5.

Hocking Schuler, J., & O’Brien, W. (1997). Cardiovascular recovery from stress and

hypertension risk factors: A meta-analytic review. Psychophysiology, 34, 649-59.

Holmes, T., & Rahe, R. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-8.

Jezova, D., Makatsori, A., Duncko, R., Moncek, F., & Jakubek, M. (2004). High trait

anxiety in healthy subjects is associated with low neuroendocrine activity during

psychosocial stress. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological

Psychiatry, 28, 1331-6.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 54

Johnsen, B., Thayer, J., Laberg, J., Wormnes, B., Raadal, M., Skaret,

E., … Berg., E.

(2003). Attentional and physiological characteristics of patients with dental

anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 75-87.

Jonsson, P., Wallergard, M., Osterberg, K., Hansen, A., Johansson, G., & Karlson, B.

(2010). Cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity and habituation to a virtual reality

version of the Trier Social Stress Test: A pilot study. Psychoneuroendocrinology,

35, 1397-403.

Juster, R., Marin, M., Sindi, S., Nair, V., Ng, Y., Pruessner, J., Lupien, S. (2011).

Allostatic load associations to acute, 3-year and 6-year prospective depressive

symptoms in healthy older adults. Physiology & Behavior, 104, 360-4.

Kanji, N. (1997). Autogenic Training. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 5, 162-7.

Kanji, N., White, A., & Ernst, E. (2004a). Autogenic training to reduce anxiety in nursing

students: Randomized controlled trial. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 53, 729-35.

Kanji, N., White, M., & Ernst, E. (2004b). Autogenic training reduces anxiety after

coronary angioplasty: A randomized clinical trial. American Heart Journal, 147,

e10.

Kanji, N., White, M., & Ernst, E. (2006). Autogenic training for tension type headaches:

A systematic review of controlled trials. Complementary Therapies in Medicine,

14, 144-50.

Kelly, M., Tyrka, A., Anderson, J., Price, L., Carpenter, L. (2008). Sex differences in

emotional and physiological responses to the Trier Social Stress Test. Journal of

Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39, 87-98.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 55

Kessler, R. (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of

Psychology, 48, 191-214.

Kibler, J., & Ma, M. (2004). Depressive symptoms and cardiovascular reactivity to

behavioral stress. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 11, 81-7.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J., McGuire, L., Robles, T., & Glaser, R. (2002). Emotions, morbidity,

and mortality: New perspectives from psychoneuroimmunology. Annual Review

of Psychology, 53, 83-107.

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K., Hellhammer, D. (1993). The ‘‘Trier Social Stress Test’’- a

tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting.

Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76-81.

Krampen, G. (1999). Long-term evaluation of the effectiveness of additional autogenic

training in the psychotherapy of depressive disorders. European Psychologist, 4,

11-8.

Kubzansky, L., Kawachi, I., Weiss, S., Sparrow, D. (1998). Anxiety and coronary heart

disease: A synthesis of epidemiological, psychological, and experimental

evidence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 47-58.

Kudielka, B., Buske-Kirschbaum, A., Hellhammer, D., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004a).

Differential heart rate reactivity and recovery after psychosocial stress (TSST) in

health children, younger adults, and elderly adults: The impact of age and gender.

International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 11, 116-21.

Kudielka, B., Wust, S., Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. (2007). The Trier Social

Stress Test (TSST). In G. Fink, Ed., Encyclopedia of stress (2nd

ed.). San Diego:

Academic Press.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 56

Labbe, E. (1995). Treatment of childhood migraine with autogenic training and skin

temperature biofeedback: A component analysis. Headache, 35, 10-3.

Lazarus, R., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

Liebowitz, M. (1987). Social Phobia. Modern Problems of Pharmacopsychiatry, 22, 141-

73.

Linden, W. (1990). Autogenic training. New York: Guilford Press.

Logan, J., & Barksdale, D. (2008). Allostasis and allostatic load: expanding the discourse

on stress and cardiovascular disease. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 17, 201-8.

Low, C., Stanton, A., & and Bower, J. (2008). Effects of acceptance-oriented versus

evaluative emotional processing on heart rate recovery and habituation. Emotion,

8, 419-24.

Lundgren, T., Dahl, J., & Hayes, S. (2008). Evaluation of mediators of change in the

treatment of epilepsy with Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. Journal of

Behavioral Medicine, 31, 225-35.

Luthar, S. (Ed.). (2003). Resilience and vulnerability: adaptation in the context of

childhood adversities. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Luthe, W. (1969-1973). Autogenic therapy (Vol. I-VI). New York, NY: Grune &

Stratton.

Maller, R., & Reiss, S. (1987). A behavioral validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index

Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 1, 256-272.

Maller, R., & Reiss, S. (1992). Anxiety sensitivity in 1984 and panic disorder in 1987.

Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 6, 241-247.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 57

Mausbach, B., Dimsdale, J., Ziegler, M., Mills, P., Ancoli-Israel, S., Patterson, T., &

Grant, I. (2005). Depressive symptoms predict norepinephrine response to a

psychological stressor task in alzheimer’s caregivers. Psychosomatic Medicine,

67, 638-42.

Mavros, M., Athanasiou, S., Gkegkes, I., Polyzos, K., Peppas, G., & Falagas, M. (2011).

Do psychological variables affect early surgical recovery? PLoS One, 6, 1-6.

McCracken, L., & Eccleston, C. (2006). A comparison of the relative utility of coping

and acceptance-based measures in a sample of chronic pain sufferers. European

Journal of Pain, 10, 23-9.

McEwen, B. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England

Journal of Medicine, 338, 171-9.

McEwen, B. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for

neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108-24.

McEwen, B. (2002) Sex, stress and the hippocampus: allostasis, allostatic load and the

aging process. Neurobiology of Aging, 23, 921-39.

McEwen, B. (2003). Mood Disorders and allostatic load. Biological Psychiatry, 201,

200-7.

McEwen, B. (2004) Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: allostasis and

allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders.

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1032, 1-7.

McEwen, B. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role

of the brain. Physiological Reviews, 87, 873-904.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 58

McEwen, B., & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual. Archives of Internal

Medicine, 153, 2093-101.

Miller, W. (1983). Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers. Behavioral

Psychotherapy, 11, 147-72.

Miller, W., & Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People to

Change Addictive Behavior. New York: Guilford.

Miller, W., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for

Change, Vol. 2. New York: Guilford.

Morgan, A., & Jorm, A. (2008). Self-help interventions for depressive disorders and

depressive symptoms: A systematic review. Annals of General Psychiatry, 7, 13.

Moussavi, S., Chatterji, S., Verdes, E., Tandon, A., Patel, V., & Ustun, B. (2007).

Depression, chronic diseases, and decrements in health: Results from the World

Health Surveys. Lancet, 370, 851–8.

Munafo, M., & Stevenson, J. (2001). Anxiety and surgical recovery: Reinterpreting the

literature. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 51, 589-96.

Nater, U., Rohleder, N., Gaab, J., Berger, S., Jud, A., Kirschbaum, C., et al. (2005).

Human salivary alpha-amylase reactivity in a psychosocial stress paradigm.

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 55(3), 333-42.

Nugent, N., Tyrka, A., Carpenter, L., & Price, L. (2011). Gene-environment interactions:

Early life stress and risk for depressive and anxiety disorders.

Psychopharmacology, 214, 175-96.

Peterson, R., & Sacks, C. (1987). Predictors of reaction to self-reported panic attacks in

college students. Paper presented at the Association for Advancement of Behavior

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 59

Therapy Convention, Boston, MA: November, 1987.

Pignay-Demaria, V., Lespérance, F., Demaria, R., Frasure-Smith, N., & Perrault, L.

(2003). Depression and anxiety and outcomes of coronary artery bypass surgery.

The Annals of Thoracic Surgery, 75, 314-21.

Reiss, S. (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clinical Psychology

Review, 11, 141-53.

Reiss, S., Peterson, R., & Gursky, D. (1988). Anxiety sensitivity, injury sensitivity, and

individual differences in fearfulness. Behavior Research and Therapy, 26, 341-

45.

Reiss, S., Peterson, R., Gursky, D., & McNally, R. (1986). Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety

frequency, and the prediction of fearfulness. Behavior Research and Therapy, 24,

1-8.

Rosamond, W., Flegal, K., Friday, G., Furie, K., Go, A., Greenlund, K.,… Hong Y.

(2007). Heart disease and stroke statistics - 2007 update: A report from the

American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics

Subcommittee. Circulation, 115, e69–171.

Ryan, R., Lynch, M., Vansteenkiste, M., & Deci, E. (2011). Motivation and autonomy in

counseling, psychotherapy, and behavior change: A look at theory and practice.

The Counseling Psychologist, 39, 193– 260.

Salomon, K., Clift, A., Karisdottir, M., & Rottenberg, J. (2009). Major depressive

disorder is associated with attenuated cardiovascular reactivity and impaired

recovery among those free of cardiovascular disease. Health Psychology, 28, 157-

65.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 60

Shinozaki, M., Kanazawa, M., Kano, M., Endo, Y., Nakaya, N., Hongo, M., & Fukudo,

S. (2010). Effect of Autogenic Training on General Improvement in Patients with

Irritable Bowel Syndrome: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Applied

Psychophysiological Biofeedback, 35, 189-98.

Schlamann, M., Naglatzki, R., de Greiff, A., Forsting, M., & Gizewski, E. (2010).

Autogenic Training alters cerebral activation patterns in fMRI. International

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58, 444-56.

Schulkin, J. (2004). Allostasis, homeostasis, and the costs of physiological adaptation.

Cambridge University Press, New York.

Schultz J. (1932) Autogenic Training. Thieme Verlag, Stuttgart.

Schultz, J., & Luthe, W. (1959). Autogenic training—A psychophysiological approach in

psychotherapy. New York: Grune & Stratton.

Seeman, T., Singer, B., Rowe, J., Horwitz, R., & McEwen, B. (1997). Price of

adaptation-allostatic load and its health consequences: MacArthur studies of

successful aging. Archives of Internal Medicine, 157, 2259-68.

Sherbourne, C., Jackson, C., Meredith, L., Camp, P., & Wells, K. (1996). Prevalence of

comorbid anxiety disorders in primary care outpatients. Archives of Family

Medicine, 5, 27-34.

Spielberger, C. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory STAI (Form Y). Palo

Alto, CA: Mind Garden.

Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R. (1970). STAI: Manual for the State-Trait

Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 61

Sterling, P., & Eyer, J. (1988). Allostasis: A new paradigm to explain arousal pathology.

In S. Fisher & J. Reason (Eds.), Handbook of life stress, cognition and health (pp.

629 – 649). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Stetter, F., & Kupper, S. (2002). Autogenic training: A meta-analysis of clinical outcome

studies. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 27, 45-98.

Stoudemire, A. (1996). Epidemiology and psychopharmacology of anxiety in medical

patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 57, 73-5.

Sullivan, G., Coplan, J., Kent, J., & Gorman, J. (1999). The noradrenergic system in

pathological anxiety: A focus on panic with relevance to generalized anxiety and

phobias. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 1205-18.

Taylor, S., Seeman, T., Eisenberg, N., Kozanian, T., Moore, A., & Moons, W. (2010).

Effects of a supportive or an unsupportive audience on biological and

psychological responses to stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

98, 47-56.

Ter Kuile, M., Spinhoven, P., Linssen, A., Zitman, F., & Van Dyck, R. (1994). Autogenic

training and cognitive self-hypnosis for the treatment of recurrent headaches in

three different subject group. Pain, 58, 331-40.

Tsigos, C., & Chrousos, G. (2002). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, neuroendocrine

factors and stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 865-71.

Vasey, M., & Thayer, J. 1987. The continuing problem of false positives in repeated

measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: A multivariate solution.

Psychophysiology, 24, 479–86.

White, W., & Anwar, Y. (2001). Evaluation of the overall efficacy of the Omron office

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 62

digital blood pressure HEM-907 monitor in adults. Blood Pressure Monitoring, 6,

107-10.

Williams, R., Hagerty, B., & Brooks, G. (2004). Trier Social Stress Test: A method for

use in nursing research. Nursing Research, 53, 277–280.

Wittchen, H., Kessler, R., Katja, B., Krause, P., Hofler, M., & Hoyer, J. (2002).

Generalized anxiety and depression in primary care: Prevalence, recognition, and

management. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63, 24-34.

Woods, D., Wetterneck, C., & Flessner, C. (2006). A controlled evaluation of acceptance

and commitment therapy plus habit reversal for trichotillomania. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 44, 639-56.

York, K., Hassan, M., Li, Q., Li, H., Fillingim, R., & Sheps, D. (2007). Coronary artery

disease and depression: Patients with more depressive symptoms have lower

cardiovascular reactivity during laboratory-induced mental stress. Psychosomatic

Medicine, 69, 521-8.

Yusef, S., Hawken, S., Ounpuu, S., Dans, T., Avezum, A., Lanas, F., … Lisheng, L.

(2004). Effect of potentially modifiable risk factors associated with

myocardial infarction in 52 countries (the INTERHEART study): case-control

study. Lancet, 364, 937-52.

Zettle, R. (2003). Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) vs. systematic

desensitization in treatment of mathematics anxiety. Psychological Record, 53,

197-215.

Zettle, R., & Hayes, S. (1986). Dysfunctional control by client verbal behavior: The

context of reason giving. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 4, 30-8.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 63

Zettle, R., & Rains, J. (1989). Group cognitive and contextual therapies in treatment of

depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 436-45.

Zsombok, T., Juhasz, G., Buravari, A., Vitrai, J., & Bagdy, G. (2003). Effect of autogenic

training on drug consumption in patients with primary headache: An 8-month

follow-up study. Headache, 43, 251-7.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 64

Appendix A. Diagram of Participant Involvement in Larger Study

Time 2 (~45 mins)

Pre-Trier Assessment

(~30 mins)

-Physiological

Measures

-Process Measures

Trier Social Stress Test

(~15 mins)

-Instructions

-Anticipation

-Free Speech

-Subtraction

Time 3 (~60 mins)

Post-Trier Assessment

(~20 mins)

-Physiological

Measures

-Process Measures

Debriefing (~15 mins)

Rest Period (~15 mins)

-Physiological

Measures

Wrap Up (~10 mins)

-Questions

Time 1 (~75 mins)

Informed Consent

(~15 mins)

Baseline Assessment (~40

mins)

-Physiological

Measures

-Psychological

Measures

Intervention Sessions

(~20 mins)

-Group 1: ACT

-Group 2: AT

-Group 3: Control

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 65

Table 1

Participant Characteristics by Group

ACT AT Control Total n

n

40

40

40

120

120

Age (SD)

19.32 (1.16)

18.95 (.90)

18.95 (.96)

19.08 (1.02)

120

Mental Health

Condition (%) 6 (15%) 8 (20%) 3 (7.5%) 17 (14.2%) 120

Medical

Condition (%) 2 (5%) 4 (10%) 3 (7.5%) 9 (7.5%) 120

Daily Medication

(%)

12 (30%)

11 (27.5%)

10 (25%)

33 (27.5%)

120

Illicit Drug Use

(%)b,c

7 (17.5%)

5 (12.5%)

4 (10%)

16 (13.3%)

120

Cigarette Smoker

(%)

2 (5%)

4 (10%)

0

6 (5%)

120

BMI (kg/m2)b,c

23.73 (4.59) 23.16 (3.83) 23.43 (3.67) 23.44 (4.03)

119

Physical Exercise

(%)a

21 (52.5%)

23 (57.5%) 22 (55%) 66 (55%) 120

Race and Ethnicity

White

Black

Asian

Hispanic

Biracial/Multiracial

25 (62.5%)

5 (12.5%)

4 (10%)

6 (15%)

0

28 (70%)

5 (12.5%)

6 (15%)

1 (2.5%)

0

26 (65%)

3 (7.5%)

4 (10%)

5 (12.5)

2 (5%)

79 (65.8%)

13 (10.8%)

14 (11.7%)

12 (10%)

2 (1.7%)

120

Consume Alcohol

(%)b

22 (55%)

23 (57.5%)

24 (60%)

69 (67.5%)

120

Time of Daya

AM (9-12 PM)

Early PM (12-3 PM)

Late PM (3-6 PM)

16 (40%)

9 (22.5%)

15 (37.5%)

8 (20%)

22 (55%)

10 (25%)

11 (27.5%)

15 (37.5)

14 (35%)

35 (29.2%)

46 (38.3%)

39 (32.5)

120

aCorrelates with baseline HR (p ≤ 0.1);

bCorrelates with baseline SBP (p ≤ 0.1);

cCorrelates with baseline DBP (p ≤ 0.1)

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 66

Table 2

Participant Characteristics by Gender

Male Female All n

n

40

80

120

120

Age (SD)

18.98 (1.03)

19.13 (1.02)

19.08 (1.02)

120

Mental Health

Condition (%)

3 (7.5%)

14 (17.5%)

17 (14.2%)

120

Medical

Condition (%)

2 (5.0%)

7 (8.8%)

9 (7.5%)

120

Daily Medication (%)

6 (15.0%)

27 (33.8%)

33 (27.5%)

120

Illicit Drug Use (%)b,c

9 (22.5%)

7 (8.8%)

16 (13.3%)

120

Cigarette Smoker (%)

2 (5.0%)

4 (5.0%)

6 (5.0%)

120

BMI (kg/m2)b,c

23.55 (4.04)

23.39 (4.04)

23.44 (4.02)

119

Physical Exercise (%)a

26 (65.0%)

40 (50.0%)

66 (55.0%)

120

Race and Ethnicity

White

Black

Asian

Hispanic

Biracial/Multiracial

30 (75.0%)

2 (5.0%)

6 (15.0%)

1 (2.5%)

1 (2.5%)

51 (63.75%)

13 (16.25%)

8 (10.0%)

7 (8.75%)

1 (1.25%)

81 (67.5%)

15 (12.5%)

14 (11.7%)

8 (6.6%)

2 (1.6%)

120

Consume Alcohol (%)b

26 (65.0%)

43 (53.8%)

69 (57.5%)

120

Time of Daya

AM (9-12 PM)

Early PM (12-3 PM)

Late PM (3-6 PM)

12 (30%)

16 (40%)

12 (30%)

23 (28.7%)

30 (37.5%)

27 (33.8%)

35 (29.2%)

46 (38.3%)

39 (32.5%)

120

aCorrelates with baseline HR (p ≤ 0.1);

bCorrelates with baseline SBP (p ≤ 0.1);

cCorrelates with baseline DBP (p ≤ 0.1)

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 67

Table 3

Descriptive Statistics for Study Measures by Group

Mean SD N

Heart Rate (bpm)

Baseline Total

ACT Group

AT Groupa

Control Group

76.47

73.6

81.78

74.03

14.72

12.11

13.96

16.61

120

40

40

40

Pre-TSST Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

66.53

65.83

67.25

66.53

10.56

9.50

10.50

11.78

120

40

40

40

Post-TSST Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

69.44

68.10

70.55

69.68

12.11

9.50

12.47

14.10

120

40

40

40

Recovery Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

67.17

65.95

67.72

67.83

11.61

9.36

12.35

12.96

120

40

40

40

Systolic Blood Pressure (mmHg)

Baseline Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

126.11

126.05

129.25

123.03

14.60

13.95

14.29

15.22

120

40

40

40

Pre-TSST Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

120.15

119.68

122.60

118.18

13.052

13.0

14.25

11.73

120

40

40

40

Post-TSST Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

131.38

130.38

134.88

128.90

14.36

12.52

15.73

14.33

120

40

40

40

Recovery Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

123.98

123.73

127.0

121.20

13.891

11.85

15.82

13.44

120

40

40

40

Diastolic Blood Pressure (mmHg)

Baseline Total

ACT Group

AT Groupb

Control Group

72.97

73.05

76.43

69.45

8.90

8.17

8.57

8.76

120

40

40

40

Pre-TSST Total

ACT Group

AT Group

70.39

70.05

71.28

8.83

7.79

8.98

120

40

40

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 68

Control Group 69.85 9.78 40

Post-TSST Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

78.59

79.65

79.13

77.0

8.14

7.59

7.96

8.78

120

40

40

40

Recovery Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

73.39

73.56

74.3

72.33

8.58

8.61

8.86

8.37

120

40

40

40

Beck Depression Inventory II

Baseline Measure Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

8.34

8.95

8.68

7.40

6.87

6.70

7.22

6.75

120

40

40

40

Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale

Baseline Measure Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

35.50

37.88

34.88

33.60

21.60

23.55

19.32

22.09

117

40

40

37

Spielberger State Trait Anxiety

Inventory- Trait

Baseline Measure Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

38.88

39.45

38.75

38.43

10.67

10.27

10.60

11.34

120

40

40

40

Anxiety Sensitivity Index

Baseline Measure Total

ACT Group

AT Group

Control Group

19.65

19.35

19.38

20.23

9.98

11.16

7.90

10.78

120

40

40

40 aStatistically significant difference compared to ACT and Control Groups (p ≤ 0.05);

bStatistically significant difference compared to Control Group (p ≤ 0.05)

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 69

Table 4

Descriptive Statistics for Study Measures by Gender

Mean SD N

Heart Rate (bpm)

Baseline Total

Male

Female

76.47

75.95

76.72

14.72

16.25

13.99

120

40

80

Pre-TSST Total

Male

Female

66.53

65.62

66.99

10.56

11.18

10.28

120

40

80

Post-TSST Total

Male

Female

69.44

69.95

69.19

12.11

14.03

11.12

120

40

80

Recovery Total

Male

Female

67.17

67.08

67.21

11.61

12.44

11.25

120

40

80

Systolic Blood Pressure (mmHg)

Baseline Total

Malea

Female

126.11

137.45

120.44

14.60

14.44

10.97

120

40

80

Pre-TSST Total

Malea

Female

120.15

131.35

114.55

13.052

12.64

9.08

120

40

80

Post-TSST Total

Malea

Female

131.38

144.07

125.04

14.36

12.92

10.29

120

40

80

Recovery Total

Malea

Female

123.98

134.18

118.87

13.891

11.70

12.0

120

40

80

Diastolic Blood Pressure (mmHg)

Baseline Total

Male

Female

72.97

73.97

72.48

8.90

10.74

7.86

120

40

80

Pre-TSST Total

Male

Female

70.39

71.05

70.06

8.83

9.05

8.76

120

40

80

Post-TSST Total

Male

Female

78.59

79.53

78.13

8.14

8.16

8.14

120

40

80

Recovery Total

Male

Female

73.39

73.8

73.19

8.58

10.41

7.58

120

40

80

Beck Depression Inventory II

Baseline Measure Total

Male

8.34

6.98

6.87

5.24

120

40

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS 70

Female 9.03 7.49 80

Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale

Baseline Measure Total

Male

Female

35.50

32.23

37.2

21.60

18.52

23.0

117

40

77

Spielberger State Trait Anxiety

Inventory- Trait

Baseline Measure Total

Malea

Female

38.88

36.05

40.29

10.67

9.1

11.16

120

40

80

Anxiety Sensitivity Index

Baseline Measure Total

Malea

Female

19.65

16.75

21.1

9.98

8.31

10.46

120

40

80 aStatistically significant difference compared to Females (p ≤ 0.05)

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 1

Autogenic Training Group: Depression and Systolic Blood Pressure

Figure 1. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

depression). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Autogenic

Training group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Autogenic Training Group: Depression and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Autogenic

71

way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Autogenic

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 2

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy:

Figure 1. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

depression). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low

Commitment Therapy group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Depression and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Acceptance and

Commitment Therapy group.

72

Depression and Systolic Blood Pressure

way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

depressive symptomatology in the Acceptance and

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 3

Control Group: Depression and Systolic Blood Pressure

Figure 3. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

depression). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Control group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Control Group: Depression and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Control group.

73

way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between subjects with high and low depressive symptomatology in the Control group.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 4

High Depressive Symptomatology: Group and Systolic

Figure 4. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

depression). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between intervention groups in those with high depressive

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

High Depressive Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between intervention groups in those with high depressive symptomatology.

74

way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

symptomatology.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 5

Low Depressive Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

Figure 5. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

depression). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between intervention groups in those with

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Low Depressive Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

between intervention groups in those with low depressive symptomatology.

75

way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time

depressive symptomatology.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 6

Autogenic Training Group: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood Pressure

Figure 6. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Autogenic

group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Autogenic Training Group: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Autogenic

76

way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Autogenic Training

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 7

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood Pressure

Figure 7. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Acceptance and

Commitment Therapy group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Acceptance and

Commitment Therapy group.

77

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood Pressure

way interaction (time by group by social

the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Acceptance and

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 8

Control Group: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood

Figure 8. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Control Group: Social Anxiety and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Control group.

78

way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

Control group.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 9

High Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood

Figure 9. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

intervention groups in those with high social anxiety

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

High Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

intervention groups in those with high social anxiety symptomatology.

79

Pressure

way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 10

Low Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

Figure 10.This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

intervention groups in those with

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Low Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

intervention groups in those with low social anxiety symptomatology.

80

Low Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Systolic Blood Pressure

way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of systolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 11

Autogenic Training: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

Figure 11. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of

subjects with high and low

group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Autogenic Training

81

way interaction (time by group by social

blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

symptomatology in the Autogenic Training

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 12

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

Figure 12. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of

subjects with high and low

Commitment Therapy group.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Acceptance and

group.

82

Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

way interaction (time by group by social

blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

Acceptance and

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 13

Control Group: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

Figure 13. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of

subjects with high and low

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Control Group: Social Anxiety and Diastolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by

It represents the measure of diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

subjects with high and low social anxiety symptomatology in the Control group

83

way interaction (time by group by social

blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

Control group.

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 14

High Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and

Figure 14. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of

intervention groups in those with high social anxiety symptomatology.

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Diastolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

intervention groups in those with high social anxiety symptomatology.

84

Blood Pressure

way interaction (time by group by social

blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

BRIEF CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Figure 15

Low Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and

Figure 15. This figure helps demonstrate the 3

anxiety). It represents the measure of

intervention groups in those with

CLINICAL INTERVENTIONS

Social Anxiety Symptomatology: Group and Diastolic Blood Pressure

This figure helps demonstrate the 3-way interaction (time by group by social

It represents the measure of diastolic blood pressure (mmHg) over time between

intervention groups in those with low social anxiety symptomatology.

85

Blood Pressure

way interaction (time by group by social

blood pressure (mmHg) over time between