24
The Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 Timothy J. Trull University of Missouri ABSTRACT The Five-Factor Model of personality disorders (FFMPD; Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2009) developed from the recogni- tion that the popular Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality could be used to describe and understand the official personality disorder (PD) constructs from the American Psychiatric Association’s (APA) diagnostic manuals (e.g., DSM-IV-TR, APA, 2000). This article provides an over- view of the FFM, highlighting its validity and utility in characterizing PDs as well as its ability to provide a comprehensive account of personality pathology in general. In 2013, DSM-5 is scheduled to appear, and the “hybrid” PD proposal will emphasize a 25–personality trait model. I present the current version of this new model, compare it to the FFMPD, and discuss issues related to the implementation of the FFMPD. This article presents an overview of the Five-Factor Model of personality disorders (FFMPD; Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2009), including its historical development, empirical support, and previous use in evaluating the DSM-IV-TR personality disorders (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000). Next, the proposed “hybrid model” of personality disorders (PDs) that is being considered for DSM-5 is discussed, focusing primarily on its 25-trait model of per- sonality, as well as its relationship to the FFMPD. I close with issues related to the implementation of the FFMPD. THE FIVE-FACTOR MODEL OF PERSONALITY Although a number of personality trait models have been discussed in the context of the DSM personality disorders, the Five-Factor Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Timothy J. Trull, 210 McAlester Hall, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211. Email: [email protected]. Journal of Personality 80:6, December 2012 © 2012 The Author Journal of Personality © 2012, Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00771.x

The Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

The Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorderand DSM-5

Timothy J. TrullUniversity of Missouri

ABSTRACT The Five-Factor Model of personality disorders(FFMPD; Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2009) developed from the recogni-tion that the popular Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality could beused to describe and understand the official personality disorder (PD)constructs from the American Psychiatric Association’s (APA) diagnosticmanuals (e.g., DSM-IV-TR, APA, 2000). This article provides an over-view of the FFM, highlighting its validity and utility in characterizing PDsas well as its ability to provide a comprehensive account of personalitypathology in general. In 2013, DSM-5 is scheduled to appear, and the“hybrid” PD proposal will emphasize a 25–personality trait model. Ipresent the current version of this new model, compare it to the FFMPD,and discuss issues related to the implementation of the FFMPD.

This article presents an overview of the Five-Factor Model ofpersonality disorders (FFMPD; Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2009),including its historical development, empirical support, and previoususe in evaluating the DSM-IV-TR personality disorders (AmericanPsychiatric Association [APA], 2000). Next, the proposed “hybridmodel” of personality disorders (PDs) that is being considered forDSM-5 is discussed, focusing primarily on its 25-trait model of per-sonality, as well as its relationship to the FFMPD. I close with issuesrelated to the implementation of the FFMPD.

THE FIVE-FACTOR MODEL OF PERSONALITY

Although a number of personality trait models have been discussedin the context of the DSM personality disorders, the Five-Factor

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Timothy J. Trull,210 McAlester Hall, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri,Columbia, MO 65211. Email: [email protected].

Journal of Personality 80:6, December 2012© 2012 The AuthorJournal of Personality © 2012, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00771.x

Model of personality has received both the most research attentionand empirical support in this context (e.g., Allik, 2005; Clark, 2007;Clark & Livesley, 2002; Costa & Widiger, 2002; O’Connor & Dyce,1998; Samuel & Widiger, 2008; Saulsman & Page, 2004; Trull &Durrett, 2005; Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt, 2009; Widiger & Simon-sen, 2005; Widiger & Trull, 2007). As stated by Clark (2007), “Thefive-factor model of personality is widely accepted as representingthe higher-order structure of both normal and abnormal personalitytraits” (p. 246).

Indeed, the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality is a popularway to conceptualize major personality traits. It has a long historyand rich tradition, and was derived through factor-analytic studies ofpersonality trait terms from the English language (see Goldberg,1993; John & Srivastava, 1999). As Widiger and Mullins-Sweatt(2009) noted:

Language can be understood as a sedimentary deposit of theobservations of persons over the thousands of years of the lan-guage’s development and transformation. The most importantdomains of personality functioning are those with the greatestnumber of trait terms to describe and differentiate the variousmanifestations and nuances of a respective domain, and the struc-ture of personality is suggested by the empirical relationshipsamong these trait terms. (p. 199)

Thus, the FFM is often termed lexical, in that it reflects the structureof personality descriptions that occur most frequently in the Englishlanguage. It also appears to be fairly universal in that this five-factorstructure in trait terms has been replicated in a variety of otherlanguages, including German, Dutch, Czech, Polish, Russian,Italian, Spanish, Hebrew, Hungarian, Turkish, Korean, and Filipino(Allik, 2005; Ashton & Lee, 2001).

The five major domains of this model are typically referred to asNeuroticism versus emotional stability, Extraversion versus intro-version, Openness versus closedness to experience, Agreeablenessversus antagonism, and Conscientiousness versus negligence. TheFFM was originally developed using nonclinical samples, and thegoal was to provide a comprehensive account of major personalitytraits and dimensions. However, several came to realize that theFFM might also be applied to issues relating to various forms of

Trull1698

psychopathology. Personality traits are indeed dimensional, and per-sonality disorders by definition involve maladaptive or extreme per-sonality traits. Further, the hierarchical structure of FFM traits (i.e.,higher order domains and lower order facets) has been replicatedacross populations (i.e., nonclinical and clinical) and cultures, andevidence suggests a heritable and biological basis for both higherorder and lower order FFM traits (Trull & Durrett, 2005).

Over the last two decades, many studies have assessed therelations between FFM constructs and personality disorders (seeWidiger & Costa, 2002; Samuel & Widiger, 2008, for reviews). Thesestudies have sampled clinical subjects, community residents, andcollege students. For example, perhaps the first study examining therelationship between the FFM and PDs in a clinical sample wasconducted by Trull (1992). Importantly, this study demonstratedstrong relationships between the FFM and personality disorder fea-tures in a clinical sample of psychiatric outpatients. FFM scoresaccounted for significant amounts of variance in individual person-ality disorders in almost every case, and many of the patterns ofFFM relations for individual personality disorders were replicatedacross three different personality disorder measures (i.e., a semistruc-tured interview and two self-report inventories). Many studies usingboth clinical and nonclinical samples have followed; studies haveconsistently demonstrated significant relations between the traitsincluded in the FFM and the DSM PD constructs (Samuel &Widiger, 2008; Saulsman & Page, 2004; Trull & Durrett, 2005;Widiger & Costa, 2002).

However, simply demonstrating that, overall, these traits arerelated to PDs is not particularly surprising. After all, by definitionPDs comprise extreme, maladaptive versions of personality traits(APA, 2000). Instead, what is of interest is the extent to whichPD–personality trait relations may help distinguish among the offi-cial PD diagnoses. Therefore, based on an understanding of theFFM as well as of personality disorders, Widiger and colleagues(Lynam & Widiger, 2001; Widiger et al.,1994; Widiger, Trull, et al.,2002) offered a set of predicted correlates between the five majordimensions of the FFM, as well as the facets composing each dimen-sion, and the DSM-IV personality disorders. Subsequent studieshave found general support for the relevance of the FFM to the fullrange of personality disorders. For example, O’Connor and Dyce(1998) used a confirmatory factor-analytic strategy to evaluate the

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1699

“fit” of the FFM across 12 data sets of personality disorder symp-toms. The authors used the proposals of Widiger et al. (1994) topredict the covariance structure, and results supported the FFM as away of conceptualizing personality disorder pathology. More recentstudies have examined FFM and personality disorder relations at thefacet level. The main reason for this more detailed focus is that betterdifferentiation among the personality disorders is possible at the levelof first-order versus higher order traits (Samuel & Widiger, 2008).Most of the personality disorders are associated with elevations onNeuroticism, introversion, antagonism, and negligence (Saulsman &Page, 2004; Samuel & Widiger, 2008). However, it appears that thepersonality disorders can be distinguished by the patterns of relationsat the first-order, facet trait level (O’Connor & Dyce, 1998; Samuel &Widiger, 2008; Trull, Widiger, & Burr, 2001).

Although these studies have primarily conceptualized personal-ity pathology and disorder from the perspective of the existingdiagnostic manual, the findings do help explain some of the comor-bidity patterns typically reported among the PDs (e.g., see Lynam& Widiger, 2001). As mentioned, Samuel and Widiger (2008)recently presented findings from a meta-analysis of studies thatexamined the relations between both the five domains and the 30facets of personality traits included in the FFM and the DSM-IVPDs. Their results for the relationships between domain scores ofthe FFM and individual PDs are consistent with the finding thatmost PDs appear to be related to each other and comorbidity ismore the rule than the exception: The majority of PDs are charac-terized by significant positive relations with Neuroticism, signifi-cant negative relations with Extraversion, significant negativerelations with Agreeableness, and significant negative relations withConscientiousness. Second, the facet-level relations provide someunderstanding of comorbidity patterns between certain pairs ofPDs (Lynam & Widiger, 2001). For example, the relatively parallelpattern of FFM facet associations with paranoid, schizoid, andschizotypal PD suggests that these diagnostic constructs mightco-occur with each other (consistent with the finding that thesewithin–cluster A correlations are significant). Finally, Samuel andWidiger’s (2008) FFM facet results also are consistent with thefinding that both antisocial and obsessive-compulsive PD, respec-tively, are consistently less highly associated with other PDs; anexamination of their respective FFM facet profiles indicates that

Trull1700

they show less personality trait overlap with the other PDs. In thecase of antisocial PD, it is characterized primarily by low levels ofAgreeableness facets (i.e., low levels of trust, straightforwardness,altruism, and compliance) and low levels of Conscientiousnessfacets (i.e., low levels of competence, dutifulness, self-discipline,and deliberation). Although borderline PD also shows a similarpattern of associations with these facets, in addition (unlike anti-social PD), borderline PD is significantly positively related to allNeuroticism facets (i.e., anxiousness, angry hostility, depressive-ness, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, and vulnerability). Thisexplains why antisocial PD is often most highly associated withborderline PD. Obsessive-compulsive PD is another case in point.This PD’s FFM facet profile is unique in that there are some smallpositive associations with Neuroticism facets (but not Extraversion,Openness, or Agreeableness facets) but stronger positive associa-tions with Conscientiousness facets (i.e., high levels of competence,order, dutifulness, achievement striving, self-discipline, and delib-eration). Obsessive-compulsive PD is the only PD to show positiverelations with these Conscientiousness facets.

DSM-IV-TR AND PERSONALITY TRAITS

As has been documented for some time now, the categorical model ofpersonality disorders, as exemplified in the DSM-IV-TR and itsimmediate predecessors, is fraught with problems. These includeexcessive heterogeneity within diagnoses, excessive diagnosticcomorbidity, inadequate coverage, arbitrary boundaries with normalpsychological functioning, and an inadequate scientific foundation(Clark, 2007; Livesley, 2001; Trull & Durrett, 2005; Widiger & Trull,2007). Although the DSM-IV-TR embraces the categorical approachto personality pathology diagnosis, it does at least mention the pos-sibility of an alternative dimensional approach: “An alternative tothe categorical approach is the dimensional perspective that Person-ality Disorders represent maladaptive variants of personality traitsthat merge imperceptibly into normality and into one another”(APA, 2000, p. 689). In addition, DSM-IV-TR acknowledges therelevancy of major personality trait models (e.g., the FFM) to thePDs.

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1701

But how are personality traits and the DSM-IV-TR personalitydisorders connected? DSM-IV-TR states:

Personality traits are enduring patterns of perceiving, relating to,and thinking about the environment and oneself that are exhibitedin a wide range of social and personal contexts. Only when per-sonality traits are inflexible and maladaptive and cause significantfunctional impairment or subjective distress do they constitutePersonality Disorders. (APA, 2000, p. 686)

Nevertheless, an examination of the criteria sets for the personal-ity disorders reveals that the majority of the criteria are not person-ality traits per se, but rather behavioral, cognitive, or interpersonalindicators of problematic levels of traits (Widiger & Mullins-Sweatt,2009). A recent analysis of the personality traits/psychopathologydimensions that underlie the DSM-IV-TR criteria for borderlinepersonality disorder (BPD) is instructive (Trull, Tomko, Brown, &Scheiderer, 2010). As can be seen in Table 1, there are only threeBPD symptoms that are directly related to the underlying personalitytrait dimension of emotional dysregulation and only one directlyrelated to impulsivity/behavioral disinhibition (labeled “Direct” in

Table 1Personality Traits and Borderline Personality

Disorder Symptoms

Borderline SymptomEmotional

Dysregulation

Impulsivity/Behavioral

Disinhibition

Frantic efforts to avoid abandonment Indirect IndirectUnstable interpersonal relationships Indirect IndirectIdentity disturbance Indirect IndirectImpulsivity DirectRecurrent suicidal behavior/

self-harm/suicidal threatsIndirect Indirect

Affective instability DirectChronic emptiness DirectExtreme anger Direct IndirectTransient, stress-related paranoid

ideation or dissociationIndirect

Trull1702

Table 1). The other criteria for BPD may be seen as either indirectindicators of these personality traits or combinations of traits.Although some might disagree with specific entries in Table 1, themajor point should not be lost. With the exception of affective insta-bility, chronic emptiness, excessive anger, and impulsivity, thesecriteria seem better conceptualized as possible indicators of traits(which may be inferred but are not identified by name). The criteriasets for other personality disorders are similar in their mix of traitsand of indicators of unnamed traits. Therefore, the DSM-IV-TR PDcriteria are really a mixture of personality traits per se and indicatorsof these traits, the latter of which differ in the degree of inferencerequired for assessment as well as for making the connection to theunderlying personality trait.

FIVE-FACTOR MODEL OF PERSONALITY DISORDER

To this point, I have evaluated the FFM in reference to its ability tocharacterize and account for the DSM-IV-TR PDs. It is clear thatPDs can be understood as maladaptive variants of the FFM (Clark,2007; Livesley, 2001; Samuel & Widiger, 2008; Saulsman & Page,2004), and the predominant models of normal and abnormal person-ality functioning converge onto at least four of the five broaddomains of the FFM (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Clark, 2007; John& Srivastava, 1999; Livesley, 2003; Markon, Krueger, & Watson,2005; Trull & Durrett, 2005; Watson, Clark, & Harkness, 1994;Widiger & Simonsen, 2005).

Findings such as these have encouraged investigators to go evenone step further. Clearly, the DSM-IV-TR does not present a com-prehensive catalog of all personality pathology, nor is it an efficientclassification system (given rampant comorbidity among the PDs,heterogeneity within diagnosis, and the high prevalence of the PDnot otherwise specified diagnosis; Trull & Durrett, 2005). It makesmuch more sense to characterize and define personality pathologyand disorder through the lens of a widely accepted, comprehensive,and empirically validated model of personality as opposed to thecurrent PD diagnostic system embodied in the official diagnosticmanual. Such an approach is likely to improve the weak constructvalidity of the DSM-IV diagnostic categories (Mullins-Sweatt &Widiger, 2006).

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1703

The Four Steps

So how can the FFM characterize personality pathology and diag-nose PD? Widiger, Costa, and McCrae (2002) proposed a four-stepprocedure for an FFM diagnosis of personality disorder. Briefly, thefirst step is to obtain a personality trait description of an individual interms of the five domains and 30 facets of the FFM. This descriptionwill provide a comprehensive description of the person’s adaptive aswell as maladaptive personality traits. There are a number of psy-chological measures that can be used for this FFM description (DeRaad & Perugini, 2002), including, for example, the questionnaire-based Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R; Costa &McCrae, 1992) and the Structured Interview for the Five FactorModel (SIFFM; Trull & Widiger, 1997), a semistructured interviewfor the assessment of the FFM. Other FFM self-report inventoriesand brief clinician rating scales can also be used (De Raad &Perugini, 2002; Mullins-Sweatt, Jamerson, Samuel, Olson, &Widiger, 2006). For example, Few et al. (2010) demonstrated the useof a brief rating form for FFM facets to evaluate patients.

The second step involves an assessment of any social and occupa-tional impairments and distress associated with extreme scores on theFFM personality traits. For example, Widiger, Costa, et al. (2002)and McCrae, Löckenhoff, and Costa (2005) identified problems likelyto be found in people scoring high or low on each of the FFM domainsand facets. In the case of the SIFFM interviews, there are questionsincluded that assess these maladaptive variants of each of the 30 traitfacets of the FFM. Further, both the SIFFM and the Five FactorModel Score Sheet (FFMSS; Mullins-Sweatt et al., 2006) includeseparate, independent assessments of many of the problems identifiedby Widiger et al. (2002) and McCrae et al. (2005).

The third step is to determine whether the dysfunction and distressreach a clinically significant level of impairment that would warranta diagnosis of personality disorder. There are several possibilities forthis assessment. For example, one could use the Global Assessmentof Functioning (GAF) scale currently used for Axis V of the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000). Other possibilities, measures that purport tomeasure personality-related impairment and dysfunction specifi-cally, include the General Assessment of Personality Dysfunction(GAPD; Livesley, 2010) and the Severity Indices of PersonalityProblems (SIPP-118; Verheul et al., 2008).

Trull1704

The fourth step is necessary only if one desires a single quantita-tive index of the match between an individual’s FFM personalityprofile and prototypic profiles of diagnostic constructs (e.g., Miller,2012; Miller & Lynam, 2003; Trull, Widiger, Lynam, & Costa,2003). Although this can be used to assess a match between anindividual’s FFM profile and a FFM characterization of the DSM-IV-TR PD constructs, it is also possible for clinicians and research-ers to develop FFM profiles for personality disorder constructs notincluded within DSM-IV (e.g., successful psychopath). However,prototypal matching with the DSM PDs is not generally recom-mended, as the purpose of the FFM diagnosis would not simplybe to provide a roundabout method of returning to the DSMdiagnostic categories (Clark, 2007).

DIAGNOSING PDS IN DSM-5: COMPARISONS WITHTHE FFMPD

Before comparing the DSM-5 revisions for PD with the FFMPD, itis useful to briefly outline this new proposal for PD diagnosis. Thenew version of the DSM (DSM-5) proposes that a diagnosis of PDinvolves a series of determinations concerning overall personalitydysfunction as well as specific personality trait elevations(www.dsm5.org). It is important to note that the original DSM-5 PDproposal (published on the Web site in February 2010) was revised toaddress concerns and clarify confusion (Skodol et al., 2011). Mostimportantly, it is now clearer how to arrive at a diagnosis of PD, andthe prototype matching approach for PD types has been dropped.

According to the latest version of the proposal (updated June 21,2011, on the DSM-5 Web site: http://www.dsm5.org/proposedrevision/Pages/PersonalityDisorders.aspx), the diagnostician is askedto first determine whether impairment in personality functioning ispresent and, if so, to what degree. The clinician is asked to rate apatient’s level of personality functioning; specifically, ratings aremade as to the level of self and interpersonal functioning for eachindividual (see also Livesley, 2001, 2003). Self functioning is defined intwo areas (identity and self-direction), as is interpersonal functioning(empathy and intimacy). A 5-point scale is used to rate overall level ofpersonality functioning for this purpose (0 = no impairment; 1 = mildimpairment; 2 = moderate impairment; 3 = serious impairment; and4 = extreme impairment). Descriptions of each quantitative rating are

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1705

provided, and the diagnostician is reminded that the ratings mustreflect functioning that is of multiple years in duration, not due solelyto another mental disorder/physical condition/effect of a substance,and not a norm within a person’s cultural background.

Next, if significant personality dysfunction is present, the clinicianconsiders whether one (or more?) of the six proposed personalitydisorder types are present: borderline, obsessive-compulsive,avoidant, schizotypal, antisocial, or narcissistic. For each of theseproposed PD types, a listing of characteristic trait elevations is pro-vided. Table 2 presents the current DSM-5 proposal for borderlinepersonality disorder, and one can see that for this PD type theclinician is asked to consider whether there are elevations on seventraits tapping negative affectivity (emotional lability, anxiousness,separation insecurity, and depressivity), disinhibition (impulsivity,risk taking), and antagonism (hostility). For obsessive-compulsive,the clinician considers two primary traits, for avoidant four primarytraits, for schizotypal six primary traits, for antisocial seven primarytraits, and for narcissistic two primary traits.

If a specific PD type is not indicated but personality dysfunctionis present, then the clinician may designate a diagnosis of personalitydisorder trait specified (PDTS), which replaces the DSM-IV-TR des-ignation of personality disorder not otherwise specified (PDNOS).Table 3 indicates how each of the 25 proposed DSM-5 PD primarytraits is mapped onto the PD types (as well as the “General definitionof personality disorder”), according to the latest proposal.

Of most relevance to the present article are the 25 traits, as well asthe higher-order trait domains, included in the DSM-5 PD proposal.The current proposal lists 25 maladaptive personality traits, orga-nized within five broad domains, which can be rated for each indi-vidual. The 25 facet traits are organized into five higher order traitdomains: Negative Affectivity (emotional lability, anxiousness,separation anxiety, perseveration, submissiveness, hostility, restrictedaffectivity [lack of], depressivity, and suspiciousness); Detachment(restricted affectivity, depressivity, suspiciousness, withdrawal, anhe-donia, and intimacy avoidance); Antagonism (hostility, manipulative-ness, deceitfulness, grandiosity, attention seeking, and callousness);Disinhibition (irresponsibility, impulsivity, rigid perfectionism[lack of], distractibility, and risk taking); and Psychoticism (unusualbeliefs and experiences, eccentricity, and cognitive and perceptualdysregulation).

Trull1706

Tab

le2

New

DSM

-5Pr

opos

al

for

Dia

gn

osin

gB

ord

erli

ne

Pers

ona

lity

Dis

ord

er(B

PD)

Adi

agno

sis

ofB

PD

requ

ires

allo

fth

efo

llow

ing:

•Si

gnifi

cant

impa

irm

ent

inP

erso

nalit

yF

unct

ioni

ngm

anif

est

by:

�Im

pair

men

tsin

self

func

tion

ing:

�Id

enti

ty:M

arke

dly

impo

veri

shed

,poo

rly

deve

lope

d,or

unst

able

self

-im

age,

ofte

nas

soci

ated

wit

hex

cess

ive

self

-cri

tici

sm;c

hron

icfe

elin

gsof

empt

ines

s;di

ssoc

iati

vest

ates

unde

rst

ress

�S

elf-

dire

ctio

n:In

stab

ility

ingo

als,

aspi

rati

ons,

valu

es,o

rca

reer

plan

s�

Impa

irm

ents

inin

terp

erso

nalf

unct

ioni

ng:

�E

mpa

thy:

Com

prom

ised

abili

tyto

reco

gniz

eth

efe

elin

gsan

dne

eds

ofot

hers

asso

ciat

edw

ith

inte

rper

sona

lhyp

erse

nsit

ivit

y(i

.e.,

pron

eto

feel

slig

hted

orin

sult

ed);

perc

epti

ons

ofot

hers

sele

ctiv

ely

bias

edto

war

dne

gati

veat

trib

utes

and

vuln

erab

iliti

es.

�In

tim

acy:

Inte

nse,

unst

able

,and

confl

icte

dcl

ose

rela

tion

ship

s,m

arke

dby

mis

trus

t,ne

edin

ess,

and

anxi

ous

preo

ccup

atio

nw

ith

real

orim

agin

edab

ando

nmen

t;cl

ose

rela

tion

ship

sof

ten

view

edin

extr

emes

orid

ealiz

atio

nan

dde

valu

atio

nan

dal

tern

atin

gbe

twee

nov

erin

volv

emen

tan

dw

ithd

raw

al.

•E

leva

ted

Per

sona

lity

Tra

its

inth

efo

llow

ing

dom

ains

:�

Neg

ativ

eaf

fect

ivit

ych

arac

teri

zed

by:

�E

mot

iona

llab

ility

:Uns

tabl

eem

otio

nale

xper

ienc

esan

dfr

eque

ntm

ood

chan

ges;

emot

ions

that

are

easi

lyar

ouse

d,in

tens

e,an

d/or

out

ofpr

opor

tion

toev

ents

and

circ

umst

ance

s.�

Anx

ious

ness

:Int

ense

feel

ings

ofne

rvou

snes

s,te

nsen

ess,

orpa

nic,

ofte

nin

reac

tion

toin

terp

erso

nals

tres

ses;

wor

ryab

out

the

nega

tive

even

tsof

past

unpl

easa

ntex

peri

ence

san

dfu

ture

nega

tive

poss

ibili

ties

;fee

ling

fear

ful,

appr

ehen

sive

,or

thre

aten

edby

unce

rtai

nty;

fear

sof

falli

ngap

art

orlo

sing

cont

rol.

�S

epar

atio

nin

secu

rity

:Fea

rsof

reje

ctio

nby

—an

d/or

sepa

rati

onfr

om—

sign

ifica

ntot

hers

,ass

ocia

ted

wit

hfe

ars

ofex

cess

ive

depe

nden

cyan

dco

mpl

ete

loss

ofau

tono

my.

�D

epre

ssiv

ity:

Fre

quen

tfe

elin

gsof

bein

gdo

wn,

mis

erab

le,a

nd/o

rho

pele

ss;d

iffic

ulty

reco

veri

ngfr

omsu

chm

oods

;pes

sim

ism

abou

tth

efu

ture

;pe

rvas

ive

sham

e;th

ough

tsof

suic

ide

and

suic

idal

beha

vior

.�

Dis

inhi

biti

on,c

hara

cter

ized

by:

�Im

puls

ivit

y:D

iffic

ulty

cont

rolli

ngbe

havi

or,i

nclu

ding

self

-har

mbe

havi

or,u

nder

emot

iona

ldis

tres

s;ac

ting

wit

hur

genc

yor

onth

esp

urof

the

mom

ent

inre

spon

seto

imm

edia

test

imul

i;ac

ting

onm

omen

tary

basi

sw

itho

uta

plan

orco

nsid

erat

ion

ofou

tcom

es;d

iffic

ulty

esta

blis

hing

orfo

llow

ing

plan

s.�

Ris

kta

king

:Eng

agem

ent

inda

nger

ous,

risk

y,an

dpo

tent

ially

self

-dam

agin

gac

tivi

ties

,unn

eces

sari

lyan

dw

itho

utre

gard

toco

nseq

uenc

es.

�A

ntag

onis

m,c

hara

cter

ized

by:

�H

osti

lity:

Per

sist

ent

orfr

eque

ntan

gry

feel

ings

;ang

eror

irri

tabi

lity

inre

spon

seto

min

orsl

ight

san

din

sult

s.

•T

heim

pair

men

tsin

pers

onal

ity

func

tion

ing

and

the

indi

vidu

al’s

pers

onal

ity

trai

tex

pres

sion

are

rela

tive

lyst

able

acro

ssti

me

and

cons

iste

ntac

ross

situ

atio

ns.

•T

heim

pair

men

tsin

pers

onal

ity

func

tion

ing

and

the

indi

vidu

al’s

pers

onal

ity

trai

tex

pres

sion

are

not

bett

erun

ders

tood

asno

rmat

ive

for

the

indi

vidu

al’s

deve

lopm

enta

lsta

geor

soci

o-cu

ltur

alen

viro

nmen

t.

•T

heim

pair

men

tsin

pers

onal

ity

func

tion

ing

and

the

indi

vidu

al’s

pers

onal

ity

trai

tex

pres

sion

are

not

sole

lydu

eto

the

dire

ctph

ysio

logi

cale

ffec

tsof

asu

bsta

nce

(e.g

.,a

drug

ofab

use,

med

icat

ion)

ora

gene

ralm

edic

alco

ndit

ion

(e.g

.,se

vere

head

trau

ma)

.

Sou

rce:

http

://w

ww

.dsm

5.or

g/P

ropo

sedR

evis

ion/

Pag

es/p

ropo

sedr

evis

ion.

aspx

?rid

=17.

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1707

Integration of the Work Group Proposal With the FFM

Table 4 provides an illustration of how the 25 trait scales proposed bythe DSM-5 Work Group would be classified within the FFM domains(also see Widiger, 2011). The placement of several of the 25 primary

Table 3Mapping of DSM-5 Personality Traits onto Personality

Disorder Types

DSM-5 Facet Trait BRD OBC AVD SZT ATS NAR GEN

Emotional Lability xAnxiousness x x xSeparation Anxiety xPerseveration xSubmissiveness xHostility x x xRestricted Affectivity xDepressivity xSuspiciousness xWithdrawal x x xAnhedonia xIntimacy Avoidance xManipulativeness xDeceitfulness xGrandiosity xAttention Seeking xCallousness xIrresponsibility xImpulsivity x x xRigid Perfectionism xDistractibility xRisk Taking x x xUnusual Beliefs

and Experiencesx

Eccentricity xCognitive and Perceptual

Dysregulationx

Note. BRD = borderline; OBC = obsessive-compulsive; AVD = avoidant; SZT =schizotypal; ATS = antisocial; NAR = narcissistic; GEN = general definition of per-sonality disorder.

Trull1708

traits by the DSM-5 Personality and Personality Disorders WorkGroup within certain personality domains appears inconsistent withthe personality literature. For example, the extensive research on theFFM indicates that suspiciousness (i.e., FFM low trust) is withinantagonism (not negative affectivity and detachment), submissiveness(i.e., FFM compliance) is within Agreeableness (not negative affec-tivity), histrionism/attention seeking (i.e., FFM gregariousness) iswithin Extraversion (not antagonism), and disinhibition andcompulsivity/rigid perfectionism are opposite ends of one bipolardimension (i.e., FFM Conscientiousness; Widiger, 2011).

Related to this latter point, although there is good evidence tosuggest that certain personality traits are bipolar in nature, theDSM-5 Work Group chose to only include unipolar facet traits.Although the Work Group acknowledged that the higher order traitdomains are bipolar in nature, they chose to include only unipolarmarkers of the extreme poles. The major consequence is that theinterpretation of a low score on a uniploar trait is ambiguous. Forexample, does a low score on emotional lability mean that one has a

Table 4Placement of the 25 DSM-5 Facet Traits Within the Domains of

the Five-Factor Model

FFM Domain DSM-5 Traits

Neuroticism-High Emotional Lability, Separation Insecurity,Depressivity, Anxiousness

Neuroticism-Low NoneExtraversion-High Attention SeekingExtraversion-Low Intimacy Avoidance, Withdrawal, Restricted

Affectivity, AnhedoniaOpenness-High Cognitive and Perceptual Dysregulation,

Unusual Beliefs and Experiences, EccentricityOpenness-Low NoneAgreeableness-High SubmissivenessAgreeableness-Low Suspiciousness, Hostility, Deceitfulness,

Manipulativeness, Grandiosity, CallousnessConscientiousness-High Perseveration, Rigid PerfectionismConscientiousness-Low Irresponsibility, Distractibility, Impulsivity, Risk

Taking

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1709

“normal” range level of emotionality or that one’s lack of emotion-ality is pathological (e.g., alexithymia; Luminet et al., 1999; Taylor &Bagby, 2004)? In addition, when viewed though an FFM lens, itappears that the DSM-5 trait model undersamples certain maladap-tive poles of personality trait domains. In particular, there are notraits that assess lower levels of Neuroticism, only one trait thatassesses high Extraversion, no traits for low Openness, and one traitfor high Agreeableness. Therefore, quite a few important traits arestill missing.

Table 5 presents a “translation” of the DSM-5 PD types into thepersonality traits from the DSM-5 personality model. For compari-son, this table also lists the FFM traits corresponding to each of thefive PDs, as proposed by Lynam & Widiger (2001) in their FFMPDtrait model. Once again, one is struck by the gaps in coverage for thePD types. For example, where is the fearlessness (low Neuroticism)that is crucial to the antisocial/psychopathy construct? Where is thealexithymia and closed-minded dogmatism (low Openness) thatcharacterizes obsessive-compulsive PD? Where is the meekness andmodesty that characterizes avoidant PD? In addition, several traitsare included within two trait domains without explanation (e.g.,restricted affectivity, depressivity, suspiciousness). Finally, some ofthe choices for DSM-5 traits seem odd: Anhedonia for avoidant PD?Why not submissiveness for avoidant PD?

These examples only concern the six PD types retained forDSM-5. Skodol (2010) argued that the personality disordersdeleted from the prior diagnostic manual (paranoid, schizoid,histrionic, dependent) can be recovered from the new traitmodel (see http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions/Pages/DSM-5TypeandTraitCross-Walk.aspx). However, the “gaps” in trait cov-erage are also apparent when one considers traits relevant todependent PD (high Agreeableness), paranoid and schizoid PD (lowOpenness), and histrionic PD (high Extraversion). Finally, it isunclear whether the four PDs excluded from DSM-5 are “recover-able” from the DSM-5 traits. Specifically, paranoid PD is character-ized by a relatively small number of traits (e.g., suspiciousness,hostility, unusual beliefs and experiences, intimacy avoidance), as istrue for schizoid (e.g., restricted affectivity, withdrawal, intimacyavoidance, anhedonia), histrionic (e.g., emotional lability, manipu-lativeness, attention seeking), and dependent (e.g., anxiousness,separation insecurity, submissiveness) PDs.

Trull1710

Tab

le5

Tra

nsl

ati

onof

PDTy

pes

/Tra

its

Into

FFM

PDTr

ait

s

Per

sona

lity

Dis

orde

rD

SM

-5T

ype

Tra

its

FF

MP

DT

rait

s1

Schi

zoty

pal

Ecc

entr

icit

y(P

),C

ogni

tive

and

Per

cept

ual

Dys

regu

lati

on(P

),U

nusu

alB

elie

fsan

dE

xper

ienc

es(P

),W

ithd

raw

al(D

T),

Res

tric

ted

Aff

ecti

vity

(DT

and

NA

),Su

spic

ious

ness

(NA

)

Idea

s(O

),lo

wW

arm

th(E

),lo

wG

rega

riou

snes

s(E

),lo

wP

osit

ive

Em

otio

ns(E

),A

nxio

usne

ss(N

),Se

lf-C

onsc

ious

ness

(N),

low

Ord

er(C

)

Ant

isoc

ial/

Dys

soci

alC

allo

usne

ss(A

),H

osti

lity

(NA

and

A),

Man

ipul

ativ

enes

s(A

),D

ecei

tful

ness

(A),

Irre

spon

sibi

lity

(DS)

,Im

puls

ivit

y(D

S),R

isk

Tak

ing

(DS)

Low

Tru

st(A

),lo

wSt

raig

htfo

rwar

dnes

s(A

),lo

wA

ltru

ism

(A),

low

Com

plia

nce

(A),

low

Mod

esty

(A),

low

Ten

der-

min

dedn

ess

(A),

low

Dut

iful

ness

(C),

low

Self

-Dis

cipl

ine

(C),

low

Del

iber

atio

n(C

),lo

wA

nxio

usne

ss(N

),A

ngry

Hos

tilit

y(N

),lo

wSe

lf-C

onsc

ious

ness

(N),

Impu

lsiv

enes

s(N

),A

sser

tive

ness

(E),

Act

ivit

y(E

),E

xcit

emen

tSe

ekin

g(E

)B

orde

rlin

eE

mot

iona

lLab

ility

(NA

),A

nxio

usne

ss(N

A),

Sepa

rati

onIn

secu

rity

(NA

),H

osti

lity

(NA

),D

epre

ssiv

ity

(NA

),Im

puls

ivit

y(D

S),R

isk

Tak

ing

(DS)

Anx

ious

ness

(N),

Ang

ryH

osti

lity

(N),

Dep

ress

iven

ess

(N),

Impu

lsiv

enes

s(N

),V

ulne

rabi

lity

(N),

low

Com

plia

nce

(A),

low

Com

pete

nce

(C),

low

Del

iber

atio

n(C

),F

eelin

gs(O

),A

ctio

ns(O

)

Avo

idan

tA

nxio

usne

ss(N

A),

Wit

hdra

wal

(DT

),A

nhed

onia

(DT

),In

tim

acy

Avo

idan

ce(D

T)

Anx

ious

ness

(N),

Self

-Con

scio

usne

ss(N

),lo

wIm

puls

iven

ess

(N),

Vul

nera

bilit

y(N

),lo

wG

rega

riou

snes

s(E

),lo

wA

sser

tive

ness

(E),

low

Exc

item

ent

Seek

ing

(E),

low

Pos

itiv

eE

mot

ions

(E),

Mod

esty

(A),

Act

ions

(O)

Obs

essi

ve-

com

puls

ive

Rig

idP

erfe

ctio

nism

(DS)

,Per

seve

rati

on(N

A)

Com

pete

nce

(C),

Ord

er(C

),D

utif

ulne

ss(C

),A

chie

vem

ent

Stri

ving

(C),

Self

-Dis

cipl

ine

(C),

Del

iber

atio

n(C

),A

nxio

usne

ss(N

),lo

wIm

puls

iven

ess

(N),

low

Exc

item

ent

Seek

ing

(E),

low

Fee

lings

(O),

low

Act

ions

(O),

low

Idea

s(O

),lo

wV

alue

s(O

)N

arci

ssis

tic

Gra

ndio

sity

(A),

Att

enti

onSe

ekin

g(A

)A

ngry

Hos

tilit

y(N

),lo

wSe

lf-C

onsc

ious

ness

(N),

low

War

mth

(E),

Ass

erti

vene

ss(E

),E

xcit

emen

tSe

ekin

g(E

),lo

wF

eelin

gs(O

),A

ctio

ns(O

),lo

wT

rust

(A),

low

Stra

ight

forw

ardn

ess

(A),

low

Alt

ruis

m(A

),lo

wC

ompl

ianc

e(A

),lo

wM

odes

ty(A

),lo

wT

ende

r-m

inde

dnes

s(A

)

Not

e.N

A=

Neg

ativ

eA

ffec

tivi

ty;

DT

=D

etac

hmen

t;A

=A

ntag

onis

m;

DS

=D

isin

hibi

tion

;P

=P

sych

otic

ism

;N

=N

euro

tici

sm;

E=

Ext

rave

rsio

n;O

=O

penn

ess;

A=

Agr

eeab

lene

ss;C

=C

onsc

ient

ious

ness

.1 F

FM

PDpr

edic

ted

rela

tions

base

don

Lyn

aman

dW

idig

er(2

001)

.

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1711

These problems perhaps reflect that the traits selected were in partthe result of Work Group member nominations (Krueger, 2010),rather than a systematic, comprehensive attempt to adequatelysample major domains of personality (and to do so as distinctly aspossible). Many have decried the overlapping PD constructs thathave appeared in previous diagnostic manuals, as evidenced by thehigh levels of comorbidity (Clark, 2007; Livesley, 2007; Widiger &Trull, 2007). However, many of the 25 traits appear to be closelyrelated and perhaps difficult to distinguish (e.g., separationinsecurity and anxiousness; perseveration and rigid perfectionism;cognitive and perceptual dysregulation and unusual beliefs andexperiences or eccentricity). Therefore, the new proposal does notseem to adequately address the concern regarding overlap andredundancy.

CONCERNS AND COMMENTS ON THE FFMPD

Although the evidence supporting the use of the FFM to characterizeand diagnose personality pathology is strong, there are some criti-cisms and limitations that should be noted as well. In particular,Krueger et al. (2011) argued that there is much confusion over exactlywhat constitutes the FFM. In particular, they highlighted that thereare some differences in the conceptual models of the FFM as opera-tionalized by the various FFM and Big Five measures that are avail-able. Although it is true that there are differences in these measures,we must not conflate measures with conceptual models (Haigler &Widiger, 2001; Krueger et al., 2011). A measure is simply an opera-tionalization of the conceptual model, and some measures mayemphasize certain aspects of the model over others. Further, in thecase of briefer measures, it is sometimes not possible to adequatelysample all aspects of a conceptual model with precision; for example,some brief FFM/Big Five measures do not adequately sample thefacets or primary traits of the five major personality domains of theFFM. In the end, however, Krueger and colleagues’ (2011) argumentcould be applied to other measures of alternative personality models.Ironically, the 25-trait model of personality disorder adopted by theDSM-5 Personality and Personality Disorders Work Group suffersfrom a more damning problem—it is not based on a well-establishedmodel of personality or personality pathology at all. Although the

Trull1712

Work Group noted that it bears some resemblance to the PersonalityPsychopathology Five (PSY-5; Harkness, McNulty, & Ben-Porath,1995), this appears to be more of a post hoc observation instead of theoutcome of a theory-driven approach to measurement.

Although four of the five domains are recognizable from theFFM, the Work Group grafted a “new” personality dimension ontofour of the five personality domains included in the FFM. Specifi-cally, the Work Group included the new domain of Schizotypy,which is now labeled Psychoticism. DSM-5 Psychoticism is definedby only three facet traits, all of which emphasize the quasi-psychoticexperiences and behavior of schizotypal PD. Although there is onlylimited support to date for this “new” personality dimension inclinical samples, when this fifth personality domain is conceptualizedas FFM Openness, there is a wealth of supporting data from clinicalsamples (see Samuel & Widiger, 2008).

One of the strongest arguments being raised against the FFMPD,as well as other dimensional models, is clinical utility (e.g., First,2005). However, the implication of such an argument is that theexisting diagnostic system has clinical utility (Kupfer, First, &Regier, 2002; Rounsaville et al., 2002; Trull & Widiger, 2008; Westen& Arkowitz-Westen, 1998). Although on the surface it might appearthat a categorical diagnosis of PD is congruent with the clinicaldecisions that must be made (i.e., whether to hospitalize, whetherto medicate, whether to provide disability, and whether to provideinsurance coverage), DSM-IV (or even DSM-5) PD diagnoses do notseem suitable for any of these decisions. The diagnostic boundariesand thresholds were not developed with these clinical decisions inmind. Dimensional systems can easily be converted to categoricalsystems, provided appropriate cut-offs are available as well as deci-sion algorithms (Trull & Widiger, 2008).

We believe that a dimensional model of classification has consid-erably greater potential to be clinically useful because one can setdifferent cutoff points along the respective dimensions that areoptimal for different clinical decisions. One can identify the levelof emotional instability that suggests the need for insurance cov-erage, pharmacotherapy, hospitalization, or disability. The diag-nostic system would be constructed explicitly for maximizingutility for different clinical decisions, an approach that is currentlynon-existent and very cumbersome (if not impossible) to imple-

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1713

ment for the existing diagnostic categories. (Trull & Widiger,2008, pp. 958–959)

Although there are published treatment manuals for many DSM-IV-TR disorders, official treatment guidelines have been developedfor only one personality disorder: borderline (APA, 2001). Oneexplanation is that the DSM-IV-TR PDs are generally not well suitedfor treatment manuals because each PD involves an array of mal-adaptive personality traits, behaviors, and other indicators of thesetraits. Furthermore, there is great heterogeneity within diagnoses(e.g., patients diagnosed with the same PD may share few of the sametraits), and traits may cut across different PD categories (e.g., impul-sivity for both borderline PD and antisocial PD).

In contrast, the FFM appears much better suited to develop spe-cific treatment plans and guidelines. First, because the model wasdeveloped iteratively, the FFM is a more conceptually (as well asempirically) coherent personality structure. For example, by defi-nition, personality disorders are diagnosed when the maladaptivepersonality traits result in “clinical significant distress or impair-ment in social, occupational, or other important areas of function-ing” (APA, 2000, p. 689). The FFM model assesses traits relevantto emotional dysregulation/distress (e.g., Neuroticism traits), tosocial/interpersonal (e.g., Extraversion and Agreeableness traits), towork-oriented behavior and responsibility (e.g., Conscientiousnesstraits), and to cognitive (e.g., Openness traits) domains of function-ing. Further, Miller, Pilkonis, and Mulvey (2006) demonstratedthat FFM traits provided unique information about treatmentutilization and satisfaction above and beyond what could beaccounted for by general distress and by the DSM-IV-TR PDsthemselves.

FFM traits may also have implications for treatment choices aswell as within-treatment behavior. Extraversion and Agreeablenessare relevant to relationship quality both within and outside the con-sultation room. Interpersonal models of therapy, marital and familytherapy, and group therapy might be suggested by maladaptive vari-ants of Extraversion and Agreeableness traits. Neuroticism traits arerelevant to mood, anxiety, and emotional dyscontrol, and problemshere might suggest pharmacologic interventions (e.g., mood stabiliz-ers) or cognitive interventions targeting these dysfunctions. In thecase of Conscientiousness, maladaptively high levels might suggest

Trull1714

treatments that target obsessionality or compulsivity (e.g., exposureplus response prevention or SSRIs). In contrast, low levels mightsuggest treatments for attention deficit, hyperactivity, and impulsiv-ity (e.g., ADHD stimulant medication). Finally, maladaptively highOpenness could indicate cognitive-perceptual aberrations and prob-lems with reality testing, conditions that might be amenable to phar-macologic treatment (e.g., low-dose antipsychotic medication).

These examples are offered to show the treatment implications ofan FFM of personality disorder, and that these are substantiallymore distinct and specific than those for the DSM-IV-TR personalitydisorders. However, an important question is whether cliniciansactually find FFM information useful in their clinical formulations.Several studies have provided encouraging findings. For example,Samuel and Widiger (2006) provided practicing psychologists withdetailed descriptions of actual persons with maladaptive personalitytraits (e.g., Ted Bundy). When asked to describe the person withrespect to the FFM and, alternatively, with the DSM-IV-TR person-ality disorders, clinicians indicated that an FFM dimensional ratingwas more useful than the DSM-IV-TR with respect to providing aglobal description of the individual’s personality, communicatinginformation to clients, encompassing all of the individual’s impor-tant personality difficulties, and even assisting in formulating effec-tive treatment interventions.

Another concern raised regarding the FFMPD is that it is cum-bersome and not user-friendly. However, as detailed above, anFFMPD evaluation can proceed in logical steps, and evaluatingindividuals’ standing on 30 trait facets is much easier and moretime-efficient than assessing almost 100 DSM-IV-TR PD criteria orthan assessing the 25 DSM-5 traits, six PD types, and four variantsof personality functioning called for in the current DSM-5 WorkGroup proposal (Trull & Widiger, 1997; Widiger & Coker, 2002;Widiger & Lowe, 2007).

CONCLUSION

For years, researchers have called for a switch to a dimensionalmodel of personality disorders (Clark, 2007; Livesley, 2007; Widiger& Frances, 1985; Widiger & Trull, 2007), and there have even beenreferences to dimensional models within the text of the DSM-IV-TR

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1715

(APA, 2000), the most recent diagnostic manual. The time seemedripe for making this change, but, regrettably, the DSM-5 WorkGroup’s proposal for a trait-based PD system falls flat for severalreasons (Widiger, 2011). First, the Work Group had the opportunityto integrate normal personality trait models researched within psy-chology with the personality disorders of psychiatry. Instead, theydecided to construct a “new” five-dimensional model of personalitydisorder. This five-dimensional model excludes normal personalitytraits, does not incorporate years of research on personality andpersonality pathology, and is operationalized in ways so as to missimportant areas of personality functioning.

The DSM-5 Personality and Personality Disorders Work Group’sproposal includes 25 maladaptive personality traits that differen-tially saturate major domains of personality, that are unipolar innature, and that overlap excessively with each other. As notedearlier, the failure to recognize the bipolarity of personality structurecontributes to a number of problems for the DSM-5 proposal,including the failure to include some important traits and the mis-placement of others. Furthermore, as discussed, this 25-trait modeldoes not adequately cover the personality pathology that is assessedin the proposed six PD types as well as that of the excluded PDs fromDSM-IV-TR (the latter which can purportedly be accounted for bythis model).

In the end, the FFMPD seems better suited for the task of repre-senting personality and personality disorder than the DSM-5 WorkGroup 25-trait model. The FFMPD has a strong conceptual andempirical base, its relationship to PD diagnoses has been demon-strated, and it can also point to areas of personality dysfunction thatare not currently represented in diagnostic systems (e.g., racism,alexithymia; Bell, 2004, 2006; Luminet et al., 1999; Taylor & Bagby,2004; Trull, 2005). In addition, the FFMPD holds promise in itsability to inform and guide treatment, as well as in other areas ofclinical utility.

REFERENCES

Allik, J. (2005). Personality dimensions across cultures. Journal of PersonalityDisorders, 19, 212–232.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual ofmental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Trull1716

American Psychiatric Association. (2001). Practice guidelines for the treatment ofpatients with borderline personality disorder. Washington, DC: Author.

Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2001). A theoretical basis for the major dimensions ofpersonality. European Journal of Personality, 15, 327–353.

Bell, C. C. (2004). Taking issue: Racism. A mental illness? Psychiatric Services, 55,1343.

Bell, C. C. (2006). Reservations and hopes. In T. A. Widiger, E. Simonsen, P. J.Sirovatka, & D. A. Regier (Eds.), Dimensional models of personality disorders:Refining the research agenda for DSM-V (pp. 195–198). Washington, DC:American Psychiatric Association.

Bouchard, T. J., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality.Behavior Genetics, 31, 243–273.

Clark, L. A. (2007). Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: Perennialissues and an emerging reconceptualization. Annual Review of Psychology, 58,227–257.

Clark, L. A., & Livesley, W. J. (2002). Two approaches to identifying the dimen-sions of personality disorder: Convergence on the five-factor model. In P. T.Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model ofpersonality (2nd ed., pp. 161–176). Washington, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEOPI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual.Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Costa, P.T., Jr., & T. A. Widiger. (2002). Personality disorders and the five-factormodel of personality (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.

De Raad, B., & Perugini, M. (Eds.). (2002). Big Five assessment. Bern, Switzer-land: Hogrefe & Huber.

Few, L. R., Miller, J. D., Morse, J. Q., Yaggi, K. E., Reynold, S. K.., & Pilkonis,P. A. (2010). Examining the reliability and validity of clinician ratings on theFive-Factor Model Score Sheet. Assessment, 17, 440–453.

First, M. B. (2005). Clinical utility: A prerequisite for the adoption of adimensional approach in DSM. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114,560–564.

Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. AmericanPsychologist, 48, 26–34.

Haigler, E. D., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Experimental manipulation of NEO-PI-Ritems. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 339–358.

Harkness, A. R., McNulty, J. L., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1995). The PersonalityPsychopathology-Five: Constructs and MMPI-2 scales. Psychological Assess-ment, 7, 104–114.

John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History,measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.),Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). NewYork: Guilford Press.

Krueger, R. F. (2010, March). Personality traits: Prospects for DSM-V. In C. J.Hopwood (Chair), Personality assessment in context of DSM-V. Symposium

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1717

conducted at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality Assessment,San Jose, CA.

Krueger, R. F., Eaton, N. R., Clark, L. A., Watson, D., Markon, K. E., Derrin-ger, J., et al. (2011). Deriving an empirical structure of personality pathologyfor DSM-5. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25, 170–191.

Kupfer, D. J., First, M. B., & Regier, D. E. (2002). Introduction. In D. J. Kupfer,M. B. First, & D. E. Regier (Eds.), A research agenda for DSM-V (pp. xv–xxiii).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Livesley, W. J. (2001). Conceptual and taxonomic issues. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.),Handbook of personality disorders: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 3–38).New York: Guilford Press.

Livesley, W. J. (2003). Diagnostic dilemmas in classifying personality disorder. InK. A. Phillips, M. B. First, & H. A. Pincus (Eds.), Advancing DSM: Dilemmasin psychiatric diagnosis (pp. 153–190). Washington, DC: American PsychiatricAssociation.

Livesley, W. J. (2007). A framework for integrating dimensional and categoricalclassification of personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 199–224.

Livesley, W. J. (2010). General Assessment of Personality Dysfunction. Port Huron,MI: Sigma Assessments Systems.

Luminet, O., Bagby, R. M., Wagner, H., Taylor, G. J., Parker, J. D. A., & James,D. A. (1999). Relation between alexithymia and the five-factor model of per-sonality: A facet-level analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73, 345–358.

Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2001). Using the five factor model to representthe DSM-IV personality disorders: An expert consensus approach. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 110, 401–412.

Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure ofnormal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 88, 139–157.

McCrae, R. R., Löckenhoff, C. E., & Costa, P. T. (2005). A step toward DSM-V:Cataloguing personality-related problems in living. European Journal of Per-sonality, 19, 269–286.

Miller, J. D. (2012). Five-factor model personality disorder prototypes: A reviewof their development, validity, and comparison to alternative approaches.Journal of Personality, 80, 1565–1591.

Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2003). Psychopathy and the five-factor model ofpersonality: A replication and extension. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81,168–178.

Miller, J. D., Pilkonis, P. A., & Mulvey, E. P. (2006). Treatment utilizationand satisfaction: Examining the contributions of axis II psychopathologyand the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20,369–387.

Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Jamerson, J. E., Samuel, D. B., Olson, D. R., & Widiger, T.A. (2006). Psychometric properties of an abbreviated instrument of the five-factor model. Assessment, 13, 119–137.

Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., & Widiger, T. A. (2006). The five-factor model of person-ality disorder: A translation across science and practice. In R. F. Krueger &

Trull1718

J. L. Tackett (Eds.), Personality and psychopathology (pp. 39–70). New York:Guilford Press.

O’Connor, B. P., & Dyce, J. A. (1998). A test of models of personality disorderconfiguration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 3–16.

Rounsaville, B. J., Alarcon, R. D., Andrews, G., Jackson, J. S., Kendell, R. E., &Kendler, K. (2002). Basic nomenclature issues for DSM-V. In D. J. Kupfer,M. B. First, & D. E. Regier (Eds.), A research agenda for DSM-V (pp. 1–29).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2006). Clinicians’ judgments of clinical utility: Acomparison of the DSM-IV and five factor models. Journal of Abnormal Psy-chology, 115, 298–308.

Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2008). A meta-analytic review of the relation-ships between the five-factor model and DSM-VI-TR personality disorders: Afacet level analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1326–1342.

Saulsman, L. M., & Page, A. C. (2004). The five-factor model and personalitydisorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review. Clinical PsychologyReview, 23, 1055–1085.

Skodol, A. (2010, February 10). Rationale for proposing five specific personalitytypes. Retrieved from http://www.dsm5.org/ProposedRevisions/Pages/RationaleforProposingFiveSpecificPersonalityDisorderTypes.aspx

Skodol, A. E., Bender, D. S., Morey, L. C., Clark, L. A., Oldham, J. M., Alarcon,R. D., et al. (2011). Personality disorder types proposed for DSM-5. Journal ofPersonality Disorders, 25, 136–169.

Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2004). New trends in alexithymia research. Psy-chotherapy and Psychosomatics, 73, 68–77.

Trull, T. J. (1992). DSM-III-R personality disorders and the five-factor model ofpersonality: An empirical comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,553–560.

Trull, T. J. (2005). Dimensional models of personality disorder: Coverage andcutoffs. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 262–282.

Trull, T. J., & Durrett, C. A. (2005). Categorical and dimensional modelsof personality disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 355–380.

Trull, T. J., Tomko, R. L., Brown, W. C., & Scheiderer, E. M. (2010). Borderlinepersonality disorder in 3-D: Dimensions, symptoms, and measurement chal-lenges. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 11, 1057–1069.

Trull, T. J., & Widiger, T. A. (1997). Structured Interview for the Five Factor Modelof personality. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Trull, T. J., & Widiger, T. A. (2008). Geology 102: More thoughts on a shift to adimensional model of personality disorders. Social and Personality PsychologyCompass, 2, 949–967.

Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., & Burr, R. (2001). A structured interview for theassessment of the five factor model of personality: Facet level relations to theaxis II personality disorders. Journal of Personality, 69, 175–198.

Trull, T. J., Widiger, T. A., Lynam, D. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2003). Borderlinepersonality disorder from the perspective of general personality functioning.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 193–202.

Five-Factor Model of Personality Disorder and DSM-5 1719

Verheul, R., Andrea, H., Berghout, C. C., Dolan, C., Busschbach, J. J. V., van derKroft, P. J. A., et al. (2008). Severity Indices of Personality Problems (SIPP-118): Development, factor structure, reliability, and validity. PsychologicalAssessment, 20, 23–34.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Harkness, A. R. (1994). Structures of personality andtheir relevance to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103,18–31.

Westen, D., & Arkowitz-Westen, L. (1998). Limitations of axis II in diagnosingpersonality pathology in clinical practice. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155,1767–1771.

Widiger, T. A. (2011). The DSM-5 dimensional model of personality disorder:Rationale and empirical support. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25, 222–234.

Widiger, T. A., & Coker, L. A. (2002). Assessing personality disorders. In J. N.Butcher (Ed.), Clinical personality assessment: Practical approaches (2nd ed.,pp. 407–434). New York: Oxford University Press.

Widiger, T. A., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (2002). Five-factor model personality disorderresearch. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders andthe five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 59–87). Washington, DC:American Psychological Association.

Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A proposal for axis II:Diagnosing personality disorders using the five factor model. In P. T. Costa,Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five factor model ofpersonality (2nd ed., pp. 431–456). Washington, DC: American PsychologicalAssociation.

Widiger, T. A., & Frances, A. (1985). The DSM-III personality disorders: Per-spectives from psychology. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 615–623.

Widiger, T. A., & Lowe, J. (2007). Five factor model assessment of personalitydisorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 89, 16–29.

Widiger, T. A., & Mullins-Sweatt, S. N. (2009). Five-factor model of personalitydisorder: A proposal for DSM-V. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5,115–138.

Widiger, T. A., & Simonsen, E. (2005). Alternative dimensional models of per-sonality disorder: Finding a common ground. Journal of Personality Disorders,19, 110–130.

Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (2007). Plate tectonics in the classification of per-sonality disorder: Shifting to a dimensional model. American Psychologist, 62,71–83.

Widiger, T., Trull, T., Costa, P., McCrae, R., Clarkin, J. F., & Sanderson, C.(1994). Description of the DSM-III-R and DSM-IV personality disorders withthe five factor model of personality. In P. Costa & T. Widiger (Eds.), Person-ality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 41–56). Washington,DC: American Psychological Association.

Widiger, T. A., Trull, T., Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R., Clarkin, J. F., & Sander-son, C. (2002). Description of the DSM-IV personality disorders with the fivefactor model of personality. In P. T. Costa, Jr., & T. A. Widiger (Eds.),Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp.89–99). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Trull1720