35
Survival Analysis and the ACT study Laura Gibbons, PhD Thanks to An Introduction to Survival Analysis Using Stata

Survival Analysis and the ACT study

  • Upload
    elwyn

  • View
    40

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Survival Analysis and the ACT study. Laura Gibbons, PhD Thanks to An Introduction to Survival Analysis Using Stata. Acknowledgement. Funded in part by Grant R13 AG030995 from the National Institute on Aging - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Survival Analysis and the ACT study

Laura Gibbons, PhD

Thanks to An Introduction to Survival Analysis Using Stata

Page 2: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Acknowledgement

• Funded in part by Grant R13 AG030995 from the National Institute on Aging

• The views expressed in written conference materials or publications and by speakers and moderators do not necessarily reflect the official policies of the Department of Health and Human Services; nor does mention by trade names, commercial practices, or organizations imply endorsement by the U.S. Government.

Page 3: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

What is survival analysis?

• Time to event data.

• It’s not just a question of who gets demented, but when.

• Event, survive, and fall are generic terms.

Page 4: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

ACT example

Risk for Late-life Re-injury, Dementia, and Death Among Individuals with Traumatic Brain Injury: A population-based study

Kristen Dams-O’Connor, Laura E Gibbons, James D Bowen, Susan M McCurry, Eric B Larson,

Paul K Crane.

J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013 Feb;84(2):177-82.

Page 5: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

TBI-LOC = Traumatic Brain Injury with Loss of Consciousness

Outcomes (different ways of defining failure)• TBI-LOC during follow-up

• Dementia

• Death

Page 6: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Survival function• The number who survive out of the total number at risk• In this example, “failure” is a TBI-LOC after baseline.• 4225 participants, with 96 TBI-LOC after baseline.

0.80

0.90

1.00

Sur

viva

l Fun

ctio

n

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

Years since baseline

No TBI-LOC before baselineTBI-LOC before baseline

Page 7: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Hazard function

• The probability of failing given survival until this time (currently at risk)

• The hazard function reflects the hazard at each time point.

• It’s usually easier to look at the cumulative hazard graph.

Page 8: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Cumulative Hazard for TBI with LOC(with 95% confidence bands)

0

.05

.1

.15

.2

Cum

ulat

ive

Haz

ard

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

Years since baseline

TBI-LOC before baselineNo TBI-LOC before baseline

Page 9: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Think carefully about onset of time at risk

• Study entry

• Time-dependent covariates

• What to do about exposures which occur before study entry (left truncation)

Page 10: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

ACT: TBI-LOC during follow-up

• Used study entry as onset of time at risk

• Exposure: report of first TBI-LOC at baselineNone (n = 3619)

At age<25 (n=371)

At age 25-54 (n=104)

At age 55 to baseline (n=131)

• No time-dependent covariates for this example

Page 11: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Time axis

Continuous – exact failure time is known

Discrete – time interval for failure is known

Page 12: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

ACT onsetdate for dementia outcomes

The midpoint between the two study visits (biennial and/or annual) that precede the date of the consensus of dementia. The date of the consensus of dementia is defined as the earliest consensus that resulted in a positive diagnosis of dementia (DSMIV) and that was not later reversed as a false positive.

Page 13: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Age as the time axis• Makes sense in an aging study.

• Often modeled as baseline age + time.

Ties • Multiple events occurring at the same time.

• Make sure your software is handling this the way you want.

Page 14: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Think carefully about censoring

• Censoring: The event time is unknown

• No longer at risk

• Missing data – random or informative?

• Hope it’s noninformative [Distribution of censoring times is independent of event times, conditional on covariates. ~ MAR.]

Page 15: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Right censoring Event is unobserved due to • Drop out• Study end• Competing event (more on this later)

Interval censoring• Know it occurs between visits, but not when• Assume failure time is uniformly distributed in that

interval• An issue in ACT (hence onsetdate)

Page 16: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Left censoring The event occurred before the study began.

• What about those whose TBI-LOC resulted in death or dementia before age 65? They are not in our study.

• Worry about this one.

Left truncation Onset of risk was before study entry.

• We used our 4-category exposure, but risk really must be defined as “TBI-LOC before age 25 and not left-censored”, etc.

Page 17: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

ACT censoring variables

• Competing event: onsetdate (dementia) or

• Visit date (visitdt)

• Withdrawal date (withdrawdt)

(The FH data does not include anyone who withdrew.)

• Death date (deathdt)

Page 18: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Modeling

Non parametric – Kaplan-Meier

0.80

0.90

1.00

Sur

viva

l Fun

ctio

n

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

Years since baseline

No TBI-LOC before baselineTBI-LOC before baseline

Page 19: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Log-rank test for equality of survivor functions  | Events Eventsp | observed expected---------------------------+-------------------------No TBI-LOC before baseline | 66 82.70TBI-LOC before baseline | 30 13.30---------------------------+-------------------------Total | 96 96.00  chi2(1) = 24.44 Pr>chi2 = 0.0000

Page 20: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Semi-parametric (Cox)Assumes the hazards are proportional

0.00

0.05

0.10

0.15C

umul

ativ

e H

azar

d

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

Years since baseline

TBI-LOC before baselineNo TBI-LOC before baseline

Looks like a reasonable assumption here, but we looked at a variety of graphs and statistics to make sure.

Page 21: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Hazard Ratios

Baseline report of age at first TBI with LOC as a predictor of TBI with LOC after study enrolment, controlling for age, sex, and years of education. Age at first TBI Late life TBI with LOCwith LOC cases/person years HR (95% CI)None prior to baseline 66/21,945 1 (Reference)< 25 15/2147 2.54 (1.42, 4.52)25-54 6/678 3.24 (1.40, 7.52)55-baseline 9/798 3.79 (1.89, 7.62)

Page 22: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Model checking

• Proportional hazards assumption

• Covariate form

• Baseline, lag or current visit covariate

• Et cetera

Page 23: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Parametric

Can be proportional hazard models

• Exponential. Constant baseline hazard.

• Weibull. Hazard is monotone increasing or decreasing, depending on the values for a and b.

• Gompertz. Hazard rates increase or decrease exponentially over time.

• See Flexible Parametric Survival Analysis Using Stata for many more.

Page 24: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Accelerated failure time

• Risk is not constant over time.

• Time ratios. Ratios > 1 indicate LONGER survival.

Page 25: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Types of accelerated failure time(AFT) models

• Gamma. 3 parameter. Most flexible. Fit a gamma model and see which parameters are relevant.

• Exponential, Weibull can also be formulated as AFT models. In the Weibull model, the risk increases over time when β > 1.

• Log-normal. The hazard increases and then decreases.

• Log-logistic. Very similar to log-normal.

Page 26: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Baseline report of TBI-LOC and the risk of dementia

• Proportional hazards assumption not tenable.

• The log-logistic AFT model was the winner, reflecting an increased risk over time.

• You can compare AICs to pick best model, or pick one based on your hypothesis.

Page 27: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Our AFT model for any dementia

• Controlling for baseline age, gender and any APOE-4 alleles

• Remember that TR > 1 => longer survival

Page 28: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

------------------------------------------------ _t | TR [95% Conf. Interval]-------------+---------------------------------- base4 | <25 | 1.02 0.87 1.20 25-54 | 1.04 0.78 1.38 55+ | 1.06 0.81 1.39 |10 years age | 0.53 0.49 0.57 female | 1.15 1.05 1.26 education | 1.02 1.00 1.04 apoe4 | 0.69 0.63 0.76------------------------------------------------ 

TBI-LOC is NS. Older baseline age and APOE associated with shorter survival. Female and education associated with longer survival.

Page 29: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Competing risks

• Individuals are at-risk for AD, vascular dementia, other dementias

• No longer at risk for one type once diagnosed with another (assuming we’re dealing with first diagnosis)

• Use cause-specific hazard functions and cumulative incidence functions.

Page 30: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

What is going on in competing risks? • Which is it?

• One process determines dementia and another says which type

• Two separate processes, and one event censors the other.• In our analysis of TBI-LOC predicting AD, we censored at

other dementia diagnoses, but we could have modeled multiple dementia outcomes (assuming adequate numbers).

• The competing risk model may be more accurate, because the time to AD and the time to other dementia are probably correlated.

Page 31: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Shared frailty, aka unobserved heterogeneity or random-effects

• There may be variability in individuals’ underlying (baseline) risk for an event that is not directly measurable.

• One way of dealing with patients from different cities, for example.

• The assumption is that the effect is random and multiplicative on the hazard function.

Page 32: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

• Need to distinguish between hazard for individuals and the population average.

• Population hazard can fall while the individual hazards rise.

• The frailer individuals have failed already, so the overall hazard rate drops. Yet time is passing, so each person’s risk is still rising.

Page 33: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

• In a shared frailty model, HR estimates are for time 0.

• Covariate effects decrease as the frail fail.

• Gamma frailty models. Covariate effects completely disappear over time.

• Inverse-Gaussian models. Covariate effects decrease but do not disappear over time.

Page 34: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Other issues in survival analysis not covered today include

• Events that can occur more than once (heart attacks, for example)

• Parallel processes • Unshared frailty

Page 35: Survival Analysis  and  the ACT study

Questions, discussion