Sullivan 98

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    1/36

    Leisure, Tastes and Gender in Britain:

    Changes from the 1960s to the 1990s*

    Tally Katz-Gerro and Oriel Sullivan

    University of Haifa Ben-Gurion University

    September 2002

    * Paper under review please do not quote without permission. Please address

    correspondence to Tally Katz-Gerro, Department of Sociology and Anthropology,

    University of Haifa, Haifa 31905, Israel, [email protected] or Oriel Sullivan,

    Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ben-Gurion University, Beer Sheva 84105, Israel,

    [email protected]

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    2/36

    2

    Leisure, Tastes and Gender in Britain:

    Changes from the 1960s to the 1990s

    Abstract

    This paper discusses various conceptual ways of linking gender, leisure and tastes

    and offers a cross-time analysis of gender differences in the time allocated to different

    leisure activities in Britain. We askwhether men and women have similar leisure

    participation patterns over time and whether we can identify changes in the relative

    distribution of leisure activities between men and women. We discuss Bourdieus theory

    of distinction and Simmels trickle down theory as possible explanations of differences in

    gender trends over time. We find some trends in participation which may be described in

    terms of Simmels trickle-down theory (for example, increasing participation in sports

    activities among women), some which support Bourdieus theory of distinction (for

    example, mens participation in electronic media) and some which cannot adequately be

    described by either.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    3/36

    3

    Introduction

    This paper discusses various conceptual ways of linking gender, leisure and tastes

    and offers a longitudinal analysis of gender differences in the time allocated to different

    leisure activities in Britain. The consumption of cultural products and cultural activities

    such as leisure reflects tastes that embody symbolic abilities and resources. For the

    purposes of empirical testing our approach therefore regards tastes as being displayed

    through participation in different fields of practice (Harvey et al. 2001), in this case

    participation in specific leisure activities. Leisure activities in our analysisare drawn from

    the areas of entertainment, socializing, travel, sport and home-based leisure, representing

    both highbrow and popular leisure tastes.

    The relationship between gender on the one hand and leisure and tastes on the

    other exemplifies the way specific behaviors are given gendered meanings. Leisure

    provides men and women with a hierarchical structure of opportunity as well as an

    effective structure of identity and cohesion. In this way, diverse social structures

    incorporate gender values and convey gender advantages. Understanding the way mens

    and womens leisure changes over time is therefore important because it reflects the

    changing gender distribution of cultural resources. We ask whether men and women

    have similar leisure participation patterns and whether we can identify changes in the

    relative distribution of leisure activities between men and women over time. For example,

    we ask whether men tend to participate in out-of-home activities more than women,

    whether men tend to engage in electronic leisure more than women, whether men

    participate in social leisure less than women and if and how these differences have

    changed over time. We further ask, what is the significance of trends that are identified in

    terms of existing theories of leisure and gender. Manytheories emphasize the

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    4/36

    4

    consequences of gender differences in leisure over time but do not empirically investigate

    trends.

    We employ British data on time-use, which give details of peoples daily

    activities, collected over the period from the 1960s through to the 1990s to discuss some

    central questions in the literature on the distribution of consumption activities. These

    data enable a longitudinal perspective on changes in cultural consumption orientations

    and on similarities and differences in consumption behavior.

    Cultural Consumption and Gender

    Sociological literature increasingly emphasizes the importance of cultural

    consumption and tastes in shaping the contours of social locations and social relations

    (Bourdieu 1984; Slater 1997). In the context of growing political and economic

    significance of consumerism and consumer culture, it is argued that more emphasis

    should be given to the cultural rather than the economic construction of social groups.

    Thus, instead of an emphasis on social analysis based on income, occupation, or

    education there is an increasing emphasis on cultural criteria such as consumption

    behavior and taste patterns. Cultural preferences and lifestyles are initiators and

    sustainers of identities and group boundaries (Warde 1994; Lamont and Molnar 2001);

    they mark and maintain social distinction (Peterson and Kern 1996; Katz-Gerro 1999);

    they reflect and create symbols and symbolic meanings (Bryson 1996); and they are

    sources of new conflicts and new social movements (Schor 1999). Cultural tribes

    (Mafessoli 1996) crystallize on the basis of lifestyles as a new form of sociality in which

    both individuals and groups objectify themselves and their values through their

    consumption acts. The shift from a social system based primarily on production to one

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    5/36

    5

    based primarily on consumption means that individuals are spending more time on leisure

    activities. Consequently, decisions surrounding the allocation of resources such as money

    or time spent on leisure, and the content of that leisure (in terms of which activities

    people do and who participates in them) are becoming more central to the dynamics of

    identity, exclusion, and solidarity.

    Consumption, taste and leisure are closely linked in that together they comprise

    the concept of lifestyle. Common lifestyle indicators in the literature include leisure

    pastimes, cultural consumption, and cultural tastes related to clothing, music, reading,

    and choice of holidays (Featherstone 1991). It is widely acknowledged that consumption

    and tastes are strongly gendered. Women and men differ in leisure activities, leisure

    preferences, time allocated for leisure, cultural tastes, household division of

    consumption, and household consumption decision making (Costa 1994; Firat 1991;

    Grazia and Furlough 1996; Horowitz and Mohun 1998; Lubar 1998; MacDonald 1995).

    Gender differences in consumption have been attributed to various factors. First, with the

    growth in the gender division of labor, production came to be associated with the public

    domain and with men while the private domain came to be associated with womens

    'non-work' activities such as recreation, leisure, family life and consumption (Firat 1991).

    Second, women are expected to be in charge of status work in the family and therefore

    they come to dominate cultural and material consumption in the household (Collins

    1988, 1992). They are more involved than men in the realm of symbolic status emulation

    and status presentation, such as in identifying status with the appearance of the

    household. For example, in the U.S., women are often responsible for consumption

    activities - shopping, preparing items for consumption, gift buying and disposal of used

    items.Typically, American men are more responsible for the purchase of certain types of

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    6/36

    6

    goods such as cars and electrical appliances than are women. Some of this gender

    dichotomization is currently breaking down but much of it remains resilient to change

    (Costa 1994). Thirdly, since women are regarded as second earners, their marginal place

    in the familys social security system leaves their free time mostly for nurturing their

    family, taking care of the home, or spending their leisure time indoors. This means that

    womens disposable time seeps away invisibly into the caring functions they perform for

    family members (Pasers 1994).

    The literature on gender differences in consumption provides equivocal findings

    since accounts of gender differences in cultural consumption and leisure activities, point

    in different directions, both empirically and theoretically. First, current research reports

    that men and women differ in their leisure activities and cultural tastes (Shaw 1985;

    Bryson 1996; Netz 1996; Bihagen and Katz-Gerro 2000; Katz-Gerro 2002), but also that

    gender differences in those realms are insignificant (Shelton 1992; Robinson and Godbey

    1997; Bittman and Wajcman 2000). Second, although women on average have more

    leisure time than men (who are constrained by the time devoted to paid work), womens

    leisure time is constrained by their housework responsibilities,their lesser command of

    material resources andthe lack of legitimacy for women to pursue their own leisure

    interests. In other words, leisure has contradictory aspects in womens and mens lives

    and can have different outcomes both for individual women and for structural relations

    between men and women in society (Shaw 1985). Third, a variety of socio-demographic

    determinants and covariates have been shown to affect men and womens leisure both

    similarly and differently. Such inconsistencies cause problems for the development of a

    consistent body of theory and are attributable to the use of different measures of

    covariates in different countries and different decades (Thrane 2000).

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    7/36

    7

    Although there have been interesting developments in the discussion of cultural

    consumption, there has been little large-scale empirical analysis on longitudinal

    differences and trends in categories of cultural consumption, cultural tastes, and lifestyle

    preferences. Scholars have neglected a specifically comparative point of view on

    differences in cultural consumption that stem from different class positions, gender

    locations and politico-economic regimes.

    Following findings in the literature, and in order best to address some of the

    existing theoretical debates about the relationship between cultural consumption and

    socio-demographic variables, we choose to focus on a set of activities representing the

    different fields of cultural consumption and a set of factors that are known to be

    associated with cultural consumption patterns. From existing research at the macro-level,

    we know that differences in leisure-time/consumption activities are related to factors

    such as class, gender, education level, occupation, and income (DiMaggio1982;

    Aschaffenburg 1995; Peterson and Kern 1996; Bryson 1996). In the analyses that follow

    we mainly focus on issues of class and gender because of the substantial existing

    theoretical literature in these areas, which permits the possibility of testing specific

    hypotheses. However,the effects of other socio-economic and demographic variables

    such as education, family structure, age, and income are also included in the analyses.

    Below, we briefly review the literature on each of these factors and discuss the

    main theoretical issues that call for attention.

    Leisure, Taste and Gender

    While gender is often present in models that explain the distribution of cultural

    consumption, the relationship between gender and consumption has not always received

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    8/36

    8

    full attention. Additionally, much of the writing about womens experience of timehas

    been philosophical in orientation and, insofar as it has drawn on empirical research, has

    been of a qualitative nature based on small-scale studies (Bittman and Wajcman 2000).

    In addition, as referred to above, accounts of gender differences in cultural

    consumption and leisure activities point in different directions, both empirically and

    theoretically. Current research reports that men and women differ in their leisure

    activities and cultural tastes (Shaw 1985; Bryson 1996; Netz 1996; Bihagen and Katz-

    Gerro 2000; Katz-Gerro 2002) but also that gender differences in those realms are

    insignificant (Shelton 1992; Bittman and Wajcman 2000). Theories that address gender

    differences in leisure and consumption range from emphasizing socialization, gender

    roles, and the link between gender and status (Collins 1988, 1992), to emphasizing

    division of labor in the household, relative resources, and constraints of time,

    opportunities and legitimacy (Green et al. 1990; Shaw 1994; Samuel et al.1996)1.

    Leisure, Taste, and Class

    A strong tradition in sociology emphasizes the link between cultural lifestyle and

    economic class position. In the Weberian tradition, class arises in the sphere of

    consumption no less than in that of production and social intercourse within class barriers

    is promoted by the similarity of manners and habits of life (Weber 1946).

    In his influential work, Bourdieu uses the concepts of economic and cultural

    capital to describe a model of class structure and class reproduction (1984:128-29). He

    1 For example, reports from Sweden regarding gender differences in leisure activities show that women spend their

    leisure time around the house and in their neighborhood while men may travel to sports arenas and other destinies

    at some distance from their home (Linden et al. 1999). Another kind of constraint is access to credit. Credit systems

    and banks are less accessible to women because they require a steady income or ownership of property.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    9/36

    9

    argues that through the mediation of the habitus -- patterns of thought, comprehension,

    consumption and lifestyle -- people internalize their class position and express it in

    cultural choices that reproduce the very class structure itself. Bourdieu (1984) suggests

    that dominant classes have distinct cultural tastes, which they use as both an indicator of

    their cultural capital and as a way to maintain their advantage in social, economic, and

    cultural arenas.

    Highbrow and lowbrow cultural tastes are often analyzed in the context of class

    differences in cultural preferences (De Graaf 1991; Aschaffenburg 1995; Spellerberg

    1995; Katz-Gerro 2002). The advantages that accompany the consumption of highbrow

    culture -- in educational and occupational attainment -- are linked to the fabrication of

    solidarity, construction of identity, and practices of exclusion (Lamont 1992). Gender

    differences in cultural consumption are often confused with class differences when

    gender is considered secondary to class and when differences in consumption patterns

    are perceived as a result of occupation, education, or income (Bourdieu 1984).

    To address the issues reviewed thus far, we pursue two main directions. First, we

    document trends and changes in class and gender patterns of leisure activities over time.

    Second, we test various theoretical hypotheses through the investigation of interaction

    effects in thevariables affecting leisure activities.

    Methods

    The Longitudinal Perspective

    The longitudinal perspectiveconcerns the collection and analysis of data over

    time. Such a perspective isnowadays considered essential if the purpose of the research

    is to understand social change. This is so because only longitudinal data permit a

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    10/36

    10

    diachronic analysis, in which the relative timing of events may be established (see

    Berthoud 2000). However, large-scale longitudinalempirical research in the area of

    leisure and tastes is to date very rare.

    There are many types of longitudinal data, ranging from true longitudinal data

    recorded for the same people over time to sets of repeated cross-sectional surveys using

    the same questionnaire but interviewing different people at each survey. For the purposes

    of this paper we will be utilizing a set of repeated cross-sectional studies. The single

    cross-sectional study is the form of data that has been the most commonly used in the

    social sciences for assessing the determinants of behavior. However, it is now relatively

    common for cross-sectional data to be recorded in a succession of surveys over several

    points of time, with a new sample on each occasion. Where cross-sectional data are

    repeated over time with a high level of consistency between questions, it is possible to

    incorporate a time trend into the analysis. The multinational time use diary data archive

    has been constructed to enable such analyses (see Sullivan and Gershuny 2001; Gershuny

    2000a for descriptions of general trends in time-use from this source).

    The Data

    We employ time-use data collected in Britain over the period from the 1960s

    through to the 1990s. These data include information on the time spent in specific leisure

    activities drawn from time-use diaries collected over a full week of activities. The full

    database comprises successive time-use diary surveys from a range of developed

    countries, which were collected from the 1960s to the 1990s. These surveys have been

    standardized to a single format, with a single range of activities, so they form a unique

    record of change in individuals use of time in different countries from the 1960s to the

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    11/36

    11

    1990s. Anticipating that this project will include other countries in its next stage, there

    are by now a substantial amount of comparative standardized time-use diary data

    available from most European and North American countries (see the web site for the

    Institute for Social and Economic Research at Essex for further details of the multi-

    national time-use study: www.iser.essex.ac.uk).

    Time-use Diary Methodology

    Self-complete time use diaries describe the pattern of the daily activities of

    individuals (see Robinson 1985). They tell us about the way in which individuals spend

    their time throughout the day expressed assequences of numerical codes representing

    the different activities that individuals engage in over the day. These diaries are now a

    well-established methodology in the social sciences and have been used to analyze the

    use of time at the level of individuals, households and societies. For example, the way

    individuals divide their time between work (paid and unpaid); and the amount and use of

    leisure time (see, for example, Grazia 1964; Szalai 1972; Berk 1985; Gershuny 1995;

    Robinson and Godbey 1997; Gershuny and Sullivan [under review]). Time-use diarydata

    are appropriate for documenting differences and trends over time and they have been

    shown to bemore accurate than survey questions asking people about theirleisure

    participation. Using time-use diary data it is possible to calculate both participation rates

    (that is the proportion of people who participate in a particular activity over a specific

    period of time) and the average time spent in specific activities.

    At the macro level, estimates can be derived of the way in which time is divided

    between production and consumption activities within a society (Gershuny 2000a).

    Multi-national data of this kind can therefore provide the opportunity to compare the

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    12/36

    12

    way in which time is divided between different activities in different societies. It turns out

    that there are well-established cross-national variations in time-use (Szalai 1972; Gronmo

    and Lingsom 1986; Gershuny 1993; Robinson and Godbey 1997), which have been

    related to factors such as differing regimes of social welfare, differing employment

    systems and levels of domestic technology. However, while there is a growing body of

    research on the cross-sectional correlates of the use of time in different societies, little is

    known about the development of patterns of activity within societies across time.

    Existing data shows that changes in patterns of time-use occur over time (for instance,

    there has been a general trend towards shorter working hours and longer durations of

    leisure - see Gershuny 1993; Robinson and Godbey 1997), but there is little knowledge

    about the processes that underpin such change.

    Variables

    We draw our leisure indicators from the areas of entertainment, socialising,

    travel, sport, and home-based leisure activities.

    -- Table 1 about here --

    A variety of different dimensions of cultural consumption may be distinguished

    from within the list of activities available from the time-use data and shown in Table 1.

    For example, we can distinguish highbrow from lowbrow cultural activities, indoor from

    outdoor leisure activities, and expensive from inexpensive leisure activities.

    In order to operationalize the concept of class, we created a standard class

    indicator from the class variables available from the time-use diary studies. These

    variables included occupationally based definitions of class such as those employed by

    national statistical bureaus and socio-economic indices as employed by market research

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    13/36

    13

    organizations. However, it was possible to amalgamate the categories to create a

    consistent series from a three-class classification of high, middle and low.

    Hypotheses and Questions

    In previous research we have established that gender differences in consumption

    are not easily reducible to occupational, educational, or other differences (Bihagen and

    Katz-Gerro 2000; Katz-Gerro 2002). In the present research we explore possible

    theoretical explanations of the features of the gender difference in cultural consumption

    that are most appropriately treated using a longitudinal perspective. The first explanation

    is that with increasing rates of womens higher educational attainment and participation

    in the labor force, more women take part in the dominant highbrow culture. But as more

    women participate in highbrow culture, this cultural field is devalued. Following the logic

    of Simmels trickle down theory (1904), which holds that subordinate social groups seek

    to establish new status claims by adopting the tastes of super-ordinate groups, we can

    suggest that women seek to establish new status claims by adopting the consumption

    patterns of men.

    The second explanation is that men as the dominant gender select and participate

    in new areas of leisure in order to maintain their distinction through the creation of new

    fields of activity. Distinctive lifestyles are necessary for group identification and are

    reinforced in the differentiation and distribution of tastes.

    Following these explanations we propose two hypotheses. First, we hypothesize

    that with the increase in womens higher education and labor force participation their

    consumption tastes become more highbrow and also more varied (to include highbrow as

    well as popular, outdoor as well as indoor leisure, expensive and inexpensive activities

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    14/36

    14

    etc.) We will look for evidence that supports Simmels trickle down theory, which

    argues that women seek over time to establish new status claims by adopting the

    consumption patterns of men. Second, we hypothesize that in order to maintain their

    distinction, men adopt new consumption tastes. Rather than maintain those areas of

    cultural consumption that women have also come to master, they seek new aspects of

    cultural consumption to become competent in, which serve as new areas of gender

    distinction.

    Results

    Figure 1 and Figure 2 show trends in participation rates in leisure activitiesfor men and

    women in 1961, 1975, 1987, and 1997. On the vertical axis we have the percentage of men who

    participate in a particular activity over the specific week of the diary. For example, we can see

    the increase in the proportion of men who watch television from 1961 up to a level of almost a

    hundred percent in 1997. The video and computing data pertain only to 1987 and 1997, and we

    can see the dramatic increase in participation between these dates.

    -- Figure 1 about here --

    -- Figure 2 about here --

    However, what is shown in Figures 1 and 2 are raw participation rates that do not

    control for other changes in the socio-demographic conditions of the population, which

    might also be related to patterns of participation in leisure activities. For example,

    employed parents of small children might be less likely to spend time in out-of-home

    leisure activities such as outings (walks, day trips, museums, galleries) or social eating

    (pubs, restaurants).

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    15/36

    15

    Since we are interested in the way gender differences change over time while taking

    account of changes in underlying characteristics such as these, we performed multivariate

    analyses, which control for the effect of several variables together. Participation rates in the

    activities are the variables we are primarily interested in (i.e. the dependent variables). In the

    analyses we control for sex, survey year, class, family status, and employment status -- all of

    which are known to affect leisure participation (Katz-Gerro 2002). In addition, we include in the

    models the interaction effects of sex by survey year and sex by class. The interaction effects

    including survey year allow us to determine which activities change over time in a different way

    both for men and women (sex by survey year), while the sex by class interaction effect controls

    for different patterns of gender participation according to class.

    -- Table 2 about here --

    Table 2 shows the adjusted mean participation rates in the different activities by

    survey and sex from the multivariate analyses while controlling for family status,

    employment status, class and a class by sex interaction effect. All these are variables that

    have been shown to have an impact on the distribution of leisure time. For example,

    research conducted in Scandinavian countries shows that the strongest negative effect on

    leisure time was full-time employment followed by school enrolment. Age showed a

    curvilinear relationship, and marriage and children reduced the amount of leisure time

    all three affecting women more than men (Thrane 2000). In Table 2 therefore it is

    possible to see: changes over time in average participation rates in the different leisure

    activities (shown by the Survey variable); the differential participation by sex over the

    period of the surveys (shown by the Sex variable); and changing patterns of participation

    over time according to sex (shown by the Survey*Sex interaction).

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    16/36

    16

    Firstly, then, we see that there are statistically significant trends over time in

    participation in leisure activities for all the activities except the two smallest categories:

    PC study and PC other. There are also statistically significant differences by sex for the

    activities of: outings, social eating, visiting friends, sports, hosting, phone calls, doing

    nothing, PC games and PC e-mail. The activities with no statistically significant gender

    differences are: going to the cinema, TV, radio, reading, video, PC study, PC paid and

    PC other (see discussion below). Only three statistically significant changing patterns of

    participation by sex were found, for the activities of sports participation, phone calls and

    PC games.

    -- Figure 3 about here --

    Figure 3 shows only those differences in participation by sex, which were found

    to be statistically significant (refer to Table 2 and text above). Over the period covered

    by the surveys women participated more in outings, visiting, hosting, phone calls (i.e. in

    most of the social activity categories) and in doing nothing. It has been suggested that

    women have adominant influence in maintaining communication with family and friends

    (Anderson et al. 1999), and previous research has also shown that women spend more

    time on the phone than men (Anderson et al. op cit). In our analyses men, however,

    participated more than women in social eating, sports, computer games (PC games) and

    e-mail (PC e-mail). Among the computing activities these two represent the main leisure

    components of PC use. Our findings therefore supportprevious research, which has

    shown that it is men who master the new area of home computing2. In the raw means

    (shown in Figure 1)women in fact do slightly more studying using a computer but this is

    2 Anderson et al. (1999) report that men spend an average of . hours of computer usage at home compared to 5.6

    hours for women. Men also appear to access the Internet for longer periods of time than women (2.4 compared to0.2 hours). Men spend 3.4 hours a week playing computer games compared to 1.2 hours for women.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    17/36

    17

    not significant when controlling for employment status and family status (which means

    probably that this applies to a specific group of women, perhaps students).

    These differences have been identified before, but our main focus in this paper is

    on different trends according to gender. We therefore need to focus on those activities

    where there was a survey by sex interaction (i.e. where there are different trends over

    time for men and women). Of the nine activities that have different means for men and

    women six show no significantly different trends by sex (walks and outings, social eating,

    visiting, hosting, doing nothing and e-mail) while three activities have different trends

    over time (phone use, sports participation and PC games).

    -- Figure 4 about here --

    Figure 4 shows the difference in the trends over time for these three activities.

    We show absolute differences in participation rates between men and women over time.

    The higher the point on the Y-axis the greater the absolute difference is at that point in

    time. Looking at the trends in participation enables us to explain what is different about

    the trends for men and women for these three activities:

    PC games: there was a small difference in participation between men and women in

    1987. This difference increased substantially in 1997 due to a much greater increase in

    mens participation.

    Phone use: up to 1987 the growth in the difference is due to women doing relatively

    more phone calls than men. However, in 1997, there is reduction in the difference due to

    men catching up. A possible explanation is the increasing technological improvements in

    the design of home telephones (cordless phones, fax, e-mail, Minitel, and all sorts of

    other buttons and displays) and the introduction of mobile cell phones that were first

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    18/36

    18

    used by businessmen. This goes with the increasing tendency we observe for men to

    engage in computing/electronic gadgets.

    Sports: we observe a very interesting process here. Mens sports participation has

    increased steadily over the period with a particularly strong increase between 1961 and

    1975. The increasing difference up to 1975 shown in the graph is the result of mens

    increasing relative participation. However, between 1975 and 1987 there was a

    substantial catching up effect in womens sports participation and this is where we see

    the difference decreasing. This is in line with other research which has shown that overall

    the trend over the period 1973 to 1997 was towards greater convergence in mens and

    womens sports participation, with a faster growth in participation among women

    (Gershuny and Fisher 1999).The difference increases again in 1997 because whereas

    mens participation continued to increase womens participation showed more stability.

    The question is, what was significant about the 1980s for womens sports participation in

    Britain? While there is no direct evidence, it is the case that this period coincided with

    the huge growth in aerobics activities among women.

    We decided to look in more detail at the new electronic technologies because this

    is an area in which we clearly observe mens distinction being formed. If this is indeed an

    area claimed by men does it mean that they substitute computing time for time spent in

    other activities that are no longer differentiated strongly by gender?

    -- Figure 5 about here --

    -- Figure 6 about here --

    Figure 5 and Figure 6 show overall mean time in minutes per week spent in several

    activities for those who do and dont do computing. The range is from zero to as many minutes a

    week as people do. We then selected a range of activities for which there is no significant sex

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    19/36

    19

    difference in participation. Among these, TV and video represent alternative electronic media

    activities in the home. Hobbies and doing nothing represent other non-electronic alternative in-

    home activities. Finally, from the out-of-home activities we chose cinema (for which there has

    been a sharp decline in participation for both men and women over time Gershuny and Fisher

    1999) to see if one of the possible effects was a time substitution with computing.

    What the figures show is that men who do computing spend less time doing

    nothing, going to the cinema and watching TV. Women who do computing also spend

    less time doing nothing and less time watching TV but, in contrast to men, they spend

    more time going to the cinema. For both men and women who do computing, time spent

    watching videos and doing hobbies is actually somewhat greater than for those who do

    not do computing. Of course, there are difficulties with interpreting this as a simple

    substitution effect. In order to be able to do that we would need to take account also of

    the activity profiles of individuals both before and after they took up computing. For

    example, a true longitudinal analysis by Gershuny has shown that those people who took

    up home computing with enthusiasm actually spent more time socializing in subsequent

    surveys than other people. As the author notes, this is contrary to the stereotype of

    computer nerds (Gershuny 2000b).

    Conclusions

    Leisure is the focus of contemporary lifestyles, particularly through its association

    with social networks, consumption, and cultural identity. Leisure is associated with

    citizenship, freedom, and self-fulfillment (Ravenscroft 1996). As such, leisure and

    cultural tastes are central to gender relations and gender inequality. In this paper, we

    askedwhether men and women have similar leisure participation patterns and whether

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    20/36

    20

    we can identify changes in the relative distribution of leisure activities between men and

    women over time.

    The overall picture seems to be one of relative stability over the period from

    1975 to 1997. The relative pattern of participation over time is different for men and

    women in only three out of 17 activities (as indicated by the survey by sex interaction).

    For most activities, then, the relative pattern is similar for men and women over time.

    For 9 out of 17 of the activities that we investigated, there were clearly

    significant over-all differences by sex even when controlling for a range of socio-

    demographic variables. Most of the activities for which we observed no gender

    differences were in-home activities (radio, TV, video, reading, the work related PC use

    activities) plus going to the cinema.

    There are two particular theories of change in gender participation in leisure that

    seem appropriate to the kind of analysis we were able to perform using these data. The

    first of these is Bourdieus theory of distinction in which elite social groups seek to

    establish cultural territories that they define, master and in which they regulate access.

    The second is Simmels trickle down theory, which describes a process whereby

    subordinate groups emulate the consumption patterns of super ordinate groups, who

    respond by adopting new patterns.

    We drew from these theories when trying to understand the three activities that

    show different gender trends over time. With regard to PC games one possible

    interpretation relates to a distinction explanation. There is a significant increase in mens

    participation over time relative to women in this most popular of PC uses. We think this

    may indicate a new realm that over the 1990s became increasingly male-dominated. In

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    21/36

    21

    the multivariate analysis we also found a clear sex differential for PC e-mail (although the

    trend over-time was not significantly different for men and women).

    We interpret the other two differential trends that we have identified as part of a

    modified trickle down process. In regard to participation in sports, we observe a

    substantial catching up effect in women sports participation between 1975 and 1987.

    The big increase in mens sports participation occurred between 1961 and 1975 while for

    women the big increase was between 1975 and 1987.

    The recent decrease in the differences in phone use between women and men is

    due to an increase in mens participation. We hypothesize that this increase is related to

    the new availability of technologically sophisticated phone paraphernalia. This

    differentiation cannot be adequately described either in terms of a trickle down theory (in

    which subordinate groups emulate super-ordinate groups) or in terms of a distinction

    theory (in which men are the dominant group). Perhaps both theories can be part of a

    possible explanation of the sex differential in computing media.

    Changes in the organization and experience of leisure activities are affected

    directly by technological developments. An example is cyberspace communities that

    emerge as a new type of leisure space (Bryce 2001). Factors such as gender and class

    limit and modify access to leisure technologies in several ways. First, gender and class

    are identified as structural leisure constraints through their effect on temporal

    constraints, economic constraints, and lack of opportunities or facilities. Second, through

    the agents of socialization, traditional expectations of gender and class are transformed,

    reproduced and reinforced into the new technological leisure space (Anderson et al.

    1999). Our findings offer some support to an approach that sees womens leisure as

    offering possibilities for resistance. Leisure is seen as a site of choice and control and

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    22/36

    22

    womens participation in non-traditional activities challenges restrictive social roles

    (Shaw 1994).

    Moving on to the issue of time substitution for computer use, one interpretation

    of our findings is that time spent computing actually substitutes for doing nothing and

    watching TV, while it does not substitute for time spent on hobbies and watching videos.

    For men specifically computing also appears to substitute for going out to the cinema.

    Truly longitudinaldata will be needed to determine whether this is indeed a direct

    substitution effect (i.e., people directly substituting their time at the cinema for time

    spent computing) or a selection effect related to the types of people who do computing.

    Their activity patterns before they took up computing will also require analysis.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    23/36

    23

    Bibliography

    Anderson, B., A. McWillaim, H. Lacohee, E. Clucas, and J. Gershuny. 1999. Family

    Life in the Digital Home: Domestic Telecommunications at the end of the 20 th

    Century.BT Technology Journal17,1.

    Aschaffenburg, Karen E. 1995. On the Distribution of Cultural Capital: Social Location

    and Cultural Participation. Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University.

    Berk, Sarah. 1985. The Gender Factory: The Apportionment of Work in American

    Households. New York: Plenum Press.

    Berthoud, Richard. 2000 Introduction. in Berthoud, R. and Gershuny, J. (Eds.) Seven

    Years in the Lives of British Households. London: Polity Press.

    Bihagen, Erik, and Tally Katz-Gerro. 2000. Culture Consumption in Sweden: The

    Stability of Gender Differences.Poetics 27:327-349.

    Bittman, Michael, and Judy Wajcman. 2000. The Rush Hour: The Character of Leisure

    Time and Gender Equity. Social Forces 79,1:165-189.

    Bourdieu, Pierre. 1984.Distinction: A Social Critique on the Judgment of Tastes.

    Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Bryce, Jo. 2001. The Technological Transformation of Leisure. Social Science

    Computer Review 19,1:7-16.

    Bryson, Bethany. 1996. Anything But Heavy Metal: Symbolic Exclusion and Musical

    Dislikes.American Sociological Review 61:884-899.

    Collins, Randall. 1988. Women and Men in the Class Structure. Journal of Family

    Issues 9,1:27-50.

    ------. 1992. Women and the Production of Status Cultures. Pp. 213-31 in Cultivating

    Differences: Symbolic Boundaries and the Making of Inequality,

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    24/36

    24

    edited by M. Lamont and M. Fournier, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Costa, Janeen. 1994. Gender Issues and Consumer Behavior. London: Sage.

    De Graaf, Nan-Dirk. 1991. Distinction by Consumption in Czechoslovakia, Hungary

    and the Netherlands.European Sociological Review 53:103-112.

    De Grazia, S. 1964. Time, Work and Leisure, New York: Anchor Books.

    De Grazia, Victoria, and Ellen Furlough. 1996. The Sex of Things: Gender and

    Consumption in Historical Perspective. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    DiMaggio, Paul. 1982. Cultural Capital and School Success: The Impact of Status

    Culture Participation on the Grades of U.S. High School Students.American

    Sociological Review 47:189-210.

    Featherstone, Mike. 1991. Consumer Culture and Postmodernism. London: Sage.

    Firat, Fuat A. 1991. Consumption and Gender: A Common History. Pp. 32-49 in

    Proceedings of the Conference on Gender and Consumer Behaviour, edited by

    J.A. Costa. Salt Lake City, Utah.

    Gershuny, Jonathan. 1993. Post-Industrial Convergence in Time Allocation.

    Futures June: 578-586.

    ------. 1995. Time Budget Research in Europe. Statistics in Transition 2/4: 1-23.

    ------. 2000a. Changing Times. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    ------. 2000b. Leisure and Home IT. Report for the Foundation for Science and

    Technology.

    Gershuny, Jonathan and Fisher, Kimberly. 1999. Leisure. Chapter 18 in Halsey,

    A. H. with Webb, J. (eds.) Twentieth Century British Social Trends (3rd

    Edition). London: Macmillan Publishers.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    25/36

    25

    Gershuny, Jonathan and Sullivan, Oriel. [Under Review] Time Use, Gender and Public

    Policy Regimes.

    Green, Eileen, Sandra Hebron, and Diana Woodward. 1990. WomensLeisure,What

    Leisure? London: Macmillan.

    Gronmo, S., and Lingsom S. 1986. Increasing Equality in Household Work: Patterns

    of Time Use Change in Norway.European Sociological Review 2(3): 176-190.

    Harvey, M., McMeekin, A., Randles, S., Southerton, D., Tether, B. and Warde,

    A. 2001. Between Demand and Consumption: A Framework for

    Research. Centre for Research on Innovation and Competition Discussion

    Paper 40. Manchester: University of Manchester and UMIST.

    Horowitz, Roger, and Arwen Mohun (Eds.). 1998.His and Hers: Gender, Consumption

    and Technology. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.

    Katz-Gerro, Tally. 1999. Cultural Consumption and Social Stratification: Leisure

    Activities, Musical Tastes, and Social Location. Sociological Perspectives

    42,4:627-646.

    Katz-Gerro, Tally. 2002. Highbrow Cultural Consumption and Class Distinction In

    Italy, Israel, West Germany, Sweden, and the United States. Social Forces 81,1.

    Lamont, Michle. 1992.Money, Morals, and Manners. Chicago: The University of

    Chicago Press.

    Lamont ,Michle, and Virag Molnar. 2001. How Blacks Use Consumption to Shape

    Their Collective Identity: Evidence from Marketing Specialists.Journal of

    Consumer Culture 1,1:31-46.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    26/36

    26

    Lubar, S. 1998. Men/Women/Production/Consumption. in Horowitz and Mohun

    (Eds.)His and Hers: Gender, Consumption and Technology. Charlottesville,

    VA: University of Virginia Press.

    Macdonald, Myra. 1995.Representing Women: Myths of Femininity in the Popular

    Media. London: Arnold.

    Maffesoli, Michel. 1996. The Time of the Tribes: The Decline of Individualism in Mass

    Society. London: Sage Publications.

    Netz, Yael. 1996. Time Allocation and Leisure Preferences of Women in Israel. Pp.

    219-233 in Women, Leisure and the Family in Contemporary Society: A

    Multinational Perspective, edited by N. Samuel, Wallingford: CAB International.

    Pasers, Ursula. 1994. Social Time Patterns, Contingency and Gender Relations. Time

    and Society 3,2:179-191.

    Peterson, Richard A., and Roger M. Kern. 1996. Changing Highbrow Taste: From

    American Sociological Review 61,5:900-907.

    Ravenscroft, Neil. 1996. Leisure, Consumerism and Active Citizenship in the UK.

    Managing Leisure 1:163-174.

    Robinson, John P. 1985 The Validity and Reliability of Diaries Versus Alternative Time

    Chapter 3 in Juster, F. T. and Stafford, F. P. (eds.) Time, Goods,

    and Well-Being. Michigan: Survey Research Centre, University of Michigan.

    Robinson, J. P., and G. Godbey. 1997. Time for Life: The Surprising Ways Americans

    Use Their Time. University Park: Pennsylvania State University Press.

    Samuel, Nicole. (Ed.). 1996. Women, Leisure and the Family in Contemporary Society:

    A Multinational Perspective. Wallingford: CAB International.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    27/36

    27

    Schor, Juliet. 1999. The New Politics of Consumption. Pp. 446-462 in The Consumer

    Society Reader, edited by Juliet Schor and Douglas Holt. New York: The New

    Press.

    Shaw, Susan M. 1985. Gender and Leisure: Inequality in the Distribution of Leisure

    Journal of Leisure Research 17,4:266-282.

    ------. 1994. Gender, Leisure, and Constraints. Towards a Framework for the Analysis

    Journal of Leisure Research 26,1:8-22.

    Shelton, Beth Anne. 1992. Women, Men and Time: Gender Differences in Paid Work,

    Housework and Leisure. New York: Greenwood Press.

    Simmel, Georg. 1904. Fashion.International Quarterly 10:130-155.

    Slater, Don. 1997. Consumer Culture and Modernity. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Spellerberg, Annette. 1995. Lifestyle and Quality of Life in West and East Germany.

    Angewandte Sozialforschung19, 1:93-106. (In German).

    Sullivan, Oriel and Gershuny, Jonathan. 2000 Cross-National Changes in Time-Use:

    Some Sociological (Hi)Stories Re-ExaminedBritish Journal of Sociology,

    52/2:331-347.

    Szalai, Andre (ed.). 1972 The Use of Time: Daily Activities of Urban and Suburban

    Populations in 12 Countries. The Hague: Mouton.

    Thrane, Christer. 2000. Men, Women and Leisure Time: Scandinavian Evidence of

    Leisure Sciences 22:109-122.

    Warde, Alan. 1994. Consumption, Identity Formation and Uncertainty. Sociology

    28,4:877-898.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    28/36

    28

    Weber, Max. 1946. Class, Status, Power. Pp. 180-95 inFrom Max Weber: Essays in

    Sociology, edited by H. H. Gerth and C. Wright Mills. New York: Oxford University

    Press.

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    29/36

    29

    Table 1: Leisure Activity Variables

    Variable Name Description

    Cinema Going to concerts, theatre, cinema

    Outings Walks, outings, etc.

    Social eating Eating out, drinking (pubs, restaurant)

    Visit friends Visiting or meeting friends or relatives

    Sports Sports participation, keeping fit

    TV Watching TV/cable/satellite TV

    Radio Listening to radio, CD, cassette

    Reading Reading newspapers, books, magazines

    Hosting Being visited by friends or relatives

    Phone calls Receiving telephone calls

    Nothing Doing nothing at home

    Video Watching video, laser disks

    PC games Games

    PC email Email

    PC study Study at home

    PC paid Paid work at home

    PC other Other

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    30/36

    30

    Figure 1: Trends in Participation Rates Men

    0%

    10 %

    20 %

    30 %

    40 %

    50 %

    60 %

    70 %

    80 %

    90 %

    100 %

    cinem

    a

    outin

    gs

    sociale

    ating

    visitfri

    ends

    sports TV

    radio

    reading

    hosting

    phon

    ecalls

    nothing

    Vide

    o

    pcgam

    es

    pcemail

    pcstu

    dy

    pcpaid

    pcoth

    er

    61

    75

    87

    97

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    31/36

    31

    Figure 2: Trends in Participation Rates Women

    0 %

    10 %

    20 %

    30 %

    40 %

    50 %

    60 %

    70 %

    80 %

    90 %

    100 %

    volu

    ntary

    cinem

    a

    outin

    gs

    socia

    leatin

    g

    visitf

    riend

    s

    sports TV

    radio

    readin

    g

    hostin

    g

    phon

    ecalls

    nothin

    g

    Vide

    o

    pcgam

    es

    pcemail

    pcstu

    dy

    pcpaid

    pcoth

    er

    61

    75

    87

    97

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    32/36

    32

    Table 2: Adjusted Mean Participation Rates from Analysis of Variance by Survey

    Year, Sex, and Survey by Sex interaction (controlling for family status,

    employment status, class and class by sex interaction).

    Activity Survey Sex Survey*Sex

    1975 1987 1997# Men Women

    Cinema .70 .43 .22*** .46 .44

    Outings .37 .60 .56*** .48 .54*

    Social eating .35 .55 .62*** .54 .47*

    Visit friends .67 .75 .73* .62 .81***

    Sports .16 .25 .28*** .28 .18*** **

    TV .95 .99 .99** .98 .97

    Radio .51 .34 .31*** .40 .37

    Reading .80 .89 .75** .82 .81

    Hosting .45 .40 .52** .35 .56***

    Phone calls .60 .50 .56* .54 .65*** *

    Nothing .71 .94 .32*** .62 .69*

    Video NA .18 .27* .25 .20

    PC games NA .01 .14*** .10 .05* **

    PC e-mail NA .01 .06** .05 .01**

    PC study NA .01 .03 .01 .03

    PC paid NA .03 .09** .08 .04

    PC other NA .01 .003 .01 .01

    *p

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    33/36

    33

    Figure 3: Significant Gender Differences in Participation Rates (adjusted means

    from multivariate analysis).

    0%

    10%

    20%

    30%

    40%

    50%

    60%

    70%

    80%

    90%

    outings visit

    friends

    hosting phone

    calls

    nothing sports social

    eating

    pc gamespc email

    men women

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    34/36

    34

    Figure 4: Trends in Gender Differences in Participation Rates

    0%

    5%

    10%

    15%

    20%

    25%

    30%

    61 75 87 97

    sports phone calls pc games

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    35/36

    35

    Figure 5: Time Substitution for Computing (Men 1987, 1997)

    020

    40

    60

    80

    100

    120

    140

    TVvid

    eo

    cinem

    a

    hobb

    ies

    nothin

    g

    Minutes

    no comp

    comp

  • 7/29/2019 Sullivan 98

    36/36

    Figure 6: Time Substitution for Computing (Women 1987, 1997)

    0

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    120

    140

    TVvid

    eo

    cinem

    a

    hobb

    ies

    nothin

    g

    Minutes

    no comp

    comp