74
Cultural Narratives Running Head: CULTURAL NARRATIVES The Cultural Narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Using an Historical Perspective to Explore the Relationship between Relative Deprivation and Collective Well- Being Evelyne Bougie Esther Usborne Roxane de la Sablonnière Donald M. Taylor Word Count : 9358 Authors’ Notes Evelyne Bougie, Statistics Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; Esther Usborne and Roxane de la Sablonnière, Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada; Donald M. Taylor, Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada 1

Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Running Head: CULTURAL NARRATIVES

The Cultural Narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Using an Historical

Perspective to Explore the Relationship between Relative Deprivation and Collective Well-

Being

Evelyne Bougie

Esther Usborne

Roxane de la Sablonnière

Donald M. Taylor

Word Count : 9358

Authors’ Notes

Evelyne Bougie, Statistics Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; Esther Usborne and Roxane de

la Sablonnière, Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Québec,

Canada; Donald M. Taylor, Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal,

Québec, Canada

1

Page 2: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Abstract

Responding to calls to contextualize social psychological variables in history (e.g., Gergen,

1973; Liu & Hilton, 2005), the present research examines the relationship between relative

deprivation and collective well-being using an historical perspective. We hypothesized that

during historical periods of high intergroup threat, collective identity clarity might be a

mechanism that accounts for a positive relationship between relative deprivation and

collective well-being. In Study 1, cultural narrative interviews were conducted with

Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers in order to pinpoint key historical chapters for these

groups and to determine which of the chapters represent important intergroup threats. In

Study 2, using the rich information obtained from these narratives, relative deprivation and

collective identity clarity were explored across the groups’ histories and related to collective

well-being. During historical periods of high intergroup threat, the relationship between

relative deprivation and collective well-being was positive for both Anglophone and

Francophone Quebecers. This relationship was mediated by collective identity clarity. In a

period of high intergroup threat, relative deprivation might serve to clarify collective identity,

which in turn is associated with greater collective well-being.

Key Words: Narrative; Relative Deprivation; Collective Well-Being; Culture

2

Page 3: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

The Cultural Narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Using an Historical

Perspective to Explore the Relationship between Relative Deprivation and Collective Well-

Being

Shared representations of history are central to the creation and maintenance of a

group’s identity (Gergen, 1973; Hammack, 2008, 2009; Liu & Hilton, 2005; Okazaki, David,

& Abelman, 2008). A group member’s collective or cultural identity is thus shaped, at least in

part, by his or her group’s historical narrative (Hammack, 2008). Among social psychologists

exploring issues of collective identity and collective well-being, there is an increasing

recognition that a group’s history plays a key role in determining group members’

experiences and evaluations of their collective identities (see Gjerde, 2004; Hammack, 2008,

Okazaki, David, & Abelmann, 2008). However, research that sets out to empirically explore

predictors of collective identity and well-being most often measures these variables in a

decontextualized manner, divorced from any historical context. In order to truly understand

how group members understand and evaluate their collective identities, a social psychological

analysis must take history into account.

Collective well-being refers to the extent to which one perceives one’s collective

identity as having worth, respect, and value (Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Taylor,

1997, 2002). One social psychological variable that has been found to be predictive of

collective well-being is social relative deprivation. It was originally theorized that relative

deprivation, the subjective feeling of threat that arises when group members perceive their

group to be receiving less than what they feel they deserve, negatively affects collective well-

being (Walker, 1999). Indeed, it makes intuitive sense that feeling deprived, a subjectively

negative affective state, would be related to poor well-being. However, when measured

empirically, primarily using methods that do not take a group’s history into account, relative

deprivation has actually been found to have inconsistent links with collective well-being. As

3

Page 4: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

was initially predicted, the relationship between relative deprivation and well-being has often

been found to be negative (e.g., de la Sablonnière, Tougas, & Lortie-Lussier, 2009; Zagefka

& Brown, 2005). However, some studies have found no link between relative deprivation and

collective well-being (e.g., Tougas & Veilleux, 1988; Walker, 1999) and numerous others

have even found a positive relationship between these variables (de la Sablonnière & Tougas,

2008; de la Sablonnière, Tougas, & Lortie-Lussier, 2009; Petta & Walker, 1992), whereby

feeling relatively deprived actually predicted greater feelings of collective well-being. This

positive relationship is particularly surprising given that it is directly opposite to the original

theorizing pertaining to relative deprivation. In the present paper, we explore what might be

the mechanism that accounts for the presence of a positive relationship. We ask when and

why feelings of social relative deprivation would be positively associated with perceiving

one’s group to have worth, respect and value.

In order to more thoroughly understand the relationship between relative deprivation

and collective well-being, recent research has implied that the historical context in which

feelings of relative deprivation are measured must be taken into account (de la Sablonnière,

2008; de la Sablonnière, et al., 2009). Such research has demonstrated that the relationship

between relative deprivation and well-being might be affected by the nature of the historical

period in which participants base their judgments of deprivation; however, this assumption

was never systematically tested. In the present research, we therefore explore feelings of

relative deprivation measured across a group’s history and systematically examine their

relationships with collective well-being. Furthermore, we seek to understand what the

historically contextualized mechanism might be that is responsible for a positive relationship

between relative deprivation and collective well-being.

We propose that the clarity of a group’s collective identity acts as such a mechanism.

We posit that, at key, particularly threatening points in a group’s history, feelings of relative

4

Page 5: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

deprivation might actually clarify one’s collective identity, which in turn would be linked

with greater collective well-being. For a real-world example of these relationships, we might

consider an individual who has a Jewish identity. World War II represents an important

period of intense threat to the Jewish people. This historical period may be perceived as

particularly influential in defining the individual’s Jewish identity. Therefore, relative

deprivation reported to be experienced during World War II might actually serve to clarify or

define the individual’s Jewish identity, thereby leading this person to have a more positive

evaluation of this identity. A positive relationship between relative deprivation experienced

during World War II and collective well-being might then be explained by collective identity

clarity.

In order to test our hypothesis that collective identity clarity is responsible for a

positive relationship between relative deprivation and well-being, we explore the cultural

narratives of two groups with histories characterized by intergroup threat—Anglophones and

Francophones living in the Canadian province of Quebec. We begin by reviewing the

literature exploring the relationship between relative deprivation and collective well-being

and argue that, in order to more thoroughly understand this relationship, a group’s history

must be taken into account. We introduce the concept of collective identity clarity measured

across a group’s history and propose that it may be a mechanism responsible for a positive

relationship between feelings of relative deprivation and collective well-being. In Study 1, we

conduct cultural narrative interviews with Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers in order

to gain a knowledge and appreciation of the history of their respective groups, and to pinpoint

the historical periods that are perceived by group members as the most threatening. In Study

2, using the rich information obtained from these narratives, we develop a methodology by

which feelings of relative deprivation and collective identity clarity can be explored across a

group’s history and related to feelings of collective well-being.

5

Page 6: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Relative Deprivation and Collective Well-Being

Group-based or social relative deprivation refers to an overall, subjective feeling of

threat that arises when group members engage in social comparison, and perceive their group

to be receiving less than what they believe they are entitled to (Walker & Pettigrew, 1984).

Relative deprivation theory rests on two main assumptions. The first is that feelings of

relative deprivation are subjectively and not objectively determined. It is not a group’s

objective reality that determines whether or not an individual will feel deprived. Instead, it is

the extent to which group members compare themselves to other groups and perceive their

group to be receiving less than what they feel it deserves. The second assumption is that

feelings of relative deprivation are very much dependent upon the social or cultural context in

which this social comparison is made (Stouffer, Suchman, De Vinney, Star, & Williams,

1949). Compared to the assumption of subjectivity, the assumption that feelings of relative

deprivation are context dependent has received much less research attention. Since Stouffer

and his colleagues originally discussed the contextualized nature of relative deprivation,

empirical research has largely ignored this assumption, and explored relative deprivation in a

decontextualized fashion (see de la Sablonnière, Tougas, & Perenlei, in press). In contrast, the

present research seeks to explore feelings of relative deprivation in the context of a group’s

history. We examine the relationship between historically contextualized feelings of relative

deprivation and collective well-being.

Originally, it was theorized that relative deprivation and well-being would be

negatively related (see Crosby, 1976; Walker, 1999). Perceiving one’s group as relatively

deprived, as worse off than other groups, was assumed to result in a more negative evaluation

of one’s group in terms of its worth, respect and value. A number of empirical studies

confirmed this negative relationship (e.g., de la Sablonnière, Tougas, & Lortie-Lussier, 2009;

Zagefka & Brown, 2005). For example, Zagefka & Brown showed that greater relative

6

Page 7: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

deprivation was associated with lower levels of collective esteem. However, in other research,

the negative relationship between social relative deprivation and collective well-being has not

been obtained. Some empirical studies have failed to find a significant relationship between

these variables (e.g., Tougas & Veilleux, 1988; Walker, 1999), and others have actually

demonstrated a positive relationship between social relative deprivation and collective well-

being (de la Sablonnière & Tougas, 2008; de la Sablonnière, Tougas, & Lortie-Lussier, 2009;

Petta & Walker, 1992). For example, de la Sablonnière and Tougas found that greater social

relative deprivation among nurses was associated with higher reported collective esteem.

Furthermore, de la Sablonnière, Tougas, and Lortie-Lussier demonstrated a positive

relationship between social relative deprivation and collective esteem among Mongolian

participants. In order to attempt to explain these positive relationships between relative

deprivation and collective well-being, the researchers looked to the context in which the

groups under investigation found themselves. Although the researchers did not directly assess

historically contextualized variables, they speculated that the social context in which

participants were basing their judgments of relative deprivation might actually have an impact

on the relationship between relative deprivation and well-being. This speculation is in line

with the assumption that assessments of relative deprivation must be contextualized.

Recent research has more specifically explored relative deprivation, measured across a

group’s history, and related it to collective well-being. Researchers have found that

considering the historical context in which a group finds itself is particularly important when

exploring the relationship between relative deprivation and collective well-being (de la

Sablonnière, 2008; de la Sablonnière, et al., 2009). Specifically, this research has

demonstrated that: 1) it is important to use more than one past or future point of social

comparison when predicting collective esteem using relative deprivation, and 2) to have an

impact on collective esteem, the selected point of social comparison has to be perceived as an

7

Page 8: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

important historical event for group members. That is, instead of an overall impression of a

group’s history, it is certain important events in the past that are the group’s primary reference

points against which their experience of well-being is assessed. Again, such findings point to

the necessity of contextualizing one’s investigation of the relationship between relative

deprivation and collective well-being in a group’s rich history. They highlight the impact that

certain, key historical events might have in shaping a group’s experience of relative

deprivation and collective well-being.

Collective Identity Clarity

What has not been explored in past research linking relative deprivation to collective

well-being is what makes a specific historical context conducive to a positive relationship

between these variables. The present research seeks to explore the historically contextualized

mechanism that might be responsible for a positive relationship between relative deprivation

and collective well-being. We propose that collective identity clarity might act as such a

mechanism.

The clarity of an individual’s collective identity, the extent to which one has clear and

confident beliefs about one’s collective identity, has been theoretically linked to collective

well-being (Taylor, 1997, 2002). Specifically, Taylor posits that without a clear understanding

of one’s collective identity, it would be impossible for an individual to engage in any process

of evaluation pertaining to this identity. Determining if one’s collective identity has worth,

respect, and value, first necessitates a clear understanding of this identity (Taylor). Such

theorizing is in line with classic and current social psychological theorists who posit that a

key factor linked to personal well-being is personal identity clarity (e.g., Baumgardner, 1990;

Campbell, 1990; Erikson, 1968). Researchers have empirically tested the association between

personal identity clarity and well-being and found that people who have a well-articulated

sense of who they are have more positive self-esteem and well-being (Baumgardner, 1990;

8

Page 9: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Campbell, 1990; Campbell et al., 1996). Furthermore, Usborne and Taylor (2009)

demonstrated an empirical link between having a clear cultural identity and greater personal

well-being. The clarity of one’s collective identity is thought to be similarly associated with

collective well-being (Taylor, 1997, 2002).

Based on such theorizing, we argue that, at particular points in a group’s history,

feelings of relative deprivation might serve to clarify collective identity, which in turn, would

be associated with greater feelings of collective well-being. Specifically, we make two

theoretical propositions: 1) Based on Taylor’s (1997, 2002) theory, we propose that collective

identity clarity will act as a mechanism that explains a positive relationship between relative

deprivation and collective well-being. 2) Based on the work of de la Sablonnière and her

colleagues (2009), we propose that the predicted relationships among relative deprivation,

collective identity clarity and collective well-being will be obtained only during certain,

important historical periods. We posit that a period in a group’s history that is considered by

group members as an important intergroup threat might be particularly influential in defining

a group’s identity, and by extension, their experience of collective well-being. Indeed, a

theme that arises when examining the historical narratives of many groups is their perceived

stance compared to another group (Hammack, 2008). The Jewish Israeli identity, for example,

is very much shaped by “historical persecution and victimization”, “vulnerability”, “threat”,

and “existential insecurity” (Hammack, 2009, p. 51). This group has, at least in part, defined

itself based upon themes of extreme intergroup threat.

We therefore hypothesize that, at a point in history that considered by group members

as particularly threatening, the clarity of a group’s collective identity would mediate the

relationship between relative deprivation and current feelings of collective well-being. At

important, threatening points in a group’s history, feelings of relative deprivation would

actually lead to more certainty or clarity about one’s collective identity, that is, it would serve

9

Page 10: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

to define one’s collective identity, which in turn would predict greater collective well-being in

the present. Specifically, a historical period characterized by an important intergroup threat

might be particularly influential in defining the Francophone Quebecer identity. Relative

deprivation that is perceived to have been experienced during this time might thus serve to

clarify the Francophone identity and be linked to positive collective well-being. For the

context of the present study, we propose that this mediational relationship among relative

deprivation, collective identity clarity, and collective well-being will be obtained for both

Francophones and Anglophones if the historical period in question represents a threatening

period for the members of each group.

Research Context

The Canadian province of Quebec revolves around a struggle between the Anglophone

and Francophone communities. The majority Francophones can be viewed as the more

historically disenfranchised group, for they have faced economic disadvantages and threats to

their language and culture due to their minority status in English-dominated North America.

Anglophones, on the other hand, have, at least in the past, enjoyed the privilege of being an

elite minority in Quebec despite living in a predominantly French province where

approximately 80% of the residents are Francophone (Statistics Canada, 2006). The growth of

Francophone nationalism in the 1960s, however, has to some extent reversed the intergroup

power distribution in Quebec, leaving Anglophone Quebecers feeling increasingly threatened

(Bourhis, 1994; Caldwell, 1984; Lepicq & Bourhis, 1995). There have been two emotionally

charged referendums on Quebec sovereignty where French Quebecers voted on whether or

not they wanted to secede from the rest of Anglophone Canada. In addition, there has been

hotly contested legislation designed to protect the French language in Quebec (Bill 101 and

Bill 178).

10

Page 11: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

The Anglophone and Francophone identities have undoubtedly been shaped by their

complex intergroup history. Exploring the psychology of these groups without taking into

account this history would limit our understanding of who they are and how they feel in

today’s world. We therefore seek to predict Anglophone’s and Francophone’s feelings of

collective well-being arising from their experience of history. In Study 1, we examine the

cultural narratives of Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers in order to gain a

comprehensive understanding of this history. Study 1 is necessary to Study 2 in that it will

help us to determine the historical periods that are perceived by participants to be important

intergroup threats. In Study 2, we explore the relationships among participants’ feelings of

relative deprivation and collective identity clarity, measured across their history, and their

feelings of collective well-being. We examine if collective identity clarity might be a

mechanism that explains a positive relationship between relative deprivation and collective

well-being during historical periods that are perceived to be particularly threatening.

Study 1

In order to explore these psychological variables using an historical perspective, a

methodology was required that would allow us to situate relative deprivation and collective

identity clarity in a group’s history. A methodology from which we were able to draw was

McAdams’ (1996, 2001) seminal narrative approach to the study of personal identity. This

approach rests on the assumption that a unified description of one’s identity can be construed

through a story, a spontaneous measure of identity. According to McAdams, individuals

confer unity and meaning to their sense of self by constructing a coherent story that provides

the individual with a purposeful self-history. McAdams (1996, 2001) has developed, and

extensively used the Life Story Interview as a method of accessing people’s representations of

their personal identity. The Life Story Interview is a structured sequence of open-ended

questions in which participants are first asked to divide their life into chapters and to then

11

Page 12: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

briefly describe the content of each chapter. Participants are also required to describe specific

critical events, such as a nadir experience, an event representing a particularly low point in an

individual’s story.

In Study 1, a Cultural Narrative Interview, modeled on McAdams (1996, 2001) Life

Story Interview, was developed. In order to fully account for the complexities of participants’

collective identities, this spontaneous and contextualized method was employed. One of the

foundations of a collective identity is the knowledge of a shared history (Ashmore, Deaux, &

McLaughlin-Volpe, 2004; Hammack, 2008; Taylor, 1997, 2002). Accordingly, each

individual in the present study was asked to “tell the story of your group”, an internally

represented narrative of the particular cultural group of which he/she is a member (Ashmore

et al., 2004). He or she was asked to divide the story into chapters and then to briefly describe

the content of each chapter.

Study 1 had two goals. The first goal was to pinpoint the key narrative chapters for

both Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers. Because the narratives do not represent an

objective history, but rather a history interpreted through the lens of the participant, we

expected that this methodology would allow both groups of participants to describe the

chapters that they perceived to make up their group’s history. These chapters could then be

used in Study 2 as time points in which participants could situate their responses to the

psychological variables of interest. The second goal was to determine which narrative

chapters were perceived by participants as representing important intergroup threats. We

predicted that both Anglophone and Francophone participants would spontaneously describe

events that were particularly threatening for their respective groups. These events, which

would likely be different for both Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers, could then be

used in Study 2 as time points in which our meditational hypothesis could be tested.

Method

12

Page 13: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Participants

Twenty Francophone (ten men and ten women) and twenty Anglophone (ten men and

ten women) Quebecers participated in Study 1. This relatively small sample size was

necessary given the labor-intensive nature of the Cultural Narrative Interview. The narratives

of two Anglophone participants were eliminated because one chose to tell a cultural narrative

related to his Asian-Canadian background and one focused on her personal narrative. A total

of eighteen Anglophone Quebecers (nine women and nine men) were retained for analysis.

The mean age for Anglophone participants was 20.2 years, ranging from 19-23 years old. The

mean age of Francophone participants was 21.5 years, ranging from 18-25. All Anglophone

participants reported speaking English as their maternal language and sixteen reported having

lived in Quebec since they were born. Two Anglophone participants were born in another

Canadian province and had been living in Quebec since the age of five and six years old. All

twenty Francophone participants reported speaking French as their maternal language, as well

as having lived in Quebec since they were born.

Materials and Procedure

The Cultural Narrative Interview was a structured sequence of open-ended questions

that asked participants to construct and narrate their group’s collective story. One male and

one female who were native speakers of English each interviewed five male and five female

Anglophone participants. Similarly, one male and one female who were native speakers of

French each interviewed five male and five female Francophone participants. Participants

took between 40-60 minutes to complete the Cultural Narrative Interview. All interviews

were tape-recorded and later transcribed.

Following McAdams’ (1995) protocol, the Cultural Narrative required participants to

outline their group’s story organized into chapters, and briefly describe the overall content,

key themes, and events of each chapter. In the second part of the interview, participants were

13

Page 14: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

asked to concentrate on a few key events that, according to them, stood out in the story of

their group as particularly important. A key event was described as a specific happening, a

critical incident, or a significant episode in their people’s past. One specific request was for

participants to report a “nadir” experience, an experience that they considered to be a low

point in their group’s history, characterized by extremely negative emotions, such as despair,

disillusionment, terror, or guilt.

Results and Discussion

The first goal of Study 1 was to pinpoint the key narrative chapters for both

Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers. In order to address this first goal, we analyzed the

content of the cultural narratives of Francophone and Anglophone participants, and describe

the significant chapters for both groups. The second goal of Study 1 was to determine which

narrative chapters were perceived by participants as representing important intergroup threats.

In order to accomplish this second goal we analyzed the extent to which participants

spontaneously described events that represented times of intergroup threat and pinpointed the

historical period to which these feelings of threat corresponded.

The Key Narrative Chapters

Francophone participants spontaneously generated an average of 5.6 chapters as

constituting their group’s story. One-hundred percent of Francophone participants described

the arrival of French colonizers in North America as constituting their first chapter. All of the

Francophone participants clearly perceived their cultural narrative beginning as early as the

1500’s, with the discovery of the new world. Next, 95% of Francophone participants

described one or more chapters having to do with early conflicts between French and English

colonizers in new France, namely the 1754-1760 Conquest War, which resulted in Great

Britain taking over New France, and or the Patriot’s Rebellions, also resulting in a defeat for

the French at the hands of the English. Finally, 80% of Francophone participants described

14

Page 15: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

events related to the theme of Francophone Quebecer nationalism as constituting one or more

chapters in their people’s story. The era when Maurice Duplessis was Quebec’s Prime

Minister (1936-1939, 1944-1959), the Quiet Revolution in the 1960s, the creation of the

separatist party “Parti Québébecois” in 1968, the election of René Levesque as Quebec’s

Premier, and/or the general theme of political independence of Quebec from Canada appear to

be important narrative building blocks for Francophone participants. Events that Francophone

Quebecers reported as being particularly important were most often the 1837-1838 Patriots

rebellion and the 1754-1760 British Conquest where the French fought and were defeated by

the English.

Compared to Francophone participants, Anglophone participants spontaneously

generated a smaller number of chapters, describing an average of 3.8 chapters as constituting

their group’s story. For eighty-one percent of Anglophone participants, Chapter 1 involved

the description of European colonizers’ arrival in North America, and/or the early interactions

and conflicts between French and English colonizers in the New World. The next important

narrative building block for Anglophones was events related to the theme of growing

Francophone nationalism from the 1960s on (the period of the Quiet Revolution), and the

consequent feelings of threat within the Anglophone Quebecer community. Seventy-eight

percent of Anglophone participants described the Francophone Quebecer separatist movement

generally, the 1980 and 1995 referendums, and the introduction of education and language

laws in the 1970s as constituting one or more chapters in their people’s history. Events that

Anglophone Quebecers reported as being particularly important were events related to

Francophone nationalism including the two referendums for Quebec sovereignty and the

introduction of Bill 101 where Francophone Quebecers threatened the advantaged status of

Anglophones.

15

Page 16: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Overall, most Francophones and Anglophones reported similar historical events in the

stories of their respective groups. A large number of participants, regardless of their group,

reported events associated with the five following historical periods: The New World era, The

Conquest era, the Duplessis era, The Quiet Revolution era, and finally the Present Time.

Thus, both groups expressed noticeable areas of consensus regarding the basic outlines of

their respective group stories. Although attributing different degrees of attention to significant

events in their histories, both Anglophones and Francophones pointed to similar periods as

making up their cultural narrative. This allowed us to, in Study 2, use these key periods to ask

participants about their perceptions of relative deprivation and collective identity clarity

during each of these periods and to examine the relationships between these perceptions and

collective well-being.

Perceptions of Threat

Even though similar events emerged in the narratives of both Francophone and

Anglophone Quebecers, the attention given to the reported key historical events differed

between the two cultural groups. In fact, both the Francophone and Anglophone narratives

were heavily focused on periods that represented important intergroup threats for their

respective groups. Francophone participants spent more time than Anglophones on the early

conflicts between Anglophones and Francophones (the Conquest era) in which the French

fought and were defeated by the English. In contrast, Anglophones accorded relatively greater

attention to more contemporary historical events, such as the Quiet Revolution era in which

the high status of Anglophone Quebecers was threatened by growing Francophone

nationalism. This is evidenced by, as we reported in the previous section, the fact that

Francophones provided an outline of their people’s story that contained significantly more

chapters than did Anglophones. Anglophone participants generated an average of 3.8 chapters

when telling their people’s story, significantly fewer than their Francophone counterparts (M

16

Page 17: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

= 5.6; t(34) = -4.60, p < .001). This difference is linked to the fact that Francophones provided

a more detailed account of the colonial historical period. Indeed, for more than half (53%) of

our Anglophone participants, Chapter 2 was situated in the second half of the 1900s.

However, Chapter 2 focused on events that took place before 1838 for all of our Francophone

participants (100%), namely, the settling of the French people in Nouvelle-France and the

inter-colonial conflicts. In short, Anglophone Quebecers’ narratives mostly took place after

the 1960s-1970s (the Quiet Revolution era); whereas, several important narrative building

blocks for Francophones refer to events that go back to the period ranging from the early

1500s to 1867 (The Conquest era).

Therefore, for each group, different historical periods, characterized by the experience

of group-level threat, appeared to shape and heavily influence the cultural narratives, and by

extension their cultural identities. We found that the early years of the Conquest Era, during

which the French were defeated by the English, played a key role in shaping the cultural

narrative of Francophone Quebecer participants. In contrast, the Anglophone Quebecer

participants described a cultural narrative that was primarily shaped by the growing threat

from Francophone Quebecers from the era of the Quiet Revolution on.

Further evidence for the perceived threat experienced during these historical periods

comes from an analysis of participants’ reported “nadir” experiences. A “nadir” experience

was defined as a low point in the participant’s cultural narrative. Such a narrative nadir

occurred between the years 1754 and 1838 for almost half of Francophones (45%), compared

to six percent of the Anglophones. In fact, for Anglophones, their main narrative nadir

occurred much more recently: in the 1970s for a third (33%) of them (compared with fifteen

percent of Francophones), and between the years 1990 to 2002 for another third (33%) of

them (compared with 10% of Francophones).

17

Page 18: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

The most threatening period for Francophone Quebecers thus appeared to be the

struggle between Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers (The Conquest era). For

Anglophones, the most threatening period appeared to be the time of the Quiet Revolution.

We therefore predicted that relative deprivation experienced during the Conquest Era for

Francophones and during the Quiet Revolution era for Anglophones would be the most

impactful in terms of shaping or clarifying the group’s collective identity perceived to be

experienced at that time. This clarity of identity would in turn be associated with feelings of

collective well-being. Indeed, initial support for this hypothesis comes from our analyses of

participants’ cultural narratives. Events characterized by intergroup threat appeared to heavily

shape the cultural narratives of both Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers. The relatively

high degree of attention accorded to such threatening events hints at the identity-defining

effect that such events might have. Relative deprivation experienced during these key events

might serve to clarify or define a group’s identity, which in turn would be associated with

greater feelings of collective well-being. This hypothesis will be more systematically tested in

Study 2.

Study 2

In Study 2, we examined perceptions of relative deprivation and collective identity

clarity at different key periods of a group’s history. Based on the narratives of both

Anglophones and Francophones obtained in Study 1, we created a measure that allowed a

new group of Anglophone and Francophone participants in Study 2 to rate their group’s

experiences of relative deprivation and collective identity clarity at these key times. We thus

measured perceived relative deprivation and collective identity clarity at each of these key

periods and examined their inter-relationships and their associations with current feelings of

collective well-being.

Method

18

Page 19: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Participants

A group of Francophone and Anglophone students who had been living in Quebec

since they were born participated in Study 2. Participants were recruited by verbal

announcements made in classrooms, and by means of posters placed in one major

Anglophone university as well as two major Francophone universities in Montreal, Quebec,

Canada. Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers, between 18 and 27 years of age and of

European descent, interested in completing a “History and Well-Being” questionnaire were

asked to contact the principal investigator to schedule an appointment.

A total of 61 Anglophone Quebecers (24 males and 37 females) and 61 Francophones

Quebecers (18 males and 43 females) completed the questionnaire. All Anglophone

participants reported English as their maternal and dominant language, and all Francophones

reported French as their maternal and dominant language. The mean age for Anglophones was

20.9 years old (ranging from 18 to 27), and the mean age for Francophones was 21.5 years old

(ranging from 18 to 27).

Measures

Study 2 was devised in a more structured manner than Study 1. In Study 1,

participants spontaneously narrated their own perception of their group’s story. In Study 2,

the key narrative chapters of the Anglophone and the Francophone Quebecer cultural

narratives that were generated in Study 1 were integrated into a single summary of the

narratives’ shared points and then presented to both Anglophone and Francophone

participants. Moreover, while the methodology in Study 1 precluded the use of a large sample

size, Study 2 used a questionnaire format, which allowed for the testing of a greater number

of Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers. Finally, Study 2 utilized self-report measures in

order to assess the constructs of collective identity clarity, relative deprivation, and collective

well-being.

19

Page 20: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

In the first part of the questionnaire, participants were asked to read the summary of

Quebec’s history. Based on the cultural narrative interviews that were conducted with

Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers in Study 1, the history of Quebec was divided into

five chapters: The New World era (Chapter 1), The Conquest era (Chapter 2), the Duplessis

era (Chapter 3), and The Quiet Revolution era (Chapter 4) and a chapter about the present

(Chapter 5). Each chapter was described as objectively and neutrally as possible by respecting

facts and refraining from editorial comments. Following is a brief description of each

chapter.1

Chapter 1, ‘The New World Era’ ranged from the late 1400s through to 1754 and

described the various European people who reached “The New World”. Chapter 2, ‘The

Conquest Era’ which ranged from 1754 to 1867, described the struggles between French and

English colonies in the New World, which resulted in the domination of Britain over the

French colonies in Quebec. Chapter 3, ‘The Duplessis Era’ ranged from the Canadian

Confederation in 1867 through to the death of Premier Maurice Duplessis in 1959. Chapter

4,‘The Quiet Revolution Era’ which ranged from 1960 to 1995, described the period of

Quebec modernization and empowerment and discussed the arrival of the separatist party

“Parti Québécois” onto Quebec’s political scene, the introduction of language laws aimed at

making French the predominant language in Quebec, and the two referendums on Quebec

sovereignty. Finally, Chapter 5, ‘The Present’ alluded to the main issues faced by Quebecers

today including Quebec’s growing ethnic diversity; and the globalization phenomenon.

For each chapter, participants were first asked to read the short description of the

chapter and then to answer questions pertaining to how they perceived what happened to their

own group during this chapter. These questions assessed relative deprivation and collective

identity clarity experienced during the historical period in question. This exercise was

completed for all five chapters. Following is a detailed description of how we assessed

1 A detailed description of the questionnaire is available upon request to the second author.

20

Page 21: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

relative deprivation and collective identity clarity for each chapter, and how we measured

collective well-being.

Relative deprivation. Measures of relative deprivation used for chapters 1 to 5 were

adapted from previous studies that assessed both the cognitive and the evaluative components

of social relative deprivation (Dambrun et al., 2006; de la Sablonnière & Tougas, 2008). The

cognitive component of social relative deprivation was assessed by asking participants to

indicate “the extent to which English/French Quebecers were advantaged or disadvantaged

compared to French/English Quebecers in terms of… a) social rights, b) political leverage, c)

economic prosperity and d) language and culture”. The scale for these questions ranged from -

5 (definitely disadvantaged), to 0 (Equal), to +5 (definitely advantaged). The following two

questions evaluated the affective component of relative deprivation: “To what extent are you

satisfied with regards to the general situation for English/French Quebecers during this

chapter.”(recoded); “To what extent are you frustrated/angry with regards to the general

situation for English/French Quebecers during this chapter.” The scale for these questions

ranged from 0 (totally satisfied/not at all frustrated), to 5 (moderately), to 10 (not at all

satisfied/totally frustrated). For these two last items, responses were recoded such that the

scale ranged from -5 to 5 to be consistent with the cognitive component of social relative

deprivation. Responses from the 4 cognitive items were reverse-coded, and the 6 items of

social relative deprivation were then added and averaged to form a composite score so that 5

indicated high relative deprivation. The Cronbach’s alpha values for Anglophones’ and

Francophones’ perceived social relative deprivation were .79 for Chapter 1, .95 for Chapter 2,

.89 for Chapter 3, .94 for Chapter 4 and .84 for Chapter 5.

Collective identity clarity. Collective identity clarity was assessed by asking

participants to rate how confident or sure they felt about their overall ratings of what

happened to their group during each chapter of Quebec history. The scale for these questions

21

Page 22: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

ranged from 0 (not at all sure) to 10 (totally sure). This format for evaluating collective

identity clarity has been used successfully by de la Sablonnière and colleagues (2009). In this

previous study, collective identity clarity at the present time was moderately related to

collective esteem (r = .25, p < .001), suggesting that although collective identity clarity and

collective esteem constitute two distinct concepts, they are indeed related.

Collective well-being. This concept was assessed using 6 items from Luhtanen and

Crocker’s Collective Self-Esteem Scale (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992). Collective self-esteem

is often used as an indicator of collective well-being. The membership component and the

private component of collective self-esteem were assessed because both refer to the evaluative

dimension of collective esteem. Membership esteem involves individuals’ judgments of how

good or worthy they are as members of their social ingroup. Private collective self-esteem

assesses one’s personal judgments of how good one’s social ingroup is (for a discussion see

also Roccas et al., 2008). Three statements assessed the membership component of collective

self-esteem: “I am a worthy member of the English (or French) Quebecer community”; “I feel

I don’t have much to offer to the English (or French) Quebecer community” (recoded) and “I

often feel that I am useless member of the English (or French) Quebecer community”

(recoded). Three questions evaluated the private component of collective self-esteem: “I often

regret that I belong to the English (or French) Quebecer community” (recoded); “In general, I

am glad to be a member of the English (or French) Quebecer community”; “I feel good about

the English (or French) Quebecer community”. All ratings were made using 11-point Likert

scales, where 0 = strongly disagree, 5 = neither agree nor disagree, and 10 = strongly agree.

Responses from the 6 items of collective esteem were added and averaged to form a

composite score. Appropriate items were reverse-scored such that a high score on this

variable indicated positive collective esteem. The Cronbach’s alpha for the collective esteem

scale was .79.

22

Page 23: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Results and Discussion

Preliminary analysis

All the scores were normally distributed. All skewness and kurtosis values fell within

an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

in the sample based on the criterion that no one deviated by more than 3 standard deviations

from the variable mean, and also showed a Mahanalobis distance greater than the exclusion

criterion set at p < .001. One-hundred-and-twenty-two participants were thus retained for the

analyses. Considering that no variable possessed more than 5% of missing data (Tabachnick

& Fidell, 2001), no variable was excluded.

Descriptive Analysis

In this section, we first present descriptive analyses for all variables. Second, using

group-based trajectory modeling (Jones & Nagin, 2007; Jones, Nagin & Roeder, 2001; Nagin,

1999), we explore each cultural group’s trajectory of perceptions of intergroup threat over

time. This was done in order to determine if group members’ experiences of threat was

consistent with what we would predict based on our analysis of participants’ cultural

narratives in Study 1. This analysis helped us determine if the Conquest Era for Francophone

Quebecers and the Quiet Revolution Era for Anglophone Quebecers, are indeed perceived by

members of these groups as periods in which their group experienced high levels of

intergroup threat.

Descriptive analyses of all variables are depicted in Table 1. These data revealed that

Anglophones reported the highest levels of relative deprivation during Chapter 4, the Quiet

Revolution Era (M =1.9, SD = 1.7) compared to their reported levels of relative deprivation

for the other chapters. In contrast, Francophones reported the highest levels of relative

deprivation during Chapter 2, the Conquest Era (M =3, SD = 1.6). Both Anglophones and

Francophones reported moderate levels of collective identity clarity for Chapter 1, but

23

Page 24: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

relatively high levels of collective identity clarity for Chapters 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6, as scores were

situated above the scale’s midpoint. Additionally, the means for collective esteem indicated

relatively high levels of collective well-being among both Anglophones and Francophones, as

scores for both groups were situated above the scale’s midpoint. An ANOVA comparing

Anglophones and Francophones revealed a significant difference on collective esteem (F (1,

120) = 13.7, p < .001), such that Francophone Quebecers had a higher level of overall

collective esteem (M = 8.2, SD = 1.4) than did Anglophone Quebecers (M = 7.4, SD = 1.1).

<Insert Table 1 here>

In order to identify Anglophones’ and Francophones’ perceptions of threat over time,

group-based trajectory modeling of participants’ relative deprivation scores (Jones & Nagin,

2007; Jones, Nagin & Roeder, 2001; Nagin, 1999) was conducted. This analysis has been

successfully used in the domain of social psychology (see de la Sablonnière, Taylor, Perozzo,

& Sadykova, 2009), and allowed us to test which historical period was judged to be the most

threatening for Francophones and Anglophones.

Group-based trajectory modeling provides a flexible method for identifying distinctive

clusters of individual trajectories within a population. The statistical model underlying group-

based trajectories uses finite mixtures of specified probability distributions to determine, by

maximum likelihood, the parameter estimates describing the model that best fits the data

(Jones & Nagin, 2007; Nagin, 1999, 2005). In order to estimate the optimal model, the

number and shape (linear or U-shaped for example) of trajectories is a key step in model

selection. Consistent with the recommendations of D’Unger, Land, McCall, and Nagin (1998)

and Nagin (2005), the Bayesian Information criterion (BIC) was used to select the optimal

model. The BIC provides an index of how well the model selected fits the data, in addition to

favouring parsimony. In general, the BIC closest to zero denotes the most appropriate model.

As all the variables explored in the present study were measured using Likert-type scales, the

24

Page 25: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

censored normal distribution (CNORM) was used to estimate trajectories and group

memberships (Jones & Nagin, 2007; Jones et al., 2001; Nagin, 1999, 2005). The program

used to perform group-based trajectory modeling is a customized SAS-based procedure

(PROC TRAJ; Jones et al., 2001). An important output of the model is the posterior

probabilities of group membership. These probabilities estimate the probabilities of belonging

to each group for each individual. Consequently, each participant in the sample is assigned to

the group with the largest posterior probabilities of group membership.

Table 2 reports BIC scores for different models tested. A model with two trajectories,

model 4, was found to be the best fitting model as revealed by the BIC. Figure 1 shows the

estimated trajectories associated with relative deprivation perceived by Anglophones and

Francophones over time. These trajectories represent the historical pattern of a group’s

perceived relative deprivation.

<Insert Table 2 here>

Inspection of Figure 1 shows that the optimal model includes two different trajectories

of relative deprivation for Francophones and Anglophones. Indeed, each trajectory is directly

linked to the particular history of each group. Specifically, all Francophones, that is 100 % or

61 persons, follow the typical trajectory of Francophones. Ninety-eight point four percent or

60 Anglophones followed the Anglophone’s typical pattern; whereas, 1.6 % of the

Anglophones, that is one Anglophone, followed the Francophone’s typical pattern. This case

was removed from further analysis because it differed significantly from other members of

the group and thus, was considered an outlier.

<Insert Figure 1 here>

From Figure 1, we see that Francophones perceived their level of relative deprivation to

increase from the New World (Chapter 1) to the struggle between Anglophone and

25

Page 26: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Francophone Quebecers (Chapter 2; the Conquest era), to then decrease in the transition

between the Conquest Era and the Quiet Revolution periods (Chapter 4). Finally, their level of

relative deprivation increases at a slightly slower pace until the time of our study (Chapter 5).

The struggle between Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers (Chapter 2, the Conquest Era)

appears to represent a major threat for Francophones.

For Anglophones, the expected trajectory shows quite a different pattern of relative

deprivation, such that their level of relative deprivation greatly decreased from the New

World (Chapter 1) to the struggle between Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers (Chapter

2; the Conquest Era), to then increase until the Quiet revolution (Chapter 4). However,

Anglophones felt less relative deprivation in the present (Chapter 5) than during the Quiet

Revolution. Accordingly, the Quiet Revolution (Chapter 4) constituted the most major threat

for Anglophone Quebecers.

Intercorrelations and Mediation Analysis

Once we were certain that these two periods (the Conquest era for Francophones and

the Quiet Revolution era for Anglophones) constituted the most threatening periods for the

two groups involved, we tested our hypothesis that collective identity clarity acts as a

psychological mechanism that mediates the relationship between group-based relative

deprivation and collective esteem for these periods. However, we first examined the

intercorrelations among relative deprivation, collective identity clarity and collective esteem

across all chapters in order to examine if these correlations might offer preliminary support

for our hypothesis. For Francophones we then tested the hypothesized mediational

relationship for the Conquest Era, and for Anglophones, we tested the mediational

relationship for the era of the Quiet Revolution.

First, the intercorrelations among relative deprivation, collective identity clarity and

collective esteem were examined across all chapters for both Francophone and Anglophone

26

Page 27: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Quebecers (See Table 3). These intercorrelations provided preliminary evidence in support of

our hypothesis. For Francophones, the link between relative deprivation and collective esteem

was positive (r =.47, p < .01) only for Chapter 2, the Conquest Era. For all other periods, this

relationship was non-significant (Chapters 1, 4, and 5) or marginally significant (Chapter 3).

Furthermore, for Chapter 2, the Conquest Era, collective identity clarity was positively

associated with both relative deprivation and collective esteem. These results indicate, that

during a historical period of high intergroup threat, relative deprivation, collective identity

clarity and collective esteem are all positively related.

Similarly, for Anglophones, the link between relative deprivation and collective

esteem was positive (r = .27, p < .05 ) only for Chapter 4, the time of the Quiet Revolution.

For all other periods, this relationship was non-significant (Chapters 1-3) or marginally

significant (Chapter 5). Furthermore, for Chapter 4, the time of the Quiet Revolution,

collective identity clarity was significantly positively associated with both relative deprivation

and collective esteem. Again, these results indicate that, for Anglophone Quebecers, during a

historical period of high intergroup threat, relative deprivation, collective identity clarity, and

collective esteem are all positively related.

<Insert Table 3 here>

In order to specifically test the hypothesis that collective identity clarity mediates the

relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem, the ratings provided by

Francophones for the Conquest Era were subjected to multiple regressions (Barron & Kenny,

1986; see Figure 2). Standardized coefficients (βs) are reported. In support of our hypothesis,

we found that group-based relative deprivation significantly predicted collective identity

clarity (a = .48, p <.001) and collective esteem (d = .47, p < .001) when it was the only

predictor. When collective identity clarity was introduced to the model, the direct effect of

relative deprivation on collective esteem dropped (c = .33, p < .05), and collective identity

27

Page 28: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

clarity significantly predicted collective esteem, b = .28, p < .05. The Sobel test indicated that

the indirect effect of relative deprivation on collective esteem via collective identity clarity

was significant, z = 1.97, p = .05, providing evidence for partial mediation. As predicted,

collective identity clarity partially mediated the relationship between relative deprivation and

collective esteem for Francophone Quebecers during the Conquest Era.

<Insert Figure 2 here>

The ratings provided by Anglophones for the era of the Quiet Revolution were also

analyzed using multiple regressions (see Figure 3). We found that relative deprivation

significantly predicted collective identity clarity (a = .35, p < .05) and collective esteem (d

= .27, p < .05) when it was the only predictor. When collective identity clarity was introduced

to the model, the direct effect of relative deprivation on collective esteem completely

disappeared (b = . 19, n.s.), and collective identity clarity was a marginally significant

predictor of collective esteem, c = .24, p = .08. The Sobel test indicated that the indirect effect

of relative deprivation on collective esteem via collective identity clarity approached

significance, z = 1.53, p = .13. Collective identity clarity mediated the relationship between

relative deprivation and collective esteem at a level that approached significance for

Anglophone Quebecers during the era of the Quiet Revolution.

<Insert Figure 3 here>

At a time of threat to the group, be it the Conquest era for Francophones, or the Quiet

Revolution era for Anglophones, the relationship between relative deprivation and collective

esteem was positive. For Francophone Quebecers, this relationship was partially mediated by

collective identity clarity. A threatening event might serve to clarify a group’s identity, which

in turn is associated with greater collective esteem. For Anglophone Quebecers, the

relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem was mediated by collective

identity clarity at a level that only approached significance.

28

Page 29: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

It is not surprising that we found stronger support for our hypothesis among

Francophone Quebecers than among Anglophones. If we examine the broader context in

which the Anglophone and Francophone Quebecer communities co-exist, it is evident that

Francophones are a relatively small minority group in a largely Anglophone North America.

Indeed, Francophones constitute only 22% of the Canadian population (Statistics Canada,

2006) and less than one percent of the American population (Shin & Bruno, 2003). Although

Anglophones are a minority in the province of Quebec, they are by far the majority in North

America. Thus, the Francophone experience of group level threat may be, in general, much

more influential in defining or clarifying the Francophone Quebecer identity. In contrast, the

experience of intergroup threat may be much less a part of the Anglophone Quebecer identity.

Historical intergroup threat might then be perceived as playing less of a role in clarifying the

Anglophone Quebecer identity, making collective identity clarity less likely to mediate the

relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem. Nonetheless, even among

Anglophone Quebecers, a group for whom intergroup threat is not an integral component of

their current identity, we found results that offer preliminary support for our hypothesis.

Collective identity clarity mediated the relationship between relative deprivation and

collective esteem at a level that approached significance during a historical period of high

intergroup threat.

General Discussion

For the first time, the relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem

was explored using an historical perspective. We used an analyses rooted in a group’s history

in order to understand a positive relationship between relative deprivation and collective well-

being. Study 1 asked Anglophone and Francophone participants to construct cultural

narratives by recounting the story of their respective groups. From these cultural narratives,

we were able to gain rich information pertaining to the collective identities of both

29

Page 30: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers. The narratives pointed to the significant chapters

for both Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers, allowing us to develop a methodology in

Study 2 that was contextualized in the group’s history. Furthermore, in Study 1, participants

spontaneously emphasized events that were particularly threatening for their group, events

that appeared to be very influential in terms of defining their group’s identity.

Study 2 was conducted in order to predict collective well-being for both Anglophone

and Francophone Quebecers based on their perceptions of group-based relative deprivation

and collective identity clarity, measured at different periods or chapters in a group’s history.

We sought to explore if collective identity clarity might be a historically contextualized

mechanism that explains a positive relationship between relative deprivation and collective

well-being. We predicted that collective identity clarity would mediate a positive relationship

between relative deprivation and collective esteem at important, threatening, and thus defining

events in a group’s history. Our prediction was supported when Francophone Quebecers were

asked about the Conquest era. This prediction was supported, at a level that approached

significance, when Anglophone Quebecers were asked about the era of the Quiet Revolution.

Theoretical Contributions

Consistent with theorists and researchers who call for a more historically

contextualized social psychology (e.g., Gergen, 1973; Gjerde, 2004; Liu & Hilton,

2005;Okazaki, David, & Abelman, 2008), our studies provide empirical evidence pointing to

the importance of exploring social psychological variables using an historical perspective.

Indeed, the relationships among our variables of interest differed depending on the historical

period in question. Specifically, for both Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers, relative

deprivation was positively associated with collective well-being only when it was examined at

key historical periods that represented a time of maximum intergroup threat. At times that

were perceived to be the most threatening to the group, relative deprivation was positively

30

Page 31: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

associated with collective identity clarity, which in turn was associated with collective well-

being. In future research then, it is important to avoid examining relative deprivation and

collective identity clarity in a decontextualized manner, solely at the present time, or as an

overall feeling of a group’s history. Such an analysis would gloss over important and

meaningful nuances in their relationships with collective well-being.

Our work also reinforces current theorizing in relative deprivation research (see de la

Sablonnière, 2008; de la Sablonnière, et al., 2009) in that it emphasizes the importance of

taking key historical events into consideration when evaluating relative deprivation and its

relationship with collective esteem. The present studies sought to understand what the

historically contextualized mechanism might be that accounts for a positive relationship

between relative deprivation and collective well-being. We found that when individuals think

of times of great intergroup threat, feelings of relative deprivation might actually serve to

define a group’s identity, thus leading group members to have a more positive evaluation of

this group. Although no causal relationships can be established here, our research points to the

potential importance of threatening periods in a group’s history for defining that group’s

identity and thus creating higher levels of collective well-being. Interestingly, it was not the

most positive or the most recent events that were the most influential for collective well-being

here. Instead, it was those that were the most threatening.

However, the results of our study also suggest that further research needs to be

conducted exploring the relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem using

an historical perspective. Our findings showed a positive relationship between relative

deprivation and collective esteem during particularly threatening historical periods and non-

significant relationships between these variables when participants were referring to other

points in their group’s history. The original assumption that relative deprivation is negatively

related to collective esteem (see Walker, 1999) was not supported here. Some recent research

31

Page 32: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

that takes a group’s history into account has actually found a negative relationship between

relative deprivation measured at important historical periods and collective esteem (see de la

Sablonnière et al., 2009); however these periods represented positive events for the group and

not threatening ones. Future research might shed more light on the inconsistent relationships

between relative deprivation and collective esteem by further exploring the nature of the key

historical period in question combined with the eventual outcome of this period. The extent to

which the period represents an eventual gain or loss for the group might influence the

relationship between relative deprivation and esteem. Again, we point to the importance of

contextualizing one’s examination of group level well-being in this group’s rich and varied

history.

Methodological Contributions

Both of the present studies employed an underused, but promising methodology—the

Cultural Narrative. Hammack (2008) along with Ashmore et al. (2004) argue for the use of

such a methodology when studying identity issues. Hammack (2008) posits that the use of

narratives is an interdisciplinary and comprehensive approach to studying identity, while

Ashmore et al. (2004), argue that a group’s shared story is an important component of their

collective identity. Inspired by McAdams Life Story Interview (1996, 2001), we employed a

narrative methodology and found that it had considerable benefits. It provided us with a

nuanced understanding of each group’s history, an understanding that was more complete

than what we might have gleaned from a more traditional approach to the study of collective

identity. Specifically, we were able to understand the differential impact of particular

historical periods on participants’ experience of collective well-being, and to pinpoint periods

that represented times of important intergroup threat. Future research examining collective

identity and related concepts would undoubtedly benefit from such a comprehensive

methodology. By situating social psychological variables in a group’s history a more

32

Page 33: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

comprehensive understanding of these variables and their impact on the individual might

emerge.

Applied Contributions

From the present research it appears that reflecting on a historical period that

represents an important threat for one’s group might serve to define one’s collective identity,

which would in turn be associated with greater feelings of collective well-being. This finding

points to the potential importance of recounting and referring to a group’s history, particularly

to times of historical threat. For example, for Jewish people, recounting the extremely

threatening events of World War II, although representing a dark and distressing historical

period, might be extremely important for defining the identity of a modern Jewish person.

Having a clearly defined Jewish identity might in turn be linked with positive feelings about

this identity. For groups faced with cultural identity challenges, for example, immigrants to a

new culture, or groups struggling with a history of colonization, articulating and defining an

especially threatening historical event, perhaps one that the group has collectively overcome,

might actually result in a clarification of their collective identity, which in turn might lead to

feelings of collective well-being. An intervention constructed around a group’s history might

be more effective than simply boosting group pride or psychological well-being on a case-by-

case basis, and would instead be a contextualized method of building the foundation of

collective identity, and by extension, collective well-being.

References

Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., & McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizational

framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of

33

Page 34: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 80-114.

Baerger, D. R. & McAdams, D. P. (1999). Life story coherence and its relation to

psychological well-being. Narrative Inquiry, 9, 69-96.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in

social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself: Self-certainty and self- affect.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1062-1072.

Bouris, R. Y. (1994). Ethnic and language attitudes in Quebec. In J. W. Berry & J. A.

Laponce (Eds.), Ethnicity and culture in Canada: The research landscape (pp. 322-

360). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

Caldwell, G. (1984). Anglo-Quebec: Demographic realities and options for the future. In R.

Y. Bourhis (Ed.). Conflict and language planning in Quebec (pp. 205-221). Clevedon:

Multilingual Matters.

Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 59, 538-549.

Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., &

Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and

cultural boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141-156.

Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review, 83, 85-

113.

Dambrun, M., Taylor, D. M., McDonald, D. A., Crush, J., & Méot, A. (2006). The relative

deprivation- gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans towards

immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 91, 1032-1044.

34

Page 35: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

de la Sablonnière, R. (2008). Le bien-être psychologique des francophones et des

anglophones : le rôle des points tournants de l’histoire du Québec. Diversité Urbaine,

automne, 131-144.

de la Sablonnière, R., Taylor, D. M., Perozzo, C., & Sadykova, N. (2009). Reconceptualizing

relative deprivation in the context of dramatic social change: The challenge

confronting the people of Kyrgyzstan. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39,

325-345.

de la Sablonnière, R., & Tougas, F. (2008). Relative deprivation and social identity in times

of dramatic social change: The case of nurses. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,

38, 2293-2314.

de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., & Lortie-Lussier, M. (2009). Dramatic social change in

Russia and Mongolia: Connecting relative deprivation to social identity. Journal of

Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 327-348.

de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., & Perenlei, O. (in press). Beyond social and temporal

comparisons: the role of temporal inter-group comparisons in the context of dramatic

social change in Mongolia. Journal of Social Psychology.

D’Unger, A. V., Land, K. C., McCall, P. L., & Nagin, D. S. (1998). How many latent classes

of delinquent/criminal careers? Results from mixed Poisson regression analyses.

Journal of Sociology, 103, 1593-1630.

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.

Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 26, 309-320.

Gjerde, P. F. (2004). Culture, power, and experience: Toward a person-centered cultural

psychology. Human Development, 47, 138-157.

Hammack, P. L. (2008). Narrative and the cultural psychology of identity. Personality and

35

Page 36: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Social Psychology Review, 12, 222-247.

Hammack, P. L. (2009). Exploring the reproduction of conflict through narrative. Israeli

youth motivated to participate in a coexistence program. Peace and Conflict: Journal

of Peace Psychology, 15, 49-74.

Jones, B. L., & Nagin, D. S. (2007). Advances in group-based trajectory modeling and an

SAS procedure for estimating them. Sociological Methods and Research, 35, 542-571.

Jones, B. L., Nagin, D. S., & Roeder, K. (2001). A SAS procedure based on mixture models

for estimating developmental trajectories. Sociological Methods and Research, 29,

374-393.

Lepicq, D., & Bourhis, R. Y. (1995). Aménagement linguistique et norme langagière au

Québec. Situations du français, 33, 109-128.

Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weighs on the present: Social representations

of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44,

1-21.

Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one's

social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 302-318.

McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self : A contemporary

framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 295-321.

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5,

100-122.

McGuire, W. J. (1984). Search for the self : Going beyond self-esteem and the reactive self.

In R. A. Zucker, J. Arnoff, & A. I. Rabin (Eds.), Personality and the prediction of

behavior (pp. 73-120). Montreal : Academic Press, Inc.

Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semiparametric, group-based

36

Page 37: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139-157.

Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge, MA : Harvard

University Press.

Okazaki, S., David, E. J. R., & Abelman, N. (2008). Colonialism and the psychology of

culture. Social and Personality Compass, 2, 90-106.

Petta, G., & Walker, I. (1992). Relative deprivation and ethnic identity. British Journal of

Social Psychology, 31, 285-293.

Roccas, S., Sagiv, L., Schwartz, S., Halevy, N., & Eidelson, R. (2008). Toward a unifying

model of identification with groups: Integrating theoretical perspectives. Personality

and Social Psychology Review, 12, 280-360.

Shin, H. B., & Bruno, R. (2003). Language use and English-speaking ability: 2000. Census

2000 Brief. US Census Bureau.

Statistics Canada (2006). Detailed Mother Tongue (148), Single and Multiple Language

Responses (3) and Sex (3) for the Population of Canada, Provinces, Territories,

Census Metropolitan Areas and Census Agglomerations, 2006 Census - 20% Sample

Data, Canadian Census 2006.

Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. S., DeVinney, L. C., Star, S. A., & Williams, R. M. (1949). The

American soldier: Adjustment during army life (Vol. 1). Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Needham

Heights: Allyn and Bacon.

Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of

intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G.

Austin & S. Worchel. (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-

37

Page 38: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

47). Monterey:Brooks/Cole.

Taylor, D.M. (1997). The quest for collective identity: The plight of disadvantaged ethnic

minorities. Canadian Psychology, 38, 174-189.

Taylor, D. M., (2002) The quest for identity: From minority groups to Generation Xers.

Westport, CT: Praeger Publications.

Tougas, F., & Veilleux, F. (1998). The influence of identification, collective relative

deprivation, and procedure of implementation on women’s response to affirmative

action: A causal modeling approach. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 20,

15-28.

Usborne, E., & Taylor, D. M. (2009, accepted). The role of cultural identity clarity for self-

concept clarity, self-esteem and subjective well-being. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin.

Walker, I. (1999). The effects of personal and group relative deprivation on personal and

collective self-esteem. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 2, 365-380.

Walker, I., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1984). Relative deprivation theory: An overview and

conceptual critique. British Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 301-310.

Zagefka, H., & Brown, R. (2005). Comparisons and perceived deprivation in ethnic minority

settings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 467-482.

Table 1.

Means and Standard Deviations for Independent and Dependent Variables

Anglophones (n=61) Francophones (n=61)

38

Page 39: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

M SD M SD

Relative deprivation (-5 low; 5 high)

Chapter 1: New World -0.7 1.0 -0.8 1.7

Chapter 2 : Conquest -2.5 1.3 3.0 1.6

Chapter 3 : Duplessis -1.6 1.3 1.6 1.5

Chapter 4: Quiet Rev. 1.9 1.7 -1.8 1.6

Chapter 5: Now 0.8 1.7 -0.8 1.3

Collective identity clarity(min. 0; max. 10)

Chapter 1: New World 4.8 2.4 5.1 2.2

Chapter 2 : Conquest 6.5 2.1 6.9 1.8

Chapter 3 : Duplessis 6.3 2.1 6.3 1.8

Chapter 4: Quiet Rev. 7.5 1.9 7.4 1.5

Chapter 5: Now 7.5 1.7 7.3 1.8

Collective esteem(min. 0; max. 10)

7.4 1.4 8.2 1.1

Table 2.

Bayesian information criterion (BIC) by model type

Model K Order BIC

1 1 2 -1368.93

2 2 2, 2 -1358.50

3 3 2, 2, 2 -1368.11

4 2 3, 3 -1213.37

Note. K, the number of groups. The order indicates

whether the trajectory was fit using a constant (0), a

linear (1), a quadratic (2), or a cubic (3) function.

39

Page 40: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives 40

Page 41: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Table 3.

Correlations among relative deprivation (RD), collective identity clarity (CIC) and

collective esteem (CE) per chapter.

Anglophones (n = 61) Francophones (n = 61)

RD-CIC CIC-CE RD-CE RD-CIC CIC-CE RD-CE

Chapter 1: New World -.07 .00 -.08 -.34** .09 -.16

Chapter 2 : Conquest -.14 .01 -.19 .48** .44** .47**

Chapter 3 : Duplessis -.06 .13 -.03 .11 .33** .24 t

Chapter 4: Quiet Rev. .36** .30* .27* -.17 .26* .13

Chapter 5: Now .37** .25 t .25 t .008 .36** .09

Note. t p ≤ .10, * p ≤ .05, ** p ≤ .01 (2-tailed).

41

Page 42: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Figure 1. The expected trajectories of Francophone and Anglophone participants’ feelings of

relative deprivation across chapters.

Figure 2. Relationships among relative deprivation, collective identity clarity and collective

esteem for Francophone Quebecers during the Conquest era.

Figure 3. Relationships among relative deprivation, collective identity clarity and collective

esteem for Anglophone Quebecers during the Quiet Revolution era.

42

Page 43: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Figure 1.

43

Page 44: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Figure 2.

Note. ** p < .001, * p < .05. a,b,c,d = standardized coefficients.

Figure 3.

44

Relative Deprivation

Collective Esteemd = .47**

Collective Esteem

Collective Identity Clarity

Relative Deprivation

a = .48** c = .28*

b = .33*

Relative Deprivation

Collective Esteemd = .27*

Collective Esteem

Collective Identity Clarity

Relative Deprivation

a = .36* c = .24†

b = .19 n.s.

Page 45: Study 2 - Université de Montréal · Web viewAll skewness and kurtosis values fell within an acceptable range of -1.59 to 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

Cultural Narratives

Note. * p < .05, † p < .10 . a,b,c,d = standardized coefficients.

45