19
Socialisation is heavily centred upon the development of the concept of self. How a sense of self emerges—the awareness that the individual has a distinct identity, separate from other? This problem of the emergence of self is a much-debated one. This is because the most prominent theories about child development emphasise different aspects of socialisation. Development of self: During the first months of life, the infant possesses little or no understanding of differences between human beings and material objects in the environment, and has no awareness of self. Children do not begin to use concepts like T, ‘me’ and ‘you’ until the age of about two or after. Only gradually do they then come to understand that others have distinct identities, consciousness and needs separate from their own. The awareness of self arises in interaction with the social and non-social environment. The social environment is especially important. The development of our personal identity—or self—is a complicated process. The realisation of a distinctive personality is an even more complicated process, which continues throughout life. The child learns to differentiate between various other people by names—Daddy, Mummy and Baby and he begins to use T which is a sign of definite self-consciousness—that

Sociology

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Theory of sociology

Citation preview

Page 1: Sociology

Socialisation is heavily centred upon the development of the concept of self.

How a sense of self emerges—the awareness that the individual has a distinct

identity, separate from other? This problem of the emergence of self is a

much-debated one. This is because the most prominent theories about child

development emphasise different aspects of socialisation.

Development of self:

During the first months of life, the infant possesses little or no understanding

of differences between human beings and material objects in the environment,

and has no awareness of self. Children do not begin to use concepts like T,

‘me’ and ‘you’ until the age of about two or after. Only gradually do they then

come to understand that others have distinct identities, consciousness and

needs separate from their own.

The awareness of self arises in interaction with the social and non-social

environment. The social environment is especially important. The

development of our personal identity—or self—is a complicated process. The

realisation of a distinctive personality is an even more complicated process,

which continues throughout life.

The child learns to differentiate between various other people by names—

Daddy, Mummy and Baby and he begins to use T which is a sign of definite

self-consciousness—that he is becoming aware of himself as a distinct human

being (Cooley, 1908). As time passes and social experiences accumulate, he

forms an image of the kind of person he is—an image of self. This self

develops gradually in the child.

How self emerges?

Page 2: Sociology

This is main focus of the problem of socialisation. Here, we shall discuss

views of some celebrated authors.

Freud’s theory (psychoanalysis):

Sigmund Freud, the Austrian psychiatrist and founder of psychoanalysis, was

not directly concerned with the problem of the individual’s socialisation (he

has not used the word ‘socialisation’ anywhere in his writings), he

nevertheless contributed amply toward the clarification of the process of

personality development. Distinguished sociologist T. Parsons has also

adopted Freud’s account of personality development to provide the

psychological underpinnings of his theory of socialisation.

Freud challenged Mead and Cooley’s concept of socialised self who saw no

separate identity of self and society. Freud believed that rational portion of

human conduct was like the visible portion of an iceberg, with the lager part of

human motivation resting in the unseen, unconscious focus which powerfully

affects human conduct.

Process of personality development:

Freud’s theory of personality (self) development rests on the following

process.

He divided the self (human mind) into three parts:

(1) The id,

(2) The ego, and

(3) The superego.

Page 3: Sociology

(1) The id represents the instinctive desires, which may be viewed as an

unsocialised aspect of human nature. It is the obscure inaccessible part of our

personality. It is the source of drives (animal impulses of man—hunger,

aggression, and sexual drives) demanding immediate satisfaction in some

way or the other. These impulses are controlled and partially repressed into

the unconscious, while a reality-oriented conscious self or ego appears.

(2) The ego is the acting individual. It serves as the mediator between desires

and action representing the urges of the id when necessary. It tries to mediate

the resultant conflicting demands of the id and the superego.

(3) The superego (the conscience) represents the social ideals (norms,

values, traditions, the idea of moral and immoral etc.). It is seen as

internalised parental and social authority. The parent is no longer outside

telling the child what to do, but is inside the psyche, invisibly overseeing the

child’s thoughts and actions, praising what is right and making the child feel

guilty for wrong doing. For Parsons, the Freudian superego is the key device

by which society’s values are transmitted to the child. Thus society’s norms

and values are passed down from generation to generation in this way.

The Freudian theory contends that people possess a number of drives or

urges connected with satisfying basic needs, such as the need for food or

sexual release. These urges, known collectively as the id, seek immediate

satisfaction.

In society, however, instant gratification is rarely possible, and id must be

controlled. This control is accomplished by what Freud called the superego,

the part of the mind that incorporates society’s rules. The id and the superego

Page 4: Sociology

are in continual conflict. When we are hungry, for example, our id urges us to

satisfy’s our hunger in the quickest way possible.

Our superego, however, tells us that this is an unacceptable way to satisfy our

hunger. Freud stated that normally developing children develop ego, which

reconciles the demands of the id and superego as much as possible.

Freud presents the relation between the id and the ego as similar to that

between a horse and its rider. The function of the ego is that of the rider

guiding the horse which is the id. But, like the rider, the ego sometimes is

unable to guide the horse as it wishes and perforce must guide the id in the

direction it is determined to go or in a slightly different direction.

Evaluation of Freud’s theory:

Freud’s all theories have inspired bitter controversies and numerous

interpretations. This theory (development of self) is opposite to the views of

Cooley and Mead. Cooley and Mead have demonstrated that the very

emergence of the self is a social process and not a psychological process as

contended by Freud. They have viewed self and society as two aspects of the

same thing, whereas Freud finds that the self and society are often opponents

and self is basically anti-social.

There is always a clash between the impulses of the self and the restraints of

society. Mead and Cooley, on the other hand, viewed self and society as

merely different expressions of the same phenomenon. Cooley (1902) writes:

“A separate individual is an abstraction unknown to experience In other words;

‘society’ and ‘individuals’ do not denote separate phenomena but are simply

collective and distributive aspects of the same thing.” Moreover, it is very

Page 5: Sociology

difficult to verify empirically the three layers of human mind—id, ego and

superego as suggested by Freud.

Cooley’s theory of the ‘looking-glass self:

How does a person arrive at a notion of the kind of person he is? According to

Charles Horton Cooley (1902), this concept of self develops through a gradual

and complicated process which continues throughout life. He pointed out that

when we refer to the self, when we use the word T (the social self is referred

to by such words as I, me, mine and myself; the individual distinguishes his

‘self from that of others), we usually not referring to our physical body.

We use the word T to refer to opinions, desires, ideas, feelings, or evaluations

of others with whom we interact. Whether one is intelligent, average or stupid,

attractive or ugly, these and many other ideas of the self are learned from the

reactions of our associates. Even, the elementary knowledge that one tends

to be fat or thin, tall or short is a comparative judgment based on the opinions

of others.

This process of discovering the nature of the self from the reactions of others

has been labelled the looking-glass self by Cooley. Looking-glass self simply

means how we see ourselves through the eyes of other people. The idea of

looking-glass seems to have been taken from Thackeray’s book Vanity Fair in

which it is said: “The world is a looking glass and gives back to every man the

reflection of his own face.”

Each to each a looking-glass,

Reflects the other that doth pass.

Page 6: Sociology

Just as we see our face, figure and dress in the mirror which gives an image

of the physical self, so the perception of the reactions of others gives an

image of the social self. We “know”, for instance, that we are talented in some

field but less talented in others. This knowledge or perception comes to us

from the reactions of other persons. Through play and other group activities,

one is also helped to perceive the feelings of others and their feelings toward

him.

Stages of formation of self:

According to Cooley, there are three steps (stages) in the process of

formation of looking-glass self:

1. The imagination of our appearance of how we look to others.

2. The imagination of their judgment of how we look or how we think others

judge our behaviour.

3. How we feel about their judgment, i.e., our feelings (self feeling) about their

judgments.

We know that we exist, that we are beautiful or ugly, serious or funny, lively or

dull etc., through the way other people think of us, of course, but we can

imagine how we appear to them and how they evaluate our appearance. We

often respond to these imagined evaluations with pride, embarrassment,

humiliation or some other feeling. In conclusion, the looking-glass self means

that we see ourselves and we respond to ourselves, not as we are and not as

other think we are, but as we imagine others think we are.

Evaluation of Cooley’s theory:

Page 7: Sociology

There is a difference of opinion among some scholars about the functioning of

the ‘looking-glass self. Several researches have been done to seek empirical

evidence of the correlation between one’s perception of responses of others

and the actual judgments they have made of him. These studies find that

there is often a significant variation between individual’s perception of how

other pictures him and the views they actually hold. Clearly, it is our

perception of the responses of others and not their mutual responses which

self-image, and these perceptions are often inaccurate (Horton and Hunt,

1964).

Theory of G.H. Mead (I and me):

American philosopher and social psychologist George Herbert Mead (1934)

developed his ideas about the same time that Cooley did in the early years of

the twentieth century. He gave particular attention to the emergence of a

sense of self. He emphasised the two-part structure of this self and

represented this by the terms ‘I’ and ‘me’. He described in detail the whole

process of child development and explained how children learn to use the

concepts off and ‘me’.

The ‘I’ is the immediate response of an individual to others. It is the unpre-

dictable and creative aspect of the self. People do not know in advance what

the action of the ‘I’ will be. “The ‘I’ is the unsocialised infant—a bundle of

spontaneous wants and desires” (Giddens, 1997). The ‘I’ reacts against ‘me’.

The ‘me’ consists of the attitudes of others that the child adopts and makes

his own. Thus, when a parent says things like ‘good child’ or ‘good behaviour’

and ‘bad child’ or ‘bad behaviour’, such communications from ‘significant

others’ (parents, siblings, playmates, teachers, relatives) become increasingly

patterned or organised into that part of the self that Mead calls the ‘me’.

Page 8: Sociology

In other words, the ‘me’ is the adoption of the ‘generalised other’, which

according to Mead is the ‘social self. Individuals develop self-consciousness

by coming to see themselves as others see them. For Freud this is the

outcome of Oedipal phase, while for ‘me’, it is the result of a developed

capacity of self-awareness.

In contrast to ‘I’, people are conscious of the ‘me’; the ‘me’ involves conscious

responsibility. It is through the ‘me’ that society dominates the individual in the

form of social control—the domination of the expression of the ‘me’ over the

expression ‘I’.

Phases of the development of self:

Mead traces the genesis of the self through two stages in child devel-

opment:

(1) Play stage:

At this stage infants and young children develop as social beings first of all by

imitating the actions of those around them. In their play small children often

imitate what adults do. They often play ‘house’ (Mummy-Papa) or ‘school’

(Teacher- Student), enacting the role of mother, father, teacher, student or

any other person important to them—significant others. Mead calls this

process as taking the role of others (role-taking)— learning what is to be in the

shoes of another person.

By taking the role of these significant others, they can better understand their

own roles as children, students, sons or daughters. By practicing the roles of

others in play, children learn to understand what others expect of them, and

they learn how to behave to meet those expectations. As a result of such play,

the child becomes cognisant of himself and obtains a picture of himself by

Page 9: Sociology

assuming the role of others. However, it is a limited self because the child can

take only the role of distinct and separate others. They lack a more general

and organised sense of themselves.

(2) Game stage:

It is the next stage of child development, which according to Mead occurs at

about eight or nine, the child starts taking part in organised games. To learn

organised games, one must understand the rules of the play, notions of

fairness and equal participation.

The child at this stage learns to grasp what Mead terms the ‘generalised

other’—the general values and moral rules involved in the culture in which he

or she is developing. This generalised other is an individual’s total impression

of the judgments and expectations that other persons have toward him. At this

stage, the sense of the self in the full sense of term emerges.

In the play stage, children are not organised wholes because they play at a

series of discrete roles. In Mead’s view they lack definite personalities.

However, in the game stage, such organisation begins and a definite

personality starts to emerge. Thus, for Mead, taking the role of generalised

other, rather than that of discrete others, is essential for the full development

of self.

Mead’s theory of development of self is less cumbersome than that of Freud.

It has also been very influential, yet it has been criticised on the ground that

the concepts used by Mead such as ‘taking the role of other’, ‘making a

gesture to one’s self and the ‘generalised other’ are not clear enough. Not

only this, the concept of self, which is a combination off and ‘me’, is also

Page 10: Sociology

ambiguous. Moreover, the theory of Mead does suggest the method of

studying social interaction.

Durkheim’s theory of collective representation:

Though Emile Durkheim has not directly talked anywhere in his writings about

the development of the sense of self or the process of socialisation of the

individual, he has definitely described the role of the society in the formation of

personality (attitudes, beliefs and behaviour) of the individuals. In his theory of

‘collective representation’, Durkheim insisted that the individual becomes

socialised by adopting the behaviour of his group.

He maintained that the individual’s thought and behaviour are determined by

collective representation. By collective representation, he meant the body of

experiences, a system of ideas, patterns of behaviour, attitudes and values

held in common by a group of people.

Durkheim’s main interest in the relationship of the individual to the group was

the group control over the individual. For him, socialisation is a one-way

process because he focussed his attention on how society develops and

moulds the individual to fit into the group. Durkheim’s conception left little

room for individual’s initiative and freedom in the process of socialisation.

This is a great weakness of his theory of collective representation. Durkheim

did not recognise any role of the individual in the process of socialisation. How

do these representations become a part of the individual or how does

collective representation exert pressure over the individual is not fully

explained by Durkheim. He utilised his theory of collective representation

Page 11: Sociology

(theory of socialisation) in explaining the causes of suicide, the social

phenomena of religion and the concept of social solidarity etc.

Page 12: Sociology

Jean Piaget: The Stage Theory In the Development of Children

By

David Nollmeyer

The psychologist Jean Piaget (1896-1980) of Switzerland is one the most seminal thinkers in the theory of childhood development. Piaget has postulated an Epigenetic Theory of Personality. Epi refers to emergence. The genetic refers to the core of natural material and socialization that comes to form the child. "So, in sum, genetic epistemology deals with both the formation and the meaning of knowledge. We can formulate our problem in the following terms: by what means does the human mind go from a state of less sufficient knowledge to a state of higher knowledge?" (Piaget, 1968). Piaget's Stage Theory of Development was one of modern psychologies first attempts to comprehend how children progress through cognitive development. His original model describes four universal stages for children. They are:

· The Sensormotor Period (birth to 2 years)

· Preoperational Thought (2 to 6/7 years)

· Concrete Operations (6/7 years to 11years)

· Formal Operations (11/12 to adult)

Piaget believed that performance of children was homogenous regardless of culture. It is a widely held position that not all children reach the formal operational period. This may be due to the socialization or externalities operating on a child under different environmental and cultural conditions (Berk, L.E. 2000). There is continued discussion on the movement of children through the development stages.

One of the most studied criticisms of Piaget's work was by Lev Vygotsky of the socio-cultural school. Uriel Bronfennbrenner pertains to this perspective. Such postulates that in the positive correct nurturant external forces shape development more than

Page 13: Sociology

innate a-priori factors. Regardless of revision, Piaget's methodology and work on the child are held by some to be equal to what Freud's work is to psychology in general.

The Sensormotor Period

At birth to two years all the intellectual and physical capabilities are underdeveloped. However the infant has sensory capabilities available. The infant learns by the exercise and utilization of reflexes in nexus with seeing, touching, sucking, feeling, and using their senses to learn things about themselves and the environment. Simple movements in response to stimuli later develop into more sophisticated coordinated acts of behavior(s). The infant through this limited expansion into its environment builds a set of concepts about reality and how it works. Object permanence or the knowledge that objects still exist after disappearing from sight has not been obtained by all children.

After a child has mastered the concept of object permanence, the emergence of directed groping begins to take place. The child begins to perform motor experiments in order to analyze the effects. A child will vary one's movements to observe differentiation in results. The child learns to use new means to achieve an end. The child discovers objects can be pulled towards oneself with the aid of a stick or string, or tilt objects to obtain such through the bars of a playpen.

Through trial and error experimentation by handling objects, the concept that the external world is not part of the self or an extension manifests. Piaget calls this the sensormotor stage because the early manifestations of intelligence appear from sensory perceptions and motor activities (Anderson, M. 2003).

Preoperational Thought

At this stage, children acquire representational skills in the areas as mental imagery, language, and symbolic thought. They are very self-oriented, and have an egocentric view. Preoperational thought is pre-logical; the child has a subjective grasp of the world. Preoperational children use representational skills only to view the world from their own perspective. The main characteristics are:

· Egocentrism - child interprets the world in terms of the self

· Centration - Fixation on one situation or object and ignore others

· Reversibility -child cannot mentally remember steps of reasoning

Page 14: Sociology

Preoperational thought is also characterized by animism. The child has the tendency to ascribe human characteristics to inanimate objects and events. Artificialism is the tendency to assume that natural objects and natural phenomena were created by human beings for human purposes as darkness so that humans may sleep.

Piaget's experiments in preoperational thought are groundbreaking and controversial. In the Three Mountain Task young children are asked to assume the perspective of a doll in relation to a model of mountains. Young children of the age four to five took their own perspectives. Children could not accomplish the task until about age nine.

Class inclusion experiments presented arrangements of six red flowers and two white flowers. When asked are there more red flowers than flowers, preoperational children chose there are greater red flowers. Conservation is also problematic. If two arrays of objects are presented and an experimenter alters the array and not the quantity of objects, preoperational children fail at deducting the transformation of the array.

Concrete Operations

Children in the concrete operations stage are able to take another's point of view and take into account more than one perspective simultaneously. They can also represent transformations as well as static situations. Concrete problems are understood. Children cannot yet perform on abstract problems; they do not consider all of the logically possible outcomes. Reversing operations emerges.

One important task that children learn during the concrete operational stage is to arrange things in order according to one attribute, such as size or weight. Logical inferences such as this are not possible until the stage of concrete operations, during which children develop the ability to make two mental transformations that require reversible thinking. The first of these is inversion (+A is reversed by -A), and the second is reciprocity (A=A).

A final ability that children acquire during the concrete operational stage is class inclusion; re-creating a relationship between a part and the whole. Concrete operational thought is decentered; a child can now focus on two classes simultaneously. While the differences between the preoperational and concrete operational stages are dramatic, concrete operational children still do not think like adults. They are very much rooted in the world as it is, and have difficulty with abstract thought.

Page 15: Sociology

Formal Operations

Children who attain the formal operation stage are capable of thinking logically and abstractly. They can also reason theoretically. Piaget stated that although the children would still have to revise their knowledge base, their way of thinking was actualized.

There is an introduction of formal thought and logical assumptions. Formal reasoning connects assumptions, propositions, hypotheses, i.e., relationships in which one does not necessarily believe, but which one admits in order to see where the consequences lead. Mathematical, scientific and logical reasoning have their basis here.

Problem-solving games, stories, movies, plays, and cartoons are important in formal operations play. Arts and crafts become more exact, realistic, and detailed. Peer approval is important. Teamwork, group cohesion, and skill in sports become important. Fluid knowledge is being developed as well as crystal knowledge.

It is not agreed that all children in all cultures develop formal operations. Children in agricultural and pre-agricultural settings do not have the same needs and appear to function well without obtaining this stage (Berk, 2000).