9
BioOne sees sustainable scholarly publishing as an inherently collaborative enterprise connecting authors, nonprofit publishers, academic institutions, research libraries, and research funders in the common goal of maximizing access to critical research. Seed preparation diminishes cache loss in least chipmunks Author(s): Jarred R. Jenkins and Lynn D. Devenport Source: Journal of Mammalogy, 95(2):276-283. 2014. Published By: American Society of Mammalogists DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1644/13-MAMM-A-123 URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/full/10.1644/13-MAMM-A-123 BioOne (www.bioone.org ) is a nonprofit, online aggregation of core research in the biological, ecological, and environmental sciences. BioOne provides a sustainable online platform for over 170 journals and books published by nonprofit societies, associations, museums, institutions, and presses. Your use of this PDF, the BioOne Web site, and all posted and associated content indicates your acceptance of BioOne’s Terms of Use, available at www.bioone.org/page/terms_of_use . Usage of BioOne content is strictly limited to personal, educational, and non-commercial use. Commercial inquiries or rights and permissions requests should be directed to the individual publisher as copyright holder.

Seed preparation diminishes cache loss in least chipmunks

  • Upload
    lynn-d

  • View
    213

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

BioOne sees sustainable scholarly publishing as an inherently collaborative enterprise connecting authors, nonprofit publishers, academic institutions, researchlibraries, and research funders in the common goal of maximizing access to critical research.

Seed preparation diminishes cache loss in least chipmunksAuthor(s): Jarred R. Jenkins and Lynn D. DevenportSource: Journal of Mammalogy, 95(2):276-283. 2014.Published By: American Society of MammalogistsDOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1644/13-MAMM-A-123URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/full/10.1644/13-MAMM-A-123

BioOne (www.bioone.org) is a nonprofit, online aggregation of core research in the biological, ecological, andenvironmental sciences. BioOne provides a sustainable online platform for over 170 journals and books publishedby nonprofit societies, associations, museums, institutions, and presses.

Your use of this PDF, the BioOne Web site, and all posted and associated content indicates your acceptance ofBioOne’s Terms of Use, available at www.bioone.org/page/terms_of_use.

Usage of BioOne content is strictly limited to personal, educational, and non-commercial use. Commercial inquiriesor rights and permissions requests should be directed to the individual publisher as copyright holder.

Journal of Mammalogy, 95(2):276–283, 2014

Seed preparation diminishes cache loss in least chipmunks

JARRED R. JENKINS* AND LYNN D. DEVENPORT

Department of Psychology, Mercer University, 1400 Coleman Avenue, Macon, GA 31207, USA (JRJ)Department of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, 455 W. Lindsey St., Norman, OK 73019, USA (LDD)

* Correspondent: [email protected]

Recent work with least chipmunks, Neotamias minimus, has identified a novel seed preparation process. N.minimus begins by hulling seeds and then creating a thick saliva-coated cluster that hardens once expelled.

Scattered and undefended, these ‘‘bolus’’ caches are at risk for theft. However, the behaviors involved in the

creation of a bolus may confer protection against such pilferage. In a series of laboratory experiments with

sunflower seeds, we found that both the hulling and saliva-clumping processes significantly reduced pilfering of

these caches by conspecifics and a competitor, Tamias striatus, under 3 cache depth conditions.

Key words: antipilfering tactics, bolus, cache fate, cache item preparation, Neotamias minimus, scatter hoarding, Tamiasstriatus

� 2014 American Society of Mammalogists

DOI: 10.1644/13-MAMM-A-123

Food hoarding involves the sequestering of resources for use

at a later time. Whereas nonhoarding species can only harvest

what they can consume in a foraging bout, hoarders are able to

stockpile (Jenkins and Peters 1992). Many mammalian

hoarders scatter-cache their food items, allocating them to

small, spatially distributed packets buried in their home

environment (Morris 1962). These undefended food items

can be consumed as needed or relocated to a more permanent

and defendable larder hoard, often found in their burrow, after

harvesting is complete. Using spatial memory and other

mechanisms (e.g., Kamil and Balda 1985; Vander Wall

1991; Devenport et al. 2000), hoarders recover scatter caches

and often rely on them to supplement or even replace daily

food gathering. This can become especially important during

periods of low resource availability (e.g., Smith and Reichman

1984; Dearing 1997). Despite the cache-recovery advantage for

cache owners (e.g., Jacobs and Liman 1991; Devenport et al.

2000), scatter caches are vulnerable and many are lost to both

intra- and interspecific pilferers (Vander Wall and Jenkins

2003).

For scatter-caching to be successful, hoarders should recover

a higher proportion of their stores than do robbers (Andersson

and Krebs 1978). However, the threat of pilferage remains

(Vander Wall and Jenkins 2003). To further counteract these

losses, hoarders could adjust their preparation and placement of

items. One species that appears to meet this expectation is least

chipmunks (Neotamias minimus). In previous field (e.g.,

Devenport and Devenport 1994) and laboratory (e.g., Deven-

port et al. 1998, 1999) studies we have observed an apparently

unique seed-processing behavior in this species. Upon finding

a seed source, they commonly remove the seed hull as they

pouch each seed. Alternatively, they may pouch unhulled

seeds, move to another place, often near cover, to carry out the

hulling and repouching. Because hulling reduces seed volume,

chipmunks typically can return to the seed source and collect

more seeds before transporting them to scatter or larder hoards.

Additionally, this hulling behavior should increase each

animal’s transport efficiency. Once the seeds have been hulled,

least chipmunks often expel a tightly packed cluster of seeds,

covered in a thick saliva-based coating. This ‘‘bolus’’ then dries

and hardens as a single unit after it is cached. Although not all

caches contain hulled seeds, many do and the adaptive benefits

of such preparation are beginning to emerge.

We have found chipmunk-made boluses expelled in Sher-

man live traps and in buried caches in the field, as well as

regularly encountering them under laboratory conditions.

Sunflower, millet, pumpkin, and flax are among the seeds we

have observed to be processed in these ways. We know of no

differences between males and females, or developmental

differences between juveniles and adults. However, these

questions have yet to be empirically addressed. To our

knowledge, only the gray jay Perisoreus canadensis; (Waite

and Ydenberg 1994) has been reported to show a similar

cementing tactic, and only a single heteromyid rodent has been

w w w . m a m m a l o g y . o r g

276

observed to hull seeds before pouching and transporting them

(Lawhon and Hafner 1981).

The distribution of the small, diurnal least chipmunk

overlaps with other species, including eastern chipmunks

(Tamias striatus). Although least chipmunks tend to prefer

slightly drier habitats, both species subsist on a variety of

seeds, nuts, forbs, and berries, placing them in frequent

resource competition (Elliot 1978). Eastern chipmunks are

much larger and more physically dominant (Verts and

Carraway 2001) and could easily supplant a least chipmunk

from a resource site. However, both species are known pilferers

and thus competition could be based largely on the theft of

undefended cached resources. It has been suggested that the

competitive success of least chipmunks is partly tied to their

unique caching and pilfering tactics. For example, least

chipmunks place their caches in less predictable locations

(away from other caches and away from environmental

beacons) when compared with their eastern counterparts.

When pilfering, both least and eastern chipmunks target these

predictable locations, much to the detriment of the eastern

chipmunks’ stored food (Penner and Devenport 2011). Seed-

hulling and bolus-making may be additional antipilfering

adaptations.

The present set of studies focuses on the adaptive nature of

least chipmunks’ seed preparation procedures, hulling and

clumping, in terms of vertebrate pilferage. We hypothesize that

the removal of the seed hull reduces pilfering and that the

clumped nature of a bolus may reduce it even more. Using

seminaturalistic laboratory arrangements and experimental

caches (unhulled, hulled, and bolus), seed type selection was

examined using both least chipmunks and a competitor, the

eastern chipmunk.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects.—Animal collection occurred during the summers

of 2003, 2006, and 2010. Both species (N. minimus and T.striatus) were selected from sites in and near the Seney

National Wildlife Refuge in Seney, Michigan and several miles

away from human visitors. Upon collection, animals were

sexed, marked for identification, and cleared of ectoparasites

using Sevin dust (GardenTech, Palatine, Illinois). Under

permits from the states of Michigan and Oklahoma, we

transported the animals to the University of Oklahoma and

housed them for use in the present series of studies. Upon

arrival, we inserted passive integrated transponder chips (Avid

Identification Systems, Inc., Norco, California) and maintained

the animals in seminaturalistic enclosures. Chipmunks were

group-housed (8–15 animals per room) and each enclosure (2.5

to 8 m2) contained beds of hardwood chips (~5 cm deep) that

allowed for naturalistic caching and pilfering behaviors,

numerous limbs and tree stumps for climbing, and a large

pile of lava rocks that allowed for retreat and nesting. Animals

were fed a varied diet of seeds, greens, fruits, and vegetables,

in addition to the standard rodent chow that was provided on

alternating days (for additional housing and care information,

see Devenport et al. 1998). Both the collection and housing

procedures followed the guidelines given by the American

Society of Mammalogists (Sikes et al. 2011) and were

approved by the University of Oklahoma’s Institutional

Animal Care and Use Committee.

Three to 5 days before a testing session, we removed each

animal from the group-housed quarters and placed it in a

smaller, solitary rodent cage. Each cage contained a bed of

hardwood chips, compressed cotton squares for nest-making,

and a glass or plastic nest bottle. Diet was limited to restricted

portions of rodent chow that brought body weight to

approximately 90% of free feeding weight (means: N. minimus:

55.1 g; T. striatus: 122.8 g). After test sessions, we returned

animals to their group quarters.

Foraging and testing arrangements.—Foraging sessions

occurred in a 0.61- 3 1.22-m box containing medium-grade

industrial sand to a depth of about 6 cm. Each box contained a

closeable access hole through which the animal entered from

its transport bottle for each foraging session. Animals had

water spout access through a small hole in the box wall. We

placed each box within a 1.5- 3 2.5-m room containing various

visual cues that could act as external landmarks. We monitored

the animals through the use of a centrally mounted ceiling

camera that connected to a nearby video analysis and

observation room as detailed elsewhere (Devenport et al.

1998).

General testing procedures.—Three separate experiments

were designed to assess the cache selection behavior of both

least and eastern chipmunks. A unique set of least chipmunks

provided data for each experiment, whereas 3 eastern

chipmunks were used in both dry procedures. Before and

after each trial, we wiped the box down with a 10% alcohol

solution to reduce and/or mask any residual odors from

previous trials and animals. All sand was mixed and sifted to

remove any remaining seeds, seed hulls, urine-clumped sand,

or fecal matter. It was then smoothed over to eliminate any

textural cues.

In each experiment, we presented chipmunks with the

opportunity to search for seeds. Intact sunflower seeds were

obtained from a stock seed source, whereas the other seed types

required extra preparation. Experimentally hulled seeds were

soaked in distilled water for approximately 1 h to loosen the

seed husk. Using forceps and latex gloves, seeds were plucked

from the water bath and the hull was carefully removed. Seeds

then dried overnight in a laboratory oven (408C). For bolus

collection, least chipmunks were placed in clear plastic rodent

cages without litter and presented with a dish of sunflower

seeds. After hulling and pouching their seeds, many of the

bolus makers then used running wheels or did flips within the

box before secreting the seeds in bolus form. We monitored the

animals for bolus production, removing each bolus shortly after

its creation. Often found in cage corners or even within the

seed dish, boli were removed while the seeds were still sticky

and the bolus still malleable. For the purposes of the present

study, larger boli were reduced to a standard size of 10 seeds

April 2014 277JENKINS AND DEVENPORT—PRECACHING SEED PREPARATION

each. After reduction, each bolus was then stored in the lab

oven (408C) to dry overnight.

Dry substrate cache discovery.—The 1st experiment

assessed the effects of both seed-hulling and bolus-making

on the prevention of intra- and interspecific pilferage in dry

sand. By suppressing visual cues, we determined if either of

these seed-preparation behaviors protected against pilfering by

each species. The least sample consisted of 4 males and 4

females (mean estimated age [MEA]: 3.00 years; mean time in

lab [MTL]: 2.88 years; 7 lab-born [LB] animals). The eastern

sample had 6 males and 2 females (MEA: 1.88 years; MTL:

1.75 years; 2 LB).

Two caches of each seed type (unhulled black oil sunflower

seeds, experimentally hulled seeds, intact animal-produced

boli) were variably located within the foraging box. The 6

locations were consistent across all 8 animals of both species

(N. minimus and T. striatus). Caches were spaced throughout

the box, maintaining a distance of at least 5 cm from other

caches and from the box walls. Collectively, a total of 32 boli

was used and obtained from 10 different animals, 3 of which

were also used for pilfering data collection. However, none of

the bolus makers was presented with its own bolus upon test.

We buried seeds and boli at a depth of 1.5 cm, which is

consistent with the average depth used by these animals for dry

sand caching (Penner and Devenport 2011). A total of 60 seeds

was available and animals foraged until they had recovered all

caches or until 15 minutes elapsed after their last discovery.

We recorded the order in which each animal found the caches

and the total number of caches found. Chipmunks with fewer

than 3 cache finds (5 least chipmunks, 2 eastern chipmunks)

were excluded from the data set. This eliminated any animals

that may have been poorly motivated and simultaneously

increased the depth of our data.

Damp substrate cache discovery.—The 2nd experiment

followed the same procedure as the 1st, but used a damp sand

substrate. We did this to examine the variable conditions that

could be expected for naturally occurring caches, as seed and

substrate hydration is known to affect cache discovery (Geluso

2005; Vander Wall 1998). Damp sand can augment an

olfactory cue emitted from caches and would allow for

generalizable conclusions about precache preparation effects.

Six male and 2 female least chipmunks (MEA: 1.88 years;

MTL: 1.75 years; 7 LB) were used in this procedure. The

eastern sample contained 4 members of each sex (MEA: 1.00

years; MTL: 0.25 years; 0 LB).

As in Experiment 1, we created six 10-seed caches, 2 of each

seed type. Seven of the 8 bolus-making least chipmunks were

used for data collection in this experiment; however, no animal

was presented with its own boli. Generally, the procedures

mimicked those described for the ‘‘Dry substrate cache

discovery’’ procedure, except for the addition of a wet substrate

and increased depth. Before cache placement, we added water

to the dry sand and thoroughly mixed it within the box. On

average, the sand’s water content was about 3.00%, with the

eastern chipmunk sample slightly wetter (X̄ ¼ 3.24%, SD ¼0.23%) than the least chipmunk sample (X̄ ¼ 2.69%, SD ¼

0.74%). Given that naive foragers show increased success in

moist substrates (Geluso 2005), we also doubled the depth of

each cache to 3.0 cm. The order in which each animal found

caches was again recorded and all animals tested took from at

least 3 caches.

Surface-cache procedure.—In the 3rd experiment, we made

seeds visually conspicuous by placing them directly on the

sand surface in 16 small clumps of 3 seeds each. Eight of the

seed caches contained unhulled sunflower seeds and the other 8

caches consisted of experimentally hulled seeds. Cache

positions were kept away from the box entrance, but

otherwise randomly placed and randomly assigned a seed

type. A total of 48 seeds was available. We recorded the order

in which animals found their caches and removed each animal

5 min after its last cache retrieval. Three male and 5 female

least chipmunks (MEA: 1.69 years, MTL: 1.50 years; 6 LB)

were used, whereas 4 male and 4 female eastern chipmunks

(MEA: 3.25 years; MTL: 2.63 years; 3 LB) participated.

Data analysis.—Data analyses were conducted using the

procedures outlined by Arthur et al. (1996). Using a multinomial

logit model, we were able to examine resource selection indices

under changing availability. In all experiments, animals

harvested from a depleting set of discrete caches. As an item

was selected, the chance probability of selection for the

remaining items changed. The Arthur et al. (1996) approach

specifically handles this type of heterogeneity in animal choice

by using a maximum-likelihood technique to estimate the

probability that a resource will be selected relative to all other

available options. These ‘‘selection indices’’ are calculated

through an iterative process involving the proportional

availability of each cache type, specific to successive choices.

Availabilities are obtained by looking at each animal’s choice set

during each trial. The product of these probabilities is the

likelihood estimate of the data set occurring under the given

selection-based model. This likelihood is then compared with a

nonselection model in which the selection values or probabilities

of choice for all options are equal. Models are compared by

testing the difference between each model’s (chance and

computed) deviance value (deviance ¼ �2loge[likelihood])

against a chi-square distribution. These analyses were

conducted using the program RSWHAC (Manly 2000), which

was specifically designed to handle the maximum-likelihood

estimations of the Arthur et al. (1996) data set.

RESULTS

Dry substrate cache discovery.— Both N. minimus and T.striatus chose seed caches in a nonrandom fashion as selection

values were highest for the unhulled seed type (index values:

N. minimus [0.78] and T. striatus [0.70]). Under conditions of

equal availability, both species were more likely to choose the

unhulled seed types first, and continued to do so until

availability declined. The calculated probabilities of both

hulled (index values: T. minimus [0.14] and T. striatus[0.19]) and bolus (index values: N. minimus [0.08] and T.striatus [0.10]) seed selection were much lower, respectively.

278 Vol. 95, No. 2JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY

The rank order across the 3 types also showed a marked

difference between the unhulled seeds as compared with the

other conditions. Thus, the removal of the seed hull seems to

play a very direct role in seed selection. In fact, only 1 least

chipmunk and only 1 eastern chipmunk chose anything other

than unhulled seeds on their 1st visit. Using a binomial test of

chance for unhulled seeds compared with the other types, the

initial choices for both least (P¼ 0.002) and eastern chipmunks

(P ¼ 0.002) are significant.

For least chipmunks, the number of available unhulled seed

caches dropped quickly, whereas the number of available

hulled and bolus caches stayed proportional across visits and

some went unfound (hulled: n ¼ 4, bolus: n ¼ 3; Fig. 1). The

same is true of eastern chipmunk pilfering. Many hulled (n¼4)

and bolus caches (n ¼ 6) remained as eastern chipmunks

exploited the unhulled caches early on (Fig. 1). Only 3 animals

of each species recovered all 6 caches in the time allotted. On

average, least chipmunks recovered 5.00 caches (SD ¼ 1.07),

whereas eastern chipmunks recovered 4.50 caches (SD¼ 1.41).

This small difference in performance was not significant (t14¼0.98, P ¼ 0.37).

As compared with the nonselection model, the selection

model with computed indices provided a significantly better fit

to the data for both least (D2¼ 19.12, P¼ 0.0001) and eastern

chipmunks (D2¼ 14.85, P¼ 0.0006). Both species were more

biased toward caches containing unhulled seeds than would be

expected under chance conditions. Both species showed a

smaller preference toward hulled seeds over bolus caches.

Damp substrate cache discovery.—In damp sand, both least

and eastern chipmunks chose seed caches nonrandomly. Like

the results seen in the dry substrate, selection indices were

highest for the caches containing the unhulled seed type (index

values: N. minimus [0.64] and T. striatus [0.60]). However, these

values were both lower than those obtained under dry

conditions. Despite the drop, the selection model produced by

RSWHAC provided a significantly better fit to the data than did

a random choice model for either species (N. minimus: D2 ¼10.59, P¼ 0.0500; T. striatus: D2¼ 7.48, P¼ 0.0237). For least

chipmunks, the rank order of the 3 seed types remained the same

as seen under dry conditions (index values: hulled [0.20] and

bolus [0.16]). However, for easterns, only a negligible difference

between hulled (0.19) and bolus cache (0.21) selection emerged.

This difference between the species can be further illustrated

by examining what they left behind (Fig. 1). Eastern

chipmunks left an equal number of hulled (n ¼ 4) and bolus

caches (n¼ 4), whereas least chipmunks showed the expected

disparity by leaving 4 boluses and only 1 hulled cache.

Additionally, 3 of the eastern chipmunks chose something

other than unhulled seeds on their 1st choice. In both least

conditions (dry and damp) and in the eastern dry condition, no

more than a single animal chose anything but unhulled seeds

on its 1st selection. As in the dry substrate condition, least

chipmunks found more caches (X̄ ¼ 5.38, SD ¼ 0.74) on

average than eastern chipmunks (X̄¼ 5.00, SD¼ 1.12), but not

significantly so (t14 ¼ 0.81, P ¼ 0.43). In total, 4 least

chipmunks recovered all of the damp caches, whereas only 3

eastern chipmunks did.

Surface-cache procedure.—When presented with 16 surface

caches containing either unhulled or hulled sunflower seeds,

both least and eastern chipmunks showed higher selection

indices for the unhulled cache types (index values: N. minimus[0.69] and T. striatus [0.82]). In each case, the selection model

identified in RSWHAC provided a significantly better fit to the

data than did the chance-based nonselection model (N.minimus: D1 ¼ 14.66, P ¼ 0.0001; T. striatus: D1 ¼ 46.57, P¼ 0.00001). This result parallels those found in the other

procedures, indicating that under conditions of equal

availability, both species predominantly select unhulled

sunflower seeds as compared with the experimentally hulled

seed type (Fig. 2). In fact, only 4 of the 16 animals chose the

experimentally hulled seed type on their first visit (P¼0.0278).

DISCUSSION

Numerous species engage in both long-term and short-term

hoarding behavior (Vander Wall 1990). By hoarding, foragers

can decrease the variability of food intake across periods of

resource scarcity or declines in foraging success (e.g., Smith

and Reichman 1984; Dearing 1997). For hoarding to be an

effective strategy, hoarders should recover more of their cached

items than do pilferers (Andersson and Krebs 1978), so it is

advantageous to use certain tactics that can reduce losses by

limiting pilferer detection and/or selection. Least chipmunks

have already been shown to use spatial placement tactics

(Penner and Devenport 2011). Seed-hulling and bolus-making

are now additional and unique robbery-prevention behaviors.

As seen in both the dry and damp conditions, least chipmunks

can diminish cache loss by hulling their seeds or by hulling and

creating a bolus before burial. Such behaviors appear to help

prevent detection and/or selection by both intraspecific and

interspecific robbers. By reducing theft from pilferers, least

chipmunks would likely increase their own personal cache

recovery and net energy intake. It has been shown that least

chipmunks can easily relocate available resources on the basis

of spatial memory (Devenport and Devenport 1994; Devenport

et al. 1998; Penner and Devenport 2011) and thus would

maintain an advantage over pilferers.

Despite the apparent adaptive benefit, the mechanism behind

the reduction in pilferage by hulling and making boli remains

unclear. Vander Wall suggested that the strength of an

olfactory signal of a seed is likely linked to the surface area

of the seed itself and that the main olfactory cue of a seed may

be contained within the seed hull (Vander Wall 2003). When

least chipmunks remove the hull, it is possible that they are also

reducing the detectability of that seed when cached, as pilferers

rely heavily on olfactory detection (Vander Wall 2003). Seed

survival should have been further enhanced by the clumped

nature of a bolus, as this reduces cache surface area. Three of

the bolus studies directly supported this notion. However, the

difference between hulled and bolus caches pales in compar-

ison with the difference between unhulled seeds and the other

April 2014 279JENKINS AND DEVENPORT—PRECACHING SEED PREPARATION

FIG. 1.—Total number of unvisited caches available to each species of chipmunk in both the dry (top) and damp (bottom) substrate conditions.

Sets of unvisited patches remain after the 6th visit as not all animals fully depleted each cache set.

280 Vol. 95, No. 2JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY

types. Our data in the dry substrate show this pattern: hulled

and bolus caches were simply taken last, if at all. Had we not

replaced the underperforming and/or poorly motivated animals,

our results may have been even stronger. Ten of the 12 dropped

cache selections in dry sand were unhulled seeds. The pattern

of cache selection remained mostly the same in damp sand.

However, the selection indices for the unhulled types were

somewhat reduced.

Despite the argument for an olfactory effect, other

mechanisms may play important roles. When seeds were

placed on the surface of the sand, animals consistently chose

the unhulled seeds over hulled seeds. Additional data collected

in our lab indicated that this was not the result of any visual

disparity between the seed types when placed on the sand

surface (Jenkins 2008). The selection of conspicuous unhulled

seeds over conspicuous hulled seeds may at least partly argue

FIG. 2.—Total number of unvisited caches available to both least (top) and eastern (bottom) chipmunks in the surface cache procedure. All

animals depleted all caches, with most choosing to deplete the unhulled seeds before depleting the hulled seeds.

April 2014 281JENKINS AND DEVENPORT—PRECACHING SEED PREPARATION

for the role of a preference as well as easier olfactory detection.

It remains possible that chipmunks may have detected both

hulled and bolus seeds at rates equal to that of the unhulled

seeds, but preferred to dig at and take from the unhulled seed

caches. However, we did not observe any such systematic

pattern of behavior in the present experiments. Additionally,

we have yet to see any strong evidence indicating a palatability

decline due to the extra processing involved in the creation of

hulled seeds. In fact in pilot data, when presented with 4

caches, 2 containing traditionally hulled seeds and 2 containing

water-soaked and dried unhulled seeds, 9 of 12 least

chipmunks still chose the unhulled seed types first.

Furthermore, the possibility of an overall preference for

unhulled seeds would be puzzling. To consume any seed, the

hull must first be removed. A preference for unhulled seeds

entails increased handling time. Thus, given the tenets of

optimal foraging theory, chipmunks should prefer hulled seeds

when available, if they are attempting to maximize the rate of

net energy intake (MacArthur and Pianka 1966). In fact,

Kaufman and Collier (1981) reported a preference for hulled

seeds by rats under laboratory conditions. Additionally, the

number of transportable seeds should decrease as the volume

of the each seed increases. By choosing them, foragers are

effectively choosing less than they could maximally carry.

However, it is possible that given the laboratory nature of the

study and size of the box, animals were not concerned with

issues of long-distance transport.

According to Penner and Devenport (2011), least chipmunks

preferentially seek out competitor caches before depleting their

own. With the preparation involved in creating hulled or bolus

seeds, robbery of those seed types should translate to the

pilfering of a competitor cache. It continues to remain unclear

as to why the animals in the present study would then prefer the

unhulled seed types. Unfortunately, given the design of this

study, a complete understanding of the mechanism behind the

reduction in pilferage is unattainable.

Despite these remaining questions, the present experiments

show that precache preparation does reduce intraspecific and

congeneric pilferage. Conducted in a naturalistic laboratory

setting, these studies should be complemented by field

assessments. Although we have observed these behaviors in

the field, it is critical to determine if the antipilfering effects

also carry over. Whereas it stands as a reasonable hypothesis

that least chipmunks better compete with robbers by decreasing

seed detectability, desirability, or both, future studies should

attempt to assess the relative contributions of these mecha-

nisms. It is likely that the behavior succeeds for several

reasons. The ease of owner transport, the suppression of

olfactory signals, and the exploitation of pilferer preferences

are not mutually exclusive predictions. They are all potential

contributors to the shaping of the precache seed-preparation

behaviors seen in least chipmunks.

LITERATURE CITED

ANDERSSON, M., AND J. KREBS. 1978. On the evolution of hoarding

behavior. Animal Behaviour 26:707–711.

ARTHUR, S. M., B. F. J. MANLY, L. L. MCDONALD, AND G. W. GARNER.

1996. Assessing habitat selection when availability changes.

Ecology 77:215–227.

DEARING, M. D. 1997. The function of haypiles of pikas (Ochotonaprinces). Journal of Mammalogy 78:1156–1163.

DEVENPORT, J. A., AND L. D. DEVENPORT. 1994. Spatial navigation in

natural habitats by ground-dwelling sciurids. Animal Behaviour

47:727–729.

DEVENPORT, J. A., L. D. LUNA, AND L. D. DEVENPORT. 2000. Placement,

retrieval, and memory of caches by thirteen-lined ground squirrels.

Ethology 106:171–183.

DEVENPORT, L., J. DEVENPORT, AND C. KOKESH. 1999. The role of urine

marking in the foraging behavior of least chipmunks. Animal

Behaviour 57:557–563.

DEVENPORT, L. D., T. HUMPHRIES, AND J. DEVENPORT. 1998. Future

value and patch choice. Animal Behaviour 55:1571–1581.

ELLIOT, L. 1978. Social behavior and foraging ecology of the eastern

chipmunk (Tamias striatus) in the Adirondack Mountains.

Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology. Smithsonian Institution

Press.

GELUSO, K. 2005. Benefits of small-sized caches for scatter-hoarding

rodents: influence of cache size, depth, and soil moisture. Journal of

Mammalogy 86:1186–1192.

JACBOS, L. F., AND E. R. LIMAN. 1991. Grey squirrels remember the

locations of buried nuts. Animal Behaviour 41:103–110.

JENKINS, J. R. 2008. Pre-caching seed preparation: a novel anti-

pilfering technique in least chipmunks. Master’s thesis, University

of Oklahoma, Norman.

JENKINS, S. H., AND R. A. PETERS. 1992. Spatial patterns of food storage

by Merriam’s kangaroo rats. Behavioral Ecology 3:60–65.

KAMIL, A. C., AND R. P. BALDA. 1985. Cache recovery and spatial

memory in Clark’s nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana). Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 11:95–111.

KAUFMAN, L. W., AND G. COLLIER. 1981. The economics of seed

handling. American Naturalist 118:46–60.

LAWHON, D. K., AND M. S. HAFNER. 1981. Tactile discriminatory

ability and foraging strategies in kangaroo rats and pocket mice

(Rodentia: Heteromyidae). Oecologia 50:303–309.

MACARTHUR, R. H., AND E. R. PIANKA. 1966. On optimal use of a

patchy environment. American Naturalist 100:603–609.

MANLY, B. F. J. 2000. RSWHAC program. Western Ecosystems Inc.,

Cheyenne, Wyoming. http://www.west-inc.com/. Accessed 1 Sep-

tember 2007.

MORRIS, D. 1962. The behavior of the green acouchi (Myoproctapratti) with special reference to scatter hoarding. Proceedings

of the Zoological Society of London 139:701–731.

PENNER, J. L., AND L. D. DEVENPORT. 2011. A comparative study of

caching and pilfering behavior in two sympatric species, least

chipmunks (Tamiasminimus) and eastern chipmunks (Tamiasstriatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 125:375–384.

SIKES, R. S., W. L. GANNON, AND THE ANIMAL CARE AND USE COMMITTEE

OF THE AMERICAN SOCIETY OF MAMMOLOGISTS. 2011. Guidelines of

the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild

mammals in research. Journal of Mammalogy 92:235–253.

SMITH, C. C., AND O. J. REICHMAN. 1984. The evolution of food

caching by birds and mammals. Annual Review of Ecological

Systems 15:329–351.

VANDER WALL, S. B. 1990. Food hoarding in animals. University of

Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois.

VANDER WALL, S. B. 1991. Mechanisms of cache recovery by yellow

pine chipmunks. Animal Behaviour 41:851–863.

282 Vol. 95, No. 2JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY

VANDER WALL, S. B. 1998. Foraging success of granivorous rodents:

effects of variation in seed and soil water on olfaction. Ecology

79:233–241.

VANDER WALL, S. B. 2003. How rodents smell buried seeds: a model

based on the behavior of pesticides in soil. Journal of Mammalogy

84:1089–1099.

VANDER WALL, S. B., AND S. H. JENKINS. 2003. Reciprocal pilferage

and the evolution of food-hoarding behavior. Behavioral Ecology

14:656–667.

VERTS, B. J., AND L. N. CARRAWAY. 2001. Tamias minimus.

Mammalian Species 653:1–10.

WAITE, T. A., AND R. C. YDENBERG. 1994. What currency do scatter-

hoarding jays maximize? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

34:43–49.

Submitted 17 May 2013. Accepted 23 October 2013.

Associate Editor was Michael A. Steele.

April 2014 283JENKINS AND DEVENPORT—PRECACHING SEED PREPARATION