S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

  • Upload
    mppudp

  • View
    226

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    1/11

    Interviews, Surveys, and the Problem of Ecological ValidityAuthor(s): Aaron V. CicourelSource: The American Sociologist, Vol. 17, No. 1 (Feb., 1982), pp. 11-20Published by: American Sociological AssociationStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/27702491.

    Accessed: 28/10/2013 15:50

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    .JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    .

    American Sociological Associationis collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The

    American Sociologist.

    http://www.jstor.org

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asahttp://www.jstor.org/stable/27702491?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/27702491?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    2/11

    The

    Problem

    of

    Ecological

    Validity

    11

    tions that

    a

    classic

    can

    serve.

    We

    may

    believe

    that students'

    minds

    are

    expanded

    by

    reading

    Durkheim

    without

    our

    having

    to believe Durkheim has many true gener

    alizations

    about the

    causes

    of suicide.

    George

    Herbert

    Mead

    can

    symbolize

    what

    is

    distinctive

    in

    symbolic

    interactionism

    even

    if

    we

    cannot

    quite

    figure

    out

    how to

    test

    the

    hypothesis

    of

    the

    independence

    of

    the

    F

    from

    the

    me,

    and

    to

    turn

    it into

    a

    puzzle

    for routine

    science.

    And

    orte

    can

    enjoy

    the taste

    of

    Marx's

    famous

    passage

    in The 18th

    Brumaire

    about

    French

    peas

    ants

    forming

    a

    vast

    mass,

    without

    that

    beauty being undermined when we find

    some

    regions

    of

    modern

    France

    where the

    peasants

    vote

    Communist.

    What

    is

    destructive

    about

    admiration of

    the

    classics,

    then,

    is

    the

    halo

    effect,

    the

    belief that

    because

    a

    book

    or

    article is

    useful

    for

    one

    purpose,

    it

    must

    have

    all

    the

    virtues.

    INTERVIEWS, SURVEYS, AND THE PROBLEM OF

    ECOLOGICAL VALIDITY*

    Aaron

    V.

    Cicourel

    University

    of

    California,

    San

    Diego

    The

    American

    Sociologist 1982,

    Vol. 17

    (February):

    11-20

    Despite

    the

    fact

    that

    virtually

    all

    social

    science

    data

    are

    derived

    from

    some

    kind

    of

    discourse

    or

    textual

    materials,

    sociologists

    have

    devoted little

    time

    to

    establishing explicit

    theoretical

    foundations

    for

    the

    use

    of

    such

    instruments

    as

    interviews

    and

    surveys.

    A

    key

    problem

    always

    has been

    the lack

    of

    clear theoretical

    concepts

    about the

    interpretation

    of

    interview

    and

    survey

    question and answer frames. We lack a theory of comprehension and communication that can

    provide

    a

    foundation for

    the

    way

    that

    question-answer

    systems

    function,

    and

    the

    way

    respondents

    understand

    them. The

    paper

    briefly

    describes

    the

    possible

    relevance

    of

    linguistic

    and

    cognitive

    processes

    for

    improving

    our

    understanding

    of

    interviews

    and

    surveys.

    The

    theoretical

    foundations

    of

    interviews

    and

    surveys

    also

    must

    address

    the

    way

    that

    artificial

    circumstances

    become

    necessary

    to

    guarantee

    adequate study

    designs.

    These

    artificial

    circumstances

    often

    violate

    ecological

    validity,

    or

    the

    way

    interviews

    and

    survey

    questions

    are

    constructed,

    understood,

    and

    answered,

    as

    contrasted

    with

    the

    way

    that

    field

    notes

    and

    tape-recordings

    of

    natural

    settings

    are

    used

    to

    address the

    same

    or

    comparable

    substantive

    and

    theoretical

    issues.

    Social scientists

    have relied

    on

    inter

    views

    for

    a

    long

    time.

    There

    is

    little

    reason

    to doubt their value and utility formany

    theoretical

    and

    practical

    purposes.

    There

    exists

    a

    huge

    literature

    on

    the

    virtues

    and

    drawbacks

    of interviews

    that

    use

    open

    ended

    questions

    and

    surveys

    that

    use

    close-ended

    questions.

    Yet there is

    little

    in

    the

    way

    of

    theory

    that

    would

    link

    inter

    *

    Presented

    at

    the

    thematic section

    Fact

    or

    Ar

    tifact:

    Are

    Surveys

    Worth

    Anything?

    held

    at

    the

    1980

    American

    Sociological

    Association

    Meetings,

    New

    York,

    August

    27,

    1980.

    The other

    speaker

    was

    Howard

    Schuman,

    taking

    a

    less critical view of

    sur

    vey

    research. I am

    grateful

    to Michael

    Cole,

    Roy

    D'Andrade,

    and

    Hugh

    Mehan for

    their

    valuable

    re

    marks and

    suggestions

    on a

    much

    longer

    first draft

    of

    the

    paper.

    [Address

    correspondence

    to:

    Aaron

    V.

    Cicourel;

    Department

    of

    Sociology; University

    of

    California,

    San

    Diego;

    La

    Jolla

    CA

    92037.]

    views and

    surveys

    to

    more

    general

    issues

    of communication and

    comprehension.

    Those researchers who are convinced that

    interviews and

    surveys

    are

    basic

    research

    tools for the

    sociologists

    are

    concerned

    about

    improvements

    in

    survey

    design

    and

    use,

    but

    see

    little

    point

    in

    challenging

    their

    routine

    use.

    In this

    paper

    I

    want to

    suggest

    a

    few

    cognitive

    and

    linguistic

    issues

    that

    can

    clarify

    our

    understanding

    of

    the

    pro

    cesses

    and

    mechanisms

    underlying

    the

    use

    of interviews

    and

    surveys.

    I

    also

    want

    to

    suggest

    some

    theoretical

    ideas

    that

    can

    strengthen

    the

    ecological validity

    of

    inter

    view and

    survey

    methods and

    findings.

    The

    necessity

    of

    writing

    a

    brief

    paper

    does

    not

    permit

    me

    to

    discuss old

    issues

    about

    current

    interview

    and

    survey prac

    tices

    that

    I

    hope

    are

    obvious

    to

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    3/11

    12

    The American Sociologist

    sociologists:

    for

    example,

    the

    way

    that

    preliminary

    qualitative

    interviews

    nor

    mally

    precede

    the construction

    of inter

    view schedules and surveys; and theway

    that

    pre-testing

    with

    in-depth

    interviews

    helps

    to

    create

    questions

    that

    respondents

    can

    understand,

    while

    helping

    to

    create

    answer

    categories

    that

    reflect

    the

    thinking

    of

    respondents

    and

    not

    simply

    the

    re

    searcher.

    Hence

    I will

    avoid

    discussing

    the

    range

    of

    practices

    that

    are

    necessary

    to

    insure

    quality

    control,

    such

    as

    using

    different

    types

    of

    questions

    on

    the

    same

    topic,

    exploring

    the

    significance

    of

    changes

    in

    wording,

    and

    other

    procedures

    too numerous to mention here.

    Interviews

    and

    surveys

    usually

    occur

    within

    a

    broader

    context

    of interaction

    that

    includes

    complex

    cognitive

    and

    lin

    guistic

    activities within

    a

    set

    of

    in

    stitutionalized

    and

    emergent

    socio

    cultural

    constraints.

    The

    questions

    used

    in

    surveys

    almost

    always

    are

    framed

    in

    a

    textual

    format

    that

    displays

    features

    in

    common

    with

    I.Q., aptitude,

    and

    reading

    tests.

    Virtually all social science

    data

    are

    de

    rived

    from

    some

    kind of

    discourse

    or

    tex

    tual

    materials

    such

    as

    reports,

    written

    ac

    counts

    of

    observations,

    interviews,

    audio

    or

    video

    recordings

    of

    natural

    settings,

    historical

    or

    contemporary

    documents,

    minutes

    of

    meetings,

    newspapers,

    maga

    zines,

    and

    the

    like.

    Questionnaires

    mailed

    to

    respondents

    presuppose

    something

    about

    the

    way

    people

    are

    able

    to

    analyze

    textual

    materials.

    For

    example,

    in

    re

    sponding

    to

    questions

    in

    a

    reading

    test,

    the

    respondent must utilize several sources of

    knowledge

    that

    the

    researcher

    used

    in

    interviews

    and

    surveys,

    therefore,

    pre

    sumes

    a

    theory

    of

    communication

    and

    comprehension

    that

    seldom

    is

    addressed

    by

    sociologists.

    The remainder

    of

    the

    paper

    will

    suggest

    aspects

    of communica

    tion

    and

    comprehension

    that

    can

    help

    es

    tablish

    some

    theoretical

    foundations

    for

    interviews

    and

    survey

    research.

    Aspects ofmemory and comprehension

    presupposed

    in

    surveys

    and

    interviews

    Recent work

    on

    learning

    and

    reading

    comprehension

    (Bransford

    et

    al., n.d.)

    reminds

    us

    that

    our

    research

    instruments

    stringently

    control

    the

    information

    re

    sources

    available

    to the

    subject

    or

    respon

    dent.

    In

    tests and

    questionnaires

    normal

    procedures presuppose an agreement

    or

    social

    contract

    between

    researcher

    and

    respondent:

    the

    contract

    does

    not

    permit

    the

    use

    of

    other

    persons

    (nor

    the inter

    viewer)

    in

    order

    to

    decide

    on

    the

    meaning

    of

    the

    question

    and

    the

    appropriateness

    of

    a

    response.

    Normal

    group

    or

    peer

    sources

    of

    help

    are

    blocked.

    The

    test

    or

    question

    naire

    item

    is

    assumed

    to

    be

    self

    explanatory

    or

    self-contained.

    These

    con

    ditions

    contrast

    with

    the

    possibility

    of

    consulting

    a

    friend

    or

    colleague

    or

    return

    ing to a textual report, newspaper article,

    book,

    and

    the

    like,

    during

    or

    after

    an

    ini

    tial

    reading

    of

    the

    text.

    The interview

    and

    survey

    seek

    to

    re

    strict

    the

    question

    frame,

    and

    in

    the

    case

    of

    surveys,

    the

    answer

    frame.

    The

    goal

    is

    to

    restrict

    the

    question

    in

    such

    a

    way

    as

    to

    anticipate

    and

    even

    designate

    (in

    surveys)

    the

    range

    of

    responses

    that

    can

    be

    used.

    The

    aggregation

    of

    responses requires

    a

    few choices

    that

    either

    are

    formally

    part

    of

    the questionnaire

    or

    are

    imposed

    on

    open-ended

    responses.

    We

    impose

    infor

    mation

    processing

    restrictions

    on

    the

    re

    spondent

    because

    they

    enable

    us

    to

    aggre

    gate

    and summarize

    a

    large

    amount of

    in

    formation

    in

    a

    fairly

    succinct

    way.

    But

    we

    pay

    a

    price,

    and

    we

    need

    to

    understand

    the

    costs

    in

    order

    to

    improve

    the

    reliability

    and

    validity

    of

    interview

    and

    survey

    data.

    We

    need

    a

    better

    understanding

    of

    the

    role

    of

    memory

    and

    the

    way

    questions

    are

    comprehended.

    Norman

    (1973)

    has

    noted

    several relationships between the organi

    zation

    of

    memory

    and

    answers

    to

    ques

    tions:

    a

    question

    may

    not

    evoke

    an

    appro

    priate

    recall

    if

    it

    is

    phrased

    differently

    from

    the

    storage

    format.

    Norman

    calls

    this

    the

    paraphrase

    problem

    because

    the

    best

    answer to

    a

    question

    may

    be

    the

    use

    of

    another

    question

    by

    the

    respondent

    in order

    to

    clarify

    what

    is intended

    by

    the

    original question.

    Norman's

    reference

    to

    memory

    brings

    up

    the

    problem

    of

    how

    people

    store

    information

    and combine

    general

    and

    specific

    sources of informa

    tion

    in

    order

    to

    reveal

    what

    they

    think

    is

    addressed

    by

    the

    question

    they

    are

    asked.

    Norman

    is

    interested

    in

    the

    pre-processing

    that

    occurs

    before

    an

    answer to

    a

    question

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    4/11

    The Problem

    of

    Ecological Validity

    13

    can

    be

    produced.

    In

    the

    case

    of

    surveys

    we are

    faced with

    the

    paradox

    that the

    respondents

    are

    not

    encouraged

    to

    pro

    vide us with reasons or explanations about

    their

    answers,

    yet

    such

    information

    gives

    us

    clues

    about

    how the

    question

    was

    un

    derstood.

    Norman

    (1973)

    states

    that retrieval

    of

    information from

    memory

    requires

    con

    struction

    by

    the

    respondent

    because

    of the

    paraphrase problem.

    It

    is

    difficult,

    there

    fore,

    to

    show

    the

    way

    questions

    and

    an

    swers are

    articulated

    by respondents

    be

    cause no

    simple algorithm

    can

    be iden

    tified that would enable

    us

    to

    specify

    a

    sequence of instructions or steps or ac

    tions that

    directly

    connects

    questions

    and

    answers.

    We

    need,

    therefore,

    an

    under

    standing

    of the

    comprehension

    process

    and

    the construction

    of

    responses

    in

    inter

    views

    and

    surveys.

    Aspects

    of

    language

    presupposed

    in

    surveys

    and interviews

    I want to

    suggest

    a

    parallel

    between

    as

    pects

    of modern

    linguistic theory

    and

    sur

    veys.

    Language

    can

    refer

    to

    a

    lexicon and

    individual

    words

    as

    a

    set

    of

    carefully

    con

    structed

    ideals that

    can

    be studied

    and

    described

    independently

    of

    actual lan

    guage

    use

    in

    social

    settings.

    The

    linguist's

    syntax-based

    theory

    of

    language provides

    the ideal

    structures

    the

    survey

    researcher

    needs

    for

    constructing

    standardized

    ques

    tionnaires

    whose forced-choice

    responses

    can

    be

    analyzed independently

    of

    the

    way

    persons

    in

    daily

    life

    actually

    discuss

    or

    pose and answer questions of each other

    within the

    constraints of

    daily

    practices

    in

    socio-cultural

    organizations.

    The

    linguist's

    normative

    theory

    of lan

    guage

    describes idealized

    prescriptive

    and

    proscriptive

    rules

    that

    are

    recipes

    for de

    ciding

    what

    are

    socially acceptable

    or

    un

    acceptable

    sentences.

    Survey questions

    are

    equally

    normative because

    they

    occur

    in

    highly

    restrictive

    settings

    that

    have little

    or

    nothing

    to

    do

    with

    actual

    discussions

    or

    practices

    in

    group

    or

    informal

    organi

    zational activities. But the

    linguist's

    and

    survey

    researcher's

    idealized

    language

    structures and substantive

    questions

    about the

    world of

    opinions,

    attitudes,

    be

    liefs,

    and

    moral

    judgments

    are an

    integral

    part

    of

    the

    way

    researchers and

    the

    public

    governmental

    agencies

    conceptualize

    and

    interpret

    the

    world

    around

    them.

    The paradox

    we

    face is that

    our

    surveys

    and

    interviews

    only

    indirectly

    reflect

    as

    pects

    of

    the

    daily

    life

    settings

    of those

    we

    interrogate, yet

    these

    instruments

    can

    be

    the

    basis for the

    development

    of

    policy

    by

    organizations

    in

    many

    complex

    nation

    states.

    If

    we

    recognize

    that

    questions

    and

    an

    swers

    are

    speech

    acts

    (Austin,

    1962;

    Searle,

    1969),

    we can

    make

    some

    sug

    gestions

    about

    the

    way

    that

    the

    structure

    of

    language

    can

    improve

    our

    understand

    ing of interviews and surveys. Speech act

    theory

    seeks

    to

    combine

    the

    analysis

    of

    the

    propositional

    content

    of

    an

    utterance

    with

    its

    illocutionary

    force;

    the intention

    of

    a

    speaker

    to

    act on

    the

    world

    by

    the

    use

    of

    a

    promise,

    assertion,

    command,

    and

    the

    like.

    Speech

    acts

    enable

    the researcher

    to

    establish

    a

    functional

    meaning

    for

    an

    ut

    terance

    by

    the

    way

    they

    are

    classified

    as

    statements

    about

    the

    world,

    a

    speaker's

    act

    on

    the

    world,

    or

    a

    symbolic

    represen

    tation

    of

    an

    event

    in

    the world.

    A

    meta

    language

    was

    felt

    to

    be

    necessary

    for dis

    cussion of

    speech

    acts.

    The

    way

    in which

    surveys

    and

    inter

    views

    are

    conducted

    presupposes

    a

    model

    of

    conversational

    behavior that

    has been

    ignored

    by

    most

    sociologists.

    The notion

    of

    a

    speech

    act

    model

    has

    been

    extended

    to

    the

    idea

    of conversational

    postulates

    by

    Grice

    (1975),

    and

    derived

    from his

    more

    general

    notion

    of

    the

    cooperative

    principle.

    The

    principle

    refers

    to

    a

    kind of

    directive to the speaker to formulate all

    aspects

    of his

    or

    her

    utterance

    in

    a

    way

    that

    will

    permit

    participants

    of

    a

    conver

    sation

    to

    facilitate

    to

    the

    utmost

    the

    achievement

    of

    the

    explicit

    and

    tacitly

    agreed

    upon

    aims of

    the conversation.

    Grice

    identifies

    four

    categories

    that

    are

    designed

    to

    orient

    the

    speaker

    to

    be

    as

    informative

    as

    possible

    but

    not

    more

    in

    formative

    than

    seems

    necessary.

    Nor

    should

    the

    speaker

    say

    anything

    believed

    to

    be

    false

    or

    anything

    that lacks

    sufficient

    evidence.

    The

    speaker

    also is

    to be rele

    vant

    and

    to

    be

    brief

    and

    orderly

    while

    trying

    not to be

    ambiguous

    or

    obscure

    in

    his

    or

    her

    use

    of

    expressions.

    Notice

    that

    the

    term

    speaker

    would

    apply

    to

    both the

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    5/11

    14

    The American

    Sociologist

    interviewer and

    respondent

    in

    the

    case

    of

    surveys

    and

    interviews.

    Another theoretical

    issue

    contained

    in

    Grice's work includes the notion of con

    versation

    implicature.

    In

    everyday

    con

    versation,

    listeners

    are

    expected

    to

    make

    inferences that

    do

    not

    necessarily

    follow

    from the

    premises

    or

    statements

    given,

    yet

    the

    statements

    are

    necessary

    for the

    com

    prehension

    of

    the

    discourse

    or

    text. The

    notion

    of

    conversational

    implicature

    is

    central

    to

    surveys

    and

    interviews

    as

    sub

    sets

    of

    discourse

    and textual

    activities

    be

    cause

    our

    questions

    require

    that

    the

    re

    spondent

    go

    beyond

    the

    information

    given

    in survey questions and presume that the

    utterances

    can

    be

    expanded

    in

    order

    to

    pursue

    their

    implications

    and

    derive

    co

    herence from what is said.

    Speech

    act

    theory

    can

    help

    us

    under

    stand

    the

    way

    that

    variations

    in

    the

    textual

    content

    and

    structure

    of

    interview

    and

    survey

    questions guide

    the

    kinds of

    in

    terpretations

    that will

    be

    made,

    and how

    these attributions of

    meaning

    will

    influ

    ence

    the

    construction of

    a

    response

    or

    the

    selection of

    an

    option

    in

    a

    set

    of forced

    choice

    responses.

    Some

    aspects

    of question-answer

    systems

    Question-answer

    systems

    deal with

    a

    sub-set of

    speech

    act

    theory.

    The

    ques

    tions

    employed

    are

    for

    themost

    part

    direct

    attempts

    to

    elicit information.

    In

    everyday

    English

    we

    often

    use

    indirect

    speech

    acts

    and

    our

    questions

    do

    not

    always

    follow

    an

    interrogative

    format.

    Scheduled inter

    views and surveys presume a respondent

    who is

    aware

    of

    the

    general procedure.

    A

    specific

    style

    of

    interrogation

    is

    used and

    respondents

    assume

    a

    response

    stance

    that

    differs

    markedly

    from routine

    every

    day

    conversation. But this

    response

    set

    is

    quite

    similar

    to

    occasions

    when

    someone

    is

    being

    interviewed for

    a

    job

    or

    is

    taking

    a

    test.

    The formal

    aspects

    of

    conversation

    out

    lined

    by

    Grice

    indirectly parallel

    some

    formal

    properties

    of

    question-answer

    systems

    described

    by

    Harrah

    (1973).

    Whereas Grice

    specifies

    some

    general

    conditions

    governing

    all

    conversations,

    including

    aspects

    of the

    reasoning

    neces

    sary

    for

    successful

    exchanges,

    Harrah

    in

    dicates

    conditions

    that

    are

    especially

    rele

    vant to

    the

    questioner.

    In

    Harrah's

    (1973)

    model

    the

    ques

    tioner:

    (a)

    Is

    presumed

    to

    know

    what the

    problem

    is

    about.

    (b)

    Knows

    how

    to

    express

    the

    question

    in

    an

    effective

    manner.

    (c)

    Knows what

    the

    set

    of

    possible

    alterna

    tives

    can

    be.

    (d)

    Can

    claim

    that

    one

    of

    the

    alternatives

    is

    true.

    (e)

    Does

    not

    know

    which alternative

    he

    wants

    to

    know.

    (f)

    Believes

    the

    respondent

    can

    help

    him

    if

    particular

    question

    is

    put

    properly.

    But

    there

    are

    various

    logics

    of

    questions,

    notes

    Harrah,

    and

    they

    will

    vary

    according

    to

    the

    social situation

    in which

    questions

    are

    used.

    Thus

    in

    a

    classroom

    setting

    a

    teacher

    puts

    questions

    to students

    and

    knows

    the

    answers

    expected.

    Harrah

    de

    scribes

    some

    of

    the

    conditions

    of

    a

    Ph.D.

    examining

    committee

    where

    their

    ques

    tions

    may

    be

    directed

    as

    much

    to

    each

    other

    as to

    the

    student.

    In the

    Ph.D.

    ex

    amination

    the

    knowledge

    base of

    the

    re

    spondent

    is

    presumed

    to

    be the

    problema

    tic

    issue.

    When

    a

    physician

    asks

    questions

    of

    a

    patient,

    the

    knowledge

    base of

    the

    respondent

    may

    be viewed

    as a source

    of

    new

    information.

    But this

    information

    re

    quires

    particular

    types

    and

    sequences

    of

    questions

    and

    answers.

    The

    patient's

    an

    swers

    also

    may

    become

    problematic

    de

    pending

    on

    the

    kinds

    of

    attributions

    made

    to

    the

    patient

    because

    of

    age,

    mental

    status,

    social

    position,

    and

    physical

    con

    dition and appearance, tomention a few

    key

    variables

    that

    affect

    all

    question

    answer

    systems.

    Questionnaire

    items

    are

    not

    merely

    in

    dividual,

    self-contained

    texts,

    but become

    the

    basis

    for

    inferring

    macro-structures

    that resemble

    those

    reported

    by

    re

    searchers

    working

    on

    text

    comprehension

    (Kintsch

    and

    van

    Dijk,

    forthcoming;

    van

    Dijk,

    1972).

    The

    respondent

    seeks

    a

    more

    comprehensive

    understanding

    of

    the

    dif

    ferent

    questions

    asked

    despite

    the

    re

    searchers

    attempts

    to randomize the pre

    sentation

    of

    questions

    that

    are

    linked

    by

    hypotheses

    in the

    research

    design.

    This

    search

    for

    a

    pattern

    on

    the

    part

    of

    the

    respondent

    is

    part

    of

    an

    attempt

    to

    create

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    6/11

    The

    Problem

    of

    Ecological

    Validity

    15

    an

    explanation

    that

    goes

    beyond

    whatever

    cryptic

    information is

    supplied by

    the

    interviewer

    or

    questionnaire.

    The

    respon

    dent becomes an active participant in the

    survey

    or

    interview

    and

    seeks

    to

    develop

    his

    or

    her

    own

    hypotheses

    about

    what

    is

    going

    on

    and

    what

    intentions the

    re

    searcher

    projects

    by

    the

    kinds

    of

    ques

    tions asked.

    Discourse and

    textual

    analysis

    as

    comprehension

    A

    key

    issue

    in

    the

    study

    of

    comprehen

    sion is

    trying

    to

    estimate

    what

    the

    respon

    dent brings to the reading test, interview,

    or

    survey.

    There

    are

    several

    strategies

    available for

    studying comprehension.

    In

    unpublished

    work

    by

    David

    Rumelhart,

    comprehension

    by

    subjects

    is

    measured

    by

    asking

    them

    to

    read

    a

    story

    line

    by

    line

    while

    indicating

    what

    is

    happening

    after

    each

    line.

    An

    unpublished

    project

    in

    prog

    ress

    on

    reading

    comprehension by

    Charles

    Fillmore

    and Paul

    Kay

    uses

    reading

    test

    items

    (that

    strongly

    resemble

    survey

    questions)

    as

    the basis for interrogating

    the

    child

    about

    his

    or

    her

    understanding

    of

    the

    text

    of

    each

    test

    question.

    More

    abstract

    types

    of textual

    analysis

    include

    the identification of

    topics

    or

    themes

    and

    their

    continuity

    over

    a

    large

    textual domain

    (Grimes,

    1980).

    A

    large

    and

    growing

    literature

    exists

    here

    that

    ad

    dresses

    textual

    analysis

    (Dressier,

    1977;

    Halliday,

    1967).

    I

    will

    not

    attempt

    to

    re

    view

    current

    work

    on

    the

    analysis

    of

    texts

    but

    only

    mention

    the

    broad

    goal

    of

    iden

    tifying those structures thatwould serve

    as a

    basis for

    interpreting specific portions

    and

    general

    aspects

    of

    a

    text.

    Particular

    conventions

    of

    language

    use,

    such

    as

    fol

    lowing

    certain forms of

    language

    structure

    when

    writing

    a

    letter,

    a

    report,

    in

    structions

    for

    filling

    out

    the

    necessary

    pa

    pers

    for

    a

    bank

    loan,

    and

    the

    like,

    generate

    expectations

    about how

    topics

    are

    intro

    duced,

    developed,

    and

    terminated.

    We

    need to

    know how interview

    and

    survey

    questions

    as

    texts

    are

    interpreted by

    the

    researcher

    and

    respondents.

    Current work

    on

    the

    analysis

    of discourse and textual

    materials

    can

    help

    us

    develop

    a

    theoretical

    foundation for

    understanding

    and

    im

    proving

    interviews. The theoretical

    foun

    dations of

    interviews and

    surveys

    must

    include

    the

    way

    that

    artificial

    circum

    stances

    necessary

    to

    guarantee

    adequate

    study designs can violate the ecological

    validity

    of

    findings

    vis-?-vis

    what

    takes

    place

    in

    daily

    life

    settings.

    Restating

    the

    ecological validity

    issue

    for

    sociology

    Social scientists

    often

    are

    so

    pre

    occupied

    with

    creating

    an

    adequate

    study

    design

    that

    they

    overlook

    the

    ecologi

    cal

    validity

    problem:

    Do

    our

    instruments

    capture

    the

    daily

    life

    conditions,

    opinions,

    values, attitudes, and knowledge base of

    those

    we

    study

    as

    expressed

    in

    their

    natu

    ral habitat?

    Recent work

    by

    Cole

    et

    al.

    (1978)

    re

    views the

    history

    and current

    efforts

    to

    deal

    with

    the

    problem

    of

    ecological

    va

    lidity.

    Cole

    et

    al.

    refer

    to

    the

    revolutionary

    impact

    of

    Wundt's

    laboratory

    psychology

    for the

    experimental

    study

    of mind in

    arti

    ficially

    constructed and

    simplified

    envi

    ronments.

    Can

    we

    extend the

    elegance

    and control of laboratory research to field

    settings?

    In

    sociology

    it is

    difficult

    to

    study

    everyday settings

    while

    using

    carefully

    formulated

    surveys.

    Much of

    survey

    re

    search

    can

    be viewed

    as

    the

    application

    of

    rigorous techniques

    to

    data elicited

    in

    simplified

    and

    artificial social

    envi

    ronments.

    In

    sociology

    and

    psychology

    ecological

    validity

    remains

    a

    minor issue

    because

    studies of

    the social

    organization

    of

    the

    laboratory and the interview or survey

    settings

    often

    are

    relegated

    to

    minor

    roles

    when data

    are

    analyzed.

    Psychologists

    have demonstrated renewed interest

    in

    pursuing laboratory-derived problems

    in

    natural

    settings

    and

    incorporating

    every

    day

    tasks

    into

    laboratory settings.

    In

    sociology

    this

    would

    mean

    contrasting

    the

    way

    interview

    and

    survey

    questions

    are

    constructed,

    understood,

    and

    answered,

    with the

    way

    that field

    notes

    and

    tape

    recordings

    of

    natural

    settings

    are

    used

    to

    address

    the

    same or

    comparable

    substan

    tive and

    theoretical

    issues.

    Psychologists

    are

    sensitive

    to

    problems

    associated

    with task

    definition,

    mental

    overload,

    and

    possible

    differences

    in

    the

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    7/11

    16

    The

    American

    Sociologist

    way

    remembering, thinking,

    and

    attending

    to

    activities

    occur

    in

    laboratory

    and

    non

    laboratory

    settings.

    But

    they

    do

    not

    study

    the cultural definitions of everyday life

    that

    are

    part

    of the

    laboratory

    setting

    and

    that

    are

    invoked

    necessarily

    when

    at

    tempting

    to conduct

    controlled

    research

    in

    non-laboratory settings.

    Psychologists

    are

    not

    sensitive

    to

    what

    their

    subjects

    must

    be

    able

    to

    do

    to

    make

    themselves

    appear

    as

    normal

    members

    of

    a

    group

    and the

    larger

    society

    in

    order

    to

    perform

    ade

    quately

    in

    an

    experiment

    or

    in

    an

    every

    day

    setting.

    The

    subjects

    and the

    exper

    imenter

    both

    must

    rely

    on

    their

    everyday

    tacit knowledge in order to satisfy stated

    and

    unstated social conditions

    that must

    be

    followed

    if

    the

    research is

    to

    be

    consid

    ered

    successful.

    Sociologists

    are

    sensitive

    to

    the fact

    that

    many

    problems

    are

    associated with the

    way

    questionnaires

    are

    administered,

    coded,

    and

    organized

    for

    analysis.

    But

    they

    are

    insensitive

    to

    the

    information

    processing

    problems

    associated with

    these

    tasks.

    Because

    so

    many

    surveys

    are

    done

    in the

    same

    culture in which the re

    searchers

    also

    are

    native,

    and

    because

    we

    gradually

    have

    socialized

    our

    respondents

    to

    be

    fairly

    docile

    to

    the

    demands

    of

    sur

    veys,

    especially

    since

    everyday

    life

    cir

    cumstances

    often force them

    to

    submit

    to

    such

    activities,

    we

    have little

    knowledge

    about

    the

    social

    practices

    of

    survey

    re

    search

    within

    field

    settings

    and

    within

    re

    search

    centers

    where

    the

    analysis

    takes

    place.

    When

    we

    administer

    surveys

    in

    other

    cultures

    we

    incorporate

    natives who

    have been trained in the same method and

    who

    can

    tacitly negotiate

    the cultural

    dif

    ferences.

    During

    everyday

    interaction

    the

    mem

    bers

    of

    a

    group

    who

    routinely

    discuss

    political,

    economic,

    and

    social

    events

    are

    sensitive

    to

    group

    resources

    of informa

    tion

    and

    the

    interpersonal

    constraints that

    are

    imposed

    on

    exchanges,

    and also

    are

    aware

    of

    the

    knowledge

    limitations of the

    members

    of the

    group.

    The

    ecological

    va

    lidity

    issue addresses

    the

    extent to

    which

    responses

    to

    interview

    and

    survey

    ques

    tions

    reflect

    or

    represent

    the

    daily

    actions

    of

    a

    collectivity.

    We

    must

    compare

    the

    way

    collective discussions

    about

    topics

    covered in

    interviews

    and

    surveys

    parallel

    or

    differ from

    the

    way

    such themes

    are

    presented

    in

    the

    formal

    setting

    created

    by

    research

    goals.

    A partial examination of the ecological

    validity

    issue

    can

    be found

    in

    recent work

    by

    Schuman

    (1966),

    and Schuman

    and

    Presser

    (1977;

    1977-1978; 1979;

    forth

    coming)

    where

    they

    show that

    changes

    in

    the

    wording

    of the

    questions

    often

    lead

    to

    changes

    in

    response

    patterns.

    The work

    by

    Schuman

    and

    Presser also

    contains

    valuable

    information

    on

    differences

    in

    the

    use

    of

    open

    and closed

    questions.

    The

    authors

    (forthcoming:9-10)

    note

    that

    if

    re

    spondents

    are

    given

    a

    question

    about

    which they know nothing, many will an

    swer

    the

    question

    if

    there

    is

    no

    explicit

    don't

    know

    option.

    Many respondents

    are

    willing

    to

    admit

    ignorance.

    Respon

    dents

    also

    are

    said

    to

    make

    an

    educated

    (though wrong) guess

    about

    a

    topic

    de

    spite

    their

    being

    uninformed

    about

    the

    issue. The authors

    note that

    these

    re

    sponses

    are

    like

    non-attitudes

    in

    the

    sense

    that

    there

    probably

    was

    no

    prior

    thought

    about

    the

    attitude before

    pre

    sented by the interviewer.

    The studies

    by

    Schuman

    and Presser

    seek

    to

    resolve

    possible

    problems

    in

    inter

    viewing

    and

    surveys

    as a

    way

    of enhanc

    ing

    their

    reliability

    and

    validity.

    These

    studies,

    though

    limited

    in

    scope

    and

    depth,

    are

    valuable contributions

    to

    the

    minimal work

    that

    has

    been

    done

    by

    re

    searchers

    working

    within

    social science

    who

    strongly

    support

    these methods

    in

    their

    existing

    form.

    Many

    serious

    prob

    lems of

    reliability

    and

    validity

    remain

    be

    cause of normal practices that are devoid

    of

    adequate

    concern

    for theoretical

    foun

    dations.

    For

    example,

    we

    restrict

    our

    col

    lection of

    information

    to

    a

    few

    categories

    in

    order

    to

    restrict

    the

    number

    of

    compari

    sons

    we

    have

    to

    make. The

    respondent's

    knowledge

    of the world

    is

    not

    a

    problema

    tic

    issue.

    In

    a

    laboratory study

    or

    in

    sur

    veys

    with

    fixed-choice

    questions, subjects

    or

    respondents

    bring categorical

    mech

    anisms

    with

    them,

    but

    the

    actual

    pack

    ages

    of information

    they employ

    must

    be

    either recoded or tailored to the

    particular

    conditions

    of

    the

    experiment

    or

    survey

    question

    and

    forced

    choices

    provided.

    The

    range

    of

    speech

    acts

    becomes

    se

    verely

    restricted

    in

    survey

    research.

    The

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    8/11

    The

    Problem

    of

    Ecological

    Validity

    17

    respondent's

    cultural

    resources

    available

    for

    searching

    one's

    memory

    are

    con

    strained,

    and

    this

    limits the

    respondent's

    ability tomake comparisons. The survey

    question

    introduces co-variation

    by

    the

    wording

    of

    the

    question.

    The

    following

    examples

    are

    from

    a

    national

    survey

    by

    E.

    C.

    Ladd,

    Jr. and

    S.

    M.

    Lipset

    (as

    cited

    by

    S.

    Lang

    (1978)).

    The

    questions

    were

    sent

    to

    college

    and

    university professors.

    (1)

    The

    statements

    below relate

    to

    teaching

    and

    student

    performance.

    Does each

    correctly

    reflect

    your per

    sonal

    judgment?

    (i)

    The

    students

    with

    whom

    I

    have

    close

    contact

    are

    seriously underprepared

    in basic

    skills?such

    as

    those

    re

    quired

    for

    written and

    oral communi

    cation.

    (a)

    Definitely

    yes

    (b)

    Only partly

    (c)

    Definitely

    no

    (2)

    Grade

    inflation

    is

    a

    serious academic

    standards

    problem

    at

    my

    institution.

    (a)

    Definitely

    yes

    (b)

    Only partly

    (c)

    Definitely

    no

    (3) American higher education should ex

    pand

    the

    core

    curriculum,

    to

    increase the

    number

    of

    basic

    courses

    required

    of all

    undergraduates.

    (a)

    Definitely

    yes

    (b)

    Only partly

    (c)

    Definitely

    no

    The

    questions

    assume

    a

    sample

    of

    re

    spondents

    familiar

    with

    the

    content

    of

    the

    items

    (in

    the

    present

    case

    American

    col

    lege

    and

    university

    professors)

    as

    well

    as

    with the

    meaning

    of

    definitely

    yes,

    only

    partly, and definitely no. The questions

    and the

    responses

    reflect

    a

    co-variation

    that

    enables

    a

    reader

    to

    parse

    the

    question

    easily.

    The

    answers

    permit

    easy

    aggrega

    tion

    so

    that

    these

    and

    other

    questions

    can

    be

    cross-tabulated with

    size

    of

    school,

    age

    of

    respondent,

    discipline

    or

    field of

    study

    and

    the

    academic

    rank

    of

    the

    respondent.

    In

    the

    present

    case

    these

    questions

    were

    mailed

    out

    to

    the

    informant.

    There

    was

    no

    way

    for

    the

    respondent

    to

    obtain

    informal

    clarification

    about

    questions

    from

    the

    interviewer. The

    problem

    solving

    aspects

    of

    answering

    a

    fixed-choice

    question

    are

    severely

    limited.

    There

    were

    no

    open

    ended

    questions

    to

    make

    it

    possible

    to

    ob

    serve

    some

    elements

    of

    a

    problem-solving

    strategy

    at

    work

    (including

    the

    limitations

    of the

    respondent's

    knowledge

    base)

    as

    the

    interviewee and

    interviewer

    negoti

    ated the questions and answers. The

    questions

    on

    the

    basic

    skills of

    students,

    grade

    inflation,

    and

    the

    idea

    of

    expanding

    the

    core

    curriculum attribute

    an

    expertise

    to

    the

    respondent

    that

    cannot

    be

    chal

    lenged.

    There

    is

    no

    possibility

    of

    exploring

    the individual

    experiences

    of

    teachers

    within

    the

    same

    subject

    area

    much

    less

    across

    disciplines.

    Nor

    can

    we

    distinguish

    respondents

    by

    the level

    of

    college

    or

    uni

    versity

    classes

    they

    teach.

    We

    have

    no

    information

    on

    the

    background

    of their

    students.

    The

    categories

    created

    by

    the

    re

    searcher

    must

    be

    negotiated

    individually

    by

    each

    respondent.

    But the researcher's

    categories

    provide

    ready-made

    classes

    and the

    response

    set

    generates

    automatic

    criteria for

    deciding

    class

    membership.

    It

    is difficult

    to

    interpret

    the

    meaning

    of

    these

    personal

    judgments

    vis-?-vis

    the

    ex

    periences

    and

    knowledge

    base of

    a

    pre

    sumed

    group

    of

    experts.

    The

    expertise

    is an automatic creation of identifying

    a

    population

    known

    as

    being

    college

    and

    university

    teachers.

    The

    responses

    we

    obtain,

    however,

    remain

    ambiguous

    per

    sonal

    judgments.

    The

    ecological

    validity

    of

    the

    response

    is

    not

    addressed.

    Class

    membership

    and

    interview

    and

    survey

    categories

    An

    important

    aspect

    of

    surveys

    and

    open-ended

    interviews

    is the

    extent

    to

    which a concept or class membership is

    presupposed

    in

    the

    way

    we

    elicit

    infor

    mation

    from

    respondents.

    Our

    ability

    to

    perceive,

    remember,

    and talk

    about

    some

    object

    or

    event

    as an

    instance

    of

    a

    class

    or

    concept

    we

    are

    presumed

    to

    know

    is

    fun

    damental

    to

    the

    way

    we use

    surveys

    and

    open-ended

    questions.

    Public

    and

    private

    bureaucracies

    so

    cialize

    their

    employees

    to

    the

    use

    of

    categories

    that

    can

    subsume

    a

    variety

    of

    activities

    under

    identifiable

    classes

    and

    thus confer a

    stability

    on the world that

    enables

    them

    to

    go

    beyond

    the

    informa

    tion

    given.

    We

    negotiate

    the

    assignment

    of

    an

    object

    or

    event

    or

    some

    aspect

    of in

    formation

    to

    a

    class

    initially

    on

    the

    basis of

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    9/11

    18 The American

    Sociologist

    expectations

    of its

    perceptible

    elements,

    and

    then

    begin

    to

    infer

    some

    of the

    ob

    ject's

    or

    event's

    nonperceptible

    attributes.

    This is like saying thatwe construct the

    identification of

    typical

    features that

    en

    able

    us

    to

    claim

    class

    membership

    for

    an

    object

    or

    event,

    or we

    claim

    that

    certain

    features

    suggest

    one

    or

    more

    possible

    classes that

    trigger

    a

    search

    for additional

    elements

    that

    enable

    us

    to

    choose

    among

    several

    possible

    candidates of

    classes. In

    our

    perception

    of

    speech,

    for

    example,

    we

    may

    be forced

    to

    imagine

    or

    recall

    or

    search

    for

    information

    that

    extends be

    yond

    conventional

    or

    dictionary interpre

    tations of what is said because of the so

    cial

    setting.

    Our

    ability

    to

    make

    the

    non

    perceptible

    visible

    and

    hence

    integral

    to

    the

    invocation of

    a

    class

    or

    concept

    is

    a

    basic

    process necessary

    for

    all

    social

    in

    teraction

    and

    bureaucratic

    practices.

    A recent

    paper

    by

    Medin

    and

    Smith

    (in

    press)

    distinguishes

    between three views

    of

    concepts.

    The

    first

    or

    classical

    view

    requires

    that

    a

    concept

    have

    common

    properties

    which

    become

    necessary

    and

    sufficient to define the concept. Every

    member

    of

    a

    class

    can

    be

    specified by

    the

    properties they

    all

    must

    possess

    through

    a

    single

    description

    of

    all

    members.

    Medin

    and

    Smith

    note

    that

    attacks of this view

    revolve

    around

    the

    properties

    of

    descrip

    tion that

    must

    be

    true

    of

    all

    members.

    The

    second

    view

    of

    concepts

    is called

    the

    prototype

    or

    poly

    the

    tic

    position.

    The

    focus

    of

    this

    view

    is

    on

    the

    way

    instances

    of

    a

    concept

    can

    vary

    in

    th?

    degree

    to

    which

    they

    all

    share

    certain

    properties.

    This view says that the different instances

    can

    vary

    in

    the

    extent to

    which

    they

    will

    embody

    the

    concept.

    A

    single

    description

    of

    some

    may

    again

    suffice,

    but the

    prop

    erties

    of the

    description

    are now

    only

    true

    for

    most

    but

    not

    all

    of

    the

    members.

    Some

    instances

    of

    the class

    will

    possess

    more

    of

    the

    critical

    properties

    than

    will

    other

    in

    stances.

    Those

    instances

    possessing

    more

    critical

    properties

    are

    said

    to be

    more

    rep

    resentative

    of

    the

    concept

    in

    question.

    The third

    view

    described

    by

    Medin

    and

    Smith is called an

    exemplar

    notion.

    This

    view

    of

    a

    concept

    states

    that

    no

    single

    representation

    exists

    for

    an

    entire

    class

    or

    concept,

    but

    instead

    only

    specific

    repre

    sentations

    of

    the

    class's

    exemplars

    occur.

    The

    example

    given

    by

    Medin and

    Smith

    for

    this

    third view is

    that

    of

    the

    class

    of

    persons

    who

    might

    be

    called

    suicidal.

    The example of persons who might be

    suicidal is used

    by

    Medin and Smith

    to

    compare

    the three views

    briefly.

    They

    note

    that

    the

    classical

    view failed when

    used

    by

    clinicians because

    no

    necessary

    and

    sufficient

    common

    properties

    could

    be found

    to

    define

    all

    people

    who have

    suicidal

    tendencies.

    The

    polythetic

    view

    fails, say

    Medin

    and

    Smith,

    because

    it falls

    short of

    revealing

    how

    someone

    decides

    that

    a

    particular

    person

    is

    suicidal.

    Fol

    lowing

    a

    suggestion

    by

    Twersky

    and

    Kahneman (1973), Medin and Smith con

    tend

    that

    because

    clinicians

    are

    not

    likely

    to

    use a

    single

    description

    of

    all

    persons

    with

    suicidal

    tendencies,

    they

    might

    in

    stead

    make the

    decision

    about

    someone

    being

    suicidal

    by comparing

    the

    individual

    to

    other

    persons

    known

    to

    be suicidal.

    The

    exemplar

    view

    would

    result

    in

    the

    class

    of

    people

    with

    suicidal

    tendencies

    being

    rep

    resented

    by

    separate

    descriptions

    for

    various

    people

    known

    to

    be members

    of

    the class of suicidal persons.

    Two

    key questions

    raised

    by

    Medin

    and

    Smith here

    are

    as

    follows:

    (1)

    Is

    it

    possible

    to

    have

    a

    single

    or

    unitary

    description

    for

    all

    members

    of the

    class?

    (2)

    Can

    we

    say

    that

    all

    of the

    properties

    specified

    in

    a

    unitary description

    are

    true

    of

    all

    members

    of the class?

    According

    to

    Medin

    and Smith

    the

    classical

    view

    would

    answer

    both

    ques

    tions

    with

    an

    affirmative

    response,

    while

    the polythetic view would say yes to (1)

    and

    no

    to

    (2).

    The

    exemplar

    view

    would

    say

    no

    to

    (1)

    and consider

    (2)

    to

    be

    irrelevant.

    When

    we use

    fixed-choice

    question

    naire items

    the

    respondents

    are

    expected

    to

    be

    able

    to

    recognize

    the

    classes

    of ob

    jects

    stated in each

    item

    as

    self

    explanatory.

    This

    expectation

    derives

    from the

    assumed

    pretesting

    of

    each

    questionnaire

    item

    prior

    to

    sending

    out

    or

    utilizing

    the final

    questionnaire.

    The

    ex

    tent to

    which

    the

    respondent

    possesses

    the

    necessary

    knowledge

    base

    in

    order to

    answer

    the

    question

    is

    seldom

    a

    relevant

    issue. The

    possibility

    that the

    concepts

    or

    classes

    presented

    to

    the

    respondent

    may

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    10/11

    The

    Problem

    of

    Ecological Validity

    19

    not

    be

    defined

    clearly

    in his

    or

    her

    mind

    is

    also

    seldom

    an

    issue.

    In

    both

    cases

    the

    forced-choice

    nature

    of

    the

    survey

    guar

    antees an adequate response so long as

    the

    respondent

    is

    willing

    to

    check off

    one

    of

    the

    choices

    available. The researcher's

    versus

    the

    respondent's

    conception

    of

    a

    particular

    class

    or

    concept

    is

    presumed

    to

    be

    resolved

    by

    the

    pretest

    done

    to

    the

    final

    questionnaire.

    The

    issue of

    possible

    social

    classes used is

    not

    directly

    testable. What

    is

    testable is

    the

    way

    different

    respondents

    can

    be

    distinguished

    by

    some

    measure

    of

    social

    class

    as

    determined

    by

    the

    way

    fixed-choice

    questions

    are

    constructed,

    answered, and coded.

    Having

    created

    several

    social

    classes

    by

    one

    set

    of

    ques

    tions,

    we

    examine

    other

    questions

    an

    swered

    differentially

    and

    can

    attribute

    the

    differences

    to

    social

    class

    membership.

    Surveys

    are

    presumed

    to

    be

    hypothesis-driven

    and

    hence

    a

    way

    of

    testing

    theory.

    But

    the

    technical

    aspects

    of

    the

    instrument

    make

    it

    difficult

    to

    clarify

    theoretical

    concepts

    or

    classes

    said

    to

    have

    motivated

    the

    use

    of

    the

    survey.

    Theoretical concepts are subservient to

    the

    mechanics

    of

    creating

    and

    imple

    menting questions

    and

    their

    coding.

    The

    discourse

    and textual

    processes

    and

    mechanisms that

    provide

    the

    theoretical

    basis

    for

    surveys

    remain

    unexamined,

    as

    are

    the

    social

    constraints

    and

    practices

    of

    the

    society's

    social

    stratification

    system

    that enable

    the

    researcher

    to

    utilize

    such

    an

    esoteric

    and

    indirect instrument

    to

    learn

    about

    the

    everyday

    activities and

    beliefs of the

    members

    of

    a

    group.

    Conclusion

    Part

    of

    the

    paper

    has

    been critical

    of

    survey

    research.

    I

    have

    slighted

    several

    issues.

    For

    example,

    conscientious

    survey

    researchers

    seek

    a

    form of

    quasi-experi

    mentation

    with

    questionnaire

    items

    in

    which

    a

    particular

    question

    frame

    is

    re

    tained but

    a

    particular

    word

    (or

    perhaps

    phrase)

    is

    altered.

    Important

    differences

    often

    are

    found

    because

    of

    these

    changes

    (cf.

    Schuman and

    Presser,

    1981),

    even if it

    is

    not

    always

    clear what

    sort

    of

    reasoning

    we

    should

    attribute

    to

    the

    respondents.

    What

    is

    important

    for

    the

    survey

    re

    searcher

    is

    the

    patterning

    that

    occurs or

    emerges

    that

    gives

    us more

    confidence

    in

    the

    overall

    survey.

    When

    the

    same

    ques

    tions

    are

    used

    across

    different

    groups

    or

    with the same group at different times,

    and

    similar,

    or

    the

    same,

    patterning

    emerges,

    then

    the researcher

    feels consid

    erably

    more

    confident

    that

    the

    question

    naire is

    reflecting something significant

    about

    the

    respondents'

    opinions,

    atti

    tudes,

    or

    beliefs.

    Knowing

    that

    some

    identifiable

    group

    was

    more

    in

    favor

    of

    some

    action

    or

    law

    this

    year

    than last

    year

    is

    part

    of the formal

    patterning

    that is

    sought.

    There still

    can

    be

    problems

    here

    when

    particular

    researchers

    throw

    out

    items that do not seem to work, but the

    general

    idea

    is

    to

    avoid

    using question

    naire

    items

    as

    if

    responses

    on a

    given

    oc

    casion

    can

    be

    treated

    in

    some

    absolute

    way.

    The

    goal

    is

    to

    look

    at

    some

    group

    or

    category

    relative

    to

    other

    groups

    or

    categories

    at

    specific

    periods

    of

    time,

    and

    not

    how

    many

    respondents

    endorse

    a

    given

    item

    at

    a

    particular

    time.

    The

    survey

    researcher

    seeks

    to

    control

    the

    way

    a

    data

    base is

    generated

    by

    creat

    ing restricted conditions under which in

    formation is

    to

    be

    elicited,

    coded,

    and

    an

    alyzed.

    The

    conditions

    simplify

    and

    dis

    tort

    the

    daily

    life activities

    of

    those

    groups

    and

    institutions

    we

    seek

    to

    understand

    and

    predict,

    but

    the

    controls and

    restricted

    data

    base

    are

    highly

    valued

    by

    many

    social

    scientists

    because

    they

    foster

    a sense

    of

    scientific

    rigor

    in

    our

    research.

    Another

    source

    of

    control

    in

    survey

    re

    search

    can

    be

    found

    in

    the

    enormous

    ad

    vances

    that

    have

    occurred

    in

    sampling

    theory and the researcher's ability to sam

    ple

    different

    respondents.

    What is

    more

    difficult is

    the

    sampling

    of behavior. In

    the

    case

    of

    voting

    behavior

    we

    find

    a

    fairly

    close

    correspondence

    between what

    people

    say

    in

    response

    to

    a

    questionnaire

    item and the

    way

    they actually

    vote.

    But

    other

    topics

    do

    not

    fare

    as

    well,

    and

    some

    not

    well

    at

    all. We

    are

    not

    clear about the

    behavior

    or

    activities

    the

    survey ques

    tionnaire items

    are

    said

    to

    index.

    People

    are

    not

    very

    accurate in

    describing

    their

    own behavior when asked to

    respond

    to

    direct

    questions.

    The

    primary

    difficulty

    remains the

    absence

    of

    strong

    theories.

    Instead of

    using

    strong

    theories

    we

    invari

    ably rely

    on

    the detection

    of

    patterning

    in

    This content downloaded from 64.76.142.170 on Mon, 28 Oct 2013 15:50:13 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 S1 Ciceral, A. (1982) Interviews, surveys and the problem of ecological validity.pdf

    11/11

    20

    The

    American

    Sociologist

    survey

    responses

    in

    order

    to

    guide

    us

    in

    making

    theoretical

    explanations

    after

    the

    fact.

    Theory

    seldom

    guides

    social

    re

    search explicitly; we depend

    on

    research

    findings

    to

    decide

    which

    theoretical

    con

    cepts

    seem

    appropriate.

    Sophisticated

    survey

    researchers

    surely

    can

    find answers

    or

    replies

    to

    the

    is

    sues

    I

    have

    raised

    and

    will

    point

    to

    the

    use

    of

    other

    sources

    of data

    or

    additional

    checks

    or

    strategies

    that

    I

    have

    not

    cov

    ered in

    the

    paper.

    W?

    need

    strong

    theories

    to

    decide

    whether

    a

    particular

    method

    and the data

    it

    yields

    tells

    us

    something

    worth

    know

    ing.We all are forced to deal with the

    same

    problem

    of

    interpretation

    regardless

    of whether

    we use

    surveys,

    census

    mate

    rials,

    vital

    statistics,

    extensive

    interview

    ing, participation

    observation,

    or

    audio

    or

    video

    tapes.

    The

    interpretation

    issue is

    seldom the focus of

    survey

    research,

    much

    less

    any

    other

    type

    of

    research

    in

    sociology.

    Notions like limited

    capacity

    processing,

    comprehension

    of discourse

    and textual

    materials,

    and

    language

    use

    in

    socially constrained contexts,

    remain

    pe

    ripheral

    topics

    in

    sociology.

    Yet

    they

    ad

    dress the

    interpretation

    issue

    in

    several

    explicit

    ways.

    Can

    we

    afford

    the

    con

    venience of

    ignoring

    these

    issues?

    REFERENCES

    Austin,

    John

    L.

    1962

    How

    To Do

    Things

    With

    Words. Cam

    bridge,

    MA:

    Harvard

    University.

    Bransford,

    John

    D.,

    Barry

    S.

    Stein,

    Tommie S.

    Shelton,

    and

    Richard A.

    Owings

    n.d.

    Cognition

    and

    adaption:

    The

    importance

    of

    learning

    to

    learn.

    Unpublished

    manuscript.

    Cole,

    Michael,

    Lois

    Hood,

    and

    Ray

    P.

    McDermott

    1978

    Ecological

    niche

    picking:

    Ecological

    inva

    lidity

    as an

    axiom

    of

    experimental cognitive

    psychology.

    Unpublished

    manuscript.

    Dressier,

    Wolfgang

    U.

    (ed.)

    1977

    Current

    Trends in

    Textlinguistics.

    Berlin,

    NY:

    de

    Gruyter.

    Grice,

    H.

    Paul

    1975

    Logic

    and

    conversation.

    Pp.

    41-58 in

    P.

    Cole and

    J.

    Morgan

    (eds.),

    Syntax

    and

    Semantics.

    New

    York:

    Academic.

    Grimes,

    Joseph

    E.

    1980

    Context structure

    patterns.

    Paper

    pre

    sented at the Nobel Symposium on Text

    Processing,

    Stockholm,

    August.

    Halliday,

    Michael

    A. K.

    1967

    Notes

    on

    transitivity

    and theme

    in

    En

    glish.

    Journal

    of

    Linguistics

    3:199-244.

    Harrah,

    David

    1973

    The

    logic

    of

    questions

    and

    its relevance

    to

    instructional

    science. Instructional Sci

    ence

    1:447-467.

    Kintsch,

    Walter and

    Teun A.

    van

    Dijk

    forth-

    Toward

    a

    model of

    text

    comprehension

    coming

    and

    production. Psychological

    Review.

    Lang, Serge

    1978 The

    professors:

    A

    survey

    of

    a

    survey.

    The New York Review of Books 25:38-42.

    Medin,

    Douglas

    and Edward

    Smith

    n.d.

    Representation

    and

    processing

    of

    lexical

    concepts.

    Unpublished

    manuscript.

    Norman,

    Donald A.

    1973

    Memory,

    knowledge,

    and

    the

    answering

    of

    questions. Pp.

    135-165 in

    R.

    S.

    Solso

    (ed.),

    Contemporary

    Issues

    in

    Cognitive

    Psychology:

    The

    Loyola

    Symposium.

    Washington,

    DC:

    Winston.

    Schuman,

    Howard

    1966

    The

    random

    probe:

    A

    technique

    for

    evaluating

    the

    validity

    of closed

    questions.

    American

    Sociological

    Review

    21:218-222.

    Schuman, Howard and Stanley Presser

    1977

    Question

    wording

    as an

    independent

    vari

    able

    in

    survey

    analysis. Sociological

    Methods

    and

    Research

    6:151-170.

    1977-

    Attitude

    measurement

    and

    the

    gun

    control

    1978

    paradox.

    The Public

    Opinion

    Quarterly

    41:427-438.

    1979

    The

    open

    and

    closed

    question.

    American

    Sociological

    Review 4:692-712.

    1981

    Questions

    and Answers

    in Attitude Sur

    veys:

    Experiments

    on

    the Effects

    of

    Ques

    tion

    Form,

    Wording

    and Context.

    New

    York:

    Academic Press,

    forth-

    Public

    opinion

    and

    public

    ignorance:

    The

    com- fine line between attitudes

    and

    non

    ing

    attitudes.

    American Journal

    of

    Sociology.

    Searle,

    John

    R.

    1969

    Speech

    Acts.

    Cambridge:

    Cambridge

    Uni

    versity

    Press.

    Tversky,

    Amos and

    Daniel Kahneman

    1973

    Availability:

    A

    Hueristic

    for

    judging

    fre

    quency

    and

    probability.

    Cognitive

    Psy

    chology

    5:207-232.

    van

    Dijk,

    Teun

    A.

    1972

    Some

    Aspects

    of Text

    Grammers.

    The

    Hague:

    Mouton.