43
Retributive Support for War and Torture Peter Liberman Queens College and Graduate Center, CUNY Flushing NY 11367 [email protected] Manuscript accepted May 27 2011. Please cite as: Liberman, Peter (in press). Retributive Support for War and Torture. Journal of Conflict Resolution. Acknowledgments The survey data analyzed here was collected as part of a collaborative project on “Integrating Research on Domestic and Foreign Policy Opinions,” sponsored by a 2009 International Studies Association Venture Grant. I am very grateful to Bill Chittick for conceiving the project and pursuing the grant, to Jason Reifler for having programmed and administered the survey, and to both of them along with Miroslav Nincic and Jennifer Ramos for writing several of the survey questions utilized here. I am also thankful to Harald Schoen and JCR’s external reviewers for their feedback. Keywords: Coercive interrogation; torture; support for war; foreign policy; public opinion; retributive justice; values.

Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

  • Upload
    buikiet

  • View
    216

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

Retributive Support for War and Torture

Peter Liberman Queens College and Graduate Center, CUNY

Flushing NY 11367 [email protected]

Manuscript accepted May 27 2011. Please cite as:

Liberman, Peter (in press). Retributive Support for War and Torture. Journal of Conflict Resolution.

Acknowledgments

The survey data analyzed here was collected as part of a collaborative project on

“Integrating Research on Domestic and Foreign Policy Opinions,” sponsored by

a 2009 International Studies Association Venture Grant. I am very grateful to Bill

Chittick for conceiving the project and pursuing the grant, to Jason Reifler for having

programmed and administered the survey, and to both of them along with Miroslav

Nincic and Jennifer Ramos for writing several of the survey questions utilized here. I am

also thankful to Harald Schoen and JCR’s external reviewers for their feedback.

Keywords:

Coercive interrogation; torture; support for war; foreign policy; public opinion;

retributive justice; values.

Page 2: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

2

Abstract

This article tests the hypothesis that ordinary people favor punishing badly

behaved foreign actors to make them “pay” for their crimes, rather than purely to protect

national security interests. In a large college-student sample, individual differences in the

endorsement of a retributive punishment principles predicted support for punishing

transgressor states (including a state sponsoring terror attacks against a rival, a nuclear

proliferator, and a small aggressor state) and terrorist detainees. These estimates

controlled for partisanship, ideology, humanitarian and security values, and beliefs about

the efficacy of force. These results provide further evidence that retributive values and

motivations contribute to laypeople’s support for punishing foreign wrongdoers.

Page 3: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

Introduction

Much of international relations theory is devoted to analyzing security incentives

for the use of force, especially against states that have behaved aggressively or greedily

(e.g., Glaser 2010). A simpler form of the same logic informs rationalist theories of

public support for war (e.g., Gelpi et al. 2009; Jentleson 1992; Jentleson & Britton 1998;

Page & Shapiro 1992). But might we favor punishing badly behaved foreign actors to

make them “pay” for their crimes, rather than purely for self-protection? In principle,

punishment can aim not only at security, but also retributive, restorative, or rehabilitative

justice. But the roles of the justice motives in foreign policy and foreign policy opinion

have received relatively scant attention.

Retributive justice motivations are particularly worth studying, because they play

such an important role in everyday thinking about domestic crime and punishment. This

is clear from the boom in research on the psychology of punishment that followed the

U.S. Supreme Court’s 1976 reinstatement of the death penalty. The Court’s majority

decision argued that public opinion should help define the boundaries of “cruel and

unusual” punishment. Moreover, because “the instinct for retribution is part of the nature

of man,” the Court contended that retributive justice was a legitimate reason for

executing murderers, regardless of the penalty’s efficacy as a deterrent (quoted in

Finckenauer 1988: 82).

Survey research has found that “a life for a life” was in fact Americans’ favored

justification for the death penalty, more important than was deterring serious crime or

preventing killers from killing again. Indeed, most death penalty supporters did not waver

when asked to suppose that the penalty had no deterrent effect (Bohm 1992; Ellsworth &

Page 4: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

2

Gross 1994; Ellsworth & Ross 1983; Gross & Ellsworth 2003; Lord et al. 1979; Tyler &

Weber 1982; Vidmar 1974). More recently, experimental research has demonstrated the

centrality of retribution in laypeople’s penal sentencing preferences for a variety of

crimes, and a surprising insensitivity to instrumental incentives for punishment (reviewed

by Carlsmith & Darley 2008).

Of course, people may think about punishing foreign wrongdoers very differently

from the way they think about punishing ordinary criminals. Wars are much more costly

than are death-row executions, and this greater cost could lead people to weigh

consequences more heavily than the gratification of moral impulses. In addition, national

identity moderates moral and emotional responses to international offenses (Gordijn et al.

2001; Gordijn et al. 2006; Yzerbyt et al. 2003). For example, the generally tepid level of

public support for humanitarian intervention suggests that people are much less upset by

crimes against unfamiliar foreigners than by crimes against their own kind (Eichenberg

2005; Jentleson 1992; Jentleson & Britton 1998; Herrmann & Shannon 2001).

However, a number of scholars have argued that desires for revenge can fuel

international conflicts (Harkavy 2000; Lebow 2008; Lebow 2010; Löwenheim &

Heimann 2008; Scheff 1994). In less guarded moments, some leaders and officials have

seemed to reveal or imply retributive motives. For example, President George W. Bush in

a private meeting in early 2003 justified his opposition to a war-avoiding settlement that

would grant Iraqi leader Saddam Hussein a safe exile, on the grounds that Saddam was “a

thief, a terrorist, a war criminal. Compared to Saddam, Milosevic would be a Mother

Teresa. When we go in, we’ll uncover many more crimes and we’ll take him to the

International Court of Justice in The Hague” (Danner 2007: 61). Bush also implied a

Page 5: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

3

retributive motive in his decision to approve the C.I.A.’s request to waterboard Khalid

Sheikh Mohammad, a man who reportedly had masterminded the September 11, 2001

terror attacks and had beheaded personally Wall Street Journal reporter Daniel Pearl. “I

thought about my meeting with Danny Pearl’s widow, who was pregnant with his son

when he was murdered,” Bush wrote in his recent memoir. “I thought about the 2,973

people stolen from their families by al Qaeda on 9/11. And I thought about my duty to

protect the country from another act of terror. ‘Damn right,’ I said” (Bush 2010).

It is difficult to determine when such feelings have played a decisive role in

decision-making. When an official emphasizes an enemy’s past crimes, this could reflect

a retributive justification for punishment, an argument about the threat posed, a case for

the need to bolster deterrence, or some combination of these reasons. In addition,

moralistic justifications are often accompanied by parallel national security arguments, as

in Bush’s reference mentioned above to “my duty to protect the country.” There are good

political and professional reasons for political leaders and officials to conceal the role of

emotion and retribution in policymaking. Thus the relative scarcity of references to moral

outrage and moral desert in historical sources on decision-making does not rule out their

having played a role. But it does make these motives difficult to study.

Retributive motives are easier to investigate in public opinion, where they also

may play a greater role. After all, ordinary citizens are arguably less able to evaluate the

efficacy and utility of war, as well as being less accountable for their preferences, than

decision-makers. Judging by their frequent emphasis on enemy regimes’ crimes and evil

nature in prowar rhetoric, political leaders apparently think that the public responds to

such moral appeals (Ben-Porath 2007; Moerk 2002).

Page 6: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

4

A handful of studies of retributive motivation in foreign policy opinion suggest

that there may be something to this belief. In prior research on U.S. opinion on the 1991

Persian Gulf War and 2003 Iraq War, I found that death penalty supporters were

particularly supportive of war against Iraq, and argued that this could reflect individual

differences in the moral belief that wrongdoers deserve harsh punishment (Liberman

2006, 2007). Other studies have found that anger–which is closely linked to the desire for

retribution–correlates strongly with support for war and for punitive occupation policies

(Huddy et al. 2007; Lambert et al. 2010; Pagano & Huo 2007; Sadler et al. 2005; Skitka

et al. 2006). Still other work has found that explicit desires to avenge the September 11th

attacks on the United States predicted support for the Afghanistan War, the War on

Terror, and the Iraq War (Liberman & Skitka 2011; Liberman & Skitka 2011). Finally,

addressing a different kind of organized violence against foreigners, Carlsmith and Sood

(2009) showed that U.S. citizens supported the coercive interrogation of terrorist

detainees as much as for retributive punishment as for intelligence collection.

These findings, though important, have left unanswered questions about the

psychology of international punishment. Correlations between support for the death

penalty and support for war may well reflect underlying retributive values, in light of the

well-known retributive basis of Americans’ death penalty support (DPS), but there are

other plausible explanations for such correlations. It is also unclear how whether earlier

findings generalize to other targets of punishment.

This paper seeks to build on prior work by examining retributiveness and support

for punishing a greater variety of foreign offenders, including nuclear proliferating states,

unthreatening aggressors, state sponsors of terrorist attacks against foreign countries, and

Page 7: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

5

terrorist detainees. The variety of offenders and offenses provides a test of the breadth of

situations that can elicit retributive reactions. Using a direct measure of retributiveness,

instead of a surrogate measure like DPS, allows a more straightforward test of its

connection to support for international punishment. And controlling for a wider array of

potential confounders offers more convincing tests of the retributive motivation

underlying observed correlations between retributive dispositions and support for

punishing foreign actors.

Analyzing survey data collected from a large college-student sample, I find that

individual differences in the endorsement of a retributive punishment philosophy

predicted support both for punishing transgressor states and for the coercive interrogation

of enemy detainees. These relationships, moreover, were not artifacts of partisanship,

ideology, humanitarian or security values, or beliefs about the efficacy of coercion.

Retributiveness also closely correlated with support for the death penalty, suggesting that

it is their retributive values that lead death penalty supporters to favor harsher punishment

of transgressor states and detainees.

In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive logic in

public attitudes about war and torture, this paper contributes to a broader research agenda

on the role of core values in political attitudes (see, e.g., Feldman 2003; Goren 2005;

Hurwitz & Peffley 1987; Schwartz et al. 2010). Though party identification and left-right

ideology remain the leading factors explaining policy positions, particularly among the

political aware (Abramowitz & Saunders 2008; Jost et al. 2009; Lewis-Beck et al. 2008;

Zaller 1992), citizens’ core values often shape their positions as well, particularly issues

where interests are ambiguous or contested and that cut across the main partisan and

Page 8: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

6

liberal-conservative divides.

The rest of the paper is divided into five sections. I begin by situating this study in

relation to prior research, and then proceed to describe the study’s main hypotheses,

survey sample, and measure of retributiveness. The following sections analyze in turn

support for punishing transgressor states and support for coercive interrogation. A brief

conclusion reviews the findings, discusses caveats and limitations, and considers their

implications for theories of public opinion and international politics.

Study Questions and Context

Research on the retributive nature of laypeople’s support for punishing foreign

enemies is still in its infancy. Although often called a “punishment philosophy” or a

“penal sentencing goal,” there is good reason to conceptualize retributiveness as being

among a larger set of fundamental human values, which values theorists define as

“cognitive representations of desirable, transsituational goals” (Schwartz et al. 2010; see

also Rokeach 1973). But retributiveness has been ignored in research on values and

political attitudes, most of which has focused on more self-evidently political values,

such as egalitarianism, humanitarianism, and libertarianism in the domestic issue domain,

and militarism, anti-communism, and ethnocentrism in the foreign policy domain (see,

e.g., Feldman 1988; Goren 2004; Hurwitz & Peffley 1987; Steenbergen 1995; and the

review by Feldman 2003).

Research on personal value inventories, such as Schwartz’ list of basic personal

values–power, achievement, hedonism, stimulation, self-direction, universalism,

benevolence, tradition, conformity, and security–has also not focused on retributiveness

per se (studies on their relationship with foreign policy attitudes include Argo 2009;

Page 9: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

7

Cohrs et al. 2005; Cohrs et al. 2005; Davidov, Meuleman, et al. 2008; Schoen 2009;

Schwartz et al. 2010; Spini & Doise 1998). None of these values quite captures the

concept of retributiveness. Retributiveness is a disposition to believe that wrongdoers

deserve to be punished for their crimes, in proportion to the degree that their crimes

caused others to suffer. While retributive punishment may affect security or conformity,

it is an end-in-itself rather than a means to personal or societal safety or stability.

Although related to benevolence and humanitarianism, these values do not reflect the

transactional nature of “an eye for an eye.” Retributivists are harsh and tough-minded

toward the guilty, but can be extremely benevolent towards the innocent.1

Retributiveness has been studied almost exclusively in research on interpersonal

vengefulness (Cota-McKinley et al. 2001; Douglas & Martinko 2001; Eisenberger et al.

2004; McCullough et al. 2001) and criminal punishment attitudes (e.g., Bohm 1992;

Okimoto et al. in press; Tyler & Weber 1982; Vidmar 1974; Warr & Stafford 1984). But

the fact that people’s personal vengefulness correlates with their endorsement of

retributive sentencing principles suggests that a more general retributive value may shape

thinking in both domains (McKee and Feather, 2008).

These studies have proceeded by analyzing correlations between people’s

retributive punishment philosophies and their punitive attitudes or behavior. This is a

better approach than directly asking people to justify their punishment preferences for

1 The inverse relationship between benevolence and retributiveness is evident in the use

of an item on forgiveness to measure benevolence scale in the full Schwartz values

questionnaire. But this item is only one of several, and is typically dropped in the most

commonly used shorter versions of the scale.

2 It is harder to use this experimental design to study interstate punishment than torture.

Page 10: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

8

two reasons. First, people often are unaware of the reasons for their preferences,

especially those guided by intuition and gut feeling rather than by deliberate reasoning.

And there is good reason to think that much moral judgment is driven by intuition; what

is often taken for moral reasoning is really post-decisional rationalization that people

engage in when pressed to justify their moral judgments (Carlsmith 2008; Haidt 2001;

Wilson 2002). Second, a taboo on revenge and retribution in many societies–rooted in

Christian teachings of forgiveness, states’ demands to monopolize the administration of

justice, and widespread utilitarian principles–prompts people to clothe their retributive

motivations in euphemisms for retribution (e.g., “let justice be done”, “God’s will”) or in

utilitarian terms (e.g., “necessary to deter others,” or “stop killers from killing again”).

As a result, some of the most compelling demonstrations of laypeople’s

retributive motivations for punishment have used experimental methods to manipulate

retributive and instrumental incentives for punishment, and determine their effects on the

severity of preferred punishments (reviewed by Carlsmith & Darley 2008). The only

work in this vein to investigate support for punishing foreign actors was Carlsmith and

Sood’s (2009) study of Americans’ support for coercive interrogation. The investigators

described to all respondents a detained Afghan terror suspect named “Farid.”

Respondents were given varying expert estimates of likelihood that Farid was

withholding vital information about an imminent terror attack, ranging from no chance to

a 95% chance. In a second, orthogonal manipulation, half the participants received

additional information about Farid’s past terrorist activities. Those informed about

Farid’s terrorist past recommended significantly harsher coercive interrogation methods

than did those told nothing about these activities. The effect of this information was as

Page 11: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

9

large as the effect of being told there was a 95% chance that Farid was withholding vital

information, compared to the “no chance” condition. An additional finding demonstrated

further that people implicitly regarded coercive interrogation as a punishment:

preferences on interrogation severity correlated closely with those on how harshly the

prisoner should be “punished.”

Although this study provided compelling evidence of the retributive nature of

support for punishing foreign detainees, it is unclear whether the findings generalize to

punishing transgressor states.2 Military punishment usually requires expending far more

national blood and treasure than does punishing helpless individual detainees. So people

might be less likely to indulge their moral and emotional desires for retribution through

war than through torture.

Prior research on the retributive nature of public support for military punishment

has used non-experimental methods. Two studies used direct measures of desires for

revenge over the specific foreign transgression of the September 11th attacks on the

United States (Liberman & Skitka 2011; Liberman & Skitka 2011). Others used DPS as a

proxy measure for a more general retributive disposition, examining correlations between

U.S. popular support for the death penalty and support for wars against Iraq. On the

2 It is harder to use this experimental design to study interstate punishment than torture.

Rightly or wrongly, people might infer security incentives as well as retributive ones for

punishment from information provided about a state’s past crimes. Respondents might

find stipulations in the vignette about the perceived security incentives of military

punishment (such as it is or is not required to restore deterrence) less credible than

stipulations about the probability that a detainee is withholding vital information.

Page 12: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

10

grounds that retributiveness correlates highly with DPS, these studies suggest that

retributiveness probably accounted for the DPS/war support associations (Liberman

2006, 2007).

The hypothesis that retribution might motivate support for military action might

explain the associations found in prior work between war support and anger. Feelings of

anger have been linked by other lines of research to retributive dispositions and to desires

for retribution (Barber et al. 2005; Carlsmith et al. 2002; Douglas & Martinko 2001;

McCullough et al. 2001). Thus findings that anger predicts support for war or other

sanctions on offending outgroups (e.g., Gordijn et al. 2006; Lambert et al. 2010; Mackie

et al. 2000; Huddy et al. 2007; Sadler et al. 2005; Skitka et al. 2006; Pagano & Huo 2007)

provides indirect evidence for the role of retributive values and motives.

However, all of these studies are vulnerable to the standard charge against

correlational findings that some omitted covariate might account for the observed

associations. This charge is particularly persuasive against work using proxy measures

for vengeance-seeking (i.e., anger) or retributiveness (i.e., DPS), to the extent that non-

retributive motivations or dispositions that also affect these proxies. But even work that

more directly measures desires vengeance-seeking or retributiveness must address the

possibility that their associations with punitiveness are due to other confounding factors.

The most plausible alternative factors are desires for security and inclinations or

aversions to using force, along with any dispositions to experience these desires/feelings.

Abstract efficacy-of-force beliefs like “bad guys will inevitably strike unless they are

incapacitated” and “punishing bad guys demonstrates that crime does not pay” might

motivate punitiveness in both the law-and-order and international domains, at least for

Page 13: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

11

pragmatic citizens. The “rational public” school contends that public opinion does in fact

respond in roughly prudent ways to obvious environmental incentives and changes, like

soaring crime, new international threats, or the prospects for military victory (Eichenberg

2005; Gelpi et al. 2009; Jentleson 1992; Jentleson & Britton 1998; Page & Shapiro

1992). Anger is related to perceptions of power and efficacy as well as to desires for

retribution (Mackie et al. 2008; Maitner et al. 2006; Valentino et al. 2009), so the

findings on anger and military punishment do not provide conclusive evidence of

retributive motivation. Finally, the fact that some people care more about security and

conformity than others offers an explanation focusing on non-retributive values for cross-

sectional correlations between DPS and retributiveness, on the one hand, and support for

military punishment, on the other (Cohrs et al. 2005; Cohrs et al. 2005; Schwartz et al.

2010).

Humanitarian values and feelings also shape attitudes toward war, but in a less

contingent way than retributive values and feelings. Prior research has found

humanitarianism and pacifism to predict broad military-policy postures as well as

attitudes about specific uses of force (Hurwitz & Peffley 1987; Schwartz et al. 2010).

In short, much work remains to be done to determine the influence of retributive

dispositions and motivations on support for punishing foreign enemies. In this article,

I focus on dispositions, and seek to test three hypotheses. First, retributiveness heightens

support for the use of military force against states that have violated international norms

(H1). This test goes beyond prior work by analyzing correlations between a direct

measure of retributiveness, instead of reliance on the proxy of DPS, and support for

punishing a variety of offender states other than Afghanistan or Iraq. The second

Page 14: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

12

hypothesis provides a cross-sectional test of Sood and Carlsmith’s (2009) argument about

torture: Retributiveness heightens support for the coercive interrogation of terrorists and

insurgents (H2). For the reasons explained above, I estimate these associations while

controlling for beliefs and values relating to security and the use of force. The final

hypothesis is that retributiveness is a stronger predictor of DPS than are other personal

values, partisanship, or ideology (H3). H3 is useful for evaluating research on

international punishment relying on DPS as a proxy for retributive dispositions.

Study Sample and Measure of Retributiveness

I tested the above hypotheses using data collected in an online survey completed

by 946 college students at a large southern university, for extra credit in a required

introductory American Politics course, from April 26th–May 6th, 2010. The sample was

more young (M=21), female (63%), African-American (39%), Asian (14%), Democratic

(52%), and liberal (44%) than the nation as a whole. There is good reason to expect

whites, males, and conservatives to be somewhat more retributive and punitive than

others, so this sample is far from ideal for U.S. population estimates. But this sample

provides useful data for the purpose of investigating hypothesized inter-relationships

between retributive values and punitive attitudes.

The two central concepts in retributive justice are deservingness and proportional

punishment. In this study I measured retributiveness using five questions addressing both

of these concepts, drawn from prior work on criminal punishment sentencing goals

Page 15: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

13

(Bohm 1992; McKee & Feather 2008; Pratto et al. 1998; Tyler & Weber 1982; Vidmar

1974):3

1. Society should not punish murderers just to avenge the victims. (reversed)

2. For a terrible crime, there should be a terrible punishment.

3. We should show mercy to those who have done wrong. (reversed)

4. Those who hurt others deserve to be hurt in return.

5. Punishment should fit the crime.

For the most part, these questions avoid loaded terms like “retribution,” “vengeance,” and

“revenge,” whose negative connotations would likely heighten measurement error. The

two con-trait items were somewhat distinct from the pro-trait items, but I retained them to

reduce acquiescence bias. All five items were combined into an additive Retributive

scale, and adjusted to range from 0-1 (like all the variables in this study), with higher

scores representing greater approval of retributive punishment (Alpha=0.68; M= 0.56;

SD=0.19).

Having a face-valid measure of retributiveness, though an improvement from

reliance on proxy measures such as DPS, does not obviate having control for utilitarian

punishment goals and beliefs as well as humanitarianism. As noted earlier, people with

utilitarian reasons for embracing tough law-and-order policies might embrace retribution

as a complementary justification for this position. In addition, some people might adopt

“tit for tat” as a rule of thumb for dealing with all sorts of transgressors, less from the

principle of desert than to signal intolerance for future misbehavior. Thus responses to

3 The response options for items 1-5 were: strongly agree, agree somewhat, agree, neither

agree nor disagree, somewhat disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree.

Page 16: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

14

questions on proportionate punishment (particularly items 2 and 5 in Retributive, in

contrast to 1 and 4 which highlight desert), even though they do not mention its

consequences, might reflect beliefs about its utility for personal and social gain (on which

see Axelrod 1984; Gintis et al. 2005). A similar argument could be made about

benevolence or humanitarianism. As noted above, humanitarians may tend to be both

nonretributive and opposed to violence of all kinds because they shrink from causing

human suffering.

Does Retributiveness Predict Support for International Punishment?

I measured support for international punishment by combining responses to three

pairs of questions about three hypothetical international transgressions:

1. How should the United States respond if one small nation attacks another that has

done it no harm? [Response options: do nothing; condemn the aggressor; impose

economic sanctions on the aggressor; bomb the aggressor; invade the aggressor

and overthrow its government]

2. How severely do you feel the aggressor should be punished for its actions? [No

punishment at all; very weak punishment; weak punishment; moderate

punishment; strong punishment; very strong punishment]

3. How should the United States respond if Iran withdraws from the Nuclear

Nonproliferation Treaty and tries to build nuclear weapons? [Do nothing;

condemn Iran; impose economic sanctions on Iran; bomb Iran; invade Iran and

overthrow its government]

Page 17: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

15

4. How severely do you feel Iran should be punished for its actions? [No punishment

at all; very weak punishment; weak punishment; moderate punishment; strong

punishment; very strong punishment]

5. How should the United States respond if the government of Pakistan was found to

have sponsored a deadly terrorist attack killing hundreds of civilians in India? [Do

nothing; condemn Pakistan; impose economic sanctions on Pakistan; bomb

Pakistan; invade Pakistan and overthrow its government]

6. How severely do you feel Pakistan should be punished for its actions? [No

punishment at all; very weak punishment; weak punishment; moderate

punishment; strong punishment; very strong punishment]

These scenarios all involve crimes against foreign nations, which should elicit less

powerful desires for retributive justice than crimes against one’s own. Retributive

reactions were also likely to be limited by the absence of vivid, realistic details about

wrongdoers, their malevolent intentions, and their heinous crimes, all of which magnify

emotional arousal and judgments of blame (Alter et al. 2007; Douglas et al. 1997; Ogloff

& Vidmar 1994; Small & Loewenstein 2005).

There are also important differences between these scenarios. Whereas the “small

nation” and “Pakistani terrorism” scenarios involved clear violations of international law,

the “Iranian proliferation” scenario involved a legally permissible action, which

nonetheless violated the global NPT norm. The transgressors also differ in terms of the

threat they pose to U.S. security. Whereas most small nations are unlikely to pose much

danger, Americans might have regarded Pakistani sponsorship of terrorism as threatening

to the United States in a post-9/11 world. A nuclear-armed Iran would probably have

Page 18: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

16

been regarded as the most threatening. In repeated polls over the last five years, more

Americans have identified Iran as the greatest foreign threat to the United States, and 60-

72% have called Iran’s nuclear program a “major threat to the well being of the United

States” (Pew Research Center for the People & the Press 2009: 20). Americans have also

widely regarded the Iranian regime as morally bad from the time of the 1979 hostage

crisis, an image no doubt accentuated by President Bush’s 2002 speech branding Iran as

part of an “Axis of Evil.”

Despite these differences, the highly intercorrelated response to these items

suggests that they measured a common underlying reaction. An exploratory factor

analysis (EFA) revealed a single dominant underlying factor (Eigenvalue=1.97 followed

by 0.29).4 I thus averaged these items into a single scale of Punishment (Alpha=0.74;

M=0.54; SD=0.17), the mean of which fell between “weak” and “moderate” punishment,

and closer to economic sanctions than to bombing.

The location of the Punishment items in the survey, immediately following

questions on the utility of force and more than seventy-five questions later than the

Retributive items, was designed to minimize the possibility that questions about

retributive punishment principles primed retributive reactions to the international

punishment scenarios. Any question-order priming effects would have favored the

alternative hypothesis that people punish transgressor states to enhance security rather

than to give them their just deserts.

4 The most unique item was the one on specific punitive responses toward the small aggressor nation (loading of 0.38 on the single factor, compared to 0.50-0.72 for the others).

Page 19: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

17

A simple regression of Punishment on Retributive, shown in Model A on Table I,

shows that the latter was a significant predictor of the former. 5 To determine whether this

relationship was a byproduct of gender, race, political partisanship, ideology,

humanitarianism, or beliefs in the utility of force, I control for each of these potential

confounders. In addition to standard seven-level measures of political partisanship

(Republican, M=0.36; SD=0.31) and left-right ideology (Conservative, M=0.43;

SD=0.27), I include right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) as a political orientation, as it

has been found in prior work to correlate with attitudes about both criminal punishment

and war (Doty et al. 1997; McFarland 2005; McKee & Feather 2008). An abbreviated

RWA scale combined six questions on agreement/disagreement with the following

statements (Alpha=.76; M=0.29; SD=0.11):6

1. Our country desperately needs a mighty leader who will do what has to be done to

destroy the radical new ways and sinfulness that are ruining us.

2. Gays and lesbians are just as healthy and moral as anybody else. (reversed)

3. Atheists and others who have rebelled against the established religions are no

doubt every bit as good and virtuous as those who attend church regularly.

(reversed)

5 All regression models analyzed here employed list-wise deletion of missing data.

6 Arguably, an element of retributiveness appears in RWA due to the statements on the

need to “destroy the…sinfulness” and “smash the perversions” in society. The negligible

effect on the Retributive term discussed below indicates little shared explained variance.

Page 20: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

18

4. The only way our country can get through the crisis ahead is to get back to our

traditional values, put some tough leaders in power, and silence the troublemakers

spreading bad ideas.

5. Our country needs free thinkers who have the courage to defy traditional ways,

even if this upsets many people. (reversed)

6. Our country will be destroyed someday if we do not smash the perversions eating

away at our moral fiber and traditional beliefs.

Table I shows results from regressing Punishment on the demographic and political

variables (Model IB) and from adding Retribution to these variables (Model IC).7 It is

clear from the scarcely diminished Retribution term that the variable’s relationship to

Punishment was not epiphenomenal to gender, race, partisanship, or ideology.

As noted above, controlling for humanitarian values, security concerns, and

instrumental beliefs about the efficacy of punishment should help provide a clearer test of

retributive motivation. The survey data included items on all of these values and beliefs,

though not all at the same level of generality. An EFA on the items specifically

addressing foreign policy attitudes revealed two underlying factors (two Eigenvalues near

2.0 and the third dropping to 0.36), a hawkish stance and a humanitarian one (see Table

II). Although clear logical distinctions can be drawn among the items loading on the

hawk/dove factor, in the data there was considerable similarity between the importance

people attached to security (item 3), their belief in the utility of force (items 9 and 10),

and their desires for military power and spending (items 1, 2, and 4). I thus combined

7 Multicollinearity was not a problem in the model, with all variance inflation factors

<1.80.

Page 21: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

19

these six items into a single scale of Hawk attitudes (Alpha=0.76; M=0.56 of a 0-1 range;

SD=0.16).

However, the patterns of responses to the item on bombing (11), as well as to two

others on the consequences of coercive interrogation (items 12-13) discussed further

below, were quite distinct from both the hawkish and humanitarian factors. I thus used

item 11 as a separate measure of belief in the coercive efficacy of Bombing (scaled 0-1 so

that higher values reflected belief in the efficacy of coercive bombing; M=.39; SD=.34).

The four items on helping poor countries (items 5-8) loaded on the second factor, and

were combined into a Humanitarian scale (Alpha=0.74; M=0.63; SD=0.18), which

correlated very modestly with Hawk (r=0.05).

Values at the personal level were measured using a short version of Schwartz’

Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ), the same 21-item scale appearing in the European

Values Survey (see e.g., Davidov, Schmidt, et al. 2008). The PVQ asks the extent to

which respondents regard themselves as similar to or different than a series of abstract

persons described as having particular values. In some cases I combined closely related

values, which the short version of the PVQ often has difficulty discriminating between

(Knoppen & Saris 2009), but this was not necessary for the PVQ value of personal

Security, based on two questions asking for the degree of similarity or difference people

felt to those for whom “It is important to her/him to live in secure surroundings. S/he

avoids anything that might endanger her/his safety” and for whom “It is important to

her/him that the government ensures her/his safety against all threats. S/he wants the state

to be strong so it can defend its citizens” (Alpha=0.60; M=0.71 of a 0-1 range; SD=0.21).

These items did not load heavily on either of the two foreign policy factors when added

Page 22: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

20

to the EFA shown in Table II, and Security correlated only moderately with Hawk

(r=0.33). Preliminary tests found that Security was the only PVQ value that was a

significant predictor of Punishment.8

As can be seen in Table I (Model ID), Hawk, Bombing, Humanitarian, and

Security all were significant and positive predictors of Punishment.9 The positive

Humanitarian coefficient indicated that, instead of being averse to international

punishment as might have been expected, humanitarians favored harsher punishment,

perhaps thinking it would prevent more suffering than it would inflict. More importantly,

as Model IE on Table I shows, Retributive remained significantly related to Punishment,

even after removing the explained variance shared with the control variables. The

Retributive term was comparable in magnitude to those of Hawk and Humanitarian,

whether measured in terms of the unstandardized coefficients in Table I, or in terms of

standardized Beta coefficients (Betas of 0.12 vs. 0.12 and 0.15). In sum, even if it is not

the main source of support for punishing foreign transgressor states, retributiveness

appears to be an important one distinct from security and humanitarian beliefs and

values.10

8 A combined Power/Achievement value, as well as Tradition and Conformity also had

statistically significant bivariate correlations with Punishment, but these were not

significant predictors when controlling for Security.

9 A separate test not shown found no positive interaction between Security and a

combination of the two utility-of-force beliefs used in the Hawk scale.

10 Not all of the Punishment scale items were equally related to Retributive. The items on

how severely the transgressor state “should be punished” correlated more strongly than

Page 23: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

21

Does Retributiveness Predict Support for Coercive Interrogation?

The same survey included four questions on support for coercive interrogation,

prefaced with a common stem: "The US has signed a number of treaties that prohibit

certain methods for trying to get information from detainees. Here are some of those

prohibitions. For each one, please select whether you agree or disagree:”11

1. Governments should never use physical torture

2. Governments should never threaten physical torture.

3. Governments should never use mental torture (such as making a detainee

believe that his family will be killed).

4. Governments should never use humiliating or degrading treatment.

These combined into an internally reliable scale of Torture, which was scaled 0-1 so that

higher values reflected greater support for coercive measures (Alpha=0.82; M=0.44;

SD=0.25). Nearly ninety questions separated the Torture items from those used to

measure Retributive, so it is highly unlikely that the latter primed retributive responses to

the former.

those asking about specific punitive policies. Across cases, with all the controls in the

model, Retributive was the strongest predictor of punishment severity for Iran

(Beta=0.16), followed by the small nation (Beta=0.13), followed by Pakistan

(Beta=0.10). Whether such differences stem from the greater threat posed by Iran or from

prior moral images the Iranian regime is an important question for future research.

11 The response options were: strongly agree; somewhat agree; somewhat disagree;

strongly disagree.

Page 24: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

22

As can be seen in Model IIIA in Table III, Retributive had a strong relationship

with Torture, even stronger than with Punishment. But as was the case with international

punishment, it is important to test whether this relationship was a byproduct of gender,

race, political partisanship, ideology, humanitarianism, beliefs in the general utility of

force, or more specific beliefs about the utility of coercive interrogation.

One would expect political predispositions to account for more variance in

Torture than Punishment, due to the prominent national debate over the use of coercive

interrogation measures such as waterboarding by the George W. Bush Administration.

This debate has been more intense and more politically polarized than discussion over the

hypothetical international punishment scenarios in the Punishment items. This probably

accounts for greater relationships between Torture and left-right ideology, party

identification, and RWA (see Model IIIB in Table III). The relationship between

Retributive and Torture was attenuated somewhat when controlling for these general

political variables, as can be seen from comparing Models IIIB and IIIC. But Retributive

remained by far the strongest predictor.

One of the issues in the debate over torture has been whether or not it works.

Proponents contend that coercive methods are sometimes necessary to extract

information from terrorist detainees in order to prevent impending terror attacks, while

critics reply that coercion yields unreliable confessions. Critics also contend that U.S.

maltreatment of enemy detainees will make American personnel falling into enemy hands

more likely to be mistreated as well. These two reasons for and against torture were

tapped respectively by questions 12 and 13 on Table II. Responses to these questions

were distinct from the “hawk” and “humanitarian” factors underlying the other foreign

Page 25: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

23

policy items, as can be seen from their high uniqueness. They were also scarcely related

to each other (r=-0.05; p=0.14), so I treated them as separate variables, Savelives (M=.47;

SD=.28) and Reciprocity (M=.61; SD=.27). Both were scored 0-1, so that higher values

of Savelives represented stronger faith in the utility of coercive interrogation, and higher

values of Reciprocity reflected greater concern that torturing terrorists would eventually

boomerang against American detainees.

As can be seen from Table III (Model IIID), Hawk, Savelives, and Humanitarian

all significantly predicted support for coercive interrogation, and in the expected

directions. Interestingly, Hawk was by far the strongest predictor, suggesting that support

for torture was shaped above all by respondents’ general hawk/dove posture on national

security issues. Reciprocity and the personal value of Security both had significant

bivariate correlations with Torture, but neither were significant predictors in this model.12

In additional tests, the Security term dropped to insignificance when controlling just for

Hawk. This is consistent with a hierarchical-opinion-constraint effect (Hurwitz & Peffley

1987), i.e., with general desires for personal security constraining people’s

hawkish/dovish foreign policy orientation, which in turn constrained support for coercive

interrogation. Additional tests also found that the Reciprocity term dropped to

insignificance when controlling for just Hawk and Humanitarian. Since the concern

12 Preliminary tests found that of the ten PVQ values, only Security and the compound

values of Universalism/Benevolence and Power/Achievement had significant bivariate

correlations with Torture. But none of these variables were significant predictors when

added to Model IIID, nor did they alter the regression coefficient for Retributive when

added to Model IIIE.

Page 26: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

24

about blowback from the U.S. practice of torture was unlikely to have shaped these broad

foreign policy orientations, this finding suggests that the blowback concern may have

functioned as a rationalization for positions on coercive interrogation stemming from

general hawk/dove and humanitarian postures.

Consistent with hypothesis H2, Retributive was a strong predictor of support for

coercive interrogation, even when controlling for all these covariates (see Model IIIE).13

Retributive was the strongest single predictor, whether measured by the unstandardized

coefficients reported in Table III or by standardized Betas. In another model not shown,

adding a measure of the terrorist threat (from an item asking “How worried are you that

in the near future there will be terrorist attacks in this country?”) did not affect the

coefficient of Retributive and failed to attain statistical significance at the p<.05 level.

The shrinking of the Retributive coefficient with the addition of the block of

security and humanitarian variables in Model IIIE indicates some shared explained

variance. Further tests indicated that Hawk and Savelives were responsible for this entire

effect; controlling for just Security did not affect the Retributiveness coefficient at all.

While one can imagine Retributive being endogenous to Security, it does not appear that

the correlation between Retributive and Torture was a spurious byproduct of underlying

personal insecurity. It is harder to imagine Retributive being endogenous to Hawk and

Savelives. How could hawkish international security preferences or a belief in the life-

saving utility of coercive interrogation have shaped retributiveness?

13 Multicollinearity was not a problem in the model, with all variance inflation factors

<1.80.

Page 27: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

25

Indeed, the reverse seems more plausible. Retributiveness might influence beliefs

about the acquisition of military power, which can support military retribution as well as

more prosaic goals. Retributiveness might also lead some to endorse the deterrent and

defensive virtues of force, to rationalize desired retributive punishment. This could

account for the diminishing coefficient for Hawk with the addition of Retributive in Table

III (and to a lesser extent in Table I). Hawkish views that stemmed originally from

retributive dispositions, moreover, could assume independent causal force. Consistent

with this hypothesis, a Sobel-Goodman mediation test with bootstrapped standard errors

indicated that Retributive had a modest but significant indirect effect (b=.04 p<0.001) on

Torture through Hawk, amounting to 12% of Retributive’s total effect.

Retributiveness and Death Penalty Support

Measures of DPS are much more common in survey data than are direct measures

of retributiveness, providing more opportunities to use secondary data analysis to study

the role of retributive dispositions in support for military punishment. The present survey

offers an opportunity to test the utility of this approach. To what extent does DPS reflect

retributive rather than security and humanitarian values?

To answer this question I measured DPS using three items:

1. Do you favor or oppose the death penalty for persons convicted of murder?

[strongly oppose, somewhat oppose, neutral, somewhat support, strongly

support]

2. If you could choose between the following two approaches, which do you

think is the better penalty for murder, the death penalty or life imprisonment,

with absolutely no possibility of parole? [strongly prefer death penalty,

Page 28: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

26

moderately prefer death penalty, neutral, moderately prefer life imprisonment,

strongly prefer life imprisonment]

3. Uphold the death penalty for the most serious crimes. [strongly agree, agree

somewhat, agree, neither agree nor disagree, somewhat disagree, disagree, or

strongly disagree]

These items were averaged and rescaled 0-1 so that higher values reflected greater DPS

(Alpha=0.77; M=0.47; SD=0.15).

I then regressed DPS on the political variables discussed above, Retributive, and

Schwartz’ personal value of Security, and a compound value using two PVQ items

measuring universalism and two measuring benevolence:

1. S/he thinks it is important that every person in the world should be treated

equally. S/he believes everyone should have equal opportunities in life.

2. It is important to him/her to listen to people who are different from her/him.

Even when s/he disagrees with them, she still wants to understand them.

3. It's very important to her/him to help the people around her/him. S/he wants to

care for their well being.

4. It is important to her/him to be loyal to her friends. S/he wants to devote

her/himself to people close to her/him.

These values are so closely related that they are often merged into higher-order

compound values, particularly when using the shortened version of the PVQ (see e.g.,

Davidov, Schmidt, et al. 2008). All four items were averaged and rescaled 0-1 so that

Page 29: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

27

higher values reflected greater universalism and benevolence, or Benevolent for short

(Alpha=0.67; M=0.82; SD=0.14).14

As Table IV shows, Retributive was by far the strongest predictor of DPS. This

suggests that the retributive nature of DPS–rather than its basis in insecurity or tough-

mindedness–accounts for its ability to predict support for punitive military action, as

found in other research (Liberman 2006, 2007).

Conclusion

To sum up these findings, differences in individuals’ belief in retributive

punishment predicted their level of support for punishing transgressor states and their

support for coercively interrogating enemy detainees. These relationships controlled for

measures of partisanship, ideology, humanitarianism, security concerns, and beliefs in the

efficacy of military force or of coercive interrogation.

The controls on humanitarianism and hawkish beliefs and values should have

removed most of their effects from the Retributive/Punishment and Retributive/Torture

associations. Undoubtedly, there remains room for improvement in these measures. But

the coalescence of diverse security beliefs and concerns along a single hawk/dove

dimension suggests that additional measures might also fall on the same dimension. If so,

they would not have accounted for the estimated effect of Retributive.

These results are consistent with prior findings on retributive motivation for 14 One other PVQ value, the compound Power/Achievement, had a significant bivariate

correlation with DPS. But it was not a significant predictor at the p<.05 level after

controlling for gender, race, Security, and Benevolent, or when added to model IVE in

Table IV, nor did adding this variable affect the coefficient of Retributive.

Page 30: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

28

international punishment and torture. The substantial relationship of retributiveness to

death penalty support lends support to inferences about retributive dispositions from

studies analyzing DPS/war support correlations (e.g., Liberman 2006, 2007). In other

words, it seems warranted to infer that death penalty supporters are disproportionately

bellicose toward rogue states because of their “eye for an eye” values, rather than their

humanitarianism or their belief in strategic punishment. Insofar as the convenience

sample analyzed here resembles the U.S. population, scholars lacking more direct

measures would be justified in using DPS as a proxy for retributiveness in future

secondary data analyses. The findings on coercive interrogation are also consistent with

prior findings that laypeople endorse torturing terrorists to give them their just deserts

rather than merely to extract vital information (Carlsmith and Sood 2009). The different

method of capturing retributive motivations used here–individual differences in

retributive values rather than situational differences in deservingness–lends added weight

to this conclusion.

More broadly, this study contributes to the accumulating evidence that values and

morality shape political attitudes (for further discussion, see Schwartz et al. 2010; Skitka

2010). It also points to the potential fruitfulness of looking beyond systems of universal

values, such as the Schwartz basic personal values inventory, to other values that might

shape judgment about particular value-laden personal, societal, and international issues.

This is not to argue that retributiveness accounts for broad structures of public foreign

and domestic policy attitudes. The ethic of desert may extend beyond criminal and

international punishment, but it applies mainly to the punishment of wrongdoing. Future

work ought to focus on the explanatory horizons of retributiveness.

Page 31: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

29

The findings presented here also point to limits of the rationalist-materialist model

of public opinion. Retributive punishment may well be rational in a broader sense that

encompasses deontological moral as well as material ends. While many philosophers

disagree with the legitimacy of retributive justice, it has had staunch and trenchant

defenders from Immanuel Kant to the present (e.g., French 2001). But rational

materialism has difficulty explaining punishment as an end in itself, especially when

punishment is costly to impose.

To be sure, none of the scenarios used here to measure punishment specifically

ruled out threats posed to the United States or the potential utility of punishment.

Moreover, rational materialism can explain why retributive values explained less

variance in attitudes about the costly enterprise of punishing states than about punishing

helpless detainees. However, had utility been the only motive for punishment, one would

have expected that people’s general beliefs about the efficacy and importance of force

would have accounted more fully for the relationship between retributive values and

support for both coercive interrogation and for international punishment. Such work

should also strive to measure Retributiveness in ways that tap the motive of punishing

transgressors because they deserve it, insofar as formulations emphasizing

proportionality might reflect an unstated strategic rule of thumb rather than an end in

itself.

The support for interstate punitiveness observed in this study was unlikely to have

been shaped by elite-led opinions, due to the hypothetical nature of the scenarios and the

lack of elite polarization, at least in recent years, over them (on opinion leadership effects

on war support, see Berinsky 2007; Zaller 1992, 1994). Coercive interrogation has been a

Page 32: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

30

more partisan issue, with contemporaneous polls showing support from two-thirds of

Republicans but only one-third of Democrats, a gap reflected in data analyzed here too.

The apparent effect of retributiveness on both sorts of attitudes suggests that elite

leadership is not a necessary condition. But political communication that frames conflicts

in morally black-and-white terms, punctuated by graphic descriptions of enemy atrocities,

is likely to greatly amplify such effects. An obvious lesson for political and media elites

that those seeking to build public support for war or coercive interrogation can do so by

recounting heinous, unpunished crimes, while those favoring restraint should instead

acknowledge offending states’ legitimate grievances and trumpet successful punishments.

Parallel lessons can be drawn, though with still more serious caveats, about

foreign policy making and international politics. To the extent that decision makers think

about international punishment like the study sample here, or to the extent that they feel

compelled to pander to such audiences, desires for retribution might actually shape actual

international conflicts (Harkavy 2000; Lebow 2008; Lebow 2010; Löwenheim &

Heimann 2008; Scheff 1994). If so, that would underline the importance of avoiding

gratuitous international injury and insult, as well as the value of international apologies in

deflating international animosities (David & Choi 2009; Lind 2009). However, there are

good reasons to expect that most leaders think about foreign policy in less emotional and

moralistic terms than do average citizens. There is also much evidence that leaders have

considerable autonomy from public pressure to go to war (Foyle 1999; Jacobs & Shapiro

2000; Sobel 2001). Much work remains to be done to investigate not only the psychology

of international punishment, but also how it might affect actual foreign policies and

conflicts.

Page 33: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

31

Bibliography

Page 34: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

32

Page 35: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

33

Abramowitz, A., and K. Saunders. 2008. "Is Polarization a Myth?" Journal of Politics 70 (02):542-55.

Alter, A., J. Kernochan, and J. Darley. 2007. "Transgression wrongfulness outweighs its harmfulness as a determinant of sentence severity." Law and Human Behavior 31

(4):319-35. Argo, N. 2009. "Why Fight?: Examining Self-Interested Versus Communally-Oriented

Motivations in Palestinian Resistance and Rebellion." Security Studies 18 (4):651-80.

Axelrod, R. 1984. The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Basic Books. Barber, L., J. Maltby, and A. Macaskill. 2005. "Angry memories and thoughts of

revenge: The relationship between forgiveness and anger rumination." Personality and Individual Differences 39:253-62.

Ben-Porath, E.N. 2007. "Rhetoric of Atrocities: The Place of Horrific Human Rights Abuses in Presidential Persuasion Efforts." Presidential Studies Quarterly 37

(2):181-202. Berinsky, A.J. 2007. "America at War: Public Opinion During Wartime, From World

War II to Iraq." Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Bohm, R.M. 1992. "Retribution and capital punishment: Toward a better understanding

of death penalty opinion." Journal of Criminal Justice 20:227-36. Bush, G.W. 2010. Decision Points. New York: Crown.

Carlsmith, K.M. 2008. "On Justifying Punishment: The Discrepancy Between Words and Actions." Social Justice Research 21 (2):119-37.

Carlsmith, K.M., and J.M. Darley. 2008. "Psychological Aspects of Retributive Justice." Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 41:193-236.

Carlsmith, K.M., J.M. Darley, and P.H. Robinson. 2002. "Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment." Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology 83 (2):284-99. Carlsmith, K.M., and A.M. Sood. 2009. "The fine line between interrogation and

retribution." Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 (1):191-96. Cohrs, J., B. Moschner, J. Maes, and S. Kielmann. 2005. "The motivational bases of

right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation: Relations to values and attitudes in the aftermath of September 11, 2001." Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin 31 (10):1425. Cohrs, J.C., B. Moschner, J. Maes, and S. Kielmann. 2005. "Personal Values and

Attitudes Toward War." Peace and Conflict 11 (3):293-312. Cota-McKinley, A.L., W.D. Woody, and P.A. Bell. 2001. "Vengeance: Effects of

Gender, Age, and Religious Background." Aggressive Behavior 27 (5):343-50. Danner, M. 2007. "The Moment Has Come to Get Rid of Saddam." New York Review of

Books:59-61. David, R., and S.Y.P. Choi. 2009. "Getting Even or Getting Equal? Retributive Desires

and Transitional Justice." Political Psychology 30 (2):161-92. Davidov, E., B. Meuleman, J. Billiet, and P. Schmidt. 2008. "Values and support for

immigration: a cross-country comparison." European Sociological Review 24 (5):583.

Page 36: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

34

Davidov, E., P. Schmidt, and S. Schwartz. 2008. "Bringing values back in: The adequacy of the European Social Survey to measure values in 20 countries." Public Opinion

Quarterly 72 (3):420. Doty, R.M., D.G. Winter, B.E. Peterson, and M. Kemmelmeier. 1997. "Authoritarianism

and American Students' Attitudes About the Gulf War, 1990-1996." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23 (11):1133-43.

Douglas, K.S., D.R. Lyon, and J.R.P. Ogloff. 1997. "The Impact of Graphic Photographic Evidence on Mock Jurors' Decisions in a Murder Trial: Probative or Prejudicial?"

Law and Human Behavior 21 (5):485-501. Douglas, S., and M. Martinko. 2001. "Exploring the role of individual differences in the

prediction of workplace aggression." Journal of Applied Psychology 86 (4):547-59.

Eichenberg, R.C. 2005. "Victory Has Many Friends: U.S. Public Opinion and the Use of Military Force, 1981-2005." International Security 30 (1):140-77.

Eisenberger, R., P. Lynch, J. Aselage, and S. Rohdieck. 2004. "Who Takes the most Revenge? Individual Differences in Negative Reciprocity Norm Endorsement."

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30 (6):787-99. Ellsworth, P.C., and S.R. Gross. 1994. "Hardening of the attitudes: Americans' views on

the death penalty." Journal of Social Issues 50 (2):19-52. Ellsworth, P.C., and L. Ross. 1983. "Public opinion and capital punishment: A close

examination of the views of abolitionists and retentionists." Crime and Delinquency (116):116-69.

Feldman, S. 1988. "Structure and consistency in public opinion: The role of core beliefs and values." American Journal of Political Science 32:416-40.

———. 2003. "Values, Ideology, and the Structure of Political Attitudes." In Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, ed. D. O. Sears, L. Huddy and R. Jervis. New

York: Oxford University Press. Finckenauer, J.O. 1988. "Public Support for the Death Penalty: Retribution As Just

Deserts or Retribution As Revenge?" Justice Quarterly 5:81-100. Foyle, D. 1999. Counting the Public In: Presidents, Public Opinion, and Foreign Policy.

New York: Columbia. French, P.A. 2001. The Virtues of Vengeance. Lawrence: University Press of Kansas. Gelpi, C., P. Feaver, and J. Reifler. 2009. Paying the human costs of war: American

public opinion and casualties in military conflicts. Princeton: Princeton Univ Press.

Gintis, H., S. Bowles, R. Boyd, and E. Fehr, eds. 2005. Moral Sentiments and Material Interests: The Foundations of Cooperation in Economic Life. Cambridge, Mass.:

MIT Press. Glaser, C.L. 2010. Theory of Rational International Politics. Princeton: Princeton

University Press. Gordijn, E.H., D. Wigboldus, and C. Taber. 2001. "Emotional Consequences of

Categorizing Victims of Negative Outgroup Behavior as Ingroup or Outgroup." Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 4 (4):317-26.

Gordijn, E.H., V. Yzerbyt, D. Wigboldus, and M. Dumont. 2006. "Emotional Reactions to Harmful Intergroup Behavior." European Journal of Social Psychology 36:15-

30.

Page 37: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

35

Goren, P. 2004. "Political Sophistication and Policy Reasoning: A Reconsideration." American Journal of Political Science 48 (3):462ff.

———. 2005. "Party identification and core political values." American Journal of Political Science 49 (4):881-96.

Gross, S.R., and P.C. Ellsworth. 2003. "Second Thoughts: Americans' Views on the Death Penalty at the Turn of the Century." In Capital Punishment and the American Future, ed. S. P. Garvey. Durham, N.C.: Duke University Press.

Haidt, J. 2001. "The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment." Psychological Review 108 (4):814-34.

Harkavy, R.E. 2000. "Defeat, National Humiliation, and the Revenge Motif in International Politics." International Politics 37 (3):345-68.

Herrmann, R.K., and V.P. Shannon. 2001. "Defending International Norms: The Role of Obligation, Material Interest, and Perception in Decision Making." International

Organization 55 (3):621-54. Huddy, L., S. Feldman, and E. Cassese. 2007. "On The Distinct Political Effects of

Anxiety and Anger." In The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior, ed. W. R. Neuman, G. E. Marcus, A. Crigler and M.

MacKuen. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Hurwitz, J., and M. Peffley. 1987. "How Are Foreign Policy Attitudes Structured? A

Hierarchical Model." American Political Science Review 81 (4):1099–120. Jacobs, L.R., and Robert Y. Shapiro. 2000. Politicians Don't Pander: Political

Manipulation and the Loss of Democratic Responsiveness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Jentleson, B. 1992. "The Pretty Prudent Public: Post-Vietnam American Opinion on the Use of Force." International Studies Quarterly 36 (1):49-74.

Jentleson, B., and R.L. Britton. 1998. "Still Pretty Prudent: Post-Cold War American Public Opinion on the Use of Military Force." Journal of Conflict Resolution 42

(4):395-417. Jost, J., C. Federico, and J. Napier. 2009. "Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and

elective affinities." Annual Review of Psychology 60:307-37. Knoppen, D., and W. Saris. 2009. Do we have to combine Values in the Schwartz¥

Human Values Scale? A Comment on the Davidov Studies. Lambert, A., L. Scherer, J. Schott, K. Olson, R. Andrews, T. O'Brien, and A. Zisser.

2010. "Rally effects, threat, and attitude change: An integrative approach to understanding the role of emotion." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

98 (6):886-903. Lebow, R.N. 2008. A cultural theory of international relations. New York: Cambridge

University Press. Lebow, Richard N. 2010. Why Nations Fight: Past and Future Motives for War. New

York: Cambridge University Press. Lewis-Beck, M., H. Norpoth, and W. Jacoby. 2008. The American voter revisited. Ann

Arbor: Univ of Michigan Press. Liberman, P. 2006. "An Eye for an Eye: Public Support for War against Evildoers."

International Organization 60 (3):687-722. ———. 2007. "Punitiveness and U.S. Elite Support for the 1991 Persian Gulf War."

Journal of Conflict Resolution 51 (1):3-32.

Page 38: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

36

Liberman, P., and L.J. Skitka. 2011. "All the Bad Guys in a Big Bucket: Retribution and Displaced Aggression in U.S. Public Support for the Iraq War." under review.

Liberman, P., and L.J. Skitka. 2011. "The War on Terror as Revenge for 9/11." under review.

Lind, J. 2009. Sorry States: Apologies in International Politics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Lord, C.G., L. Ross, and M.R. Lepper. 1979. "Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered

Evidence." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (3):2098-109. Löwenheim, O., and G. Heimann. 2008. "Revenge in International Politics." Security

Studies 17 (4):685-724. Mackie, D., E. Smith, and D. Ray. 2008. "Intergroup Emotions and Intergroup

Relations." Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (5):1866-80. Mackie, D.M., T. Devos, and E.R. Smith. 2000. "Intergroup emotions: Explaining

offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (4):602-16.

Maitner, A.T., D.M. Mackie, and E.R. Smith. 2006. "Evidence for the Regulatory Function of Intergroup Emotion: Emotional Consequences of Implemented or

Impeded Intergroup Action Tendencies." Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 42:720-8.

McCullough, M.E., C.G. Bellah, S.D. Kilpatrick, and J.L. Johnson. 2001. "Vengefulness: Relationships with Forgiveness, Rumination, Well-Being, and the Big Five."

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27 (5):601. McFarland, S.G. 2005. "On the Eve of War: Authoritarianism, Social Dominance, and

American Students' Attitudes Toward Attacking Iraq." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31 (3):360-67.

McKee, I.R., and N.T. Feather. 2008. "Revenge, Retribution, and Values: Social Attitudes and Punitive Sentencing." Social Justice Research 21 (2):138-63.

Moerk, E.L. 2002. "Scripting War-Entry to Make It Appear Unavoidable." Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology 8 (3):229-48.

Ogloff, J.R.P., and N. Vidmar. 1994. "The Impact of Pretrial Publicity on Jurors." Law and Human Behavior 18 (5):507-26.

Okimoto, T.G., M. Wenzel, and N.T. Feather. in press. "Retribution and Restoration as General Orientations Toward Justice." European Journal of Personality.

Pagano, S.J., and Y.J. Huo. 2007. "The Role of Moral Emotions in Predicting Support for Political Actions in Post-War Iraq." Political Psychology 28 (2):227-55.

Page, Benjamin I., and Robert Y. Shapiro. 1992. The Rational Public: Fifty Years of Trends in Americans’ Policy Preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Pew Research Center for the People & the Press. 2009. "America's Place in the World." Washington, DC: Pew Research Center for the People & the Press.

Pratto, F., L.M. Stallworth, and S. Conway-Lanz. 1998. "Social dominance orientation and the ideological legitimization of social policy." Journal of Applied Social

Psychology 28 (20):1853-75. Rokeach, M. 1973. The Nature of Human Values. New York: Free Press.

Page 39: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

37

Sadler, M.S., M. Lineberger, J. Correll, and B. Park. 2005. "Emotions, Attributions, and Policy Endorsement in Response to the September 11th Terrorist Attacks." Basic

and Applied Social Psychology 27 (3):249-58. Scheff, T. 1994. Bloody Revenge. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Schoen, H. 2009. "Basic Values and Attitudes toward Foreign and Domestic Policies in Germany." Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political

Science Association in Toronto. Schwartz, S., G. Caprara, and M. Vecchione. 2010. "Basic Personal Values, Core

Political Values, and Voting: A Longitudinal Analysis." Political Psychology 31 (3):421-52.

Skitka, L. 2010. "The psychology of moral conviction." Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4 (4):267-81.

Skitka, L.J., C.W. Bauman, N.P. Aramovich, and G.S. Morgan. 2006. "Confrontational and preventative policy responses to terrorism: Anger wants a fight and fear wants

"them" to go away." Basic and Applied Social Psychology 28 (4):375-84. Small, D.A., and G. Loewenstein. 2005. "The Devil You Know: The Effects of

Identifiability on Punitiveness." Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 18 (5):311-18.

Sobel, R. 2001. The Impact of Public Opinion on U.S. Foreign Policy Since Vietnam. New York: Oxford.

Spini, D., and W. Doise. 1998. "Organizing principles of involvement in human rights and their social anchoring in value priorities." European Journal of Social

Psychology 28 (4):603-22. Steenbergen, M.R. 1995. "Compassion and American Public Opinion: An Analysis of the

NES Humanitarianism Scale." Tyler, T.R., and R. Weber. 1982. "Support for the Death Penalty: Instrumental Response

to Crime or Symbolic Attitude?" Law & Society Review 17 (1):121-45. Valentino, N., K. Gregorowicz, and E. Groenendyk. 2009. "Efficacy, Emotions and the

Habit of Participation." Political Behavior 31 (3):307-30. Vidmar, N. 1974. "Retributive and Utilitarian Motives and Other Correlates of Canadian

Attitudes toward the Death Penalty." Canadian Psychologist 15 (4):337-56. Warr, M., and M. Stafford. 1984. "Public Goals of Punishment and Support for the Death

Penalty." Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 21 (2):95-111. Wilson, T. 2002. Strangers to ourselves: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Yzerbyt, V., M. Dumont, D. Wigboldus, and E. Gordijn. 2003. "I feel for us: The impact of categorization and identification on emotions and action tendencies." British

Journal of Social Psychology 42 (4):533-49. Zaller, J.R. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press. ———. 1994. "Elite Leadership of Mass Opinion: New Evidence from the Gulf War." In

Taken by Storm: The Media, Public Opinion, and U.S. Foreign Policy in the Gulf War, ed. W. L. Bennett and D. L. Paletz. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Page 40: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

38

Table I. Support for International Punishment

IA b/(se)

IB b/(se)

IC b/(se)

ID b/(se)

IE b/(se)

Male 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) Black 0.00 -0.00 -0.01 -0.01 (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) Republican 0.05* 0.04 0.05† 0.04 (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) Conservative -0.01 -0.02 -0.02 -0.02 (0.03) (0.03) (0.03) (0.03) RWA 0.04 0.04 -0.06 -0.04 (0.06) (0.06) (0.06) (0.06) Hawk 0.18*** 0.16*** (0.04) (0.04) Bombing 0.06* 0.06* (0.02) (0.02) Humanitarian 0.12*** 0.13*** (0.03) (0.03) Security 0.11*** 0.10*** (0.03) (0.03) Retributive 0.17*** 0.16*** 0.11*** (0.03) (0.03) (0.03) Constant 0.44*** 0.51*** 0.43*** 0.26*** 0.23*** (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) (0.04) (0.04) R2 0.03 0.01 0.04 0.09 0.10 N 898 892 889 888 885

Note: The figures in the table are unstandardized, ordinary-least-squares (OLS)

regression coefficients, with standard errors in parentheses. All variables range from 0-1.

Two-tailed significance levels are indicated by: †p<.10, * p<.05, ** p<.01, ***p<.001.

Page 41: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

39

Table II. Exploratory Factor Analysis of Foreign Policy Goals and Beliefs

Question Factor I Factor II Uniqueness

1. The US must maintain superior military power worldwide.

0.66 0.11 0.56

2. US strategic nuclear forces are a necessary part of national security strategy.

0.61 0.12 0.63

3. Defending the homeland is the most important government priority.

0.58 0.17 0.65

4. The US needs to allocate more resources to combating international terrorism abroad.

0.58 0.12 0.66

5. International trade agreements should protect indigenous labor and the global environment.

0.05 0.56 0.69

6. International organizations should help Less Developed Countries find collective solutions to their economic problems.

0.04 0.64 0.60

7. US foreign aid to less developed countries is important.

0.10 0.65 0.57

8. Reducing world hunger is an urgent problem. 0.01 0.64 0.60

9. It is sometimes useful to go to war against a rogue state just to deter others from challenging the status quo.

0.56 -0.19 0.65

10. War is the only way to stop aggressive states from challenging our interests.

0.54 -0.32 0.60

11. Bombing enemy states rarely convinces them to change their policies.

-0.09 0.33 0.88

12. Getting information from suspected terrorists could save many innocent people's lives. It would be immoral to limit ourselves from using whatever methods may be useful for getting that information.

-0.28 0.13 0.90

13. If the US makes exceptions to international laws against torture and abuse, other countries and groups will feel freer to make exceptions themselves, thus making it more likely that detained Americans will be tortured or abused.

0.09 -0.24 0.94

Note: The figures in the first two columns are obliquely rotated factor loadings. The response options for items 1-11 were: strongly agree, agree somewhat, agree, neither agree nor disagree, somewhat disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree. Those for items 12-13 were: very unconvincing, unconvincing, somewhat unconvincing, neither unconvincing nor convincing, somewhat convincing, convincing, or very convincing.

Page 42: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

40

Table III. Support for Coercive Interrogation

IIIA b/(se)

IIIB b/(se)

IIIC b/(se)

IIID b/(se)

IIIE b/(se)

Male 0.07*** 0.06*** 0.05*** 0.05*** (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) Black 0.03† 0.02 0.02 0.01 (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) Republican 0.12*** 0.08** 0.09** 0.06* (0.03) (0.03) (0.03) (0.03) Conservative 0.11** 0.09* 0.03 0.02 (0.04) (0.04) (0.04) (0.03) RWA 0.23** 0.22** -0.06 -0.02 (0.08) (0.08) (0.08) (0.08) Hawk 0.37*** 0.30*** (0.06) (0.05) Savelives 0.21*** 0.18*** (0.03) (0.03) Reciprocity -0.04 -0.07* (0.03) (0.03) Humanitarian -0.25*** -0.24*** (0.04) (0.04) Security -0.02 -0.04 (0.04) (0.04) Retributive 0.44*** 0.38*** 0.31*** (0.04) (0.04) (0.04) Constant 0.19*** 0.25*** 0.06† 0.28*** 0.19*** (0.02) (0.02) (0.03) (0.05) (0.05) R2 0.11 0.10 0.18 0.24 0.29 N 899 892 889 883 880

Note: The figures in the table are unstandardized, OLS regression coefficients, with

standard errors in parentheses. All variables range from 0-1. Two-tailed significance

levels are indicated by: †p<.10, * p<.05, ** p<.01, ***p<.001.

Page 43: Retributive Support for War and Tortureqcpages.qc.cuny.edu/Political_Science/profmat/Retributiveness.JCR... · In addition to gaining a better understanding of the role of retributive

41

Table IV. Support for the Death Penalty

IVA b/(se)

IVB b/(se)

IVC b/(se)

IVD b/(se)

IVE b/(se)

Male 0.04*** 0.03*** 0.04*** 0.03*** (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) Black -0.03* -0.04*** -0.04** -0.04*** (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) (0.01) Republican 0.04* 0.01 0.04† 0.01 (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) Conservative 0.05* 0.02 0.03 0.01 (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) RWA 0.08 0.08† -0.02 0.02 (0.05) (0.04) (0.05) (0.05) Benevolent -0.19*** -0.14*** (0.04) (0.03) Security 0.15*** 0.09*** (0.03) (0.02) Retributive 0.41*** 0.40*** 0.38*** (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) Constant 0.23*** 0.41*** 0.21*** 0.49*** 0.30*** (0.01) (0.02) (0.02) (0.04) (0.03) R2 0.26 0.07 0.30 0.11 0.32 N 907 900 896 898 894

Note: The figures in the table are unstandardized, OLS regression coefficients, with

standard errors in parentheses. All variables range from 0-1. Two-tailed significance

levels are indicated by: †p<.10, * p<.05, ** p<.01, ***p<.001.