Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    1/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams

    KAMARAN FATHULLA

    Abstract

    The role and value of diagrams in advancing human knowledge throughout

    history is evident in the literature. However, persistent evidence from the re-

    search and practitioner communities from across a range of disciplines points

    to difculties and problems with understanding and using diagrams. Whether

    it is at the application level or as an abstract denition, our existing frame-

    works for understanding diagrams reduce the full meaning of diagrams to a

    single perspective or emphasis. This paper advocates placing multi-aspectual

    human functioning in a central role in the development of an enriched meta-

    approach to understanding diagrams. Two such aspects, namely, the spatial

    and symbolic, are essential to the understanding of diagrams across applica-

    tions. The philosophical underpinnings and practical contributions of this ap-

    proach to dealing with long standing problems of diagrams are discussed.

    Keywords: symbolic (Sy); spatial (Sp); mapping (M); redundant Sp; main

    SySpM

    1. Introduction

    Understanding diagrams plays an important role in our sense making of the

    world and solving problems. Almost every discipline or study makes use of

    diagrams and other forms of visual aids (Novick and Hurley 2001; Vekiri

    2002; Pisan 1995). Ambrose et al. (1999) investigated difculties students

    have with physics diagrams and found that, although some of the difculty

    may be attributed to lack of understanding of subject matter, a signicant pro-

    portion of the problems were to do with how students interpret diagrammatic

    formalisms. A recent study by Ornek, Robinson, and Haugan (2008) attributes

    the difculties to the different graphical representations students have to dealwith, and, furthermore, the need to convert from one representation to another.

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    2/16

    124 K. Fathulla

    Scaife and Rogers (1996) argue that, despite the vast body of empirical studies

    in the eld, little is known about why and how graphics facilitate learning. The

    authors found many shortcomings in empirical studies on graphical representa-

    tion, such as assumptions merely based on intuition, lack of integration oftheoretical concepts, and non-systematic analyses. Uesaka, Manalo, and Ichi-

    kawa (2007) argue that despite the uses of diagrams in solving math problems,

    research and educational practice reports indicate that students lack spontane-

    ity in their use of diagrams. Diezmann (2000) and Paquette et al. (2006) report

    that generating an appropriate diagram is problematic for many students. They

    explored primary-aged students difculties in generating diagrams for novel

    problems. Their results suggest that there is a need for a methodology in the

    use of diagrams. The specic areas of concern related to the ambiguity of dia-

    grams and their dynamic features. Ambiguity is often related to the potential of

    one spatial feature being used to express a number of different semantics. A

    report by The Royal Statistical Society expressed concern at the extremely

    poor quality of graphs and diagrams submitted in recent years by candidates at

    all levels of the Societys examinations (Oliver 1998). Anastopoulou, Sharples,

    and Baber (2003) point to dynamics of diagramming as yet another source of

    confusion and difculty for students. Problems with understanding UML dia-

    grams have been discussed in Evans (1998) and Li, Hosking, and Grundy (2007).

    2. Existingwaysofunderstandingdiagrams

    Two inuential approaches dominate the underlying thinking behind many of

    the existing frameworks of understanding diagrams, and are summarized in

    Table 1.

    These two approaches have inuenced many lines of inquiry into the area of

    diagrams and their understanding in a fundamental way. They polarize the

    nature of the topic. Examples of these poles include internal versus external

    debate, abstract versus application, etc. Whatever the nature of the poles may

    be, the overriding assumptions are that diagrams are understood in terms ofone single perspective, as shown in Table 2.

    There is emerging dissatisfaction with the potential of these ways of under-

    standing diagrams (Norman 2000; Kupla 2003; see Bresciani and Eppler in a

    Table 1. Inuential frameworks of understanding diagrams

    Source Emphasis

    Peirce (CP), semiotics What we do with diagramsBertin (1983), semiology of graphics Structure of diagrams

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    3/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 125

    managerial context). Bishop (1994) argues that the centuries-held assumption

    that a drawing is a drawing is a drawing is progressively shown to be invalid.Horn (2001) claims that our current ways of understanding diagrams is one of

    confusion.

    This paper responds to these calls and advocates a rethinking of our under-

    standing of diagrams and proposes a way that runs parallelwith rather than

    opposing existing frameworks, but has distinctly different philosophically-

    guided underpinnings.

    3. Towardsarethinking

    The main feature of existing ways of understanding diagrams is to either de-

    tach the diagram from its human creator and treat it in purely spatial or graph-

    ical terms or reduce the diagram to a property of human cognition. This later

    approach ignores the spatial aspect of diagrams. However, there are many

    examples that call for the need to think of diagrams in a more integrated way,

    i.e., combining spatial and cognitive aspects.

    A number of quotes from practitioners in the eld illustrate the point. The

    way people use diagrams, irrespective of the application, has been eloquentlydescribed by J. D. Watson, co-discoverer of the structure of DNA and Nobel

    prize winner in 1968: drawing and thinking are frequently so simultaneous

    that the graphic image appears almost an organic extension of the thinking

    process. In Gombrichs discussion of Leonardos creative process, he sug-

    gested that: in searching for a new solution Leonardo projected new mean-

    ings into the forms he saw in his old discarded Sketches (1966). Similarly, the

    famous American architect Daniel H. Burnham says: . . . a noble, logical dia-

    gram once recorded will not die, but long after we are gone be a living thing,

    asserting itself with ever-growing insistence (quoted in Moore 1921). Simi-larly, Braham (2000) talks about the suffering of diagrams in the context of

    Table 2. Poles of the debate

    View of diagrams Nature of reduction

    Internal

    (Human centered)

    Cognitive, or

    Sensory (visual), or

    Psychological, or

    Linguistic

    External/Abstract Spatial, or

    Graphical

    Application This can be any single perspective depending on the nature of the

    application

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    4/16

    126 K. Fathulla

    architecture. This characteristic of diagrams calls for the need for a more

    human element in our understanding of diagrams.

    The message conveyed by these quotes highlights a softer, human friendly,

    side to diagrams as well as their traditional spatial characteristics. It is essentialthat both sides are treated equally and simultaneously for the appreciation and

    a good understanding of the full meaning and purpose of diagrams. This type

    of understanding requires a radical shift in traditional approaches that are

    mainly reductionist in nature and assume a single perspective of what consti-

    tutes diagrams. This paper presents one such non-reductionist approach, the

    foundations of which are centered on the notion of Aspects.

    4. Aspect(s)ledrethink

    The dening nature of this approach is underpinned in the notion of aspect,

    which is part of Herman Dooyeweerds (1955) philosophical framework. An

    aspect is a sphere or modality of meaning within which things, including

    diagrams, are meaningful. Aspectual philosophy stipulates that nothing can be

    of one aspect alone. This central element of the philosophy forms the corner-

    stone from which, in this paper, a proposal for a better understanding of dia-

    grams is outlined.

    Each aspect has a distinct set of laws that guide and enable functioning.

    All 15 aspects, shown in Table 3, together constitute a framework which

    Dooyeweerd called the law side of created reality. This is a framework that

    enables everything to exist and be. The aspects are what explain the diversity

    and coherence of everyday experience. Therefore, each aspect provides us

    with a distinct way in which things have become meaningful or make sense.

    The philosophical foundations of the notion of aspecthave a number of

    distinct features that will inform the vision pursued in this paper:

    emphasis on (human) functioning rather than on entities: We function

    according to laws that are both determinative and normative. irreducibility of aspects: Aspects are fundamentally irreducible. This

    means that no aspect can be derived from another, and that each aspect

    must be given proper respect in a situation.

    sphere universality : that these aspects, though irreducible, are nevertheless

    closely intertwined, such that in each there are echoes of each of the others.

    dependency among aspects: The aspects form a sequence, in which the

    laws of an aspect depend on those of earlier aspects for their proper func-

    tioning, even though they may not be reduced to them.

    the notion of a qualifying aspect: That (almost) all human activities, and(almost) all entities are qualied by one aspect, even though the function-

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    5/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 127

    ing involved in the activity is in fact multi-aspectual. It is the qualifying

    aspect that gives an activity or entity its primary meaning, and also pro-

    vides the most useful criterion for evaluating whether the activity or entity

    is good or impaired.

    The notion of enkapsis: What we experience as an entity is often, in fact,

    an enkaptic intertwinement of several distinct entities, each of which is

    qualied by a different aspect. Enkapsis speaks of what individuality struc-

    tures are necessary to the proper understanding of an enkaptic structural

    whole, rather than what individuality structures could be part of it in var-ious circumstances.

    diagrams and aspects: The notion of qualifying aspects allows us to distin-

    guish diagrams from other types of graphical material, such as paintings or

    decoration. Both diagrams and paintings are founded in the spatial aspect

    (although one might argue that, where color is important, the sensitive as-

    pect is also important). But diagrams are qualied by the lingual aspect of

    symbolic meaning and communication, while paintings and decoration, on

    the other hand, are often qualied by the aesthetic aspect.

    The notions ofsphere universality and irreducibility of aspects also inu-ence the proposed approach because it means that the spatial and lingual

    Table 3. The fteen aspects and diagrams

    Aspect Relevance to diagrams

    Numeric Concerned with the number of shapes, etc.

    Spatial Concerned with things like shapes in the diagram, spatial arrangement,

    connectivity, etc.

    Kinematic This concerns the activity of diagramming itself when conveying symbolic

    meaning

    Physical This concerns the specic physical medium or substrate used to display the

    diagram

    Biotic This aspect concerns life functions that are a necessary precondition for the

    sensory and nervous systems to function well enough for diagram creation or

    reading.

    Sensitive This concerns perceiving color, texture, etc., and also emotions

    Analytic This aspect concerns the activity of distinguishing what is meaningful in a diagram

    from its background

    Formative The laws of this aspect govern the creation or formation of the symbol structure

    being expressed, and also of the method by which a drawing is composed

    Symbolic This concerns conveyance of meaning

    Social This concerns social impact and norms used when diagramming

    Economic This concerns the cost of producing the diagram

    Aesthetic This concerns how well a diagram is presented

    Juridical This concerns issues such as copyright

    Ethical Diagrams should not be offensive

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    6/16

    128 K. Fathulla

    aspects of a diagram must be considered separately and yet in relation to each

    other. In this context, separately means:

    Phenomena of each aspect must be recognized in terms of the aspect and

    not subsumed by the other.

    Appropriate recognition must be given to the distinct laws of each aspect.

    The terminology used for phenomena must, as far as possible, avoid meta-

    phor and be that which is appropriate to the aspect. For example, in a box

    and arrows diagram, we should use the term item when speaking from

    the point of view of the lingual individuality structure and the term box

    (or, better, rectangle although it is often a different shape) when speak-

    ing from the point of view of the spatial individuality structure.

    Although all fteen aspects are important when discussing diagrams, neverthe-less, some of them only come to the forefront only when diagrams are in use

    through some human application. However, only two aspects pertain to dia-

    grams in themselves regardless of use or context: the symbolic and spatial.

    These two aspects are brought together through the notion of enkapsis.

    Enkapsis provides precision in our understanding of the nature of diagrams.

    Namely, a diagram, as an enkaptic structural whole, comprises two individual-

    ity structures, both of which are necessary for a proper understanding of a dia-

    gram as a:

    Spatially qualied structure, which is subject to the laws of the spatial

    aspect, and in which we recognize spatial shapes, arrangements, and

    relationships;

    Symbolically qualied structure, which is subject to the laws of the lingual

    aspect, and in which we recognize symbolic objects such as items, relation-

    ships, numbers, etc., and the relationships between them, and we must take

    account of their use for purposes of symbolic meaning, which includes

    both communication and stimulation of thinking.

    The identication of these two aspects is not made on an ad hoc basis but ratheraccording to principled reasoning:

    1. They conform to our intuitive understanding of diagrams, i.e., spatial

    marks used to convey some meaning.

    2. They resonate with what we discussed in the above mentioned quotes. The

    symbolic/lingual aspect accommodates the human friendly or soft ele-

    ments of diagrams while the spatial aspect covers the traditional view of

    diagrams.

    3. Their selection is philosophically underpinned.

    In the following section, the integration of these two aspects is elaborated.

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    7/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 129

    5. TheSySpMframework

    Full discussion of this framework and its contributions is beyond the scope of

    this paper and can be found in Fathulla (2006). Here we briey introduce themost important elements of the framework. The central thrust of this frame-

    work is: separating Symbolic from Spatial but allowing for their Mapping

    (SySpM). The framework is based on the notion that Sy is distinct and separate

    from Sp and is irreducible to it. From this framework individual SySpMs

    could be constructed. The term SySpM is used to denote a distinct (particular)

    collection of Sy and a distinct collection of Sp and a distinct Mapping between

    the two. Synonymous with this term are drawing styles or types of diagrams.

    We grouped the Sy and Sp terms into primary and secondary lists. The former

    includes Sy and Sp terms that are used in the simplest or basic form of the

    SySpM. This category of Sy and Sp is also in all instances of diagrams of the

    relevant SySpM. The latter includes terms that are used in complex diagrams

    of the relevant SySpM and it also includes things that have to do with the rela-

    tionships or interaction between the various Sy things of the relevant SySpM.

    Development of each SySpM also includes list of Sp things that do not map

    onto any Sy known as redundant Sp things. These could then become available

    for other SySpMs to use to give mixed diagrams, list of constraints and why

    they occur, and list of events or changes relevant to the SySpM under consid-

    eration. Special features of a SySpM use redundant Sp features to bring in

    secondary Sp features rather than another SySpM as mixed diagrams. An ex-

    ample of this in a box and arrows SySpM is when lines are allowed to cross

    other lines. This happens when one line is given a kink or a gap to indicate

    clearly that one is passing over or under the other rather than connecting to it,

    e.g., electronic circuits. Subtypes are diagrams in which the original and sim-

    ple SySpM is constrained/complicated because of specic needs usually asso-

    ciated with a type of application. This is achieved by bringing in an extra Sy

    constraint that, owing to Mapping, also gives a different Sp feel to the diagram.

    There are at least three different ways of Sy subtypes in a box and arrows

    SySpM: networks, lists, and trees. For each SySpM there might be specialcases that do not t well. Many spatial applications involve several of such

    special cases such as holes, discontinuities, and other irregularities. We need to

    identify these and explain the problems they give, that is, what constraints they

    break, either spatial (as here) or symbolic.

    A SySpM could contain features that are outside the range of its base sym-

    bolic types. This recognizes that each SySpM will be able to express only a

    subset of the symbol level, not all of it. To express the whole wide range of

    things at the Sy level requires several different SySpMs. Within this context,

    two types of mixedness are identied. One is when several SySpMs are pres-ent in a diagram but none dominates the overall meaning of the diagram. An

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    8/16

    130 K. Fathulla

    Table 4. Types of diagrams generated from the SySpM framework

    The SySpM Mapping

    Boxes and Arrows Item mapped onto box, relationship

    mapped onto arrow

    Communicating Similarity A collection of items mapped onto a

    collection of shapes

    Map of Objects Item location mapped onto icon

    position

    Set Membership A shape inside a loop mapped onto

    member of a set

    Bar Charts Magnitude mapped onto length of a

    bar

    Route Maps Route mapped onto curvilinear line

    Contour Maps The set of location with the same

    quantitative value mapped onto

    closed continuous curve

    Surface Coverage Region mapped onto area

    example of this is the Napoleons march on Moscow diagram shown below.

    This type of mixedness is referred to as True mixed diagrams. The other type

    is referred to as Augmented diagrams. This type of mixedness has one

    SySpM occupying a primary importance while other SySpMs are added in

    and have secondary importance. This type of mixedness occurs when redun-dant Sp features of a SySpM are used to bring in Sy from other SySpMs. For

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    9/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 131

    example, in a Box and Arrows SySpM thickness of lines could be used to bring

    in quantitative value from Bar Chart SySpM.

    6. Thegenerativecontributionoftheframework

    The SySpM framework is a way of understanding diagrams that can be used to

    generate types of diagrams. In Fathulla (2006), eight such types or SySpMs

    were developed namely:

    1. Boxes and Arrows

    2. Communicating Similarity

    3. Map of Objects

    4. Set Membership5. Bar Charts

    6. Route Maps

    7. Contour Maps

    8. Surface Coverage

    These SySpMs are not seen as a nal list but rather to demonstrate the poten-

    tial of SySpM as a framework in accounting for a diverse range of types of

    diagrams.

    7. ApplicationoftheSySpMs

    The various SySpMs have been used in the interpretation of four real world

    semantically rich diagrams. The full methodology and analysis is found in

    Fathulla (2006). Figure 1 shows how to apply the SySpMs to one of those

    diagrams.

    Table 5 shows an example of how the various SySpMs can be used in the

    understanding of Minards well-known and celebrated diagram of Napoloens

    march on Moscow (see Figure 2).The potential of using knowledge of the various SySpM to understand dia-

    grams goes beyond the task of interpretation. A number of useful insights can

    be acquired with this approach.

    It becomes possible to identify possible or potential errors or confusions

    with diagrams. For example, the thin vertical lines may be confused with splin-

    ter or diversions because they look visually similar. However, when considered

    from a Boxes and Arrows SySpM, these lines are seen as relationships between

    two objects: one is temperature value and the other is size of army.

    The small sideway kinks (edges), Figure 3, along the route suggest an errorof display and the need for smoothing out the edges. However, knowing that

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    10/16

    132 K. Fathulla

    Figure

    1.

    (topleft)Aquantied

    owchart;(topright)Pressure-vo

    lumegraph;(bottomleft)Charles

    JosephMinard(17811870),depicting

    thefateofNapoleonsarmysmarchonMoscow.Source:CollectionEcoledespontsaveclacoteENPCduoudesdocuments.(bottomright)

    Ordinancesurvey(OS)mapwith

    OSpermission.

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    11/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 133

    Table 5. SySpM analysis of the Napoleons march on Moscow (see Figure 1 [bottom left])

    Diagram

    feature

    Description SySpM analysis

    Sp SySpM Sy

    1 A long straight

    horizontal line

    is drawn across

    the entire length

    of the diagram

    symbolizing the

    division of the

    space.

    Line Surface Coverage Space divider

    2 A number of

    vertical lines

    Line Boxes and Arrows

    where boxes are

    nominal entities

    that are important

    values that should

    be linked

    Relationship

    3 The proximity of the

    end of the black

    line to the start

    of the textured

    line is to allow

    the comparison

    of two extremely

    different values.

    Proximity of

    position

    Bar Chart Comparison of

    quantitative

    values:

    422,000

    with 10,000

    4 Discontinuity of

    textured splinter

    line from its

    black segment

    Break or gap

    between the

    lines at the

    start of the

    campaign

    (use of empty

    space)

    Variant of Route

    Map

    Location

    5 Distances between

    lines joining the

    two spaces of

    the diagram

    Proximity Variant of Boxes

    and Arrows

    Extreme

    changes in

    values

    6 A pair of X and Y

    axis lines on the

    bottom surface

    of the diagram

    depicting

    temperature scales

    Length Bar Chart sub

    type in which the

    orientation of the

    bar chart is

    turned upside

    down

    Comparison of

    quantitative

    values

    7 The vertical lines

    linking the retreat

    path to the graph

    Lines

    Length of bars

    Boxes and Arrows

    where boxes are

    nominal entities

    that are important

    values that should

    be linked

    B Ch S S M

    Relationship

    Discrete value

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    12/16

    134 K. Fathulla

    the width of the line is Bar Chart SySpM explains the need for this feature to

    help the reader interpret the distinct values along the route.

    Towards the end of the campaign, the end of the advance route seems to

    have shifted its position suggesting that the army jumped its position south-

    ward, see Figure 4 (left panel). This hints at the possibility of an error in the

    diagram around Moscow. The apparent error is explained through Route MapSySpM, which allows lines to touch each other. However, this part of the

    Figure 2. Minards well-known and celebrated diagram of Napoloens march on Moscow

    Figure 3. A kink pointed to by an arrow

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    13/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 135

    diagram could have been improved with a better, more continuous bend

    around that region, see Figure 4 (right panel).

    The approach could also help with assessing the quality of diagrams. This

    particular diagram has a number of good quality features. The main message of

    this diagram is the disastrous fate of the army. Comparison of the thickness of

    the white and black line segments (Sp) on the left hand side of the diagram

    depicts the armys fate. The comparison of these two extreme magnitudes (Sy)

    is expressed using Bar Chart SySpM to show two extreme magnitudes.

    The good quality of the diagram also lies in the naturalness with which three

    or more SySpMs work together without confusion or ambiguity because they

    do so by:

    Judicious use of redundant Sp features of each SySpM namely Route Map

    and Bar Chart.

    Splitting the diagram into two areas with Route Map and Bar Chart in oneand XY Bar Chart in the other.

    Examples of this naturalness can be found in the effective linking of the upper

    route of the campaign and lower XY Bar Chart using Boxes and Arrows.

    Sp features are used redundantly in the same SySpM to bring in another

    SySpM. This is illustrated in using the width of a line to bring in the length of

    a bar in a Bar Chart SySpM to express size of the army and in using the width

    of a bar, which is a redundant Sp feature, to bring in a Route Map SySpM.

    8. Conclusion

    This paper is a response to the persistence with which the literature cites prob-

    lems with our understanding and using diagrams. Existing frameworks that

    have inuenced our understanding of diagrams for much of the time are prov-

    ing to be insufcient to deal with the range of diversity of diagrams and ap-

    plications that make use of them. A philosophically guided framework is pro-

    posed based on the notion ofaspects, which states that no entity is of a single

    aspect but rather all reality, including diagrams, function with a given set ofaspects. This philosophical approach was developed to work out a richer

    Figure 4. (left) The apparent jump; (right) a better rounded route

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    14/16

    136 K. Fathulla

    understanding of diagrams based on symbolic and spatial aspects. The pro-

    posed SySpM framework offers a richer vocabulary for discussing static and

    dynamic features of diagrams across a range of applications.

    References

    Ambrose, B. S., P. R. L. Heron, S. Vokos, & L. C. McDermott. 1999. Student understanding of

    common representations of light as an electromagnetic wave: Relating the formalism to physical

    phenomena.American Journal of Physics 67(10). 891.

    Anastopoulou, S., M. Sharples & C. Baber. 2003. Multimodality and learning: Linking science to

    everyday activities. In C. Staphanidis & J. Jacko (eds.), Proceedings of HCI 03 conference,

    Crete, June 2227, 576580. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Bertin, J. 1983. Semiology of graphics. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.

    Bishop, I. 1994. The role of visual realism in communicating and understanding spatial change and

    process. In A. M. MacEachren & D. R. Fraser Taylor (eds.), Visualization in modern cartogra-

    phy, 6064. Pergamon.

    Braham, W. 2000. After typology: The suffering of diagrams.Architectural Design 70(3). 911.

    Bresciani, S. & M. J. Eppler. 2007. Usability of diagrams for group knowledge work: Toward an

    analytic description. In Conference Proceedings I-KNOW 07, Graz, Austria, 416423.

    Diezmann, Carmel M. 2000. The difculties students experience in generating diagrams for novel

    problems. In T. Nakahara & M. Koyama (eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-fth annual con-

    ference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education, 241248.

    Hiroshima.

    Dooyeweered, H. 1955.A new critique of theoretical thought. Jordan Station: Paideia Press.

    Evans, A. 1998. Reasoning with UML class diagrams. In Proceedings from the second IEEE

    Workshop on Industrial strength Formal specication Techniques (WIFT98) 102113. Los

    Alamitos: IEEE Computer Society Press.

    Fathulla, K. 2006. Understanding diagrams based on symbolic and spatial mapping. Salford:

    University of Salford dissertation.

    Gombrich, E. H. 1966. Norm and form: Studies in the art of the Renaissance. Oxford: Phaidon

    Press.

    Horn, R. E. 2001. Visual language and converging technologies in the next 1015 years (and

    beyond). Paper presented at the National Science Foundation Conference on Converging

    Technologies (Nano-Bio-Info-Cogno) for Improving Human Performance, December 34.

    Kupla, Z. 2003.From picture processing to interval diagrams. Warsaw: IFTR PAS Reports.

    Li, L., John G. Hosking & John C. Grundy. 2007. EML: A tree overlay-based visual language for

    business process modelling. International Conference on Enterprise Information Systems 3.

    131137.

    Moore, C. 1921. Closing in 19111912. InDaniel H. Burnham,architect, planner of cities, vol. 2,

    1921. Boston: Houghton Mifin.

    Norman, J. 2000. Differentiating diagrams: A new approach. Lecture Notes in Computer Science

    1889. 105116.

    Novick, L. & M. Hurley. 2001. To matrix, network, or hierarchy: That is the question. Cognitive

    Psychology 42. 158216.

    Oliver, F. 1998. How to present information in graphs and diagrams. London: Royal Statistical

    Society.

    Ornek F. William, R. Robinson & Mark P. Haugan. 2008. What makes physics difcult? Interna-

    tional Journal of Environmental & Science Education 3(1). 13063065.

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    15/16

    Rethinking our understanding of diagrams 137

    Paquette, G., M. Lonard, K. Lundgren-Cayrol, S. Mihaila & D. Gareau. 2006. Learning design

    based on graphical knowledge-modeling.Educational Technology & Society 9(1). 97112.

    Peirce, Charles S. 19311966. The collected papers of Charles S. Peirce, 8 vols., C. Hartshorne,

    P. Weiss & A. W. Burks (eds.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Reference to Peirces

    papers will be designated CPfollowed by volume and paragraph number.]Pisan, Y. 1995. A visual routines based model of graph understanding. InProceeding of the seven-

    teenth annual conference of the cognitive science society, 692697. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum

    Associates.

    Scaife, Mike & Yvonne Rogers. 1996. External cognition: How do graphical representations

    work?International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 45(2). 185213.

    Uesaka, Y., E. Manalo & S. Ichikawa. 2007. What kinds of perceptions and daily learning be-

    haviors promote students use of diagrams in mathematics problem solving? Learning and

    Instruction 17. 322335.

    Vekiri, I. 2002. What is the value of graphical displays in learning? Educational Psychology

    Review 14(3). 261312.

    Kamaran Fathulla (b. 1961) is a senior lecturer at the University of Northampton . His research interests include understanding diagrams and philoso-

    phy. His publications include Frameworks for understanding diagrams: Enriching the debate

    (with A. Basden, 2007); Symbolic, spatial, mapping (SySpM): A framework for interpreting

    maps (with A. Basden, 2008); Understanding diagrams: A pointer to the development of

    diagramming software (2008); and A framework for understanding diagrams in education

    (with F. Hameed, 2009).

  • 8/3/2019 Rethinking Understanding FATHULLA

    16/16

    Copyright of Semiotica is the property of De Gruyter and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple

    sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,

    download, or email articles for individual use.