42
Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle Theory * Michele F. Margolis May 26, 2015 Abstract While social identities have long been shown to influence political attitudes and behaviors, this paper asks the reverse. Are political identities ever strong enough to influence identification and involvement with a deep-seated identity? Looking at an identity that has become an increasingly strong predictor of partisanship and vote choice, religion, I develop a theory that there is a time period in an individual’s life when a fully-formed political identity can have a meaningful effect on a weak religious identity. Using an experiment and two panel studies to test my theory, I find that when people are in the process of raising children–a time that pulls many back into the religious fold–their partisanship can change key aspects of their religious identity. Although many bemoan religion’s role in creating a polarized political arena, this blame may be unfairly assigned as partisans themselves help produce an increasingly polarized electorate. * For comments, suggestions, and feedback, I thank Adam Berinsky, Anthony Fowler, Gabe Lenz, Charles Stewart III, Justin de Benedictis-Kessner, Matthew Levendusky, Krista Loose, Marc Meredith, Diana Mutz, Ethan Porter, Michael Sances, and Adam Ziegfeld.

Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Rethinking the Relationship between Religion andPolitics

A Test of the Life Cycle Theory∗

Michele F. Margolis

May 26, 2015

Abstract

While social identities have long been shown to influence political attitudes andbehaviors, this paper asks the reverse. Are political identities ever strong enough toinfluence identification and involvement with a deep-seated identity? Looking at anidentity that has become an increasingly strong predictor of partisanship and votechoice, religion, I develop a theory that there is a time period in an individual’s lifewhen a fully-formed political identity can have a meaningful effect on a weak religiousidentity. Using an experiment and two panel studies to test my theory, I find thatwhen people are in the process of raising children–a time that pulls many back intothe religious fold–their partisanship can change key aspects of their religious identity.Although many bemoan religion’s role in creating a polarized political arena, this blamemay be unfairly assigned as partisans themselves help produce an increasingly polarizedelectorate.

∗For comments, suggestions, and feedback, I thank Adam Berinsky, Anthony Fowler, Gabe Lenz, CharlesStewart III, Justin de Benedictis-Kessner, Matthew Levendusky, Krista Loose, Marc Meredith, Diana Mutz,Ethan Porter, Michael Sances, and Adam Ziegfeld.

Page 2: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Scholars of identity politics have long asked how deep-seated social identities affect po-

litical attitudes. In the American context, a venerable line of research has shown how race

(Kinder and Sanders 1996), social class (Brooks and Manza 1997; Hout, Brooks, and Manza

1995; Lipset 1981), and gender (Verba, Scholzman and Brady 1995) shape political attitudes,

identities, and allegiances. These social identities are often taken as fixed in their salience

and largely impervious to political influence. Yet, much research across political science and

allied disciplines has shown that the strength of social identities varies across different times,

places, and social contexts (Chandra 2001; Kasfir 1979; Posner 2005; Waters 1990). Fur-

thermore, a long line of scholarship in American politics demonstrates that partisanship can,

itself, be an important and meaningful identity (Green, Palmquist, and Schickler 2002; Ger-

ber and Huber 2010; 2009). Is it therefore possible that, under certain conditions, political

identities influence an individual’s social identities?

To begin to answer this question, I focus on one identity often touted as one of the primary

drivers of contemporary political attitudes in the United States: religion. Republicans, on

average, attend church more regularly and are more likely to identify with a religious faith

than Democrats. A common explanation for this gap in the religiosity of partisan groups,

often known as the “God gap”, is that the highly devout have sorted into the Republican

Party, while the less religious and secular have joined the Democratic ranks. But, might

there not be another explanation? If political identities are potentially very strong and the

salience of an individual’s religious identity waxes and wanes over the course of her lifetime,

might the former ever influence the latter? This paper theorizes when a person’s political

identity might impact her religious identity and provides evidence in support of the theory’s

observable implications.

I begin by presenting a novel, yet intuitive, theory that explains why political identities

can influence religious identities. In brief, the development of religious and political iden-

tities occurs at different periods in an individual’s life, and the distinct timings of the two

socialization processes create a window during which partisanship can shape identification

1

Page 3: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

with and involvement in the religious sphere. I then offer three empirical tests of my theory,

one experimental and two observational. In each, I show that the contemporary political

landscape–one which links the Republicans to traditional religious values and the Democrats

to more secular and liberal religious beliefs–can affect partisans’ religious identifications and

behaviors. Importantly, the relative strength of religious and political identities’ matters, as

all three sets of results are driven by individuals at a life stage when religious identities tend

to be weak and open to outside influence.

The theory and empirics make three contributions. First, this paper highlights the need

for American politics scholars to consider how politically-relevant social identities are formed

and made salient, as political identities may actually affect membership strength under

certain circumstances. Second, building on Huddy’s claim that social identities have variable

strength (2001), the paper shows that researchers miss important nuances when they assume

that identities are fixed and stable. And third, the paper presents a new way of thinking

about the contemporary religious and political landscape. Religion becoming increasingly

intertwined in politics does more than change the political landscape; it also changes the

religious make up of America.

A life cycle theory of religion and politics

Although previous work has suggested that political identities can shape religious involve-

ment (Hout and Fischer 2014; 2002; Patrikios; Putnam and Campbell 2010), the empirical

findings are limited and do not offer expectations about when political identities should

shape religious outlooks.1 I fill in this hole by developing a theory, which brings together the

religious and political socialization literatures, that predicts when we should expect political1While Hout and Fischer (2002) open the door to the question by showing that the increases in religious

non-identification comes from the rise of the Religious Right in the 1980s, concerns about omitted variablebias and reverse causation limit the inferences they can make with their cross-sectional data. Further,previous empirical work lacks a generalizable theory. Putnam and Campbell (2010) were surprised whenthey find that religious responses shifted over a two-wave panel to be consistent with their partisanship: “Wewere initially skeptical of this finding, since it seemed implausible that people would hazard the fate of theireternal soul over mundane political controversies” (145).

2

Page 4: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

identities to influence religious identities.

The religious life course

For most individuals, religious identification and beliefs are dynamic, not static. Sociologists,

developmental psychologists, and scholars of religion have noted that as people develop and

age, their relationship with both formal religious institutions and their own personal beliefs

change (Argue, Johnson, and White 1999; Chaves 1991; Hoge, Johnson, and Luidens 1993;

Ingersoll-Dayton, Krause, and Morgan 2002; Roof 1993; Wilson and Sherkat 1994).

The sociological “religious life cycle” theory begins with the observation that teenagers

and young adults distance themselves from both the religion in which they were raised and

religious practice in general (Arnett and Jensen 2002; Desmond, Morga, and Kikuchi 2010;

Gallup and Castelli 1989; Hoge, Johnson, and Luidens 1993; Hunsberger and Brown 1984;

Roof 1993; Smith 2009; Uecker, Regnerus, and Vaaler 2007; Willits and Crider, 1989; Wilson

and Sherkat 1994; Wuthnow 2007).2 Across multiple generations, young adults are the least

likely to identify with a religious tradition, attend religious services, pray, and report religion

being an important part of their lives (Smith 2009). Other studies have shown that Mainline

Protestant, Catholic, Mormon, and Jewish faiths all lose more members during this life stage

than any other single period (Albrecht, Cornwall, and Cunningham 1988; Hoge, Johnson,

and Luidens 1993; Uecker, Regnerus, and Vaaler 2007). Across generations and communities,

young adults’ religious identities are noticeably weaker than at other points in their lives.2There are many sociological explanations for why young adults leave religion. First, leaving home,

changing peer groups, and adopting new roles and responsibilities often result in cutting ties with childhoodreligious institutions (Goldscheider and Goldscheider 1999; Hoge, Johnson, and Luidens 1993). These transi-tions automatically place young adults on the outskirts of organized religion. Second, young adults willfullyassert their independence from their parents’ religious beliefs and practices (Arnett and Jensen 2002; Smith2009). In fact, church attendance decreases most dramatically among young adults who attended religiousservices regularly as children and who have parents who regularly attend (Petts 2009). Third, Smith (2009)argues that the cognitive dissonance that comes from the young adults’ exposure to and experimentationwith binge drinking (Perkins 1987), drug use (Engs and Mullen 1999), and premarital sex (Zaleski and Schi-affino 2000) pushes young adults away from religion. A final, more passive, reason for decreased religiosityamong young adults is that they simply lose interest or are too busy with other activities. For many, thedecline in religiosity is not a concerted decision, but rather an unintended eventuality once given free reinover their time (Dinges et al. 1998; Smith 2009).

3

Page 5: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

As these young people emerge into full adulthood, which often begins with starting a

family (Chaves 2011), they must decide whether to remain on the outskirts of religion or

re-enter the religious realm. Sociologists note that getting married (Hadaway and Roof

1988; Roof 1993; Sandominsky and Wilson 1990) and having children (Argue, Johnson, and

White 1999; Arnett and Jensen 2002; Gallup and Castelli 1989; Ingersoll-Dayton, Krause,

and Morgan 2002; Wilson and Sherkat 1994; Wuthnow 2007) are strongly associated with

increased church attendance, and that religious participation peaks when married couples

have school-age children (Argue, Johnson, and White 1999; Stolzenberg, Blair-Loy, and

Waite 1995). Scholars claim that these parents re-embrace religion to provide the next

generation with a religious upbringing.

Despite the tendency to return to religion, newly minted adults do not necessarily raise

the next generation in the same way in which they were raised. Instead, individuals accept

and reject religious beliefs and practices that suit their needs and are consistent with their

pre-existing attitudes and identities (Arnett and Jensen 2002; Newport 1979; Sherkat and

Wilson 1994; Stump 1984). And although certain life events tend to move people out of

religion (like divorce) or toward religion (like death of a loved one or serious illness), an

adult’s resultant religious identity–measured by beliefs and participation–remain stable over

time (Dillon and Wink 2007).

The upper panel of Figure 1 presents a visual illustration of the religious life cycle model.

The socialization process creates a window in adolescence and early adulthood when many

people’s religious identities are relatively weak. While in this window, however, other as-

pects of a young adult’s identity continue to grow and develop, which can subsequently

impact religious decisions as individuals transition into adulthood. One potential influence

is politics.

4

Page 6: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

The political life course

The political socialization literature explains how political attitudes first form and to what

extent these early attitudes affect opinions and ideas later in life. Though party identification

is largely stable during adulthood (Abramson 1979; Alwin, Cohen, and Newcomb 1991;

Green, Palmquist, and Schickler 2002; Sears 1983; Sears and Funk 1990), scholars have

explored when these political identifications solidify. This process does not happen overnight

but occurs as individuals grow, learn, and engage with the political sphere.

The “impressionable years” hypothesis claims–as the name suggests–that adolescents and

young adults are highly “impressionable.” During this time, outside influences and events

shape long-term political outlooks, such as political identification (Abramson 1979; Sears

1990; 1975).3 Adolescents’ partisanship can be affected both by their parents’ political

leanings (Beck and Jennings 1991; Chaffee, McCleod, and Wackman 1973; Jennings and

Niemi 1981; 1974; Niemi and Jennings 1990; Tedin 1974) as well as current events and the

political climate of the day (Alwin, Cohen and Newcomb 1991; Beck 1974; Firebaugh and

Chen 1995; Green, Palmquist and Schickler 2002). The events affecting those coming of age

can be both large in scale, such as the Vietnam War or the September 11th terrorist attacks,

or regularly occurring political events, such as campaigns and elections. Political campaigns

boil down complex issues into simple, digestible ideas and allows adolescents to sort out their

political leanings in the process of becoming a partisan (Sears and Valentino 1997).4

The resultant partisanship from this socialization process is more than just a stable

affiliation with a political party; it is a powerful identity in its own right that often lasts

a lifetime (Green, Palmquist, and Schickler 2002) and influences other relevant attitudes

and behaviors. Partisanship shapes responses to survey questions asking about the economy3The hypothesis distinguishes itself from previous socialization theories by recognizing that core beliefs

are largely stable over the course of one’s adult life, while also conceding that attitudes can change a greatdeal during childhood and early adolescence (Jennings and Niemi 1984; 1981; Vaillancourt 1973).

4Related to the impressionable years hypothesis, a generational effect occurs when people in the sameage cohort experience similar, “historical, social, cultural, and political experiences” at the same period intheir lives (Delli Carpini 1989). The result is groups of otherwise different individuals having a more similaroutlook than they otherwise would due to the time in which they came of age (Sears 1990).

5

Page 7: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

(Bartels 2002; 2000), levels of trust in the government (Keele 2005), and feelings about the

fairness of elections (Sances and Stewart 2012). Gerber and Huber (2010; 2009) even show

that partisanship drives consumption behavior and self-reported spending decisions. All

told, partisan identification develops in adolescence and young adulthood, is quite stable

throughout adulthood, and can produce meaningful changes in the attitudes and behaviors

of partisans. The lower panel of Figure 1 presents the political socialization timeline.

Predictions within the American political landscape

The two panels in Figure 1 reveal that political identities are being formed and internalized

at the very time when religious identities are relatively weak. Upon reaching adulthood,

an individual has a solidified political affiliation as she makes decisions about her religious

identity. Partisan identities, which are routinely activated through elite cues differentiating

Republicans and Democrats on questions related to religion, may cause people to rely on their

partisanship when updating their religious identities. Just as elite cues encourage partisans to

adopt attitudes and behave politically in a manner consistent with their party (Cohen 2003;

Druckman, Peterson, and Slothuus 2013; Lavine, Johnston, Steenbergen 2012), partisans at

this point in their life may bring their religious identity in line with their pre-existing partisan

affiliation. However, religious identities are not always in flux. Once religious identities have

solidified, they are quite stable over long periods of time. Consequently, for those squarely

in adulthood–that is, those who have already made their re-entrance (or not) into religion–I

expect partisanship’s effect on religion to be muted.

If partisanship were to influence religious decisions, in what direction would we expect

Republicans and Democrats to move? Republicans are painted as the party aligned with

religious groups and the champion of morally conservative issues (Bolce and De Maio 2002;

2014), while the Democratic Party has taken liberal positions on issues related to morality

and religion since the early 1970s (Layman 2001). These cues, provided by both politicians

and the media, have subsequently affected partisans’ views. Voters are more likely to clas-

6

Page 8: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

sify evangelical Christians and religious people as Republicans and seculars as Democrats

(Campbell, Layman, and Green 2011), and they are more likely to view the Republican

Party as “friendlier” toward religion than the Democratic Party (Pew 2009). Consequently,

if partisans were to update their religious identity to be consistent with their partisanship,

Republicans would become more religious, while Democrats would become less religious.

Below I present three empirical tests of the life cycle theory. In the analyses, all respon-

dents were born in the United States. Individuals raised in another country experienced

different socialization processes, and the same theoretical expectations may not apply. In

the next section, I present an experiment whose purpose is to estimate directly how political

identification can influence religious identification.

Partisan identity priming experiment

The goal of the experiment is straightforward: prime an individual’s partisan identity. Prim-

ing, or highlighting, an identity allows researchers to circumvent the problem that identities

are not exogenous by instead measuring attitudes (Jackson 2001; Johnson, Rowatt, and

LaBouff 2012; Klar 2013) and behaviors (Dijksterhuis and van Knippenberg 1998; McLeish

and Oxoby 2008; Shariff and Norenzayan 2007; Shih et al. 1999) after randomly assigning

whether a particular identity is made salient. In this case, if partisans link conservative

religious values with the Republican Party and equate more secular and morally permissive

positions with the Democratic Party, then priming an individual’s partisan identity may

cause them to re-evaluate their religious identities.

7

Page 9: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Research design

The two-wave experiment took place in August 2013 using a diverse national sample.5 In

the first wave of the survey, I collected party identification. The experimental portion of the

study took place two weeks later using 1,230 respondents. In the beginning of the second

wave, respondents were randomly assigned to a treatment or control condition.6

In order to prime partisan identities, treated respondents rated the aesthetics of three

flyers advertising a voter registration and political engagement drive put on by the fictitious

Ohio Voters’ Council. Self-identified Democrats and Democratic leaners in wave 1 rated

flyers for a Democratic event, while self-identified Republicans and Republican leaners rated

flyers for a Republican version of the event.7 Respondents rated the flyers in three head-

to-head match ups. For each comparison, respondents were asked to choose which flyer

was easier to read, which flyer made the event seem more attractive, and which flyer the

respondent preferred overall. I present the flyers and experimental text in the Appendix.

The experimental stimulus is a weak treatment whose purpose is only to remind individ-

uals of their partisan identities, but does not make reference to specific policies, politicians,

or groups linked to one of the parties. And importantly, there is no mention of religion or

morality politics. After answering the final question of the section, respondents moved on to

the next part of the study which asked a series of attitudinal and behavioral questions. The

main dependent variable–religious identity–was asked among other demographic questions5The sample was obtained through Survey Sampling International (SSI). SSI recruits participants through

online communities, social networks, and website ads, and makes efforts to recruit hard-to-reach groups, suchas ethnic minorities and seniors. Potential participants are then screened and invited into the panel. Whendeploying a particular survey, SSI randomly selects panel participants for survey invitations. I did not employquotas but asked SSI to recruit a target population that matched the (18 and over) census population oneducation, gender, age, geography, and income. The resulting sample is not a probability sample but is adiverse national sample. Numerous studies using sample from SSI have been published recently in politicalscience (Berinsky, Margolis, and Sances 2014; Kam 2012; Malhotra and Margalit 2010; Malhotra, Margalit,and Mo 2013).

6I randomly assigned subjects into the treatment conditions at the beginning of Wave 2 which reducesconcerns related to panel attrition and internal validity.

7Pure Independents in the political priming condition were randomly assigned to one of the two partyprimes.

8

Page 10: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

later in the survey.8

The dependent variable is a four-point measure of religious identification strength based

on two questions. Respondents first answered a standard religious identification question,

and then one follow-up question based on their initial response. If the respondent identifies

with a religion, she receives a follow up that asks whether or not she identifies strongly or not

strongly with the religion.9 Approximately 40% of religious identifiers classify themselves

as “weak” identifiers while 60% think of themselves as “strong” identifiers. For the roughly

23% of respondents who do not identify with any religion, they are asked: “Do you think of

yourself as closer to one particular religion over another?” and are given the same response

options as the initial identification question along with an option that they do not feel any

closer to one particular religion. A full quarter of respondents who initially said that they did

not identify with a religious tradition “leaned” toward one religion. The result is a four-point

scale of religious identification, ranging from 0 (strong non-identifier) to 1 (strong identifier).

This paper is first and foremost interested in testing whether the relative strengths of

different identities vary depending on a person’s position within the life cycle. To do so, I

focus on two subsamples. First, I look at individuals with school-aged children, as they are

theoretically the most likely to have weak religious identities that are open to partisanship’s

influence. Second, I look at individuals with grown children, whose solidified religious iden-

tities should make them more immune to external influence. Full parametric results for the

full sample, subsamples, other measures of religious identity, and models that include control

variables are in the Appendix.8As a manipulation check, I also asked respondents attitudinal questions about whether more religious

people should run for office and whether the political parties focus too much or not enough on religion.Primed partisans were more likely to move toward the extreme–Democrats wanting less religion in politics andRepublicans wanting more. Half of the respondents received these questions before the religious identificationquestion and half answered the questions after. There is no systematic difference in dependent variable basedon when they answered these questions about religion in the public sphere.

9For example, if a respondent identifies as Catholic, her follow-up would read: “Do you identify stronglyas a Catholic or not very strongly as a Catholic?” If a respondent identified as an “other” religion, thereligion she specified in the open-ended box was carried over to the follow-up question.

9

Page 11: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Priming partisanship results

The top panels of Figure 2 presents what a hypothetical treatment and null effect look

like. The x-axis represents the treatment condition and the y-axis represents the average

dependent variable response for the group. Here we care how the treatment changes the

relative size of the identification gap. In the left panel, Republicans (Democrats), denoted by

squares (circles), in the treated condition report being a stronger (weaker) religious identifier

than Republicans (Democrats) in the control condition. This hypothetical result would

indicate that the partisan prime changed reported religious identities and increased the

identification gap between Democrats and Republicans. The top right panel, on the other

hand, indicates a null effect. Again using hypothetical data, the gap does not change on

account of the treatment.

The bottom panels of Figure 2 present the average identifications scores for those with

children at home (left) and those with grown children (right). Although unsurprising, I find

that Republicans with children at home are stronger identifiers than Democrats in the con-

trol condition10 More importantly, the partisan primes influenced religious identities among

those with children at home. The average score for the four-point religious identification

measure increased from 0.80 to 0.88 for Republicans, but decreased from 0.72 to 0.66 among

Democrats. The partisan gap increased by 0.14 points, more than doubling the original

identification gap (p-value = 0.03).11 In contrast, religious identification rates remained

relatively stable among those with grown children. The identification gap actually shrunk

slightly, but this result is statistically insignificant (p-value = 0.41). A further test confirms

that the partisan treatment effect differs between those with children at home and those

with grown children (p-value = 0.04).12

Importantly, this divergence is not driven by strong partisans and occurs among Democrats1065% of Republicans reporting that they are a strong identifier, compared to 48% of Democrats.11The corresponding p-value comes from a difference-in-difference model testing whether the treatment

affected Republicans and Democrats differently.12The parametric test involves a triple interaction model that includes treatment condition, partisanship,

and parental status.

10

Page 12: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

and Republicans alike. Although there is not enough data to fully explore the treatment

effects using a seven-point party identification measure, the treatment effects found among

strong partisans are noticeably smaller than the effects for weak partisans and partisan lean-

ers. In fact, re-running the analyses excluding strong identifiers actually increases both the

magnitude and statistical significance of the results. Religious updating is not only occurring

among extreme partisans. Moreover, I find symmetric results indicating that Republicans

and Democrats both contributed to the increased identity gap.

The controlled experiment provides a direct test of whether and when partisanship might

influence religious identification. Despite being a weak test of politics’ influence, I find

partisans updating their religious identity to be “consistent” with their partisan identity at

a certain point in their lives. The experiment, however, leaves two questions unanswered.

First, are the results indicative of a life cycle theory, as I suggest, or are the two cohorts

simply different? Second, is there any evidence in the real world of partisanship influencing

religious identities? In the sections that follow I present two sets of observational results

which corroborate and build on the experimental findings.

Testing partisanship’s influence using two panel studies

Observational studies have explored how aspects of a person’s religious identity have come

to influence political behaviors. In particular, attendance at religious services (generally

referred to as church attendance) became an important predictor of Republican support for

the first time beginning in the 1980s (Layman 1997) and continues today (Green 2010).

Higher levels of church attendance, within any denomination, are positively correlated with

Republican identification and vote choice.

In the next sections I use panel data to test whether part of this increased correlation arose

from political identities influencing church attendance. Church attendance is an outward

sign of and a common proxy for religious identity, and sociologists’ religious life cycle work

11

Page 13: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

has focused on involvement in churches–particularly being members and attending (Argue,

Johnson, and White 1999; Chaves and Schleifer 2014; Stolzenberg, Blair-Loy, and Waite

1995). Church attendance is also asked in both data sets, which allows for comparability. I

discuss and replicate the main results for other dataset-specific variables in the Appendix.

I identify two cases, one in the 1970s and one in 2004, when scholars have found evidence

of voters receiving clear signals distinguishing the parties on the basis of religiosity or moral-

ity politics. Although the sample composition differs and the data are collected decades

apart, the empirical strategy for both cases is similar. In both cases, I measure whether

an individual’s partisanship–measured years before–changes her reported church attendance

over time.13

Although the panel data reduce concerns about reverse causation and omitted variable

bias that plague cross-sectional research, the strategy is not an inferential panacea. A first

concern is that individuals may change their partisanship over time and not just their reli-

giosity. Despite the literature noting that partisanship is stable, even over long periods of

time and in the wake of political turmoil (Green, Palmquist, and Schickler 2002), partisan-

ship is not wholly fixed. That said, time lags are more likely to mask an effect rather than

exaggerate it.14 The more variation there is in partisanship, the harder it will be to detect an

effect. As an added measure, however, I run two robustness checks on all the results. First, I

replicate the results with a restricted sample using only respondents whose answers on parti-

sanship remained the same across the two survey waves. Second, I replicate the results using

previous measures of partisanship (Xt−1) as an instrument for present-day partisanship (X).

Although both strategies offer their own problems, these alternative specifications produce13I run change models that includes a lagged dependent variable rather than models that use a differenced

dependent variable because the differenced specification assumes that the lagged dependent variable (Yt−1)does not influence either the dependent variable at time t (Yt) or the change in dependent variable acrossthe survey waves (∆Y) (Finkel 1995; Morgan and Winship 2007). It is highly improbable that partisanshipor church attendance at one point in time is uncorrelated with a change in these variables over time. As arobustness check, however, I also ran the models using a differenced DV approach and find similar results.

14For example, if a nonchurch-attending Democrat in time t becomes a frequently-attending Republicanby time t+1, my data would show a time t Democrat becoming more religious between times t and t + 1.This goes against the theoretical expectation.

12

Page 14: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

similar results to the main findings presented.

A second concern relates to the generalizability of these findings. The subsequent analyses

include individuals of and control variables for all religious faiths, both large and small.

While the samples are too small to test how minority religions, such as Jews or Mormons,

behave, the main results remain the same if they are included or excluded from the models.

For the larger religious faiths, however, I run a series of interaction analyses and replicate

the main results excluding the large religious groups–mainline Protestants, Catholics, and

evangelical Protestants–one at a time in order test whether the overall results are driven

by one particular religious group. For all analyses, the results are not driven by one major

religion, but rather apply broadly to (at least) the three main religious denominations.15

1970s: The beginning of religious elite divergence

The 1970s represents a watershed moment during which the Republican and Democratic

Parties diverged along religious and moral lines for the first time. These elite-level changes

serve as the first instance in which we might find evidence of partisanship influencing decisions

related to one’s religious identity.

Although discussions about the relationship between religion and politics emphasize Re-

publican strategies, the Democratic Party actually made the first move on cultural issues

(Layman 2001). George McGovern, the party’s presidential nominee, was a liberal Senator

who favored more lenient penalties for marijuana use and opposed calls to ban abortion

at the national level (White 1973). 1972 also marked the first time in which secularists

and cultural liberals were a significant presence among Democratic activists and convention

delegates (Kirkpatrick 1976; Layman 2001) and the Democratic Party platform contained

liberal stances on cultural issues, including sections on “The Right To Be Different,” “Rights

of Women,” and “Family Planning” (Layman 2001, 114).

Although a wholesale Democratic shift to the cultural left was mitigated by the election15The three major religions make up approximately 90% of the first sample and 60% of the second sample.

13

Page 15: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

of Democrat Jimmy Carter, the first born-again President, Republicans began to strategi-

cally court religious activists with a culturally conservative issue agenda (Himmelstein 1983;

Oldfield 1996; Reichley 1987). In the 1980 election, Ronald Reagan reached out devout

voters–including members of Jerry Falwell’s newly-formed Moral Majority and the Religious

Roundtable–by emphasizing issues such as abortion, prayer in public school, subsidizing pri-

vate religious education, and gay rights (Layman 2001; Miller and Wattenberg 1984; Oldfield

1996). Reagan railed against federally-funded textbooks that taught “moral relativism”, the

Federal Communications Commission for trying to limit religious broadcasting, and the IRS

for launching an “unconstitutional regulatory vendetta” against Christian schools (Williams

2010, 141-2).

The large-scale shifts in official party positions, candidate rhetoric, and the salience of

moral issues mark the first instance in which the parties became known for being on opposing

sides of the religious and cultural aisle. If we think partisanship can drive religious decisions,

this would be the first time we should see this occurring.

Politics affecting religiosity for the first time

To test how partisanship influenced changes in church attendance, I use the Youth-Parent

Socialization Panel data (YPSP), started in 1965 by M. Kent Jennings. The YPSP first

interviewed a national sample of high school seniors and their parents in 1965. Subsequent

follow-up surveys were conducted using the same individuals in 1973, 1982, and 1997. The

data capture the cohort at key life stages–at ages 18, 26, 35, and 50–that allow for a test

of partisanship’s influence as life cycle status changes. At age 26 (in 1973) many, but not

all, are married and individuals have very young children, if they have children at all. In

contrast, by 35 (in 1982), nearly everyone in the sample has school-aged children. This

cohort, therefore, represents one of the first generations who were making religious decisions

for themselves and their families against the new religious-political backdrop.

14

Page 16: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Although not the primary interest of the paper, I begin by showing how young partisans’

church attendance changed after graduating from high school and becoming young adults. To

provide an intuition behind the results, the left panel of Figure 3 plots the raw percentage of

the student generation who reported attending church almost weekly or weekly in 1965 and in

1973, separately for self-identified Republicans and Democrats (measured in 1965). Church

attendance declined dramatically for both Republicans and Democrats: while roughly 67%

(64%) of Republicans (Democrats) reported attend church nearly every week, that numbers

plummets to 30% (29%) in 1973. These results corroborate an expansive sociology literature

showing that religion becomes a peripheral concern as students leave home and begin to

make a life for themselves. The right panel of Figure 3 is a partial residual plot, which

shows the variation in church attendance (ordinal measure ranging between 0 and 1) due

to partisanship after accounting for 1965 levels of church attendance and control variables

that may be correlated with partisanship and changing religiosity.16 The line represents

partisanship’s effect when respondents change their church attendance over time. The flat

slope indicates that while church attendance declined dramatically between 1965 and 1973,

Republicans and Democrats responded in the same manner over time. Parametric models

that treat party identification as binary variables, rather than linear, are presented in the

Appendix. In contrast, the parent generation’s levels of church attendance remained stable

between 1965 and 1973.

More pertinent to the current research question is how Democrats’ and Republicans’

church attendance changed between 1973 and 1982. The left panel of Figure 3 again assesses

how an individual’s partisanship–measured in 1973–corresponds to changes in regular church

attendance between 1973 and 1982. Regular church attendance among Republicans increased

from 32% in 1973 to 42% in 1982, while Democrats only increased from 30% to 33%. While

there was no church attendance gap between Democrats and Republicans in 1973, a 7% gap16Controls include: 1965 measures of the student’s religion, region of residence, gender, race, high school

curriculum, and closeness with parents; whether the student had attended college, was married, and hadchildren in 1973; and measures of the student’s upbringing based on responses from the parents: averagechurch attendance, party identification, education of head of household, and average income.

15

Page 17: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

emerged by 1982. The right panel of Figure 3 corroborates the raw results with a partial

residual graph from a change model. Again, I use an ordinal church attendance variable that

ranges between 0 and 1.

The change model includes three sets of control variables. First, I include demographic

controls: the student generation’s region of residence, education, income, marital and parental

status, gender, whether they served in Vietnam, and closeness with parents from 1973, as

well their religious affiliation, church attendance, and high school curriculum from 1965.

Second, I include individuals’ attitudes on important policy issues of the day. The 1960s

and 1970s were a time of great social upheaval and broader societal attitudes may have

triggered a move toward or away from religion. I therefore include respondents’ opinions on

the Vietnam War, school busing, marijuana legalization, government aid to minorities, equal

rights for women, and economic liberalism.17 And third, I use responses from the parent

generation data to control for additional aspects of students’ upbringing that likely affect the

development of both partisan and religious identities: average church attendance, party iden-

tification, family income, and education of the household head. By 1982, a partisan-driven

church attendance gap–representing about 7% of the church attendance scale–is noticeable

in the data. Republicans “returned” to the pews to a greater extent than Democrats. Im-

portantly, this attendance gap remains for many years to come. By 1997, Democrats’ levels

of church attendance had not “caught up” to Republicans. In the Appendix I show that

Democrats in 1973 are still less frequent church attenders in 1997, at 50 years old, compared

to their Republican counterparts.

To understand the magnitude of the results, I compare the partisan results to a well-

documented explanation of religious involvement. A child’s religious environment is a con-

sistently strong predictor of her religiosity in adulthood. Individuals raised in religious house-17Including policy attitudes as control variables raises concerns about whether individuals choose a partisan

identification that matches their policy positions (which would make it important to control for policypreferences) or whether partisans adopt policy positions to match their partisanship (which might result inpost-treatment bias on the partisanship variable). I run the model both with and without the policy controlvariables and the relationship between partisanship and religiosity remains the same.

16

Page 18: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

holds are more likely to be religious themselves (Kelly and De Graff 1997; Myers 1996). I

measure an individual’s religious upbringing with a church attendance question asked of the

parent generation in 1965. Students raised in households in which the parents attended

church almost every week shifted 0.14 on the church attendance scale compared to those

raised in homes where the parents did not attend church (p-value = 0.03), 0.09 compared

to those raised in a home where the parents attended a few times per year (p-value = 0.03),

and 0.06 compared to those who were raised in a home where the parents attended once or

twice a month (p-value = 0.11). The gap that emerged between Republicans and Democrats

is half the size of the gap that emerges between individuals raised in non-attending versus

frequently attending households (0.07 versus 0.14). Partisanship’s effect is noteworthy and

its effect size meaningful in comparison to one’s religious upbringing which represents an

important predictor of religious identity in adulthood.

I replicate the same results among the parent generation in order to rule out the possibility

that partisanship was driving changes in religious behaviors for everyone. If so, the results

would be evidence of a period effect, not a life cycle effect which predicts that religious

identities are stable in adulthood. Figure 4 replicates the findings for the parents’ generation.

Not only are church attendance rates quite stable and the partisan attendance gap does not

change statistically over time, the trend actually runs in the opposite direction: Republicans’

reported church attendance declined slightly relative to Democrats.

Finally, how does political non-identification factor in? Although nearly half–48%–of

this cohort initially reported not identifying as either a Republican or Democrat, nearly

60% of these initial independents leaned toward one party. Partisan leaners look virtually

indistinguishable to those who readily identified as a partisan. While the analyses looking at

the leaners produces more uncertainty around the point estimates due to the smaller sample

size, the substantive take away is the same: Republicans (and Republican leaners) became

more religious over time relative to Democrats (and Democratic leaners).

While the small number of pure Independents (20%) makes it difficult to provide defini-

17

Page 19: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

tive answers, two results are particularly suggestive. First, Independents attended church

at a lower rate than both Democrats and Republicans in 1973, before religion became a

salient political issue. If we think of both politics and religion as something that people

join or identify with, it makes sense that political Independents are also more religiously

independent. Second, Independents fall squarely between Democrats and Republicans on

changing church attendance rates between 1973 and 1982. Independents returned to church

at a slightly higher rate than Democrats (0.04) and a slightly lower rate than Republicans

(0.03). Independents’ moderate religious shift is consistent a general move toward religion

at this time coupled with the political environment sending Republicans and Democrats

opposite religious cues.

Although the YPSP allows me to track changes over much of one cohort’s life, its strength

is also its weakness. All cohorts are unique, and the graduating class of 1965 is especially so.

Men from this cohort could have been (and were) drafted, disaffiliation from the two main

political parties was high, and this cohort was coming of age during a rapid transformation

about attitudes related to race, sex, and drugs. In the next section, I replicate the main

findings using a different sample in a different generation that experiences a different shift

in the political landscape.

2004: Gay marriage comes to the forefront

Gay marriage claiming the national spotlight offers a second opportunity to assess what

happens when visible elite cues link religion and politics together. In November 2003, the

Massachusetts Supreme Court declared that marriage licenses would be granted to same-sex

couples, and three months later San Francisco Mayor Gavin Newsom ordered city officials to

issue marriage licenses to couples of the same gender in violation of California Family Code.

At the same time, President Bush was becoming a vocal advocate in support of a con-

stitutional amendment defining marriage as between one man and one woman. In his 2004

18

Page 20: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

State of the Union address he said: “If judges insist on forcing their arbitrary will upon

the people, the only alternative left to the people would be the constitutional process. Our

nation must defend the sanctity of marriage” (quoted in Oldmixon and Calfano 2004). And

one month later he again called for a constitutional amendment banning gay marriage in

a second prime time television address. Although the Federal Marriage Amendment that

was re-introduced was eventually defeated in the Senate in Summer 2004, the public debate

on gay marriage did not end. Eleven states had anti-gay marriage initiatives on the ballot

in November. As a result, gay marriage was a salient political issue from November 2003

through November 2004.

An original content analysis of newspapers reveals that there were three times as many

articles about gay marriage in 2004 than in the previous three years combined, and that the

articles in 2004 were more likely to mention religion explicitly than in previous years.18 My

content analysis on gay marriage corroborates Bolce and De Maio’s (2014) analysis which

counts the number of articles in the New York Times and Washington Post that discuss

Democrats and Republicans being divided along secular(ist) and religious lines between 1987

and 2012. The press rarely covered the religious divide between 1987 and 2002, averaging

fewer than 5 articles per year. In 2003, however, the number of stories increased to 9 before

ballooning to 52 stories in 2004. Between 2003 and 2004, the media repeatedly informed

citizens about religious and secular coalitions in the political sphere. The changing media

environment allows me to test whether increased attention to the prominent linkages between

religion and politics changed partisans’ religious practices.

The influence of gay marriage: a three-wave test

The American National Election Study (ANES) panel data were collected in 2000, 2002, and

2004. I use the 2002 and 2004 waves to assess whether the politicized issue of gay marriage

affected individual-level survey responses about church attendance. Further, the 2000 and18Details from the content analysis are available in the Appendix.

19

Page 21: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

2002 waves serve as a placebo test of what happens when there is not a large-scale increase

in discussion about the current religious-political landscape. If the salience of gay marriage

affected partisans’ religious practices, I expect to find a widened religious gap between 2002

and 2004, but not between 2000 and 2002.

I again use reported church attendance as the dependent variable, and replicate the re-

sults using a question that asks how much guidance religion plays in respondents’ day-to-lives

in the Appendix. Once again, I include control variables in the change model that may influ-

ence both partisanship and religiosity: race, age, age-squared, marital status, parental status,

education, household income, employment status, political ideology, religious identification

(in 2000), and region of residence. The results are substantively unchanged when I also con-

trol for attitudinal measures of: abortion policy, feeling thermometer toward homosexuals,

feeling thermometers toward feminists, economic liberalism.19 Similar to the experiment, I

distinguish between those who are married with children living at home during the four years

of the panel, who are in the process of making religious decisions, and those individuals with

grown children, who should have stable religious identities.20

Again using a partial residual plot, the top left panel of Figure 5 compares partisanship

in 2000–ranging from Democrat to Republican along the x-axis–to the change in church

attendance–ranging from 0 to 1–between 2000 and 2002 along the y-axis for the full sample

of respondents. The slope is basically flat, indicating that while Republicans were more

regular churchgoers than Democrats in 2000 and 2002, the size of the gap between them

remained stable over time.

The third wave of interviews took place in 2004. Between the second and third waves, gay

marriage became a salient political issue. The top right panel of Figure 5 shows partisanship

in 2002 produced changes in church attendance between 2002 and 2004. Here, the slope19I also include support for the Iraq war in the 2002-2004 models.20In order to have comparable subsamples across the survey waves, I classify respondents as having chil-

dren at home if they have children living at home during the entire four-year period. Similarly, I classifyrespondents as having grown children if their children are over 18 in the 2000 wave. This strategy excludes24 respondents who had children living at home in 2002, but not 2004. Including the additional respondentsin the analysis produces identical results.

20

Page 22: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

is steeper–0.05–indicating that between 2002 and 2004, Democrats changed to report lower

church attendance while Republicans changed to report higher church attendance (p-value =

0.04). Partisans updated their reported church attendance between 2002 and 2004 resulting

in more (less) religious Republicans (Democrats).

The lower panels of Figure 5 reproduces the results on the two subsamples of interest.

The middle panels of Figure 5 graphically shows how partisans with children living at home

changed over time. Again using partial residual plots, the left panel presents change in re-

ported church attendance between 2000 and 2002, before gay marriage became salient. The

partisan gap in church attendance remained stable across the two-year period. The right

panel, however, tells a different story. Here I find the religious gap between Democrats and

Republicans widened substantially after gay marriage became salient. Democrats’ church

attendance decreased by approximately 0.10 points while Republicans’ attendance increased

by roughly 0.04 points. The resultant attendance gap between Democrats and Republicans

in 2004 had increased by 0.14 points–or about three-quarters of the distance between survey

response options–compared to the 2002 gap (p-value < 0.01). Interestingly, Independents’

reported church attendance remained generally stable. Although a church attendance gap

appears between Democrats and Independents, this occurs on account of Democrats decreas-

ing their attendance.21 The bottom panels of Figure 5 presents a similar set of graphs for

respondents with grown children. Here, both panels reveal the same trend: church atten-

dance rates were stable among both Democrats and Republicans. Although there is some

movement across the years, the results are both substantively small and statistically insignif-

icant. Finally, parametric tests confirm that the 2002 to 2004 changes for respondents with

children at home are statistically different from those with grown children (see parametric

results in Appendix). Having replicated the YPSP results, I next address some remaining

alternative explanations that have not yet been discussed.21Independents’ church attendance changed from 0.38 in 2002 to 0.37 in 2004.

21

Page 23: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Alternative explanations

The results have thus far provided evidence in support of the life cycle theory. Political

identities seemed to affect religious decisions at a specific time period in a person’s life, and

these decisions are felt even as these individuals grow older. Evidence in support of a theory,

however, does not rule out alternative explanations. In this section, I present and discuss a

series of plausible explanations. Detailed results from the analyses are in the Appendix.

What about religion affecting politics? Newly formed adults made religious choices con-

sistent with their pre-existing political identities in both sets of data; however, it might also

be the case that religious identities affected political attitudes during this time. For exam-

ple, in the YPSP data a highly devout respondent may have started to feel uneasy in the

Democratic Party with its emphasis on cultural liberalism and may have appreciated the

Republican Party’s religious language and emphasis on cultural issues. If so, then religious

identities and religiosity are still potent predictors of change among this group. I do not

find evidence that an individual’s religiosity–measured by church attendance–was a driver

of political change. Church attendance did not affect reported partisanship or vote choice

between 1973 and 1982 in the YPSP data, and it did not affect reported partisanship or

a feeling thermometer rating toward President Bush between 2002 and 2004 in the ANES

data. These findings hold even when replicating the analyses using only respondents whose

level of attendance remained the same over the waves.

Are certain generations weird? Cohort effect as an alternative. While each set of results

can be explained as a unique cohort, I find the same evidence on three cohorts from the

1970s and 1980s, early 2000s, and 2014. Moreover, the student generation in the YPSP

data, for whom I found evidence of partisanship affecting religious decisions, would have

grown children in the ANES data. Although their partisanship exerted influence at one

point in their life, it does not at another.

Is this a story about getting older? Aging effect as an alternative. Another alternative

explanation is that the results occur naturally because of aging, not because of changing

22

Page 24: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

positions within the life cycle. While I cannot test this alternative with the YPSP data

because the overwhelming majority of the student generation had children by 1982 when

they were 35, I test this possibility with the ANES data by comparing respondents with and

without children who are in the same narrow age bracket. Respondents without children

are typically on the outskirts of religion (see second panel of Figure 1). With religious

identities and practices on the backburner, we should not expect politics to have a large

influence on religion.22 As such, despite being in the same age range, people with and

without children should respond differently to the changing political landscape between 2002

and 2004. Across six age groupings (under 40, under 35, between 30 and 40, between 25 and

40, between 25 and 35), I consistently find that Republicans and Democrats with children

diverged in their reported religious attendance between 2002 and 2004. Among similarly-

aged respondents without children, however, partisans did not diverge. I additionally find

that church attendance diverged among partisans with children at home, irrespective of

respondent age, while partisans of all ages with grown children similarly provided stable

responses across the time period. These results indicate that an individual’s age is secondary

to, albeit correlate with, her placement in the life cycle in explaining the findings from the

previous section.

Are partisans diverging in societal participation more generally? Perhaps Republicans

and Democrats began to differ on all sorts of civic and political engagement. If so, my results

would not be about religion but rather a broader life cycle trend of societal engagement. I test

for this possibility by running a series of placebo tests using political and civic engagement

questions. In the YPSP, I find no evidence of Republicans becoming more involved in politics,

measured by voting rates and contacting elected officials, or other societal groups such as

fraternal organizations, informal social groups, civic organizations, and professional groups.22While possible that some young, childless individuals have made the active decision to forgo children and

already made decisions pertaining to their religious identities, Gallup (2013) findings that 87% of individualsaged 18-40 without children plan to have them in the future. It is still the overwhelming norm to plan onhaving children. It is therefore reasonable to assume that the majority in this subsample will try to becomeparents in the future.

23

Page 25: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

And in the ANES, I do not find evidence of Republicans becoming more likely to attend a

political rally or a meeting about a community issue. In fact, Democrats with children at

home actually became more likely to attend a political rally between 2002 and 2004 relative

to Republicans. The behavioral divergence is constrained to religious participation.

What’s war got to do with it? The relevant waves of the YPSP data were collected

during the Vietnam War, with the men in the student generation being eligible for the draft,

while the Iraq war began in March of 2003 and was an important issue throughout the 2004

presidential campaign. While attitudes toward the wars are included as control variables in

the models, I also look for heterogeneous effects based on attitudes toward the war. In both

data sets, the magnitude of partisan effects is similar among both those who supported and

opposed the war that was going on during the data collection.

Is this occurring because of the South? The South is not only the most religious region

in the country, but it also underwent a major political transition in the 1970s and 1980s.

This raises concerns about how Southerners and non-Southerners differed in the YPSP data.

If young Southerners were disproportionately likely to become Republican and more likely

to return to religion after a hiatus (possibly on account of their religious upbringing), then

Southerners may be driving the main findings. I do not find evidence supporting this.

First, Southerners were not more likely to become Republicans relative to non-Southerners

between 1965 and 1973. Although Republican identification increased from 21% to 26% be-

tween the first two survey waves, a full 55% of respondents living in the South identified as

a Democrat in 1973. Moreover, Southerners were no more likely to become a Republican

between 1965 and 1973 than individuals living in other parts of the country.

Second, Southerners’ religious trajectories did not differ from those living in the rest of

the country. Southerners were no more likely to fall away from religion between 1965 and

1973 and were no more likely to return between 1973 and 1982.23 Finally, I replicate the23These results are consistent with Sherkat and Wilson’s (1994) findings that conservative Protestants were

no more or less likely to return to identifying with religion after previously being a non-identifier comparedto other religious denominations.

24

Page 26: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

main results after dropping Southern respondents. Despite the smaller sample size, the same

results appear.

Discussion and Conclusion

This paper begins to explore how political and social identities interact, and how variation

in identification strength can have meaningful effects on the development of and involvement

with politically-relevant social identity groups. Using two types of analytic strategies, I show

that partisanship can influence citizens’ religious identities at a particular point in their lives.

The experiment offers a controlled test of how partisans answer standard demographic

questions on religion after being encouraged to “think like a partisan.” As the stimulus did

not link religion and politics together, the results are from an especially difficult test that rely

on how individuals themselves think about the religious-political landscape. Moreover, these

results offer an important first test of the life cycle theory: individuals with theoretically

weaker religious identities were more open to political influence.

I then test how, when the relationship between organized religion and politics changed,

partisans’ religious involvement also changed. The YPSP data offer what is likely the first

evidence of partisanship affecting church attend, a defining behavioral characteristic of one’s

religious identity. Not only did partisanship affect decisions related to returning to religion,

the effect was still evident when these respondents were 50 years old. I find partisanship

affecting religiosity at the same time scholars have first found evidence of religiosity pre-

dicting partisanship and vote choice (Green 2010; Layman 2001; 1997). The second set of

observational findings–which uses the public debate about gay marriage–produced a similar

set of results. Those theoretically most susceptible to partisanship’s influence updated their

religious responses to be consistent with their partisan identities.

These results begin to fill in the gaps that currently exist in the identity literature. First,

rather than thinking about identities as exogenous variables that influence political attitudes

25

Page 27: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

and behaviors, these results demonstrate the need to think of identities as constructs whose

strength varies over time and can be partially created by politics. As this paper shows,

the emergence of the “God gap” is more complicated than academics and pundits initially

thought. The simple explanation that secular and less religious people moved toward the

Democrats while the devout made a home within the Republican camp excludes a key el-

ement. Democrats and Republicans also alter their religious identities to better fit with

their chosen political party. Second, the theory and findings also help explain how multiple

identities work together to create an overarching belief structure. We see strong and stable

identity coalitions in the political realm that are slow to change because identities can rein-

force each other. If partisans adopt the dominant religious identities of their political party,

the relationship between religious and politics identities is reinforced and become stronger.

The results from this paper not only help explain the creation of our politico-religious

landscape; it also affects its future. Church is a breeding ground for political mobilization

and activism (Djupe and Gilbert 2009; Verba, Schlozman and Brady 1994). By Republicans

(Democrats) self-selecting into (out of) religion, Republican campaigns are well-positioned

to have a direct and easy way to mobilize their constituency through churches. Religious

rhetoric in speeches (Albertson 2011; Domke and Coe 2010), mobilization campaigns target-

ing religious voters (Green 2006), and reaching out to religious leaders (Marrapodi 2011) all

excite a core part of the Republican base. Religious voters therefore are a doubly captive

audience for the Republicans.

Inactivity in religion, however, does not ignite the same levels of political fervor. Although

secular humanist and atheist organizations exist, their membership and reach are minuscule

compared to that of organized religion. This is largely driven by the fact that most non-

identifiers and non-religious people are not necessarily opposed to religion, nor do they

have strong anti-religious tendencies. Rather, they simply happen to not be involved (Lim,

MacGregor, and Putnam 2010). In contrast to Republicans who can rally people under the

banner of religion, the Democratic Party cannot use secularism to mobilize its members.

26

Page 28: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

The result of politics affecting religion is that the bases of the two parties–as well as their

ability to be energized–are now also different.

This paper approaches an old question about the relationship between religion and politics

from a new angle. My theory and results not only show that a reverse relationship between

religious and political attitudes exists but that researchers should not find such a relationship

surprising. We must reconsider the conventional wisdom linking religion and politics together

and begin thinking about the conditions under which different identities are likely to exert

influence.

27

Page 29: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

References - to be updated

Abramson, Paul R. 1979. “Developing Party Identification: A Further Examination of Life-

Cycle, Generational, and Period Effects.” American Journal of Political Science 23(1):

78-96.

Albertson, Bethany L. 2011. “Religious Language and Implicit Attitudes.” Political Psy-

chology 32(1): 109-130.

Albrecht, Stan L., Marie Cornwall, and Perry H. Cunningham. 1998. “Religious Leave-

Taking: Disengagement and Disaffiliation among Mormons.” In Fall from the Faith:

The Causes, Course, and Consequences of Religious Apostasy, ed. David G. Bromley.

Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Alwin, Duane F., Ronald L. Cohen, and Theodore M. Newcomb. 1991. Political Attitudes

over the Life Span: The Bennington Women after Fifty Years. Madison, WI: University

of Wisconsin Press.

Argue, Amy, David R. Johnson, and Lynn K. White. 1999. “Age and Religiosity: Evidence

from a Three-Wave Panel Analysis.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 38(3):

423-435.

Arnett, Jeffrey J. 2000. “Emerging Adulthood. A Theory of Development from the Late

Teens through the Twenties.” American Psychologist 55(5): 469-480.

Arnett, Jeffrey J. 1998. “Learning to Stand Alone: The Contemporary American Transition

to Adulthood in Cultural and Historical Context.” Human Development 41(5): 295-315.

Arnett, Jeffrey J., and Lene A. Jensen. 2002. “A Congregation of One: Individualized

Religious Beliefs among Emerging Adults.” Journal of Adolescent Research 17(5): 451-

467.

Babington, Charles. 2011. “GOP Hopefuls Pay Homage to Religious Right.” Associated

Press Retrieved from: http://bigstory.ap.org/content/charles-babington.

Bartels, Larry. 2002. “Beyond the Running Tally: Partisan Bias in Political Perceptions.”

Political Behavior 24(2): 117-150.

28

Page 30: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Bartels, Larry. 2000. “Partisanship and Voting Behavior 1952-1996.” American Journal of

Political Science 44(1): 35-50.

Beck, Paul Allen. 1974. “A Socialization Theory of Partisan Realignment.” in The Politics of

Future Citizens: New Dimensions in Socialization, ed. Richard G. Niemi. San Francisco,

CA: Jossey-Bass, 199-219.

Beck, Paul Allen, and M. Kent Jennings. 1975. “Parents as ‘Middlepersons’ in Political

Socialization.” Journal of Politics 37(1): 83-107.

Bolce, Louis, and Gerald De Maio. 2014. “The Evolution of the Religion Gap Metaphor in

the Language of American Political Journalists: 1987-2012.” Geolinguistics 39.

Brenner, Philip S. 2011. “Identity Importance and the Overreporting of Religious Service

Attendance: Multiple Imputation of Religious Attendance using American Time Use

Study and the General Social Survey.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 50(1):

103-115.

Brooks, Clem, and Jeff Manza. 1997. “Social Cleavages and Political Alignments: US

Presidential Elections, 1960 to 1992.” American Sociological Review 62(6): 937-946.

Campbell, Angus, Philip E. Converse, Warren E. Miller, and Donald E. Stokes. 1960. The

American Voter. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Chaffee, Steven H., Jack M. McLeod, and Daniel B. Wackman. 1973. “Family Communi-

cation Patterns and Adolescent Political Participation.” In Socialization to Politics, ed.

John L. Dennis. New York: Wiley.

Chang, Linchiat, and Jon A. Krosnick. 2009. “National Surveys via RDD Telephone Inter-

viewing Versus the Internet” Public Opinion Quarterly 73(4): 641-678.

Chaves, Mark A. 2011. American Religion: Contemporary Trends. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Chaves, Mark A. 1991. “Family Structure and Protestant Church Attendance: The Soci-

ological Basis of Cohort and Age Effects.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion

30(4): 501-514.

29

Page 31: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Clark, Cynthia A., and Everett L. Worthington. 1987. “Family Variables Affecting the

Transmission of Religious Values from Parents to Adolescents: A Review.” Family Per-

spectives 21(1): 1-21.

Delli Carpini, Michael X. 1989. “Age and History: Generations and Sociopolitical Change.”

In Political Learning in Adulthood: A Sourcebook of Theory and Research, ed. Roberta

S. Sigal. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Desmond, Scott A., Kristopher H. Morgan, and George Kikuchi. 2010. “Religious Devel-

opment: How (and Why) Does Religiosity Change from Adolescence to Adulthood?”

Sociological Perspectives 53(2): 247-270.

Dillon, Michele, and Paul Wink. 2007. In the Course of a Lifetime: Tracing Religious Belief,

Practice, and Change. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Dinges, William, Dean R. Hoge, Mary Johnson, and Juan L. Gonzales Jr. 1998. “A Faith

Loosely Held: The Institutional Allegiance of Young Catholics.” Commonwealth 17(1):

13-18.

Djupe, Paul A., and Christopher P. Gilbert. 2009. The Political Influence of Churches.

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Domke, David, and Kevin Coe. 2010. The God Strategy: How Religion Became a Political

Strategy. New York: Oxford University Press.

Ebaugh, Helen RF., and C. Allen Haney. 1978. “Church Attendance and Attitudes Toward

Abortion: Differentials in Liberal and Conservative Churches.” Journal for the Scientific

Study of Religion 17(4): 407-413.

Engs, Ruth C., and Kenneth Mullen. 1999. “The Effect of Religion and Religiosity on

Drug Use Among a Selected Sample of Post Secondary Students in Scotland.” Addiction

Research and Theory 7(2): 149-170.

Erickson, Joseph A. 1992. “Adolescent Religious Development and Commitment: A Struc-

tural Equation Model of the Role of Family, Peer Group, and Educational Influences.”

Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 31(2): 131-152.

30

Page 32: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Firebaugh, Glen, and Kevin Chen. 1995. “Vote Turnout of Nineteenth Amendment Women:

The Enduring Effect of Disenfranchisement.” American Journal of Sociology 100(4): 972-

996.

Gallup, George Jr., and Jim Castelli. 1989. The People’s Religion: American Faith in the

’90s. New York: Macmillan Press.

Gerber, Alan S., and Gregory A. Huber. 2010. “Partisanship, Political Control, and Eco-

nomic Assessments.” American Journal of Political Science 54(1): 153-173.

Gerber, Alan S., and Gregory A. Huber. 2009. “Partisanship and Economic Behavior: Do

Partisan Differences in Economic Forecasts Predict Real Economic Behavior?” American

Political Science Review 103(3): 407-26.

Goldscheider, Frances, and Calvin Goldscheider. 1999. The Changing Transition to Adult-

hood: Leaving and Returning Home. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc.

Green, Donald, Bradley Palmquist, and Eric Schickler. 2002. Partisan Hearts and Minds:

Political Parties and the Social Identities of Voters. New Haven, CT: Yale University

Press.

Green, John C. 2010. The Faith Factor: How Religion Influences American Elections. Wash-

ington, DC: Potomac Books, Inc.

Green, John C. 2006. The Values Campaign?: The Christian Right and the 2004 Elections.

Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

Hadaway, Kirk C., and Wade C. Roof. 1988. “Apostasy in American Churches: Evidence

from National Survey Data.” In Falling from the Faith: Causes and Consequences of

Religious Apostasy, ed. David Bromley. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Haidt, Jonathan and Marc J. Hetherington. 2012. “Look How Far We’ve Come Apart.” The

New York Times. Retrieved from: http://campaignstops.blogs.nytimes.com/2012/

09/17/

Hoge, Dean R., Benton Johnson, and Donald A. Luidens. 1993. “Determinants of Church

Involvement of Young Adults who Grew Up in Presbyterian Churches” Journal for the

31

Page 33: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Scientific Study of Religion 32(3): 242-255.

Hoge, Dean R., Greg H. Petrillo, and Ella I. Smith. 1982. “Transmission of Religious and

Social Values from Parents to Teenage Children.” Journal of Marriage and the Family

44(3): 569-580.

Hout, Michael, Clem Brooks, and Jeff Manza. 1995. “Class Struggle in the United States,

1948-1992.” Amerian Sociologial Review 60(6): 805-828.

Hout, Michael and Claude Fischer. 2002. “Why More Americans Have No Religious Prefer-

ence: Politics and Generations.” American Sociological Review 67(2): 165-190.

Hunsberger, Bruce, and Laurence B. Brown. 1984. “Religious Socialization, Apostasy, and

the Impact of Family Background.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 23(3):

239-251.

Hunter, James D. 1991. Culture Wars: The Struggle to Define America. New York: Basic.

Ingersoll-Dayton, Betit, Neal Krause, and David Morgan. 2002. “Religious Trajectories and

Transitions Over the Life Course.” International Journal of Aging and Human Develop-

ment 55(1): 51-70.

Iyengar, Shanto, Gaurav Sood, Yphtach Lelkes. 2012. “Affect, Not Ideology. A Social

Identity Perspective on Polarization.” Public Opinion Quarterly 76(3): 405-431.

Jennings, M. Kent, and Richard G. Niemi. 1981. Generations and Politics. Princeton, NJ:

Princeton University Press.

Jennings, M. Kent, and Richard G. Niemi. 1975. “Continuity and Change in Political

Orientations: A Longitudinal Study of Two Generations.” American Political Science

Review 69(4): 1316-1335.

Jennings, M. Kent, and Richard G. Niemi. 1974. The Political Character of Adolescence:

The Influence of Families and Schools. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Keele, Luke J. 2005. “The Partisan Roots of Trust in Government.” Journal of Politics

67(3): 432-451.

Kinder, Donald R., and Lynn M. Sanders. 1996. Divided by Color Chicago: Chicago

32

Page 34: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

University Press.

Layman, Geoffrey C. 1997. “Religion and Political Behavior in the United States: The Im-

pact of Beliefs, Affiliations, and Commitment from 1980-1994.” Public Opinion Quarterly

61(2): 288-316.

Lenz, Gabriel S. 2012. Follow the Leader? How Voters Respond to Politicians’ Performance

and Policies. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

Lim, Chaeyoon, Carol Ann MacGregor, and Robert D. Putnam. 2010. “Secular and Liminal:

Discovering Heterogeneity among Religious Nones.” Journal for the Scientific Study of

Religion 49(4): 596-618.

Lipset, Seymour M. 1981. Political Man: The Social Bases of Politics. Baltimore: Johns

Hopkins University Press.

Marrapodi, Erin. 2011. “Why Ralph Reed Matters.” CNN Retrieved from: http://

religion.blogs.cnn.com/2011/06/03/why-ralph-reed-matters/.

Moyers, Bill. 2012. “The Resurrection of Ralph Reed.” Moyers and Company. Retrieved

from: http://billmoyers.com/episode/full-show-the-resurrection-of-ralph-reed.

Myers, Scott M. 1996. “An Interactive Model of Religiosity Inheritance: The Importance of

Family Context.” American Sociological Review 61(3): 858-866.

Newport, Frank. 1979. “The Religious Switcher in the United States.” American Sociological

Review 44(4): 528-552.

Niemi, Richard G., and M. Kent Jennings. 1991. “Issues and Inheritance in the Formation

of Party Identification.” American Journal of Political Science 35(4): 970-988.

Nteta, Tatishe M., and Kevin J. Wallsten. 2012. “Preaching to the Choir? Religious Leaders

and American Opinion on Immigration Reform.” Social Science Quarterly 93(4): 891-

910.

Olson, Laura R., Wendy Cadge, and James T. Harrison. 2006. “Religion and Public Opinion

about Same-Sex Marriage.” Social Science Quarterly 87(2): 340-360.

Patrikios, Stratos. 2008. “American Republican Religion? Disentangling the Causal Link

33

Page 35: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

between Religion and Politics in the US.” Political Behavior 30(3): 367-389.

Perkins, Wesley, H. 1987. “Parental Religion and Alcohol Use Problems as Intergenerational

Predictors of Problem Drinking Among College Students.” Journal for the Scientific

Study of Religion 26(3): 340-357.

Petts, Richard J. 2009. “Trajectories of Religious Participation from Adolescence to Young

Adulthood.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 48(3): 552-571.

Putnam, Robet D., and David E. Campbell. 2010. American Grace: How Religion Divides

and Unites Us. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Roof, Wade C. 1993. A Generation of Seekers: The Spiritual Journeys of the Baby Boom

Generation. New York: Harper Collins.

Sances, Michael and Charles Stewart III. 2012. “Partisanship and Voter Confidence, 2000-

2010.” Working Paper.

Sandominsky, Sharon, and John Wilson. 1990. “Processes of Disaffiliation: Religious Mo-

bility among Men and Women.” Social Forces 68(4): 1211-1229.

Sears, David O. 1990. “Whither Political Socialization Research? The Question of Per-

sistence.” In Political Socialization, Citizenship, Education, and Democracy, ed. Orit

Ichikov. New York: Teachers College Press.

Sears, David O. 1983. “The Persistence of Early Political Predispositions: The Roles of

Attitude Object and Life Stage.” In Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Volume

4), eds. Phillip Shaver and Ladd Wheeler. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Sears, David O. 1975. “Political Socialization.” In Handbook of Political Science (Volume

2), eds. Fred I. Greenstein and Nelson W. Polsby. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Sears, David O., and Carolyn L. Funk. 1999. “Evidence of Long-Term Persistence of Adults’

Political Predispositions.” Journal of Politics 61(1): 1-28.

Sears, David O., and Nicholas A. Valentino. 1997. “Politics Matters: Political Events as

Catalysts for Preadult Socialization.” American Political Science Review 91(1): 45-65.

Smith, Christian. 2009. Souls in Transition: The Religions and Spiritual Lives of Emerging

34

Page 36: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Adults. New York: Oxford University Press.

Stolzenberg, Ross, M., Mary Blair-Loy, and Linda L. Waite. “Religious Participation in

Early Adulthood: Age and Family Life Cycle Effects on Church Membership.” American

Sociological Review 60(1): 84-104.

Stump, Roger W. 1984. “Regional Migration and Religious Commitment in the United

States.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 23(3): 292-303.

Tedin, Kent L. 1974. “The Influence of Parents on the Political Attitudes of Adolescents.”

American Political Science Review 68(4): 1579-1592.

Tesler, Michael. Forthcoming. “Priming Dispositions and Changing Policy Positions: An

Account of When Mass Opinion is Primed or Changed.” American Journal of Political

Science.

Tourangeau, Roger, J., Lance Rips, and Kenneth Rasinski. 2000. The Psychology of Survey

Response Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Uecker, Jeremy E., Mark D. Regnerus, and Margaret L. Vaaler. 2007. “Losing My Religion:

The Social Sources of Religious Decline in Early Adulthood.” Social Forces 85(4): 1667-

1692.

Vaillancourt, Pauline M. 1973. “Stability of Children’s Survey Responses.” Public Opinion

Quarterly 37(3): 373-387.

Verba, Sidney, Kay L. Schlozman, and Henry E. Brady. 1995. Voice and Equality: Civic

Voluntarism in American Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wald, Kenneth D., Dennis E. Owen, Samuel S. Hill, Jr. 1988. “Churches as Political

Communities.” American Political Science Review 82(2): 531-548.

Willits, Fern K., and Donald M. Crider. 1989. “Church Attendance and Traditional Religious

Beliefs in Adolescence and Young Adulthood: A Panel Study.” Review of Religious

Research 31(1): 68-81.

Wilson, John, and Darren E. Sherkat. 1994. “Returning to the Fold.” Journal for the

Scientific Study of Religion 33(2): 148-161.

35

Page 37: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Wuthnow, Robert. 2007. After the Baby Boomers: How Twenty- and Thirty-Somethings

Are Shaping the Future of American Religion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Zaleski, Ellen H., and Kathleen M. Schiaffino. 2000. “Religiosity and Sexual Risk-Taking

Behavior during the Transition to College.” Journal of Adolescence 23(2): 223-227.

36

Page 38: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Figures

Figure 1: Life cycle theory of religious and political attitudes

37

Page 39: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Figure 2: Experimental treatment effects varies by life cycle

a) Hypothetical

Democrats

Republicans

.6

.7

.8

.9

Rel

igio

us Id

entifi

catio

n

Control Treated

Example of null effect

Democrats

Republicans

.6

.7

.8

.9

Rel

igio

us Id

entifi

catio

n

Control Treated

Example of a treatment effect

b) Life cycle effects

Democrats

Republicans

New partisan gap = 0.14

.6

.7

.8

.9

Rel

igio

us Id

entifi

catio

n

Control Treated

Respondents with grown children

Democrats

Republicans

New partisan gap = -.04

.6

.7

.8

.9

Rel

igio

us Id

entifi

catio

n

Control Treated

Respondents with children at home

Note: Top panels are examples of null and treatment effects using hypothetical data. Bot-tom panels compare experimental treatment effects respondents with children at home andrespondents with grown children. Dependent variable is a four-point religious identificationscale that ranges from 0 (strong non-identifier) to 1 (strong identifier). Parametric testsconfirming raw results are in the Appendix. Data: Priming partisanship experiment

38

Page 40: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Figure 3: Partisans return to religion at different rates

a) 1965 to 1973

Democrats in 1965

Republicans in 1965

20

30

40

50

60

70

Perc

ent a

ttend

ing

chur

ch a

lmos

t wee

kly

1965 1973

Raw percentages over time

Change between Reps and Dems = -0.005-.09

-.06

-.03

0

.03

.06

.09

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 1

973

to 1

982

Democrat RepublicanParty ID in 1973

Partial residual plot

b) 1973 to 1982

Republicans in 1973

Democrats in 1973

20

30

40

50

Perc

ent a

ttend

ing

chur

ch a

lmos

t wee

kly

1973 1982

Raw percentages

Change between Reps and Dems = 0.07-.09

-.06

-.03

0

.03

.06

.09

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 1

973

to 1

982

Democrat RepublicanParty ID in 1973

Partial residual plot

Note: Top panels measure change in church attendance between 1965 and 1973. Bottompanels measure change in church attendance between 1973 and 1982. Left panels presentraw changes in the percentage of regular (almost every week or more) church attenders overtime. Right panels are partial residual plots that show the variation in church attendancedue to partisanship after accounting for previous levels of church attendance and controlvariables described in the text. Data: YPSP student generation

39

Page 41: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Figure 4: Parents’ church attendance rates are stable

1973 to 1982

Democrats in 1973

Republicans in 1973

20

30

40

50

60

70

Perc

ent a

ttend

ing

chur

ch a

lmos

t wee

kly

1973 1982

Raw percentages

Change between Reps and Dems = -0.02 (n.s.)-.09

-.06

-.03

0

.03

.06

.09

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 1

973

to 1

982

Democrat RepublicanParty ID in 1973

Partial residual plot

Note: Left panel presents raw changes in the percentage of regular (almost every week ormore) church attenders over time. Right panels are partial residual plots that show thevariation in church attendance due to partisanship after accounting for previous levels ofchurch attendance and control variables described in the text. Data: YPSP parent generation

40

Page 42: Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics ...dwh/margolis_workshop_june2015.pdf · Rethinking the Relationship between Religion and Politics A Test of the Life Cycle

Figure 5: Partisans with children at home produce religious gap

a) Full sample

Change between Reps and Dems = 0.01-.15

-.1

-.05

0

.05

.1

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 2

000

to 2

002

Democrat RepublicanParty ID, 2000

Before gay marriage became salient

Change between Reps and Dems = 0.04-.15

-.1

-.05

0

.05

.1

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 2

002

to 2

004

Democrat RepublicanParty ID, 2002

After gay marriage became salient

b) Respondents with children at home

Change between Reps and Dems = 0.003-.15

-.1

-.05

0

.05

.1

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 2

000

to 2

002

Democrat RepublicanParty ID, 2000

Before gay marriage became salient

Change between Reps and Dems = 0.14-.15

-.1

-.05

0

.05

.1

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 2

002

to 2

004

Democrat RepublicanParty ID, 2002

After gay marriage became salient

c) Respondents with grown children

Change between Reps and Dems = 0.007-.15

-.1

-.05

0

.05

.1

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 2

000

to 2

002

Democrat RepublicanParty ID, 2000

Before gay marriage became salient

Change between Reps and Dems = -0.006-.15

-.1

-.05

0

.05

.1

Chu

rch

atte

ndan

ce c

hang

e, 2

002

to 2

004

Party ID, 2002

After gay marriage became salient

Note: Three-wave test of partisanship influencing reported church attendance before andafter gay marriage became a salient issue in 2004. The graphs are partial residual scatterplotsand show the variation in church attendance due to partisanship after accounting for previouslevels of church attendance. Left panels test change between 2000 and 2002 and right panelstest change between 2002 and 2004. Panel A presents the results for the full sample, panelB shows changes among respondents with children under the age of 18, and panel C showschanges among respondents with children over the age of 18.

41