17
This article was downloaded by: [University of North Texas] On: 30 November 2014, At: 17:40 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The Curriculum Journal Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rcjo20 Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes Benő Csapó a a Department of Education , University of Szeged , Research Group on the Development of Competencies, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary Published online: 28 Jun 2007. To cite this article: Benő Csapó (2007) Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes, The Curriculum Journal, 18:2, 195-210, DOI: 10.1080/09585170701446044 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585170701446044 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions

Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

  • Upload
    beno

  • View
    212

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

This article was downloaded by: [University of North Texas]On: 30 November 2014, At: 17:40Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Curriculum JournalPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rcjo20

Research into learning to learnthrough the assessment of quality andorganization of learning outcomesBenő Csapó a

a Department of Education , University of Szeged , ResearchGroup on the Development of Competencies, Hungarian Academyof Sciences, HungaryPublished online: 28 Jun 2007.

To cite this article: Benő Csapó (2007) Research into learning to learn through the assessmentof quality and organization of learning outcomes, The Curriculum Journal, 18:2, 195-210, DOI:10.1080/09585170701446044

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585170701446044

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever orhowsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

Research into learning to learn through

the assessment of quality and organization

of learning outcomes

Ben}o Csapo*Department of Education, University of Szeged, Research Group on the Development of

Competencies, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary

This article examines the problem of learning to learn from the perspective of research on the

organization and quality of students’ knowledge. This approach is based on the assumption that

students’ learning competencies can be studied through the analysis of the outcomes of schooling.

The article synthesizes findings of a long-term research programme assessing the development of

components of propositional and procedural knowledge, and relationships between them. A model

was devised classifying indicators of school outcomes on four levels: (1) teachers’ grading;

(2) objective knowledge tests; (3) indicators of quality of knowledge (application, understanding);

and (4) higher order thinking skills. A set of tests and questionnaires were administered to Year 7

and Year 11 students in two consecutive projects; then, relationships between them and differences

between the two cohorts were analysed. As results indicate, a number of points in schooling should

be changed in order to improve schools’ impact on learning to learn, for example, inadequate

feedback given by grading, and shifting attention from reproductive to productive learning. The

instruments developed in this programme assess science application, science misconception,

mathematical understanding, historical reasoning, and inductive, deductive, spatial and critical

reasoning. They can also be utilized for monitoring learning to learn.

Keywords: Assessment; Learning to learn; Organization of knowledge; Quality of learning

outcomes; Thinking skills

Introduction

It is generally accepted that learning to learn is a complex concept (Hautamaki et al.,

2002). There is no single interpretation of it, as several views exist on how it should be

interpreted. Two main types of interpretation include a narrower and a broader one

(Waeytens et al., 2002). In this article I accept the broader interpretation and argue

*Department of Education, University of Szeged, Pet}ofi sgt. 30–34, 6722 Szeged, Hungary.

Email: [email protected]

The Curriculum Journal

Vol. 18, No. 2, June 2007, pp. 195 – 210

ISSN 0958-5176 (print)/ISSN 1469-3704 (online)/07/020195–16

ª 2007 British Curriculum Foundation

DOI: 10.1080/09585170701446044

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

that, in order to make educational systems more supportive, the learning processes in

which students are engaged should be modified at a number of points.

One of the most striking experiences of schooling is that students learn a lot at

school: they master a large body of knowledge but they are unable to apply it outside

their educational contexts. In many cases, the application of knowledge is limited to

the school subject in which it was acquired. This problem is not at all new. During the

long history of formal education, a perennial question has persisted: ‘how should we

teach our students in schools in order to provide them with valuable and applicable

knowledge?’ The Non scholae, sed vitae discimus! slogan has remained an ideal that has

never been reached, either in ancient or modern times. In modern societies, where

lifelong learning is a demand, the vision concerning the application of knowledge

should be extended to another dimension: schools are expected to prepare students to

be successful in a rapidly changing social environment; in brief, to apply their

knowledge in the unknown future.

The concept of ‘learning to learn’, as it is known in the literature, especially in the

early formulations of the idea, implies that students should learn more. On the other

hand, everyday experiences as well as rigorous investigations indicate that students

spend a large amount of time learning, and they are able to reproduce textbook

knowledge if it is measured by knowledge tests strictly based on curricula. Therefore,

in order to make students better at learning, the outcomes of school learning have to

be analysed, and those features of school practice which prevent schools from

accomplishing their expected mission in this domain have to be identified.

This approach was adopted by a group of researchers at the University of Szeged

when a long-term research programme was launched in the early 1990s with the aim

of a comprehensive evaluation of the outcomes of school learning. It began with

preliminary investigations and continued with two complex projects entitled ‘School

knowledge’, with major data-collection waves in 1995 and 1999. This was followed

by another wave to assess the development of abilities and skills most relevant in the

context of lifelong learning (1997–2002). These cross-sectional assessments were

carried out on nationally representative samples involving 5–10,000 students per

cohort.

The results and implications of these studies initiated further studies, with more

focused investigations of some crucial issues related to the quality of learning

outcomes. Finally, a long-term longitudinal study was launched in 2003 on nationally

representative samples in three cohorts to investigate causal relationships between

the characteristics of early development and success in later school learning. Although

the whole research programme is not directly aimed at studying the issue of

learning to learn, this body of interrelated investigations allows us to look into this

paradigm.

The present article provides an overview of the main findings from the perspective

of learning to learn. First, the projects related to the organization and quality of

students’ knowledge will be reviewed, and since these projects applied a complex

system of variables, they will be described in detail. Then, the most recent projects

and work in progress will be summarized, indicating how the focus of research has

196 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

shifted to integrate further questions of learning to learn. Finally, implications will be

discussed.

Theoretical and research background

Two main types of indicators signal problems with students’ abilities to learn. One

obvious sign is that children do not learn, or they do not learn enough. This kind of

problem is mainly related to the affective domain, mostly to interest and motivation.

The other, mostly cognitive problems are experienced when students actually learn—

in terms of spending a lot of time with learning—and they are able to reproduce what

they have learnt in certain conditions, but the result of learning is inappropriate; the

knowledge is quickly forgotten, good for nothing, or way below expectations. The two

types of problem are usually in causal relationship with each other. At the beginning

of schooling, most children are naturally interested in learning and motivated to

master new knowledge; however, if they are regularly forced to learn something

irrelevant and incomprehensible for them, they gradually lose motivation and interest.

Thus, the first problem that triggers research into the issue of learning to learn is

that students do not learn well. The problem with their knowledge—that is not trivial

and deserves scientific analysis—is not related to quantity of knowledge, but a

problem of quality. This issue has been conceptualized in several ways.

The research presented here was mostly influenced by theoretical frameworks

identifying two types of knowledge, procedural (abilities and skills) and propositional

(declarative, e.g. facts, information) knowledge, and the examination of their relation-

ships. It is generally assumed that one of the main functions of abilities and skills is

processing (e.g. analysing and organizing) declarative knowledge. Therefore, their

development is a precondition of successful learning and their low level hinders

learning and results in poor quality of knowledge.

Several research paradigms offer guidelines for the identification of the most

important skills and abilities which play a central role in organizing knowledge. The

psychometric tradition (with intelligence research at its core) emphasizes general

cognitive abilities (for a synthesis see Carroll, 1993). From this paradigm, those

general abilities that were supposed to have a major contribution to knowledge

accumulation and knowledge utilization in new situations were taken into account:

inductive reasoning and analogical reasoning. These abilities were studied in our

research programme and their development and functions were assessed in several

contexts. The development of inductive reasoning was assessed on smaller (Csapo,

1997a) and countrywide representative samples (Csapo, 2001) and in projects

examining the organization of knowledge.

Another important source are the Piagetian and Neo-Piagetian traditions; from

these a constructivist view of learning and some areas of operational reasoning was

adopted. Those operations that, under normal circumstances, appear at an early stage

of development are particularly important. Such operations are class inclusion,

seriation, classification, multiple classification and combinatorial operations and, in

the later stages of development, operations of propositional logic. These operations

Quality of learning outcomes 197

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

are essential in identifying elements of learning materials, discovering relationships

between them, and understanding the meaning of complex sentences; therefore, if

they are not developed well enough, meaningful conceptual learning is impossible.

(Several projects carried out in this paradigm are published in an edited volume by

Demetriou et al., 1992).

Some models attempt to integrate the results of these two paradigms and include a

third influential paradigm, the information-processing approach. One of the most

elaborated and empirically tested models is developed by Demetriou (2004).

One obvious implication of these relationships between procedural knowledge and

learning is that skills and abilities, essential for successful learning, should first be

enhanced, if they are not developed well enough when they are to be used. Many

developmental programmes were inspired by this simple relationship, acknowledging

the importance of preliminary knowledge in terms of abilities and skills. Improving

thinking and learning skills has long been considered a central mission of schooling

(see Glaser, 1984; Bransford et al., 1985); however, empirically tested methods rarely

progressed beyond the experimental phase.

Most research and developmental programmes concerning teaching and learning

thinking skills (see Hamers & Csapo, 1999) aimed at providing students with the

skills necessary for further learning. From our point of view, content-based training is

especially relevant, since it uses teaching materials for composing developmental

exercises. These techniques of devising training materials are often called enrichment,

infusion or embedding. Besides developing students’ crucial general abilities and

thinking skills, they improve understanding of the content of teaching materials

(Csapo, 1990, 1999). For example, the CASE programme (Cognitive Acceleration

through Science Education—see Adey & Shayer, 1994; Adey, 1999) utilizes science

materials for improving students’ general cognitive abilities.

Studies on improving students’ general abilities and some specific thinking skills

that play critical roles in school learning have direct consequences for learning to

learn. Our experiments aiming to develop students’ operational abilities (e.g. logical

and combinatorial reasoning) in the context of teaching disciplinary knowledge

(grammar and science in 4th grade, chemistry and physics in 7th grade) have clearly

shown that training empowers students with the skills necessary to comprehend the

structure of learning materials. Developing reasoning skills ‘on the spot’, in the

context of learning disciplinary knowledge, removes the obstacles that hinder

successful learning (Csapo, 1990, 1992, 1997b, 2003). For example, improving

students’ logical abilities in specific exercises using statements of the teaching

material helps them to understand sentences otherwise incomprehensible to students

whose logical abilities are not developed enough without training (Vidakovich, 1996).

In the context of learning to learn, ‘dynamic’ experimental studies complement

‘static’ research studying the structure and quality of knowledge in a more direct way.

This latter approach has often been conceptualized by distinguishing two different

kinds of learning that result in different quality of knowledge. The first is more

natural, often incidental, and it takes place in authentic contexts, based on interest

and intrinsic motivation, usually outside school or in other formal instructional

198 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

settings. Although the knowledge acquired in these circumstances may be limited, it

is immediately applicable in the appropriate context. The second type is school

learning, when students are supposed to master a large volume of knowledge, but

they go through the instructional processes along a prescribed agenda, independent of

their interest, motivation and, very often, of their prior knowledge.

A number of terms are used for this problematic process (e.g. rote learning), and its

compartmentalized result consisting of isolated elements called scholastic or inert

knowledge (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1985). This is far from the ideal of meaningful

conceptual learning building knowledge on proper prior knowledge and deep

structural understanding.

In our research we coined the term school knowledge for the outcomes of school

instruction. The aim of the programme was to examine what school knowledge

comprises in reality and where real learning processes on the rote learning—

meaningful learning scale can be placed. Results have been presented at several

conferences (Csapo, 1996, 1997c) and published in journal articles and in two

comprehensive volumes in Hungarian (Csapo, 1998a, 2002). In this article

I summarize the results of this research and draw its conclusions for learning to learn.

Studies on the quality and organization of students’ knowledge:

framework, research questions and overall design of projects

These projects intended to devise methods and instruments to characterize the

quality of school learning and to identify areas where improvement is most needed;

therefore, a large system of variables was applied. We developed a framework that

organizes the indicators of the outcomes of schooling into a system of variables that

describe students’ knowledge at four levels.

At the first level, grades given by the teachers were collected. These are indicators

of students’ achievements that both students and their parents first face. Depending

on the general culture of schooling, the preparation of teachers and the evaluation

technique available, the reliability of grading practice may vary to a great extent. It

may depend on local school values and the subjective value judgement of the

individual teachers. This is the most important feedback that orients students in their

learning, shapes their self-concept, attitudes and motivation, and influences parents’

decisions concerning their children’s future. Therefore, the first research question

was related to the quality of feedback provided by teachers’ grading. This was

conducted by correlating grades with other variables.

At the second level, objective knowledge tests of school subjects were applied.

Since the same tests were administered to all participants, these data resulted in

indicators that are independent from the local variety of school expectations, value

systems and teachers’ subjectivity. These tests were based on school curricula and

mapped the knowledge represented in textbooks and other learning materials into

tests. Since they asked questions in the context of a given subject in the same way

in which students are usually tested and are expected to demonstrate their knowledge

at school, their results are indicators of how well schools meet their external goals.

Quality of learning outcomes 199

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

The second research question is related to the knowledge tests: how much subject

matter students master at school and how it is distributed over students and schools

(i.e. how large are the differences between students and schools).

At the third level, tests go beyond the immediate reproduction of subject matter

knowledge. They examine the quality of their knowledge: how consistent it is, how

well students understand what they have learnt, how they can apply it in new contexts

that are different from where it was mastered. These tests were constructed on the

basis of general claims concerning schooling, stating that schools should provide

students with knowledge broadly applicable beyond school in private and later

professional life. In relation to this issue, the third research question examined the

quality of knowledge mastered at school.

At the fourth level, students’ general cognitive skills (and some learning-related

affective attributes) were assessed. First of all, those thinking skills were included that

play an important role in learning, understanding, knowledge transfer and application

processes, and that are important in self-development. For the two latter levels,

theoretical resources and empirical results of modern cognitive research were utilized.

To place the problem into a developmental context, the assessments were carried

out in two age groups. In the framework of a cross-sectional design (beyond the

subject matter specific knowledge tests), the same tests (the third and fourth levels)

were administered to Year 7 (age 13) and Year 11 (age 17) students. These two

cohorts represent crucial points of schooling, since they precede the closing phase of

primary and secondary education by one year. During the four years between these

two points students are supposed to master a great deal of learning materials, and

schools have a lot of time and opportunities to develop students’ applicable

knowledge and general skills. The fourth research question is related to changes in the

quality of knowledge and development of general skill; it can be answered by

comparing the results of the two age groups.

Both projects—the one in mathematics and sciences and the other in the

humanities—were carried out by a research team, where each member was

responsible for certain areas—developing the instruments and performing in depth

analyses of the given area. Beyond the main research questions listed above, a number

of other particular issues were also examined. The main publications that resulted

from these specific analyses will be cited in connection with the instruments.

Methods

Participants

The samples for the projects were drawn from Szeged and its metropolitan area.

Szeged (about 150,000 inhabitants) is one of the largest towns in the south-east of

Hungary, with diverse primary and secondary schools and higher educational

institutions. National surveys show that student achievements in the schools of

Szeged and its neighbourhood are close to the national average and sometimes

surpass it.

200 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 8: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

For the study of age differences, samples were selected from two age groups. The

Year 7 sample was drawn from the elementary schools and it was considered

representative for the whole population of this age group. The Year 11 sample was

proportionally selected from two types of four-year secondary schools (grammar

schools and comprehensive schools) that prepare students for entry to higher

education. Since vocational school students were not involved in the survey, the 11th

grade sample represents only students attending four-year high schools. The number

of students in each sample exceeded 500.

Sampling units were whole classes proportionally selected from the schools of the

area for a proper representation of different school types. School classes as units were

also used in some data analyses. Sampling and data analysis procedures were identical

in the two consecutive studies and in both cohorts.

Instruments and design of data collection

Due to limitations in educational research (students can be tested only in a few areas,

in a relatively short period), data were collected in two phases using the same model.

One focused on mathematics and science (MS); and another examined humanities

and social studies (HS). Some instruments and questionnaires were used in both

surveys in order to check the stability of findings and the consistency of results,

whereas other instruments were specific for the given project. In the following

description of the instruments the acronyms MS and HS indicate the projects in

which instruments were applied in the survey. Figure 1 summarizes the system of

variables for the MS study, while Figure 2 visualizes the HS study. Besides the tests, a

general questionnaire was also administered to students to collect an extensive set of

background data.

Grades (MS, HS). In Hungary a five-point scale is used for grading students’ school

achievements: 1 means ‘fail’ and 5 is ‘excellent’. At the end of each semester, students

Figure 1. The system of variables in the mathematics and science (MS) project

Quality of learning outcomes 201

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 9: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

receive a summative grade in all school subjects that reflects their global achievement

during that period. These grades were collected in all main subjects (mathematics,

chemistry, physics, biology, Hungarian literature, Hungarian grammar, history and

foreign languages).

Knowledge tests. These tests were summative measurement tools covering students’

knowledge mastered during a school year. They were compiled by independent

experts and were administered at the end of the school year. Mathematics (MS),

chemistry (MS), physics (MS), biology (MS), Hungarian literature (HS), history

(HS) and English as a foreign language (HS) tests were administered in the surveys.

The main principle of constructing knowledge tests was that they measured students’

knowledge in the same contexts where they mastered it.

Understanding and application. These instruments were designed to assess the quality

of knowledge: how well it was understood and could be applied. The tests were

designed by utilizing a number of recent cognitive issues (e.g. conceptual change,

science misconceptions, history misconceptions, modes of representation, transfer of

knowledge). At this level we administered a Science application test (MS) validated and

used previously (see B. Nemeth & Csapo, 1996; Csapo, 1997a; B. Nemeth, 1998,

2000). It consists of thirty-five items that can be solved by applying knowledge of

everyday problems learned in science subjects. The Mathematics comprehension test

(MS) contains problems that can be solved only if students have a deeper

understanding of corresponding areas of mathematics (Dobi, 1998). A Science

misconceptions task battery (MS) was used to study students’ conceptual development

and conceptual consistency; from among the seven tasks of the battery, two were

borrowed from the literature and five were devised for our studies (Korom & Csapo,

1996; Korom, 1997a, 1997b, 1998, 2000). As these different tasks do not cover a

consistent body of knowledge, they do not form a single test. However, relationships

between the achievements on these tasks were strong enough to represent them as a

single variable in further computations.

Figure 2. The system of variables in the humanities and social sciences (HS) project

202 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 10: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

The History-related reasoning test (HS) comprised twelve tasks and was constructed

according to similar principles (Szebenyi & Vass, 2002). These instruments were

designed to examine students’ misconceptions and were evaluated both qualitatively

and quantitatively. A questionnaire was administered to examine students’ text

production strategies (HS) and participants wrote a composition (Text creation test—

HS) that was analysed by both qualitative and quantitative techniques (E. K. Molnar,

2000, 2002). An English language competence (HS) test measured students’ language

skills in lifelike contexts (e.g. a letter-writing task; see Bukta & Nikolov, 2002). The

Visual arts test battery (Spatial reasoning test, Visual environmental test, Art taste

test—HS) includes a number of tasks tapping into spatial abilities and recognition of

styles (Karpati, 2002).

Higher order thinking skills, motivation, self-concept. The Inductive reasoning test (MS)

consists of a verbal analogy, a number analogy and a numbers series sub-tests

(Csapo, 1997a, 1998b). In the HS project only verbal analogy sub-scales were used.

The Deductive reasoning test (MS) involves the basic operations of propositional

logic (Vidakovich, 1996, 1998). The Probabilistic/correlative reasoning test (MS)

examines how children perceive probabilistic relationships (Ban, 1998), whereas the

Critical thinking test (HS) requires critical and evaluation-type reasoning (L. Molnar,

2002).

Background variables. A questionnaire about participants’ parental education, self-

esteem, future plans and other factors that may influence their achievements was

administrated to each sample (MS, HS), while two further questionnaires enquired

into students’ motivation and academic self-concept (Jozsa, 2002).

Results and discussion

The two projects resulted in a rich description of the quality of school knowledge.

I will summarize some of the relevant findings according to the research

questions.

Immediate feedback that orients students’ learning: characteristics of teachers’ grading

practices

This included the way students learn to learn at school, that their study habits change

over time and that their learning-related attitudes develop in strong relationship with

feedback received from their teachers. Since grades play a determining role in

students’ careers, teachers’ grading practices were analysed at three levels.

In the student-level analyses, the correlations were computed between students’

grades in school subjects and other variables. These analyses showed what the real

basis of teachers’ grading was. The grades correlated with achievement test results to

varying degrees: they were acceptably high in mathematics and sciences (0.5–0.6),

but quite low in social studies; the lowest correlation was found in history (0.28) and

Quality of learning outcomes 203

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 11: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

literature (0.25) in Year 11. Much lower, in many cases insignificant correlations,

were found with other cognitive variables, including ones indicating understanding or

applicability of knowledge in the same area; for example, in Year 11 the correlation

between chemistry grade and science application test was low (0.23—see B. Nemeth,

1998), whereas no relationship was found between history grade and historical

reasoning (70.05—see Szebenyi & Vass, 2002).

Between-class analyses indicated large differences between school subjects

(correlations were computed between means of classes: see Table 1). For example,

Year 11 mathematics teachers grade their students very similarly, as the correlation

between the class means of mathematics grade and mathematic test result is high,

while in other subjects (e.g. in Year 7 history and in Year 11 literature) teachers’

grading is less consistent.

Within-class correlation coefficients (correlations were computed for every single

class) were much lower than the above ones. These data indicate that many teachers

lack a professional value system that would give a firm basis for evaluating students’

knowledge.

Distribution of students’ subject matter knowledge

Results of knowledge tests indicate that students mastered a large portion of the

materials represented in their textbooks; the means usually varied around 50 per cent.

These results confirm the hypothesis that students learn what they are expected to

learn.

On the other hand, there were large variations between students, and the way they

differ from each other indicates several problems. Large differences were found

between the means of school classes, and also between students according to the

level of education of their parents and other indicators of family socio-economic

status. These results were later confirmed by the PISA (OECD, 2004) and inter-

national comparisons indicate that these types of differences are especially high in

Hungary, therefore problems behind this phenomenon are more serious than in other

countries.

Table 1. The correlation coefficients between the means of grades and knowledge test results

Subject Year 7 Year 11

Biology 0.67 –

Physics 0.59 0.61

Chemistry 0.63 –

Mathematics 0.75 0.92

English as a foreign language 0.52 0.58

Literature 0.46 0.25

History 0.27 0.42

Source: Data are summarized from Csapo, 1998a, 2002.

204 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 12: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

The major impact of family background means small school impact, and from the

point of view of learning to learn, this indicates that students’ learning is probably less

shaped by schools than by other environmental factors.

The quality of students’ knowledge: application, understanding and thinking skills

A detailed analysis of the other cognitive test revealed further problems that can be

related to students’ inadequate learning. For example, the science application test

indicates that students can hardly interpret some basic scientific facts in everyday

contexts (B. Nemeth, 1998). School learning fails to replace misconceptions with

sound scientific knowledge (Korom, 1998; Szebenyi & Vass, 2002). Mathematics is

not well understood and students cannot solve simple problems if they go beyond the

well-practised routines, even if they are simpler than the regular textbook problems

(Dobi, 1998). Students cannot solve tasks embedded in communicative situations,

although they perform well on vocabulary and grammar tests (Bukta & Nikolov,

2002).

Analyses that studied the relationships between the cognitive variables, for example

cluster analyses, indicated that students’ knowledge is divided into two independent

bodies: one mastered and applied within school learning contexts and another one

originating from everyday experiences. The first one is correct from an academic,

disciplinary point of view, but not usable outside the school context, and the other is

more pragmatic but often leads to false generalizations; transfer is limited in both

cases. Correlational analyses also revealed the major role inductive and, especially,

analogical reasoning play in learning.

These results indicate a number of inadequacies in students’ learning that may be

related to inefficiencies in school instruction that condition students for memoriza-

tion and reproductive learning. These indications of poor learning strategies by

Hungarian students were later also confirmed by the PISA study (OECD, 2003).

The impact of schooling on the development of general abilities and thinking skills

The most striking results of these projects were the ones that revealed how slowly

general cognitive skills develop; some of them even decrease during the years of

schooling. The results of some tests (identical for the two cohorts) are summarized in

Table 2.

The proportion of changes over the four years of schooling varies broadly from test

to test. In general, development is very slow. In two cases (correlative reasoning and

visual environment), changes are negative. The more students study at school, the

less they are able to recognize and accept probabilistic relationships.

Effects of schooling on affective variables

Another negative tendency was observable concerning affective variables. Three sets

of data were available in these studies on affective variables. The background

Quality of learning outcomes 205

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 13: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

questionnaire contained questions on how much students liked to learn each subject.

In most cases, students in Year 11 showed more negative attitudes than students in

Year 7; English as a foreign language was the only exception. This problem was later

examined with the same technique over a broader age range (from Year 5 to Year 11),

and the results were similar: physics is the least, chemistry the second least popular

subject (Csapo, 2000). Both mathematics and biology are more popular, so there

must be something wrong with the teaching of these two science subjects. The

negative attitudes towards chemistry and physics indicate that students are forced to

learn them without finding pleasure in them. Such practices are damaging for

learning to learn; therefore, these distracting features should be identified and

eliminated.

In the HS study, a mastery motivation test was administered to both cohorts. The

same negative tendency was observed: Year 11 students were consistently less

motivated than their younger schoolmates (Jozsa, 2002).

General conclusions and implications for learning to learn and areas

for further research

In our interpretation, the problems addressed by the issues of learning to learn are

related to the quality, and not to the quantity, of the knowledge students master at

school. On the other hand, these problems can be conceptualized so that the quality

of knowledge can be characterized by quantitative measurable indicators. Such

indicators have to be related to the understanding, organization, transferability

and applicability of knowledge. Furthermore, learning results in better knowledge

if it is supported by an elaborated affective system—which can also be characterized

by quantitative indicators. This approach implies that learning competencies are

not mastered in a single straightforward way, but that they are shaped by a broad

range of developmental influences to which learners are exposed during their

activities in and out of school. In the projects presented here, we have identified a

number of problems related to the quality of learning experiences and the accuracy

Table 2. Performance of the two age groups on some tests (in % of the maximum test score)

Test Year 7 Year 11

Science application 33 55

Misconception 50 64

Mathematics understanding 34 47

Inductive reasoning 45 64

Deductive reasoning 61 76

Correlative reasoning 57 54

Spatial reasoning 49 53

Visual environment 74 68

Critical thinking 33 40

Source: Data are summarized from Csapo, 1998a, 2002.

206 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 14: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

and adequacy of feedback students receive from their school environment—

predominantly, their teachers.

Since the problems which hinder meaningful conceptual learning that results in

well-understood transferable knowledge are manifold, there is not a single solution

to treat them. Teaching and learning processes in school must be changed in a

number of ways. One major problem is that skills, especially students’ general

information processing skills, are not practised in the context of the learning of school

subjects. Therefore, the experiments on developing thinking skills using the embedded

approach have been extended to other areas. For example, analogical reasoning was

developed in biology materials, and the effects of training were also detected in terms

of deeper understanding and better mastery of disciplinary knowledge (Nagy, 2006).

Further experiments are in progress to improve younger students’ inductive reasoning

skills.

Further research is needed into the application of knowledge in new situations.

Based on the models of knowledge transfer and the interpretation of complex

problem solving (CPS) as application of existing knowledge to novel situations,

assessment instruments were devised that can be applied in the context of teaching

disciplinary knowledge. Such a test battery was developed by using science and

mathematics materials (Gy. Molnar, 2001, 2006). As for improving CPS, experi-

ments are being prepared for problem-based learning.

Another set of problems concerns the organization of propositional knowledge,

concept development and misconceptions. Previous assessments were extended and

further instruments were devised to monitor concept development and to promote

conceptual change (Korom, 2005). Further research was initiated to apply concept

maps both for assessing and representing students’ knowledge structures and to

devise training exercises to make their learning more meaningful (Habok, 2004). An

experiment is presently in progress in this area.

As was realized at several points in the research, a major difficulty is related to

the evaluation of students’ knowledge. To solve this problem a number of instru-

ments have been devised to assess the quality of students’ knowledge and to monitor

their development. To improve the feedback students receive from their teachers,

we should further differentiate and enrich the evaluation technique and instru-

ments; thus, a variety of formative and diagnostic tests is needed. Furthermore,

since teachers are the most important resources of school instruction, better

preparation of teachers and more elaborated knowledge about student learning is

required.

Finally, empirical studies have resulted in a theoretical generalization that

synthesizes different approaches to identify and classify the goals of schooling and

the organization of knowledge (Csapo, 2004).

Acknowledgements

The work presented here is part of a long-term project of the Research Group on the

Development of Competencies (MTA-SZTE Kepessegkutato Csoport). The data

Quality of learning outcomes 207

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 15: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

collection was supported by the Hungarian National Science Foundation (OTKA

T018577, T030555 and 46659).

References

Adey, P. (1999) Thinking science: science as a gateway to general thinking ability, in:

J. H. M. Hamers, J. E. H. van Luit & B. Csapo (Eds) Teaching and learning thinking skills

(Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger), 63–80.

Adey, P. & Shayer, M. (1994) Really raising standards: cognitive intervention and academic achievement

(London, Routledge).

Ban, S. (1998) Gondolkodas a bizonytalanrol: valoszın}usegies korrelatıv gondolkodas [Reasoning

about the uncertain: probabilistic and correlational reasoning], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai

tudas [School knowledge] (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 221–250.

B. Nemeth, M. (1998) Iskolai es hasznosıthato tudas. A termeszettudomanyos ismeretek

alkalmazasa [School knowledge and usable knowledge. Application of science knowledge],

in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai tudas (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 115–138.

B. Nemeth, M. (2000) A termeszettudomanyos ismeretek alkalmazasa [Application of science

knowledge], Iskolakultura, 10(8), 60–68.

B. Nemeth, M. & Csapo, B. (1996) Learning for the schools or learning for life?, poster presented at

‘The Growing Mind’, International Conference in Honour of the Centennial of Jean Piaget’s Birth,

Geneva, Switzerland, September.

Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1985) Cognitive coping strategies and the problem of ‘inert

knowledge’, in: S. F. Chipman, J. W. Segal & R. Glaser (Eds) Thinking and learning skills,

vol. 2 (Hillside, N.J., LEA).

Bransford, J. D., Arbitman-Sith, R., Stein, B. S. & Vye, N. J. (1985) Improving thinking and

learning skills: an analysis of three approaches, in: J. W. Segal, S. F. Chipman & R. Glaser

(Eds) Thinking and learning skills, vol. 1 (Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates),

133–206.

Bukta, K. & Nikolov, M. (2002) Nyelvtanıtas es hasznos nyelvtudas [Language teaching and usable

language knowledge], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai m}uveltseg (Budapest, Osiris Kiado),

169–193.

Carroll, J. B. (1993) Human cognitive abilities (Cambridge, Cambridge University Press).

Csapo, B. (1990) Integrating the development of the operational abilities of thinking and the

transmission of knowledge, in: H. Mandl, E. De Corte, N. Bennett & H. F. Friedrich (Eds)

Learning and instruction. European research in an international context, vol. 2.2, Analysis of

complex skills and complex knowledge domains (Oxford, Pergamon Press), 85–94.

Csapo, B. (1992) Improving operational abilities in children, in: A. Demetriou, M. Shayer &

A. Efklides (Eds) Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development. Implications and applications for

education (London, Routledge), 144–159.

Csapo, B. (1996) Educating the mind: what school does and does not do, paper presented in the

symposium, ‘Shaping the mind through education’, ‘The Growing Mind’, International

Conference in Honour of the Centennial of Jean Piaget’s Birth, Geneva, Switzerland, September.

Csapo, B. (1997a) Development of inductive reasoning: cross-sectional measurements in an

educational context, International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20(4), 609–626.

Csapo, B. (1997b) Operational enrichment: improving operational reasoning through the content

of teaching, in: J. H. M. Hamers & M. Th. Overtoom (Eds) Teaching thinking in Europe.

Inventory of European programmes (Utrecht, Sardes), 235–239.

Csapo, B. (1997c) The validity of school knowledge, in: R. H. Lehmann, G. Venter, J. van Buer, S.

Seeber & R. Peck (Eds) Erweiterte Autonomie fur Schule. Bildungscontrolling and Evaluation

(Studien zur Wirtschafts- und Erwachsenenpadagogik aus der Humboldt-Universitat zu

Berlin), Band 13, 2, 129–140.

208 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 16: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

Csapo, B. (Ed.) (1998a) Az iskolai tudas [School knowledge] (Budapest, Osiris Kiado).

Csapo, B. (1998b) Az uj tudas kepz}odesenek eszkozei: az induktıv gondolkodas [Tools for creating

new knowledge: inductive reasoning], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai tudas (Budapest, Osiris

Kiado), 251–280.

Csapo, B. (1999) Improving thinking through the content of teaching, in: J. H. M. Hamers,

J. E. H. van Luit & B. Csapo (Eds) Teaching and learning thinking skills (Lisse, Swets &

Zeitlinger), 37–62.

Csapo, B. (2000) A tantargyakkal kapcsolatos attit}udok osszefuggesei [Students’ attitudes towards

school subjects], Magyar Pedagogia, 100(3), 343–366.

Csapo, B. (2001) Az induktıv gondolkodas fejl}odesenek elemzese orszagos reprezentatıv felmeres

alapjan [An analysis of the development of inductive reasoning on the basis of a large-scale

survey], Magyar Pedagogia, 101(3), 373–391.

Csapo, B. (Ed.). (2002) Az iskolai m}uveltseg [Literacy at school] (Budapest, Osiris Kiado).

Csapo, B. (2003) A kepessegek fejl}odese es iskolai fejlesztese [The development of competencies and their

improvement in a school context] (Budapest, Akademiai Kiado).

Csapo, B. (2004) Knowledge and competencies, in: J. Letschert (Ed.) The integrated person. How

curriculum development relates to new competencies (Enschede, CIDREE), 35–49.

Demetriou, A. (2004) Mind, intelligence, and development: a general cognitive, differential, and

developmental theory of the mind, in: A. Demetriou & A. Raftopoulos (Eds) Developmental

change: theories, models and measurement (Cambridge, Cambridge University Press), 21–73.

Demetriou, A., Shayer, M. & Efklides, A. (Eds) (1992) Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive

development. Implications and applications for education (London, Routledge).

Dobi, J. (1998) Megtanult es megertett matematikatudas [Mathematics knowledge mastered and

understood], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai m}uveltseg (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 169–190.

Glaser, R. (1984) Education and thinking. The role of knowledge, American Psychologist, 39(2),

93–104.

Habok, A. (2004) A tanulas tanulasa az ertelemgazdag tudas elsajatıtasa erdekeben [Learning to

learn in order to acquire meaningful knowledge], Magyar Pedagogia, 104(4), 443–470.

Hamers, J. H. M. & Csapo, B. (1999) Teaching thinking, in: J. H. M. Hamers, J. E. H. van Luit &

B. Csapo (Eds) Teaching and learning thinking skills (Lisse, Swets & Zeitlinger), 11–36.

Hautamaki, J., Arinen P., Eronen, S., Hautamaki, A., Kupiainen, S., Lindblom, B., Niemivirta, N.,

Pakaslahti L., Rantanen, P. & Scheinin, P. (2002) Assessing learning-to-learn: a framework

(Helsinki, Helsinki University).

Jozsa, K. (2002) Tanulasi motivacio es human m}uveltseg [Mastery motivation and literacy], in:

B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai m}uveltseg (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 239–268.

Karpati, A. (2002) A vizualis m}uveltseg [Visual literacy], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai m}uveltseg

(Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 91–133.

Korom, E. (1997a) Naiv elmeletek es tevkepzetek megjelenese a termeszettudomanyos fogalmak

tanulasa soran [Naive theories and misconceptions in learning science], Magyar Pedagogia,

97(1), 19–41.

Korom, E. (1997b) The effects of science education on children’s naive theories, paper presented at

the 7th European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Athens, Greece, August.

Korom, E. (1998) Az iskolai tudas es a hetkoznapi tapasztalat ellentmondasai: termeszettudoma-

nyos tevkepzetek [Contradictions of school knowledge and everyday experience: science

misconceptions], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai tudas (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 139–167.

Korom, E. (2000) A fogalmi valtas elmeletei [Theories of conceptual change], Magyar Pszichologiai

Szemle, 55(2–3), 179–205.

Korom E. (2005) Fogalmi feijl}odes es fogalmi valtas [Concept development and conceptual change]

(Budapest, M}uszaki Kiado).

Korom, E. & Csapo, B. (1996) Development of children’s misconceptions of the world, poster

presented at ‘The Growing Mind’, International Conference in Honour of the Centennial of Jean

Piaget’s Birth, Geneva, Switzerland, September.

Quality of learning outcomes 209

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 17: Research into learning to learn through the assessment of quality and organization of learning outcomes

Molnar, E. K. (2000) A fogalmazasi kepesseg fejlodesenek merese [Measuring the development of

composition skills], Iskolakultura, 10(8), 49–59.

Molnar, E. K. (2002) Az ırasbeli szovegalkotas [Creation of written texts], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az

iskolai muveltseg (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 193–216.

Molnar, Gy. (2001) Az eletszer}u feladat-helyzetekben torten}o problemamegoldas vizsgalata

[Examination of problem solving in real life situations], Magyar Pedagogia, 101(3), 347–373.

Molnar, Gy. (2006) Tudastranszfer es komplex problemamegoldas [Knowledge transfer and complex

problem solving] (Budapest, Muszaki Kiado).

Molnar, L. (2002) A kritikai gondolkodas [Critical thinking], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai tudas

(Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 217–237.

Nagy, L. (2006) Az analogias gondolkodas fejlesztese [Developing analogical reasoning] (Budapest,

M}uszaki Konyvkiado).

OECD (2003) Learners for life. Student approaches to learning. Results from PISA 2000 (Paris, OECD).

OECD (2004) Learning for tomorrow’s world. First results from PISA 2003 (Paris, OECD).

Szebenyi, P. & Vass, V. (2002) Tortenelmi tevkepzetek, tortenelemszemlelet, nemzeti azonossag-

tudat [History misconceptions and national identity], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai m}uveltseg

(Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 135–167.

Vidakovich, T. (1996) Development of deductive reasoning, poster presented at ‘The Growing

Mind’, International Conference in Honour of the Centennial of Jean Piaget’s Birth, Geneva,

Switzerland, September.

Vidakovich, T. (1998) Tudomanyos es hetkoznapi logika: a tanulok deduktıv gondolkodasa

[Scientific and everyday logic: students’ deductive reasoning], in: B. Csapo (Ed.) Az iskolai

tudas (Budapest, Osiris Kiado), 191–220.

Waeytens, K., Lens, W. & Vandenberghe, R. (2002) ‘Learning to learn’: teachers’ conceptions of

their supporting role, Learning and Instruction, 12, 305–322.

210 B. Csapo

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f N

orth

Tex

as]

at 1

7:40

30

Nov

embe

r 20

14