130
Qigong: Acute affective responses

Qigong: Acute affective responses

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Qigong: Acute affective responses

“Qigong and emotion/mood”.

Örebro Studies in Sport Sciences 4

Mattias Johansson

Qigong: Acute affective responsesin a group of regular exercisers

© Mattias Johansson, 2009

Title: Qigong: Acute affective responsesin a group of regular exercisers

Publisher: Örebro University 2009www.publications.oru.se

Editor: Heinz [email protected]

Printer: Intellecta Infolog, V Frölunda 04 /2009

issn 1654-7535 isbn 978-91-7668-665-2

ABSTRACT

Qigong is a Chinese mind-body therapy that aims to, through the use of movements,

relaxed breathing and relaxation/meditation, create a healthy flow of life force, qi, in

the body, and consequently improve health. A growing number of Qigong studies

report beneficial effects on health and well-being. However, little attention has fo-

cused on the acute affective responses that accompany single sessions of Qigong ex-

ercise. The aim of the present thesis was therefore to study affective reactions to

Qigong exercise. In Study I, the effects of Qigong exercise on mood and anxiety were

compared to a control group. Results showed partial support for the superiority of

Qigong exercise compared to controls. In Study II, different lengths of session time

were compared, resulting in similar affective benefits for the 30 and 60-minute ses-

sions. In Study III, affective responses were also assessed during the session, using

mean scores and individual responses. Results showed an increase toward greater

Activated and Deactivated Pleasantness during the session, with the greatest changes

at the end of the bout. The majority of individuals reported increased Pleasantness

during the Qigong session. Expectations of positive outcomes were significantly as-

sociated with only few affective responses. Responses to open-ended questions of af-

fective experiences displayed affective reactions mostly toward greater Deactivated

Pleasantness. This thesis contributes to a greater understanding of the limited area of

Qigong-related affective responses. For the exercisers, Qigong is associated with a

greater momentary emotional state. However, due to the highly select group of regu-

lar Qigong exercisers, generalizing the results outside the sample population is lim-

ited. Theories on active mechanisms in the Qigong-affect relationship, and results

from studies of affective responses to similar activities, suggest that other groups of

people would also benefit affectively from Qigong exercise. Given the many benefits

of positive affect, Qigong exercise may also pose great promises for the enhancement

of other areas related to health and well-being. This calls for additional studies.

Key words: Qigong, affective responses, mind-body therapy, low-intensive physical

activity, affect regulation

3

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

Writing a doctoral thesis is like climbing a mountain. Starting out easily, it then

gradually becomes steeper and more difficult, until you are finally standing on top of

the mountain. You sit down and feel proud of yourself, having managed to reach the

top. Then you look up and see that what you thought was the top of the mountain

was just a cliff, and that the mountain continues further and further up into the sky.

So you have to keep on climbing… During my years of PhD work, there have been

hardships and struggles, followed by times of accomplishments and relaxation, then

again followed by new difficult challenges… These cycles are repeated (like in all

parts of life), but gradually it does get easier – you find new tools that simplify the

tasks, and the times of struggle don’t last as long as they used to. Having reached

one of the tops (still not aware of the next top!), I feel proud and satisfied. But no

matter whether you choose to climb a mountain or write a PhD thesis, you always

need friends and colleagues who support and encourage you to keep going. Without

them, the task would be nearly impossible and much less fun.

First I would like to thank my supervisor, Peter Hassmén, who has continuously

supported me, giving me instant feedback on my work, challenging me, and pushing

me into becoming an independent researcher. Peter, I am grateful for your encour-

agement and support when I was struggling, and for you believing in me when I

couldn’t find the way up the mountain.

I would also like to thank my assistant supervisor, Anna Hertting, who has shared

her knowledge and supported me in the latter part of my studies. Returning from our

talks I always feel inspired, full of new ideas, and happy about being a researcher.

Anna, I have especially enjoyed our talks on mind-body therapies, where you have

shared your knowledge.

Naturally, the thesis could not have been completed without the participants in the

three studies. Thank you for taking part in my studies. I am also grateful to the

4

Green Dragon, the member association of Biyun Qigong in Sweden. First I thank

Qigong master Fan Xiulan for sharing her great knowledge of Qigong, and letting

me conduct research on her method. I am also grateful to Niko Nygren, Ewa Den-

ward-Olah, Göran Jacobson, and Helena Norman, who have been of valuable assis-

tance and who have shared their knowledge of Qigong.

Being part of the PhD student group in Sport Sciences at Örebro University has been

so much fun. Thank you Henrik Gustafsson, Helena Andersson, John Jouper, Jenny

Isberg, Anders Heden, Jing Li (thank you for the beautiful calligraphy), John Hell-

ström, Sören Hjälm, Lina Wahlgren, Peter Mattsson, Mattias Folkesson, and Karin

Andersson, for making these years such a memorable experience. I would especially

like to mention John Jouper who inspired me to begin doing research on Qigong,

Helena Andersson for many inspiring talks about music and life, and Henrik

Gustafsson, who with his humor and friendship has made life and work especially

enjoyable.

I have also had the pleasure of working as a teacher in Sport Sciences. I feel grateful

to have had the opportunity to work, learn from, and enjoy the positive, humorous,

and warm atmosphere of the people in “the corridor”. In addition, I have also had

the opportunity to broaden my skills and knowledge by teaching in Public Health. It

has been great to be part of this small but warm, hard-working, enthusiastic and

creative group of people. From Sport Sciences and Public Health, I would particu-

larly like to mention Hanna Arneson, Jeanette Åkerström, Nathalie Hassmén, He-

lena Sjöberg, Peter Marklund, Stephan Svenning, Helena Ragnarsson, Elisabeth

Solin, Anita Cierzniewski and Kerstin Norman for making each day at work a warm

and meaningful experience.

I am grateful to Magnus Lindwall and Petra Lindfors, who read and gave me valu-

able criticism on earlier versions of the present thesis. Thank you also, Stig Wenne-

berg, for sharing your knowledge of research on meditation and related areas. Over

the years I have also had the pleasure to chat with, learn from, and enjoy the com-

pany of other people in the Sport psychology area: Carolina Lundqvist, Hansi Hinic

Urban Johnson, Göran Kenttä, and Johan Fallby.

During my PhD student time I have also taken many interesting courses. I especially

think of Professional Communication with Margaret Kerr, and the Exercise Psychol-

ogy workshop at Exeter University with Adrian Taylor, Panteleimon Ekkekakis, and

Elaine Rose, for its high quality and high value for me as a researcher.

I feel blessed to have so many wonderful friends. In their company I have been able

to rest from the hardships of PhD work. Thank you Tomas Engström, Mattias

Danielsson, Christer Rangefil, Hanna Forsgärde, Anna Sandberg, Alice Hartmann,

Nathalie Runeborg, Rie Hjorth, Paul Nowak, Anna Solgevik, Carlos Wedin, An-

dreas Holmquist, Klaus Nesse, Anna Krafft, Helena Edman, Eva Gustafsson, and

My Rådmark – you are fantastic!

As I’ve survived and enjoyed these years, the list of thanks wouldn’t be complete if I

didn’t mention my experiences and friends from the alternative communities around

Sweden, who have deeply enriched my life. I especially think of the courses Passion

For Life and The Five Rhythms©.

Life does not wait on you while you write your PhD! Over the years I have experi-

enced times of hardship, sorrow, strain, happiness, joy, gratefulness and lots of other

emotions. I would like to express my deep gratitude to life – which although it is

sometimes tough, keeps expressing itself in ways that challenge me and enable me to

grow – for sharing its many wonders and mysteries.

I am eternally grateful for the endless love and support I have received from my par-

ents, Kerstin and Per-Olof, and my sister Maria. You have always wanted the best

for me. My warm thoughts also go to Daniel and Sanna, and my immediate family.

6

Finally, my dearest Anna, you challenge and support me with your love and honesty,

and remind me of what is truly important in life.

7

The thesis is based on the following three studies, which are referred to in the text by

their Roman numerals:

I. Johansson, M., Hassmén, P., & Jouper, J. (2008). Acute effects of qigong exer-

cise on mood and anxiety. International Journal of Stress Management, 15,

199-207.

Re-produced with permission by The American Psychological Association

II. Johansson, M., & Hassmén, P. (2008). Acute psychological responses to qigong

exercise of varying durations. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 36,

449-458.

Re-produced with permission by World Scientific Publishing Company.

III. Johansson, M., Hassmén, P., & Hertting, A. Affective responses to qigong ex-

ercise: A pilot study. Submitted for publication.

8

TABLE OF CONTENTS

SVENSK SAMMANFATTNING AV AVHANDLINGEN .........................................15

INTRODUCTION .................................................................................................19

QIGONG ..........................................................................................................21 Chinese philosophy and medicine ...................................................................21

History and defi nitions ..................................................................................23

Different styles of Qigong .............................................................................. 24

Research on Qigong exercise ...........................................................................25

BIYUN QIGONG ..............................................................................................28

Biyun Qigong ..................................................................................................28

Physical activity ..............................................................................................29

Relaxation .......................................................................................................31

Attentional regulation (meditation).................................................................32

Imagery ...........................................................................................................33

Respiration ......................................................................................................34

Self-massage ....................................................................................................35

STUDYING AFFECTIVE RESPONSES ............................................................36

Differentiating between affect, mood and emotion .........................................36

Regulating affective states...............................................................................41

Antecedents and dynamics of affective states .................................................42

Measuring affective responses ........................................................................43

A framework for the study of affective responses .......................................... 44

ACUTE AFFECTIVE RESPONSES ..................................................................46

Suggested mechanisms for acute affective benefi ts ..........................................46

Acute affective responses to physical activity ..................................................51

Acute affective responses to mind-body therapies ...........................................52

Acute affective responses to Qigong exercise ..................................................54

UNANSWERED QUESTIONS IN THE QIGONG-AFFECT RELATIONSHIP ..............................................................................................55

RATIONALE FOR THE PRESENT THESIS .....................................................56

AIM OF THE THESIS .........................................................................................59

STUDIES ............................................................................................................61

STUDY I ..........................................................................................................61

Introduction ....................................................................................................61

Method ...........................................................................................................61

Results and discussion ....................................................................................62

MJ_Inneha ̊ll_230409.indd 13 2009-04-23 14.38

STUDY II ........................................................................................................63

Introduction ....................................................................................................63

Method ...........................................................................................................64

Results and discussion ....................................................................................64

STUDY III ........................................................................................................66 Introduction ....................................................................................................66

Method ...........................................................................................................66

Results and discussion ....................................................................................69

GENERAL DISCUSSION ....................................................................................73

MAIN FINDINGS.............................................................................................73

RESEARCH QUESTIONS ...............................................................................73

Are the affective responses associated with Qigong exercise signifi cantly different from a control condition? .............................................73

Are the affective responses associated with Qigong exercise dependent on length of session? ......................................................................74

Is there a relationship between affective responses and expectations of affective benefi ts? ........................................................................................75

How do affective responses display themselves and change, from pre-, to during, to post-exercise, using mean averages and individual scores? ........76

How do the exercisers describe the experience associated with Qigong exercise, during and after the session? ................................................77

METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES .........................................................................78

Participants .....................................................................................................78

Design .............................................................................................................79

Procedure ........................................................................................................80

Measures ........................................................................................................81

Statistical analysis ...........................................................................................85

Ethical considerations .....................................................................................85

CONCLUDING DISCUSSIONS ........................................................................87

Affective changes associated with Qigong exercise .........................................87

A proposed model of mechanisms of affective benefi ts ...................................90

Causality in the Qigong-affect relationship ...................................................94

Is Qigong benefi cial for everybody? ................................................................96

Qigong – affective regulation and alleviation of physiological arousal? .........97

Long-term benefi ts of regular Qigong exercise ................................................98

Future studies .................................................................................................99

CONCLUSIONS ...............................................................................................103

REFERENCES .................................................................................................105

MJ_Inneha ̊ll_230409.indd 14 2009-04-23 14.38

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 15

SVENSK SAMMANFATTNING AV AVHANDLINGEN

Qigong är en kinesisk kroppsrörelse och meditationsform (mind-body therapy) som

har utövats i årtusenden för dess hälsofrämjande effekter. Qi kan översättas med

livskraft och gong med arbete. Grundprinciperna för qigong är att genom avspän-

ning och naturlig andning fokusera på och inhämta qi (livskraft) till kroppens alla

delar, och få till stånd ett hälsosamt flöde av qi. Många människor upplever positiva

effekter i samband med qigongträning.

Sedan några årtionden har qigong blivit föremål för ökat intresse från forsknings-

världen. Trots att vissa studier lider av bristande vetenskaplig kvalitet ökar antalet

studier av god kvalitet. Den samlade forskningen pekar på hälsofrämjande effekter

inom områden som stresshantering, ökad livskvalitet, känslomässig balans, en för-

bättrad förmåga att hantera negativa symtom av och leva med kronisk sjukdom,

samt ökad motståndskraft vid sjukdom.

Även om forskningen på qigong ökar har studier av akuta känslomässiga effekter

av enstaka qigongpass varit få. Det affektiva området inkluderar emotioner, sinnes-

stämningar och känslor, dvs. känslomässiga reaktioner. Mer kunskap om aktiviteter

som får människor att må bra är av stor vikt då många människor lider av såväl fy-

sisk som psykisk ohälsa. Även om ökat känslomässigt välmående i stunden är viktigt

i sig, klingar känslomässiga reaktioner efter ett tag av. Det har emellertid visats att

personer som regelbundet aktiverar sig också kan få mer långvariga effekter. Ett po-

sitivt känslomässigt tillstånd påverkar exempelvis hur vi ser på oss själva och livet i

stort. Vi tolkar omgivningen mer positivt, minns fler positiva minnen och möter oss

själva och andra mer positivt. Positiva känslotillstånd kan också göra att negativa

fysiologiska reaktioner i samband med stress och negativa känslor snabbare klingar

av. Känslomässiga effekter som är kopplade till en viss aktivitet påverkar också om

vi väljer att fortsätta att engagera oss i aktiviteten.

Syftet med avhandlingen var att studera känslomässiga reaktioner av enstaka qi-

gongpass. Studiedeltagarna rekryterades i samband med qigongläger; majoriteten var

högutbildade medelålders (55 år) kvinnor som under några år regelbundet tränat qi-

gong. I Studie I, var syftet att jämföra effekterna av ett enstaka qigongpass med en

11

16 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

kontrollgrupp som fick lyssna på ett föredrag av grundaren av Biyunmetoden (själv-

skattningsinstrumenten Profile of Mood States, POMS, och state-versionen av State

and Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI användes). Resultatet visade att oro/ängslan, de-

pression, ilska och utmattning minskade signifikant i qigonggruppen jämfört med

kontrollgruppen. Denna skillnad var dock inte synlig i självskattad spändhet, förvir-

ring och vigör. Att jämföra känslotillståndet efter qigong med det efter ett föredrag

av grundaren av den aktuella qigongformen är inte optimalt; de något varierande re-

sultaten kan möjligtvis förklaras av detta. En bättre kontrollbetingelse skulle ha varit

en grupp som satt och läste en bok.

I Studie II, jämfördes känslomässiga reaktioner efter ett qigongpass på 30 minuter

respektive efter ett på 60 minuter. Det visades sig att båda passen förbättrade det

känslomässiga tillståndet lika mycket (POMS, STAI och självkonstruerade skatt-

ningsskalor för upplevd aktivering och välmående användes). Detta resultat är vik-

tigt för dem som inte har tid att träna längre pass, vilket är ett vanligt skäl till att inte

engagera sig i fysisk aktivitet. En viss försiktighet med denna slutsats är dock nöd-

vändig sett ur ett qigongperspektiv. Trettiominuterspasset bestod nämligen av Jichu

gong och 60-minuterspasset av Jichu gong och Dong gong tillsammans. Jichu gong

är den första formen i Biyunqigong och Dong gong den nästkommande. De två for-

merna har olika mål. Medan Jichu gong fokuserar på att mjuka upp kroppens leder

och skapa förutsättning för qi-flödet syftar Dong gong till att inhämta qi från om-

givningen.

I Studie III fick qigongutövarna även skatta sitt känslomässiga tillstånd under qi-

gongpasset. De nomotetiska mätningarna kompletterades med individuella beskriv-

ningar av stämningsläget. Resultaten visade att de känslomässiga förbättringarna

startar efter 10-20 minuters qigong och ökar i styrka under resterande del av passet.

Qigongutövarna rapporterar skönare känslomässiga tillstånd som både är mer akti-

verande och avslappnande. Den stora majoriteten av utövarna upplever att det känns

behagligt under tiden de tränar qigong. Utövarna fick också fylla i en enkät om deras

förväntningar om positiva känslomässiga förbättringar i samband med qigongpas-

sen. Få signifikanta samband återfanns i relation till de verkliga känslomässiga reak-

tionerna. Slutligen fick också utövarna beskriva den qigongrelaterade känslomässiga

12

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 17

upplevelsen i ord. I motsats till resultaten på självskattningsinstrumentet (Swedish

Core Affect Scale) beskrev majoriteten endast avslappnande sköna känslotillstånd

(och inte också aktiverande känslotillstånd).

Sammantaget bidrar avhandlingen till en större förståelse av den tidigare begrän-

sade kunskapen om qigongrelaterade känslomässiga reaktioner av akut slag. Utövar-

na upplever att deras känslomässiga välmående förbättras i samband med qigongt-

räning. Då urvalet består av självvalda, regelbundna utövare på qigongläger, begrän-

sas möjligheten att generalisera resultatet till andra grupper av människor. Resulta-

ten stämmer dock överens med liknande studier av tai chi, meditation och promena-

der vilket indikerar att också andra grupper av människor skulle kunna uppleva

känslomässiga förbättringar i samband med qigong. Fortsatt forskning kring dessa

frågor är därmed motiverad.

13

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 19

INTRODUCTION

In recent decades people have shown a growing interest in alternative and comple-

mentary medicine (Kelner & Wellman, 2000). Many individuals have been attracted

to mind-body therapies (relaxation techniques, Yoga, meditation, hypnosis, guided

imagery, Tai Chi, Qigong, etc.). In an American survey, 20% of the respondents had

some experience of mind-body therapies (Wolsko, Eisenberg, Davis, & Phillips,

2004). Reasons for use were to treat medical conditions, general prevention and

wellness promotion, and as leisure use. In our own country, activities ranging from

mindfulness meditation (Åsberg, Wahlberg, Sköld, & Nygren, 2006), relaxation and

somatic awareness training (Blomberg, 2004) to physical exercise (FYSS, 2008) are

on the rise and pose great promise for living associated with balance, reduced stress,

and well-being. Goldstein (2000) argues that the recent increased interest in alterna-

tive and complementary medicine is related to a greater interest in fitness. He pro-

poses that this increased interest is connected to people viewing health as wellness

(not only the absence of disease), health becoming more our own responsibility, a

greater belief in the mind-body connection, skepticism toward the modern techno-

logical world, and people regarding health as a search for the natural. Frisk (1998)

argues in a similar vein when describing the interest in new religious movements

(New Age) in Sweden. Modern medicine may successfully treat more disease symp-

toms of than ever before in history, but it cannot give the individual answers related

to the meaningfulness of disease, health, suffering, or life and death. Thus, mind-

body therapies may provide a means to greater meaningfulness, health and healing,

which may be pursued through different kinds of belief systems and activities, as well

as personal and spiritual development.

The present thesis discusses Qigong exercise, a mind-body therapy that originated

in China some thousands of years ago. I chose to study a group of regular exercisers

at the Biyun School of Qigong (Medical Qigong), because it is one of the larger

Qigong schools in Sweden. Since the end of the 80s, a growing number of studies on

Qigong reporting health-enhancing effects can be found. However, little focus has

been placed on the momentary/acute affective (affect, mood, & emotion) responses

14

20 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

associated with qigong exercise. Studying Qigong associated responses in the affec-

tive domain may have a number of benefits. Experiencing positive affect is of course

a positive end in itself and important for subjective well-being. Positive affect may

also lead to other health benefits (e.g., a more positive outlook on life). Many indi-

viduals (e.g., the elderly) may not be able to adhere to higher intensity exercise and

may miss out on the beneficial psychological benefits associated with these activities.

The main purpose of the present thesis is the investigation of acute affective re-

sponses associated with Qigong exercise. Affective responses (See also affective

states; Scherer, 1984) is used here as a generic term referring to any affective re-

sponse (be it a mood state, an emotion or on the level of basic affect). As such, it is

focused on the conscious feeling aspect of the affective domain and not on the

physiological changes (facial, autonomic, brain-based, vocal, and other physiological

changes; Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999) that accompany any subjective affective ex-

perience. Acute refers to the responses specifically associated with a single session of

Qigong, in contrast to chronic, which refers to the effects of a series of sessions over

a longer time (e.g., a few months). Given that new mind-body therapies are being

used by more individuals and are also in demand by health care consumers

(Landstingsförbundet, 2001), it is necessary to know more about their possible bene-

fits as well as when caution is warranted in their use.

A number of Qigong forms exist. Biyun Qigong and the forms discussed by the

majority of studies mentioned may be classified as Medical Qigong; hence their main

focus is on prevention of disease and enhancement of health (although spiritual

characteristics may also exist). When Qigong is mentioned in the present thesis, it is

mainly the medical form that is referred to.

15

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 21

Qigong

Chinese philosophy and medicine

Qigong is an integrated part of Traditional Chinese Medicine (TCM). In order to

understand Qigong one also needs to understand the traditional medicine and phi-

losophy from which Qigong originated. A main difference between Western and

Eastern philosophy is grounded in basic ontological and epistemological assump-

tions (Ames, 1993). Western philosophical perspectives, influenced by Greek and

Christian traditions as well as the Scientific Revolution, focus on strict distinctions

between concepts, such as body/soul, beginning/end, open/closed, subject/object,

birth/death, etc. Eastern philosophical perspectives, influenced by Asian religions

and philosophies such as Taoism, Confucianism, Buddhism and Hinduism, instead

view the world in terms of dynamic processes that are complementary, co-

dependent, cyclical and non-linear, and a more holistic view of mind and body. The

yin and yang symbol can illustrate this perspective. Composed of a dark and a white

side of a circle, the two sides are co-dependent on, and also part of, each other. A

very important aspect of the co-dependency view in relation to TCM is that humans

are like a microcosm and are influenced by everything in the cosmos. This is in stark

contrast to the reductionist scientific medical view, in which humans are to some

extent viewed as a machine with parts that can be fixed and exchanged if necessary

(O’Connor, 2000).

Yin-Yang theory is essential to the understanding of TCM and Chinese philoso-

phy. First mentioned in Tao Te Ching, 300 BC, yin and yang were the names used

in the ancient interpretation that all manifestations of the world could be grouped

into two opposites. At the same time, these two opposites are dependent on each

other and contain each other. This illustrates the view that all aspects of life are in-

terrelated with everything else. Yin and yang are visible in all aspects of Chinese life.

Yin can be described as shadow, passivity, female, the moon, while Yang is light,

activity, male, the sun. In the context of TCM, all the body’s organs are grouped

into yin or yang. Too much yin or yang in the body is seen as an imbalance and can

16

22 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

lead to disease (Kaptchuk, 2000). In the context of Qigong, yin and yang can be il-

lustrated as being in a state of simultaneous stillness and change (Cohen, 1997).

An essential characteristic of Chinese philosophy and medicine that does not exist

in the West is the concept of qi. Qi can be described as “a type of energy or dy-

namic force that sustains and nourishes life” (Dorcas & Yung, 2003, p. 198). It is

also sometimes referred to as vital force or life force. Similar concepts of life energy

exist around the world, for example as prana in the Yoga tradition of India (Cohen,

1997). Qi may stem from different sources, coming from our parents, food and

drink, and by way of our breath through the air. According to TCM, energy chan-

nels or meridians cover our inner bodies. Qi, the life force, flows through specific

meridians. There are twelve regular meridians that are connected to the bodily or-

gans, and eight extraordinary meridians that are not connected to the organs. A

steady flow of qi through the meridians is vital to one’s health. It can be compared

to a river in which there is a constant flow of water. Stagnation or a flow that is too

wild is considered unhealthy (Kaptchuk, 2000).

According to TCM, the flow of qi may be regulated by way of different medical

modalities. Acupuncture uses needles to treat special points on the meridians, acu-

points. Specific acupoints have different functions and affect the flow of qi and the

health of the individual in specific ways. Depending on the imbalance or disease the

individual is experiencing, needles are inserted at certain acupoints. Acupressure and

Qigong also directly aim to affect the flow of qi in the meridians, by way of massage

and mental focusing to direct the flow of qi (Tse, 1998).

Emotions play a significant role in relation to health and well-being in TCM.

Health is related to a healthy balance of different emotions, and overly strong emo-

tions may cause disturbances and even disease. All emotions – not only anger, for

example, which people in the West may find counterproductive to health (e.g., car-

diovascular disorders) but also joy – in extremes may affect our health. According to

TCM, strong emotions like anger can cause disturbances in the liver, joy can be

negative for the heart, and sadness can be negative for the lungs (Fan, 2000).

17

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 23

History and definitions

Qigong belongs to a group of exercises called mind-body therapies (See also mind-

body medicine, NCCAM, 2007; mindful exercise, Chow & Tsang, 2007; meditative

movement, Larkey, Jahnke, Etnier, & Gonzales, in press; and meditation, Ospina et

al., 2007), to which meditation, Yoga, Tai Chi, dancing, etc., also belong. These

methods date back to prehistoric times and have religious connotations. Mind-body

therapies have sprung out of repetitive, rhythmic chants and offerings to appease the

gods (Joseph, 1998), and written records of them and their predecessors can be

found in Hindu, Taoist, Buddhist, Christian, and Moslem scriptures (Everly & Lat-

ing, 2002). The goal of mind-body therapies can be therapeutic (health and well-

being) as well as spiritual (finding peace and tranquility and transcending normal

consciousness; Ospina et al., 2007).

Qigong originated in China, but it is possible that it has been influenced by In-

dian Yoga (Cohen, 1997) as the two traditions share some characteristics. Its earli-

est records date back some 3000 years (Chen, 2007). Qi is translated as life force

and gong as work (Cohen, 1997). Qigong can be seen as a generic term referring to

methods, “… working with the life energy, learning how to control the flow and

distribution of qi to improve the health and harmony of mind and body. “ (Cohen,

1997, p. 4). The name Qigong was introduced in the 1950s and it was originally

known as Dao-yin (Cohen, 1997). It is suggested (Cohen, 1997) that the core of

Qigong exercise originated from farmers observing the cycles of planting and har-

vesting, and of life and death (other Qigong origins come from mimicking the

movements of animals, e.g. the Five Animal Frolics). To ensure the growth of seeds

it is important to establish strong, healthy roots for the plant to find a good support

for growth; also important is daily care through optimal watering and sunshine.

Many Qigong forms therefore advocate first creating a strong foundation/roots to

build upon. Plants need water to grow, but in optimal doses. Qigong exercise is

therefore recommended in daily optimal doses for a particular individual. Too little

training may be insufficient to have any effect, and too much training may not cre-

ate better health but might instead have negative effects for the individual (Ng,

18

24 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

1999). By further observing the effects of water resources (Cohen, 1997), people liv-

ing at the time Qigong developed came to the conclusion that a healthy, steady flow

of clean water was important: Too much causes flooding, too little causes drought,

and stagnant water can cause disease. This may have influenced the view on qi (life

force), which flows in the meridians of the body, by way of Qigong exercises being

enhanced and creating a healthy flow throughout all parts of the body. Through the

performance of Qigong, the flow of qi can be optimized. Thus, one may illustrate a

healthy human being with the example of a plant: A healthy plant is strong yet soft,

swaying with the wind but not breaking. However, when the plant is sick it becomes

stiff and rigid and easily breaks (Cohen, 1997).

Different styles of Qigong

Over the years the principals of Qigong exercise have been used in different forms

and settings and for different aims. It can be divided into Active Qigong (Dong

Gong), which includes slow movements and postures, and Passive Qigong (Jing

Gong), which focuses more on mental concentration, visualization, and breathing.

Active Qigong is the more popular form, in both China and the West. Most Qigong

forms do, however, include both active and passive aspects, reflecting the impor-

tance of the yin and yang philosophy, encompassing the contrasting aspects of life

(Cohen, 1997).

Today, Qigong is mostly known for its health-promoting characteristics, but it

was initially created for the cultivation of the mind and spirituality (Chen, 2007).

Historically it has had many different focuses, such as promoting moral develop-

ment (Confucian Qigong) and spiritual development (Buddhist and Taoist Qigong),

and preparing the body for combat and healing wounds (Martial Arts Qigong), in

addition to Medical Qigong (Chen, 2007).

Qigong may also be grouped into Internal and External Qigong (Chen, 2007). In-

ternal Qigong refers to the activity described above, carried out by the exerciser in-

dividually. External Qigong is a form of healing. According to TCM, the cultivation

of qi and the ability to move it to different parts of the body thanks to many years

19

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 25

of Qigong practice may also create an ability to emit qi, break qi blockages, and re-

move sick qi from another person’s body. Many Qigong clinics in China provide

External Qigong.

Although different Qigong forms may differ, most include adjusting the mind,

body and breathing (Chen, 2008). Some forms may include actively performing spe-

cific movements (Dong Gong), standing like a pole (Zhuang Gong), and static forms

of meditation (Jing Gong). The dynamic forms of Qigong that include movements

are sometimes introductory forms, with the movements guiding and helping the ex-

erciser to better concentrate on their internal body and induce qi flow. Static forms

of Qigong include meditation (concentrative and mindfulness), relaxation, breathing

manipulation, guided imagery, and incantation. Static forms are supposed to train

intentional power (consciousness stability) while cultivating qi flow, and by way of

the mind guide the flow of qi in the body. Standing like a pole refers to when an ex-

erciser is in a standing up position, and spontaneous movements may occur. Thus,

although different forms of Qigong exist, generally the exerciser first tries to attain a

state of mind that is quiet and calm. When this is accomplished, the exerciser’s next

goal is the cultivation of an awareness of experiencing qi and its movements through

the body. Finally, different paths of cultivation can be followed such as gathering qi

in the body, cultivating qi circulation, and other forms of guiding the flow of qi us-

ing the mind (Zhang & Rose, 2001).

Research on Qigong exercise

Qigong exercise has been shown to positively affect the psychological, neuroendo-

crine, and immune systems (Ryu et al., 1995). Interventions involving Qigong exer-

cise have been found to reduce blood pressure and triglycerides as well as increase

high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol (Lee, Lee, & Kim, 2004), and to reduce

work-related stress (heart rate, noradrenalin urine excretion, finger temperature,

and subjective experienced stress reactions) significantly more than controls

(Skoglund & Jansson, 2007). In a psychological vein, cross-sectional studies show a

20

26 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

significant negative relationship between Qigong (length of practice) and neuroti-

cism (Leung and Singhal, 2004) and perceived stress (Lee, Ryu, & Chung, 2000).

Qigong has also been used as an intervention (two to three months) for coping

with and alleviating symptoms of diseases. Multiple sclerosis patients showed sig-

nificant improvements in depression and balance (Mills, Allen, & Morgan, 2000),

and fibromyalgia patients improved on measures of pain, physical and psychological

functioning, anxiety and depression (Astin et al., 2003), and enhanced movement

harmony (Mannerkorpi & Arndorw, 2004). Muscular dystrophy patients main-

tained perceived general health when performing Qigong while the control group

deteriorated (Wenneberg, Gunnarsson, & Ahlström, 2004b). Hypertension patients

reduced their blood pressure (and norepinephrine, epinephrine, cortisol, and per-

ceived stress) significantly more than a control group did (Lee, Lee, Kim, and Moon,

2003). In a similar study, self-efficacy and self-esteem increased and blood pressure

decreased (Lee, Lim, & Lee, 2004). Negative symptoms in cancer patients receiving

chemotherapy were reduced and improvements in pain, numbness, heartburn and

dizziness, as well as a greater will to live, were found (Lee, Chen, & Yeh, 2006). A

randomized controlled trial including depressed elderly found improvements in

mood, self-efficacy, and personal well-being (Tsang, Fung, Chan, Lee, & Chan,

2006). Thus, the increasing support for the benefits of Qigong seems promising in

many ways.

Acute physiological responses to Qigong exercise have been predominantly stud-

ied by a group of Korean researchers. In their studies they have used the Chun Do

Sun Bop style of Qigong, which in addition to the common characteristics of Qigong

(e.g., relaxation and slow movements) also uses sounds. The Chun Do Sun Bop style

is performed for 60 minutes, and a majority of fairly young male participants have

been studied. Training background ranges from beginners having learned the proce-

dure on certain occasions before the experiment to regular exercisers (up to a year).

Studies show an increased secretion of growth hormone (Lee, Kang, Ryu, & Moon,

2004; Ryu, Lee, Jeong, Lee, Kang, Lee et al., 2000), improved immune function (Lee,

Kang, & Ryu, 2005; Lee, Kang, Ryu, & Moon, 2004; Lee, Kim, & Ryu, 2005), and

decreased breathing and heart rate during qigong (Lee, Kim, Huh, Ryu, Lee, &

21

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 27

Chung, 2000) – promising acute results, indeed; however, control groups were used

only in the Lee, Kang, and Ryu study (2005).

Negative health effects of Qigong exercise are also reported. Ng (1999) reports in-

creased psychiatric symptoms in some individuals, Qigong deviation syndrome. This

may be associated with an extra sensitivity to the effects of relaxation: relaxation-

induced anxiety (Heide & Borkovec, 1984), which may cause an individual to dis-

play increased anxiety. Detrimental effects may also stem from faulty training, such

as overly long sessions (Ng, 1999). Fan Xiulan of the Biyun School states that when

training recommendations have been followed, no detrimental effects have been seen.

However, there are some conditions under which Qigong training should be avoided:

epilepsy, serious psychiatric illnesses, and when under the influence of drugs or

strong emotions. Special training precautions are also cited for menstruating and

pregnant women (Fan, 2000).

Research on Qigong exercise highlights promising health-related benefits in many

areas, such as the management and reduction of symptoms of diseases (fibromyalgia,

hypertension, multiple sclerosis, and cancer), improved psychological and physical

well-being, and a positive acute affect on the psychological, neuroendocrine, and

immune systems. Some caution is warranted on scientific rigor grounds (e.g., use of

control groups in only some of the studies).

22

28 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Biyun Qigong

Biyun Qigong

Biyun Qigong is one of the most widespread Qigong styles in Sweden, and was cre-

ated and developed by Qigong master Fan Xiulan (Fan, 2000). Biyun Qigong can be

described as a Medical Qigong style. Thereby, the focus is on the flow of blood and

qi in the body, strengthening the function of the lungs, digestion, and other bodily

functions. Biyun also focuses on strengthening muscles and joints that easily get

worn out and exhausted. Yet, as TCM and qigong are holistic in perspective, typical

spiritual elements are also included in Biyun Qigong, including the intention to raise

consciousness (insight and a greater understanding of consciousness) by way of

mindfulness (accepting and not clinging to emotions) and living in harmony with

oneself, other people, and nature. By way of Qigong exercise, in which typical medi-

cal and spiritual perspectives are equally important, intentions of spiritual growth

(de) and naturalness (wu wei) may lead to Tao, a state of harmony, peace and free-

dom. Fan describes Biyun Qigong as a “focused mindful concentration on the physi-

cal body to systematically increase qi in the body and to develop its ability to gather

natural qi from the universe” [translated from the Swedish] (Fan, 2000, p. 31).

The Biyun School of Qigong consists of a number of specific forms of Qigong ex-

ercise. These range from beginner forms (Jichu Gong) to more advanced ones (Dong

Gong, Five Elements Qigong, and Wai Chi). The beginner forms focus on softening

the joints and opening up for the life force (qi) to flow in the body. The exerciser

later focuses on gathering qi from the environment and cleansing the body of bad qi

(Dong Gong). The more advanced forms focus on moving qi through the body, get-

ting more attuned to the flow of qi and letting the body move spontaneously, and

creating one’s own individual form of Qigong (Fan, 2000). All forms stress the im-

portance of concluding by gathering qi by way of visualization as well as physically

(by way of arm movements) and directing it into the Dantien (an energy center, a

few centimeters below the navel).

23

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 29

Characteristics of Biyun Qigong, as well as those of many other forms of Qigong,

include slow movements (physical activity), relaxation, concentration/meditation,

imagery, natural breathing, and self-massage. These methods will be presented and

discussed below, with a focus on their possible positive influences on affective states.

Physical activity

Qigong also includes physical activity in the form of slow movements. Physical ac-

tivity refers to the movement of the body produced by the skeletal muscles, resulting

in energy expenditure, which varies from low to high, and a positive correlation

with physical fitness. In addition to what is stated regarding physical activity, exer-

cise also includes planned, structured and repetitive bodily movements and the ob-

jective to maintain or improve physical fitness (Caspersen, Powel, & Christenson,

1985). Qigong exercise can be defined as a low-intensity physical activity and exer-

cise (in addition to its mindful aspects), although its influence on physical fitness

may be limited compared to jogging.

It is sometimes stated that the human body is designed for physical activity. Al-

though this statement is difficult to test in an experimental setting, three lines of evi-

dence support it (Bouchard, Blair, & Haskell, 2007). Firstly, the human body can

adapt to the metabolic demands of its environment. Secondly, a low level of physical

activity is associated with increased frequency of disease, loss of functional capacity,

and premature death. Thirdly, without the ability to perform demanding physical

work, early humans would not have been able to survive. Thus, we can conclude

that being physically active lies inherent in us, and that the sedentary lifestyle of to-

day have negative physical and mental effects (Bouchard et al., 2007). There is now

a large body of evidence linking physical activity with the prevention of poor health,

as well as the enhancement of health and well-being. The Surgeon General’s Report

on Physical Activity and Health (USDHHS,1996) cites numerous diseases and

health-related aspects of life that are affected by physical activity, like lower mortal-

ity rates, decreased risk of cardio-respiratory disease, reduced risk of some forms of

cancer, reduced risk of developing non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus, mainte-

24

30 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

nance of normal muscle strength, normal skeletal development during childhood,

reduction of risk of falling among the elderly, lower rates of obesity and mainte-

nance of a healthy diet, relief from symptoms of depression and anxiety, reduced

risk of developing depression, improved health-related quality of life such as psycho-

logical well-being, and improved physical functioning.

A growing body of evidence from reviews of the literature supports the impor-

tance of physical activity for psychological health (Biddle & Ekkekakis, 2005;

Biddle & Mutrie, 2001; Berger & Tobar, 2006; Landers & Arent, 2006; Mutrie &

Faulkner, 2004; Scully, Kremer, Meade, Graham, & Dudgeon, 1998; Warburton,

2006). Physical activity is associated with reduced state and trait (acute and chronic)

anxiety (Landers & Arent, 2006, Petruzzello et al., 1991; Scully et al., 1998; Taylor,

2000). Individuals suffering from clinical and non-clinical anxiety may benefit from

physical activity (Landers & Arent, 2006; Taylor, 2000). Further, physical activity

also has positive effects on psychosocial stress. Bouts of physical activity can reduce

stress, and improved fitness is also associated with reduced stress reactivity and bet-

ter coping (Berger & Tobar, 2006; Landers & Arent, 2006; Scully et al., 1998; Tay-

lor, 2000).

Studies also show beneficial effects associated with physical activity on those suf-

fering from depression (Paluska & Schwenk, 2000; Scully et al., 1998), and physical

activity is also associated with a decreased risk of developing clinical depression

(Mutrie, 2000). The effect of physical activity on depression is comparable to that of

psychotherapy (Mutrie & Faulkner, 2000; Paluska & Schwenk, 2000).

A physically active lifestyle is associated with increased self-esteem (Biddle &

Mutrie, 2000), improved cognitive functioning (Landers & Arent, 2006), and im-

proved health-related quality of life, including physical functioning, physical symp-

toms, and emotional, social and cognitive functioning (Berger & Tobar, 2006; Re-

jeski, Brawley, & Shumaker, 1996). Thus, a large body of evidence now supports

the relationship between a physically active life and improved psychological health.

Acute affective benefits of single bouts of exercise also show promising results; this

will be dealt with in a later section.

25

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 31

Relaxation

Biyun Qigong sessions begin with a few minutes of relaxation. Although the term

relaxation techniques often includes a large number of relaxation-associated tech-

niques like progressive relaxation, self-hypnosis, autogenic training, meditation, im-

agery, and biofeedback-assisted relaxation (Orne & Whitehouse, 2000; Poppen,

1998), relaxation may be a sole intervention in itself, sometimes being used in

preparation or in combination with other techniques (Lichstein, 2000) like in

qigong. Contrasting relaxation and meditation, relaxation is used primarily to re-

duce physiological arousal whereas meditation is more focused on directing one’s

attention to the present moment (and not directly trying to change the level of

arousal; Shapiro, Schwartz, & Santerre, 2005).

According to Biyun Qigong, relaxation training entails a systematic, mental

screening of the inner and outer parts of the body, inducing relaxation. Qigong fo-

cuses on an optimal state in which we can gather qi from the environment. Relaxa-

tion is therefore stressed before beginning Qigong exercise, and when one is experi-

encing strong emotions Qigong exercise is not recommended (Fan, 2000). Also,

through relaxing, thoughts and emotions can be prevented from interfering with our

concentration and we may even forget ourselves (Fan, 2000). As no muscular activ-

ity is carried out (as it is in progressive relaxation, in which the muscles are first

tensed and then relaxed), the form of relaxation in Biyun Qigong is associated with

autogenic training, in which the practitioner by way of mental focus induces states

of warmth and heaviness in different body parts (Sadigh, 2001). Words like warm

and nice feeling, relaxation and pleasantness are used to induce relaxation in audio-

recorded instructions in Jichu gong and Dong gong in the Biyun School (Fan, 2001,

2004). The exerciser is instructed to let go of all tension, irritation, and worries

(Fan, 2000). Relaxation has been found to reduce physiological arousal (Scheufele,

2000) and emotional distress symptoms (Farné & Gnugnoli, 2000), and improve the

capacity for control over imagery (Singer, 2006). Relaxation is also used in prepara-

tion for meditation to reduce arousal (Girdano, Everly, & Dusek, 2001). Qigong

26

32 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

exercisers may benefit from relaxation training before actual Qigong, making it eas-

ier to relax and direct their attention to the body and mental imagery.

Attentional regulation (meditation)

The next phase in Biyun Qigong (Jichu Gong) refers to the actual Qigong activity.

Systematically softening the joints from feet to head, the practitioner also focuses on

the corresponding body part. This entails holding the attention at the specific area

of focus. For example, when slowly rotating the ankle joint, focus is directed at the

foot and the joint. Words like strengthening and promoting (Fan, 2001, 2004) as

well as “thinking positively of the body part performing the movement” (Fan, 2000,

p. 41) are employed. The concentrative/focusing techniques used are meditative

methods. One definition of meditation refers to “… a family of self-regulation prac-

tices that focus on training attention and awareness in order to bring mental proc-

esses under greater voluntary control and thereby foster general mental well-being

and development and/or specific capacities such as calm, clarity, and consciousness”

(Walsh & Shapiro, 2006, pp. 229). Meditative disciplines are usually divided into

concentrative and mindfulness forms of meditation (Carrington, 2008). In concen-

trative meditation, attention is focused on a specific object like the breath, a mantra,

sounds, or different parts of the body like in Qigong exercise. If the mind wanders,

thoughts come into one’s awareness or other stimuli interrupt, the exerciser is in-

structed to ignore these and return to focusing on the object of choice. Mindfulness

meditation entails opening one’s attention to all kinds of stimuli, inside and outside,

witnessing all the contents of consciousness, accepting everything. Qigong exercise

according to the Biyun School is primarily focused concentrative meditation, but

there are also examples of mindfulness meditation (Fan, 2000). The exerciser directs

her attention/concentrates on specific body parts, in a relaxed and balanced manner.

She may become unaffected of the things going on around her, and may even forget

herself (Fan, 2000). In this state (the Qigong state) “… a new state of awareness

that is not contaminated, tinted or distorted by the contents and processes of con-

27

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 33

sciousness comes into being, and the natural forces or energies of life (qi) are able to

freely flow.” (Dorcas & Yung, 2003, p. 200). This state of consciousness is what

most meditative traditions attempt to attain (Dorcas & Yung, 2003). It is the bal-

ance point between yin and yang (Fan, 2000), where the division between subject

(me) and object (not me) disappear, and one may feel at one with the entire universe

(Dorcas & Yung, 2003). Concentrative meditative practice may “allow the partici-

pant to feel inner balance, calm, and the ability to transcend the continuous flow of

cognitions and emotions” whereas mindfulness meditation “may encourage insights

into the maladaptive cognitive, emotional, and behavioral patterns” (Deikman,

1982). Meditation may improve well-being and stress management, and provide for

important insights into one’s own life (Bogart, 1991), reduce state and trait anxiety

and psychological distress (Shapiro, Schwartz, & Bonner, 1998), offer some support

for the alleviation of anxiety disorders (although the small number of studies limit

conclusions; Krisanaprakornkit, Krisanaprakornkit, Piyavhatkul, & Laopaiboon,

2006), and help the fostering and cultivation of positive emotions (Fredrickson,

Coh, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008). Fan Xiulan stresses the importance of focusing

(Fan, 2000). The ability to concentrate during Qigong exercise is its single most im-

portant feature. Support for the importance of concentrative ability has been found

empirically (Jouper, Hassmén, & Johansson, 2006).

Imagery

Several forms of Biyun Qigong include forms of imagery. Imagery can be provided

from verbal suggestions in recorded instructions (it is common to use recorded in-

structions when performing Biyun Qigong). Verbal instructions (Fan, 2001, 2004)

include thinking about an inner smile and imagining taking in positive, energizing

life energy from the environment and directing it into the head, through the body,

and down into the energy center of the body (the Dantien), a few centimeters below

the navel. Further suggestions include focusing on health, strength, feelings of

warmth, harmony, positive emotions and positive aspects of one’s life. These sugges-

tions are examples of guided imagery. The aim of guided imagery is to affect one’s

28

34 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

physical, emotional or spiritual state by way of visualization (Barrows & Jacobs,

2002). Reviews of guided imagery show its beneficial effects on mood and well-

being, and as a means of coping with stress (Astin, Shapiro, Eisenberg, & Forys,

2003; Gruzelier, 2002; Luskin et al., 1998). Visualizing pleasurable, relaxing images

has been used to counteract anxiety (Singer, 2002). Through repetition, individuals

may also learn to distract themselves from negative images (by replacing them with

more positive ones) of consciousness, which may lead to an increased sense of mas-

tery of internal experiences (Singer, 2002). The use of guided imagery in Qigong ex-

ercise may contribute to acute beneficial affective changes.

Respiration

Breath training is a part of many cognitive-behavioral, somatic, and mind-body

therapies (Wilhelm, Gevirtz, & Roth, 2001). Whereas some mind-body therapies

advocate controlling and changing the breath (e.g. Yoga; Brown & Gerbarg, 2005),

as do some forms of Qigong (Cohen, 1997), Fan Xiulan, founder of Biyun Qigong,

recommends natural breathing. This means letting go of one’s breath and not trying

to control it. It is also common for the exerciser to use different breathing patterns

during the Qigong session, as respiration is influenced by the different movements

(Fan, 2000). Respiration is essential for maintaining life, and its regulation includes

complex homeostatic interactions. It is central for emotion, health, and conscious-

ness. Dysregulation of respiration may therefore have severe health consequences

(Wilhelm et al., 2001). Different patterns of respiration affect individuals in specific

ways. Shallow breathing may make people more anxious (Conrad et al., 2007),

slowing down breathing frequency can reduce state anxiety (Han, Stegen, Valck, &

Clément, 1996), and different emotions can be experienced through the induction of

different breathing patterns (e.g. joy through regular, slow, deep breathing through

the nose (Philippot, Chapelle, & Blairy, 2002). Physiologically, the slowing down of

breathing frequency is associated with increased parasympathetic activity (Bernardi

et al., 2002; Recordati, 2003). Patients are taught to use slow diaphragmatic breath-

ing to manage and reduce anxiety disorders; through practice they may gain a

29

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 35

greater understanding of their symptoms and thereby reduce their fears (Wilhelm et

al., 2001). In addition, when one gains feelings of mastery over one’s mental states,

self-efficacy may increase (Bandura, 1982), followed by a reduction in anxiety.

Self-massage

Oxytocin has been shown to be associated with the pleasurable responses found in

breast feeding, sexual activities, social interaction and massage (Uvnäs-Moberg,

Arn, & Magnusson, 2005). It has been suggested as a vital part of the calm and

connection system; a relaxed growth, a restorative processes system, and a counter-

part to the fight or flight stress response. Massage has been shown to reduce anxiety

(Moyer, Rounds, & Hannum, 2004), and self-massage has led to anxiety reduction

in cigarette cravings (Hernendez-Reif, Field, Hart, 1999). It has also been suggested

as an active mechanism providing the positive effects of mind-body therapies (Uv-

näs-Moberg, 2000). In Biyun Qigong, most forms end with a number of minutes of

self-massage (sometimes clapping) all over the body.

30

36 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Studying affective responses

Psychological responses to Qigong exercise can include several aspects. Human

mental functioning is generally divided into cognition, conation (e.g. motivation),

and affect (Parkinson, Totterdell, Briner, & Reynolds, 1996). The aim of the present

thesis is focused on affective responses. Below, the affective domain will be dis-

cussed. Affect and cognition are generally viewed as separate constructs (or as repre-

senting the opposite ends of a continuum; Panksepp, 2003). On a fundamental level,

“cognition refers to representation of knowledge (truth and falsity)” whereas “affec-

tive refers to representations of value (goodness and badness)” (Clore et al., 2001, p.

29). Cognitive and affective processes are, however, seldom completely separate

from each other, and are instead intertwined in each other’s processes; both are es-

sential to basic human functioning (Forgas, 2008).

Affect influences cognition in many ways. Affect may influence “how people re-

member, perceive, and interpret social situations and execute interpersonal behav-

iors” (Forgas, 2008, p. 99). When we consciously experience a mood, it influences

not only how we interpret our situation, but also how we see our relation to that

situation, and our affective reactions to it (Parkinson et al., 1996). Affect may func-

tion as conscious information, helping our rational thinking make choices (Schwarz

& Clore, 1983). It may be interconnected with ideas and memories and may auto-

matically prime cognitions – mood congruency (Parkinson et al., 1996), and it may

influence cognition through different ways of information processing. When in a

happy mood people tend to remember happy memories, and vice versa (Eich &

Macauley, 2006); happy people perceive more happy behaviors in themselves and

others (Forgas, Bower, Krantz, 1984), and think happier thoughts (Parkinson et al.,

1996).

Differentiating between affect, mood and emotion

The study of affective responses to physical activity and Qigong exercise pose some

basic problems as no consensus exists regarding what an emotion is, several con-

31

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 37

structs coexist side by side (Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1997), and affect, mood, and

emotion are used interchangeably, which creates confusion (Ekkekakis &

Petruzzello, 2000). See Figure 1, for a simplified summary of affective states.

32

38 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

The difference between affect, mood, and emotion (See Figure 3) can be seen from an

evolutionary perspective, by which affect is considered the most basic, mood on an

intermediate level, and emotion as a later stage of human evolutionary development,

including cognitive components, and therefore a more complex phenomenon (Ek-

kekakis & Petruzzello, 1999). Affect refers to “the irreducible aspect that gives feel-

ings their emotional noncognitive character” (Frijda, 1993, s. 383). Affect is re-

garded as all valenced (“good-bad”) responses, including emotions and moods (Fri-

jda, 1994; Gross, 1999; Scherer, 1984. It colors our experiences, and without it they

would be a neutral gray (Batson, Shaw, & Oleson, 1992). The word feeling is syn-

onymous with affect (Berkowitz, 2000) and refers to conscious experience.

Moving on to emotion, most theorists agree that in addition to affect, which is

probably the most essential component (Feldman Barrett & Russell, 1999), emotion

also includes appraisals/cognitions, action tendencies and physiological responses

(Frijda, 1994). Although some argue that mood is made up of the same components

as emotion is (Frijda, 1994), it is the conscious, subjective experience – affect – that

is the focus in mood research (Watson, 2000). Some even consider mood to be an

extended form of affect (or core affect; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999).

Although they display some similar characteristics, emotion and mood differ on

some important issues, making them related yet separate constructs. An emotion is

considered a short (a few seconds), highly intensive affective state. It also has a clear

object that sets it off (Frijda, 1994). “They are about something. One is happy about

something, angry at someone, afraid of something” (p. 381). In contrast, moods are

of longer duration (from hours to days) and are milder than emotions. Emotions can

be viewed as occupying the foreground of consciousness, whereas moods occupy the

background (Rosenberg, 1998). Another distinguishing characteristic is the absence

of a clear object eliciting the moods. Moods are therefore more diffuse compared to

emotions (Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 2000). External stimuli may induce moods, but

whereas emotions indicate “reactions to specific affectively important events” mood

is “a cue to one’s current global state of action readiness and/or evaluation of the life

situation” (Morris, 1999, p. 171). Emotions influence action, while moods influence

cognitions (Davidson, 1994; Frijda, 1994). Emotions and moods may influence each

33

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 39

other. Moods may modulate the threshold for eliciting an emotion (Frijda, 1994).

Being in an irritated mood may, for example, lower one’s threshold for anger (emo-

tion). Conversely, some emotional episodes may combine into a mood state (Parkin-

son et al., 1996).

The issue of defining affective states has created many different conceptualiza-

tions, the two main groups of which are the discrete/categorical view and the dimen-

sional view. Proponents of the discrete/categorical view argue for the existence of a

number of distinct categories of emotions. This is supported by research that shows

that people around the world display similar facial expressions related to a number

of specific emotions, including fear, anger, sadness, enjoyment, and disgust (Ekman,

1992).

Conversely, proponents of the dimensional view of emotion (affect) argue that af-

fective states can be described as two or three dimensions. One such example, the

circumplex model of affect (Russell, 1980), is represented by “two orthogonal and

bipolar dimensions, valence (pleasure-displeasure) and activation (low-high; see Fig-

ure 2). Specific affective states are considered combinations of the two dimensions”

(Rose & Parfitt, 2007, p. 282). A few different variations of this structure, the cir-

cumplex structure of affect, exist (Larsen & Diener, 1992; Russell, 1980; Thayer,

1996; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). Russell’s circumplex structure of affect has also

been found across different cultures (Russell, Lewicka, & Niit, 1989).

Watson and Tellegen (1985) emerged with a different variation of Russell’s cir-

cumplex structure. Instead of separate valence and activation constructs (Russell,

1980), Positive Affect (high Positive Affect is Pleasant Activation in Figure 1) and

Negative Affect (high Negative Affect is Unpleasant Activation in Figure 1) inter-

twined (rotational variant; Feldman Barrett & Russell, 1999). Yik and colleagues

(Yik, Russell, & Feldman Barrett, 1999) showed that the four different views of the

affect circumplex (Larsen & Diener, 1992; Russell, 1980; Thayer, 1996; Watson &

Tellegen, 1985) are actually the same structure viewed from different angles. These

different views can be grouped into un-rotated and rotated variations. Biological cor-

relates exist for both perspectives (Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 2002).

34

40 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

There has been a debate between those who advocate that positive and negative af-

fect are independent of each other – unipolar scales (e.g. Watson & Tellegen, 1985),

and those who argue that they are poles on a continuum – bipolar scales (e.g. Rus-

sell, 1980). There is still some controversy around this issue, but one could conclude

(Berkowitz, 2000) that when looking at a narrow time span (the experience of mo-

ment-to-moment affective states) we sometimes have a mixture of affective states,

but when we are having one very strong positive feeling it is unlikely that we will

also report a very strong negative feeling. When reporting milder affective states, in-

dividuals may report a mix of both positive and negative feelings.

So which view best describes affective states, the discrete/categorical view or the

dimensional view? They complement each other. The categorical view is preferred

for studying specific emotions (which can be done in great detail), while the dimen-

35

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 41

sional view is preferred when the focus of study is feelings on a more basic level (e.g.,

pleasantness-unpleasantness), and less is known about the area of focus (Ekkekakis

& Petruzzello, 2000; Larsen & Diener, 1992).

Regulating affective states

Affect regulation is defined as “any process directed at modifying or maintaining

moods or emotions whose operation depends on monitoring of affective informa-

tion” (Parkinson et al., 1996). Affect regulation includes coping, emotion regula-

tion, mood regulation, and psychological defenses (Gross & Thompson, 2007).

Reasons for regulating affective states are many: adapting to challenges in life,

health and relationships, and maintaining a global sense of subjective well-being

(Larsen & Prizmic, 2004). The inability to regulate affective states (particularly

emotion) is related to a large number of psychiatric disorders and reduced mental

health (Gross, 1999).

Gross (1999) makes distinctions between where in the emotional process the regu-

lating intention is directed. Focus may be directed at the situation. We can choose

situations that will provide us with desirable emotions – situation selection. We can

also try to modify the situation, acting in a way that causes us to receive desirable

emotions. In the specific situation we may also modify what we attend to (distraction

& concentration). As the emotional response is building up in us, we may also cogni-

tively change by way of reappraisal. A situation that was earlier appraised as fright-

ening may now be appraised differently. Finally, we may deal with emotions (re-

sponse modulation) by way of drugs, exercise or relaxation (Gross, 1999). How we

attend to affective states may also be different based on whether they are negative or

positive (Larsen & Prizmic, 2004). Behaviors often used to regulate negative affective

states include distraction, venting (expressing the negative affect), suppression, cogni-

tive reappraisal, self-reward, exercise, relaxation, eating and other physical manipu-

lations, socializing, and withdrawal (spending time alone), while behaviors to in-

crease positive affect instead include gratitude, helping others, and humor. Some re-

searchers argue for the benefits of accepting the emotional content without doing

36

42 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

anything with it (Fruzzetti, Shenk, Mosco, & Lowry, 2003), like in mindfulness

meditation. Thayer and colleagues (Thayer, Newman, & McClain, 1994) report that

the most effective way to change a bad mood is a combination of relaxation, stress

management, and cognitive and exercise techniques. In this context, Qigong may

also serve as a tool for regulating affect (reducing negative affect and enhancing posi-

tive affect).

Antecedents and dynamics of affective states

What determines our affective states? Is it as simple as feeling good when we do

something we like and feeling bad when we do something we do not like (Parkinson

et al., 1996)? A great number of antecedents that influence our affective states are

always present. Watson (2000) suggests that moods are influenced by affective traits

(personality traits like neuroticism have been associated with negative affect; Larsen

& Diener, 1992), situational/environmental factors (activities, food and drink, physi-

cal aspects of the environment, and hassles), endogenous/sociocultural rhythms (cir-

cadian rhythms may influence during the day, menstrual cycles in women during the

month, and seasonal changes), and individual characteristics. Further, research

shows that positive mood is usually low at the beginning and the end of the day

(peaking around midday). People generally report better mood during the weekend

than on weekdays, and from April to September than from October to March (in the

northern hemisphere). He concludes that positive moods are associated with active

behavior (doing) and negative moods are more associated with thoughts (thinking)

(Watson, 2000). It is also important to consider what state the individual was in be-

fore he/she assessed his/her affective state. A pleasant mood may not be exceptionally

high, but may rather be a consequence of feeling less bad (Parkinson et al., 1996).

Determinants may not only influence the individual from outside in a causal rela-

tionship uniform to all individuals. A different perspective entails the person-

environment fit, or the transactional perspective (Parkinson et al., 1996). A conse-

quence of the transactional approach is not what kind of antecedents influence af-

fective states, but how we interpret them. In the context of Qigong exercise, there

37

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 43

may be characteristics in the regimen (e.g., slow movements and relaxation) that are

known to influence affective states positively; additionally (from a transactional per-

spective), it is important that the exerciser experience enjoyment (Motl, Berger, &

Leuschen, 2000) for the benefits to occur. Thus, cause and effect and transactional

approaches may work at different times or simultaneously (Parkinson et al., 1996)

to influence affective states.

Measuring affective responses

A number of self-report measures have been created to assess affective responses (af-

fect, mood, and emotion) associated with exercise. The assessment of affect, based

on (or in concordance with) Russell’s (1980) view of affect (pleasantness and activa-

tion) can be made with one-item scales, like the Affect Grid (Russell, Weiss, & Men-

delsohn, 1989), the Self Assessment Manikin (SAM; Bradley & Young, 1994), and

the Feeling Scale (Hardy & Rejeski, 1989) together with the Felt Arousal Scale of the

Telic State Measure (Svebak & Murgatroyd, 1985). An example of a multi-item

scale is the Activation Deactivation Adjective Check List (AD ACL), which is based

on Thayer’s view of affect (consisting of tense and energetic arousal; Thayer, 1989).

Another multi-item scale is the Positive And Negative Affect Scale, based on Watson

and colleagues’ conceptualization (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Af-

fect measures have also been created in Swedish: Sjöberg, Svensson, & Persson

(1979), the Swedish Core Affect Scale (SCAS; Västfjäll, Friman, Gärling, & Kleiner,

2002) and Knez and Hygge (2001). Several of the above-mentioned affect scales are

often also defined as mood measures (AD ACL, PANAS, and SCAS), given that some

researchers view mood as an extended form of affect (Russell, 1999) or as similar to

affect (Watson, 2000).

The Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr, & Droppleman, 1981) is the

most widely used measure in Sport and Exercise Psychology (LeUnes & Burger,

1998). Another very common measure is the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory

(STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Luschene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983). The POMS assesses

six mood states and is considered a mood questionnaire (Gauvin & Spence, 1998).

38

44 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

The STAI measures anxiety and is considered to assess emotion anxiety (Gauvin &

Spence, 1988).

A group of measures are based on the idea that exercise-induced affective

changes are specific to that context. Self-report measures have therefore been cre-

ated to assess the specific affective changes taking place when exercising. These in-

struments include the Exercise-induced Feeling Inventory (EFI; Gauvin & Rejeski,

1993), the Subjective Exercise Experience Scale (SEES; McAuley & Corneya, 1994),

and the Physical Activity Affect Scale (PAAS; Lox, Jackson, Tuholski, Wasley, &

Treasure, 2000).

A framework for the study of affective responses

As the area of affective sciences includes several different views of affect, there is a

need to decide how to view affective responses in the context of Qigong exercise. I

have adopted the view presented by Ekkekakis and Petruzzello (2000), who advo-

cate a functionalist view of affect in which all kinds of information processing (emo-

tional and cognitive) are adaptational to the demands of the environment through

pairing survival and well-being with survival and well-being-enhancing behavior

(Keltner & Gross, 1999). The functionalist perspective view distinguishes between

affect and emotion, on different levels of information processing, stemming from

earlier and later stages of human evolution, cognition being a later development in

consciousness than affect (Panksepp, 2003). Emotions, which include cognitive (and

other) components, therefore comprise a later and more complex structure than af-

fect (Izard, 1993; Leventhal & Scherer, 1987). Leventhal and Scherer’s model

(1987) spans from sensory-motor levels (automatic and reflex-like) to more complex

levels of information processing: schematic (more cognitive level, integration be-

tween automatic processes and feelings) and conceptual levels (highly cognitive level

with abstract rules about emotions). Ekkekakis and Petruzzello (2000) suggest that

exercise affects the exerciser on all levels (from basic to more complex). The state-

ment about vigorous exercise that “it hurts so good” may therefore be interpreted

from different levels of processing (Ekkekakis, 2003, p. 217): On a more basic level,

39

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 45

the signals of exhaustion and depletion of energy from the body may signal negative

effects, while on a more complex level, the fact that one is engaging in an exercise

regimen may induce feelings of pride and satisfaction. Similarly, in a Qigong con-

text, the slow movements may be experienced as pleasant, and on a higher level as

joy or happiness for some or as boring for others as the movements may make them

feel restless and wish they were doing something “more useful”.

40

46 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Acute affective responses

Suggested mechanisms for acute affective benefits

Given that Qigong exercise constitutes a number of different characteristics, includ-

ing slow movements, relaxation, meditation, imagery, natural breathing and self-

massage, a number of different theories on active mechanisms may work simultane-

ously and interactionally to affect the exerciser’s affective state. Below are some

suggestions and theories that intend to explain the Qigong exercise-affect relation-

ship. Chen (2008) speculates on how Qigong may enhance health and healing from

a TCM perspective: 1) First, vital qi may flow more strongly through the meridians

after Qigong practice and enhance health; 2) The cultivation of consciousness and

intention (yi) may release suppressed emotions and resolve mental disturbances. Fan

(2000) states that emotional disturbances may be cleared by Qigong exercise. Fur-

ther, it may release the individual from the socialized self (determinant of stress) and

allow him/her to return to a more natural original self. 3) Finally, Qigong exercise

may uncover the body’s self-healing capabilities by strengthening the immune sys-

tem.

From a Western scientific point of view, Qigong exercise may affect the exerciser

in a number of different ways, including the nervous system, by way of the relaxa-

tion response, placebo/positive expectations, cultivation of positive emotions, al-

tered states of consciousness, and other suggestions and theories related to physical

activity and exercise science. The autonomic nervous system consists of two parts,

the sympathetic (mobilizes and utilizes energy) and the parasympathetic (promotes

restorative processes), which enables the body to adapt to different environmental

situations and secure the individual’s survival. Certain behavioral conditions are

more prevalent in one or the other of the two systems (Recordati, 2003). Sympa-

thetic behaviors include the transition from sleep to wakefulness, muscle exercises

and mental arousal. Among conditions such as sleep, hibernation and post-exercise,

the voluntary control of breathing, grooming and repetition of a stimulus seem es-

pecially relevant as they are important aspects of Qigong exercise. Grooming refers

41

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 47

to activities concerned with the primary biological functions of caring for the body

surface, and may in this case be related to self-massage and clapping of the body, as

well as the focus on the inner body which are performed during qigong. Also, the

repetition of a stimulus is evident in Qigong activities in the repetitive, slow move-

ments. Finally, slow, deep breathing increases parasympathetic activity (Bernardi,

2002), also a vital part of Qigong exercise. It is suggested that sympathetic activity

focuses on work related to the external environment, while parasympathetic activity

instead focuses on work related to the internal environment, self-protection and re-

covery (Recordati, 2003).

Research on meditation at the beginning of the 70s led to the suggestion that

mind-body therapies share specific characteristics that may elicit the Relaxation Re-

sponse (Benson, 1983; Esch, Fricchione, & Stefano, 2003). These characteristics in-

clude a mental device (a sound, a word, or some other object for mental focus), a

passive attitude (not worrying about how well one is performing, or if distracting

thoughts occur, redirecting one’s attention to the repetition of the mental device),

decreased muscle tonus (a comfortable posture or sitting position), and a quiet envi-

ronment (Benson, Greenwood, & Klemchuk, 1975). The Relaxation Response is

associated with decreased sympathetic nervous system activity, reduced oxygen con-

sumption, reduction in heart and respiratory rates, decreased blood lactate concen-

tration, increased alpha-wave activity, and stabilization of muscle blood flow (Ben-

son, 1983; Benson et al., 1975; Levander, Benson, Wheeler, & Wallace, 1972).

Mind-body therapies (Jin, 1992) and physical activity (O’Halloran, Murphy, &

Webster, 2002, 2005) have been associated with the placebo effect or positive ex-

pectations. A number of theories attempt to explain how placebo may affect psycho-

logical and physiological health: Expectancy Theory, classical conditioning, and

Emotional Change Theory (Catanzaro & Mearns, 1999; Olson, Roese, & Zanna,

1996). In the context of the Schematic Processing Approach, Lund (1987) suggests

that by taking a placebo or engaging in something one believes will lead to a posi-

tive outcome, a cognitive schema of expected effects is activated. When activated,

individuals are more likely to recall and notice information or experiences that are

consistent with that schema, interpret ambiguous information consistent with the

42

48 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

schema, and overlook mildly inconsistent information or dismiss it as invalid. Given

that the placebo effect is enhanced when one has an inner bodily focus (Geers,

Helfer, Weiland, & Kosbab, 2006), it is likely that positive expectations and affec-

tive Qigong outcomes will be associated (or perhaps also have an influence), as the

qigong exerciser focuses on the inner body.

Qigong exercise and similar relaxation techniques may enable the cultivation of

positive emotions (Fredrickson, 2000). While negative emotions may narrow the

thought-action repertoire (e.g., fear is linked with the need to escape), positive emo-

tions may work and function in a different way. In her Broaden-and-Build Model of

positive emotions (1998), Fredrickson suggests that positive emotions may broaden

an individual’s thought-action repertoire so that it is incompatible with that of nega-

tive emotions. This can loosen the hold that negative emotions such as depression or

anxiety may have over the individual’s psychological and physiological responses

(the Undoing Hypothesis). Specifically, Qigong exercise may cultivate positive emo-

tions like contentment (an appreciation for the present moment and also integrating

those experiences to better appreciate one’s place in the world). Relaxation tech-

niques are often multicomponental (Qigong consists of relaxation, visualization, re-

laxed breathing, etc.). They may focus on one component that is included in the

emotion. The relaxation of muscles in relaxation training may be associated with

the physiological relaxed state of the body that is associated with experiencing posi-

tive emotions. Inducing one component of the emotion may jump-start other com-

ponents of the same emotion (e.g., cognitions and facial expressions).

Qigong exercise may also induce an Altered State of Consciousness (ASC; Vaitl,

Birbaumer, Gruzelier, Jamieson, Kotchoubey, Kübler et al., 2005) by way of respira-

tory maneuvers, relaxation, etc. These states are associated with changes in activa-

tion, awareness span, self-awareness (e.g., absorption in the activity and forgetting

the self), the experience of sensations, body image, suggestibility, time experience,

emotions, self control, and sense of personal identity (Farthing, 1992; Vaitl et al.,

2005). One example of ASC is Flow or Optimal Experience (Nakamura & Csik-

szentmihalyi, 2002). The Flow state may be induced when the individual’s ability

matches the demands of the activity and when goals and feedback are clearly visible.

43

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 49

The Flow state is characterized by great focus on the present moment, a merging of

action and awareness, a loss of reflective self-consciousness, a sense of control and

of time distortion, and is rewarding in itself (intrinsic). A related concept is absorp-

tion, in which an individual forgets everything else except the focus of attention.

Whereas absorption is characterized by passivity, concentration or attention is more

active, although they are related (Davidson & Schwartz, 1976). The ability to con-

centrate has been associated with perceived health in the context of Qigong (Jouper,

Hassmén, & Johansson, 2006) and is very important in Qigong practice (Fan,

2000). In his cognitive-behavioral model of relaxation, Smith (1990) stresses the

importance of focusing, passivity, and receptivity for inducing a relaxed state, sug-

gesting an optimal level between activity and passivity for relaxation. A suggestion

was recently presented for biological correlates of ASC experiences: The Transient

Hypofrontality Hypothesis (Dietrich, 2003). This theory states that by narrowing

attention (through focusing on the body, breathing, or a mantra), higher cortical

functions (the prefrontal cortex) responsible for analyzing, differentiating, and logi-

cal reasoning may be down-regulated, leading to a state of unity, timelessness, ab-

sence of reflection, and reduced depression and anxiety. Thus, because of the char-

acteristics of Qigong an altered state of consciousness can be induced, which can be

experienced as a pleasant affective state.

Leaving the more general suggestions for the Qigong-affect relationship, I now

turn to those that are specific to physical activity and the area of exercise science.

Although it is only one component of Qigong, the role of low-intensity physical ac-

tivity is important. By engaging in an activity the exerciser can feel good about

him/herself for being able to adhere to the regimen, master difficult movements, or

reduce arousal (mastery/self-efficacy; Bandura, 1982). This may lead to a sense of

accomplishment affecting physical self-efficacy and self-concept, which may lead to

increased global self-esteem (Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989). The Time-Out Hypothe-

sis (Bahrke & Morgan, 1978) suggests that anxiety may be reduced due to a leaving

behind of, or taking a time-out from, worries. The activity in itself does not lead to

a better affective state, but instead simply distracts the exerciser from the stressors

of daily life. The secretion of endorphins (Hoffman, 1997) and neurotransmitters

44

50 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

(serotonin, dopamine, epinephrine, etc.) as the biological correlate of value for well-

being (Chaouloff, 1997; Dishman, 1997) has been suggested as the reason for the

emotional benefits associated with physical exercise. The affective benefits of exer-

cise may also be influenced by increased body temperature (Thermogenic Hypothe-

sis; Petruzzello, Landers, & Salazar, 1993), by being regularly influenced by the

stress of physical activity (Physiological Toughness Model; Dienstbier, 1989), the

fact that being physically active is inherent in our genetic makeup (living a sedentary

life therefore creates negative mental health; Anthropological Hypothesis, Martin-

sen, 2002), and by socially interacting with other people. All the suggestions men-

tioned may provide for affective responses in the context of Qigong (except perhaps

the Thermogenic Hypothesis and the Physiological Toughness Model), and social

interaction may not be solely responsible, as solitaire exercise has led to improved

affective states (Szabo, 2003).

The Opponent Process Theory (Solomon & Corbit, 1973) has been associated

with physical activity-associated affective benefits. This theory is based on the as-

sumption that the brain is organized to oppose both pleasurable and aversive emo-

tional states, by creating a countering reaction. As the body is greatly activated dur-

ing exercise, upon extinction of the bout, the opponent process, a pleasant deacti-

vated state will follow. Extending the Opponent Process Theory, Bixby and collabo-

rators suggest that the intensity level of the exercise influences the affective change

(Bixby, Spalding, & Hatfield, 2001). Affective changes of high intensities, the Re-

bound Model, are characterized by an initial decline in perceived affect (aversive)

and later an increase in affect reaching higher affective levels (than baseline) at post-

measurements. At low intensities, the affective change is characterized by an eleva-

tion of affect until it reaches a plateau, referred to as the Maintenance Model. Both

high and low intensities lead to post-exercise elevation of affect, but affective

changes during exercise take different pathways.

A recent important suggestion is the Dual-Mode Model (Ekkekakis, 2004), which

is based on the assumption that physical activity is an essential component of hu-

man evolution. Affective responses are evolved psychological mechanisms that are

adaptational in that they promote health and well-being by way of pleasure (utility)

45

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 51

and displeasure (danger). Affective responses are dependent on a hierarchically or-

ganized system with different controlled components and encompass functions from

primitive ones that show little inter-individual variability to more advanced ones

that show greater variability. Further, the affective responses to physical activity are

dependent on the interplay between two factors: cognitive processes (meaning of ex-

ercise, goals, self-perceptions) and interoceptive cues (exercise-induced physiological

changes). Low intensities are proposed to be characterized by a low influence of

cognitive factors and most individuals experiencing pleasure during the bout. Me-

dium intensities display a strong influence of cognitive factors and great variability

between individuals; some experience more pleasantness, some more unpleasantness

(during). Finally, high intensity levels of exercise are instead theorized as displaying

a strong influence of interoceptive factors and with most individuals experiencing

displeasure. The majority of individuals are proposed to experience pleasantness

post-exercise.

Acute affective responses to physical activity

Reviews of the literature show that affective improvements accompany the termina-

tion of most forms of physical activity (Biddle, 2000; Ekkekakis & Petruzzello,

1999; Reed & Ones, 2006; Yeung, 1996). Focusing on low and moderate-intensity

exercise, a number of studies on walking display acute affective benefits. Five min-

utes of walking was associated with increased energy post-exercise (Thayer, Peters,

Takashi, & Birkhead-Flight, 1993) and perceived energy and tension reduction

(Thayer, 1987). State anxiety was reduced after a 40-minute walk on a treadmill

(Porcari, et al., 1998). Ekkekakis and colleagues (Ekkekakis, Hall, Van Landuyt, &

Petruzzello, 1999) found that short walks (10-15 minutes) were associated with in-

creased activation and more positive affective valence during the bout. Recovery

from walking was associated with calmness and relaxation. Further, active middle-

aged and elderly individuals were prescribed 15 minutes of walking or silent read-

ing. Results displayed increased self-reported energy during and post-exercise (Ek-

kekakis, Backhouse, Gray, & Lind, 2008) significantly better than the control con-

46

52 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

dition. The above studies show that low-intensity exercise of short duration may

also provide for a better affective state.

Other variables important for the improvement of affective reactions in relation

to physical exercise include self-selected intensity (Parfitt, Rose, & Marklund,

2000), the activity being enjoyable (Daley & Maynard, 2003; Lane, Jackson, &

Terry, 2005, Parfitt & Gledhill, 2004), similar affective benefits independent of du-

ration (Daley & Welch, 2004; Hansen, Stevens, & Coast, 2001; Petruzzello &

Landers, 1994), the bout not exceeding 75 minutes (Reed & Ones, 2006), cogni-

tions during the bout (Blanchard, Rodgers, & Gavin, 2004), and the affective state

before the bout (more negative affective state before is associated with more positive

state after (Reed et al., 2004)).

For a long time, the recommended intensity level of physical activity for optimal

affective benefits was that of moderate levels (cf. Berger, 2001), and it was suggested

that a certain intensity threshold was necessary for the provision of these benefits

(Kikcaldy & Shephard, 1990). In his Dual-Mode Model (described elsewhere), Ek-

kekakis (2004) posits that pleasurable responses can be found at all intensities ex-

cept high intensity. This model has some support (Ekkekakis, Hall, & Petruzzello,

2005; Welch, Hulley, Ferguson, & Beauchamp, 2007), and from the discussion

above about investigations of activities ranging from low intensities like walking to

higher intensities, many levels of intensity are associated with affective benefits.

Acute affective responses to mind-body therapies

Investigations of specific mind-body therapies show affective benefits in a number of

studies. Yoga is related to a predominantly sympathetic tone of the nervous system

during the activity and parasympathetic after (Savang & Telles, 2006). Negative

mood and anxiety were reduced after a Yoga session, but there was no increase in

Vigor (positive mood; Berger & Owen, 1992). Tai Chi showed a reduction in sympa-

thetic nervous system activity and an increase in parasympathetic activity after a

bout (Lu & Kuo, 2003). In another study, at post-measures, Tai Chi displayed in-

creased heartbeat, increased urinary noradrenalin, decreased salivary cortisol, and

47

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 53

increased positive mood (Vigor) as well as decreased negative mood (anxiety, ten-

sion, depression; Jin, 1989). Relaxation was associated with a reduction in arousal

and distress (Peveler & Johnston, 1986) and an alleviation of somatic and cognitive

anxiety (Gill, Kolt, & Keating, 2004).

Comparing the effects of different mind-body therapies and other activities, both

relaxation and short walks were associated with a reduction in tense arousal, but

only walks were associated with feeling energized afterwards (Saklofske, Blomme, &

Kelly, 1997). Yoga was associated with increased perceived physical energy and feel-

ing alert, while relaxation increased sluggishness (Wood, 1998). Studies by Berger

and associates (Berger, Friedmann, & Eaton, 1988; Berger & Owen, 1992) display

similar post-exercise effects (reduction in negative mood) that were significantly dif-

ferent from controls. Low-intensity mindful activities (Feldenkreis & Yoga) were su-

perior in enhancing subjective well-being and reducing state anxiety and depression

compared to a high-intensity non-mindful activity (dance aerobics; Netz & Lidor,

2003). Finally, Tai Chi, Yoga and Martial Arts were associated with a reduction in

psychological distress, fatigue and exhaustion to the same extent, however only the

Tai Chi and Yoga displayed increased tranquility (Szabo, Meskó, Caputo, & Gill,

1998).

Mind-body therapies show changes comparable to other physical activities, as

they share similar characteristics. However, when they lack the physically active as-

pect, like in relaxation, people generally do not feel activated like they do in Yoga

and Tai Chi. Feeling activated is probably associated with the intensity of the activ-

ity. Naruse and Hirai (2000) showed a positive relationship between exercise inten-

sity and arousal level (lower intensity and lower arousal). It has been proposed that

mind-body methods may reduce negative mood while not enhancing positive mood

(Yeung, 1996). Few results of the above-mentioned studies show increased positive

mood; however, this may be due to the use of scales predominantly covering nega-

tive aspects of mood, such as the POMS (five negative subscales and one positive).

Mind-body therapies seem to affect the exerciser in ways that stabilize the nervous

system and provide a reduction in negative mood and sometimes an increase in posi-

tive mood.

48

54 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Acute affective responses to Qigong exercise

Qigong studies have also focused on acute psychological responses to single ses-

sions. Qi-training (Qigong) of a group of male beginners (having learned the form

prior to the study) was studied, comparing an actual session with a sham-control

(movements without mindful intention) qi session (Lee, Kang, Lim, & Lee, 2004).

State anxiety (STAI) was reduced significantly more (26% to 9%) in the actual Qi-

training session. Also, plasma concentration of ACTH, cortisol, and aldosterone de-

creased in the Qi-training condition but not in the sham session. It is concluded that

Qigong has effects on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Kjos and Etnier

(2006) used the PANAS (Watson, 1988) to study affect before, after and at three

15-minute intervals pre-exercise, for a group of older (mean age 65 years) female

beginners who had learned Qigong prior to the study. The effects of Qigong were

compared to walking. There was a significant increase in Positive Affect from pre-

exercise to post-exercise (similar to Pleasant Activation in Figure 2) that was compa-

rable to walking. Positive Affect rapidly decreased after the bout to below pre-

exercise levels at the post-15-minute assessment. No significant change was found

for Negative affect (similar to Unpleasant Activation of Figure 2). Qigong was con-

sidered a moderate-intensity activity that was comparable to walking.

The above two sole studies of acute affective responses to Qigong exercise display

promising benefits. However, in the context of Unpleasant Activation (Figure 2),

where anxiety and Negative Affect can be found, contrasting results are found:; a

reduction in anxiety in the Lee et al. (2004) study, and no significant reduction in

Negative Affect in the Kjos and Etnier (2006) study. The reasons for this difference

are not known, but there were only men in the Lee et al. (2004) study and only

women in the Kjos and Etnier (2006) study. This also shows the limitations associ-

ated with using the PANAS (restricted to only the high activated poles of the cir-

cumplex structure of affect) in the context of Qigong. It seems reasonable that

Qigong is also associated with the deactivated poles of the circumplex (e.g., Un-

pleasant Deactivation).

49

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 55

Unanswered questions in the Qigong-affect relationship

When work was commenced on the first study of the present thesis, Qigong exercise

had only been found to reduce state anxiety (Lee et al., 2004). However, anecdotal

reports, a master’s thesis (Brinker, 1998), and studies of mind-body therapies (Berger

et al., 1988; Berger & Owen, 1992; Jin, 1992; Gill et al., 2004; Peveler & Johnston,

1986; Saklofske et al., 1997; Szabo et al., 1998) and low-intensity physical activity

(Ekkekakis et al., 1999; Porcari et al., 1998; Thayer, 1987; Thayer et al., 1993) also

suggested that Qigong exercise would perhaps display an association with affective

benefits.

The continued study of Qigong-related affective responses is important for a

number of reasons. In addition to commencing studies in a new field of research, the

increasing interest from the public in mind-body therapies (Kelner & Wellman,

2000; Landstingsförbundet, 2000; Wolsko et al., 2004) is indeed an important incen-

tive for further study. Studying acute (positive) affective responses appeals to re-

searchers, because most people want to be happy or experience subjective well-being.

Physical activity is often recommended for the enhancement of mental health (FYSS,

2008). However, not everyone (e.g. the elderly) is able to engage in higher-intensity

activities, and some may not like it. In studying affective responses to Qigong exer-

cise we may find new ways for people to increase their health and well-being. From a

theoretical standpoint, studying Qigong, which includes both mindful (relaxation

and meditation) characteristics and physical activity, is a fascinating prospect. Thus,

the continued study of the acute affective responses to qigong exercise may provide

promising benefits in the areas of positive affect and subjective well-being

(Fredrickson, 2000), affect regulation (Wallace & Shapiro, 2006), and managing and

reducing physiological and psychological arousal (e.g., stress; Gunnarsson, 2004).

50

56 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Rationale for the present thesis

The study of Qigong-related acute affective responses is a fairly new avenue of re-

search. A very limited number of studies had been carried out prior to the present

thesis. Given the similar research context, I chose to study Qigong from an exercise

psychology perspective. Although there may be some dissimilarities between, for ex-

ample, jogging and Qigong (the foremost being the focus on the mindful intention in

Qigong), the physical activity part is included in both exercises and may allow for

the use of similar ways of study. Perspectives and methodologies in the present the-

sis are therefore influenced by the area of exercise psychology.

Given the rather broad population of Biyun Qigong exercisers in Sweden, I chose

to commence my investigations at the annual Biyun Qigong training camp, which

takes place a few days every summer. This meant having the opportunity to meet up

to a hundred Qigong exercisers at the same time and place. All members of the

Green Dragon (the member association of Biyun Qigong in Sweden) who have taken

a course in the basic qigong form (Jichu Gong) are welcome to attend the camp,

which includes lectures on Qigong and TCM, Qigong exercise, and meeting others

interested in Qigong.

Jouper and colleagues published a study on the demographics of the members of

the Green Dragon (Jouper et al., 2006), showing a majority of women in the associa-

tion: In 2007 there were 102 men and 1348 women (G. Jacobson, personal commu-

nication). The majority of members have a university degree (57%), and have com-

pleted an average of four (SD = 4) qigong courses. At the time, 44% were employed,

45% retired, and 11% students. They are an active group, engaging in other physical

activities 4.2 (SD = 2.9) times a week. The reasons for beginning Qigong exercise are

mainly curiosity, but also to find a means of promoting personal health and recuper-

ating from illness. Reasons for continuing to practice include feelings of personal

well-being and physical health preservation (Jouper et al., 2006). The members of

the Green Dragon display means in the middle range (unpublished data) compared

to norm means for Antonovsky’s Sense of Coherence Scale (Antonovsky, 1991), and

51

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 57

Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Scales (Lindfors, Berntsson, & Lundberg, 2006; Ryff

& Keyes, 1995).

52

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 59

AIM OF THE THESIS

The aim of the present thesis is to study Qigong-related acute affective responses in

a group of regular exercisers. Affective responses will be assessed on different levels

of the affective hierarchy: from the level of affect to moods and emotions, using cor-

responding self-report measures. Further, through the use of mean scores, individual

responses, and open-ended questions, the affective Qigong experience will also be

studied. Assessments will be made before, during, and after the Qigong bout. Spe-

cifically, the following research questions are posed in the thesis:

Are the acute affective responses associated with Qigong exercise signifi-

cantly different from a control condition?

Are the acute affective responses associated with Qigong exercise dependent

on the length of the session?

Is there a relationship between acute affective responses and positive expecta-

tions of affective benefits?

How are affective responses displayed and how do they change, from pre-, to

during, to post-exercise, using means and individual responses?

How do the exercisers describe their experience associated with Qigong exer-

cise, during and after the session?

53

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 61

STUDIES

Study I

Johansson M., Hassmén, P., & Jouper, J. (2008). Acute effects of qigong exercise on

mood and anxiety. International Journal of Stress Management, 15, 199-207.

Introduction

Today, stress causes widespread ill health, demonstrated by increased rates of mus-

culoskeletal disorders, anxiety, and depression (Bergdahl & Bergdahl, 2002; Krantz,

& Lundberg, 2006; Ursin, 2000). Increasing evidence points to the benefits of using

physical activity and mind-body therapies (e.g. Yoga, Tai Chi, and Qigong) as a

means for managing and alleviating stress by improving mood and reducing anxiety

(Berger & Motl, 2001; Granath, Ingvarsson, von Thiele, & Lundberg, 2006;

Petruzzello, Landers, Hatfield, Kubitz, & Salazar, 1991; Wolsko et al., 2004). Chi-

nese traditions have emerged with a method, Qigong, which combines slow move-

ments, relaxed breathing, and deep mental focusing. Studies show that Qigong may

increase the overall health and well-being of the exerciser (Lee, Ryu, & Chung,

2000; Mayer, 1999; Ryu et al., 1995). Acute affective (mood and emotional) re-

sponses to single sessions of Qigong exercise are fairly limited, yet one study (Lee,

Kang, Lim, Lee, 2004) reported reduced anxiety after one hour of Qigong exercise.

Given that everyone does not have the time to engage in an hourly Qigong bout, it

would be interesting to investigate whether a 30-minute bout would also yield posi-

tive affective benefits. The aim of the present study was therefore to study the acute

effects on mood and anxiety after 30 minutes of Qigong exercise, compared with a

control condition.

Method

Participants at a Qigong camp were asked to participate, and those willing (59 indi-

viduals, 8 men and 51 women; 50.8 years of age, SD = 3.1) were consequently first

54

62 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

matched according to age, instructor versus exerciser, and gender, and were later

randomized to a Qigong group or a control group (attending a lecture on Tradi-

tional Chinese Medicine by Biyun founder and Qigong master Fan Xiulan).

Mood and anxiety were measured using the Profile of Mood States (POMS;

McNair et al., 1992) and the state form of the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory

(STAI; Spielberger et al., 1983). The POMS consists of 65 items assessing six affec-

tive states: tension/anxiety, depression/dejection, anger/hostility, vigor/activity, and

confusion/bewilderment. The respondents rate their feelings on a five-point (0-4) in-

tensity scale; the question “How are you feeling right now?” was chosen. The POMS

has been validated by different groups of individuals: psychiatric outpatients and col-

lege groups (McNair et al., 1992), and adult and geriatric populations (Nyenhuis,

Yamamoto, Luchetta, Terrien, & Permentier, 1999).

The STAI has been validated in a number of college student samples (Spielberger

et al., 1970) and has strong psychometric qualities (Gauvin & Spence, 1998). The

STAI comes in two versions, a trait and a state version. The state version (SAI) was

used here. Respondents assess their level of anxiety on a four-point (1-4) intensity

scale, ranging from Not at all to Very much so. The state version of the STAI con-

sists of 20 statements.

Before and after the Qigong and control intervention, the POMS and the STAI

were filled out. Initial t-tests on all dependent variables were used to rule out any

pre-intervention differences. Separate mixed (Group x Time) ANOVAs for each de-

pendent variable (7) were made. Alpha was set at .007 to reduce for type-I errors,

due to multiple testing (Bonferroni correction).

Results and discussion

Pre-scores on all dependent variables showed no differences between groups. No

gender differences were found in a comparison of pre-scores on the STAI and the

POMS. Taken together, the two groups displayed reduced state anxiety (STAI),

Tension, Depression, Fatigue, Confusion, and enhanced Vigor (POMS). Comparing

the two groups, STAI, Depression, Anger, and Fatigue were reduced significantly

55

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 63

more in the Qigong group (Tension, Confusion and Vigor changed more in the

Qigong group, but not significantly).

The results of the study are discussed comparing different magnitudes of anxiety

reduction with another Qigong study, and the influence of having regular exercisers

in the study. Given that the control condition was attending a lecture by Qigong

master Fan Xiulan, an important person to the individuals in the study, possible

limitations and suggestions for future optimal control conditions are discussed. Thus

it seems equally beneficial for the Qigong exercisers to listen to a lecture by the

Qigong master on Tension, Confusion, and Vigor. In particular, the non-significant

difference on the Confusion subscale (POMS) between the Qigong and control

groups may be explained by the cognitive characteristics of the lecture. Possible ac-

tive mechanisms for the effects on mood and anxiety are suggested by way of in-

creased parasympathetic tone (focus on internal body, repetitive tasks and breath-

ing; Recordati, 2003), elicitation of the relaxation response (Benson et al., 1975),

Oxytocin secretion by way of self-massage (Uvnäs-Moberg et al., 2005), and by cul-

tivation of positive emotions (the Broaden-and-Build Model; Fredrickson, 2000).

Study II

Johansson, J., & Hassmén, P. (2008). Acute psychological responses to qigong exer-

cise of varying durations. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 36, 449-458.

Introduction

Previous Qigong studies investigating acute anxiety responses show different magni-

tudes of reduction. Although not involving the same Qigong styles, a 60-minute bout

of qigong exercise (Lee et al., 2000; 24% reduction) displayed a greater reduction in

anxiety compared to a 30-minute bout (Johansson, Hassmén, & Jouper, 2008; 12%

reduction). On the contrary, studies of physical exercise do not display greater affec-

tive effects after longer sessions of exercise compared to shorter ones (e.g. Daley &

56

64 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Welch, 2004; Hansen, Stevens, & Coast, 2001). The aim of this study was therefore

to compare a 30-minute Qigong session with a 60-minute Qigong session.

Method

A matched (age, instructor versus exerciser, and gender) randomization was per-

formed with regular Qigong exercisers at a Qigong summer camp who were willing

to participate in the study (N = 41). Participants (35 women and 6 men; 56.7 years

of age, SD = 12.4) were allocated to a repeated measures cross-sectional design. On

the first day, one group began with 30 minutes of exercise and the other with 60

minutes of exercise. On the following day conditions were reversed, resulting in all

participants taking part in both conditions and a controlling for order effects. Meas-

ures of mood (POMS; McNair et al., 1992), state anxiety (STAI; Spielberger et al.,

1983), and Affect (self constructed one-item scales, end points representing Highest

possible and Lowest possible, measuring Activation and Valence). See Study I for

psychometric properties of the POMS and the STAI. Separate 2 x 2 within-subject

ANOVAs were carried out for each dependent variable (9). Type-I error was pro-

tected by Bonferroni correction, setting alpha-level at .006. Given that the depression

subscale of the POMS has been used as a screening instrument for depression (Grif-

fith, Szaflarski, Kent, Schefft, Howe, & Privitera, 2005), participants with scores of

more than 1 SD above the norm of healthy females (McNair et al., 1991) were ex-

cluded from subsequent analysis (five individuals).

Results and discussion

All measures (except Activation) displayed significant beneficial changes. No signifi-

cant differences (p < .006) on the six subscales of the POMS, the STAI, and the self-

made measures of Affect, in the magnitude of changes, were found when the two

conditions were compared. Both conditions, 30 and 60 minutes, displayed significant

beneficial psychological changes (except on Activation). A longer Qigong session

therefore does not induce greater psychological responses than a shorter session

57

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 65

does. Many people report having limited time to engage in an exercise regimen; it is

therefore positive that a short session (30 minutes) is enough to induce positive psy-

chological responses of the same magnitude as a longer one (60 minutes).

The Relaxation Response (Benson et al., 1975) has been suggested as a possible

active mechanism for the beneficial effects on affective states, associated with

Qigong and mind-body therapies. The results of this study are discussed in light of

the fact that regular Qigong exercisers may be more skilled and thus require less

time to elicit the Relaxation Response, and therefore similar affective benefits in

both the 30 and 60-minute conditions. Further, the equivocal results of Vigor but

not Activation being significant are proposed to be due to the POMS being a vali-

dated instrument while the wording of the Activation measure may have an influ-

ence. A point is also made in Study II regarding the narrowness of the present per-

spective. The focus is on acute affective changes. Qigong exercise is not focused

solely on improving affective states, but rather on the continuous daily work of im-

proving health for the whole individual: physiologically, psychologically, spiritually,

and energetically. Jichu Gong is the first Qigong maneuver the beginner learns, and

Dong Gong is the second. These two different maneuvers have different aims, ac-

cording to Fan (2000), and cannot really be compared. The success of Dong Gong

practice is dependent on some experience of Jichu Gong; it is actually recommended

that the student practice Jichu Gong daily for three months before learning Dong

Gong (Fan, 2000). In the 30-minute condition Jichu Gong was performed, and in

the 60-minute condition Jichu Gong and Dong Gong were performed. However,

from a perspective of affective regulation, a single session of 30 minutes of Qigong

exercise may give the exerciser a boost in positive mood. In future studies, one

should observe even shorter bouts of Qigong exercise to see if they also provide af-

fective benefits. The focus of Study II is on people reporting not having time to exer-

cise, and therefore studies shorter bouts to determine whether they are as beneficial

as longer ones.

58

66 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Study III

Johansson, M., Hassmén, P., & Hertting, A. (manuscript) Affective responses to

qigong exercise. A Pilot study. Submitted for publication.

Introduction

In this study we wanted to extend and develop ways of studying acute affective re-

sponses to Qigong exercise, which have not previously been done, and adopted re-

cent recommendations from exercise psychology. Hence, we chose to study affective

responses by way of a dimensional perspective on the level of affect, employing as-

sessments before, twice during (10 and 20 minutes), and after exercise. In addition to

studying average mean scores, we presented individual scores to show the individual

variation in affective changes. This perspective has been proposed by a number of

researchers (Backhouse, Ekkekakis, Biddle, Foskett, & Williams, 2007; Van Landuyt

et al., 2000) and may more realistically display the variations in affective responses

during and following physical activity. This is a new and more realistic perspective

on the classical idea that exercise makes you feel better, which has been criticized for

being too simplistic and may have been detrimental in making people become more

physically active. Given that placebo or positive expectations have been associated

with Tai Chi (Jin, 1992), we chose to study a possible relationship with the actual

affective outcomes. Finally, open-ended questions were employed to allow the par-

ticipants to describe affective experiences associated with Qigong.

Method

Forty-six women chose to participate in the study, out of 88 attending participants

(men and women) at a Biyun Qigong summer training camp. Mean age was 59

years (SD 9.2). We chose to study affect from a dimensional perspective using a

modified version of the short Swedish Core Affect Scale (SCAS; Västfjäll et al.,

2002; Västfjäll & Gärling, 2007). We thus measured affect in four dimensions: Un-

pleasantness-Pleasantness, Deactivation-Activation, Unpleasant Deactivation-

59

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 67

Pleasant Activation, and Unpleasant Activation-Pleasant Deactivation (See Figure 2).

The SCAS was validated and determined to be a reliable measure of the dimensions

of the affect circumplex (Västfjäll et al., 2002). As affect is assessed on only two di-

mensions in the short version (Västfjäll & Gärling, 2007), we included the extra two

(rotated in italics) that are included in the original version (Västfjäll et al., 2002),

while keeping the rating format of the short version (for multiple assessments). As

such, respondents rated their present affective state in a bipolar rating format (1-9).

Thus, affective responses could be assessed on four dimensions (as recommended by

Larsen & Diener, 1992).

In addition to tracking affective responses as mean averages, we studied inter-

individual responses on the Unpleasantness-Pleasantness dimension on the SCAS.

This was done from Pre- to In-task 1, and to In-task 2, categorizing responses ac-

cording to Positive, Negative, Fluctuations, and No change (Rose & Parfitt, 2007);

see Table 1. Inter-individual responses were also measured in a similar manner be-

tween each assessment (including post-measurement).

A Swedish version of the Beliefs Concerning Mood Improvements Associated

With Running Scale (O’Halloran, Murphy, & Webster, 2002) was administered to

measure beliefs regarding positive expectations related to Qigong exercise, and corre-

lations (Pearson) with actual outcomes were calculated. The scale consists of five

items measuring expected mood changes during exercise, and two items assessing

expected mood changes after the bout. Scores on the two subscales were calculated

using mean scores of the items belonging to each subscale (possible scoring on each

subscale 1-5). Examples of statements include “I experience feelings of improved

mood during a session of Qigong”. The authors report satisfactory internal consis-

tency (Cronbach alphas = .77 and .86 respectively).

To measure perceived exertion, the Borg scale (6-20) of perceived exertion (RPE)

was used in Study III. The scale ranges from No exertion at all (6) to Maximal exer-

tion. Psychometric information can be found in Borg (1998).

60

68 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

The participants were also told to describe the experience and how they generally

feel during and after a Qigong session. Adjectives describing how they feel were

grouped into the eight end points of the circumplex structure of affect, by way of

quantitative content analysis (Berg, 2001).

61

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 69

Four separate repeated measures ANOVAs were carried out for each affective

scale on the short modified SCAS. Post hoc tests (t-test) were employed when

ANOVAs were significant. The Bonferroni correction was employed to correct for

the risk of type-I error, setting alpha at .01. When violations of sphericity were de-

tected, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were used.

Results and discussion

Significant increases of affect (on repeated ANOVAs) between each assessment (Pre-

In-task 1, -In-task 2, and Post) were found (p < .01). Significant changes were found

(p < .01) between each assessment and every dimension (Bonferroni corrected t-tests)

on the SCAS, resulting in a shift toward increased Pleasant Activated and Deacti-

vated affect. However, the Unpleasantness-Pleasantness dimension did not display

significant affective benefits between Pre- and In-task 1. A recommendation is there-

fore that for affective benefits to be significant, the duration should not be shorter

than 20 minutes.

In a comparison of individual scores before and during, the majority of responses

indicated positive affective change, followed by no change. Further, studying changes

between single assessments, a majority of participants first show (Pre- to In-task 1)

no affective changes; some individuals report positive affective changes and some re-

port negative changes. In the two following comparisons (In-task 1 to In-task 2 and

In-task 2 to Post), individuals showing increased positive affect increase and domi-

nate, while participants showing no changes decrease. Responses in the direction of

more negative affect decrease to a minimum. When comparing Pre with Post, 87%

report increased Pleasantness and no individual reports increased Unpleasantness.

Thus, inter-individual affective patterns of change during Qigong exercise display

favorable affective changes for most individuals, particularly at the end of the bout.

Positive expectations with actual affective outcomes were only significant for one

dimension of the four scales of the SCAS. The Unpleasant Activation-Pleasant Deac-

tivation dimension was significantly correlated with expected mood outcomes at

post-measures (.31).

62

70 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

The results of the open-ended questions display a uniform positive affective ex-

perience, yet this may vary. Most individuals report affective experiences associated

with Pleasant Deactivation, followed by Pleasantness. The most common combina-

tion is Pleasantness and Pleasant Deactivation. Individuals also report beneficial

changes associated with cognition, bodily experiences, altered states of conscious-

ness, and increased well-being. The simultaneous increased activating and deactivat-

ing pleasant state, visible in the modified short SCAS, could not be seen here. During

Qigong, perceived exertion was rated at a mean average of 10.1, which can be found

between very light and light on the Borg scale.

These results were found despite the fact that the participants were interrupted

twice during the Qigong session. Qigong is advocated to be carried out without in-

terruption, and some participants displayed irritation at having to assess their affec-

tive state during the bout. It is possible that without the stops the affective responses

would have been even more positive. Some limitations of the study are mentioned,

including the absence of a control group in addition to self-selected participants at a

Qigong camp. Possible practical benefits are discussed and the pleasant affective

state reported by the qigong exercisers may have important value for adherence, sub-

jective well-being and fostering personal resources and positive affect in the future.

63

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 71

64

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 73

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Main findings

The aim of present thesis was to study Qigong-related acute affective responses in a

group of regular Qigong exercisers. In the three studies this has been done on differ-

ent levels of the affective hierarchy: basic affect, mood and emotion. Affective re-

sponses show uniform benefits on all levels (however not significantly more benefi-

cial than listening to a lecture on all mood states of the POMS; Study I). Benefits are

independent of training session length (30 and 60 minutes; Study II). Affect is sig-

nificantly improved during the bout, beginning with small changes, and greater at

the end (Study III). In line with the Dual-Mode Model (Ekkekakis, 2004) Qigong’s

low intensity characteristics create beneficial affective responses in the majority of

individuals (Study III). When responding to open-ended questions of what respon-

dents usually feel and experience during and after a Qigong session, reports are

mostly of Pleasantness and Pleasant Deactivation in nature, somewhat in contrast to

those measured by the modified short SCAS, in which a simultaneous pleasant acti-

vating and deactivating state were reported.

Complementing the few studies of acute affective responses to Qigong exercise

(Lee et al., 2004; Kjos & Etnier, 2006), the present thesis strengthens the scientific

knowledge in this area. Acknowledging some important limitations, more evidence

has now been gathered for the effectiveness of Qigong exercise, as a means for im-

proving affective states.

Research questions

Are the affective responses associated with Qigong exercise significantly dif-

ferent from a control condition?

Study I showed that State Anxiety, Depression, Anger, and Fatigue were significantly

better compared to the control group, whereas Tension, Confusion, and Vigor were

not (although pre-post comparisons were greater in the Qigong group). Studies of

65

74 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

physical activity display significantly better acute affective benefits compared to a

control group (e.g. Ekkekakis et al., 2007). State anxiety showed a significantly bet-

ter reduction from Qigong compared to sham Qigong (Qigong without the mindful

focus; Lee et al., 2004), and this superiority of mind-body therapies compared to

controls is also shown elsewhere (Berger et al., 1988; Berger & Owen, 1992; Netz &

Lidor, 2003). There are also examples of the opposite: Morgan and Bahrke (1979)

found the anxiolytic effects of Relaxation Response meditation similarly beneficial to

silently sitting in a chair. Thus, the answer to this question is partly positive; yet,

probably due to the choice of control group (a better choice would have been silent

sitting), it cannot be concluded that Qigong exercise is more effective in decreasing

Tension and Confusion, and increasing Vigor, and further studies are needed with

more appropriate control groups.

Are the affective responses associated with Qigong exercise dependent on

length of session?

Comparing affective responses to the two different durations (30 minutes of Jichu

Gong, and 60 minutes of Jichu Gong and Dong Gong) in Study II, results of the

STAI (state anxiety), the six POMS subscales, and the self-constructed measures of

Hedonic tone (valence) and Activation, all showed non-significant results. Thus, for

the sample in this study, the results imply that acute affective benefits of Qigong are

independent of duration. Yet, as Fan Xiulan reasons (Personal communication,

2009-01-21), Jichu Gong and Dong Gong are different forms and have different

aims, and it is therefore likely that experiences from the two would be different.

Hence, according to the results based on the scope of instruments of the present

study, similar effects were found, but it is possible that with different instruments or

with a qualitative approach, differences between the 30 and 60-minute bouts would

have been found (perhaps even more so if Jichu Gong had been compared with Dong

Gong). To my knowledge, affective benefits of mind-body therapies of different du-

rations have not been studied, but the result of the present thesis is in line with re-

sults from comparisons of physical activity of durations from 10 to 30 minutes

66

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 75

(Daley & Welch, 2004; Hansen et al., 2001; Petruzzello & Landers, 1994). From an

acute affective perspective, practical benefits entail allowing those with little time to

engage in activities (often a reason for not exercising; King et al., 2000) to be sure of

benefitting to a similar extent from a shorter session compared to a longer one.

Is there a relationship between affective responses and expectations of affec-

tive benefits?

Given the characteristics of Qigong exercise, suggesting extra strong placebo effects,

an association between expectations and actual outcomes was expected. However,

on the four scales of the modified short SCAS that were compared to expectations of

affective benefits during and after the actual Qigong bout, it was only in the Un-

pleasant Activation-Pleasant Deactivation dimension where a significant correlation

with expected mood outcomes at post-measures was found. The reason for this may

be partly due to different affective measure and activity (P. D. O’Halloran, personal

communication, February 25, 2008) than previously reported correlations between

mood and expected mood outcomes in the literature (O’Halloran, Murphy, & Web-

ster, 2002; O’Halloran, Murphy, & Webster, 2005). A ceiling effect, with the major-

ity of individuals showing positive expectations regarding possible Qigong-related

affective benefits, may also be responsible for the absence of significant correlations.

In addition, O’Halloran et al. (2002) used young runners to validate the scale, in

contrast to the older sample of the present thesis. On the contrary, the significant as-

sociation with increased Pleasant Deactivation may be how the relationship between

positive expectations and Qigong-related affective outcomes is displayed.

Thus, from the results of Study III, one could conclude that a significant relation-

ship between positive expectations and affective outcomes was only found in one

(increased Pleasant Deactivation at post-assessments) of eight comparisons. It seems

reasonable that there should be more significant relationships based on theoretical

suggestions (Lund, 1987), empirical findings of somatic focus (Geers et al., 2006) in

addition to Tai Chi (Jin, 1992), and jogging (O’Halloran et al., 2002). Apparently,

67

76 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

this issue cannot be resolved at this time, and clearly needs more research in which

choice of participants, self-reports, and research design are of great importance.

How do affective responses display themselves and change, from pre-, to dur-

ing, to post-exercise, using mean averages and individual scores?

Based on the results of Study III, all the dimensions of the modified short SCAS first

show small positive changes, which later increase as the session proceeds (one of the

dimensions does not display significant changes comparing Pre and In-task 1).

However, Unpleasant Activation-Pleasant Deactivation instead shows the greatest

change at the beginning, reasonably due to the relaxation phase at the beginning of

the Qigong session. The pattern is thereby more closely related to that of the Main-

tenance Model (characteristic of low-intensity exercise) than the Rebound Model

(characteristic of higher intensity exercise; Bixby, Spalding, & Hatfield, 2001). Ac-

cording to the maintenance model, affective benefits are proposed to level out into a

plateau. This does not happen, however; instead, affective benefits increase until the

last measurement. Extra post-measurements could have shown the lingering effects

of the affective responses.

Individual responses on the Unpleasantness-Pleasantness dimension from Pre to

In-task 1 and to In-task 2 show 61% of individuals categorized as Positive, 7% as

Negative, 7% as Fluctuations, and 26% as No change. The few individuals reporting

increase in negative affect (7%) are in stark contrast to the 26% presented for self-

chosen moderate intensities of jogging (Rose & Parfitt, 2007), which is also pre-

dicted for low-intensity exercise (in comparison with moderate intensities) in the

Dual-Mode Model (Ekkekakis, 2004). Comparing each measurement, the majority

first display No changes, followed by Positive, and a few Negative. As the session

proceeds this changes, and in the end the majority display Positive responses fol-

lowed by No change and a minimum of Negative responses. Comparisons between

Pre and Post displayed 87% of the individuals reporting increased Pleasantness and

non-increased Unpleasantness. Despite the select group of study (regular Qigong ex-

68

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 77

ercisers), it is interesting to see the dominating positive affective responses, which

was very different from a study of regular joggers (Rose & Parfitt, 2007).

How do the exercisers describe the experience associated with Qigong exer-

cise, during and after the session?

This enquiry was addressed in Study III through open-ended questions. Affective de-

scriptions and adjectives were analyzed by way of categorization into the eight poles

of the affect circumplex (See Figure 2). Although there may be some limitations to

the conclusions that can be drawn from two open-ended questions, all participants

report that they associate Qigong exercise with a more beneficial affective state. Re-

ports were similar in response to open-ended questions during and after Qigong. The

most common categorizations are Pleasant Deactivation and Pleasantness. These are

also the most common dimensional combinations (same individual describing in

both dimensions). Some, albeit just a few, report simultaneously experiencing being

activated and deactivated (as the results of the modified short SCAS suggest). In ad-

dition, people also report experiences that are not directly transferrable to the cir-

cumplex structure of affect, such as clearer thoughts (cognitions), feeling warm and

soft in the body (somatic experiences) and being one with everything (altered states

of consciousness), as well as feeling love, harmony and healing, and feeling light and

free. These statements are in concordance with qualitative studies of Qigong, report-

ing mental strength, relaxation, reduced mental tension, and feeling refreshed and

energized (Wenneberg, Gunnarsson, & Ahlström, 2004), and feeling at ease with

themselves, calm, centered, and refreshed for the day (Brinker, 1998).

69

78 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Methodological issues

Participants

Studies I, II, and III took place at the annual Biyun Qigong summer camps, during

2003, 2006, and 2007. Given that many individuals return every year to the camp it

is likely that at least some individuals took part in all the studies of the present the-

sis. Thus, a specific select group participated in this thesis.

In Studies I and II there was a majority of women (9-14% men) and in Study III

only women were included. There is a dominating majority of women in the Green

Dragon (1348 women and 100 men in 2007; G. Jacobson, personal communica-

tion). An American survey (Wolsko et al., 2004) displayed a slight majority of

women reporting mind-body therapy usage, but in a study of Swedish individuals

engaged in different kinds of New Age groups (meditation, personal and spiritual

development, Yoga, etc.) there is a large majority (83%) of women (Frisk, 1998).

There are suggestions as to why especially women are highly attracted to these envi-

ronments. One refers to women actively seeking out non-patriarchal environments

that are more pro-women. Another refers to the focus on feelings, intuition, com-

munication, and relationships (more than intellect and dogma) that is found in these

alternative therapies and communities. These characteristics are considered feminine

in our society, and women may therefore feel more attracted than men (Frisk,

1998). Gender differences were initially compared on all dependent variables at pre-

measures (Studies I and II). While no statistical differences were found, means were

collapsed for gender in subsequent analyses. Gender differences were not studied

any further.

In most studies of physical activity and mental health, volunteers are employed,

and this creates some problems (Morgan, 1997). For example, studies of acute affec-

tive responses to Tai Chi (Jin, 1989), Qigong (Kjos & Etnier, 2006), and other

forms of mindful exercise (Netz & Lidor, 2003; Szabo et al., 1998) employed self-

selected volunteers from existing exercise classes. The samples of the three studies of

the present thesis were drawn from regular Qigong exercisers attending a Qigong

70

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 79

camp. It is possible that when beginning Qigong exercise, those who found the ac-

tivity affectively positive adhered, and that those who did not, quit. Thus, the ex-

perienced sample of the present thesis may possess certain qualities that predispose

them especially to the beneficial effects of Qigong exercise. Further, those who

choose to attend a Qigong summer camp have the economic means and the social

possibility of attending (leaving family and children at home), in addition to proba-

bly being positive about Qigong. This may limit external validity, or the possibility

to generalize the results to other groups of people. However, it seems important to

emphasize that, despite the fact that the sample consisted of regular Qigong exercis-

ers (with a number of years’ experience) attending a Qigong summer camp (and

meeting friends), the results still show significant beneficial affective responses in all

three studies. One could perhaps imagine that the sample was already in a pleasant

affective state and that room for improvement was thereby limited.

Design

The benefits of conducting a study at a Qigong camp include encountering a large

number of Qigong exercisers at the same time and place. However, this quasi-

experimental setting also created some restrictions, including a loss of control over

the experimental setting, and having to somewhat adjust my research to the context

of the camp.

In Study I, the Qigong intervention was compared to a control group; in Study II,

the participants served as their own controls; and in Study III no control group was

used as the focus was on affective responses during the Qigong bout (See Table 1).

Given that a control group should be as similar to the experimental condition as

possible but without the treatment (Whitley, 2002), the choice of control condition

in Study I presents some limitations. A more suitable control condition would have

been silent sitting, a non-mindful exercise or relaxation. Also, due to the probable

affection that the Qigong exercisers may possess for the master of the Qigong

method, it is possible that Qigong exercisers lacking this affection or those simply

71

80 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

listening to an unknown lecturer about Qigong would have displayed different ef-

fects. As Study I was carried out at the Qigong summer camp, and to avoid disrupt-

ing the camp schedule too much, a different control condition was not possible. At

some times the experimental designs (as recommended in exercise psychology for

physical activity; Ekkekakis & Petruzzello, 1999) interfered with the way Qigong is

normally advocated to be performed, as in Study III when participants were inter-

rupted during Qigong to rate their affective state (although it did significantly in-

crease affective benefits). Perhaps not optimal according to Qigong philosophy, it did

provide some important new results. In future Qigong studies, care should be taken

to recruit a relevant control condition.

Procedure

A number of biases can be associated with intervention studies (Morgan, 1997). The

issue of self-selected participants was discussed above. This may increase the possi-

bility of demand characteristics (the participants figure out the purpose of the study

and try to respond accordingly). Precautions were taken to minimize demand charac-

teristics by describing the purpose of the studies as “examining Qigong-associated

psychological responses”. In Studies I and II, some reduction of the Hawthorne effect

(Morgan, 1997) was possible as both groups/conditions received the same amount of

attention. These possible biases need to be taken into consideration when evaluating

the results of the present thesis.

Biyun Qigong consists of a number of specific forms of Qigong. Jichu Gong, the

basic form, was used in all three studies. However, in Study II, the shorter condition

(30 min) consisted of Jichu Gong and the longer condition (60 min) consisted of

Jichu Gong and Dong Gong performed together. Dong Gong is the second Qigong

form taught to students of the Biyun School. Qigong is practiced to improve health

on psychological, physiological, spiritual, and energetical levels. Founded in the

principles of Traditional Chinese Medicine and Qigong philosophy, certain training

requirements are advocated for safe and optimal Qigong training. Examples are op-

timal training dose and regularity. Fan Xiulan of the Biyun School emphasizes build-

72

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 81

ing from the ground up (2000). This means starting with softening the joints of the

entire body, a prerequisite for a healthy flow of qi. When one has become more ac-

customed to and has opened up for the qi flow, other more advanced forms of

Qigong techniques may be performed, such as moving qi through the body. How-

ever, this is based on the joints of the body being opened. As all forms consist to a

certain degree of similar characteristics (e.g., relaxation and meditation) affective

responses ought to be similar, whichever form one chooses to study. Yet there may

also be variations. Hence, it is important to know that the results of this thesis are

based on the most basic Qigong forms of the Biyun School.

Measures

The present studies employed self-report measures to investigate affective states as-

sociated with Qigong exercise. The procedure of rating one’s affective state implies

that the individual is consciously aware of her own emotional state, which may not

always be the case (Swinkels & Guiliano, 1995). Further, by directing attention to

an unconscious mood state the procedure may influence what it intends to study.

This may also lead to an urge to regulate one’s mood state (Parkinson et al., 1996).

Some other important possible biases associated with self-report scales (Watson,

2000) are: Social desirability may influence individuals to report more positively

than what they actually experience, as they do not want to admit to having a nega-

tive mood or experiencing anxiety. Individuals may want to cooperate and therefore

report in a way that they expect the experimenter wants them to. In addition, many

of the individuals in the present thesis have years of experience of the benefits of

Qigong, and consequently may want the results to be positive as well as make them

more prone to the benefits of Qigong. This may cause individuals to expect benefits

from the activity studied. At all times, I have urged the participants to respond as

truthfully as possible. Another bias in self-report measures concerns how respon-

dents interpret the anchoring points of scales. They may not be comparable between

individuals, but they are analyzed as comparable (nomothetic approach).

73

82 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Despite the mentioned disadvantages of self-report measures, they have been the

main source of data gathering in the three studies of this thesis. Given that the focus

of the thesis is on the conscious, subjective experience, self-report measures are the

most sensitive and discriminating way of data gathering (Larsen & Fredrickson,

1993; Parkinson et al., 1996; Thayer, 1989).

In the three studies, affective states have been assessed from a level of emotion

(the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory; Studies I and II), from a level of mood (cate-

gorical perspective) using the Profile of Mood States (Studies I and II), and from an

affect level using a self-made affect scale (Study II) and a modified version of the

short Swedish Core Affect Scale (Study III). More specifically, in Study I, the STAI

was used to compare with a study by Lee and colleagues (2000) that used the same

measure. Interesting results (different magnitude of state anxiety reductions between

the two studies) led to the use of the STAI in Study II also. The state form of the

STAI measures anxiety.

The STAI has been criticized in the context of measuring affective changes after

bouts of physical activity (Rejeski, Hardy, & Shaw, 1991; Ekkekakis, Hall, & Ek-

kekakis, 1999), because an increase in activation is assumed to be related to or in-

duced by anxiety. The measure may therefore yield some confusion in the assessment

of changes in the context of vigorous exercise, as activation is increased in response

to increased metabolism (and is not related to, e.g., increased fear). However, as

Qigong exercise is a low-intensity activity (10.1 on the Borg scale), the risk of con-

fusing anxiety with activation seems less pertinent (note that activation does increase

by way of the Vigor subscale of the POMS, Study II, and on the modified short

SCAS, Study III). Further, some argue that even though the state version of the STAI

assesses emotion anxiety, due to the time it takes to fill out the measure it may actu-

ally measure a mood state.

The POMS was used in Studies I and II. Although it has been criticized for floor

and ceiling effects (Gauvin & Brawley, 1993), too much focus on negative states

(Gauvin & Spence, 1998), and not covering the entire affective experience (Back-

house et al., 2007), it is also the single most used mood measure in sport and exer-

74

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 83

cise psychology (LeUnes & Burger, 1998). The POMS is therefore worthwhile in

comparing the results with changes in other physical activities.

Finally, the SCAS (Västfjäll et al., 2002) provides a valuable complement as it is

based on the dimensional view of affect and thereby covers the entire affective spec-

trum (the STAI measures a specific discrete emotion, while the POMS measures six

discrete mood states). With the SCAS, the basic affective responses from a dimen-

sional perspective can be assessed (note that the PANAS was used in a Qigong study

by Kjos and Etnier, 2006, but the measure is limited to the high activated poles of

the circumplex). The SCAS also measures affect, the most basic level of the affective

hierarchy. See Figure 3 for the affective space covered by the POMS and the STAI in

the circumplex structure.

Figure 3. The STAI1 and subscales of the POMS2, set in a circumplex structure. Adapted

from Backhouse et al., 2007. The subscale Confusion of the POMS is left out.

75

84 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Other affective measures could also have been used, such as the Activation Deactiva-

tion Adjective Check List (AD ACL; Thayer, 1978), or a Swedish version (Knez &

Hygge, 2001), both based on the circumplex model of affect and covering the entire

affective space. Assessments could also have been made with the Feeling Scale

(Hardy & Rejeski, 1989) together with the Felt Arousal Scale of the Telic State

Measure (Svebak & Murgatroyd, 1985), for multiple assessments during the bout

(one-item measures).

In Study II, two self-made one-item scales were created to assess affect according

to the circumplex structure (Russell, 1980). Thus, one scale set out to measure acti-

vation (from lowest possible to highest possible), and one scale to assess hedonic

tone/valence (from lowest possible pleasure to highest possible pleasure). Taken in

perspective, the “lowest possible…” and “highest possible…” wording may be a bit

unfortunate as these extreme end points may make respondents reluctant to assess

near the extremes, given the rather extreme phrase. For example, the Feeling Scale

(FS; Hardy & Rejeski, 1989) uses “I feel very good” and “I feel very bad” as its ex-

tremes. Another example is the Swedish Core Affect Scale (SCAS; Västfjäll et al.,

2002; used in Study III), in which extreme end points are only symbolized by highest

and lowest numbers. It is possible that the non-significant change in Activation may

be partly explained by the wording, as Vigor increased significantly, and Activation

and Pleasant Activation (of the modified short SCAS) also increased significantly in

Study III. Thus, some caution may be warranted in interpreting the results of the two

self-made scales of affect in Study II.

In accordance with recent recommendations (Backhouse et al., 2007; Rose & Par-

fitt, 2007; Van Landuyt et al., 2000) in which the use of mean scores is comple-

mented with that of individual scores, Study III displays single individuals’ affective

responses associated with Qigong exercise. Specifically, a methodology presented by

Rose and Parfitt (2007) was used. Thus, the Pleasantness-Unpleasantness scale on

the SCAS was used to study individual scores. I believe that complementing with in-

dividual assessments gives extra strength to Study III and the present thesis by show-

ing the variability between individuals and showing how affective patterns change

during the Qigong session. This may be particularly essential in the context of

76

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 85

Qigong exercise, as many variables influence the experience and make it very indi-

vidualistic.

Statistical analysis

Mixed (between-within, Study I), two-way repeated (Study II) ANOVAs, and re-

peated measures ANOVAs and t-test, Study III) were carried out in the studies. Non-

parametric equivalents could have been used (e.g., Friedman) in Study II, however no

non-parametric equivalent to mixed ANOVA exists. Parametric statistics were cho-

sen because of their greater power to detect actual differences (Hassmén & Hassmén,

1996). However, sometimes abnormally distributed, sample sizes of the studies (N =

57, 41, and 46) assures robustness of violations (N > 30; Pallant, 2007). When mul-

tiple testing, Bonferroni corrections were employed to protect for violations of type-I

error. If violations of sphericity were detected, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were

applied (Study III). Correlations (Pearson) were calculated in Study III. Samples were

first screened and extreme outliers were deleted.

Ethical considerations

The three studies were designed and administered in accordance with recommenda-

tions by Forskningsetiska principer inom humanistisk-samhällsvetenskaplig

forskning [Research Ethics Principles in Humanistic-Social Scientific Research]

(Vetenskapsrådet, 2002). These demands were complied with by way of informing

the participants that the purpose of the studies was to study psychological responses

to Qigong exercise for research only, participants signing an informed consent form

about participating voluntarily and having the possibility to quit the study at any

time, keeping data locked up, and coding participants.

Study II was judged for ethical soundness by the Ethical Review Board of the

Uppsala region. Given that Study II was judged to not be in need of ethical evalua-

tion, Study III, of similar design, was not sent for evaluation. Before January 2004,

77

86 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

non-sensitive behavioral research was not deemed to require ethical review; thus

Study I was not ethically reviewed by any board.

78

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 87

Concluding discussions

Affective changes associated with Qigong exercise

Based on the present thesis, a greater understanding of the affective responses asso-

ciated with Qigong exercise is now emerging, contributing greatly to the field, in

which, at the time the work on the present thesis commenced, Qigong had been

found to only decrease anxiety (Lee et al., 2004). The understanding that is now

emerging, however, shows greater clarity and depth concerning acute affective

Qigong-related responses. Study III perhaps provides the greatest understanding of

these affective changes, given that a dimensional perspective of affective states was

used, thereby covering the entire affective spectrum. Following recommendations

(Larsen & Diener, 1992) on assessing affective responses on both rotated and unro-

tated dimensions of the circumplex (See Figure 2), the perceived affective responses

of the group of regular qigong exercisers display changes in the direction of greater

Activation, Pleasant Activation, Pleasantness, and Pleasant Deactivation. This is

perhaps an odd finding. Can one feel more enthusiastic and relaxed at the same

time? This may be due to the assessment of all the eight end points of the circum-

plex of the modified short SCAS. This simultaneous activated yet deactivated pleas-

ant state has been reported by individuals engaging in Transcendental Meditation

(Wallace, 1970), in which reduced physiological activation was found to coexist

with increased mental alertness, restful alertness. In Thayer’s mood model this is re-

ferred to as calm energy, and is associated with the use of Yoga and meditation

(Thayer, 2001). A balance between activation and deactivation, fang song gong

(characterized by awareness and tranquility, effortlessness, sensitivity, warmth, and

rootedness) is also advocated in Qigong exercise. This is also closely related to the

Taoist term Tai Chi – the balance point between yin and yang (Cohen, 1997), and

descriptions of experiences associated with Qigong and other mind-body therapies

(Dorcas & Yung, 2003). Could it be that a relaxed state of full alertness and height-

ened sense of awareness, in the context of Qigong, is related to a positive affective

state? Descriptions of the Qigong state (Cohen, 1997; Dorcas & Yung, 2003; Fan,

79

88 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

2000), associated scientific theories (e.g. the Hypofrontality Hypothesis, Dietrich,

2003; Optimal Experience, Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2002; the Relaxation

Response, Benson et al., 1975), and empirical findings in the present thesis, does

suggest this. This may also correspond to a balance of the parasympathetic and

sympathetic nervous systems, given the simultaneous activating and deactivating

characteristics of Qigong exercise – being alert, yet experiencing an alleviation of the

negative effects of physiological arousal.

Another advantage of using a dimensional perspective in Study III concerns the

proposition of Yeung (1996) that mind-body methods may decrease negative affect

but may not provide changes in the direction of greater positive affect as physical

exercise may do (note the contrasting finding in the 2006 Kjos and Etnier Qigong

study, in which Positive Affect increased but Negative Affect did not). Judging from

the results of Study III, Qigong exercise is associated with greater affective benefits

(of the present sample) in many directions, including increased Pleasant Activation

and Pleasant Deactivation.

Further, Study III shows that Qigong exercise is related to greater Pleasantness

not only post-exercise, which was found in Studies I and II, but also during the ses-

sion. According to the Dual-Mode Model (Ekkekakis, 2004) low-intensity activities

are proposed to provide greater pleasantness both during and after the session. The

Dual-Mode Model also suggests that low-intensity activities show uniform changes

in the direction of increased pleasantness for the majority of individuals, which was

found in the inter-individual responses. Although, the Dual-Mode Model may pro-

vide for a greater understanding of low-intensity activities, including Qigong, there

are also aspects of Qigong that are different from mere physical activity. The Dual-

Mode Model proposes that in the context of low-intensity exercise, cognitive factors

exert limited influence on affective outcomes. In the context of Qigong, on the con-

trary, it seems likely that cognitive factors (e.g., ability to direct ones attention) are

essential for affective outcomes.

Other characteristics of the experience of Qigong-related affective changes entail

a slow increase in positive affectivity. This change is evident after 10-20 minutes.

This is related to that which is proposed for low-intensity activities according to the

80

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 89

Maintenance Model (Bixby et al., 2001). However, a plateau, which was expected

some time into the bout as suggested by the Maintenance Model, was not found.

In Studies I and II, affective responses were assessed on a higher level in the affec-

tive hierarchy (except those of the self-made measures of valence and activation of

Study II); on the levels of emotion (anxiety, the STAI) and mood states (the POMS).

In both Studies I and II anxiety (STAI), Tension, Depression, Anger, Vigor, Fatigue,

and Confusion (POMS) had significantly increased as measured before and after the

bout. The contribution of using these measures also shows that the positive affective

changes are uniform across different levels of the affective hierarchy.

When the affective Qigong experience is described in words (in the open-ended

questions of Study III), the simultaneous activating and deactivating characteristics

are not visible. Instead, most individuals describe it as Pleasant Deactivation and

Pleasantness (actually, all individuals describe it as a positive experience). However,

adjectives not directly fitting the eight end points of the circumplex structure of af-

fect are also reported, like harmony, healing, stability, love, clearness of thought,

increased sense of somatic focus, and feeling free and light. Thus, to allow for an

understanding of the experience of Qigong, the basic affect level may describe it to

some degree but not completely. To more fully understand the whole Qigong ex-

perience in relation to affective states, one may need to broaden the range of focus,

and also include other areas related to beneficial states. This includes cognitive

(thinking, appraising, and perceiving), conscious (the experience of awareness and

altered states of consciousness), and somatic changes (bodily changes).

So how can the affective experience of Qigong be expressed? Based on a number

of self-reports of mean scores and individual responses, and open-ended statements,

a picture emerges. However, this picture is dependent on how the researcher chooses

to assess affective responses. For example, in Study III the use of the SCAS shaped

an affective picture in which the Qigong state was characterized by a simultaneous

pleasant activating and deactivating experience. This would not have emerged if a

different measurement had been used, for instance one scale assessing pleasantness

and one measuring activation (solely the unrotated dimensions on the circumplex;

see Figure 2), which is common in today’s exercise psychology studies (e.g. Back-

81

90 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

house et al., 2007). This was done in Study II, and resulted in increased activation

(however not significant) and pleasantness. In contrast to this are the results of the

open-ended questions, in which when the Qigong participants could freely describe

their affective experience, most reported it as a pleasant deactivated state (e.g., re-

laxed and tranquil). Individuals may experience different feelings on a moment-to-

moment basis; however, it is not likely that they would simultaneously experience

both strong positive and negative feelings (Berkowitz, 2000). Such is not the case

here; even though it is both activating and deactivating, the experience is pleasant.

Given that Qigong exercise includes energizing (e.g., stretching one’s arms over the

head) and relaxing features (e.g., relaxation practice and slow movements), the

somewhat diversified affective reports seem logical. Another explanation has to do

with that the SCAS assesses affective responses on the specific session at hand, while

the open ended questions ask for how the exercisers generally feel when performing

Qigong (sessions may vary). A final possible explanation for the differences between

results of the SCAS and those of the open-ended questions, in addition to open

(questions) and closed (SCAS) response formats, have to do with that the exercisers

rate their affective state based on different time spans. The SCAS uses a “right now”

format, while the open ended questions were answered after the Qigong bout and

focuses on the exercisers affective experience during the entire session. In addition,

Qigong was associated with more beneficial mood states (Depression, Tension, An-

ger, Fatigue, Vigor, and Confusion; the POMS) and reduced anxiety (the STAI).

Thus, to answer the question of how the affective experience of Qigong is character-

ized in the present sample of regular exercisers: It is pleasant. But this pleasant affec-

tive state varies somewhat depending on the individual and how it is measured.

A proposed model of mechanisms of affective benefits

In an earlier section, possible active mechanisms responsible for acute affective bene-

fits associated with traditional physical activity and mind-body therapies were de-

scribed. Now a model for suggested active mechanisms for acute affective responses

will be presented (See Figure 4). It is important to note that this conceptual model

82

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 91

83

92 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

was not tested empirically. Instead, relevant theories of possible active mechanism

are discussed. Given that Qigong exercise includes a number of characteristics (at-

tentional regulation, relaxation, imagery, breathing, self-massage, and slow move-

ments) it seems reasonable that a number of different active mechanisms also work

simultaneously (and probably in interaction) to provide for the beneficial affective

states found in the three studies. Other models also exist. Tsang and Fung (2008)

present a model of neurobiological pathways of anti-depressive effects associated

with Qigong exercise. Anti-depressive effects may be influenced by increased brain

serotonin synthesis, by reducing negative cognitive/affective signals to the brain (and

reducing HPA activity), and by the down-regulation of cortisol (and promotion of

neurogenesis in the hippocampus). See also Chow and Tsang for a similar model fo-

cused on anxiety reduction (2007). The present model presents some possible

mechanisms that are not included in the Chow and Tsang model. The regulation of

attention in the context of Qigong exercise refers to focusing on different parts of

the body. This narrowing of attention may create a state in which the exerciser is

absorbed by what he/she is doing at the moment (Davidson & Schwartz, 1976).

Other suggestions concerning this state are presented as Optimal Experience (Na-

kamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2002), deep concentration, an Alternative State of

Consciousness (Vaitl et al., 2005), and the elicitation of the Relaxation Response

(Benson et al., 1975). By way of the Hypofrontality Hypothesis (in which the pre-

frontal cortex, which segregates, differentiates, and analyzes our experience is

down-regulated; Dietrich, 2003), a state of no thoughts and a unity with oneself and

life is experienced, which may create a time-out from worries (Barhrke & Morgan,

1978). Another route is based on the effects of imagery (e.g. Astin et al., 2003), re-

laxed breathing (e.g. Philippot et al., 2002, relaxation (e.g. Scheufele, 2000), self-

massage (oxytocin secretion; Uvnäs-Moberg, 2005) and the cultivation of positive

emotions (Broaden-and-Build Model of positive emotion; Fredrickson, 2000). All

these activities are related to increased parasympathetic nervous system activity (Re-

cordati, 2003) and may create a more optimal state in the nervous system, absent

from the effects of stress (Gunnarsson, 2004). As suggested by the Anthropological

Hypothesis (Martinsen, 2002) and the Dual-Mode Model (Ekkekakis, 2005), low-

84

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 93

intensity activities are beneficial from an evolutionary perspective and may create

positive affective experiences. Further, from a TCM perspective, Qigong may be a

way to release suppressed emotions (Chen, 2008), and also includes an accepting

attitude toward the contents of consciousness: mindfulness (Carrington, 2008). This

may create a relaxed perspective on oneself and one’s life. It seems logical that pla-

cebo (Catanzaro & Mearns, 1999) and positive expectations (Lund, 1987) may also

be responsible for Qigong-related affective outcomes, even though few significant

correlations were found in Study III. The characteristics of Qigong suggest that ex-

pectations should play a role (Lund, 1987), as should memories of earlier positive

affective responses to Qigong exercise, and may connect Qigong exercise with a

positive affective state (mood congruency; Bower, 1981). In performing the some-

times difficult mental and physical maneuvers of Qigong, one may feel proud of the

accomplishment by way of increased mastery (Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989) and

self-efficacy (the ability to regulate arousal, affective states, complex bodily maneu-

vers; Bandura, 1982), and the act of regularly spending time on promoting one’s

health may create increased self-love. A final suggestion is that of social interaction

(Cox, Martin Ginis, & Petruzzello, 2006). Given that the designs of Studies I, II,

and III were carried out in a group setting, it cannot be ruled out that social factors

may have affected the participants; however, as most individuals belonging to the

Green Dragon do perform Qigong regularly in their homes by themselves and re-

ceive affective benefits (Jouper et al., 2006), the social interaction hypothesis proba-

bly plays a minor role.

All the active mechanisms suggested above may, in interaction, provide for the af-

fective benefits found in the three studies. A final factor also entails individual char-

acteristics. To be able to adhere to Qigong exercise and receive affective benefits, it

is probably important that an individual enjoys performing Qigong (Motl, Berger,

& Leuschen, 2000), perhaps that the philosophy and lifestyle of Qigong seems at-

tractive (Jin, 1992), and that the individual has the ability to relax (although this

may be learned), and does not suffer from too much relaxation-induced anxiety

(Heide & Borkovec, 1984) or any other severe psychiatric disorder (Ng, 1996). Fi-

nally, scoring high on the trait absorption may make it easier to enter the Qigong

85

94 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

state and thus obtain affective benefits (Weinstein & Smith, 1992), as may the abil-

ity to concentrate (Fan, 2000).

Causality in the Qigong-affect relationship

Acute affective benefits have been displayed in the three studies of the present thesis.

Although it is not possible to infer a cause-effect relationship, whether Qigong per

se is responsible for the positive affective responses, I will speculate on this issue. A

taxonomy of eight characteristics of inferring causality from mere association has

been suggested by Hill (1965). Below these will be presented and discussed in the

light of the present thesis:

Strength of association. Based on a power analysis comparing effect sizes (eta

squared) for interacting effects between the Qigong group and the control

group of Study I, large effect sizes are found on significant measures and sub-

scales: STAI, Depression, Fatigue, and Anger. Non-significant subscales

(Vigor, Tension, & Confusion; Study I) and the self-constructed Activation

scale in Study II showed too-low power (.30 - .60) for finding a difference

(power > .80), and are thereby not valid for drawing any realistic conclu-

sions. Effect sizes of the repeated measures differences (pre-post) of Studies II

and III were all in the range of large to very large.

Consistency (effect appearing in different individuals, places, and circum-

stances). Limited consistency is found in the present studies as a homogene-

ous sample (regular exercisers) is used and in similar settings (Qigong camp).

Specificity (the influence of alternative explanations). Other variables may in-

clude social influence (not studied), certain individuals not being attracted to

Qigong (not studied), and positive expectations/placebo (Study III found one

out of eight possible significant correlations).

Temporality (order of cause-effect between independent and dependent vari-

ables). Causality is supported by the increase in affective benefits occurring

after the manipulation (Qigong). But given that the participants are self-

86

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 95

selected, it may be that they are an exclusive group that may react extra

beneficially to the effects of Qigong exercise.

Plausibility (whether causation is biologically plausible) and Coherence

(cause-effect interpretation conflicting with generally known facts based on

science). A number of theories based on the modalities used in Qigong exer-

cise (e.g. parasympathetic associated behaviors, mimicking positive emotions,

and narrowing of attention) support the biological plausibility of causality.

Biological gradient (dose-response relationship). According to Study II,

longer sessions of Qigong are not associated with stronger affective re-

sponses.

Experiment. In Study I, Qigong exercise was compared with a control condi-

tion. However, the limited nature of the control group (attending a lecture by

the Qigong master) restricts the benefits of the experimental design. That the

attendance of a lecture by Fan Xiulan yields positive effects may not be sur-

prising, given the importance of the Qigong master to the camp participants.

Other forms of control conditions may have been better suited. This may en-

tail sitting quietly and reading, meditating, or walking.

Analogy (similar kinds of activities associated with effects). Walking (Ek-

kekakis et al., 1999), other Qigong (Lee et al., 2004; Kjos & Etnier, 2006),

Tai Chi (Jin, 1989), Yoga (Berger & Owen, 1992), and relaxation (Saklofske

et al., 1992) have shown beneficial affective responses.

To some extent, the taxonomy of Hill (1965) supports a cause-effect relationship.

To more fully understand the cause-effect relationship between Qigong and acute

affect, a randomized controlled trial should be employed that can strengthen scien-

tific rigor in all parts of the design. In this vein, some limitations of the present the-

sis (self-selected regular Qigong exercisers at a Qigong camp and a somewhat lim-

ited control condition) may be bridged.

87

96 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Is Qigong beneficial for everybody?

In a positivistic paradigm of research, the researcher wants to generalize the findings

from the sample to include the entire (Qigong) population; this is also true here.

However, some apparent limitations, self-selected participants at a Qigong camp

being the most important, restrict the possibility to generalize the findings of the

present thesis to other potential Qigong exercisers. The findings of this thesis should

be seen mainly in the light of focusing on Qigong-related affective responses of a se-

lect, specific group of individuals who engage in Qigong exercise and probably find

it enjoyable. This group is made up mainly of middle-aged women (The reason

Qigong attracts women was discussed earlier). The present thesis may not resolve

the question of whether men and women react differently to Qigong exercise, as one

may suppose from comparing the study of Lee and colleagues (Lee et al., 2004) and

the Kjos and Etnier (2006) study. Qigong and Tai Chi may be extra suited for

health issues related to the needs of middle-aged and older individuals (Jouper,

2009; Tsang, Cheung, & Lak, 2002). Qigong is gentle, has flowing movements and

fits individuals looking for a low-intensity activity and those unable to perform

more vigorous activities (Kemp, 2004).

What about Qigong beginners? Would they benefit to the same extent that the

regulars in the present thesis do? Given that Qigong exercise requires some time to

learn the demands of visualization and bodily maneuvers, for beginning exercisers

this may require so much effort that they may not be able to relax and benefit to the

same extent as those who are more experienced. However, Fan Xiulan states that it

is also possible for beginners to experience similar affective benefits as more experi-

enced individuals do, if they are able to concentrate to the same extent during

Qigong as those who are experienced (Personal communication, 2009-01-21).

With the limitations having been mentioned, a great effort has also been taken to

study and understand which theories and suggestions have been proposed for how

an activity like Qigong (and other similar) may provide for acute affective benefits

for the exerciser. Similarly, a great effort has also been made to study the empirical

support for acute affective benefits found in activities similar to Qigong exercise

88

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 97

(e.g. walking, relaxation, Yoga and Tai Chi). Although they sometimes do not use

proper scientific rigor (as in mind-body therapies; Ospina et al., 2007), many studies

are promising (particularly in their support of physical exercise). From the perspec-

tive of the three empirical studies in the present thesis, generalizing the findings to

other groups is somewhat limited. However, based on the other empirical studies,

suggestions and theories of active mechanisms, it seems likely that other groups

might also benefit from engaging in Qigong exercise.

Qigong – affective regulation and alleviation of physiological arousal?

Based on the proposed model of affective responses to Qigong exercise (Figure 3), a

number of simultaneous processes may provide for affective benefits. On a daily ba-

sis, Qigong may function (for the participants in the present thesis) as an affect-

regulating strategy and as a means for alleviating physiological arousal (e.g., induced

by stressful events).

Qigong exercise can be associated with subjective well-being, which includes hap-

piness or affective well-being, and how people view their world and themselves (Die-

ner, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Qigong may have a beneficial influence on both

these factors. In the three studies, Qigong has been associated with positive affect.

Being in a positive state of mind has numerous beneficial effects. By way of mood

congruency (Parkinson et al., 1996), Qigong exercisers may view the world and

themselves more positively and remember more pleasant memories (Forgas, Bower,

& Krantz, 1984), thus experiencing more frequent positive moods as well as antici-

pating them occurring in the future. It has been found that the practice of meditation

was able to produce future increased amounts of positive emotions and life satisfac-

tion (Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008). Further, frequent small inci-

dences of positive affect have been shown to be more beneficial for subjective well-

being than few strong emotional incidences (Diener, Larsen, Levine, & Emmons,

1985). Having a sense of meaning and purpose in life (Myers, 1992) is also impor-

tant for subjective well-being. This can be provided by engaging in an activity

(Qigong) that is also way of life and has spiritual connotations as well. Emmons ar-

89

98 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

gues that this entails enabling a person to create a new identity based on a life narra-

tive focused around connections to a larger, transcendent purpose (Emmons, 1999).

Thus, Qigong may provide for a potent means for affective regulation, in line with

other methods like exercise and relaxation (Thayer, Newman, & McCain, 1994),

thereby promoting a more pleasant affective state, possibly preventing psychiatric

disorders (Gross, 1999), and improving subjective well-being.

Positive affect may influence other psychological variables. But does it also influ-

ence our physical health? Reviews point to the benefits of positive affect on physical

health as well (Pressman & Cohen, 2005; Steptoe & Wardle, 2005). Qigong and Tai

Chi have been found to stabilize the cardiovascular and autonomic nervous systems

(Lee et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2002; Lu & Kuo, 2006), and Qigong may improve self-

efficacy by way of arousal regulation (Bandura, 1982). Humans who were exposed

to stressors were significantly more likely to experience hypothalamic-pituitary-

adrenal (HPA) activation if the stressors were uncontrollable (Kemeny, 2005).

Through the experiencing of positive affective states, the lingering harmful effects of

physiological arousal (e.g., high blood pressure) have been found to alleviate at a

faster pace (the Undoing Hypothesis; Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998). Thus, Qigong

exercise may reduce physiological arousal, thereby removing the numerous harmful

effects of stress on the body (Gunnarsson, 2004).

Long-term benefits of regular Qigong exercise

In their review of Tai Chi, Sandlund & Norlander (2000) conclude that it may not

result in aerobic fitness but it may enhance flexibility and psychological well-being. I

believe this is also a reasonable conclusion for Qigong exercise. Perhaps only aero-

bically enhancing for the old or physically impaired (very light or light on the Borg

scale), the coordinated slow movements may increase balance and flexibility, and to

some extent strength. Chen (2007) reports that common effects of the regular prac-

tice of Qigong are associated with a harmonious and relaxed state of mind and

body, reduction in prior ailments, stress, increased resistance to illness, a heightened

sensitivity to the body’s internal organs and an increased ability to regulate one’s

90

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 99

own health and vitality. Granqvist & Hjelm (1997) suggest that the regular per-

formance of Qigong exercise may create greater self-awareness. When someone

looks inward daily, the borders between conscious and unconsciousness processes

may become less pronounced. Letting thoughts, emotions and memories pass

through awareness may create an understanding of our own inner processes. This

may lead to greater wisdom and insight into our lives and how they should be lived.

The difference between tense and relaxed muscles becomes more visible. The slow,

delicate (sometimes complex) movements can lead to a greater sensitivity of subtle

bodily movements. Heightened awareness of healthy and detrimental postures,

movements, and breathing patterns, and in general greater physical sensations from

the body, may create a greater range of possibilities of how to take care of ourselves.

Other long-term effects (studies on meditation, which can also be compared to

Qigong) include decreased sympathetic nervous system reactivity (by way of the Re-

laxation Response), thereby alleviating effects of stress (Hoffman et al., 1981), a

more sensitive and varied emotional experience and an increased ability to discrimi-

nate between different emotional states (Nielsen & Kaszniak, 2006), and fewer nega-

tive reactions to stress as well as increased brain patterns associated with positive af-

fect (Brefzcynski-Lewis et al., 2007).

Future studies

When a new area or research is delved into, insights are reached that continuously

improve the personal understanding of the area of research. To more fully under-

stand acute affective responses associated with Qigong exercise, qualitative research

designs would provide an important complement to the quantitative results found in

the present three studies. Only a few published studies (e.g. Wenneberg et al., 2005)

and master theses (Brinker, 1998; Murakawa, 2002) have investigated the experi-

ence of Qigong exercise through qualitative inquiries. For example, there were some

equivocal results in Study III, in which self-report measures displayed simultaneous

perceived activating and deactivating responses, however through the use of open-

ended questions, most individuals described Qigong as relaxing (Pleasant Deactiva-

91

100 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

tion), and only a few described it as simultaneously activating. Further studies com-

paring/complementing qualitative with quantitative designs are needed to fully un-

derstand the qigong experience. One such study of experiences associated with

Qigong exercise is in progress by the author. Another new line of enquiry not stud-

ied in the present thesis entails the duration of lingering effects of the affective im-

provements associated with Qigong exercise. According to the Maintenance Model

of affective change that is associated with low-intensity exercise, improvements of

affective responses are proposed to level out some time into the bout. Affective re-

sponses to Qigong exercise (Study III) increased continuously through the last as-

sessment upon cessation of the bout. Kjos and Etnier (2006) found increased Posi-

tive affect (PANAS) at post-exercise, but mean average decreased to below pre-

measures already after 15 minutes of post-exercise rest. This opens up for new ven-

ues of research to study the lingering effects of Qigong – perhaps several hours post-

exercise up to the whole day?

A randomized controlled study, in which beginners would learn the Qigong form

and be randomized to a Qigong or control condition (e.g., silent reading) would

provide for a greater understanding of causality as well as the ability to generalize.

Given that Qigong is intended to be performed daily, a longer intervention study

would also be of interest as it could show its chronic effects in relation to subjective

well-being, for example.

Two issues worthy of further study relate to the moderating effects of positive

expectations and depth of concentration, and their influence on acute affective out-

comes. Theories (e.g. Lund, 1987), and empirical support (Geers et al., 2006; Jin,

1992; Jouper et al., 2006; O’Halloran et al., 2002) point in the direction of the

probability of Qigong having placebo/expectation influences. Practical and personal

empirical suggestions from the founder of Biyun Qigong (Fan, 2000), theoretical

suggestions (Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2002, Smith, 1990), and empirical

evidence (Jouper et al., 2006) suggest that the level of concentration or the ability to

be absorbed by the focus of interest is important for the possibility of a beneficial

affective outcome. Finally, it seems worthwhile to continue the methodology that

was followed in Study III, including inter-individual assessments (in addition to

92

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 101

mean averages), as this may show the variation in experience between individual

Qigong exercisers.

93

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 103

CONCLUSIONS

In the three studies of the present thesis, acute affective responses associated with

Qigong have been studied. The thesis makes some important contributions to the

further understanding of and support for Qigong-related affective health benefits. It

can be concluded that the affective benefits of Qigong are partially superior to a

control condition, independent of length of session, and that there are few associa-

tions between affective outcomes and expectations of positive mood outcomes. Af-

fective responses have been studied through mean scores, individual responses and

open-ended questions, in addition to different levels of the affective hierarchy.

Qigong exercise is associated with a simultaneously activated and deactivated pleas-

ant affective state, reduced anxiety, and positive mood states. However, there is

some variation depending on the individual and how he/she chooses to measure af-

fective responses (open-ended questions showed the majority of experiences related

to Pleasant Deactivation). Positive affective responses begin to increase 10-20 min-

utes into the bout and continue to rise until cessation of exercise. A model for theo-

ries of possible active mechanisms associated with acute affective Qigong responses

is presented. It can be concluded that Qigong is associated with more beneficial af-

fective (acute) states for the sample studied. Other research, theories and suggestions

also support an association with increased pleasant affect.

94

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 105

REFERENCES

Ames, R. T. (1993). The meaning of body in classical Chinese philosophy. In T. P.

Kasulis, R. T. Ames, & W. Dissanayake (Eds.), Self as body in Asian theory and

practice (pp. 157-174). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Antonovsky, A. (1991). Hälsans mysterium [Unraveling the mystery of health: How

people manage stress and stay well]. Stockholm: Natur och Kultur.

Astin, J. A., Berman, B. M., Bausell, B., Lee, W-L., Hochberg, M., & Forys, K. L.

(2003). The efficacy of mindfulness meditation plus qigong movement therapy in

the treatment of fibromyalgia: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of

Rheumatology, 30, 2257-2262.

Astin, J. A., Shapiro, S., Eisenberg, D. M., Forys, K. (2003). Mind-body medicine:

State of the science, implications for practice. Journal of the American Board of

Family Practice, 16, 131-147.

Backhouse, S. H., Ekkekakis, P., Biddle, S. J. H., Foskett, A., & Williams, C. (2007).

Exercise makes people feel better but people are inactive: Paradox or artifact?

Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29, 498-517.

Bandura. A. (1982). The self and mechanism of agency. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychologi-

cal perspectives on the self. (Vol. 1). (pp. 3-37). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Bahrke, M. S., & Morgan, W. P. (1978). Anxiety reduction following exercise and

meditation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 2, 323-333.

Barrows, K. A., & Jacobs, B. P. (2002). Mind-body medicine. An introduction and

review of the literature. The Medical Clinics of North America, 86, 11-31.

Batson, C. S., Shaw, L. L., & Oleson, K. C. (1992). Differentiating affect, mood, and

emotion: toward functionally based conceptual distinctions. In M. S. Clark (Ed.),

Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 13) (pp. 294-326). Newbury

Park, CA: Sage.

Benson, H. (1983). The relaxation response: its subjective and objective historical

precedents and physiology. TINS, 6, 281-284.

95

106 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Benson, H., Greenwood, M. M., & Klemchuk, M. M. (1975). The relaxation re-

sponse: Psychophysiologic aspects and clinical applications. International Journal

of Psychiatry in Medicine, 6, 87-98.

Berg, B. L. (2001). Qualitative research methods for the social sciences. Needham

Heights, MA: Pearson.

Bergdahl, J., & Bergdahl, M. (2002). Perceived stress in adults: Prevalence and asso-

ciation of depression, anxiety and medication in Swedish populations. Stress and

Health, 18, 235-241.

Berger, B. G., Friedmann, E., & Eaton, M. (1988). Comparison of jogging, the re-

laxation response, and group interaction for stress reduction. Journal of Sport &

Exercise Psychology, 10, 431-447.

Berger, B. G., & Motl, R. (2001). Physical activity and quality of life. In R. Singer,

H. A. Hausenblas, & C. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 636-

671). New York & Chichester: Wiley.

Berger, B. G., & Owen, D. R. (1992). Mood alterations with yoga and swimming:

Aerobic exercise may not be necessary. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1331-

1343.

Berger, B. G., Owen, D. R., Motl, R. W., & Parks, L. (1998). Relationship between

expectancy of psychological benefits and mood alterations in joggers. Interna-

tional Journal of Sport Psychology, 29, 1-16.

Berger, B. G., & Tobar, D. A. (2006). Physical activity and quality of life. Key con-

siderations. In G. Tenenbaum, & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psy-

chology (3rd ed.) (pp. 598-620). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Berkowitz, L. (2000). Causes and Consequences of Feeling. West Nyack, New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Bernardi, L., Porta, C., Spicuzza, L., Bellwon, J., Spadacini, G., Frey, A. W., et al.

(2002). Slow breathing increases arterial baroreflex sensitivity in patients with

chronic heart failure. Circulation, 105, 143-145.

96

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 107

Biddle, S. J. H. (2000). Emotion, mood and physical activity. In S. J. H. Biddle, K. R.

Fox, & S. H. Boutcher (Eds.). Physical Activity and Psychological Well-Being (pp.

63-87). London: Routledge.

Biddle, S. J. H., & Ekkekakis, P. (2005). Physically active lifestyles and well-being. In

F. A. Huppert, N. Baylis, B. Keverne, B. (Eds.), The Science of Well-being (pp.

141-168). New York: Oxford University Press.

Biddle, S. J. H., & Mutrie, N. (2001). Psychology of Physical Activity. Determi-

nants, Well-being and Interventions. London: Routledge.

Bixby, W. R., Spalding, T. W., & Hatfield, B. D. (2001). Temporal dynamics and

dimensional specificity of the affective responses to exercise of varying intensity:

Differing pathways to a common outcome. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychol-

ogy, 23, 171-190.

Blanchard, C. M., Rodgers, W. M., & Gauvin, L. (2004). The influence of exercise

duration and cognitions during running on feeling states in an indoor running

track environment. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 5, 119-133.

Blomberg, M. (2004). Träning i ökad kroppskänndedom och avspänning hjälp för

att hantera stress. [Body awareness training and relaxation: help to manage

stress]. Läkartidningen, 101, 1398-1400.

Bogart, G. (1991). The use of meditation in psychotherapy: A review of the litera-

ture. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 45, 383-412.

Borg, G. (1998). Borg’s Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics.

Bouchard, C., Blair, S. N., & Haskell, W. L. (2007). Why study physical activity and

health? In C. Bouchard, S. N. Blair, & W. L. Haskell (Eds.). Physical Activity and

Health (pp. 3-19). Champaign, Ill: Human Kinetics.

Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.

Brefczynski-Lewi, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J.

(2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long term-meditation practi-

tioners. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United

States of America, 27, 11483-11488.

97

108 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Brinker, L. (1998). The experience of qigong in healing and self-care. Unpublished

Master Thesis.

Brown, R. P., & Gerbarg, P. L. (2005). Sudarshan kriya yogic breathing in the

treatment of stress, anxiety, and depression: Part I – Neurophysiological model.

The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 11, 189-201.

Carrington, P. (2007). Modern forms of mantra meditation. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L.

Woolfolk, W. E. Sime (Eds.) Principles and practice of stress management (pp.

363-392). New York: Guilford Press.

Caspersen, C. J., Powel, K. E., & Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exer-

cise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research.

Public Health Reports, 2, 126-131.

Catanzaro, S. J., & Mearns, J. (1999). Mood-related expectancy, emotional experi-

ence, and coping behavior. In I. Kirsch (Ed.), How Experiences Shape Experi-

ence. (pp. 67-91). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association.

Chaouloff, F. (1997). Effects of acute physical exercise on central serotonergic sys-

tems. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 29, 58-62.

Chen, K. (2007). Qigong therapy for stress management. In P. M. Lehrer, R. L.

Woolfolk, W. E. Sime (Eds.), Principles and practice of stress management (pp.

428-448). New York: Guilford Press.

Chow, Y. W. Y., & Tsang, H. W. H. (2007). Biopsychological effects of qigong as a

mindful exercise for people with anxiety disorders: A speculative review. Journal

of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13, 831-840.

Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S. (Jr.), Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L. (2001).

Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In L. L., Martin &

G. L., Clore (Eds.), Theories of Mood and Cognition. A User’s Handbook (pp.

27-62). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cohen, K. S. (1997). The way of qigong. The art and science of Chinese energy heal-

ing. London, Bantam Books.

Conrad, A., Müller, A., Doberenz, S., Kim, S., Meuret, A. E., & Wollberg, E., et al.

(2007). Psychophysiological effects of breathing instructions for stress manage-

ment. Applied Psychophysiological & Biofeedback, 32, 89-98.

98

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 109

Cox, C. L., Martin Ginis, K. A., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2006). The psychology of exer-

cise: integrating theory and practice. Scottsdale, AZ: Holcomb Hathaway.

Davidson, R. J. (1994). On emotion, mood, and related affective constructs. In P.

Ekman, & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: fundamental questions

(pp. 51-55). New York: Oxford University Press.

Davidson, R. J., Goleman, D. J., & Schwartz, G. E. (1976). Attentional and affec-

tive concomitants of meditation: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 85, 235-238.

Deikman, A. (1982). The observing self. Boston: Beacon Press.

Diener, E., Larsen, R. J., Levine, S., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Intensity and fre-

quency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. Journal of Personal-

ity and Social Psychology, 48, 1253-1265.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. (1999). Subjective well-being:

Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276-302.

Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for

mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84-100.

Dishman, R. K. (1997). The norepinephrine hypothesis. In W. P. Morgan, (Ed.),

Physical Activity and Mental Health. (pp. 199-212). Philadelphia: Taylor &

Francis.

Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness:

The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 231-

256.

Dorcas, A., & Yung, P. (2003). Qigong: Harmonising the breath, the body and the

mind. Complementary Therapies in Nursing & Midwifery, 9, 198-202.

Eich, E., & Macaulay, D. (2006). Cognitive and clinical perspectives on mood-

dependent memory. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior

(pp. 105-121). New York: Psychology Press.

Ekkekakis, P. (2003). Pleasure and displeasure from the body: Perspectives from ex-

ercise. Cognition & Emotion, 2, 213-239.

Ekkekakis, P. (2004). Exercise and affect – the study of affective responses to acute

exercise: The dual mode model. In R. Stelter, & K. K. Roessler (Eds.). New Ap-

99

110 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

proaches to Sport and Exercise Psychology (pp.119-146). Aachen: Meyer &

Meyer Sport.

Ekkekakis, P., Backhouse, S. H., Gray, C., & Lind, E. (2008). Walking is popular

among adults but is it pleasant? A framework for clarifying the link between

walking and affect as illustrated in two studies. Psychology of Sport and Exercise,

9, 246-264.

Ekkekakis, P., Hall, E. E., Van Landuyt, L. M., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2000). Walking

in (affective) circles: Can short walks enhance affect? Journal of Behavioral Medi-

cine, 23, 245-275.

Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (1999). Acute aerobic exercise and affect: Current

status, problems, and prospects regarding dose-response. Sports Medicine, 28,

337–374.

Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2000). Analysis of the affect measurement co-

nundrum in exercise psychology: I. Fundamental issues. Psychology of Sport and

Exercise, 1, 71-88.

Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2002). Analysis of the affect measurement co-

nundrum in exercise psychology: IV. A conceptual case for the affect circumplex.

Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3, 35-63.

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6,

169–200.

Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns: Motivation and spiri-

tuality in personality. New York: Guilford Press.

Emmons, R. A., & Diener, E. (1986). A goal-affect analysis of everyday situational

choices. Journal of Research in Personality, 20, 309-326.

Esch, T., Fricchione, G. L., & Stefano, G. (2003). The therapeutic use of the relaxa-

tion response in stress-related diseases. Medical Science Monitor, 9, RA23-34.

Everly, G. S., & Lating, J. M. (2003). A clinical guide to the treatment of the human

stress response. New York: Kluwer Academic.

Fan, X. (2000). Qigong enligt Biyun. Låt livskraften återvända [Biyun qigong. Allow

the life force to return]. Stockholm, Sweden: Svenska Förlaget.

100

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 111

Fan, X. (2001). Biyun, den hälsobevarande metoden. Jichu gong. [CD]. Örbyhus:

Biyunskolan.

Fan, X. (2004). Biyun, den hälsobevarande metoden. Dong gong. [CD]. Örbyhus:

Biyunskolan.

Farné, M. A., & Gnugnoli, D. (2000). Effects of autogenic training on emotional

distress symptoms. Stress Medicine, 16, 259-261.

Feldman Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1999). Independence and bipolarity in the

structure of current affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 967-

984.

Feldman Barrett, L., and Russell, J. A. (1999). The structure of current affect: con-

troversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science,

8, 10-14.

Forgas, J. P. (2008). Affect and cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2,

94-101.

Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of general psy-

chology, 2, 300-319.

Fredrickson, B. L. (2000). Cultivating positive emotions to optimize health and well-

being. Prevention & Treatment, 3. Article 0001a. Retrieved March 24, 2009,

from http://www.unc.edu/peplab/publications/cultivating.pdf

Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery

from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition & Emotion,

12, 191-220.

Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts

build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build

consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

5, 1045-1062.

Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. Lewis, & J.

M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381-403). New York: Guilford.

Frijda, N. H. (1994). Varieties of affect: emotions and episodes, moods and senti-

ments. In P. Ekman, & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: fundamen-

tal questions (pp. 59-67). New York: Oxford University Press.

101

112 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Frisk, L. (1998). New age, paramedicin och helande. [New age, Para medicine, and

Healing]. In O. Wikström (Ed). Att se det dolda. Om new age och ockultism inför

millenniumskiftet. [Seeing what is hidden. New age and the occult at the turn of

the millennium]. Stockholm: Natur och Kultur.

Fontaine, K. L. (2005). Complementary & alternative therapies for nursing practice.

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Forgas, J.P., Bower, G. H., & Krantz, S. E. (1984). The influence of mood on per-

ceptions of social interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 497-513.

Fruzzetti, A.E., Shenk, C., Mosco, E., & Lowry, K. (2003). Emotion regulation. In

W.T. O´Donohue, J.E. Fisher, & S.C. Hayes (Eds.), Cognitive behavior therapy:

Applying empirically supported techniques in your practice (pp. 152-159). New

York: Wiley.

FYSS. (2008). Fysisk aktivitet i sjukdomsprevention och sjukdomsrehabilitering

[Physical activity in the prevention and treatment of disease]. Ödeshög, Sweden:

AB Danagårds Grafiska.

Gauvin, L., & Brawley, L. R. (1993). Alternative Psychological Models and Meth-

odologies for the Study of Exercise and Affect. In P. Seraganian (Ed.), Exercise

Psychology: The Influence of Exercise on Psychological Processes, pp. 146–71.

New York: Wiley.

Gauvin, L., & Rejeski, W. J. (1993). The exercise-induced feeling inventory: devel-

opment and initial validation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 15, 403-

423.

Gauvin, L., & Spence, J. C. (1998). In J. L. Duda (Ed.) Advances in sport and exer-

cise psychology measurement (pp. 325-336). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Informa-

tion Technology.

Geers, A. L., Helfer, S. G., Weiland, P. E., & Kosbab, K. (2006). Expectations and

placebo response: A laboratory investigation into the role of somatic focus. Jour-

nal of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 171-178.

Gill, S., Kolt, G. S., & Keating, J. (2004). Examining the multi-process theory: an

investigation of the effects of two relaxation techniques on state anxiety. Journal

of Bodywork and Movement Therapies, 8, 288-296.

102

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 113

Girdano, D. A., Everly, G. S. Jr., & Dusek, D. E. (2001). Controlling Stress and

Tension. (6th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Goldstein, M. S. (2000). The culture of fitness and the growth of CAM. In M. Kel-

ner, B. Wellman, B. Pescosolido, & M. Saks (Eds.), Complementary and alterna-

tive medicine: Challenge and change (pp. 27-38). Amsterdam: Harwood Aca-

demic Publishers.

Granath, J., Ingvarsson, S., von Thiele, U., & Lundberg, U. (2006). Stress manage-

ment: A randomized study of cognitive behavioural therapy and yoga. Cognitive

Behaviour Therapy, 35, 3-10.

Granqvist, H., & Hjelm, N. (1997). Qigong i teori och praktik. [Qigong: theory and

practice]. Stockholm: Svenska Förlaget.

Greenberg, L. S., & Paivio, S. (1997). Working with emotions in psychotherapy.

New York: The Guildford Press.

Griffith, N. M., Szaflarski, J. P., Szaflarski, M., Kent, G. P., Schefft B. K., Howe, S.,

et al. (2005). Measuring depressive symptoms among treatment-resistant seizure

disorder patients: POMS Depression scale as an alternative to the BDI-II. Epilepsy

and Behavior, 7, 266-272.

Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion regulation: past, present, future. Cognition and Emo-

tion, 13, 551–573.

Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: conceptual founda-

tions. In J. J. Gross, (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3-24). New

York: Guilford Press.

Gruzelier, J. H. (2002). A review of the impact of hypnosis, relaxation, guided im-

agery and individual differences on aspects of immunity and health. Stress, 5,

147-163.

Gunnarsson, L-G. (2004). Skolmedicinsk forskningmetodik – vad kan beforskas? In

M. Eklöf, (Ed.), Perspektiv på komplementär medicin. [Perspectives on comple-

mentary medicine]. (pp. 165-175). Lund, Sweden: Studenlitteratur.

Han, J. N., Stegen, K., De Valck, C., Clément, J., & Van De Woestijne, K. P.

(1996). Influence of breathing therapy on complaints, anxiety and breathing pat-

103

114 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

tern in patients with hyperventilation syndrome and anxiety disorders. Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 41, 481-493.

Hansen, C. J., Stevens, L. C., & Coast, J. R. (2001). Exercise duration and mood

state: How much is enough to feel better? Health Psychology, 20, 267-275.

Hardy C. J., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). Not what, but how one feels: The measure-

ment of affect during exercise. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 304-

317.

Hassmén, P., & Hassmén, N. (1996). Variansanalys. [Analyses of variance]. Lund,

Sweden: Studenlitteratur.

Heide, F. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (1984). Relaxation-induced anxiety: Mechanisms

and theoretical implications. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1, 1-12.

Hernendez-Reif, M., Field, T., & Hart, S. (1999). Smoking cravings are reduced by

self-massage. Preventive Medicine, 28, 28-32.

Hill, A. B. (1965). The environment and disease: association or causation. Proceed-

ings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 58, 295-300.

Hoffmann P. (1997). The endorphin hypothesis. In W. P. Morgan (Ed.), Physical

Activity and Mental Health (pp. 163-177). Washington: Taylor & Francis.

Hoffman, J. W., Benson, H., Arns, P. A., Stainbrook, G. L., Landsberg, L., Young, J.

B., & Gill, A. (1981). Reduced sympathetic nervous system responsitivity associ-

ated with the relaxation response. Science, 215, 190-192.

Izard, C. E. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: cognitive and noncognitive

processes. Psychological Review, 100, 68–90.

Jin, P. (1989). Changes in heart rate, noradrenalin, cortisol and mood during tai chi. Jour-

nal of Psychosomatic Research, 33, 197-206.

Jin, P. (1992). Efficacy of tai chi, brisk walking, meditation, and reading in reducing men-

tal, and emotional stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 36, 361-370.

Joseph, M. (1998). The effect of strong religious beliefs on coping with stress. Stress

Medicine, 14, 219-224.

Jouper, J. (2009). Qigong in daily life – motivation and intention to mindful exer-

cise. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Örebro, Sweden.

104

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 115

Jouper, J., Hassmén, P., & Johansson, M. (2006). Qigong exercise with concentra-

tion predicts increased health. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 34,

949-957.

Kaptchuk, T. J. (2000). The web that has no weaver. Understanding Chinese medi-

cine. Chicago: Contemporary.

Kelner, M., & Wellman, B. (2000). Introduction. In M. Kelner, B. Wellman, B.

Pescosolido, & M. Saks, Complementary and alternative medicine: Challenge

and change (pp. 1-38). Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers.

Keltner, D., & Gross, J. J. (1999). Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition and

Emotion, 13, 467–480.

Kemeny, M. E. (2005). The psychobiology of stress. In G. Miller & E. Chen (Eds.),

Current directions in health psychology (pp. 124-129). Upper Saddle River, NJ:

Pearson Education.

Kemp, C. A. (2004). Qigong as a therapeutic intervention with older adults. Journal

of Holistic Nursing, 22, 351-373.

King, A. C., Castro, C., Wilcox, S., Eyler, A. A., Sallis, J. F., & Brownson, R. C.

(2000). Personal and environmental factors associated with physical inactivity

among different racial-ethnic groups of U.S. middle-aged and older-aged women.

Health Psychology. 19, 354-364.

Kirkcaldy, B. C., & Shephard, R. J. (1990). Therapeutic implications of exercise. In-

ternational Journal of Sport Psychology, 21, 165-184.

Kjos, V., & Etnier, J. L. (2006). Pilot study comparing physical and psychological

responses in medical qigong and walking. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity,

14, 241-253.

Knez, I., & Hygge, S. (2001). The circumplex structure of affect: A Swedish version.

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 389-398.

Krantz, G., & Lundberg, U. (2006). Workload, work stress, and sickness absence in

Swedish male and female white-collar employees. Scandinavian Journal of Public

Health, 34, 238-246.

Krisanaprakornkit, T., Krisanaprakornkit, W., Piyavhatkul, N., & Laopaiboon, M.

(2006). Meditation therapy for anxiety disorders. Cochrane Database of System-

105

116 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

atic Reviews. Issue 1. Art. No: CD004998. DOI: 10.1002/14651858.

CD004998.pub2.

Landers, D. M., & Arent, S. M. (2006). Physical activity and mental health. In G.

Tenenbaum, & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psychology (3rd ed.) (pp.

469-491). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Lane, A. M., Jackson, A., & Terry, P. C. (2005). Preferred modality influences on

exercise-induced mood changes. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine, 4, 195-

200.

Landstingsförbundet (2001). Den komplementära medicinens utbredning och till-

lämpning inom landets landsting. En kartläggning på uppdrag av Landstingsför-

bundet. Komplementära Hälsostudier 2. Örebro universitet.

Larkey, L., Jahnke, R., Etnier, J., & Gonzales, J. (In press). Meditative movements

as a category of exercise: Implications for research. Journal of Physical Activity

& Health.

Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex

model of emotion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychol-

ogy: Vol. 13. (pp. 25-59). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Larsen, R. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1999). Measurement issues in emotion research.

In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: Foundations of

Hedonic Psychology (pp. 40-60). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Larsen, R. J., & Prizmic, Z. (2004). Affect regulation. In R. F. Baumeister, & K. D.

Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of Self-Regulation. Research, Theory, and Applications

(pp. 40-61). New York: The Guilford Press.

LeUnes, A., & Burger, J. (1998). Bibliography on the Profile of Mood States in sport

and exercise psychology research, 1971-1998. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21, 53-

70.

Lazarus, R. (1994). The stable and the unstable in emotions. In P. Ekman, & R. J.

Davidson, The nature of emotion: fundamental questions (pp. 79-85). New York:

Oxford University Press.

106

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 117

Lee, S. L., Kang, C-W., Lim, H-J., & Lee, M-S. (2004). Effects of Qi-training on

anxiety and plasma concentrations of cortisol, ACTH, and aldosterone: A ran-

domized placebo-controlled pilot study. Stress and Health, 20, 243-248.

Lee, M-S., Kang, C-W., & Ryu, H. (2005). Acute effect of qi-training on natural kil-

ler cell subsets and cytotoxic activity. International Journal of Neuroscience, 115,

285-297.

Lee, M. S., Kang, C-W., Ryu, H., & Moon, S-R. (2004). Endocrine and immune ef-

fects of qi-training. The International Journal of Neuroscience, 114, 529-537.

Lee, M. S., Kim, B. G., Huh, H. J., Ryu, H., Lee, H-S., & Chung, H-T. (2000). Ef-

fect of qi-training on blood pressure, heart rate and respiration. Clinical Physiol-

ogy, 20, 173-176.

Lee, M-S., Kim, M-K., & Rye, H. (2005). Qi-training (qigong) enhanced immune

functions: What is the underlying mechanism? International Journal of Neurosci-

ence, 115, 1099-1104.

Lee, M-S., Lee, M-S., & Kim, H-J. (2003). Qigong reduced blood pressure and cate-

cholamine levels of patients with essential hypertension. International Journal of

Neuroscience, 113, 1691-1701.

Lee, M-S., Lee, M-S., & Kim, H-J. (2004). Effects of qigong on blood pressure, high

density lipoprotein cholesterol and other lipid levels in essential hypertension pa-

tients. International Journal of Neuroscience, 114, 777-786.

Lee, M-S., Lee, M-S., Kim, H-J., & Moon, S-R. (2003). Qigong reduced blood pres-

sure and catecholamine levels of patients with essential hypertension. Interna-

tional Journal of Neuroscience, 113, 1691-1701.

Lee, M-S., Lim, H-J., & Lee, M-S. (2004). Impact of qigong exercise on self-efficacy

and other cognitive perceptual variables in patients with essential hypertension.

The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 4, 675-680.

Lee, S. L., Ryu, H., & Chung, H-T. (2000). Stress management by psychosomatic

training: effects of Chundosunbup qi-training on symptoms of stress: a cross-

sectional study. Stress Medicine, 16, 161-166.

107

118 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Lee, T-I., Chen, H-H., & Yeh, M-L. (2006). Effects of chan-chuang qigong on im-

proving symptom and psychological distress in chemotherapy patients. The

American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 34, 37-46.

Leung, Y. & Singhal, A. (2004). An examination of the relationship between qigong

meditation and personality. Social Psychology and Personality, 4, 313-320.

Levander, V. L., Benson, H., Wheeler, R. C., & Wallace, R. K. (1972). Increased

Forearm Blood Flow During a Wakeful Hypometabolic State. Federation Pro-

ceedings, 31, 405.

Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: a

functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3–28.

Lichstein, K. L. (2000). Relaxation training. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of

Psychology (pp. 28-30). Washington, D.C.: American psychological Association;

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lindfors, P., Berntsson, L., & Lundberg, U. (2006). Factor structure of Ryff’s psy-

chological well-being scales in Swedish female and male white-collar workers.

Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 1213-1222.

Lox, C. L., Jackson, S., Tuholski, S., Wasley, D., & Treasure, D. C. (2000). Revisit-

ing the measurement of exercise-induced feeling states: The Physical Affect Scale

(PAAS). Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 4, 79-95.

Lu, W-A., & Kuo, C-D. (2003). The effect of tai chi chuan on the autonomic nerv-

ous system modulation in older persons. Medicine and Science in Sports & Exer-

cise, 35, 1972-1976.

Lund, L-G. (1987). Placebo, belief, and health. A cognitive – emotional model. Scan-

dinavian Journal of Psychology, 28, 128-143.

Luskin, F. M., Newell, K. A., Griffith, M., Holmes, M., Telles, S., Marvasti, F. F., et

al. (1998). A review of mind-body therapies in the treatment of cardiovascular

disease. Part 1: implications for the elderly. Alternative Therapies in Health and

Medicine, 4, 46-61.

Lox, C. L., Martin Ginis, K. A., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2006). The psychology of exer-

cise: integrating theory and practice. Scottsdale: Holcomb Hathaway.

108

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 119

Mannerkorpi, K., & Arndorw, M. (2004). Efficacy and feasibility of a combination

of body awareness therapy and qigong in patients with fibromyalgia: a pilot

study. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 36, 279-281.

Martinsen, E. W. (2002). The role of exercise in the management of depression. In

D. L. Mostofsky, & L. D. Zaichowsky (Eds.), Medical and psychological aspects

of sport and exercise (pp. 205-214). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information

Technology.

Mayer, M. (1999). Qigong and research: A critique of research. The Journal of Al-

ternative and Complementary Medicine, 5, 371-382.

McCauley, E., & Corneya, K. S. (1994). The Subjective Exercise Experience Scale

(SEES): Development and preliminary validation. Journal of Sport and Exercise

Psychology, 16, 163-177.

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. F. (1992). EdITS manual for the pro-

file of mood states. San Diego: EdITS.

Mills, N., Allen, J., & Morgan, C. (2000). Does tai chi/qi gong help patients with

multiple sclerosis? Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies, 4, 39-48.

Morgan, W. P. (1997). Methodological considerations. In W. P. Morgan (Ed.),

Physical activity and mental health (pp. 3-32). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.

Morris, N. (1999). The mood system. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & E. Schwarz

(Eds.), Well-being: the foundation of hedonic psychology (pp. 169-189). New

York: Russell Sage foundation.

Motl, R. W., Berger, B. G., & Leuschen, P. S. (2000). The role of enjoyment in the

exercise-mood relationship. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 3, 347-

363.

Moyer, C. A., Rounds, J., & Hannum, J. W. (2004). A meta-analysis of massage

therapy research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 3-18.

Murakawa, H. (2002). Phenomenology of the experience of qigong: A preliminary

research design for the intentional bodily practice. Doctoral Dissertation. Cali-

fornia Institute of Integral Studies.

109

120 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Mutrie (2000). The relationship between physical activity and clinically defined de-

pression. In S. J. H. Biddle, K. Fox & S. H. Boutcher (Eds.), Physical Activity and

Psychological Well-being (pp. 46–62). London: Routledge.

Mutrie, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1995). The effects of exercise on mental health in

nonclinical populations. In S. J. H. Biddle (Ed.). European perspectives on exer-

cise and sport psychology (pp. 50-70). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Mutrie, N., & Faulkner, G. (2004). Physical activity: Positive psychology in motion.

In P. A. Linley & S. Joseph (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice. Hoboken, NJ:

John Wiley & Sons.

Myers, D. G. (1992). The pursuit of happiness. New York: Avon Books.

Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In C. R. Sny-

der, & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89-105). New

York. Oxford University Press.

NCCAM (2007). Mind-body medicine: An overview. National Center For Com-

plementary and Alternatives Medicine, 16, National Institutes of Health, U.S.

Department of Health and Human Services. [http://nccam.nih.gov/health/whati-

scam/ mind-body/mindbody.htm]

Naruse, K., & Hirai, T. (2000). Effects of slow tempo exercise on respiration, heart

rate and mood state. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 3, 729-740.

Netz, Y., & Lidor, R. (2003). Mood alterations in mindful versus aerobic exercise

modes. The Journal of Psychology, 137, 405-419.

Ng, B-Y. (1999). Qigong-induced mental disorders: a review. Australian and New

Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 197-206.

Nielsen, L., & Kaszniak, A. W. (2006). Awareness of subtle emotional feelings: A

comparison of long-term meditators and nonmeditators. Emotion, 3, 392-405.

Nyenhuis, D. L., Luchetta, T., Yamamoto, Terrien, A., & Parmentier, A. (1999).

Adult and geriatric normative data for the profile of mood states. Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 55, 79-86.

O’Connor, B. B. (2000). Conceptions of the body in complementary and alternative

medicine. In M. Kelner, B. Wellman, B. Pescosolido, & M. Saks (Eds.), Comple-

110

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 121

mentary and alternative medicine. Challenge and Change (p. 39-57). Amsterdam:

Harwood Academic publishers.

O’Halloran, P. D., Murphy, G. C., & Webster, K. E. (2002). Measure of beliefs

about improvements in mood associated with exercise. Psychological Reports, 90,

834-840.

O’Halloran, P. D., Murphy, G. C., & Webster, K. E. (2005). Moderators of mood during 60

minute treadmill run. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 36, 241-250.

Olson, J. M., Roese, N. J., & Zanna, M. P. (1996). Expectancies. In E. T. Higgins &

A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology. Handbook of Basic Principles (pp.

211-238). New York: Guildford Press.

Orne, M. T., & Whitehouse, W. G. (2000). Relaxation techniques. In G. Fink (Ed.),

Encyclopedia of Stress (pp. 341-348). San Diego: Academic Press.

Ospina, M. B., Bond, T. K., Karkhaneh, M., Tjosvold, L., Vandermeer, B., & Liang,

Y., (2007). Meditation Practices for Health: State of the Research. Evidence Re-

port /Technology Assessment No. 155. AHRQ Publication No. 07-E010. Rock-

ville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.

Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS Survival Manual. A step by step guide to data analysis using

SPSS for windows (3rd ed.). Maidenhead: Open University Press & McGraw-Hill.

Paluska, S. A., & Schwenk, T. L. (2000). Physical activity and mental health. Cur-

rent concepts. Sports Medicine, 29, 167-180.

Panksepp, J. (2003). At the interface of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive neu-

rosciences: decoding the emotional feelings of the brain. Brain and Cognition, 52,

4-14.

Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P., Briner, R. B., & Reynolds, S. (1996). Changing Moods.

The Psychology of Mood and Mood Regulation. Harlow: Addison Wesley Long-

man.

Parfitt, G. and Gledhill, C. (2004) The effect of choice of exercise mode on psycho-

logical responses. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 2, 111-117.

Parfitt, G., Rose, E. A., & Markland, D. (2000). The effects of prescribed and pre-

ferred intensity exercise on psychological affect and the influence of baseline

measures of affect. Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 231-240.

111

122 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Petruzzello, S. J., & Landers, D. M. (1994). Varying the duration of acute exercise:

implications for changes in affect. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International

Journal, 6, 301-310.

Petruzzello, S. J., Landers, D. M., Hatfield, B. D., Kubitz, K. A., & Salazar, W.

(1991). A meta-analysis on the anxiety-reducing effects of acute and chronic exer-

cise. Outcomes and mechanisms. Sports Medicine, 11, 143-182.

Petruzzello, S. J., Landers, D. M., & Salazar, W. (1993). Exercise and anxiety reduc-

tion: Examination of temperature as an explanation for affective change. Journal

of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 15, 63-76.

Peveler, R. C., & Johnston, D. W. (1986). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24,

413-419.

Philippot, P., Chapelle, G., & Blairy, S. (2002). Respiratory feedback in the genera-

tion of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 605-627.

Poppen, R. (1998). Behavioral Relaxation Training and Assessment. 2nd ed. Thou-

sand Oaks: Sage.

Porcari, J. P., Ward, A., Morgan, W., Mance, M., Ebbeling, C., Kline, G., et al.

(1998). Effect of walking on state anxiety and blood pressure. Medicine and Sci-

ence in Sports and Exercise, 20, S85.

Pressman, S. D., & Cohen, S. (2005). Does positive affect influence health? Psychological

Bulletin, 131, 925-971.

Reed, J., & Ones, D. S. (2006). The effect of acute aerobic exercise on positive activated

affect: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 477-514.

Recordati, G. (2003). A thermodynamic model of the sympathetic and parasympa-

thetic nervous system. Autonomic Neuroscience: Basic & Clinical, 103, 1-12.

Reed, J., & Ones, D. S. (2006). The effect of acute aerobic exercise on positive acti-

vated affect: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 477-514.

Rejeski, W.J., Brawley, L.R., & Shumaker, S.A. (1996). Physical Activity and Health

related Quality of Life. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 24, 71-108.

Rejeski, W. J., Hardy, C. J., & Shaw, J. (1991). Psychometric confounds of assessing

state anxiety in conjunction with acute bouts of vigorous exercise. Journal of

Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 65-74.

112

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 123

Rose, E. A., & Parfitt, G. (2007). A quantitative analysis and qualitative explanation

of the individual differences in affective responses to prescribed and self-selected

exercise intensities. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 281-309.

Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect. Review of

General Psychology, 3, 247-270.

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

Russell, J. A., & Feldman-Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional

episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. Journal of Per-

sonality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819.

Russell, J. J., Lewicka, M., & Niit, T. (1989). A cross-cultural study of a circumplex

model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 848-856.

Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item

scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,

493-502.

Ryff, C., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revis-

ited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 719-727.

Ryu, H., Mo, Y. H., Mo, G. D., Choi, B. M., Chang, D. J., Seo, C. M., et al. (1995).

Delayed cutaneous hypersensitivity reactions in qigong (chundosunbup) trainees

by multitest cell mediated immunity. American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 23,

139-144.

Ryu, H., Lee, M-S., Jeong, S-M., Lee, J-H., Kang, C-W., Lee, D-Y., et al. (2000).

Modulation of neuroendocrinological function by psychosomatic training: acute

effect of chundosunbup qi-training on growth hormone, insulin-like growth fac-

tor (IGF)-1, and insulin-like growth factor binding protein (IGFBP)-3 in men,

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25, 439-451.

Sadigh, M. R. (2001). Autogenic Training: A Mind-Body Approach to the Treat-

ment of Fibromyalgia and Chronic Pain Syndrome. New York: Haworth Press.

Saklofske, D. H., Blomme, G. C., & Kelly, I. W. (1992). The effects of exercise and

relaxation on energetic and tense arousal. Personality and Individual Differences,

13, 623-625.

113

124 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Sandlund, E. S., & Norlander, T. (2000). The effects of tai chi chuan and exercise on

stress responses and well-being: An overview of research. International Journal of

Stress Management, 7, 139-149.

Sarang, P., & Tellas, S. (2006). Effects of two yoga based relaxation techniques on

heart rate variability (HRV). International Journal of Stress Management, 13,

460-475.

Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process

approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293-

317). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Scheufele, P. M. (2000). Effects of progressive relaxation and classical music on

measurements of attention, relaxation, and stress responses. Journal of Behav-

ioral Medicine, 23, 207-228.

Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-

being: Informative and directive function of affective states. Journal of Personal-

ity and Social Psychology, 45, 513-523.

Scully, D., Remer, J., Meade, M. M., Graham, R., & Dudgeon, K. (1998). Physical

exercise and psychological well being: a critical review. British Journal of Sports

Medicine, 32, 111-120.

Shapiro, S., Schwartz, G. E., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based

stress reduction on medical and premedical students. Journal of Behavioral Medi-

cine, 21, 581-599.

Shapiro, S. L., & Schwartz, G. E. R., & Santerre, C. (2002). Meditation and posi-

tive psychology. In C. R. Snyder, & J. Shane (Eds.), Handbook of Positive Psy-

chology (pp. 632-645). New York: Oxford University Press.

Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E. R., & Santerre, C. (2002). Meditation and positive

psychology. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychol-

ogy (pp. 632-645). New York: Oxford University Press.

Singer, J. L. (2006). Imagery in Psychotherapy. Washington, D.C.: American Psy-

chological Association.

Sjöberg, L., Svensson, E., & Persson, L-O. (1979). Scandinavian Journal of Psychol-

ogy, 20, 1-18.

114

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 125

Skoglund, L., & Jansson, E. (2007). Qigong reduces stress in computer operators.

Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 13, 78-84.

Smith, J. C. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral relaxation training: A new system of

strategies for treatment and assessment. New York: Springer Publishing Com-

pany.

Sonstroem, R. J., & Morgan, W. P. (1989). Exercise and self-esteem: Rationale and

model. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 21, 329-337.

Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation:

I, temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119-145.

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A.

(1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, Ca: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Steptoe, A., & Wardle, J. (2005). Positive affect and biological function in everyday

life. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, (suppl1), S108-S112.

Svebak, S., & Murgatroyd, S. (1985). Metamotivational dominance: A multimethod

validation of reversal theory constructs. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-

chology, 48, 107-116.

Swinkels, A., & Giuliano, T. A. (1995). The measurement and conceptualization of

mood awareness: Monitoring and labeling one's mood states. Personality and So-

cial Psychology Bulletin, 21, 934-949.

Szabo, A. (2003). Acute psychological benefits of exercise performed at self-selected

workloads: implications for theory and practice. Journal of Sports Science and

Medicine, 2, 77-87.

Szabo, A., Meskó, A., Caputo, A., & Gill, É. T. (1998). Examination of exercise-

induced feeling states in four modes of exercise. International Journal of Sport

Psychology, 29, 376-390.

Taylor, A. H. (2000). Physical activity, anxiety, and stress. In S. J. H. Biddle, K. R.

Fox, & S. H. Boutcher. Physical Activity and Psychological Well-Being (pp. 10-

45). London: Routledge.

Thayer, R. E. (1978). Toward a psychological theory of multidimensional activation

(arousal). Motivation and Emotion, 2, 1-34.

115

126 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Thayer, R. E. (1987). Energy, tiredness, and tension effects of a sugar snack versus

moderate exercise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 119-125.

Thayer, R. E. (1989). The Biopsychology of Mood and Arousal. New York: Oxford

University Press

Thayer, R. E. (2001). Calm energy. How people regulate mood with food and exer-

cise. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Thayer, R. E., Newman, R., & McClain, T. M. (1994). Self-regulation of mood:

Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising energy, and reducing tension. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 910-925.

Thayer, R. E., Peters, D. P., Takahashi, P. J., & Birkhead-Flight, A. M. (1993).

Mood and behavior (smoking and sugar snacking) following moderate exercise: A

partial test of self-regulation theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 14,

97-104.

Tsang, H. W. H., Fung, K. M. T. (2008). A review on neurobiological and psycho-

logical mechanisms underlying the anti-depressive effect of qigong exercise. Jour-

nal of Health Psychology, 13, 857-863.

Tsang, H. W. H., Fung, K. M. T., Chan, A. S. M., Lee, G., & Chan, F. (2006). Ef-

fect of qigong exercise programme on elderly with depression. International

Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21, 890-897.

Tsang, H. W. H., Mok, C. K., Au Yeung, Y. T., & Chan, S. Y. C. (2003). The effect

of qigong on general and psychosocial health of elderly with chronic physical ill-

nesses: a randomized clinical trial. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,

18, 441-449.

Tse, M. (1998). Qigong: så finner du din inre energi. [Qigong: Finding your inner

energy]. Västerås: ICA bokförlag.

Ursin, H. (2000). Psychosomatic medicine: State of the art. Annals of Medicine, 32,

323-328.

U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996). Physical activity and

health: A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health

and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Cen-

ter for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion.

116

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 127

Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2000). Lugn och beröring: oxytocinets läkande verkan i krop-

pen. [The Oxytocin factor: tapping the hormone of calm, love, and healing] Stockholm:

Natur och Kultur.

Uvnäs-Moberg, K., Arn, I., & Magnusson, D. (2005). The psychobiology of emo-

tion: The role of the oxytocinergic system. International Journal of Behavioral

Medicine, 2, 59-65.

Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J., Jamieson, G. A., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A.,

et al. (2005). Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness. Psychological Bul-

letin, 131, 98–127.

Van Landuyt, L. M., Ekkekakis, P., Hall, E. E., & Petruzzello, S. J. (2000). Throw-

ing the mountains into the lakes: On the perils of nomothetic conceptions of the

exercise-affect relationship. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 22, 208-

234.

Västfjäll, D., Friman, M., Gärling, T., & Kleiner, M. (2002). The measurement of

core affect: A Swedish self-report measure derived from the affect circumplex.

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 19-31.

Västfjäll, D., & Gärling, T. (2007). Validation of a Swedish short self-report meas-

ure of core affect. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48, 233-238.

Vetenskapsrådet. (2002). Riktlinjer för humanistisk och samhällsvetenskaplig forsk-

ning. [Research Ethics Principles in Humanistic-Social Scientific Research.] Stock-

holm: Vetenskapsrådet.

Wallace, R. K. (1970). Physiological effects of transcendental meditation. Science,

167, 1751-1754.

Wallace, A. B., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). Mental balance and well-being: Building

bridges between Buddhism and Western psychology. American Psychologist, 61,

690-701.

Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). The meeting of meditative disciplines and west-

ern psychology: A mutually enriching dialogue. American Psychologist, 3, 227-

239.

117

128 I mattias johansson ✍ Qigong: Acute affective responses

Warburton, D. E. R., Nicol, C. W., & Bredin, S. S. D. (2006). Health benefits of

physical activity: the evidence. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 6, 801-

810.

Watson, D. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and

negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

6, 1063-1070.

Watson, D. (2000). Mood and Temperament. New York: The Guilford Press:

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of

brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Per-

sonality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psy-

chological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.

Weinstein, M., & Smith, J. C. (1992). Isometric squeeze relaxation (progressive re-

laxation) vs meditation: absorption and focusing as predictors of state effects.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1263-1271.

Welch, A. S., Hulley, A., Ferguson, C., & Beauchamp, M. R. (2007). Psychological

responses of inactive women to a maximal incremental exercise test: A test of the

Dual Mode Model. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 401-423

Wenneberg, S., Gunnarsson, L-G., & Ahlström, G. (2004a). Using a novel exercise

programme for patients with muscular dystrophy. Part I: a qualitative study. Dis-

ability and Rehabilitation, 26, 586-594.

Wenneberg, S., Gunnarsson, L-G., & Ahlström, G. (2004b). Using a novel exercise

programme for patients with muscular dystrophy. Part II: a quantitative study.

Disability and Rehabilitation, 26, 595-602.

Wilhelm, F. H., Alpers, G. W., Meuret, A. E., & Roth, W. T. (2001). Respiratory

pathophysiology of clinical anxiety outside the laboratory: Assessment of end-

tidal pCO-sub-2, respiratory pattern variability, and transfer function RSA. In J.

Fahrenberg, & M. Myrtek (Eds.), Progress in ambulatory assessment. Computer-

assisted psychological and psychophysiological methods in monitoring and field

studies (pp. 313-343). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber.

118

Qigong: Acute affective responses ✍ mattias johansson I 129

Wilhelm, F. H., Gevirtz, R., & Roth, W. T. (2001). Respiratory dysregulation in

anxiety, functional cardiac, and pain disorders. Assessment, phenomenology, and

treatment. Behavior Modification, 25, 513-545.

Whitley, B. E. (2002). Principles of research in behavioral science. New York:

McGraw-Hill.

Wolsko, P. M., Eisenberg, D. E., Davis, R. B., & Phillips, R. S. (2004). Use of mind-

body medical therapies. Results of a national survey. The Journal of General In-

ternal Medicine, 19, 43-50.

Wood, C. (1998). Mood change and perceptions of vitality: a comparison of the ef-

fects of relaxation, visualization and yoga. Journal of the Royal Society of Medi-

cine, 86, 254-258.

Yeung, R. R. (1996). The acute effects of exercise on mood state. Journal of Psycho-

somatic Research, 40, 123-141.

Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999). Structure of self-

reported current affect: integration and beyond. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 77, 600-619.

Åsberg, M., Wahlberg, K., Sköld, C., & Nygren, Å. (2006). Mindfulnessmeditation

– nygammal metod för att lindra stress. [Mindfulness meditation – ancient new

method of stress reduction]. Läkartidningen, 103, 3174-3177.

119