21
Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging honey bees Sara RODNEY 1 , Vincent J. KRAMER 2 1 Intrinsik Corp, 208-2120 Robertson Rd., Ottawa, ON K2H 5Z1, Canada 2 Corteva Agriscience, 9330 Zionsville Rd., Indianapolis, IN 46268, USA Received 6 December 2018 Revised 24 July 2019 Accepted 12 September 2019 Abstract Recent concerns regarding potential effects of pesticides on pollinators have prompted regulatory agencies to estimate dietary ingestion rates for honey bees (Apis mellifera ). The task is difficult because of the complex caste and food storage systems in honey bee colonies. Considerable data on the nutrition and energetics of honey bees have recently been collated. These data were used to parameterize a probabilistic model estimating nectar requirements of nectar foragers. Median estimates were more than 6× lower than the recommended median value from the North American government agencies, of 292 mg nectar/bee/day. The distribution of estimates had much greater variability than those of the agencies. The differences are due primarily to the disparate assumptions regarding how much time nectar foragers spend flying and foraging. Risk assessors considering honey bee dietary exposure should take account of current and emerging data providing insight into nectar forager dietary require- ments, foraging activity, and feeding behavior. probabilistic / modeling / nectar / Apis mellifera / honey bee / energetics / foraging 1. INTRODUCTION Dietary exposure is typically estimated by multiply- ing residues in food by an assumed food ingestion rate. This has been a long-standing approach for vertebrates in regulatory risk assessment and is more recently found in regulatory guidance for pollinators, including honey bees (Apis mellifera ; EFSA 2013; EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2014). In 2014, the US Environ- mental Protection Agency (EPA), in collaboration with the Canadian Pest Management Regulatory Agency (PMRA) and the California Department of Pesticide Regulation (CDPR), released a guidance document for assessing pesticide risk to bees (EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2014). The protection goals specified in the guidance are to ensure bee biodiversity, provision of pollination services, and production of hive products. At the individual level of biological organization, the measurement endpoints are worker survival, larval emergence, and queen fecundity/reproduction. Nectar foragers are expected to have the highest dietary ex- posure of all honey bee castes because of their elevated energy requirements during flight (EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2012; 2014). To assess dietary exposure of nectar foragers, the Agencies modeled daily nectar requirement rates. This analysis was presented in detail in a White Paper that preceded the guidance document (EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2012). Pollen requirements were also estimated, but as discussed in the White Paper and in Rodney and Purdy ( this issue ), pollen is not a significant food source for bees of nectar- foraging age. The Agencies model was probabilistic and included input variables accounting for the amount and duration of flying trips and the amount of sugar Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s13592-019-00693-w) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. Corresponding author: V. Kramer, [email protected] Manuscript editor: James Nieh Apidologie Original article * The Author(s), 2019. This article is an open access publication DOI: 10.1007/s13592-019-00693-w

Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    7

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirementsfor nectar-foraging honey bees

Sara RODNEY1, Vincent J. KRAMER

2

1Intrinsik Corp, 208-2120 Robertson Rd., Ottawa, ON K2H 5Z1, Canada2Corteva Agriscience, 9330 Zionsville Rd., Indianapolis, IN 46268, USA

Received 6 December 2018 – Revised 24 July 2019 – Accepted 12 September 2019

Abstract – Recent concerns regarding potential effects of pesticides on pollinators have prompted regulatoryagencies to estimate dietary ingestion rates for honey bees (Apis mellifera ). The task is difficult because of thecomplex caste and food storage systems in honey bee colonies. Considerable data on the nutrition and energetics ofhoney bees have recently been collated. These data were used to parameterize a probabilistic model estimating nectarrequirements of nectar foragers. Median estimates were more than 6× lower than the recommended median valuefrom the North American government agencies, of 292 mg nectar/bee/day. The distribution of estimates had muchgreater variability than those of the agencies. The differences are due primarily to the disparate assumptionsregarding how much time nectar foragers spend flying and foraging. Risk assessors considering honey bee dietaryexposure should take account of current and emerging data providing insight into nectar forager dietary require-ments, foraging activity, and feeding behavior.

probabilistic / modeling / nectar /Apismellifera / honey bee / energetics / foraging

1. INTRODUCTION

Dietary exposure is typically estimated bymultiply-ing residues in food by an assumed food ingestion rate.This has been a long-standing approach for vertebratesin regulatory risk assessment and is more recentlyfound in regulatory guidance for pollinators, includinghoney bees (Apis mellifera ; EFSA 2013; EPA,PMRA and CDPR 2014). In 2014, the US Environ-mental ProtectionAgency (EPA), in collaborationwiththe Canadian Pest Management Regulatory Agency(PMRA) and the California Department of PesticideRegulation (CDPR), released a guidance document for

assessing pesticide risk to bees (EPA, PMRA andCDPR 2014). The protection goals specified in theguidance are to ensure bee biodiversity, provision ofpollination services, and production of hive products.At the individual level of biological organization, themeasurement endpoints are worker survival, larvalemergence, and queen fecundity/reproduction. Nectarforagers are expected to have the highest dietary ex-posure of all honey bee castes because of their elevatedenergy requirements during flight (EPA, PMRA andCDPR 2012; 2014). To assess dietary exposure ofnectar foragers, the Agencies modeled daily nectarrequirement rates. This analysiswas presented in detailin aWhite Paper that preceded the guidance document(EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2012). Pollen requirementswere also estimated, but as discussed in the WhitePaper and in Rodney and Purdy (this issue), pollen isnot a significant food source for bees of nectar-foraging age. The Agencies’ model was probabilisticand included input variables accounting for the amountand duration of flying trips and the amount of sugar

Electronic supplementary material The online version ofthis article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s13592-019-00693-w)contains supplementary material, which is available toauthorized users.

Corresponding author: V. Kramer,[email protected] editor: James Nieh

Apidologie Original article* The Author(s), 2019.This article is an open access publicationDOI: 10.1007/s13592-019-00693-w

Page 2: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

required to fly and rest. The Agencies selected themedian estimate from the output distribution for nectaringestion rate (i.e., 292mg nectar/bee/day) as the basisfor estimating exposure in screening-level assessments.In their proposed exposure assessment, conservatively,it is assumed that there is no degradation or dissipationof the pesticide between application and ingestion.Available data from Harano and Nakamura (2016)demonstrate that foragers acquire fuel for foragingfrom processed nectar in the hive in advance ofdeparture.

The Agencies’ model was based on sparse inputdata and was not subject to a sensitivity analysis. Acomprehensive review of the literature was under-taken to characterize the available data relating to thenectar requirements of nectar foragers (Rodney andPurdy (this issue)). The objective of the analysisdescribed in this paper is to compare ingestion rateestimates derived in theAgencies’ originalmodelingwith those from an alternative model (RP model)employing data compiled by Rodney and Purdy(this issue) and with the Agencies’ model parame-terized with data compiled by Rodney and Purdy(this issue). Additionally, the sensitivity of themodels was assessed and the effect of potentialcorrelations in the Agencies’ model was explored.

2. METHODS

2.1. Agencies’ model

The model presented in the White Paper fornectar consumption rate of nectar foragers is afunction of trips per day (T ), trip duration (D i ),sugar required for flight (S F), sugar required tomeet resting metabolic rate (SR), and sugar contentof nectar (P i and P ave), as presented below:

Dnectar ¼ S F*∑Ti¼0

Di*Fi

Pi

� �

þ SR* 24− ∑Ti¼0Di*Fi

� �� �Pave

ð1Þ

where

D nectar Nectar requirement (mg nectar/bee/day)T Number of trips per day

S F Sugar required for flight (mg/h)S R Sugar required to meet resting metabolic

rate (mg/h)F i Fraction of time spent flying during trip

iD i Duration of foraging trip i (h)P i Sugar content of collected nectar during

trip iP ave Average sugar content of nectar

The Agencies used a probabilistic version of thismodel to simulate 10,000 nectar-foraging bees. Forfive of the seven parameters listed above, theAgenciesassumed continuous distributions. For SR and P ave,point estimates were used (Table I). The model de-scribed by the Agencies in the White Paper wasreproduced in MS Excel® 2016 with Oracle® CrystalBall (Release 11.1.2.4.600), hereafter referred to as theRA model (Reproduction of the Agencies’model).

In the RA model, the number of trips per day,T , was set up as a single assumption using theinputs from Table I. Subsequently, for each poten-tial trip i = 1 to 150,D i , F i , and P i were selectedfrom the continuous distributions specified inTable I. For each of these trips, D i*F i (the timeon the trip spent flying in hours) was calculated.The resulting value was divided by P i ([D i*F i ]/P i ).

The selected number of trips, T , was then usedin each trial to determine how many of the tripssimulated would be used in each trial to estimate anectar consumption rate. The Agencies did notspecify how the continuous lognormal estimateswere used to estimate whole numbers of trips, i =1 to 150. This can be done by rounding the sam-pled value to the nearest whole number, which iswhat we did.

2.2. Revised input assumptions for theAgencies’ model

Subsequently, we ran the RA model with re-vised input assumptions based on data collated inRodney and Purdy (this issue). The RA modelwith revised inputs is referred to herein as theRARI (Reproduction of Agencies’ with RevisedInputs) model.

For each of the revised input assumptions, datawere considered for quantitative use if they met the

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 3: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

Tab

leI.Com

parisonof

Agencies’andrevisedmodelinputassum

ptions

forhoneybeenectar

foragernectar

requirem

entestim

ates

Parameter

Agencies’assumptions

aRevised

inputsfortheRARImodel

Dist.

Min

Max

Mean

St.

dev.

Dist.

Min

Max

Mean

St. dev.

Com

ment

Num

berof

tripsper

day,T

Lognorm

al1

150

103

Lognorm

al1

450

2.91

2.50

Tobe

roundedto

thenearestw

holenumber.

Locationparameter

settominim

umof

1

Tripduratio

n(h),D

iUniform

0.5

1.33

NA

NA

Lognorm

al0.0167

150.59

2.76

Assum

ingacentrallocation(K

ansasCity)on

June

20,w

epresum

e15

hof

daylight,and

thus

15hof

potentialforagingtim

e

Fractionoftripspent

flying,F

i

Uniform

0.5

0.9

NA

NA

Uniform

0.5

0.9

NA

NA

Not

modifiedon

thegroundsthatthereare

insufficient

datato

parameterizethisdistributio

n.To

befurtherexplored

inthesensitivity

analysis

Sugar

requirem

ent

forflight

(mg/h),

SF

Uniform

712

NA

NA

Lognorm

al0

Not speci-

fied

12.57

6.10

Based

onfree-flying,untethered

flight

experiments

Sugar

required

tomeetrestin

gmetabolicrate

(mg/h),S

R

None

Fixedat0.7

Lognorm

al0

Not speci-

fied

0.37

0.58

Whererestingmeans

relativ

elymotionless,andnot

necessarily

ectothermic

Sugarcontento

fnectar

(w/w)

Lognorm

al0.1

0.6

0.3

0.1

Beta

0.02

0.79

0.30

0.10

UsedAgencies’meanandstandard

deviationbut

appliedbetadistributio

nwith

fixedlim

its

Maxim

umtim

eoutsidethehive

(h)

––

––

–None

NA

15NA

NA

Assum

ingacentrallocation(K

ansasCity)on

June

20,w

epresum

e15

hof

daylight,and

thus

15hof

potentialforagingtim

e

aThese

inputsarefrom

EPA

,PMRAandCDPR

(2012)

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 4: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

following quality criteria: (1) sample size was re-ported, (2) sample mean and/or sample standarderror, standard deviation, or variance were reported,(3) there was no application of a pesticide, and(4) there were no significant treatment-related effects if a stressor (other than apesticide) was applied to a sample group.When results for metabolism were present-ed at multiple air temperatures, we selectedthose closest to the average air temperaturein June of approximately 22 °C in the cen-tral USA. Also, with respect to flights (fre-quency and duration), we only considereddata from bees foraging on plants, as op-posed to prepared sugar solutions, to avoidusing unrealistic observations in the model-ing. All data used to generate model inputsare provided in Online Resource 1.

Weighted means were calculated based on re-ported means and sample sizes from availablestudies. That is, reported means were weightedaccording to the corresponding sample size. Thecorresponding pooled sample variance calcula-tions were based on reported sample sizes, means,and standard deviations or standard errors(Headrick 2010; Table I).

2.2.1. Trip inputs, T and D i

The number of trips taken in a given dayby a nectar forager varies among subspecies,colonies, individuals, and temporally for eachindividual depending on the age and healthof the forager, colony needs, available re-sources, and weather conditions (reviewedin Rodney and Purdy, this issue). The mostcomprehensive data currently available forthe frequency of trips taken by workers isbased on radio-frequency identification(RFID) tracking of individual bees over ex-tended periods of time, typically days toweeks (Thompson et al. 2016; Dosselliet al. 2016). The results are presented asaverages of individual bees over numerousflying days. Thus, these results, as well asthe less robust datasets from other publishedstudies investigating trips per day, do notinclude day-to-day variability in the numberof trips taken by an individual. Based on the

results of He et al. (2013), we generallyexpect to see an increase in individual forag-ing time with age.

Bees avoid foraging on days of inclementweather. As reviewed in Rodney and Purdy (thisissue), not all bees of foraging age forage everyday, even if the weather conditions are favorable.A conservative approach is to only consider beesthat are foraging when estimating the number oftrips taken per day by bees of foraging age (i.e.,the minimum number of trips per day is one), aswas done by the Agencies.

Based on quantitative data from five studies, amean of 2.91 ± 2.50 trips per day was calculatedfor T (n = 2713 for sample mean, n = 2257 forsample standard deviation; Heberle 1914; Thomet al. 2000; Lach et al. 2015; Dosselli et al. 2016;Thompson et al. 2016; Online Resource 1, Ta-ble S1-1 Data used to estimate the distributionparameters for the number of trips per day (T )taken by honey bee nectar foragers in the RARImodel). Other studies did not report sample size orvariance but did report mean flights per bee perday of between 7 and 13.5 trips (Park 1922;Lundie 1925) with single day maxima of 17 and24, for bees foraging on white clover in Iowa(Park 1922). These results were based on countsmade by observers, which may be more prone toerror than RFID tracking.

The data used to estimate the number of trips perday suggest a right skew. A discrete distribution,such as Poisson, may be considered a more appro-priate distribution for count data. In this case, how-ever, the variance indicates overdispersion if thePoisson distribution is assumed. Thus, we assumeda lognormal distribution, which is consistent withthe Agencies’ approach, with a location parameter(minimum) of 1. The selected number of trips wasrounded to a whole number in the simulations. Weassumed (1) 15 h of daylight (approximately thelongest day in the center of the contiguous USA)and (2) arbitraryminimum trip and turnaround timesof 1 min and accordingly set a theoretical maximumnumber of round trips to 450. We note here that theAgencies’ upper limit of 150 trips per daywas basedon a value presented in Winston (1987) that origi-nated from Butler et al. (1943). The latter reportedthat two bees foraging on sugar solution made ap-proximately 150 trips by early evening. Most of the

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 5: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

literature sources for trip duration data, Di , overlapwith the sources for the number of trips per day, T.Those results are reported as individual bee averagesand do not account for daily intra-individualvariability.

The revised model input for the number of tripsper day, T , has a lower average (mean of 2.91compared with 10) and greater range than theassumption used by the Agencies (Figure 1A).

Perry et al. (2015) reported the average totalnumber of trips recorded per bee (over her life-time) between approximately 15 and 50. Lachet al. (2015) reported total trips up to 98 over a

4-week experiment, with medians of 12 and 13trips per forager lifetime. By comparison, theAgencies assumed a mean of 10 trips per day.Averages of 5–10 flying days (Thompson et al.2016) with approximately 3 trips per day equate to15 to 30 trips per forager lifetime on averagewhich fall within the range of reported valuesfrom Perry et al. (2015).

Based on quantitative data from six studies, amean of 0.591 ± 2.76 h per trip (D i ) was calcu-lated (n = 3329 for sample mean, n = 3320 forsample standard deviation; Heberle 1914; Rib-bands 1949; Eckert et al. 1994; Perry et al. 2016;

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

0.3

0.35

0.4

0.45

0 5 10 15 20

Prob

abili

ty

Trips per Day,

Agencies' Assumption

Revised Assumption

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5

Prob

abili

ty

Trip Duration (hr)

Agencies' Assumption

Revised Assumption

A

B

Figure 1. Probability density functions for trip model inputs: (A) trips per day, T , and (B) trip duration, D i .

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 6: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

Dosselli et al. 2016; Thompson et al. 2016). Of theother relevant studies, Park (1922) reported twomean trip durations of 0.567 h and 0.817 h.Lundie (1925) estimated a range of 0.150 to1.73 h. The highest reported single trip durationobserved was 2.5 h (Ribbands 1949). These re-sults are in line with the data used to estimate D i

(Online Resource 1, Table S1-2 Data used toestimate distribution parameters for average tripduration (D i ) for honey bee nectar foragers in theRARI model). A lognormal distribution was as-sumed. For each trip, the distribution was truncat-ed at 15 h (assumed daylight). Also, the number oftrips was limited such that the sum of trip dura-tions (D i ) was not allowed to exceed 15 h (in thiscase, the maximum number of trips that did notexceed 15 h was taken in lieu).

In contrast to the Agencies’ uniform distribu-tion assumption, the revised model input assump-tion is right skewed with a much larger range(Figure 1B).

The fraction of trips spent flying, F i , was notmodified from the Agencies’ assumption of auniform distribution ranging from 0.5 to 0.9.There are insufficient data to estimate this distri-bution, and the Agencies’ assumption was basedon a supposition of Rortais et al. (2005).

2.2.2. Sugar requirements, S F and S R

For sugar requirements for flight, S F, wepooled sample means and variances from energet-ics studies that reported values for bees in free,untethered flight (Coelho and Mitton 1988; Wolfet al. 1989; Balderrama et al. 1992; Feuerbacheret al. 2003; Table I). Sample values originatedfrom four studies reporting on 216 measurementstaken from 125 bees (Online Resource 1, Ta-ble S1-3 Data used to estimate distribution param-eters for sugar requirement for flight (S F) of hon-ey bee nectar foragers in the RARI model). Weassumed a lognormal distribution, given the re-striction to positive values and observed highvariance, and estimated a mean of 12.6 ±6.10 mg sugar/h.

The revised assumption for S F has a mean thatexceeds the Agencies’ maximum value, a widerrange, and a right skew compared with the

Agencies’ uniform distribution assumption(Figure 2A).

For this model, it was not the intent to quantifytrue resting metabolism for S R. True resting me-tabolism is the energy consumption associatedwith motionless and not actively thermoregulatingbees. True resting metabolism occurs when thereare comparable thoracic and ambient air tempera-tures. In the open literature, there has been someconfusion over resting metabolic rates becausemany authors have included motionless or restingbees that were also likely thermoregulating andtherefore not at complete rest (see Rothe andNachtigall 1989). We anticipate that nectar for-agers will actively thermoregulate as needed whenthey are not foraging. In the absence of robust datafor time spent thermoregulating, here we assumethat any measures of relatively motionless beesreflect a random sample of non-flight metabolismof nectar foragers. As discussed in Rodney andPurdy (this issue), metabolism generally increaseswith increasing activity level. Nectar foragers thatare not in flight may spend time at levels of energyexpenditure greater than resting levels. However,in this simple model, a gradient of energy expen-diture is purposely omitted.

From available studies meeting our criteria, amean of 0.372 ± 0.576 mg sugar/h/bee was calcu-lated from 329measurements taken from 243 bees(Cahill and Lustik 1976; Rothe and Nachtigall1989; Goller and Esch 1991; Stabentheiner et al.2003; Kovac et al. 2007; Online Resource 1,Table S1-4 Data used to estimate the distributionparameters for sugar required to meet resting met-abolic rate (S R) of honey bee nectar foragers in theRARI and RP models). We believe that theseestimates for resting metabolism overestimate en-ergy expenditure of resting bees in the hive duringthe growing season because bees in clusters tendto have lower metabolism than isolated bees(Southwick 1983; Fahrenholz et al. 1989). Mostbees from which resting metabolism was mea-sured were isolated.

Southwick (1982) reported a range of 3.4 to33.5 W/kg of honey bees in the hive. This studyincluded air temperatures ranging from − 20 toover 25 °C and also measured energy consump-tion over full 24-h periods, with and withoutbrood. The range reported translates to

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 7: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

approximately0.072 to 0.72 mg sugar/h per 100mgbees. This range is comparable to our estimates,despite the inclusion of air temperatures below thoseexpected during the growing season in the USA.Given the relatively high observed variability andrequirement for positive metabolism values, we as-sumed that the distribution for resting metabolismhas a right skew. Thus, we assumed a lognormaldistribution. The distribution assumption for sugarrequired to meet resting metabolic rate, SR, derivedhere for the RARI model was also subsequently

used in the RP model as the base assumption forsugar required inside the hive, S I. For sugar require-ments, upper and lower non-zero thresholds werenot set for model inputs (Tables I and II) but wereexplored in subsequent sensitivity analyses.

Compared with the Agencies’ assumptionfor resting metabolic rate, S R, of 0.7 mgsugar/h, the revised assumption is probabi-listic with a right skew and mean valuelower than the Agencies’ deterministic val-ue (Figure 2B).

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

0 5 10 15 20 25 30

Prob

abili

ty

Sugar Required for Flight (mg sugar/hr)

Agencies' Assumption

Revised Assumption

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

3.5

0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5

Prob

abili

ty

Sugar Required for Resting (mg sugar/hr)

Agencies' Assumption

Revised Assumption

A

B

Figure 2. Probability density functions for sugar requirements: (A) for flight (mg/h), SF, and (B) for resting (mg/h), SR.

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 8: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

2.2.3. Sugar content of nectar, P i

For the sugar content of collected nectar duringtrip i, P i , we retained the Agencies’ mean andstandard deviation of 0.3 and 0.1, respectively.These values were based on the Agencies’ reviewof the sugar content of nectars presented in theWhite Paper (EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2012).These data are not necessarily representative ofnectar that would be collected by nectar foragersin US agroecosystems because the plant specieslisted were largely non-crop plants, and nectarforagers are expected to prefer flowers with highsugar content (Knopper et al. 2016; Rodney andPurdy, this issue). This issue was explored in thesensitivity analysis.

In contrast to the Agencies’ approach using alognormal distribution with minimum and maxi-mum of 0.1 and 0.6, respectively, we applied abeta distribution to the P i assumption, which waspermitted to range from 0.02 to 0.79 (see Fig. S1-1 in Online Resource 1). The lower limit hererepresents the lowest value from the Agencies’review, and the upper limit, the solubility of fruc-tose in water at 20 °C (Chen and Chou 1993).Sugar content of nectar has been reported as highas 0.84 (see review by Knopper et al. 2016), andaverage crop load contents have been reported ashigh as approximately 0.65. Therefore, a higherthreshold than established by the Agencies’ valueof 0.6 is expected based on available nectars andsugar content preferences of foragers (see reviewby Rodney and Purdy, this issue). The averagesugar content of nectar, P ave, in accordance withthe Agencies, was retained as a point estimate of0.30.

Differences between the Agencies’ assump-tions and our revised model inputs are presentedin Table I.

2.3. RP model

The RP model is a simplified version of theAgencies’ model. It is based solely on theestimated amount of time spent outside thehive, the metabolism that occurs inside andoutside the hive, and the proportion of sugar(w/w) in nectar. By using time outside the hiveper day instead of number of trips and tripdurations, we bypass the need to quantify apossible negative correlation between tripsper day and duration. We also account for tripstaken outside the hive that contribute to meta-bolic requirements but are not necessarily as-sociated with nectar foraging (e.g., defecationand orientation flights). Given that the fractionof trip spent flying cannot currently be accu-rately quantified, a fraction of time spent out-side the hive in which the forager would not beflying or foraging (i.e., a time outside resting)was not included in the model. The RP modelis given in Eq. 2.

Dnectar ¼ SO*TOð Þ þ SI* 24−TO½ �ð ÞP

ð2Þwhere

D nectar Nectar requirement (mg nectar/bee/day)S O Sugar required outside the hive (mg/h)S I Sugar required inside the hive (mg/h)TO Time spent outside the hive (h/day)P Proportion of sugar in nectar (w/w)

The distributions for sugar required inside thehive (mg/h), S I, and sugar content of nectar (w/w),P, were assumed to be the same as in the RARImodel for S R and P i .

Data collated to estimate time outside the hive,TO, was primarily derived from literature sources

Table II. Summary of input assumptions for the RPmodel for honey bee nectar forager nectar requirement estimates

Input variable Distribution Min Max Mean Standard deviation

Time outside the hive (h), TO Lognormal NA NA 2.08 7.49

Sugar required for time outside the hive (mg/h), SO Lognormal NA NA 9.41 7.16

Sugar required inside the hive (mg/h), S I Lognormal NA NA 0.37 0.58

Sugar content of nectar (w/w), P Beta 0.02 0.79 0.30 0.10

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 9: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

that were also used in establishing distributionsfor the number of trips per day, T , and trip dura-tion, D i , in the RARI model. These studies fo-cused on the bee as the experimental unit, andmeasurements collected over numerous days wereaveraged on a per bee basis. The result is thatsample distributions represent bee averages anddo not capture intra-individual temporal variation.He et al. (2013) demonstrated that as bees age,they tend to spend more time outside the hive.

Based on available data from two comprehen-sive studies carried out in Australia and the UKwith 1615 bees, we calculated a mean of 2.08 ±7.49 h of average amount of time spent outside thehive per day, which was used to parameterize TO

(Online Resource 1, Table S1-5 Data used toestimate the distribution parameters for time spentoutside the hive (TO) for honey bee nectar for-agers in the RP model and Fig. S1-2). Thesevalues are in good agreement with He et al.(2013) who reported a maximum time for a singlebee from a cohort of 47 bees surviving to 36 daysof 6.25 h. Thom et al. (2000) reported that of 300tagged bees, over 70% were out of the hive forless than 4.5 h. Given the high variance and thelower limit of 5 s to 1 min based on the defaultvalue used in both studies, we presume a right-skewed distribution. We also assume an upperlimit of 15 h based on our fixed assumption of acentral location about June solstice (June 20, Kan-sas City). We assumed a lognormal distribution(Online Resource 1, Fig. S1-6) and explored theimpact of changing the distribution assumptionand adding lower thresholds in the sensitivityanalysis.

Thompson et al. (2016) reported that bees onaverage flew on 5.0 to 10.3 days only (observationperiod was 5 weeks). In total, bees were outsidethe hive on average 9.05 to 24.51 h across theentire observation period of 5 weeks (Thompsonet al. 2016). These results are in line with Perryet al. (2015) who reported mean total hours out-side the hive between 5 and 15 h (40-day obser-vation period). Total lifetime spent outside thehive was up to 88 h, with medians of 11 and13 h reported by Lach et al. (2015). The lowestmedian age of the first forage in the study was9 days, and the median age of the last forage was23 days. Accordingly, it is reasonable to assume

that foragers will forage over approximately2 weeks, and of this time, one to two-thirds ofdays will typically involve flights outside the hive(for foraging and other activities). This is fairlyconsistent with the individual data presented byHe et al. (2013; Figure 8b therein). Our assump-tion based on the available data was ~ 2 h outsidethe hive per day (RP model), which is consistentwith these data, in that 2 h per 5 to 10 flying daysis equivalent to 10 to 20 total hours of time outsidethe hive. This range falls within the mean range ofThompson et al. (2016) and overlaps the high endof the range reported by Perry et al. (2015). Bycomparison, the RA model predicted a median of8.71 h/day outside the hive. If we again assume anaverage of 5 to 10 flying days per forager, thiswould equate to 44 to 88 h outside the hive overthe forager’s lifetime. These estimates based onthe Agencies’ model exceed the upper boundsreported by Thompson et al. (2016), Perry et al.(2015), and Lach et al. (2015). The study by Lachet al. (2015) included monitoring of 447 bees.

An upper threshold of 15 h/day was specifiedfor time outside the hive (TO) and trip duration(D i ) for both RP and RARI models. The variabil-ity in the underlying data was high due to someindividual bees spending > 24 h continuously out-side the hive (Thompson, 2018; personal commu-nication). In these cases, which were rare, it isunlikely that the bees were foraging for that long,and it is more likely that they drifted to a differenthive or found cover overnight elsewhere. Ulti-mately, the upper threshold of 15 h/day for tripduration (D i ) or time outside the hive (TO) led toa lower mean and standard deviation (1.68 ±2.94 h) in the values used than originally estimat-ed based on author reported values (2.07 ±7.49 h).

The data used to parameterize the sugar re-quired for flight, S F, distribution in the Agencies’model with revised inputs were used to estimatethe sugar requirement outside the hive. In addi-tion, data for bees foraging inside a large (6 L)respirometer (Balderrama et al. 1992) and flyingbetween artificial flowers were also included asrepresentative of outdoor behavior of foragers. Amean of 9.41 ± 7.16 mg sugar/h was calculated(n = 769; Online Resource 1, Table S1-6 Dataused to estimate the distribution parameters for

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 10: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

sugar required outside the hive (SO) by honey beenectar foragers for the RP model and Fig. S1-3).

Several energetics studies identified in Rodneyand Purdy (this issue) were reviewed subsequentto the modeling effort presented herein. Most ofthe additional studies provided data that are con-sistent with those used in the modeling (see On-line Resource 3 for Rodney and Purdy, this issue).However, most did not meet the criteria for inclu-sion in the quantitative modeling. Those that did(Heusner and Stussi 1964; Moffatt 2000) fellwithin the range of values used to estimated S R,S F, and SO. Their addition to the datasets used toestimated distributions would have negligible im-pact on model inputs and predictions and wasomitted from the parameter distributionestimations.

Monte Carlo simulations were carried out with100,000 trials per model.

2.4. Exploratory sensitivity and correlationanalyses

When models contain fixed assumptions,sensitivity to variability in those assumptionscan be explored by changing the inputs andobserving how the results affect model predic-tions (Vose 1996). Alternatively, simulationscan be generated that account for the potentialdistribution of inputs (e.g., Monte Carlo anal-yses, see EPA ( 2009)). In the case of theAgencies, the RA, RARI, and Rodney-Purdy(RP) nectar forager models, inputs are predom-inantly probabilistic and already account forestimated variability. A concern, given limiteddata, is how alternate assumptions about thedistribution of a variable may impact (1) themodel predictions, (2) the contribution to var-iance of the inputs, and (3) the correlationbetween inputs and predictions (Paleari andConfalonieri, 2016).

We investigated the effects on predictions whenchanging the mean or standard deviation of eachinput variable by ± 50%,while holding other inputsconstant. This type of sensitivity analysis, alsoknown as an elasticity analysis, is used to deter-mine those input variables for which consistentchanges to the parameters have the most influenceon model predictions. Depending on the input

variable, we also explored changes to thresholds(minima and maxima), as well as different distri-bution assumptions (Online Resource 1, Table S1-7 Summary of exploratory sensitivity analyses ofthe RARI model with fixed changes to modelassumptions and Table S1-8 Summary of explor-atory sensitivity analyses of the Rodney-Purdymodel with fixed changes to model assumptions).We consider here (1) impacts onmodel predictions,(2) contribution to variance, and (3) rankcorrelation.

Applying upper and lower thresholds to availabledata in themodeling can be problematic. For example,take specifying a lower threshold of 30 min for tripduration (or time outside the hive). The data used toestimate the distributions for trip duration and timeoutside the hive did not contain this threshold. Accord-ingly, means and standard deviations were based inpart on trips and times outside the hive that were lowerthan this threshold. When this threshold is applied tothe distribution defined by these data (for a lognormaldistribution), the applied mean and standard deviationare retained, and the distribution is reshaped tomeet allparameters. Clearly, this is not the same as removingtrips or durations less than 30 min from the initialdataset. The lower limit of 30 min was selected forexploration in accordance with the Agencies’ model;however, available data indicate that workers of forag-ing age will take trips outside the hive with durationsfar less than 30 min.

Exploratory correlation analyses were con-ducted with the RA and RARI models. Thecorrelations that were intuitively expected andexplored included (1) high and moderate nega-tive correlation between trips per day and tripduration (i.e., fewer trips are taken per day whentrips are of longer duration), (2) high and mod-erate negative correlation between trip durationand fraction of trip spent flying (i.e., the propor-tion of a trip spent flying decreases with increas-ing trip duration), and (3) high forage fidelity(i.e., bees will forage on the same patch overmultiple trips, and there is thus dependencyamong the number of trips on a given day).We assumed that trips on a given day wouldbe the same length, with the same fraction oftime spent flying and the same sugar content ofnectar. The scenario in which all correlationslisted above are concurrently high was also run.

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 11: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

High correlation coefficients were set at − 1.0,and moderate correlation coefficients were set at− 0.5.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Agencies’ model

We were unable to exactly reproduce the per-centile estimates generated by the Agencies fornectar forager daily nectar requirements (On-line Resource 2, Table S2-1 Nectar requirementestimates for nectar foragers (D nectar) with the RAmodel). We estimated a median of 253 mg nectar/bee/day, which is 13% lower than the Agencies’value of 292 mg nectar/bee/day, and a mean of265 ± 78.1 mg nectar/bee/day. In general, our per-centile estimates were between 6 and 19% lowerthan those reported in the Agencies’ White Paper(EPA, PMRA and CDPR 2012), with differencegenerally increasing with increasing percentiles ofthe distributions. Note the Agencies did not reportmeans or standard deviations for comparison.With our reproduction of the Agencies’ model,we estimated a mean total flying time of 6.39 ±2.03 h and a cumulative total daily trip time of9.13 ± 2.86 h. The Agencies reported a medianflying time of 8.9 h; by comparison, we estimateda median flying time of 6.10 h with our reproduc-tion of their model.

Assuming a negative correlation between tripsper day, T , and duration, D i , resulted in notablylower variability in predictions of nectar require-ment but had little impact on predicted mediansand means (< 6% lower). Assuming a negativecorrelation between trip duration,D i , and fractionof trip spent flying, F i , also resulted in lowervariability and lower central values, though evenless pronounced. All tabled percentile estimateswere slightly lower than the base case (Online Re-source 2, Table S2-23 Nectar requirement esti-mates for nectar foragers (D nectar) from the RAmodel with explored correlations and Figs. S2-16and S2-17).

The fidelity assumption removed variabilitybetween numbers of trips in a day and assumedall trips would be of the same duration, D , thesame fraction of trip spent flying, F , and the samesugar content of nectar, P . This assumption

resulted in much lower predictions of nectar re-quirement up to approximately the 65th percentilebut at and beyond the 75th percentile resulted inhigher predictions (Online Resource 2, Table S2-23 Nectar requirement estimates for nectar for-agers (D nectar) from the RA model with exploredcorrelations and Fig. S2-18). Overall the impactwas greater variability with the same predictedmean and a predicted median that was approxi-mately 6% lower than the base case.

Assuming high fidelity and high negative cor-relations between trip duration, D i , and frequen-cy, T , and fraction of trip spent flying, F i , resultedin slightly lower variability in predictions thanbase case and also lower estimates of nectar re-quirement. The mean declined from the base caseof 265 to 250 mg nectar/bee/day, and the mediandeclined from 253 to 239 mg nectar/bee/day (On-line Resource 2, Table S2-23 Nectar requirementestimates for nectar foragers (D nectar) from the RAmodel with explored correlations and Fig. S2-19).

3.2. Agencies’ model with revised inputs

The RARI model resulted in a much largerrange of predicted values, with a considerableright skew (Figure 3A, B). The median estimatefrom the RARI model is 43.6 mg nectar/bee/day,over 6× lower than the Agencies’ estimate of292 mg nectar/bee/day. From the RARI model,the mean nectar requirement was 75.2 ± 100 mgnectar/bee/day. The assumption with the largestcontribution to variance in predicted nectar re-quirement with the RARI model is the sugar re-quired to meet resting metabolic rate, S R, with avalue of 44%. The next largest contributor tovariance is trip duration (D i ), with a value of26%, followed by the number of trips per day(T ), 20%; the amount of sugar required for flight(S F), 6.2%; the sugar content of nectar (P i ),2.5%; and the proportion of the trip spent flying(F i ), 1.3%. Ranked correlation follows this samepattern in terms of magnitude, except, as expect-ed, the sugar content of nectar collected duringtrips (P i ) is negatively correlated with the predict-ed nectar requirement (Online Resource 2, Ta-ble S2-3 Sensitivity of nectar requirement esti-mates (D nectar) to the RARI model inputs).

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 12: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

3.2.1. Number of trips per day, T

For the number of trips per day (T ) in the RARImodel, most changes to the input distribution (i.e.,switch to beta distribution, lower maximum num-ber of trips per day, changing the variance) had anegligible impact on model predictions (On-line Resource 2, Table S2-5 Nectar requirementestimates for nectar foragers (D nectar) from theRARI model with explored assumptions for the

number of trips per day (T ) (mg nectar/bee/day)and Figs. S2-2 through S2-4). Changing the meanby ± 50%, however, resulted in a considerabledifference with mean and median predictions fornectar requirement of 51.6 to 98.8 mg nectar/bee/day and 28.8 to 60.0 mg nectar/bee/day, respec-tively (Online Resource 2, Fig. S2-2). From thesensitivity analyses, the ordering of contributionto variance and rank correlation did not change forany of the explored changes, except for when the

Figure 3. Frequency distributions for model results of (A) the RAmodel, (B) the RARI model, and (C) the RP model.

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 13: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

high mean was applied. This resulted in trip dura-tion (D i ) having the highest contribution to vari-ance and the highest rank correlation (as opposedto sugar required to meet resting metabolic rate(S R)) (Online Resource 2, Table S2-6 Contribu-tion to variance for RARI model inputs with ex-plored assumptions for the number of trips per day(T ) and Table S2-7 Rank correlation for RARImodel inputs with explored assumptions for thenumber of trips per day (T )).

3.2.2. Sugar required for flight, S F

The only change to the distribution for sugarrequired for flight (S F) that substantially affectedmodel predictions was changing the mean by ±50% (Online Resource 2, Table S2-8 Nectar re-quirement estimates for nectar foragers (D nectar)from the RARI model with explored assumptionsfor sugar required for flight (S F) and Figs. S2-5 toS2-7). The mean and median predictions fromchanging the mean ± 50% were 51.4 to 98.8 mgsugar/bee/day and 30.3 to 54.9 mg sugar/bee/day,respectively.

Only the high mean changed the ordering ofcontributions to variance and rank correlation.Increasing the mean by 50% resulted in trip dura-tion having the highest contribution to varianceand the highest correlation with nectarrequirement.

3.2.3. Sugar required to meet restingmetabolic rate, S R

The lower percentile predictions of nectar re-quirement were particularly sensitive to changesin the distribution parameters for sugar required tomeet resting metabolic rate, S R (Online Resource2, Table S2-11 Nectar requirement estimates fornectar foragers (D nectar) from the RARI modelwith explored assumptions for sugar required forrest (S R) and Figs. S2-8 through S2-10). Highmean, low standard deviation, and setting a min-imum threshold of 0.1 mg sugar/h led to higherpredictions for nectar requirements at lower per-centiles, and in the case of the high mean, alsohigher percentiles. The Weibull model assump-tion, high standard deviation, and low mean ledto lower predictions of nectar requirement at

lower percentiles and also at higher percentilesin the case of the lowmean assumption. Changingthe mean ± 50% resulted in the widest range ofpredicted measures of centrality, i.e., means andmedians of 61.6 to 89.5 mg nectar/bee/day and28.5 to 60.1 mg nectar/bee/day, respectively.

Sugar required to meet resting metabolic rate(S R) had the highest contribution to variance andthe highest rank correlation of all input variablesin the base case and also across all other changesto the input distribution, except when reducing themean by 50%. The latter scenario resulted in tripduration, D i , contributing more to variance andhaving the highest rank correlation among theinput variables (Online Resource 2, Table S2-12Contribution to variance for RARI model inputswith explored assumptions for the sugar requiredfor rest (S R) and Table S2-13 Rank correlation forRARI model inputs with explored assumptionsfor the sugar required for rest (S R)).

3.2.4. Duration of trips, D i

Changing the lower threshold for trip duration(D i ) to 1 min had a negligible effect on thepredictions for nectar requirement. However, in-creasing the minimum threshold to 30 min had alarger effect on predictions, particularly the lowerpercentiles (Figure 4). Assuming a Weibull modelgenerally decreased predictions of nectar require-ment in the central percentiles. Changes (± 50%)to the mean or standard deviation had a clearimpact on nectar requirement predictions, withchanges to the mean resulting in greater effect(Online Resource 2, Table S2-14 Nectar require-ment estimates for nectar foragers (D nectar) fromthe RARI model with explored assumptions fortrip duration (D i ) and Figs. S2-11 and S2-12). Ofall the elasticity analyses, the greatest mean nectarrequirement stemmed from the high mean scenar-io (98.8 mg nectar/bee/h), and the highest medianarose with the assumption of a minimum of30 min per trip (61.6 mg nectar/bee/h).

For each of the changes to the input distributionfor trip duration (D i ), the order of contributionto variance and rank correlations did notchange from the base case, except for thehigh mean scenario. The high mean scenarioresulted in trip duration (D i ) contributing the

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 14: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

most to the variance in nectar requirementand also resulted in trip duration (D i ) havingthe highest rank correlation with nectar re-quirement (Online Resource 2, Table S2-15Contribution to variance for RARI modelinputs with explored assumptions for trip du-ration (D i ) and Table S2-16 Rank correlationfor RARI model inputs with explored as-sumptions for trip duration (D i )).

.

3.2.5. Fraction of trip spent flying, F i

The assumed changes to the distribution forfraction of trip spent flying had little impact onthe predicted nectar requirements (Online Re-source 2, Table S2-17 Nectar requirement esti-mates for nectar foragers (D nectar) from the RARImodel with explored assumptions for fraction oftrip spent flying (F i ) and Fig. S2-13). The rangesof means and medians were 65.2 to 85.2 mg nec-tar/bee/day and 39.3 to 47.8 mg nectar/bee/day,respectively. The changes to the input variable forfraction of trip spent flying did not alter the order-ing of contributions to variance or rank correla-tions of input variables (Online Resource 2, Ta-ble S2-18 Contribution to variance for RARImodel inputs with explored assumptions for frac-tion of trip spent flying (F i ) and Table S2-19

Rank correlation for RARI model inputs withexplored assumptions for fraction of trip spentflying (F i )).

3.2.6. Sugar content of nectar, P i and P ave

Changes of ± 50% to the standard deviation forsugar in nectar did not dramatically change thepredictions for nectar requirement; however,changing the mean ± 50% did result in substantialchanges to the prediction of nectar requirementthroughout most of the distribution. Mean andmedian ranges were 60.3 to 321 mg nectar/bee/day and 28.3 to 98.4 mg nectar/bee/day, respec-tively (Online Resource 2, Table S2-20 Nectarrequirement estimates for nectar foragers (D nectar)from the RARI model with explored assumptionsfor sugar content of nectar (P i ) and Figs. S2-14and S2-15).

No changes to the order of contribution tovariance or rank correlation of the model inputvariables were observed with changes to the inputdistribution for sugar content of nectar (Online Re-source 2, Table S2-21 Contribution to variance forRARI model inputs with explored assumptionsfor sugar content of nectar (P i ) and Table S2-22Rank correlation for RARI model inputs withexplored assumptions for sugar content of nectar(P i )).

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1 10 100 1000

Percentile

Nectar Requirement (mg nectar/bee/day)

Base Case

Trip Duration - Minimum 1 min.

Trip Duration - Minimum 30 min.

Reproduction of Agencies' Model

Figure 4. Predicted cumulative distributions of nectar requirement for nectar-foraging honey bees with changes tothe lower threshold assumption for trip duration (D i ) in the RARI model.

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 15: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

3.2.7. Correlation analyses

Assuming a negative correlation between tripsper day, T , and duration, D i , resulted in notablylower variability in predictions of nectar require-ment and generally lower predicted nectar require-ments, in contrast to the results for the RA model.Assuming a negative correlation between trip du-ration and fraction of trip spent flying did notresult in much change to predictions of nectarrequirement, which was similar for the RAmodel.The fidelity assumption resulted in lower predic-tions of nectar requirement throughout the distri-bution (Online Resource 2, Table S2-24 Nectarrequirement estimates for nectar foragers (D nectar)from the RARI model with explored correlationsand Figs. S2-20 through S2-21).

Assuming high negative correlations betweentrip duration,D i and frequency, T , and fraction oftrip spent flying, F i , resulted in generally lowerestimates of nectar requirement. The mean de-clined from the base case of 75.2 to 46.3 mgnectar/bee/day, and the median declined from43.6 to 29.8 mg nectar/bee/day (Online Resource2, Table S2-24 Nectar requirement estimates fornectar foragers (D nectar) from the RARI modelwith explored correlations and Figs. S2-20through S2-22).

.

3.3. RP model

The RP model predictions for nectar require-ments of nectar foragers are similar to that of theRARI model (Figure 3B and C; Online Resource3, Table S3-1 Nectar requirement estimates fornectar foragers (D nectar) with the Rodney-Purdymodel). The median estimate from the RP modelis 44.4 mg nectar/bee/day. From the RPmodel, themean nectar requirement was 92.4 ± 161 mg nec-tar/bee/day, and the mean time outside the hivewas 1.68 ± 2.94 h. The assumption with the larg-est contribution to variance in predicted nectarrequirement with the RP model is the time outsidethe hive, TO, with a value of 0.49 (Online Re-source 3, Table S3-2 Sensitivity of nectar require-ment estimates for nectar foragers (D nectar) to theRodney and Purdy model inputs). The next largestcontributor to variance is sugar required inside the

hive (S I), with a value of 0.32, followed by thesugar content of nectar (P ), 0.11, and the sugarrequired outside the hive (S O), 0.087. Rankedcorrelation follows this same pattern in terms ofmagnitude.

3.3.1. Sugar required outside the hive, SO

The only input modification that substantiallyaffected predicted nectar requirement was chang-ing the input mean ± 50% (Online Resource 3,Table S3-3 Nectar requirement estimates for nec-tar foragers (D nectar) from the Rodney-Purdymodel with explored assumptions for sugar re-quired outside the hive (S O) and Figs. S3-1through S3-3). The resulting mean range was62.4 to 107 mg nectar/bee/day, and the medianrange was 30.6 to 61.7 mg nectar/bee/day.

The order of contribution to variance and rankcorrelation did not change from the base case forany changes to the input variable, except for thelow mean scenario (Online Resource 3, Table S3-4 Contribution to variance for Rodney-Purdymodel inputs with explored assumptions for sugarrequired outside the hive (S O) and Table S3-5Rank correlation for Rodney-Purdy model inputswith explored assumptions for sugar required out-side the hive (SO)). A lowmean for sugar requiredoutside the hive resulted in sugar required insidethe hive having the highest contribution to vari-ance and the highest rank correlation with thepredicted nectar requirement (over time outsidethe hive).

3.3.2. Sugar required inside the hive, S I

Modifying the distribution for sugar insidethe hive had considerable impact on the pre-dictions of nectar requirement (Online Re-source 3, Nectar requirement estimates fornectar foragers (D nectar) from the Rodney-Purdy model with explored assumptions forsugar required inside the hive (S I) and Figs.S3-4 through S3-6). This was particularly truefor adjustments not only to the mean but alsoto the lower percentiles of nectar requirementwith changes to the standard deviation, changeto Weibull distribution, and implementation ofa lower threshold of 0.1 mg sugar/h. When the

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 16: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

input mean was altered ± 50%, the mean nectarrequirement ranged from 75.9 to 107 mg nec-tar/bee/day, and the corresponding medianranged from 28.0 to 61.7 mg nectar/bee/day.

None of the modifications to the distributionfor sugar required inside the hive impacted theordering of contributions to variance or rank cor-relation of input variables (Online Resource 3,Table S3-7 Contribution to variance for Rodney-Purdy model inputs with explored assumptionsfor sugar required inside the hive (S I) andTable S3-8 Rank correlation for Rodney-Purdymodel inputs with explored assumptions for thesugar required inside the hive (S I)).

3.3.3. Proportion of sugar in nectar, P

Changes to the standard deviation assumptionfor sugar content of nectar, P, had little impact onpredictions of nectar requirement. However,changing the mean to 15% or to 45% from thebase assumption of 30% had a considerable effecton predicted nectar requirements (Figure 5; On-line Resource 3, Table S3-9 Nectar requirementestimates for nectar foragers (D nectar) from theRodney-Purdy model with explored assumptionsfor the sugar content of nectar (P ) and Figs. S3-7and S3-8). In the latter case, mean range was 56.8to 265 mg nectar/bee/day, and the median rangewas 28.4 to 105 mg nectar/bee/day.

No changes to the distribution for sugar innectar, P, affected the order from base case forcontributions to variance or rank correlation ofinput variables (Online Resource 3, Table S3-10Contribution to variance for Rodney-Purdy modelinputs with explored assumptions for the sugarcontent of nectar (P ) and Table S3-11 Rank cor-relation for Rodney-Purdy model inputs with ex-plored assumptions for the sugar content of nectar(P )).

3.3.4. Time outside the hive, TO

Setting a lower threshold of 1 min for timeoutside the hive (TO) had a negligible impact onthe predictions for nectar requirement. However,setting the lower threshold to 30 min decreasedthe variability and generally increased the predic-tions for nectar requirement. Assuming a Weibulldistribution not only led to generally decreasedpredictions through the center of the distributionbut also increased variability in nectar require-ment. Changes to the mean and standard deviationled to notable changes in predicted nectar require-ment. Modifying the mean ± 50% had the mostsignificant impact with a resulting mean range innectar requirement of 61.4 to 124 mg nectar/bee/day and a median range of 29.3 to 62.7 mg nectar/bee/day (Online Resource 3, Table S3-12 Nectarrequirement estimates for nectar foragers (D nectar)

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1 10 100 1000

Percentile

Nectar Requirement (mg nectar/bee/day)

Base Case

Sugar Content of Nectar - High Mean

Sugar Content of Nectar - Low Mean

Reproduction of Agencies' Model

Figure 5. Predicted cumulative distributions of nectar requirement for nectar-foraging honey bees with changes tothe mean assumption for sugar content of nectar (P ) in the RP model.

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 17: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

from the Rodney-Purdy model with explored as-sumptions for the time spent outside the hive (TO)and Figs. S3-9 through S3-11).

For the most part, there were no changes to theorder of contribution to variance or rank correla-tion of input variables across this exploration oftime outside the hive (TO). However, both the lowmean and minimum threshold of 30 min led tosugar requirement overtaking time outside thehive for both contribution to variance and rankcorrelation (Online Resource 3, Table S3-13 Con-tribution to variance for Rodney-Purdy modelinputs with explored assumptions for the timespent outside the hive (TO) and Table S3-14 Rankcorrelation for Rodney-Purdy model inputs withexplored assumptions for the time spent outsidethe hive (TO)).

3.3.5. Multiple changes to RP inputdistributions

We explored the impact of setting all meaninputs to conservatively high values (+ 50% ofestimated mean, − 50% for sugar content). Thisresulted in a major increase from the base case forpredicted nectar requirements, to the point of in-tersection with the Agencies’ predictions but withgreater variability (Online Resource 3, Table S3-15 Nectar requirement estimates for nectar for-agers (D nectar) from the Rodney-Purdy modelwith explored changes to multiple assumptionsand Fig. S3-12). This scenario did not result inany changes to the order of contribution to vari-ance or rank correlation (Online Resource 3, Ta-ble S3-16 Contribution to variance for Rodney-Purdy model inputs with explored changes tomultiple assumptions and Table S3-17 Rank cor-relation for Rodney-Purdy model inputs with ex-plored changes to multiple assumptions).

Subsequently, we changed the above scenarioto also include a lower threshold of 30 min fortime outside the hive, TO. This modification re-sulted in further increases to predictions of nectarrequirement, particularly in the lower portion ofthe distribution. This scenario also resulted in thesugar content of nectar taking top rank in contri-bution to variance and rank correlation withpredictions.

4. DISCUSSION

That the RARI model estimates are similar tothose of the RP model suggests that the differ-ences between the RP model and the Agencies’model are predominantly due to model parame-terization, as opposed to model form. That said,the Agencies’ model specifies seven differentmodel parameters, three of which are varied with-in individual (bee) simulations (D i , F i , and P i ).The RP model achieves similar predictions with asimpler model, with only five variables, suggest-ing that the Agencies’ model is overly complexfor the available data. Further, the fraction of tripspent flying, F i , a parameter found in the Agen-cies’model, is a supposition that is not document-ed to be based on any real-world data. The pro-portion of sugar content of nectar during trip i ,P i ,is also a problematic variable, given the evidencethat foragers take concentrated nectar from thehive to fuel part or all of their foraging trips(Harano and Nakamura 2016). In part, the simi-larity between the predictions of the RARI and RPmodels can be attributed to the fact that many ofthe data used to parameterize the models wereacquired from the same studies.

The marked differences between the RAmodelpredictions and those of the RARI and RP modelscan be primarily attributed to (1) the general lowervariance assigned to the Agencies’ model inputsand (2) the large difference in estimates for timespent outside the hive and foraging.

There were differences and similarities in thecorrelation analyses carried out for the RA andRARI models. When negative correlation was as-sumed between the number of trips T and tripdurationD i , the RAmodel predicted predominant-ly less variability, while the RARI model predictedgenerally lower estimates of nectar requirement,D nectar. This is likely predominantly due to theswitch from assuming trip duration, D i , is uniformto assuming that it is lognormal (Table I). Bothanalyses suggest that when the fraction of trip spentflying, F i , is negatively correlated with the numberof trips per day, T, the correlation has only a smallimpact on estimated nectar requirement, D nectar

(slightly lower). The fidelity assumption with theRA model resulted in greater variability in D nectar

predictions, whereas with the RARI model

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 18: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

predictions were generally lower with similar var-iability. This is also likely related to the switch indistribution from uniform to lognormal for D i .When high negative correlations and fidelity wereassumed concurrently, bothmodels predicted lowerestimates for nectar requirement, D i , but the effectwas more pronounced with the RARI model. Ingeneral, if central point estimates are used in expo-sure assessment with the Agencies’ model, corre-lations may not be of great concern; however, ifprobabilistic exposure estimates are to be used, itmay be important to consider possible correlationsand seek empirical data to support correlationassumptions.

Sensitivity analyses revealed that the mostimportant model inputs in the base cases, interms of contribution to variance and rank cor-relation, are sugar required to meet resting met-abolic rate, S R, in the RARI model (44% con-tribution to variance in the base case), and timeoutside the hive, T O, in the RP model (49%contribution to variance in the base case). No-tably, in the RARI model, trip duration, D i , andnumber of trips, T , contributed a total of 46% tothe variance. Since these variables are the basisfor time outside the hive, we can conclude thatin both models, time outside the hive contribut-ed most to variance. This is followed by sugarrequired for rest, S R, and sugar required insidethe hive, S I, for the RARI and RP models,respectively. These inputs are relatively impor-tant based on contribution to variance and rankcorrelation. This is likely because bees are pre-sumed to be resting most of the time. S R wastreated as a constant in the Agencies’model andcontributed to the much lower variability in theAgencies’ predictions.

Other inputs contribute ≤ 11% to variance inboth models’ estimates. Results of the elastic-ity analyses suggest that plausible changes tomodel input standard deviations, distributionassumptions, and threshold values generallyhave low impact to model predictions for boththe RARI and RP models, with mean andmedian estimates generally resulting in ≤ 26%difference from the base case. Changes tomean estimates for input distribution had thegreatest impact on model predictions and insome ins tances changed the order of

contribution to variance and ranked correla-tions of input variables. In the RARI model,trip duration, D i , replaced sugar required forrest, S R, for top contributor to variance andtop rank correlation when the mean for sugarrequired for flight, S R, or the mean for tripduration, D i , was increased by 50%, and whena 50% reduction in mean was assumed forsugar required for rest, S R. In the RP model,sugar required inside the hive, S R, took topcontributor to variance and top rank correla-tion when a 50% lower mean was assumed forsugar required outside the hive, S O, or timespent outside the hive, T O. Thus, sugar re-quired when not foraging may be a more im-portant variable in the models if bees spendless time outside foraging and if the energydifference between foraging and not foragingis lower. The greatest changes to predictions ofnectar requirement, D nectar, were seen with50% change in the means of sugar content ofnectar in both the RARI and RP models withmean and median predictions ranging from 35to 264% of the base case predictions. In pesti-cide risk assessment of specific crops or cropgroups, uncertainty could be reduced by usingactual nectar sugar content of the treated cropand surrounding vegetation that may havecome in contact with the pesticide. Additionaldata on nectar sugar content of crop plantscould be useful to improve representativenessof the Agencies’ model for risk assessment inUS agroecosystems.

The experiments reported in Dosselli et al.(2016) and Thompson et al. (2016) which wereheavily relied on in the generation of time-relatedinput parameters (T ,D i , and TO) were carried outunder specific site and environmental conditionsin Australia and the UK and may not berepresentative of colonies foraging in USagroecosystems. Potential disparities, withrespect to flight statistics, may stem fromdifferences in climate, available food resources,hive management practices, and colony genetics.Thompson et al. (2016) designed their Lincoln-shire study such that the monitored foragerswould predominantly forage on oilseed rapegrown under typical agricultural conditions. Theaverage temperatures reported in Lincoln during

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 19: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

the months of the study (May and June 2013)were 11 and 14°C, which is lower than would beexpected over the growing season in the contigu-ous USA. The Dosselli et al. (2016) study wasconducted at the University of Western Australia,which is located in Crawley, near Perth. The areais generally urban with nearby parks and botanicalgardens. Also, in neither study was the subspeciesreported. Subspecies and strains of Apis melliferamay display subtle differences in metabolism andforage differently (e.g., Harrison and Fewell 2002;Ibrahim et al. 2010). Additional data capturingintra-individual variability from typical Apisme l l i f e r a f o r a g i n g on ne c t a r i n USagroecosystems would be useful to confirm thatthe data used here are suitably representative.

The use of readily available data from the liter-ature, in particular relatively recent RFID obser-vations, enhances predictions of nectar require-ments for nectar foragers. These data capture thewide variability expected among individuals. Riskassessors should consider available data relevantto dietary intake when estimating honey bee ex-posure via the diet.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Thanks to DwayneMoore for comprehensive reviewof earlier versions of this manuscript.

AUTHOR’S CONTRIBUTIONS

VK conceived the idea, SR carried out the model-ing, andVK reviewed and contributed to revisionsof it. All authors approved the final manuscript.

FUNDING INFORMATION

Funding for this effort was provided by the Polli-nator Research Task Force.

COMPLIANCE WITH ETHICALSTANDARDS

Conflict of interest SR declares that she has no potentialconflict of interest in relation to the study in this paper. VKdeclares that his employer, Corteva Agriscience, developsand markets crop protection products that are subject toagency regulatory approvals that include consideration of

pollinator risk assessments. Corteva Agriscience is a mem-ber of the Pollinator Research Task Force, an internationalconsortium of producers of crop protection products orga-nized for the purpose of improving the science of pollinatorrisk assessment.

OPEN ACCESS

This article is distributed under the terms of theCreative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Li-cense (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/),which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and repro-duction in any medium, provided you give appropriatecredit to the original author(s) and the source, provide alink to the Creative Commons license, and indicate ifchanges were made.

Évaluation probabiliste des besoins en nectar desabeilles qui récoltent le nectar.

probabiliste / modélisation / nectar / Apis mellifera /abeille mellifère / besoin énergétique

Wahrscheinlichkeitsabschätzung der Anforderungenfür Nektar bei Nektarsammlerinnen der Honigbiene.

Wahrscheinlichkeit / Modellierung / Nektar /Anforderungen für Nektarsammlerinnen

REFERENCES

Balderrama, N. M., Almeida, L. O., Núῆez, J. A. (1992).Metabolic rate during foraging in the honeybee. J.Comp. Physiol. B. 162, 440–447.

Butler, C. G., Jefree, E. P. Kalmus H. (1943). The behaviourof a population of honeybees on an artificial and on anatural crop. J. Exp. Biol. 20, 65–73.

Cahill, K., Lustik, S. (1976). Oxygen consumption andthermoregulation in Apis mellifera workers anddrones. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 55A, 355–357.

Chen, J.C.P., Chou, C. C. (1993). Cane Sugar Handbook,12th Edition, AManual for Cane Sugar Manufacturersand their Chemists. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., NewYork, New York. Page 24.

Coelho, J. R., Mitton, J. B. (1988). Oxygen consumptionduring hovering is associated with genetic variation ofenzymes in honey bees. Funct. Ecol. 2(2), 141–146.

Dosselli, R., Grassl, J., Carson, A., Simmons, L. W., Baer,B. (2016). Flight behaviour of honey bee (Apismellifera ) workers is altered by initial infections of

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees

Page 20: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

the fungal parasite Nosema apis . Sci. Rep. 6, 36649;https://doi.org/10.1038/srep36649.

Eckert, C. D., Winston, M. L., Ydenberg, R. C. (1994). Therelationship between population size, amount of brood,and individual foraging behaviour in the honey bee,Apis mellifera L . Oecologia 97, 248–255.

EFSA (European Food Safety Authority). 2013. EFSAGuidance Document on the risk assessment of plantprotection products on bees (Apis mellifera, Bombusspp. and solitary bees). EFSA J 11(7) 3295.

Fahrenholz, L., Lamprecht, I. Schricker, B. (1989). Micro-calorimetric investigations of the energy metabolism ofhoneybee workers, Apis mellifera carnica .Thermochim. Acta 1951, 13–21.

Feuerbacher, E., Fewell, J. H., Roberts, S. P., Smith, E. F.,Harrison, J. F. (2003). Effects of load type (pollen ornectar) and load mass on hovering metabolic rate andmechanical power output in the honey bee Apismellifera . J. Exp. Biol. 206, 1855–1865.

Goller, F. and Esch, H. E. (1991). Oxygen consumption andflight muscle activity during heating in workers anddrones of Apis mellifera. J. Comp. Physiol. B. 161,61–67.

Harano, K., Nakamura, J. (2016). Nectar loads as fuel forcollecting nectar and pollen in honeybees: adjustmentby sugar concentration. J. Comp. Physiol. 202, 435–443.

Harrison, J. F., Fewell, J. H. (2002). Environmental and geneticinfluences on flight metabolic rate in the honey bee, Apismellifera. Comp. Biochem. Phys. A. 133(2), 323–333.

He, X., Wang,W., Qin, Q., Zeng, Z. Zhang S., Barron A. B.(2013). Assessment of flight activity and homing abil-ity in Asian and European honey be species, Apiscerana and Apis mellifera, measured with radio fre-quency tags. Apidologie 44, 38–51.

Headrick, T. C. (2010). Statistical Simulation: PowerMeth-od Polynomials and other Transformations. Chapman& Hall/CRC, Boca Raton, FL.

Heberle, J. A. (1914). Notes from Germany: how manytrips to the field does a bee make in a day? How longdoes it take to fetch a load? How long does a beeremain in the hive between trips? Glean. Bee Cult.42, 904–905.

Heusner, A., Stussi, T. H. (1964). Métabolisme énergétiquede l'abeille isolée: Son role dans la thermorégulation dela ruche. Insectes Sociaux. 11(3),239–265.

Ibrahim, C., Song, D., Mixson, T., Serrano, E., Clement, M.et al; (2010). Foraging Response of Turkish HoneyBee Subspecies to Flower Color Choices and RewardConsistency. J. Insect Behav. 23(2), 100–116.

Knopper, L. D., Dan, T., Reisiq, D. D., Johnson, J. D.,Bowers, L.M. (2016). Sugar concentration in nectar:a quantitative metric of crop attractiveness for refinedpollinator risk assessments. Pest Manag. Sci. 72(10),1807–1812.

Kovac, H., Stabentheiner, A., Hetz, S. K., Petz, M.,Crailsheim, A. (2007). Respiration of resting honeybees. J. Insect Physiol. Behav. 53, 1250–1261.

Lach, L., Kratz, M., Baer B. (2015). Parasitized honey beesare less likely to forage and carry less pollen. J.Invertebr.Pathol. 130, 64–71.

Lundie, A. E. (1925). The Flight Activities of the Honey-bee. United States Department of Agriculture, Depart-ment Bulletin No. 1328. Washington, DC. May, 1925.

Paleari, L., Confalonieri, R. (2016). Sensitivity analysis of asensitivity analysis: We are likely overlooking the im-pact of distribution assumptions. Ecological Modeling.340, 57–63.

Park, W. (1922). Time and labor factors involved in gath-ering pollen and nectar. Am Bee J. 42(6), 254–255.

Perry, C. J., Søvik, E., Myerscough, M. R., Barron, A. B.(2015). Rapid behavioral maturation accelerates failureof stressed honey bee colonies. PNAS 113(30), 3427–34232.

Ribbands, C. R. (1949). The foraging method of individualhoney-bees. J. Anim. Ecol. 18(1), 47–66.

Rodney, S., Purdy, J. This issue. Nectar provisioning andingestion by honey bee foragers, and colony-level foodallocation: A review to support assessment of dietaryexposure to pesticides. Apidologie.

Rortais, A., Arnold, G., Halm, M-P., Touffet-Briens, F.(2005). Modes of honeybee exposure to systemic in-secticides: estimated amounts of contaminated pollenand nectar consumed by different categories of bees.Apidologie 36(1): 71–83.

Rothe, U., Nachtigall, W. (1989). Flight of the honey bee. J.Comp. Physiol. B. 158:739–749.

Southwick, E. E. (1982). Metabolic energy of intact honeybee colonies. Comp. Biochem. Physiol.. 71A, 227–281.

Southwick, E. E. (1983). The honey bee cluster as a ho-meothermic superorganism. Comp. Biochem. Physiol.75A(4), 641–645.

Stabentheiner, A., J. Vollmann, H. Kovac and K.Crailsheim. 2003. Oxygen consumption and body tem-perature of active and resting honeybees. J. InsectPhysiol. 49:881–889.

Thom, C., Seeley, T. D. Tautz, J. (2000). A scientific noteon the dynamics of labor devoted to nectar foraging ina honey bee colony: number of foragers versus indi-vidual foraging activity. Apidologie 31(6), 737–738.

Thompson, H., Coulson, M., Ruddle, N., Wilkins, S.Harkin, S. (2016). Thiamethoxam: assessing flightactivity of honeybees foraging on treated oilseed rapeusing radio frequency identification technology. Envi-ron. Toxicol. Chem. 35(2), 385–393.

Thompson, H., (2018) Personal communication with S.Rodney.

USEPA (US Environmental Protection Agency). (2009).Guidance on the Development, Evaluation, and Appli-cation of Environmental Models., Office of the ScienceAdvisor, Council for Regulatory EnvironmentalModeling, Washington D.C. EPA/100/K-09/003

USEPA (US Environmental Protection Agency), PMRA(Pest Management Regulatory Agency) and CDPR(California Department of Pesticide Regulation).

S. Rodney, V. J. Kramer

Page 21: Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar ...pollinatorresearchtaskforce.com/wp-content/uploads/... · Probabilistic assessment of nectar requirements for nectar-foraging

(2012). White Paper in Support of the Proposed RiskAssessment Process for Bees. Submitted to the FIFRAScientific Advisory Panel for Review and Comment,September 11–14, 2012. Office of Chemical Safetyand Pollution Prevention (US Environmental Protec-tion Agency), Pest Management Regulatory Agency(PMRA) Health Canada, and California Department ofPesticide Regulation (CDPR). http://www.regulations.gov/#!documentDetail;D=EPA-HQ-OPP-2012-0543-0004), Accessed 21 October 2019.

USEPA (US Environmental Protection Agency), PMRA(Pest Management Regulatory Agency) and CDPR(California Department of Pesticide Regulation).(2014). Guidance for Assessing Pesticide Risks to Bees.Office of Pesticide Programs, US Environmental Pro-tection Agency, Washington, DC, Health Canada PestManagement Regulatory Agency, Ottawa, ON, and Cal-ifornia Department of Pesticide Regulation, Sacramento,

CA. https://www.epa.gov/sites/production/files/2014-06/documents/pollinator_risk_assessment_guid-ance_06_19_14.pdf, Accessed 21 October 2019.

Vose, D. (1996). Quantitative risk analysis: A guide toMonte Carlo simulation modeling. John Wiley andSons. Chichester, UK. 328 pp.

Wolf, T. J., Schmid-Hempel, P., Ellington, C. P., Stevenson,R. D. (1989). Physiological correlates of foraging ef-forts in honey-bees: Oxygen consumption and nectarload. Funct. Ecol. 3(4), 417–424.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutralwith regard to jurisdictional claims in published mapsand institutional affiliations.

Probabilistic Assessment of Nectar Requirements for Nectar-Foraging Honey Bees