15

Click here to load reader

Personality & Psychological Contract

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Nikolaou, I., Tomprou, M. & Vakola, M. (2007). Individuals’ Inducements and the Role of Personality: Implications for Psychological Contracts. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 22, 649-663.

Citation preview

Page 1: Personality & Psychological Contract

Individuals’ inducements and therole of personality: implications

for psychological contractsIoannis Nikolaou and Maria Tomprou

Department of Management Science and Technology,Athens University of Economics and Business, Athens, Greece, and

Maria VakolaDepartment of Marketing and Management,

Athens University of Economics and Business, Athens, Greece

Abstract

Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to explore and identify relationships between psychologicalcontract inducements and the five-factor model of personality (FFM) in Greece.

Design/methodology/approach – A survey questionnaire that incorporated measures of intrinsicand extrinsic psychological contract inducements and a Greek personality measure of the FFM wascompleted by 299 respondents. Hierarchical regression analyses were used to explore the hypotheses.

Findings – The paper finds that extroversion and conscientiousness are associated with intrinsic butnot extrinsic psychological contract inducements and that neuroticism is associated with extrinsic butnot intrinsic inducements. The hypothesis pertaining to openness to experience was rejected, becauseit was not associated with intrinsic psychological contract inducements, as expected.

Research limitations/implications – The study design was cross-sectional and used onlyself-report measures. Therefore, it should be cross-validated with different research designs (e.g.longitudinal research) and in other countries.

Practical implications – The findings provide further support on the significance of personalitymeasures for the selection, development and motivation of employees.

Originality/value – The most significant contribution of the study is that it explores therelationship between personality and psychological contract inducements rather than psychologicalcontract types. Another contributing factor is that the study is carried out in Greece.

Keywords Personality measurement, Psychological contracts, Personality, Greece

Paper type Research paper

IntroductionOrganizations are constantly changing, and their employees are expected to go alongwith and adapt quickly and successfully to the changes. A natural consequence oforganizational changes is the transformation of the employment relationship. Thetraditional working relationship is shifting to a “new deal” that is characterized byshort-term contracts and a lack of stability, employability and alternative forms ofworking (for a more detailed description, see Herriot and Pemberton, 1997; Millward

The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at

www.emeraldinsight.com/0268-3946.htm

The authors would like to thank the Associate Editor Hetty van Emmerik and the twoanonymous reviewers for their helpful comments. The order of authorship is alphabetical,indicating the equal contribution of the authors to the production of this article.

Implications forpsychological

contracts

649

Received September 2006Revised April 2007,

June 2007Accepted June 2007

Journal of Managerial PsychologyVol. 22 No. 7, 2007

pp. 649-663q Emerald Group Publishing Limited

0268-3946DOI 10.1108/02683940710820082

Page 2: Personality & Psychological Contract

and Hopkins, 1998). A noticeable sign of the collapse of the traditional employmentrelationship is the decline of trade union membership and the collective values relatedto it (Guest, 2004) and the rise of a more dyadic-level agreement.

Within this context, Rousseau (2001) suggested that there are three main featuresthat reflect the transition of the employment deal: standard, position-based andidiosyncratic. The standard deal reflects the traditional employment agreement, whichis based on legislative or collective rights. The position-based deal also partlyrepresents the old deal because it refers to certain features available to a specific groupof workers. The idiosyncratic deal reflects aspects of the agreement negotiated on amore individualized level (Rousseau, 2001), but it appears to be more salient in thecontemporary work context (Guest, 2004; Rousseau et al., 2006).

The theoretical background for analyzing this new individualized and less explicitdeal is psychological contract theory (Guest, 2004; Rousseau, 1990). The mostacceptable definition of psychological contract, which is also adopted in the currentstudy, is “the beliefs, based upon promises expressed or implied, regarding anexchange agreement between an individual and, in organizations, the employing firmand its agents” (Rousseau, 2004, p. 120).

According to Porter et al. (1998, p. 770), this exchange agreement entails “thecontributions to be offered by employees in return for certain inducements provided bythe employing organization”. March and Simon (1958, p. 84) defined inducements as“the payments by the organizations to the employee, independent of utility”. Alongwith the contributions employees offer employers, they form one of the two main partsof the exchange agreement that constitute psychological contracts. Anderson andSchalk (1998) claimed that March and Simon’s (1958) inducement-contribution model isone of the main social psychology theories on exchange relationships, which shaped asignificant characteristic of psychological contracts: the exchange relationshipbetween the employer and the employee.

Two lines of thought exist regarding this exchange relationship: a broad approach,in which we compare the expectations and obligations of different levels (organizationand individual), and a narrow approach, which conceives of the psychological contractas the employee’s individual perceptions of the exchange relationship. Rousseau andTijoriwala (1998) suggested that by definition, the psychological contract is anindividual perception. Individual perceptions can define what the psychologicalcontract is and therefore can be influenced by individual characteristics, such aspersonality traits (Rousseau, 2001, 2004; Rousseau and Tijoriwala, 1998). Thesubjective nature of the psychological contract implies that personality should have aneffect on employees’ perceptions of their employers’ inducements, thus affecting thedevelopment, formation, maintenance and breach or violation of their psychologicalcontract. Porter et al. (1998) also suggested that the issue of differential perceptions ofinducements has been highly neglected. Therefore, our focus in the present paper is onindividual perceptions of the significance of various inducements and the role of thefive-factor model of personality (FFM) on these perceptions.

Types of inducementsKickul and Lester (2001) suggested that a common theme appears between thedifferent conceptualizations of psychological contract inducements: a focus on theintrinsic characteristics of the job (e.g. challenging and meaningful work, increased

JMP22,7

650

Page 3: Personality & Psychological Contract

participation in decision making, advancement opportunities) and a focus on theextrinsic features of the job (e.g. health benefits, reasonable workload, secure workenvironment).

In this study, we adopt a distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic inducements.Porter et al. (1998) suggested that the content of the psychological contract centers onthe contributions employees offer in return for certain inducements provided by theirorganizations. Because we were interested in exploring how personality characteristicsinfluence the significance that individuals attribute to various motives provided byemployers, we adopted the intrinsic – extrinsic inducements distinction, rather thanthe relational-transactional features of the psychological contract, because we believethat the former provides employees with a better description of their employers’offerings, whereas the latter is more strongly related to the type of the psychologicalcontract employees adopt.

The role of personality on prioritizing psychological contract inducementsThe profusion of research and meta-analyses in the FFM (Barrick and Mount, 1991;Barrick et al., 2001; Mount et al., 1998) sheds light on employees’ and prospectemployees’ preferences of inducements. The five factors, usually labeled neuroticism,extroversion, openness to experience, agreeableness and conscientiousness (Costa andMcCrae, 1992), have provided personality psychology with a clear measurementframework and are responsible for the resurgence of interest in personality researchand practice. These five factors have been identified across several cultures andradically different languages, providing further support for the existence of the FFMand its universal application (Saucier et al., 2000).

As discussed previously, the idiosyncratic feature of the employment relationshiptends to overshadow the other more tangible features of the relationship. Therefore,researchers and practitioners need to focus on the more flexible and less explicitaspects of the employment agreement. This observation calls for a closer examinationof the influence of personality dispositions on the inducement priorities. To ourknowledge, two empirical studies that examine the impact of personality theory on thepsychological contract theories have been conducted (i.e. Ho et al., 2004; Raja et al.,2004). In short, Raja et al. (2004) explored the association between employee personalityand psychological contract type and between perceptions of contract breach andemotional reactions of contract violation. Employing the experimental method, Ho et al.(2004) examined the influence of personality features on individuals’ reactions tobroken promises. These two studies focused mainly on the effect of personality traitson broken promises and agreements. However, how individuals with differentialpersonality attributes prioritize psychological contract inducements remains an openissue. Therefore, the current research aims to explore the association between thepersonality attributes and the potential psychological contract inducements. In thefollowing paragraphs, we briefly describe the dimensions of the FFM and explain ourexpectations with regard to their relationship to psychological contract inducements.

Extroversion is related to the experience of positive emotions and is closely linkedwith increased social activity and more rewarding social relationships (Watson andClark, 1997). Extroverted individuals enjoy being with people, attending social eventsand being adventurous; are assertive, frank, sociable and talkative; and make friendseasily; in contrast, introverts tend to be quiet, reserved, shy and unsociable (Costa and

Implications forpsychological

contracts

651

Page 4: Personality & Psychological Contract

McCrae, 1992). Extroverts are bold and easily state their opinion, seeking excitement toalleviate boredom. In a study that explored the relationship between Eysenck’s (1967)three-factor model of personality and Herzberg’s (Herzberg et al., 1959)two-dimensional work motivation theory, Furnham et al. (1999) concluded thatextroverts are intrinsically motivated and reported higher correlations with motivatorsthan with hygiene factors. Extroverted individuals prefer being involved in manyactivities and seek out working environments that value praise and influence at work,where they are encouraged by reminders of potential rewards commensurate withcompetence performance (Furnham et al., 1999). Raja et al. (2004) suggested thatextroverts tend to seek long-term working relationships that will provide them withincreased opportunities to gain status, power or recognition. Thus:

H1. Extroverted individuals will consider intrinsic contract inducements moreimportant than extrinsic contract inducements.

Neuroticism refers to a lack of positive psychological adjustment and emotionalstability. Individuals who score high on neuroticism are more likely to experiencestress proneness, personal insecurity, irritability, bad moods and so forth (Costa andMcCrae, 1992). Anxiety leads them to prefer low-stress tasks with well-defined jobresponsibilities and low workload. Research exploring the relationship betweenneuroticism and various employee attitudes has demonstrated that high neuroticism isrelated to increased job dissatisfaction, low morale, high turnover and withdrawalintentions, a lack of commitment and so on. As a result, Bozionelos (2004) suggestedthat neurotic individuals tend not to be goal or career oriented, and therefore we expectthat they will also be more attracted to extrinsic contract inducements because they donot seek a long-term investment with an organization. In a cross-cultural studyexploring the link between personality and Herzberg’s (Herzberg et al., 1959) workmotivation theory, Furnham et al. (2005) have shown that neurotic individuals rateextrinsic work values and hygiene factors as more significant than motivators (see alsoFurnham et al., 1999). Raja et al. (2004) have also shown that neurotic individuals tendto create transactional contracts. Thus:

H2. Neurotic individuals will consider extrinsic contract inducements moreimportant than intrinsic contract inducements.

Openness to experience is defined by intellection, creativity, unconventionality andbroad-mindedness (Barrick et al., 2001). Open-minded individuals, who areimaginative, inventive, creative, curious and unconventional (Costa and McCrae,1992), tend to be inherently interested in new ideas. These individuals areunconventional (Costa and McCrae, 1992), and curiosity is an internal motive thatleads them to prefer jobs that fulfill their needs for gaining experience. High scores onopenness are an indication of individuals who love to play with ideas, are open-minded,are eager to try new activities, are adventurous and detest routines. Their quest forexperimentation, excitement and variety directs them to positions with varied jobduties and increased responsibilities, which enable them to take full advantage of theirintellectual capacities. Their source of happiness at work is not a result of thefulfillment of the extrinsic characteristics of the job, such as salary, benefits and jobsecurity, which they tend to underestimate (Furnham et al., 2005). Thus:

JMP22,7

652

Page 5: Personality & Psychological Contract

H3. Individuals who are open to experiences will consider intrinsic contractinducements more important than extrinsic contract inducements.

Conscientiousness has been repeatedly identified as the best predictor of successful jobperformance across jobs and even continents (Barrick and Mount, 1991; Salgado, 1997).It is related to an individual’s degree of self-control and need for achievement, orderand persistence (Costa and McCrae, 1992). The dimension of conscientiousness is ameasure of how competent, dutiful, orderly, responsible and thorough a person is(Costa and McCrae, 1992) and is a term bound with self-discipline and deliberation.Costa and McCrae (1998) viewed achievement striving as a basic facet ofconscientiousness. People who are high in conscientiousness are more likely to setgoals, have higher expectations that their efforts will result in favorable consequences,and believe that they can do more (have higher self-efficacy) than those who are low inconscientiousness. Therefore, we expect that they will underestimate the extrinsiccharacteristics of a job, which will prevent them from demonstrating their ability toperform well and succeed at their work. Raja et al. (2004) claimed that highlyconscientious individuals are attracted to jobs in which they will be able to formlong-term exchange relationships, with greater opportunities for achievement andsuccess; however, as a result, they might be “offered” more enduring relational contractterms because of their proclivity for high performance and job satisfaction. Furnhamet al. (2005) have also shown that conscientious individuals rate intrinsic work valueshigher. Thus:

H4. Conscientious individuals will consider intrinsic contract inducements moreimportant than extrinsic contract inducements.

Finally, agreeableness describes people who are compliant, soft-hearted and goodnatured; avoid tensions and disagreements in the workplace; are not jealous; and aremild and gentle (Costa and McCrae, 1985). Agreeable people display altruism, care, andlove to provide emotional support and are considerate to others. They rely on their ownintegrity as well as others’, and their ability to trust and care for people enables them toform deep relationships. A lack of agreeableness is expressed as competitiveness andimpatience (Costa and McCrae, 1992). However, because previous research has notprovided a clear-cut direction of the kind of psychological contract inducements towhich agreeable individuals are prone, we do not provide a specific researchhypothesis regarding this inducement.

A final contributing factor of the current study is the country in which it wasconducted. More specifically, we explored the aforementioned hypotheses in Greece, acountry-member of the European Union, with a central role in the financial andbusiness development of south-eastern Europe. Psychological contract is still relativelyunknown in Greece among both researchers and practitioners. We identified only onepublished study (Bellou, 2007) that explored the features of the psychological contractin Greece in mergers and acquisitions. Several other studies have explored the role ofpersonality in occupational settings in Greece (e.g. Nikolaou and Robertson, 2001;Vakola et al., 2004), and Furnham et al.’s (2005) work was a cross-cultural study thatexplored the relationship between personality and work values in the United Kingdomand Greece, a theme related to the topic of the current study. Therefore, the currentstudy attempts to fill in this gap.

Implications forpsychological

contracts

653

Page 6: Personality & Psychological Contract

MethodSample and procedureIn total, there were 290 participants, of which 199 (68.6 percent) were undergraduate orpostgraduate full-time students without work experience and 91 (31.4 percent) wereexecutive MBA students who worked full-time. The undergraduate studentsparticipated in exchange for course credit, and the postgraduate and the MBAstudents participated voluntarily. Because work experience could be a significantmoderator, we carried out a series of independent sample t-tests of the key variablesunder investigation (i.e. psychological contract inducements and personality dimensions)without identifying any statistically significant differences between students with andwithout work experience. As a result, we treated working and nonworking students asone sample. Of the sample, 165 (57 percent) were women, with a mean age of 25.3 years(SD ¼ 6:6). Half the participants completed the personality measure first and thepsychological contracts inducements measure second, and the other half completed themeasures in the reverse order. The researchers informed the participants aboutconfidentiality issues and that they had the right to withdraw from the administration atany time and any stage. The questionnaires were distributed toward the end of class togive the participants enough time to complete the measures. On completion, eachparticipant returned the completed questionnaire to the researcher in a sealed envelope.

MeasuresThe psychological contract inducements measure. An extensive search of thepsychological contract literature identified only one measure that described intrinsicand extrinsic psychological contract inducements, developed by Kickul (2001). Kickuland Lester (2001) ran a confirmatory factor analysis in an attempt to promote thedistinction between extrinsic and intrinsic outcomes but with vague results. Thus, wecarried out a maximum likelihood with oblimin rotation exploratory factor analysis ofthe initial 23-items measure, but the results were also unclear. Therefore, we repeatedthe analysis including only the contract inducements with the highest factor loadings(.0.50) and without secondary loadings, which resulted in a two-factor solution. Wecarried out these analyses using only the subsample of the executive MBA students, tocarry out a confirmatory factor analysis with the remaining sample. We selected 14contract inducements with the highest factor loadings:

Extrinsic contract inducements:

(1) Retirement benefits.

(2) Health care benefits.

(3) Job security.

(4) Well-defined responsibilities.

(5) A reasonable workload.

(6) Safe work environment.

Intrinsic contract inducements:

(1) Challenging and interesting work.

(2) Meaningful work.

JMP22,7

654

Page 7: Personality & Psychological Contract

(3) Participation in decision making.

(4) Freedom to be creative.

(5) A job that provides autonomy and control.

(6) Continual professional training.

(7) Career guidance and mentoring.

(8) Tuition reimbursement.

Eight items loaded on the intrinsic dimension and six loaded on the extrinsicdimension. Next, we carried out a confirmatory factor analysis using the remainingsample to confirm the factor structure of the adopted measure. The following indiceswere used: an absolute index; the chi-square/degrees of freedom (x2/df), in whichfigures less than 4.0 (Bollen, 1989) indicate acceptable fit; and four relativegoodness-of-fit indices – the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI; Tucker and Lewis, 1973), thenormed fit index (NFI; Bentler and Bonett, 1980), and the comparative fit index (CFI;Bentler, 1990), in which values higher than 0.90 indicate a model with good fit, and theroot mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Browne and Cudeck, 1993), inwhich values less than 0.1 indicate a model with good fit. The perspective results werewithin the expected range, with the exception of the TLI and NFI, which failedmarginally (x2/df ¼ 1:91, TLI ¼ 0:89, NFI ¼ 0:84, CFI ¼ 0:92, and RMSEA ¼ 0:07).Participants were asked to indicate the inducements they considered most importantby using a Likert-type scale that ranged from not at all important (1) to extremelyimportant (5). Scales were created by the sum of items. The alphas reliabilities were0.78 and 0.82 for the extrinsic and the intrinsic factors, respectively, for the wholesample.

We translated the original measure by Kickul (2001) into Greek, and then twobilingual occupational psychologists back-translated it into English. Differencesbetween the original English and the back-translated version were discussed, andmutual agreements were made as to the most appropriate translation. This proceduretries to balance the competing needs of making the translation meaningful andnaturally readable to native participants with the need to preserve the integrity of theoriginal measure and its constructs (Brislin, 1980); it has also been regarded as acompromise between completely “etic” and “emic” approaches to cross-culturalpsychology (Church, 2001).

Personality measure. We assessed the FFM using the Traits PersonalityQuestionnaire 5 (TPQue5; see Tsaousis and Kerpelis, 2004). The TPQue5 is a Greekmeasure of the FFM. It is a short version (101 items) of the TPQue (Tsaousis, 1999) andcomprises scales of neuroticism, extroversion, openness, agreeableness, andconscientiousness. TPQue5 has been used in several studies with Greek adults andhas demonstrated excellent psychometric properties (e.g. Furnham et al., 2005; Vakolaet al., 2004). The alpha reliabilities in the current study ranged from 0.77 to 0.87.

ResultsTable I lists the means, standard deviations, reliabilities and intercorrelations for thevariables. The correlation matrix provided some initial support for our hypotheses.Two personality variables were positively correlated with the extrinsic inducementsfactor; that is, neurotic and agreeable participants rated the extrinsic contract items

Implications forpsychological

contracts

655

Page 8: Personality & Psychological Contract

MS

Da

23

45

67

1.E

xtr

insi

c22

.50

4.58

0.78

0.17

9*

*2

0.09

20.

194

**

20.

040

0.17

3*

*2

0.01

72.

Intr

insi

c35

.29

5.90

0.82

0.27

4*

*2

0.06

90.

043

20.

035

0.20

6*

*

3.E

xtr

over

sion

51.5

88.

260.

832

0.29

4*

*0.

198

*0.

192

0.16

0*

*

4.N

euro

tici

sm42

.83

9.78

0.87

20.

023

0.04

82

0.12

4*

5.O

pen

nes

sto

exp

erie

nce

50.9

68.

430.

780.

180

**

20.

063

6.A

gre

eab

len

ess

50.3

06.

990.

772

0.02

17.

Con

scie

nti

ousn

ess

51.1

77.

720.

80

Notes:

*p,

0.05

;*

*p,

0.01

Table I.Means, standarddeviations, reliabilitiesand correlations(N ¼ 290)

JMP22,7

656

Page 9: Personality & Psychological Contract

highly. Furthermore, two other personality dimensions were positively associated withthe intrinsic factor; that is, extroverted and conscientious participants rated intrinsiccontract outcomes highly. There were no clear associations between openness toexperience and the psychological contract inducement scales.

To test our hypotheses, we performed separate hierarchical regression analyses foreach component of the psychological contract inducement factors and each personalityvariable (see Table II). The hypotheses predicted that extroversion, openness toexperience and conscientiousness would demonstrate stronger associations withintrinsic psychological inducements and that neuroticism would demonstrate strongerassociations with extrinsic psychological inducements. To investigate thesepredictions, we regressed each of the psychological contract inducements scales oneach of the FFM dimensions. Most of the expected associations were confirmed;extroversion (b ¼ 0:27, p , 0.000) and conscientiousness (b ¼ 0:21, p , 0.000) wereclearly associated with the intrinsic but not the extrinsic inducements (b ¼ 20:09, n/s;b ¼ 20:02, n/s, respectively), and neuroticism was associated with the extrinsicinducements (b ¼ 0:19, p , 0.01) but not with the intrinsic inducements (b ¼ 20:07,n/s). Our hypothesis regarding openness to experience was rejected, because it was notassociated with intrinsic psychological contract inducements, as expected. Finally,contrary to our expectations, agreeableness was associated with extrinsic inducements(b ¼ 0:17, p , 0.010).

DiscussionThe main purpose of this study was to explore the role of personality dispositions onthe prioritization of psychological contract inducements, because one of the mainfeatures of psychological contracts is its subjective nature. We carried out this study inGreece, in one of the first attempts to explore the features and characteristics ofpsychological contracts in this country. Our data supported three of the fourhypotheses. Extroversion and conscientiousness were positively associated withintrinsic psychological contract inducements, and neuroticism was positively

Predictors B S.E. B b Adjusted R 2

FDependent variable: intrinsic inducementsExtroversion 0.20 0.04 0.27 * * 23.37 * * 0.07Neuroticism 20.04 0.03 20.07 1.35 0.00Openness to experience 0.03 0.04 0.04 0.54 0.00Conscientiousness 0.16 0.04 0.21 * * 12.76 * * 0.04Agreeableness 20.03 0.05 20.03 0.34 0.00

F (1,289)Dependent variable: extrinsic inducementsExtroversion 20.05 0.03 20.09 2.48 0.00Neuroticism 0.09 0.03 0.19 * 11.27 * 0.03Openness to experience 20.02 0.03 20.04 0.45 0.00Conscientiousness 20.01 0.03 20.02 0.08 0.00Agreeableness 0.11 0.04 0.17 * 8.89 * 0.03

Notes: * p , 0.01; * * p , 0.001

Table II.Regression of the five

variables of the FFM onboth psychological

contract inducementfactors (N ¼ 290)

Implications forpsychological

contracts

657

Page 10: Personality & Psychological Contract

associated with extrinsic psychological contract inducements, suggesting thatpersonality characteristics captured by the FFM are useful indicators of employees’perceptions of psychological contracts.

Most psychological contract theorists argue that individual dispositions play apivotal role in the formation, management and breach of psychological contracts (DeVos et al., 2005; Morrison and Robinson, 1997; Rousseau, 1995, 2001, 2004). Building onthe current study’s findings, and combined with Raja et al.’s (2004) results, we can go astep further and argue that individuals who are attracted more by intrinsicinducements may create relational contracts, whereas those who prefer extrinsicinducements may be more prone to transactional contracts because, in both cases, thecontent is similar.

Extroverted and conscientious individuals appear to prefer intrinsic inducements.In terms of extroversion, this finding is consistent with Furnham et al.’s (2005) researchon personality and work values. Extroverts seem to be attracted to jobs not where theyare rewarded for their short-term investment in or tenure with the organization butrather where they are attracted to the specific characteristics of the job content, such asthose expressed by the intrinsic psychological contract inducements. Raja et al. (2004)did not support Furnham et al.’s hypothesis that extroverted employees would formrelational contracts but showed that extroversion was negatively related totransactional contracts. Our finding of the positive association between extroversionand relational-like psychological contracts, such as the intrinsic contract inducements,is an indicator of the need for further examination on this issue. Conscientiousness isalso positively related to intrinsic contract inducements, in confirmation of H4. Thisfinding is congruent with Raja et al.’s (2004) assumption that conscientious individualsform relational contracts. They claimed that conscientious individuals seekadvancement opportunities and personal growth in their career choices, followed byrespective behaviors that are valued highly by their supervisors (Rousseau, 2004).Conscientious individuals concentrate on goal accomplishment to accelerate in theirorganization, and they can only succeed in their attempt if they focus on the internalcharacteristics of their work, as is the case for the intrinsic psychological contractinducements.

According to H2, neuroticism is associated with extrinsic psychological contractinducements. Our results supported this expectation, confirming the results ofprevious studies on personality (Furnham et al., 1999; Furnham et al., 2005) andpsychological contract theory (Raja et al., 2004); that is, neurotic individuals are moreprone to transactional-like inducements. Individuals who are high in neuroticism valuemore highly the job features that, when satisfied, reduce their anxiety and irritability(Costa and McCrae, 1992), such as job security and benefits. Neurotic individuals arenot concerned with “going the extra mile” – a key issue for relational contracting.Rather, they are more interested in keeping away any concerns in the form ofadditional job responsibilities that may increase their stress levels. Neuroticism hasbeen repeatedly linked with decreased job satisfaction and negative employee attitudes(Furnham and Zacherl, 1986; Tokar and Subich, 1997). Neurotic individualsdemonstrate a lack of goal and career orientation (Bozionelos, 2004), and as a result,they are more attracted by the extrinsic features of a job.

With regard to openness to experience, contrary to our expectations and previousfindings, the results of this study did not support the existence of a positive association

JMP22,7

658

Page 11: Personality & Psychological Contract

between openness to experience and intrinsic psychological contract inducements. Weexpected that the imaginative, adventurous features of the individuals who are open toexperience would lead them to assess favorably the job characteristics that will enablethem to take full advantage of their creative personality. Nevertheless, openness toexperience remains the most controversial dimension of the FFM. It includes severaldiverse components, leading Hough (2003) to describe it as the most amorphous andheterogeneous dimension of the FFM. As a result, openness to experience demonstratesthe weakest association with individual outcomes in organizations, such as employees’work performance and attitudes.

Finally, note that despite our expectations, agreeableness demonstrated a positiverelationship to extrinsic psychological contract inducements. In a meta-analysisexploring the personality – job performance relationship, Tett et al. (1991) identifiedagreeableness as a strong predictor of successful job performance when a confirmatory(i.e. theoretically driven) approach is followed. Nevertheless, psychological contractinducements are not features of performance at work. Rather, we argue that they entailaspects of the motivational drives of an individual. Judge and Ilies (2002) identified astrong negative association between agreeableness and goal-setting motivation andexplained it as the tendency of agreeable individuals to set less ambitious performancegoals because they are motivated more by communion (a desire to be part of acommunity) than by agency (a desire to achieve mastery). These results may partiallyexplain the findings of the current study on agreeableness.

The findings of this study also provide rich implications for theory and practice. Forpractitioners, these results offer fruitful guidelines for selecting, managing andrewarding employees. Personality tests have been widely used in personnel selection(Rothstein and Goffin, 2006) as a tool to predict future job performance andperson-organization fit (Nikolaou, 2003). The results of the current and other similarstudies suggest that personality tests may also be used to predict psychologicalcontract inducements. If systematic differences in the psychological contractinducements that different people value and pursue indeed exist, it would be usefulfor managers to pay more attention to employees’ personality dispositions to gain abalanced and healthy psychological contract. Respectively, if extroverted andconscientious individuals seek out intrinsic psychological contract inducementswhereas neurotic job applicants pursue extrinsic psychological contract inducements,managers and supervisors may need to fulfill their psychological contracts differentlyfor different groups of employees. For example, in contrast to extroverted orconscientious employees, neurotic individuals may not consider a promotion orincreased decision-making opportunities a significant contract fulfillment.

With regard to research implications, this study contributes to the domains ofpersonality theory and psychological contract theory. According to Raja et al. (2004),knowledge of the dark side of the role of personality dispositions on organizationalbehavior has just begun, and the current research facilitates this process. From thepsychological contract perspective, this study provides a clear-cut distinction amongcontract inducements, supporting the subjective nature of psychological contract andhow it can be influenced by personal factors. Nevertheless, further research shouldexplore the effect of personality dispositions, such as those expressed by the FFM andothers (e.g. proactive personality), on psychological contracts and especially duringtheir early development stage of psychological contract creation. Further research

Implications forpsychological

contracts

659

Page 12: Personality & Psychological Contract

should also employ the use of longitudinal data because they can provide fruitfulinformation about the causalities of personality differences and preferences on contractinducements and contract type. Greater attention should be paid to agreeablenessbecause it is a highly neglected disposition that can contribute to the dynamics ofpsychological contract formation and violation (Ho et al., 2004).

This study is not devoid of methodological problems. We obtained all the data usinga one-shot questionnaire methodology, and it is often argued that common methodvariance rather than causal links may explain some of the relationships identified. Inthis case, the correlation among the measures will be higher than it ideally should bebecause participants will apply the same biases to each task. However, the differentpattern and direction of results observed across the variables of the study suggest thatcommon method bias is an unlikely explanation for the results. In reviewing the effectsof shared method variance on organizational research, Spector (1987, 2006) concludedthat it is largely mythical, reaching the status of urban legend. He argued thatwell-developed measures with sound psychometric properties were free of thisproblem. In the current study, we took all the measures from previous studies withsound psychometric properties. Furthermore, even if shared method variance exists,there is no reason to expect that the differences in correlations among our variables aredue to its effect, because its presence would not be expected to exert differential bias onthe observed relationships.

In conclusion, this study demonstrated further the significance of personality and,more specifically, the FFM on an important topic for personality and appliedpsychology – namely, psychological contracts. Although readers may not argue aboutthe causal explanations of the results because of the research design adopted, thefindings offer significant insights into understanding the role of individual differenceson psychological contract inducements.

References

Anderson, N. and Schalk, R. (1998), “The psychological contract in retrospect and prospect”,Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 19, pp. 637-47.

Barrick, M.R. and Mount, M.K. (1991), “The big five personality dimensions and job performance:a meta-analysis”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 4, pp. 1-26.

Barrick, M.R., Mount, M.K. and Judge, T.A. (2001), “Personality and performance at thebeginning of the new millennium: what do we know and where do we go next?”,International Journal of Selection and Assessment, Vol. 9, pp. 9-30.

Bellou, V. (2007), “Psychological contract assessment after a major organizational change: thecase of mergers and acquisitions”, International Journal of Human Resource Management,Vol. 29, pp. 68-88.

Bentler, P.M. (1990), “Comparative fit indexes in structural models”, Psychological Bulletin,Vol. 107, pp. 238-46.

Bentler, P.M. and Bonett, D.G. (1980), “Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis ofcovariance structures”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 88, pp. 588-606.

Bollen, K. (1989), “A new incremental fit index for general structural equation models”,Sociometry Methods Research, Vol. 17, pp. 303-16.

Bozionelos, N. (2004), “The big five of personality and work involvement”, Journal of ManagerialPsychology, Vol. 19, pp. 69-81.

JMP22,7

660

Page 13: Personality & Psychological Contract

Brislin, R.W. (1980), “Translation and content analysis of oral and written material”, inTriandis, H.C. and Berry, J.W. (Eds), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol. 1,Allyn & Bacon, Boston, MA, pp. 389-444.

Browne, M.W. and Cudeck, R. (1993), “Alternative ways of assessing model fit”, in Bollen, K.A.and Long, J.S. (Eds), Testing Structural Equation Modes, Sage, Newbury Park, CA, p. CA.

Church, A.T. (2001), “Personality measurement in cross-cultural perspective”, Journal ofPersonality, Vol. 69, pp. 979-1006.

Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1985), The NEO Personality Inventory Manual, PsychologicalAssessment Resources, Odessa.

Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1992), Neo PI-R Professional Manual, Psychological AssessmentResources, Odessa.

Costa, P.T. and McCrae, R.R. (1998), “Trait theories of personality”, in Barone, D.F. andHersen, M. (Eds), Advanced Personality, The Plenum Series in Social/Clinical Psychology,Plenum Press, New York, NY, pp. 103-21.

De Vos, A., Buyens, D. and Schalk, R. (2005), “Making sense of a new employment relationship:psychological contract-related information seeking and the role of work values and locusof control”, International Journal of Selection and Assessment, Vol. 13, pp. 41-52.

Eysenck, H.J. (1967), The Biological Basis of Personality, Thomas Publishing, Springfield, IL.

Furnham, A., Forde, L. and Ferrari, K. (1999), “Personality and work motivation”, Personality andIndividual Differences, Vol. 26, pp. 1035-43.

Furnham, A., Petrides, K.V., Tsaousis, I., Pappas, K. and Garrod, D. (2005), “A cross-culturalinvestigation into the relationships between personality traits and work values”, TheJournal of Psychology, Vol. 139, pp. 5-32.

Furnham, A. and Zacherl, M. (1986), “Personality and job satisfaction”, Personality andIndividual Differences, Vol. 7, pp. 453-9.

Guest, D. (2004), “The psychology of employment relationship: an analysis based onpsychological contract”, Applied Psychology: An International Review, Vol. 53, pp. 541-55.

Herriot, P. and Pemberton, C. (1997), “Facilitating new deals”, Human Resource Management,Vol. 7, pp. 45-56.

Herzberg, F., Mausner, B. and Snyderman, B. (1959), The Motivation to Work, Wiley, New York,NY.

Ho, V.T., Weingart, L.R. and Rousseau, D.M. (2004), “Responses to broken promises: doespersonality matter?”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 65, pp. 276-93.

Hough, L. (2003), “Emerging trends and needs in personality research and practice: beyond maineffects”, in Barrick, M.R. and Ryan, A.M. (Eds), Personality and Work, Jossey-Bass,San Francisco, CA, pp. 289-325.

Judge, T.A. and Ilies, R. (2002), “Relationship of personality to performance motivation: ameta-analytic review”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 87, pp. 797-807.

Kickul, J. (2001), “When organizations break their promises: employee reactions to unfairprocesses and treatment”, Journal of Business Ethics, Vol. 29, pp. 289-307.

Kickul, J. and Lester, S.W. (2001), “Broken promises: equity sensitivity as a moderator betweenpsychological contract breach and employee attitudes and behavior”, Journal of Businessand Psychology, Vol. 16, pp. 191-216.

March, J.G. and Simon, H.A. (1958), Organizations, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY.

Millward, L.J. and Hopkins, L.J. (1998), “Organizational commitment and the psychologicalcontract”, Journal of Social and Applied Psychology, Vol. 28, pp. 16-31.

Implications forpsychological

contracts

661

Page 14: Personality & Psychological Contract

Morrison, E.W. and Robinson, S.L. (1997), “When employees feel betrayed, a model of howpsychological contract violation develops”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 22,pp. 226-56.

Mount, M.K., Barrick, M.R. and Stewart, G.L. (1998), “Five-factor model of personality andperformance in jobs involving interpersonal interactions”, Human Performance, Vol. 11,pp. 145-65.

Nikolaou, I. (2003), “Fitting the person to the organisation: examining the personality-jobperformance relationship from a new perspective”, Journal of Managerial Psychology,Vol. 18, pp. 639-48.

Nikolaou, I. and Robertson, I.T. (2001), “The five-factor model of personality and work behaviourin Greece”, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 10, pp. 161-86.

Porter, L.W., Pearce, J.L., Tripoli, A.M. and Lewis, K.M. (1998), “Differential perceptions ofemployers’ inducements: implications for psychological contracts”, Journal ofOrganizational Behaviour, Vol. 19, pp. 769-82.

Raja, U., Johns, G. and Ntalianis, F. (2004), “The impact of personality on psychologicalcontracts”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 47, pp. 350-67.

Rothstein, M.G. and Goffin, R.D. (2006), “The use of personality measures in personnel selection:what does current research support?”, Human Resource Management Review, Vol. 16,pp. 155-80.

Rousseau, D.M. (1990), “New hire perceptions of their own and employer’s obligations: a study ofpsychological contracts”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 11, pp. 389-400.

Rousseau, D.M. (1995), Psychological Contracts in Organizations: Understanding Written andUnwritten Agreements, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.

Rousseau, D.M. (2001), “The idiosyncratic deal: fexibility versus fairness?”, OrganizationalDynamics, Vol. 29, pp. 260-73.

Rousseau, D.M. (2004), “Research briefs”, Academy of Management Executive, Vol. 18, pp. 120-7.

Rousseau, D.M. and Tijoriwala, S.A. (1998), “Assessing psychological contracts, issues,alternatives and types of measures”, Journal of Organizational Behaviour, Vol. 19,pp. 679-95.

Rousseau, D.M., Ho, V.T. and Greenberg, J.I-d.e. (2006), “I-deals: idiosyncratic terms inemployment relationships”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 31, pp. 977-94.

Salgado, J.F. (1997), “The five factor model of personality and job performance in the EuropeanCommunity”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 82, pp. 30-43.

Saucier, G., Hampson, S.E. and Goldberg, L.R. (2000), “Cross-language studies of lexicalpersonality factors”, in Hampson, S.E. (Ed.), Advances in Personality Psychology, Vol. 1,Routledge, London, pp. 1-36.

Spector, P. (1987), “Method variance as an artifact in self-reported affect and perceptions at work:myth or significant problem?”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 72, pp. 438-43.

Spector, P.E. (2006), “Method variance in organizational research: truth or urban legend?”,Organizational Research Methods, Vol. 9, pp. 221-32.

Tett, R., Jackson, D. and Rothstein, M. (1991), “Personality measures as predictors of jobperformance: a meta-analytic review”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 44, pp. 703-42.

Tokar, D.M. and Subich, L. (1997), “Relative contributions of congruence and personalitydimensions to job satisfaction”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 50, pp. 482-91.

Tsaousis, I. (1999), “The traits personality questionnaire (TPQUE): a Greek measure for the fivefactor model”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 26, pp. 271-83.

JMP22,7

662

Page 15: Personality & Psychological Contract

Tsaousis, I. and Kerpelis, P. (2004), “The Traits Personality Questionnaire 5 (TEXAP5):psychometric evaluation of the short version of a personality test”, European Journal ofPsychological Assessment, Vol. 20, pp. 180-91.

Tucker, L. and Lewis, C. (1973), “A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factoranalysis”, Psychometrika, Vol. 38, pp. 1-10.

Vakola, M., Tsaousis, I. and Nikolaou, I. (2004), “The role of emotional intelligence andpersonality variables on attitudes toward organizational change”, Journal of ManagerialPsychology, Vol. 19, pp. 88-110.

Watson, D. and Clark, L.A. (1997), “Measurement and mismeasurement of mood: recurrent andemergent issues”, Journal of Personality Assessment, Vol. 68, pp. 267-96.

About the authorsIoannis Nikolaou is an Organizational Psychologist and Lecturer in Organisational Behaviour atAthens University of Economics and Business. He received his PhD in OrganizationalPsychology from Manchester School of Management, UK. He has published in the field oforganisational behaviour and psychology in international peer-reviewed journals (e.g.International Journal of Selection and Assessment, Personality & Individual Differences,Employee Relations, Stress & Health, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology,Journal of Managerial Psychology, etc.). He teaches courses at undergraduate and postgraduatelevel, while maintaining active links with the industry through consulting projects and executivetraining. His main research interests concentrate on employee recruitment, selection, assessment,performance and well-being. He is the corresponding author and can be contacted at:[email protected]

Maria Tomprou is an organizational psychologist and she is currently conducting her PhDthesis in the Department of Management Science and Technology in the Athens University ofEconomics and Business. She received her MSc in Occupational Psychology from the Universityof Surrey, UK. She has participated in national and international conferences presenting herwork. Maria’s current research area focuses on psychological contract formation and herresearch interests include psychological contracts, socialization and emotions at work.

Maria Vakola is an organisational psychologist and she is currently working as an AssistantProfessor at the Athens University of Economics and Business. She received her PhD inOrganisational Behaviour from the University of Salford, UK. Her research interests concentrateon change management, individual and organizational readiness to change. She teaches coursesat undergraduate and post-graduate level, while maintaining active links with the industrythrough consulting projects and executive training. Maria has published in the field oforganizational change in academic journals, such as Communications of the ACM, Journal ofOrganizational Change Management, Employee Relations and Journal of Managerial Psychology.

Implications forpsychological

contracts

663

To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: [email protected] visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints