71
Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of performance on entry to school and the progress made in the first year in England and four other jurisdictions Research report June 2014 Peter Tymms, Christine Merrell, David Hawker & Frazer Nicholson - CEM Centre, Durham University

Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

  • Upload
    lydien

  • View
    227

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

Performance Indicators in Primary

Schools: A comparison of

performance on entry to school and

the progress made in the first year in

England and four other jurisdictions

Research report

June 2014

Peter Tymms, Christine Merrell, David Hawker & Frazer Nicholson - CEM Centre, Durham University

Page 2: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

2

Contents

List of figures 5

List of tables 6

Executive summary 8

Introduction 10

Background to the Performance Indicators in Primary Schools (PIPS) monitoring system11

Establishing representative samples of PIPS data for England, Scotland, New Zealand and

two Territories/States in Australia 13

Data available from the samples 13

The PIPS assessment 16

Background variables for England 17

Age 18

Gender 18

English as an Additional Language (EAL) 18

Deprivation 18

Establishing the measurements 19

The full scale 20

Item behaviour by age 21

Item behaviour by gender 21

Item behaviour by EAL 21

Item behaviour by deprivation 21

Item map 22

Early mathematics 25

Early reading 26

Picture vocabulary 27

Page 3: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

3

Phonological awareness 29

Differences between groups 31

What levels of personal social and emotional development do children have at the start of

Reception in England? 35

Progress in cognitive development and PSED between the start and end of Reception year in

England 38

Cognitive development (excluding children with EAL) 38

Personal, social and emotional development 39

Comparing jurisdictions 41

Early mathematics at the start of the year 41

Early reading 42

Personal, social and emotional development 43

Progress across jurisdictions 44

Early mathematics across jurisdictions 44

Early reading across jurisdictions 44

Multilevel models 45

Early mathematics 46

Early reading 47

Is it possible to have an international assessment for children starting school? 49

References 51

Appendix 1: The Samples 53

Establishing representative samples of PIPS data for England, Scotland, New Zealand and

two territories/states in Australia 53

England 53

Scotland 54

New Zealand 56

PIPS pupil sample for Australia (one territory and one state) 57

Page 4: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

4

Appendix 2: Reliability and Predictive Validity 58

Reliability 58

Predictive validity 59

Appendix 3: Differential Item Functioning 60

Appendix 4: Pre-school 61

Appendix 5: Dimensionality 64

Appendix 6: Measures for PSED 66

Adjustment – Comfortable 66

Adjustment – Independence 66

Personal – Confidence 67

Personal – Concentration (Self-directed activities) 67

Personal – Concentration (Teacher-directed activities) 67

Personal – Actions 68

Social – Relationship to peers 68

Social – Relationship to adults 69

Social – Rules 69

Social – Cultural awareness 69

Social – Communication 70

Page 5: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

5

List of figures

Figure 1: Distribution of children’s ages at the start of school ............................................... 17

Figure 2: The distribution of deprivation measures (IDACI) based on home postcodes. High

score indicates high deprivation ........................................................................................... 19

Figure 3: Map of items and pupils for the Total measure in England..................................... 23

Figure 4:Distribution of scores on the total measure ............................................................. 24

Figure 5: Map of items and pupils for the early mathematics measure in England ............... 25

Figure 6: Map of items and pupils for the early reading measure in England ....................... 27

Figure 7:Map of items and pupils for the vocabulary measure in England ............................ 28

Figure 8: Map of items and pupils for the phonological awareness measure in England ..... 30

Figure 9: Children’s PSED at the start of school in England ................................................ 36

Figure 10: PSED at the start and end of the Reception year in England .............................. 40

Figure 11: Early mathematics at the start of school (without EAL) ....................................... 42

Figure 12: Early reading at the start of school (without EAL) ............................................... 43

Figure 13: PSED at the start of school (with EAL) in England and Scotland ........................ 43

Figure 14: Start and end of year scores for early mathematics (no EAL) ............................. 44

Figure 15: Start and end of year scores for early reading (no EAL) ..................................... 45

Figure 16: Number of terms at nursery related to the total score ......................................... 61

Figure 17: Number of terms at playgroup related to the total score...................................... 62

Figure 18: Number of terms at nursery related to the total score after controlling for age and

deprivation ............................................................................................................................ 62

Figure 19: Dimensionality of the total measure .................................................................... 64

Page 6: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

6

List of tables

Table 1: Numbers of pupils for whom start of year cognitive development data were available

............................................................................................................................................. 13

Table 2: Numbers of pupils for whom end of year cognitive development data were available

............................................................................................................................................. 13

Table 3: Pupils for whom start of year non-cognitive development and behaviour data were

available ............................................................................................................................... 14

Table 4: Pupils for whom end of year non-cognitive development and behaviour data were

available ............................................................................................................................... 14

Table 5: Additional background data available ..................................................................... 15

Table 6: Cognitive constructs used in PIPS .......................................................................... 16

Table 7: Correlations between the scales for children in England whose first language is

English ................................................................................................................................. 31

Table 8: Differences between the sexes at the start of the year ........................................... 32

Table 9: Correlations with age at the start of the year .......................................................... 32

Table 10: Correlations with deprivation at the start of the year ............................................ 33

Table 11: PSED ratings ....................................................................................................... 35

Table 12: Links between PSED and gender, age, deprivation and EAL in effect sizes ........ 37

Table 13: Gains in cognitive development for England (excluding EAL) .............................. 39

Table 14: Gains in PSED measures for England (including EAL) ........................................ 40

Table 15: Multi-level models with early mathematics as the outcome .................................. 47

Table 16: Multi-level models with early reading as the outcome .......................................... 48

Table 17: Number of pupils in each decile in Reception in England 2012 (Population

609,983) ............................................................................................................................... 53

Table 18: Number of pupils in each decile in Reception in PIPS sample England 2012

(Sample 8,746) ..................................................................................................................... 53

Table 19: Number of pupils in each decile in Reception in the PIPS sample for England 2012

matched to National Distribution (Sample 6,986) .................................................................. 54

Table 20: Number of pupils in each decile in Primary 1 in 2012 (Population 56,349) ............ 54

Page 7: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

7

Table 21: Number of pupils in each decile from PIPS in 2012 based on postcodes (Sample

6,519) ................................................................................................................................... 55

Table 22: Number of pupils in each decile from PIPS in 2012 based on SCNs (Sample

9,911) ................................................................................................................................... 55

Table 23: Number of pupils in each decile from PIPS in 2012 based on SCNs and postcodes

(Sample 12,123) ................................................................................................................... 55

Table 24: Number of pupils in each decile in Primary 1 in PIPS sample Scotland 2012

matched to National Distribution (Sample 6,627) .................................................................. 56

Table 25: Decile rankings for New Zealand schools participating in PIPS ........................... 56

Table 26: Number of pupils in each Decile in Primary 1 in PIPS sample New Zealand 2012

matched to two PIPS schools per Decile .............................................................................. 56

Table 27: Reliability and separation based on data from England for this report without EAL

children................................................................................................................................. 58

Table 28: Test/retest reliability abstracted from the PIPS Technical Report (2001) .............. 58

Table 29: Test/retest reliability ............................................................................................. 59

Table 30: Numbers of children by numbers of terms at Playgroup or Nursery ..................... 61

Table 31: Links between PSED and nursery attendance in effect sizes ............................... 63

Page 8: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

8

Executive summary

This report describes secondary analysis of existing data from the Performance Indicators in

Primary Schools (PIPS) On-entry Baseline and Follow-up assessments, as used in five

jurisdictions: England, Scotland, New Zealand and one State and one Territory in Australia.

Children across each jurisdiction were assessed using the Baseline assessment on entry to

school and using the Follow-up assessment at the end of their first year of formal education.

Firstly, the report establishes the validity of the PIPS measures of cognitive and non-cognitive

development for particular groups of children, and to produce a national average of what

children can do on entry to school in each country.

For the first time, using a nationally representative sample and objective1 data, what children

know and can do when they start school in England is described. The report then sets out

the links between these measures and key background variables including gender, age,

deprivation, school attended, pre-school experience and jurisdiction.

Finally, it draws conclusions regarding the feasibility of a robust international comparative

study of children starting school and progress during their first year.

The cognitive measures of early reading, early mathematics, vocabulary and phonological

awareness were found to work well in England for the full target age range, the

socioeconomic spectrum and both sexes. Comparisons were more challenging for children

whose first language was not English or who may have been encountering English for the

first time when they started school. The measures of early reading and early mathematics

were found to work consistently across the five jurisdictions (after some items were dropped),

and therefore it is possible to make direct comparisons.

Children in all five jurisdictions were found, on average, to make substantial progress by the

end of their first year at school in all the cognitive domains. They also made considerable

progress on the non-cognitive measures of personal, social and emotional development.

The baseline assessments revealed that, of the five jurisdictions, children starting school in

England had the lowest overall scores when they started school. However, age for age the

English children were in line with children from Scotland and New Zealand for early

mathematics but significantly behind their peers from Australia. For early reading, children

from England had the lowest scores at the start of school, age for age and this was

statistically significant. Scotland was slightly ahead of England and children from the

Antipodes ahead of both England and Scotland.

The follow-up assessments showed that progress in early reading and early mathematics

during the first year at school varied widely from pupil to pupil and from school to school. The

1 In the sense that the results are not dependent on who collected the data

Page 9: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

9

study confirmed previous research (including studies using PIPS, for example Tymms,

Jones, Albone and Henderson, 2009) which found that children’s progress is much greater

during one year at this age than at any other age during their school career. It also found

that their progress is more dependent on the school attended than has traditionally been

found in most other studies of schooling internationally. For early mathematics, the progress

was generally similar across jurisdictions whereas for early reading, the progress was

greatest in England.

An international comparative study of children starting school faces two challenges. The first

is that children start school at different ages and the second is that languages and context

vary from country to country. This report has shown that it is possible to make comparisons

of what children can do at an early age across English-speaking countries, using the PIPs

assessments, despite the differing ages.

Page 10: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

10

Introduction

This report looks at the cognitive and non-cognitive development of children starting school

and their progress to the end of the academic year. The instruments used for the

assessment were the Performance Indicators in Primary Schools (PIPS) On-Entry Baseline

and Follow-up assessments and the data came from England, Scotland, New Zealand and

one State and one Territory in Australia. The first data were collected at the start of the

academic year in England in 2011 and again at the end of the academic year in 2012. The

Australian and New Zealand data were collected during 2012 and the Scottish data were

collected during the year 2012/13.

The analysis required clean and nationally representative datasets from the different

jurisdictions, therefore the first stage of the analysis was to establish these from the data

available in each jurisdiction. Appendix 1 describes the procedures used to establish the

datasets.

Once a representative sample was established for England, the data were used to explore

what children know and can do when they start school in England and create the

performance scales which are used in later analyses.

In order to help assess the quality of the data and to provide a measure which places both

items and children on a comparable scale in order to be able to make judgements and

comparisons, a particular approach known as Rasch measurement was used.

The extent to which the assessment instruments behave differently in different contexts and

where it was possible to equate scales was first investigated. The relative progress made by

children from the five jurisdictions was compared. Progress is firstly presented in graphical

format and then presented using multi-level models with children nested within schools.

Jurisdictions are identified by dummy variables in the models. The impact of other variables

is also considered.

The report finally considers whether it is possible to run a robust international project

designed to study children’s development at the start of school across different countries.

The judgement takes account of the analyses within this report as well as practical and

technical issues. It is concluded that such an international study is quite feasible.

Page 11: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

11

Background to the Performance Indicators in Primary Schools (PIPS) monitoring system

The Performance Indicators in Primary Schools (PIPS) monitoring system began nearly 20

years ago in England with the intention of providing baseline data for primary schools in order

to be able to generate confidential, nationally based, value-added scores at a later stage in

the children’s education. It quickly shifted to providing information to teachers about what

their pupils knew and could do, how their performance compared against norms and their

progress throughout primary school. The starting point for the system was the PIPS On-entry

Baseline Assessment. The assessment was initially used by a small number of schools in

the north-east of England and Solihull but spread rapidly by word of mouth and with backing

from the National Association of Head Teachers. As its use by teachers increased, they

asked for a repeat assessment at the end of the Reception year in order to be able to record

the substantial progress made by children during their first year at school, and it was

thereafter referred to as the PIPS On-entry Baseline and Follow-up.

The use of the PIPS On-entry Baseline and Follow-up (PIPS) has gradually expanded and

the assessments have been translated and adapted into a dozen languages. It has now

been used to assess more than a million children across several countries.

Full documentation outlining the PIPs assessments are available on the Centre for Evaluation

and Monitoring’s website2. In short, the assessment has two major parts to it: the first

assesses cognitive development; and the second, non-cognitive development. The first part

is designed to assess those aspects which are the best predictors of later success or

difficulty (Tymms, 1999a and 1999b; Tymms, Brien, Merrell et al, 2003; Tymms, Merrell,

Henderson et al, 2012). It is a computer-based assessment and the content is very

comprehensive but no individual child sees all of it, because it is presented in a series of

sequences with stopping rules. This adaptive nature means that a very wide range of

children can be assessed in an efficient, reliable and enjoyable way. The cognitive section is

made up from sub-units which assess Early Mathematics, Early Reading, Picture Vocabulary

and Phonological Awareness. There is also a Short Term Memory Assessment but that is

optional and the results are not reported here.

The non-cognitive section assesses personal social and emotional development and is based

on teachers’ observations of their pupils’ behaviour in the school setting. There is also an

additional section for the identification of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, which is

based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Version IV) published by

the American Psychiatric Association (1994).

2 http://www.cem.org/pips-baseline/introduction

Page 12: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

12

This report focusses predominantly on the cognitive element of the assessment and reports

some of the findings from the personal, social and emotional development section. It starts

by concentrating on England and then broadens out to consider other jurisdictions.

Page 13: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

13

Establishing representative samples of PIPS data for England, Scotland, New Zealand and two Territories/States in Australia

The data which were available from England, Scotland, New Zealand and the two Australian

territories/states (Australia A and Australia B) were not collected as representative samples.

Rather, representative samples for this project were defined. The way that this was done is

described in Appendix 1.

Data available from the samples

The major part of this report is the analysis of the assessment of children’s cognitive

development. The numbers of pupils in the representative samples at the start of the year

are shown below inTable 1. Table 2 gives the numbers at the end of the academic year.

Table 1: Numbers of pupils for whom start of year cognitive development data were available

Jurisdiction Early Maths Early Reading Picture

Vocabulary

Phonological

Awareness

England 6,983 6,983 6,983 6,983

Scotland 6,623 6,623 6,623 6,623

New Zealand 847 847 847 847

Australia A 3,457 3,457 3,421 3,457

Australia B 1,313 1,313 1,311 1,313

Table 2: Numbers of pupils for whom end of year cognitive development data were available

Jurisdiction Early Maths Early Reading Picture

Vocabulary

Phonological

Awareness

England 5,939 5,939 5,939 5,939

Scotland 6,627 6,627 6,627 6,627

New Zealand 594 594 594 594

Australia A 3,205 3,205 3,205 3,205

Australia B 1,228 1,228 1,228 1,228

Schools could, optionally, complete the non-cognitive part of the assessment, which contains

items designed to measure personal, social and emotional development (PSED) and the

behaviour section which assesses characteristics associated with inattention, hyperactivity

Page 14: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

14

and impulsivity (behaviour). The number of pupils for whom data were available is given

below.

Table 3: Pupils for whom start of year non-cognitive development and behaviour data were available

Jurisdiction PSD Behaviour

England 1,566 0

Scotland 669 0

New Zealand 65 0

Australia A 0 103

Australia B 0 75

Table 4: Pupils for whom end of year non-cognitive development and behaviour data were available

Jurisdiction PSD Behaviour

England 1,264 925

Scotland 726 641

New Zealand 17 12

Australia A 0 172

Australia B 0 52

Data were available on various background variables. These are listed below as

percentages of the representative sample.

Page 15: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

15

Table 5: Additional background data available

Jurisdiction School Class DoB Free

meal

entitle

ment

Terms

at

Nursery

Terms at

Playgroup

Flagged fo

r English

as an

additional

language

Ethnic

origin

Flagged for

Special

educational

needs

(communic

ation,

deafness,

cognitive,

behaviour

or physical)

Deprivation

(IDACI for

England,

SMID for

Scotland)

England 100 100 90 15 34 7 85 13 7 100

Scotland 100 100 100 40 0 2 22 2 4 100

New

Zealand 100 100 89 0 0 0 55 66 5 0

Australia A 100 100 100 0 0 0 100 85 4 0

Australia B 100 100 100 0 0 0 100 23 8 0

Page 16: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

16

The PIPS assessment

The PIPS assessment has more than 200 items within the cognitive part. Some of those are

individual items located within long reading passages whilst most are standalone items.

They are all organised into a series of sub-assessments corresponding to constructs relevant

to early reading and early mathematics and each of the items has been shown to be

predictive of later success in literacy and numeracy (Tymms 1999b, Tymms et al 2012,

(Table 6).

Table 6: Cognitive constructs used in PIPS

Construct Description

Writing Writing – the child is asked to write his/her own name and the quality of writing is scored

against examples.

Vocabulary Vocabulary – the child is asked to identify objects embedded within a picture.

Ideas About

Reading

Ideas about reading – assesses many of the ideas found in Marie Clay’s Concepts about

Print. (Clay 1972)

Phonological

awareness

Repeating Words - the child hears a word and is asked to repeat it.

Rhyming Words – the child selects a word to rhyme with a target word from a choice of three

options.

Letters Letter identification – a fixed order of mixed upper and lower case letters.

Early Reading Word recognition and reading.

This starts with word recognition and moves on to simple sentences that the child is asked to

read aloud.

The words within these sentences are high frequency and common to most reading

schemes.

This is followed by a more difficult comprehension exercise which requires the child to read a

passage and at certain points select one word from a choice of three that best fits that

position in the sentence.

Ideas About

Mathematics

Ideas about mathematics – assessment of understanding of the vocabulary associated with

mathematical concepts.

Counting Counting and numerosity – the child is asked to count four objects. These disappear from

the screen and then the child is asked how many objects they saw. This is repeated with

seven objects.

Digits Digit identification – single, two-digits and three-digits.

Shapes Identification of a variety of geometric shapes

Number Number manipulation – the child is asked how many more or less a number is than a target

Sums A (Informal) Sums – addition and subtraction problems presented without symbols.

Sums B (Formal) More difficult maths problems including sums presented with formal notation.

Page 17: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

17

Pupils are shown each section of the assessment on-screen. They start with easy items and

continue on to progressively harder ones within that section until they get three wrong in a

row or four wrong altogether. They then move to the next section and so on.

The non-cognitive part of the assessment is completed by teachers based on their

knowledge of working with the children. There are two parts. One concerns personal, social

and emotional development and this is made up of 11 items, each of which has a rating

scale. The items and scales are set out in Appendix 6. The other relates to behaviour and,

since the data are collected at the end of the year in England, it is not reported here, but

several academic papers have been published which are based on this information (Merrell,

Styles, Jones et al, 2013; Sayal, Owen, White et al, 2010; Merrell and Tymms, 2001).

Background variables for England

The first part of this report explores the PIPS data from England, how children vary when

they start school and their progress during the first year. In order to contextualise the

information and to make fair comparisons, four key background variables are used: Age,

gender, English as an Additional Language (EAL) and deprivation (home postcode used to

link to Income Deprivation Affecting Children Index - IDACI). Further information is given

below.

Figure 1: Distribution of children’s ages at the start of school

Page 18: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

18

Age

The vast majority of pupils started school in September (99.5%) with a few starting in

January and even fewer after Easter. The distribution approximates a rectangle with a

very small number joining after the age of five and a similar number aged just under four3.

Gender

The sample was made up from 48.5% girls and 51.5% boys.

English as an Additional Language (EAL)

1,110 pupils (15.9%) were identified as having English as an Additional language4.

Deprivation

The distribution of deprivation measures (IDACI scores) is shown in Figure 2. This is

based on home postcodes and a high score indicates high deprivation. The distribution is

very similar to the distribution for all children staring school in England5.

3 Data were not available at the time of writing to compare the sample to the full population of children starting

school 4 Data were not available at the time of writing to compare the sample to the full population of children starting

school 5 The PIPS sample is based on 6,986 cases and has a mean of 0.23 and a Standard Deviation of 0.17. The

national data are based on 610, 023 cases. Their mean is 0.24 and their Standard Deviation is 0.18

Page 19: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

19

Figure 2: The distribution of deprivation measures (IDACI) based on home postcodes. High score

indicates high deprivation

Establishing the measurements

PIPS is now nearly 20 years old and it has given rise to almost 50 academic articles. These

papers and the associated research have gone a long way to establish the reliability and

validity of the assessment, and some key figures are given in Appendix 2. One important

point to note is that in order to establish the consistency of PIPS, a group of children, chosen

at random, were reassessed by a researcher after they had been assessed by their teachers.

The two results were almost identical6; the assessment is extremely reliable. A second key

point is that children have now been followed up for many years and links between PIPS and

later assessments have been published (Tymms, Jones, Albone et al, 2009; Tymms, Merrell,

Henderson et al, 2012). Figures are given in the appendix which show good predictive

validity7.

6 The correlation between the total scores, excluding phonological awareness, was 0.98 for 29 pupils

7 The correlation to the end of Key Stage 1 average level was 0.66

Page 20: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

20

The data were analysed using a technique known as Rasch measurement, which very

usefully puts items and individuals on the same scale. This presents a clear visual illustration

of what children know and can do. The approach is also attractive because it takes account

of missing data which occurs when assessing children using an adaptive algorithm above,

and because it creates an equal interval scale. Much has been written about the Rasch

model (Bond and Fox, 2001; Linacre, 2003; Andrich, 2004), since the original ideas were

published a little over 50 years ago.

The full scale

In establishing the validity of the measures for use in this analysis, it is important to build on

the previous work mentioned above, and to:

a) Confirm that children’s performance on the PIPS assessment can be placed on an

equal interval scale. This was done using Rasch8 measurement techniques. PIPS

showed very strong psychometric properties. In particular, the derived performance

measures were shown to be extremely reliable person measures with good

separation.

b) Check that it is appropriate to report the results on a single scale. A section of

Appendix 5 investigates this under the heading “dimensionality” and it confirms that

the total score forms a coherent scale. It also shows that the expected sub-scales of

early reading, early mathematics, vocabulary and phonological awareness can be

separated out statistically. It further identifies an interesting subscale which might be

termed “symbol identification” comprising letter and number identification.

c) Ensure that the items behave similarly for the different subgroups. In other words, it is

important to ensure that items are not biased for or against particular groups9. One of

the advantages of Rasch Measurement is that if the assessment is working well, the

item difficulties should be the same for each sub-group. Rasch essentially creates a

ruler which remains constant for all groups of interest. This is in contrast to traditional

methods of analysis where the difficulties of items are dependent on the particular

group being assessed.

8 Technically Rasch measurement can be seen as one of a family of models which come under the heading of

Item Response Theory but many argue that Rasch measurement is a unique approach that gets to the heart of assessment (fundamental measurement) in a way that other methods do not. 9 Technically Differential item functioning (DIF) analyses were conducted for age, gender, EAL and deprivation.

The details of cut-scores and p values used to identify DIF are given in the appendices.

Page 21: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

21

Item behaviour by age

There was no evidence that the items behaved differently for pupils grouped by age bands.

(Four age categories were used: 4 to 4.2510, 4.25 to 4.5, 4.5 to 4.75 and 4.75 to 5).

Item behaviour by gender

Twelve items showed Differential Item Functioning (DIF) by gender. Four significantly

favoured girls; writing, two vocabulary items and the letter Q, (bizarrely) whereas eight of the

number identification items were relatively easier for boys. These were not thought to be

sufficient to affect the properties of the scale unduly11.

Item behaviour by EAL

A large number of the items behaved differently for EAL pupils compared to those whose first

language was English and those from whom data were missing on this variable. PIPS does

allow communication in the child’s own language and there are three languages other than

English available in the software. However, the vocabulary section assesses English

vocabulary acquisition and the phonological awareness section has the same target sounds

in all languages. Furthermore, although delivery in different languages is allowed, it is

necessary for the administrator to be fluent in that language for the assessment to run

smoothly.

Since many of the items were language-based and all involved language, it is hardly

surprising to see that the difficulty of items varied, amongst other things, the order in which

vocabulary is acquired differs between first and second language learners.

It follows that further analysis of PIPS data to compare EAL children with others at the start of

the year is potentially problematic.

Item behaviour by deprivation

The IDACI scores of deprivation of pupils’ neighbourhoods were used to create three equal

sized groups. Nine items behaved significantly differently by deprivation: one vocabulary

item, one Ideas about Reading item, one repeating words item, two letters (including Q; what

is it about Q?), one sentence, one item from the Ideas about Maths group and two of the very

hard maths items. This was not considered to greatly affect the measurement overall

because they represented a small proportion of the total number of items.

10 This group corresponds to the summer born children the very youngest being born in August

11 The correlation between the total score with and without the 12 items was 0.999

Page 22: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

22

Item map

The map of the items and the individuals, excluding children with EAL, is shown in Figure 3.

The equal-interval scale which is produced is represented by logits. The higher the logit, the

more difficult the item or, in the case of a child’s ability, the more able the child. The item map

gives a very clear idea of what children know and can do when they start school. It also

shows a normal distribution of pupils against the general measure which we may

call“cognitive development” at the start of school. The detailed percentages of pupils in each

bin are given in Figure 4.

Page 23: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

23

Figure 3: Map of items and pupils for the Total measure in England

Tabl

e 5c

: Map

of i

tem

s and

pup

ils fo

r the

Tota

l mea

sure

in En

gland

TABLE 12.2 SOR England Data v2.sav ZOU083WS.TXT Jan 8 14:33 2014

INPUT: 19504 PERSON 168 ITEM REPORTED: 5440 PERSON 165 ITEM 334 CATS WINSTEPS 3.74.0

----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

MEASURE PERSON - MAP - ITEM

<more>|<rare>

6 +

| M or L 8 more t

| M or L 21 more

|

5 + M or L 6 less t Sums B half of Sums B 4+11

|T M or L 10 less Sums B 9-6

. | Sums B 15+21

| Sums B 7+3

4 . +

. | M or L 3 less t Sums B 42-17 Sums B Sequence

. | M or L 3 more t Num 3dig1 Pass Walking t

. | Num 3dig2 PV cosmetics PV yacht

3 . + Num 3dig3 Sent Sentences Sums B Sequence

. | M or L 2 more t Sent Sentences Sent Story 2 Sums B 12p ora

. | IAR full Num 2dig1 Sent Story 1

. |S Num 2dig2 Sent Sentences Sums B 15-4

2 .# T+ IAR capital Num 2dig3 Sent Sentences Sent Sentences Sums A rockets W ball W tree

.## | Sent Sentences W cat W flower W house

.## | IAR sentence Num 3dig4 Pass Cats long W car W duck W rabbit

.#### | IAR start Num teen2 W dog

1 .##### + Num 3dig5 Num teen1 Num teen3 Sent Sentences Sums B half th W butterfly

.####### S| IAR sentence PV microscope Sums A pips

.######### | W Writing Lett D Lett Q M or L 1 more t Sums A ice crea

.########### | Lett A Lett H Lett J Lett Y Lett t Sums A puppies

0 .############ +M Lett G Lett K PV jewellery PV toadstool Rep observatory Rhy mouse Sums A bikes Sums B 5p appl

.############ M| IAR Letter Lett B Lett V Lett c Lett o Lett u PV padlock Rhy bin Rhy dish Sums A rabbits

.########### | Lett F Lett n Lett w PV saxophone Rep frigglejang Rhy pan Rhy toes

.########## | IAR word Lett L Lett S Lett e Lett r Lett z Num 9 PV cash Rhy cherries Rhy drum Rhy hat Rhy sun Sums B three q

-1 .####### + Lett x Num 6 Rep juxtapose Sh hexagon

.######## | Lett Name Lette Lett m Num 0 Num 8 Rep riotous Sums A cars

.###### S| Count fish here IAM least Lett i Lett p Num 7 Rep denalty

.#### | Count fish ther PV pigeon Rep enterprisin Sums A balls

-2 #### + IAM shortest Num 3 Num 5 PV bowl PV violin

.## |S IAR writing Num 1 Num 2 Num 4 Rep mantle Sh square

.# | Count apples th PV windmill Sh triangle

.# T| Count apples he PV knife PV pan

-3 . + PV cherries Rep stop

. | IAM most PV fork PV wasp Sh circle

. | IAM tallest IAR someone

. | IAM more PV butterfly

-4 . + IAR someone PV cupboard

. | PV carrots PV kite

. | IAM smallest PV turtle

. |T PV castle Sh star

-5 + IAM biggest

. |

|

. |

-6 +

|

. |

|

-7 . +

<less>|<frequent>

EACH "#" IS 41. EACH "." IS 1 TO 40

Page 24: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

24

Figure 4:Distribution of scores on the total measure

The items mapped on the right hand side in Figure 3 show us that the average child starting

school in England could easily answer vocabulary items such as “butterfly”, “kite” and

“carrots” but that virtually no children could identify a “yacht” or “cosmetics”; those words

were outside most children’s vocabularies at that particularly stage. The average (or median)

child was likely to know the word “jewellery” as well as “padlock”. In terms of early reading,

they were likely to have identified a number of letters such as “T” and about half will know the

letter “N” and “J” but they are unable to read sentences, although the most able could identify

words such as “dog”, “duck” and “car”.

In terms of early mathematics, a high proportion were able to identify single digit numbers

such as “6” and “9”, but two digit numbers were beyond most children starting Reception.

Similarly the question “What is two more than six?” is beyond the ability of most children at

this stage.

In the following sections, the full scale is broken down into four major dimensions: early

mathematics, early reading, picture vocabulary and phonological awareness. Each is

analysed, excluding the EAL children.

Page 25: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

25

Early mathematics

The early mathematics items form a single reliable measure with good internal reliability

(details are provided in Appendix 2). There is some evidence of additional dimensions in the

form of number identification and informal sums/ideas about maths but the scale can be

treated as one.

There was little evidence of items behaving differently by children’s age, deprivation or

gender although girls did find the identification of double digit numbers relatively harder than

boys. The map of early mathematics items and pupils is displayed in Figure 5 below.

Figure 5: Map of items and pupils for the early mathematics measure in England

5440 PERSON 66 ITEM

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

MEASURE PERSON - MAP - ITEM

<more>|<rare>

8 +

| M or L 21 more

|T

7 . +

. | M or L 8 more t

| Sums B 15+21

6 + Sums B 42-17 Sums B 9-6

. | M or L 10 less Sums B 4+11

| M or L 6 less t Sums B half of

5 . +

. |

|

4 . + Sums B 12p ora Sums B 7+3

. |S Num 3dig1 Sums B 15-4 Sums B Sequence

. | M or L 3 less t M or L 3 more t Num 3dig2 Num 3dig3

3 . +

. | Sums B Sequence

. | M or L 2 more t Num 2dig1

2 .# T+ Num 2dig2 Num 3dig4

.# | Num 2dig3 Num 3dig5 Sums A rockets

.## |

1 .### +

.#### | Num teen2

.####### S| Num teen1 Num teen3 Sums B half th

0 .######## +M M or L 1 more t Sums A pips

.########### | Sums A ice crea Sums A puppies

.############ | Sums B 5p appl

-1 .########### + Sums A bikes Sums A rabbits

.########### M| Sums B three q

.######### | Num 9

-2 .####### + Num 0 Num 6 Sh hexagon

.######## | Num 7 Num 8 Sums A cars

.####### | Count fish here Count fish ther IAM least

-3 .####### S+ Num 5 Sums A balls

.###### | IAM shortest Num 2 Num 3

.##### |S Count apples th Num 1 Num 4 Sh square

-4 .### + Sh triangle

.# | Count apples he

.# T| IAM most Sh circle

-5 . + IAM tallest

. | IAM more

. |

-6 . +

. | IAM smallest Sh star

. | IAM biggest

-7 +

|T

. |

-8 . +

<less>|<frequent>

EACH "#" IS 39. EACH "." IS 1 TO 38

Page 26: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

26

The map gives a clear picture of what children know and can do in relation to early

mathematics when they start school in England. The least able children could use words

such as “biggest” correctly but had trouble counting four objects. By contrast, children in the

middle of the distribution could identify single digit numbers and were able to respond to

questions involving simple informal sums. The more able children could do more complex

informal sums and identify two digit numbers, but it was a very unusual child who could do

formal sums which needed the knowledge of symbols such as “+”, “-“ and “=” which are

generally not known before being taught at school.

Early reading

The early reading items form a single reliable measure (details in Appendix 2). There is

some evidence of additional dimensions in the form of letter identification as one group, the

reading of passages as another and ideas about reading as a third but the whole section can

be treated as one.

Using the criteria set out earlier there was little evidence of the items in this scale behaving

differently by children’s age, deprivation or gender with just one item behaving differently by

age, none by gender and none by deprivation.

The map of early reading items and pupils is displayed in Figure 6.

Page 27: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

27

Figure 6: Map of items and pupils for the early reading measure in England

5440 PERSON 58 ITEM 116 CATS WINSTEPS 3.74.0

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

MEASURE PERSON - MAP - ITEM

<more>|<rare>

6 . +

|

|

. |

5 . + Pass Walking t

. |

. |T

. |

4 . +

. | Sent Sentences

. | Sent Sentences

. | Sent Sentences

3 . + Pass Cats long Sent Sentences Sent Sentences Sent Sentences Sent Story 2

. | Sent Story 1

. |

. |S Sent Sentences

2 .# + W ball W tree

.# T| IAR full W flower

.# |

.# | IAR capital W car

1 .## + IAR sentence W duck

.## | W butterfly W dog W house

.### | Lett Q W cat W rabbit

.### | Lett D Lett Y

0 .#### S+M IAR sentence IAR start Lett J

.##### | Lett G

.#### | Lett H Lett V Lett u

.##### | Lett n Lett t Lett w

-1 .###### + Lett F Lett K

.####### | Lett A Lett B Lett L

.###### | Lett c Lett e Lett i Lett p Lett r Lett z

.####### | Lett x

-2 .###### M+ IAR Letter Lett o

.########## |S Lett m

.####### | IAR word Lett S

####### |

-3 .########## +

. | Lett Name Lette

.########## |

.### S|

-4 +

.############ |

.# |T IAR writing

|

-5 +

.###### |

. |

T|

-6 + IAR someone

|

.## | IAR someone

|

-7 .# +

<less>|<frequent>

EACH "#" IS 36. EACH "." IS 1 TO 35

Children generally learn to read after they start school and the map indicates that on entry,

almost all children had some knowledge of the ideas about reading. The average child could

identify the first letter of his or her name and perhaps one or two more letters. The top

quartile of children were starting to read simple words and a much smaller proportion could

read simple short sentences. Very few were able to read longer texts.

Picture vocabulary

The vocabulary items go well together to form a single reliable measure (details in Appendix

2). There was no evidence of additional dimensions.

Page 28: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

28

Figure 7:Map of items and pupils for the vocabulary measure in England

5440 PERSON 23 ITEMS

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

MEASURE PERSON - MAP - ITEM

<more>|<rare>

7 . +

|

. | PV cosmetics

|

|

6 + PV yacht

|

|T

|

.### T|

5 +

|

|

|

|

4 .####### +

|

| PV microscope

.######### S|

| PV jewellery

3 . +

|S PV saxophone

.##### |

.###### |

|

2 .## + PV toadstool

.######## | PV cash PV padlock

.###### M|

|

.##### |

1 +

.############ |

|

. |

.###### |

0 +M PV pigeon

.##### S| PV violin

. | PV bowl

.## |

.## | PV windmill

-1 . +

.## |

. | PV knife PV pan

.# | PV wasp

. | PV cherries

-2 . +

.# T| PV fork

| PV butterfly

|

. |S PV kite PV turtle

-3 . + PV cupboard

|

| PV carrots PV castle

|

. |

-4 .# +

<less>|<frequent>

EACH "#" IS 56. EACH "." IS 1 TO 55

Using the criteria set out earlier there was little evidence of differential behaviour by children’s

age, deprivation or gender. Just two items showed evidence of behaving differently by

deprivation and two different items by gender.

Page 29: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

29

Words such as ‘carrots’ and ‘castle’ were known by almost all children starting school,

whereas words like ‘cash’ and ‘padlock’ were known by about half the children. The hardest

words were ‘cosmetics’ and ‘yacht’ and were known by very few children starting school.

Phonological awareness

The phonological awareness items formed a reliable measure (details in Appendix 2).

Although dimensional analysis clearly indicates that the scale could be split into measures of

rhyming and repeating words, it was analysed as a single dimension here.

Using the criteria set out earlier there was no evidence of the items behaving differently by

children’s age, deprivation or gender.

Page 30: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

30

Figure 8: Map of items and pupils for the phonological awareness measure in England

5440 PERSON 17 ITEMS

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

MEASURE PERSON - MAP - ITEM

<more>|<rare>

4 .######## +

|

|

|

|

|

|

T|

3 .######### +

|

|

|

|

|

.######### |

|

2 +

|T

.########## S|

. |

. |

.########### |

.#### |

.### | Rep observatory

1 .############ +S Rhy dish

.######## | Rhy mouse Rhy pan

.######## | Rhy bin

.####### |

.### |

.######## M|

.######## | Rep frigglejang Rhy drum Rhy toes

.## | Rep juxtapose Rhy cherries Rhy sun

0 .######## +M Rhy hat

.### |

.#### |

.## |

.######## | Rep denalty Rep riotous

.## | Rep enterprisin

### |

.##### S|

-1 .# +S

.# |

.### |

.#### |

|

. | Rep mantle

.###### |

.## |T

-2 . +

|

T|

|

|

. | Rep stop

.#### |

. |

-3 .#### +

<less>|<frequent>

EACH "#" IS 29. EACH "." IS 1 TO 28

The least able children had difficulty repeating words such as ‘mantle’ and ‘stop’ but the

average pupils could repeat most words that were presented to them and were starting to get

the idea of rhymes. The most able groups could repeat all words presented to them and

could correctly pick out all rhyming words.

Page 31: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

31

Differences between groups

The previous section indicated that the measures formed good scales which are comparable

across groups, except those for whom English was an additional language. Those children

were therefore excluded from the figures below which quantify the differences in ability

between boys and girls, the links to age, deprivation and attendance at pre-school.

Before comparing groups, it makes sense to look at the association between the scales. The

correlations are presented below.

Table 7: Correlations between the scales for children in England whose first language is English

Normal Score of Total

Normal Score of Reading

Normal Score of

Maths

Normal Score of Picture

Vocabulary

Normal Score of Phonological Awareness

Normal Score of Total

1 .882 .898 .641 .694

Normal Score of Reading

.882 1 .734 .460 .493

Normal Score of Maths

.898 .734 1 .508 .549

Normal Score of Picture Vocabulary

.641 .460 .508 1 .476

Normal Score of Phonological Awareness

.694 .493 .549 .476 1

The figure is based on 4,816 cases and all correlations are significant at the 1% level.

The scales all correlated with one another positively at least the 0.48 level which is not

surprising, since they all measure general cognitive development. The early reading and

early mathematics measures were most strongly related with a correlation of 0.7. Picture

vocabulary and phonological awareness correlated with early reading and early maths at

around the 0.5 level.

Differences between mean scores are presented as effect sizes and the spread of scores are

also recorded since there are clear differences between boys and girls for some measures.

Page 32: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

32

Table 8: Differences between the sexes at the start of the year

Measure Effect Size

(Male-

Female)12

Standard

Deviation Male

Standard Deviation

Female

Total -0.25** 0.85 0.81

Early Reading -0.26** 1.01 0.95

Early Mathematics -0.12** 0.87 0.76

Picture Vocabulary -0.27** 0.87 0.83

Phonological Awareness -0.23** 0.90 0.89

The Effect sizes (Hedges’ d) were calculated using the pooled standard deviation

**

significant at the 1% level

The girl’s scores were higher, on average, than the boy’s scores for the total score, early

reading, picture vocabulary and phonological awareness with an effect size of about 0.25.

This can be categorised as a “small effect” following Cohen (1969) and who referred to 0.2 as

being small, 0.5 as medium and 0.8 as large. For early mathematics, the girls were still

ahead but only by an effect size of 0.12. Interestingly, the boys’ scores were more varied

than the girls’ scores and this was particularly noticeable for early mathematics.

Differences by age

The figure below shows how the scores correlate with age. These are converted to effect

sizes in the last column.

Table 9: Correlations with age at the start of the year

Measure Correlation with age Effect Size

Total 0.31** 0.67

Early Reading 0.25** 0.53

Early Mathematics 0.30** 0.65

Picture Vocabulary 0.22** 0.45

Phonological Awareness 0.21** 0.44

** p<0.01

Effect size calculated using the formula from Fitz-Gibbon and Morris 198713

12 Excluding items which behaved differently for males and females (DIF) made a difference to the fourth

decimal point for the total score.

Page 33: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

33

Unsurprisingly, all the measures were positively correlated with age and this was most

strongly evident for the total score and early mathematics where the correlation was about

0.3 corresponding to an effect size of nearly 0.7; a medium to large effect. To get a clearer

indication of what this means the regression equation linking the total score to age is given

below:

Total = -4.43 + 0.86 * age (in years)

It shows that the total score rises by about 0.07 standard deviation units per month or a

difference of 0.86 standard deviation units between the youngest and eldest in a Reception

class on average. This corresponds, for example, to the progression from counting a few

objects to doing informally presented sums. It also corresponds to the move from knowing a

few letters to reading some words.

Difference by deprivation

The correlations of the five measures with deprivation are shown in Table 10.

The strongest link is with the total score and a correlation of -0.2 and indicates that the more

deprived had lower scores on average. This is significant in statistical and substantive terms,

but is weaker than the correlations that appear for these measures with older children.

Table 10: Correlations with deprivation at the start of the year

Measure Correlation with IDACI Effect Size

Total -0.20** -0.41

Early Reading -0.16** -0.33

Early Mathematics -0.17** -0.35

Picture Vocabulary -0.17** -0.34

Phonological

Awareness

-0.17** -0.36

** p<0.01

Effect size calculated using the formula from Fitz-Gibbon and Morris 1987.

NB1 If EAL children are included, the correlations become stronger, e.g. for the total score it becomes -0.25

NB2 The negative correlations indicate that greater deprivation is associated with lower levels of attainment

13 Based on Hedges and Olkin (1985) The formula is

Page 34: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

34

Differences by pre-school experience

The number of terms at pre-school was recorded by some of the teachers into the contextual

data section of the software. These data were provided on an optional basis and so limited

samples are available and caution must be exercised in analysing and interpreting the

information. Therefore, the breakdown of results and the links to attainment are to be found

in Appendix 4 where the conclusion is, that although based on limited data, the data suggests

that one or two terms has little impact on the total score at the start of school, but that three

or more terms are positively linked to the total score after taking into account age and

deprivation. Further analysis does not suggest that that the conclusion varies across

deprivation groups.

Page 35: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

35

What levels of personal social and emotional development do children have at the start of Reception in England?

Just over 1,500 pupils were rated by their teachers on the personal, social and emotional

development (PSED) items in the PIPS baseline assessment a few weeks after starting

school. The assessment was repeated at the end of the school year. Children with EAL

were included in this analysis because their development was assessed through teachers’

ratings and their assessments should not be dependent on the child’s language although the

children may differ on that dimension as is investigated below.

The PSED section consists of 11 areas. Ratings for each area were made on a five point

scale going from low to high. A full list of items can be found in Appendix 6. The results are

presented in Table 11 and Figure 9. It can be seen that, on average, teachers rated children

around the middle of the scale although on the cultural awareness item the score was an

exception to this with the mean closer to two. The spread of scores, the standard deviation,

is important because it is large and suggests that a noticeable proportion of children were

given the highest rating of five and some were given the lowest rating of one for each item.

Table 11: PSED ratings

N Mean Standard

Deviation

Social rules 1,565 3.38 0.85

Social relationship (to adults) 1,565 3.15 0.85

Adjustment comfortable 1,566 3.14 0.95

Personal confidence 1,565 3.06 0.91

Adjustment independence 1,565 3.02 0.86

Personal concentration (teacher-directed) 1,565 2.99 0.88

Personal actions 1,565 2.99 0.87

Social relationship (to peers) 1,565 2.97 0.76

Social communication 1,565 2.97 1.04

Personal concentration (self-directed) 1,565 2.87 0.88

Social cultural awareness 1,565 2.22 0.85

Page 36: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

36

Figure 9: Children’s PSED at the start of school in England

1

2

3

4

5M

ean

The PSED ratings were further analysed by the gender of the child, their age, the IDACI

score derived from the home postcode deprivation, attendance at pre-school and whether the

child’s first language was English. Table 12 shows the differences between groups,

expressed as effect sizes. For all the PSED items, girls were, on average, rated at a higher

level than the boys. The advantage was as high as nearly 0.5 of standard deviation, or just

less than half a point, and all comparisons were statistically significant except for the items

relating to feeling comfortable on arrival at school. One must be wary when interpreting this

kind of information because it is based on teachers’ judgements, which may vary from

teacher to teacher and may be biased.

The older the child on starting school, the higher the ratings tend to be on each item. The

effect sizes are modest but clear; the older children were seen to concentrate more, to feel

more comfortable, to communicate better, to have better relationships and so on. There was

a fairly constant effect across all items.

The link to deprivation was weak and much smaller than the links to gender and age. It was

non-significant except for concentration on self-directed activities and for rules where the

more deprived the child’s home background, the lower their rating on the concentration items

and the less they were seen as responding positively to rules. Concentration is important

because it relates positively to later academic performance, just as inattention is a predictor

of less progress in later years (Merrell and Tymms, 2001; Merrell and Bailey, 2012; Pingault,

Tremblay, Vitaro et al, 2011).

Attendance at nursery was investigated but, as noted earlier, the data were collected

optionally and may not provide a representative picture. The analysis is shown in Appendix 4

Page 37: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

37

where positive links are found between attendance at nursery and all items. They were

found to be statistically significant for seven of the items.

Table 12: Links between PSED and gender, age, deprivation and EAL in effect sizes

Measure Male-female Age Deprivation

1st

Language

English -

EAL

Social rules -0.46** 0.25** -0.18** 0.03

Social relationship (to adults) -0.28** 0.35** -0.02 0.23**

Adjustment comfortable -0.10 0.36** 0.01 -0.05

Personal confidence -0.16** 0.45** -0.04 0.22**

Adjustment independence -0.29** 0.42** 0.02 -0.03

Personal concentration (teacher-

directed) -0.43** 0.38** -0.13 0.06

Personal actions -0.46** 0.28** -0.08 0.08

Social relationship (to peers) -0.35** 0.30** -0.06 0.26**

Social communication -0.30** 0.33** 0.03 0.44**

Personal concentration (self-

directed) -0.47** 0.44** -0.18** 0.05

Social cultural awareness -0.26** 0.23** 0.06 0.15

All figures are based on 1,565 (+-2) cases except the nursery attendance data which is based on 661 cases. **

p<0.01

The last column in Table 12 compares the children whose first language was English with

those whose first language was identified as not English and there were just a few

differences. The children with EAL were rated as less confident and not so adept in relating

to both other children and to adults when compared to children whose first language is

English. Not surprisingly, the biggest differences were seen in their communication skills

ratings.

Page 38: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

38

Progress in cognitive development and PSED between the start and end of Reception year in England

Cognitive development (excluding children with EAL)

The gains in cognitive development during the first year of school are set out in Table 13. The

growth in this first year is massive with an effect size of 2.44 for the total score. Effect sizes in

education are usually less than one (Hattie,1999; Hill, Bloom, Black et al, 2008). As noted

earlier, Cohen (1969) suggests that an effect size of 0.8 to be considered as large. The

effect size reported here (2.44) is amongst the largest reported in education research and in

agreement with Hill, Bloom, Black et al (2008), is probably larger than any effect size in any

other year at school. One would expect an effect size of around 0.3 per year during much of

the primary stage and then less than that as the children progress through secondary

school14. Of course, during the first year at school children start to acquire basic skills and it

is not unreasonable that they will make the longest strides early on; but they really do seem

to be very long strides and it may be worth recalling that cognitive growth curves often have

an elongated S shape with slow progress at the start followed by rapid gains, which then

flatten off (Demetriou and Raftopoulos, 2005).

Part of the gain is due to maturation, as the children were three quarters of a year older when

they were assessed for the second time. The third column in the figure sets out the gain

calculated to be due to maturation alone, and in the case of the total score this amounts to an

effect size of 0.4815. It follows that the effect size due to schooling is 1.96, which is still a very

large gain.

The figure also sets out the gains for early reading, early mathematics, picture vocabulary

and phonological awareness separately. The major progress was in early reading and early

mathematics but very large gains were also made in vocabulary and phonological

awareness.

14 This is based on the expected level of progress in terms of levels per year and the standard deviations at the

ends of Key Stages. 15

There is a linear relationship between PIPS score and age. This was used to estimate the gain that would be expected by maturation alone.

Page 39: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

39

Table 13: Gains in cognitive development for England (excluding EAL)

Measure Effect Size

overall

Effect Size by

maturation

Effect Size due to

schooling

Total 2.44 0.48 1.96

Early Reading 2.49 0.73 1.76

Early Mathematics 2.50 0.72 1.78

Picture Vocabulary 1.47 0.57 0.90

Phonological Awareness 1.49 0.60 0.89

Personal, social and emotional development

Teachers rated their pupils on the same PSED items at the start and the end of the

Reception year and very large gains were seen, typically around one standard deviation, or

one point on the rating scale, although some were larger than that, as reported in Table 14

and Figure 10. Some of the gain is due to maturation and it was noted earlier that age was

clearly related to all the items. As with the cognitive development measures, the gain

explained by maturation alone was calculated and it is reported in the third column of the

figure. This was used to generate the fourth column which shows the residual gain

associated with schooling.

Clearly, some children were hitting the top of the scale and therefore the estimates are

probably understated. Nevertheless the gains are substantial. All of the effect sizes are at

least 0.25 and most are much larger, especially the one for cultural awareness, which was

1.13.

Page 40: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

40

Table 14: Gains in PSED measures for England (including EAL)

PSED Measure Effect Size

Overall

Effect Size by

Maturation

Effect Size due

to Schooling

Social rules 0.67 0.33 0.34

Social relationship (to adults) 0.98 0.48 0.50

Adjustment comfortable 1.04 0.49 0.64

Personal confidence 0.96 0.60 0.36

Adjustment independence 1.42 0.60 0.82

Personal concentration (teacher-directed) 1.01 0.49 0.52

Personal actions 1.03 0.36 0.67

Social relationship (to peers) 1.07 0.38 0.69

Social communication 1.07 0.48 0.59

Personal concentration (self-directed) 0.90 0.53 0.27

Social cultural awareness 1.45 0.29 1.13

Figure 10: PSED at the start and end of the Reception year in England

Page 41: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

41

Comparing jurisdictions

The next part of the report compares the cognitive development of children starting school in

different jurisdictions, taking into account the age of those children. Before making

comparisons, it is important to ensure that the items from the assessments behave similarly

across jurisdictions and Rasch measurement techniques were used to check this.

Vocabulary was not compared because language varies so much across cultures and the

items had different orders of difficulty in different jurisdictions. For example, an analysis by

items revealed that the item ‘wasp’ from the vocabulary section was particularly difficult for

children in Australia compared with the other jurisdictions whereas the item ‘toadstool’ was

particularly difficult in England. After removing all the items that behaved differently across

the jurisdictions, just eight items remained, which was too few to form a reliable scale for

making comparisons.

The phonological awareness scale has been adjusted in Australia, where a bespoke section

had been introduced, leaving too few items in common to be able to make comparisons

between jurisdictions.

For both early mathematics and for early reading, there were sufficient items to be able to

proceed.

Early mathematics at the start of the year

Forty of the early mathematics items behaved differently across jurisdictions and they were

excluded from further analysis, leaving a group of items which were comparable. These

items were used to create a measure of early mathematics for each child which was

comparable across jurisdictions.

To control for children’s varying starting ages across countries, the age at the time of

assessment for each child was calculated from the dates of birth and these were grouped

into categories, split into quarter year slices. The youngest category included children from

age four to four and a quarter years, and the oldest included children from age six to six and

a quarter years. Figure 11 is a plot of the average ability score for each of the age

categories, restricted to groups where there were at least 200 children.

As expected, there was a positive relationship between ability scores and age. The children

starting school in England were the youngest of the jurisdictions included in the analysis.

The ability scores rose quite noticeably by age, as did the scores for Scotland. Both of the

Australian groups had high scores for their age and the relationship to age is rather flatter

than would be expected by comparison with the English and Scottish data. The children in

New Zealand start school just after their fifth birthday and were in line with the results from

England and Scotland.

Page 42: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

42

Figure 11: Early mathematics at the start of school (without EAL)

Early reading

Following an analysis of the behaviour of items across jurisdictions, the items which varied in

relative difficulty across jurisdictions were removed to leave a scale of 44 items. Some of the

easier items in the ideas about reading section showed some variation in behaviour between

jurisdictions but they were retained in the scale to ensure that the lower end of the scale was

sufficiently represented.

Figure 12, which compares early reading scores by age category, shows similar patterns for

England and Scotland as were seen for early mathematics, with an increase in ability by age,

although the children in Scotland were slightly ahead of those in England and the line was a

little flatter. The two Australian groups were ahead of the children in the other jurisdictions,

age for age, and the children in New Zealand were positioned between the two.

Page 43: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

43

Figure 12: Early reading at the start of school (without EAL)

Personal, social and emotional development

There was sufficient data to make comparisons for England and Scotland but not for other

jurisdictions. Figure 13 shows the results. Although the children from Scotland scored

higher than the children from England, they were, on average, six months older and we have

already noted that the PSED scores increase with age. However, further investigation using

the General Linear Model and controlling for age showed that the ratings for children were

still higher for Scotland than for England by between a tenth and a half a point on the rating

scale. These differences were significant at the 1% level except for the two ratings of

concentration.

Figure 13: PSED at the start of school (with EAL) in England and Scotland

Page 44: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

44

Progress across jurisdictions

The progress during the first year at school is analysed and presented in two ways. First the

mean pupil ability scores were compared at the start and the end of the year, and this

progress is reported graphically. Then multi-level models were run with two levels; pupils

and schools with jurisdictions identified by dummy variables. The early reading and early

mathematics scales used in the previous section were analysed.

Early mathematics across jurisdictions

Figure 14 shows the pupils’ ability scores by age category at the start and the end of the year

for the five jurisdictions. The graphical representation of the data for England indicated a

very large impact of schooling and this was paralleled by the data from Scotland. The other

jurisdictions exhibited similar rises, as schooling in all areas appeared to have similar

impacts. The multi-level models, which are set out later, give a more accurate picture of

changes and quantify, for example, the different amount of time between assessments of the

New Zealand children compared to the other jurisdictions. They were assessed a year later

whereas in the other areas they were assessed at the start and end of the same school year.

Figure 14: Start and end of year scores for early mathematics (no EAL)

NB There are fewer points for Australia A and B at the end of the year compared with the start because some cases were lost during the year and points were only plotted on the chart where there were at least 200 cases.

Early reading across jurisdictions

The patterns for early reading were different from those seen for early mathematics.

Although all jurisdictions showed large gains, the children in England seemed to make more

progress, closely followed by the children in Scotland and New Zealand. By the end of the

Page 45: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

45

year, although all the jurisdictions have close scores for each age categories, there are still

differences. New Zealand has the highest score of all.

Figure 15: Start and end of year scores for early reading (no EAL)

NB There are fewer points for Australia A and B at the end of the year compared with the start because some cases were lost during the year and points were only plotted on the chart where there were at least 200 cases.

Figure 15 could be interpreted as all groups coalescing at a similar high early reading score,

suggesting a ceiling on the assessment. But further investigation showed that this was not a

valid interpretation as the reading scale went much higher than the hypothesised ceiling.

Multilevel models

The multi-level models seek to partition the variance of the end of year scores in early

mathematics and early reading between pupils and schools, and account for that variance by

changes to the model. Ideally, one would include good measures of home background but

they were not available in the present data because we did not collect comparable socio-

economic status measures across jurisdictions. Nevertheless, the key prior attainment

measures were available. The models are set out for early mathematics and then for early

reading, which can be related back to the charts in Figures 13 and 14 that display almost the

same attainment data used in the models. Unlike the previous analyses of the cognitive

measures, the children with EAL were included. This group represented 9.3% of the 19,228

cases studied. A dummy variable was used to separate them from the other children.

Page 46: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

46

Early mathematics

The models are set out in Table 15 below. They build up in complexity from the so-called null

model through to Model 3. In each case, more variables were added to the model, thus

reducing the variance. In the null model, the Constant (0.59) is the mean for the whole

sample and the figure in parenthesis (0.02) shows the standard error on that mean. The end

of year mathematics variance was partitioned between pupils (0.20) and schools (0.40); in

other words, a third of the variance was found at the school level. In Model 1, dummy

variables were used to identify jurisdictions in comparison to England, which had the lowest

scores with the youngest children. Just above England came New Zealand and Scotland,

which both had a score of 0.37, and above them came the two Australian jurisdictions with

Australian B having the highest score of 1.10. The school level variance dropped by just over

half, but the pupil variance did not change, leaving around a fifth of the variance at school

level.

In Model 2, the key explanatory variables from the start of school were introduced. Since

early mathematics is the outcome, it is not surprising to see that the early mathematics score

at the start of school was the most important variable with a coefficient of 0.68. Early reading

was also important, whereas age had a small positive coefficient. Children with EAL had a

positive coefficient, indicating that they made more progress than children for whom English

was their first language16. The coefficients for the different jurisdictions were very similar and

none differed significantly from England. In other words, the progress measures for each

jurisdiction were essentially identical. By including the various variables in the model, the

pupil level variance dropped by about 60% and a fifth came off the school level variance. In

Model 2, a third of the variance was associated with school membership. This is a very high

proportion of variance suggesting the school attended made a very large difference to the

progress that children made in early mathematics17. By contrast, the jurisdiction in which a

child was educated made almost no difference.

Finally, in Model 3, the mean school mathematics score at the start of the year was included.

It had a negative and significant coefficient, indicating a tendency for the schools with the

highest average score at the start of the year to make the least progress. Generally, Model 3

was similar to Model 2, except that Australia B now had a higher and significant coefficient.

This suggests that the Australia B schools with their high-scoring children at the start of the

year were making better progress than other jurisdictions after taking into account the

general finding that schools with higher intakes tend to make less progress.

16 If the slope is linked to EAL then this positive coefficient becomes non-significant and the slope for the first

measure of early maths is less positive for the EAL children 17

Typically school effectives students report the proposition of variance associated with school membership in the range 10-30% (Guldemond and Bosker, 2009)

Page 47: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

47

Table 15: Multi-level models with early mathematics as the outcome

Null Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Fixed

Constant 0.59 (0.02)* 0.30 (0.02)* 0.06 (0.05) 0.93 (0.06)

Australia A 0.77 (0.04)* -0.06 (0.04) 0.02 (0.05)

Australia B 1.10 (0.05)* 0.06 (0.04) 0.18 (0.06)*

New Zealand 0.37 (0.10)* -0.08 (0.09) -0.05 (0.09)

Scotland 0.37 (0.03)* -0.03 (0.02) 0.02 (0.03)

Age 0.03 (0.01)* 0.03 (0.01)*

EAL 0.08 (0.01)* 0.07 (0.01)*

Start Reading 0.16 (0.01)* 0.16 (0.01)*

Start Mathematics 0.68 (0.01)* 0.69 (0.10)*

School av mathematics -0.09 (0.03)*

Random

School level variance 0.20 (0.01)* 0.098 (0.006)* 0.080 (0.005)* 0.079 (0.005)*

Pupil level variance 0.40 (0.004)* 0.40 (0.004)* 0.162 (0.002)* 0.163 (0.002)*

% at School level 33.3 19.7 33.0 32.6

% Drop at school level 55.5 20.0 1.2

% Drop at pupil level 0 59.5 -0.6

Figures in parentheses are standard errors * p<0.05

Early reading

In the null model for early reading, nearly 20% of the variance was ascribed to the schools.

In Model 1, the jurisdictions are identified by dummy variables and England, with the

youngest children, has the lowest scores with Scotland being slightly ahead, Australia A was

slightly further ahead, New Zealand further ahead and Australia B just ahead of New

Zealand. By introducing the dummy variables for jurisdictions, the school level variance

dropped by almost a quarter and the pupil level variance dropped by a very small amount.

The proportion of variance associated with schools was about 15%.

In Model 2, as for early mathematics, the other measures were introduced into the model and

now it is clear that the progress made in England was more than in any other jurisdiction

although not significantly more than New Zealand. Scotland made a little less progress

whereas England made a lot more progress than the two Australian jurisdictions, which

initially had started at a higher level. All the jurisdictions finished at a similar level. Perhaps

surprisingly, the age coefficient was negative which meant that the older children made less

progress than younger children on average. This can be seen inTable 16. The children with

Page 48: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

48

EAL were, on average, making more progress than others18. Early Reading ability at the

start of school was the most important variable in the model. After the addition of measures

in Model 2, the pupil level variance dropped by over a half and the school level variance by

about 14%; it leaves 25% of the variance linked to schools which is a very high proportion in

comparison with other school based studies.

In Model 3, the school average reading score at the start of the year was introduced. It had a

statistically significant negative coefficient but with only a little impact on other coefficients,

however, it did impact on Scotland’s coefficient, removing its statistical significance.

Table 16: Multi-level models with early reading as the outcome

Null Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Fixed

Constant 0.73 (0.01)* 0.56 (0.02)* 1.60 (0.5)* 1.44 (0.06)*

Australia A 0.29 (0.04)* -0.39 (0.03)* -0.27 (0.04)*

Australia B 0.42 (0.04)* -0.38 (0.02)* -0.24 (0.04)*

New Zealand 0.40 (0.08)* -0.09 (0.08) 0.02 (0.08)

Scotland 0.23 (0.02)* -0.09 (0.02)* -0.03 (0.02)

Age -0.04 (0.1)* -0.04 (0.01)*

EAL 0.14 (0.01)* 0.14 (0.01)*

Start Reading 0.50 (0.01)* 0.51 (0.01)*

Start Mathematics 0.32 (0.01)* 0.32 (0.01)*

School av reading -0.17 (0.02)*

Random

School 0.084 (0.006)* 0.064 (0.004)* 0.055 (0.003)* 0.052 (0.003*)

Pupil 0.375 (0.004)* 0.365 (0.004)* 0.164 (0.002)* 0.163 (0.002)*

% at School level 18 14.9 25.1 24.2

% Drop at school l 23.8 14.0 1.2

% Drop at pupil l 2.7 55.1 0.1

* p<0.05

18 As with mathematics the slope associated for the first best predictor (Early Reading in this case) was

significantly more shallow and the intercept for EAL became non-significant.

Page 49: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

49

Is it possible to have an international assessment for children starting school?

The case for creating an international comparative study of the sort that exists for later

primary and secondary years (e.g. TIMSS, PIRLS and PISA) has been made in a number of

places, and broadly welcomed in principle by policy makers in national governments and

international agencies. Not only would such a study help to contextualise and interpret these

later assessments, but it would help to shed light, in a way that no other type of study could,

on some important policy issues, such as ‘how effective are different types of early years

programmes?’, ‘how important is it to have effective teachers in the first year of school?’,

‘how do different factors influence children’s learning?’, ‘what are the best policies for long

term effectiveness?’

This study has not addressed these policy questions directly; rather, it has sought to examine

the feasibility of such a study using existing data. This final section therefore builds on the

findings in the report to consider whether or not it is possible to compare the development of

children starting school across different countries.

There are two major issues concerning comparability. The first is that children start school at

different ages in different parts of the world and, at that young age, cognitive progress is very

rapid, thus challenging the possibility of comparability. The second is that children in different

countries grow up in different cultures, speak different languages and often learn in different

ways with different materials. Given these issues, is it possible to make valid comparisons?

The analyses carried out in this paper started by looking at the data from children at the start

of school. With its great diversity of content and range of difficulty, the PIPS On-entry

Baseline assessment is able to accommodate a very wide range of children. It was not found

to be appropriate to include children for whom English was an Additional Language (EAL) for

several of the analyses and this point is touched on below. However, the fact that the

assessment could accommodate very slow learners at the age of just four years and a day

up to strong learners at the age of six and half at the end of their first year of school, and that

the data can all be put onto the same scale for early mathematics and early reading is very

promising. This range represents a considerable variation in cognitive development, more or

less covering the full range of children starting school around the world. Further, these data

can all be collected in around 20 minutes per child. Early reading and early mathematics can

form a single linear scale, using Rasch measurement, and this has been shown to be valid

for both sexes, across the full socioeconomic range and across five English-speaking

jurisdictions.

The second issue relates to the language and the context of the child. The evidence that we

have presented for the differential item functioning in England for children with EAL is not

relevant, because these children were being assessed in an English context as opposed to

children in a different jurisdiction speaking in the language of that country. It is interesting to

note that for the early mathematics section the evidence is that there is little difference in how

Page 50: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

50

items perform for different groups of children, and it would be quite possible to create a

meaningful scale which incorporated children with EAL. It was not possible to follow this up

as part of the report because of time constraints.

This analysis highlights some problems with an assessment of early reading, particularly

regarding vocabulary and phonological awareness. Although not part of this study, other work

has shown that PIPS can be adapted quite successfully to other languages and cultures. To

date, versions have been created and used in Dutch (where it is known as OBIS), German

(where it is known as FIPS), Russian, Spanish, French, Slovenian and Chinese (both

Cantonese and Mandarin), Afrikaans and Sepedi (another Southern African language).

The mathematics items do not generally present any problems when translated, although

some visual material such as coins need to be changed, and some of the later and/or more

able starters (from our research in Russia and China) required the development of a range of

harder items to avoid a ceiling effect in the assessment.

The reading items generally prove more challenging to adapt, particularly for languages

whose syntax does not bear a very close resemblance to English, however, in those

languages where the most research has been carried out to date (German, Dutch and

Russian) it has been possible to develop versions of the items which are rated as equivalent

by professional linguists and early years educators competent in both languages, and which

work well when used with experimental cohorts of children.

The vocabulary and phonological awareness scales have proved the most challenging in

terms of generating equivalent versions for the different languages and cultures, and this

lends support to the idea that these two scales should not be used for international

comparisons, but adapted for use internally by policy makers within individual countries as

these are important features widely regarded as fundamental to children’s language

acquisition and developing literacy skills.

In conclusion, we believe that it is both possible and desirable to have an international study

of children’s cognitive and non-cognitive development starting school, and of their progress

during the first school year. The research and policy questions are too important to leave

unexplored. As with any research, such a study is unlikely to give definitive answers to

questions such as what age children should start school, but, in providing an important set of

international benchmarks and very useful policy-relevant information within countries, which

is not available to many at the present time, it will certainly provide some very important and

additional information and help policy makers evaluate existing policies in individual

countries.

Page 51: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

51

References

Andrich, D. (2004). Controversy and the Rasch Model: A Characteristic of Incompatible

Paradigms? Medical Care, 42(1 suppl), I-7 to I-16.

Bond, T. G. and Fox, C. M. (2001). Applying the Rasch Model: Fundamental Measurement in

the Human Sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

CEM (2001). Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: Technical Report.

Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. NY: Academic

Press.

Clay, M. M. (1972). The Early Detection of Reading Difficulties: A Diagnostic Survey. Auckland: Heineman. Demetriou, A., and Raftopoulos, A. (Eds.) (2005). Cognitive developmental change: Models,

methods, and measurement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Department of Education (2001). National Pupil Database Spring Census 2012.

Unpublished raw data.

Fitz-Gibbon, C. T. and Morris, L. (1987). How to analyze data. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage

Publications.

Guldemond, H. and Bosker, R. (2009). School effects on students' progress – a dynamic

perspective. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 20(2), 255-268.

Hattie, J. (1999). Influences on student learning: Inaugural Lecture. Auckland, New Zealand: University of Auckland. Hill, C. J., Bloom, H. S., Black, A. R. and Lipsey, M. W. (2008). Empirical benchmarks for interpreting effect sizes in research. Child Development Perspectives, 2 (3), 172-177.

IDACI scores (2010). Pages accessed 10.01.14:

http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20120919132719/http://www.communities.gov.uk/c

ommunities/research/indicesdeprivation/deprivation10/

Linacre, J. M. (2003). Data Variance: Explained, Modeled and Empirical. Rasch

Measurement Transactions, 17(3), 942-943.

Merrell, C. and Bailey, K. (2012). Predicting Achievement in the Early Years: How Influential

is Personal, Social and Emotional Development? Online Educational Research Journal.

http://www.oerj.org/View?action=viewPaper&paper=55.

Merrell, C., Styles, I., Jones, P., Tymms, P and Wildy, H. (2013). Cross-country Comparisons

of Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Behaviour in School-Based Samples of Young

Children. International Research in Early Childhood Education, 4 (1).

Page 52: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

52

Merrell, C. and Tymms, P. (2001). Inattention, hyperactivity and impulsiveness: Their impact

on academic achievement and progress. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 43-

56.

New Zealand Education Counts (2013). Downloaded 10.01.13:

http://www.educationcounts.govt.New Zealand/directories/list-of-New Zealand-schools

New Zealand decile (2013). Downloaded 10.01.14:

http://www.minedu.govt.New Zealand/New Zealand/EducationPolicies/Schools/

SchoolOperations/Resourcing/OperationalFunding/Deciles/HowTheDecileIsCalculated.aspx

Pingault, J. B., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Carbonneau, R., Genolini, C., Falissard, B., and Côté, S. M. (2011). Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: A 16-year longitudinal population-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(11), 1164-1170.

Sayal, K., Owen, V., White, K., Merrell, C., Tymms, P. and Taylor, E. (2010). Impact of Early

School-Based Screening and Intervention Programs for ADHD on Children's Outcomes and

Access to Services. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 164(5), 462-469.

Scottish Educational Analytical Services Division (2013). Unpublished raw data.

SIMD (2013). Pages accessed 10.01.14:

http://www.scotland.gov.uk/Topics/Statistics/SIMD/BackgroundMethodology

Tymms, P. (1999a). Baseline Assessment and Monitoring in Primary Schools: Achievements,

Attitudes and Value-added Indicators. London, David Fulton Publishers.

Tymms, P. B. (1999b). Baseline assessment, value-added and the prediction of reading.

Journal of Research in Reading, 22(1), 27-36.

Tymms, P., Brien, D., Merrell, C., Collins, J. and Jones, P. (2003). Young deaf children and

the prediction of reading and mathematics. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 1(2), 197-

212.

Tymms, P., Jones, P., Albone, S., and Henderson, B. (2009). The first seven years at school.

Educational Assessment and Evaluation Accountability, 21, 67-80.

Tymms, P., Merrell, C., Henderson, B., Albone, S., and Jones, P. (2012). Learning Difficulties

in the Primary School Years: Predictability from On-Entry Baseline Assessment. Online

Educational Research Journal.

Page 53: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

53

Appendix 1: The Samples

Establishing representative samples of PIPS data for England, Scotland, New Zealand and two territories/states in Australia

England

Because schools can choose whether or not to use PIPS, it is necessary to establish first

whether the range of pupils’ backgrounds is representative of children starting state schools

in England and, if it is not, whether a representative sample can be established.

Schools complete background data for each pupil within the assessment, when they start

school in Reception aged four. This includes forename, surname, gender and date of birth.

These items were used to match the data recorded in the PIPS database to the National

Pupil Database held by the Department of Education to allocate an Income Deprivation

Affecting Children Index (IDACI, 2010) Score to individuals.

The Spring Census 2012 was linked to the PIPS data for the academic year 2011/2012,

which is the cohort analysed within this report.

The IDACI score, by decile, for the home postcode for all state pupils attending Reception

class in England 2012 matched to the Pupil Spring Census Dataset (Department for

Education, 2013) is shown in Table 17 below.

Table 17: Number of pupils in each decile in Reception in England 2012 (Population 609,983)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 86,565 76,002 67,287 62,047 57,870 53,949 53,597 52,741 51,263 48,662 609,983

Proportion 0.14 0.12 0.11 0.10 0.09 0.09 0.09 0.09 0.08 0.08

This can be compared with Table 18 which shows the distribution of the Pupils’ IDACI scores

for the home postcode put into the same deciles from the PIPS dataset assessment for

autumn 2012 that were matched to the Spring Census data 2012 for Reception children by

the Department of Education National Pupil Database.

Table 18: Number of pupils in each decile in Reception in PIPS sample England 2012

(Sample 8,746)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 991 1,105 1,093 966 897 684 654 735 794 827 8,746

Proportion 0.11 0.13 0.12 0.11 0.10 0.08 0.07 0.08 0.09 0.09

Page 54: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

54

The proportions shown in Table 19 are fairly similar, but in order to equate them, an

appropriate proportion of pupils were randomly sampled in each decile and to give a

representative sample of the National Dataset. The sample was reduced to 6,986 pupils and

this sample was used in the subsequent analyses.

Table 19: Number of pupils in each decile in Reception in the PIPS sample for England 2012 matched to

National Distribution (Sample 6,986)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 991 870 771 711 663 618 614 604 587 557 6,986

Proportion 0.14 0.12 0.11 0.10 0.09 0.09 0.09 0.09 0.08 0.08

Scotland

The data from Scotland were treated in a similar way although the measure of deprivation

was different and the links to that measure were different. As in England, schools completed

background data for each pupil within PIPS when they started school in Primary 1 aged

between four and a half and five and a half. This includes home postcode and Scottish

Candidate Number (SCN). Either of these measures can be used to allocate an individual to

one of the deciles linked to the Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD). However, the

assessment can be carried out without these details being completed and for the

assessments administered in autumn 2012, the following information was available:

Number of pupils assessed: 20,775

Number of valid pupil postcodes: 6,519

Number of valid Scottish candidate IDs: 9,991

Number of pupils with either a valid postcode or SCID or both: 12,123

Table 20 shows the distribution of pupils in Primary 1 by Scottish Index of Multiple

Deprivation (SIMD 2013) decile from data supplied by the Educational Analytical Services

Division. 335 pupils with unknown data zone are not included.

Table 20: Number of pupils in each decile in Primary 1 in 2012 (Population 56,349)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 6,841 6,146 5,568 5,593 5,456 5,417 5,736 5,567 5,440 4,585 56,349

Proportion 0.12 0.11 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.08

This distribution can be compared with the distribution of pupils in the PIPS dataset based

only on postcode information.

Page 55: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

55

Table 21: Number of pupils in each decile from PIPS in 2012 based on postcodes (Sample 6,519)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 632 578 706 729 599 687 737 734 683 434 6,519

Proportion 0.10 0.09 0.11 0.11 0.09 0.11 0.11 0.11 0.11 0.07

The distribution of the number of pupils in the PIPS data set for whom home postcodes were

available was fairly even across the deciles. The proportions were close to the full dataset

shown in Table 22 with slightly fewer than expected for some groups (deciles 1, 2 and 10).

Table 22: Number of pupils in each decile from PIPS in 2012 based on SCNs (Sample 9,911)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 1,015 1,227 1,064 1,194 1,024 914 911 901 1,136 525 9,911

Proportion 0.10 0.12 0.11 0.12 0.10 0.09 0.09 0.09 0.12 0.05

The distribution of the number of pupils in the PIPS data set for whom SCNs were available

was also fairly even across the deciles. The proportions were close to the full dataset shown

in Table 23 with slightly fewer than expected for some groups (deciles 1, 6, 7 and 10)

For 4,275 pupils, deciles were available which were derived from a postcode and SCN. The

vast majority of these deciles were identical but in some cases they were not. The two sets

of deciles were combined and, where there was conflict, the decile derived from the SCN was

preferred. The resultant deciles derived from both postcodes and SCN are shown below

inTable 23.

Table 23: Number of pupils in each decile from PIPS in 2012 based on SCNs and postcodes (Sample

12,123)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 1,147 1,372 1,264 1,437 1,240 1,170 1,222 1,189 1,373 709 12,123

Proportion 0.10 0.11 0.10 0.12 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.11 0.06

A proportional random sample of pupils in each decile from PIPS data was selected to give a

representative sample of the National Dataset as supplied by the Scottish Government

Education Analytical Services. This is reported inTable 24. This sample was used for further

investigation.

Page 56: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

56

Table 24: Number of pupils in each decile in Primary 1 in PIPS sample Scotland 2012 matched to

National Distribution (Sample 6,627)

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 788 722 656 656 656 656 656 656 656 525 6,627

Proportion 0.12 0.11 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.10 0.08

New Zealand

The data from New Zealand differ from the rest of the data in a number of ways. The first is

that children start school just after their fifth birthday and so arrive at school throughout the

year. They do PIPS just after arrival and then one year later, rather than at the end of the

academic year.

The number of schools in the sample from New Zealand is smaller than the datasets for

either England or Scotland and national pupil level data on home background were not

available. But school level information was available, in the form of the decile rankings for

state schools participating in PIPS (New Zealand Education Counts 2013). Details of how

the deciles, which are based on demographic information, are calculated are published (New

Zealand Deciles 2013). Table 25 shows the distribution of the New Zealand schools

represented in the PIPS dataset.

Table 25: Decile rankings for New Zealand schools participating in PIPS

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 2 1 1 1 2 3 1 1 4 6 22

All Deciles are covered by at least one school in the PIPS dataset. To gain a sample of

students with an even distribution for New Zealand two schools were selected randomly for

Deciles 6, 9 and 10. Deciles 2, 3, 4, 7 and 8 have only one PIPS school per Decile. The

data for these Deciles are replicated to simulate two schools. Deciles 1 and 5 have two PIPS

schools per Decile. This sample was used for further analyses.

Table 26: Number of pupils in each Decile in Primary 1 in PIPS sample New Zealand 2012 matched to

two PIPS schools per Decile

Decile 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Total

Number 64 70 32 38 140 88 88 138 88 101 847

Proportion 0.08 0.08 0.04 0.04 0.17 0.10 0.10 0.16 0.10 0.12

Page 57: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

57

PIPS pupil sample for Australia (one territory and one state)

PIPS data are collected in all Australian States and Territories but, for one territory and one

state (A & B), data are available for all schools. This report only includes these complete

datasets where there were 46 and 65 schools in A and B respectively. Because of an

agreement within Australia, the Independent, state and catholic schools data cannot be

separated and so, unlike England, Scotland and New Zealand the Australian data includes

some independent schools. These data amount to 32% in A and 8% in B and all of the data

were used in the analyses which follow.

Page 58: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

58

Appendix 2: Reliability and Predictive Validity

Reliability

All of the data were initially analysed together using Rasch measurement in the Winsteps

(version 3.74.0) package. The reading passages (which range from simple sentences to

longer passages) were dichotomised into those which were scored very highly and those less

highly. The Rasch model showed very strong psychometric properties. There were

extremely reliable person measures (alpha=0.93) with good separation (around 4), extremely

high reliability of the items (0.99) and excellent separation (around 30). Just a few of the

items were not reached by any pupils and just a few pupils had such extreme scores that

they could not be measured. The fit statistics for the items were good, with the Infit mean

Square averaging 0.98 with a standard deviation of 0.1. This suggests most items fitted well

but that a few either over or under fitted.

More details about the total score and the subscales can be found below.

Table 27: Reliability and separation based on data from England for this report without EAL children

Name Person reliability from

Rasch (Alpha)

Person Separation

from Rasch

Total 0.93 4.00

Early mathematics 0.90 2.94

Early reading 0.84 2.32

Picture vocabulary 0.78 1.87

Phonological awareness 0.73 1.64

Additional data on reliability from the PIPS Technical Report (2001) is given in the table

below. These figures add to an understanding of PIPS because they are based on

reassessments whereas the table above relates to internal consistency

Table 28: Test/retest reliability abstracted from the PIPS Technical Report (2001)

Name Test retest

Total (without phonological awareness) 0.98

Early mathematics 0.90

Early reading 0.97

N=29

Page 59: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

59

Predictive validity19

As the years go by, more and more data is collected and this can be related to later

assessments. In the tables below we can see the correlations between the Baseline score

and the End of Reception assessment in PIPS based on the reading and the maths scores

and correlations up to the PIPS reading and maths scores at Year 2, 4-5 terms after End of

Reception, and also in relation to KS1 results. The results are based on the CD based

version of PIPS. In general these findings give considerable confidence in the predictability

of attainment; at least to the extent that anything can be expected to be predictable at this

particular stage. Reports which give further details of the connections between the PIPS

baseline assessments and later assessments are available from the CEM Centre.

Table 29: Test/retest reliability

Correlation with

PIPS total

Number of pupils Date

End of reception maths 0.67 14460 94/95

End of reception reading 0.75 14460 94/95

Y2 maths (PIPS) 0.59 2512 97/00

Y2 reading (PIPS) 0.61 2516 97/00

KS1 reading task level 0.56 5975 97/00

KS1 reading comprehension level 0.55 6137 97/00

KS1 writing level 0.60 6964 97/00

KS1 spelling level 0.48 6790 97/00

KS1 maths level 0.59 6989 97/00

KS1 average level 0.66 7045 97/00

19 This section is taken from the PIPS Technical Report 2001

Page 60: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

60

Appendix 3: Differential Item Functioning

Differential item functioning (DIF) were therefore conducted for age, gender, EAL and

deprivation. For each item, the size of the difference between each subgroup was inspected

and the difference was tested for statistical significance. Because the sample sizes are so

large in the majority of cases the former were taken as the most important and only if the size

of the difference was greater than 0.5 logits was the p value inspected for t values which

tested the difference between the average score and the score for a particular group. If that

was less than 0.01 then it was concluded that there was DIF for that item.

Page 61: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

61

Appendix 4: Pre-school

The numbers of terms at pre-school, as entered by the teacher into the contextual data

section of the software are shown in Table 30 below. These items of data were provided on

an optional basis and so limited samples are available.

Table 30: Numbers of children by numbers of terms at Playgroup or Nursery

Number of terms Playgroup Nursery

0 57 84

1 9 24

2 11 41

3 119 1,067

4 37 123

5 21 101

6 112 388

The graphs below show how these categories relate to normalised total score.

Figure 16: Number of terms at nursery related to the total score

The mean Total score in plotted on the Y axis with 95% Confidence Intervals

Page 62: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

62

Figure 17: Number of terms at playgroup related to the total score

Visually, there appears to be little link between the standardised scores and attendance at

play group (Figure 17) although when broken down by sub-measures there was a significant

relationship for phonological awareness (p<0.01) which remains after controlling for age and

deprivation. But, the link was not manifested as a clear trend and was not significant for early

reading, early mathematics or picture vocabulary.

On the other hand, Figure 16 does suggest a trend related to nursery attandance and after

controlling for age and deprivation a relationship between terms at nursery remains

significant (p<0.01) and this held true for all sub-assessments. However, the trend appears

not to be monotonic as the chart below shows.

Figure 18: Number of terms at nursery related to the total score after controlling for age and deprivation

Although based on limited data the chart suggests that one or two terms has little impact and

that three or more terms are similarly positive. Further analysis does not suggest that that

the link varies across deprivation groups.

Page 63: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

63

Attendance at nursery was also link to PSED and was initially dichotomised into less than a

year or more than a year on the basis that that was the cut off which mattered for the link to

cognitive measures. But this did not produce a significant result (p>0.01) perhaps because

the statistical test was underpowered as not all teachers chose to rate children on the PSED

items and nursery attendance data were not available for all pupils. Further, a more detailed

approach suggested that pupils with five or six terms at nursery were indeed scoring more

highly on many of the items. Therefore the direct correlation with the total numbers of terms

at nursery was used and this was converted to an effect size for the figure. For all items

there was a positive link and it was significant for more than half of the items with modest

effect size.

Table 31: Links between PSED and nursery attendance in effect sizes

Measure Terms at nursery

Adjustment Comfortable 0.23**

Adjustment Independence 0.23**

Personal Confidence 0.19

Personal Concentration (self-directed) 0.28**

Personal Concentration (teacher-directed) 0.26**

Personal Actions 0.30**

Social Relationship (to peers) 0.17

Social Relationship (to adults) 0.22**

Social Rules 0.18

Social Cultural Awareness 0.18

Social Communication 0.24**

Based on 661 cases. **

p<0.01

Page 64: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

64

Appendix 5: Dimensionality

An interesting question arises as to whether the data represent a single scale or if it should

be broken up into more dimensions. Figure 19 below is designed to help answer this

question and gives the results of a principal components analysis of the residuals from the

Rasch model. They clearly suggest one basic dimension dominating the data but it is also

clear from the Eigen value for the first contrast, which is 4.8, that a second dimension is

present. There is also evidence of a third dimension and more.

Figure 19: Dimensionality of the total measure

INPUT: 168 ITEM REPORTED: 6458 PERSON WINSTEPS 3.74.0

----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------

Table of STANDARDIZED REffect SizeIDUAL variance (in Eigenvalue units)

-- Empirical -- Modeled

Total raw variance in observations = 292.5 100.0% 100.0%

Raw variance explained by measures = 127.5 43.6% 44.3%

Raw variance explained by persons = 45.2 15.4% 15.7%

Raw Variance explained by items = 82.3 28.1% 28.6%

Raw unexplained variance (total) = 165.0 56.4% 100.0% 55.7%

Unexplned variance in 1st contrast = 4.8 1.6% 2.9%

Unexplned variance in 2nd contrast = 3.3 1.1% 2.0%

Unexplned variance in 3rd contrast = 2.9 1.0% 1.7%

Unexplned variance in 4th contrast = 2.8 1.0% 1.7%

Unexplned variance in 5th contrast = 2.5 .8% 1.5%

STANDARDIZED REffect SizeIDUAL VARIANCE SCREE PLOT

VARIANCE COMPONENT SCREE PLOT

+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+

100%+ T +

| |

V 63%+ +

A | U |

R 40%+ M +

I | |

A 25%+ I +

N | |

C 16%+ +

E | P |

10%+ +

L | |

O 6%+ +

G | |

| 4%+ +

S | |

C 3%+ +

A | |

L 2%+ 1 +

E | |

D 1%+ 2 +

| 3 4 5 |

0.5%+ +

+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+--+

TV MV PV IV UV U1 U2 U3 U4 U5

VARIANCE COMPONENTS

The first contrast grouped the identification of numbers and letters together. This possibly

corresponds to children who have learnt letter and number identification in pre-school or

home, but it could also be that some children are particularly attracted by symbols. This set

of items is juxtaposed with another group of items from vocabulary, phonological awareness,

Ideas about Maths and Ideas about Reading. Interestingly, the same pattern appears

whether EAL children are included in the analysis or not.

Page 65: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

65

The second dimension picks out three and two digit identification as well as the reading of

some sentences.

The third dimension largely picks out reading passages as one group and maths items as

another group.

There has been much debate in the literature about which scale to use when given statistics

such as those derived above. The consensus seems to be that decisions must be influenced

by the purpose of the measures and not solely based on quantitative indicators. In this case,

we are initially interested in describing the general cognitive development of children when

they start school, and in particular, how this relates to later literacy and numeracy. None of

the groups from the first or second contrasts correspond well to the variables which the

assessment was designed to measure although the third dimension does seem to split early

reading and early mathematics.

The approach adopted here was to first set out a map of items and individuals and then

explore the other scales, with a particular focus on the development of literacy and early

mathematics.

Page 66: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

66

Appendix 6: Measures for PSED

Adjustment – Comfortable

1. Upset on separation from carer at the start of the session. Not at ease during the day.

Does not cope easily with transitions between activities or locations within the school

setting.

2. Sometimes upset on separation from carer at the start of the session. Fairly settled

during the day. Occasionally finds transitions a problem.

3. Rarely upset on separation from carer at the start of the session. Copes well with

transitions between activities or locations within the school. Fairly settled during the

day.

4. Never upset on separation from carer at the start of the session. Comfortable for most

of the time during the session. Has no difficulty coping with transitions.

5. Never upset upon separation from carer at the start of the session. Very comfortable,

never ill at ease during the session. Has no difficulty coping with transitions between

activities or locations.

Adjustment – Independence

1. Dependent on adults or another child for guidance and support for much of the time.

Generally needs help with clothing and personal activities (coat, toilet, etc.).

2. Some dependence on adults or other children. Needs help with some clothing and

personal activities (coat, toilet, etc.).

3. Independence of others for most of the time but still needs occasional support. Can

cope with some clothing and personal activities, but not all (e.g. can put on coat but is

unable to fasten it).

4. Independence of others for most of the time but still needs occasional support. Copes

well with most clothing and personal activities.

5. Independence. Seeks assistance only when special help is required. Can put on and

fasten coat, go to the toilet, etc.

Page 67: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

67

Personal – Confidence

1. Very hesitant. Does not join in group activities and rarely talks.

2. Fairly hesitant. Reluctant to participate in group activities or talk.

3. Will join in group activities or talks when prompted.

4. Quite confident. Keen to join in group activities or talk within the school setting.

5. Very confident. Keen to participate in group activities within the school.

Personal – Concentration (Self-directed activities)

1. Finds it extremely difficult to concentrate. Very rarely settles to one thing and very

easily distracted.

2. Short concentration span. Finds it difficult to settle down to one thing. Easily

distracted.

3. Able to settle to a task and concentrate for a sustained period. May be distracted.

4. Attends quite well. Able to maintain concentration and is not disturbed by mild

distractions.

5. Can focus attention, even in the face of competing activities. Has been seen to

concentrate for a long period (e.g. 15 minutes).

Personal – Concentration (Teacher-directed activities)

1. Finds it extremely difficult to concentrate. Very rarely settles to one thing and very

easily distracted.

2. Short concentration span. Finds it difficult to settle down to one thing. Easily

distracted.

3. Able to settle to a task and concentrate for a sustained period. May be distracted.

4. Attends quite well. Able to maintain concentration and is not disturbed by mild

distractions.

5. Can focus attention, even in the face of competing activities. Has been seen to

concentrate for a long period (e.g. 15 minutes).

Page 68: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

68

Personal – Actions

1. Acts impulsively without any consideration for the well-being of themselves and others.

Demonstrates inappropriate behaviour in all situations. Unable to cope with changes

in routine.

2. Occasionally considers the well-being of themselves and others before acting, but still

exhibits frequent impulsive behaviour. Will interact appropriately with others when

prompted, sharing and taking turns.

3. On about half of all occasions, considers the well-being of themselves and others

before acting. Sometimes interacts appropriately with others but still needs frequent

prompts. Copes with changes in routine reasonably well but sometimes gets over-

excited.

4. Frequently considers the well-being of themselves and others before acting. Usually

interacts appropriately with others without being prompted to do so. Copes quite well

with changes in routine.

5. Almost always considers the well-being of themselves and others before acting.

Unless severely provoked, always interacts appropriately with others without being

prompted to do so. Responds positively to changes in routine.

Social – Relationship to peers

1. Finds it difficult to communicate with other children and make friends. Seems to take

no account of others and is frequently inconsiderate.

2. Often has difficulty communicating with other children and making friends.

3. Communicates quite easily with other children and able to form friendships. Takes

notice of the feelings of others when they become very obvious.

4. Communicates quite easily with other children and readily forms friendships. Takes

notice of the feelings of others.

5. Communicates very easily with other children and readily forms friendships. Is aware

of others and responds to their needs. Sensitive.

Page 69: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

69

Social – Relationship to adults

1. Finds it difficult to communicate with adults. Does not approach adults or speak to

them. Inappropriate behaviour whilst interacting with adults.

2. Often has difficulty communicating with adults. Reluctant to approach adults or speak

to them. Usually interacts appropriately with adults.

3. Communicates with adults but some difficulty. Will approach adults and speak to

them. Rarely demonstrates inappropriate behaviour whilst interacting with adults.

4. Confident approaching adults when necessary. Relates well to adults, and with

appropriate behaviour.

5. Confident approaching adults when necessary. Relates easily to adults, and with

appropriate behaviour. Speaking to adults is natural and easily understood.

Social – Rules

1. Takes no notice of rules. Distracts others and interrupts activities.

2. Takes little notice of rules. Can distract others and interrupt activities.

3. Sometimes ignores rules. May distract others on occasion.

4. Usually obeys rules and rarely distracts others.

5. Always obeys rules and never distracts others

Social – Cultural awareness

1. Shows an awareness of the routines in their home environment. For example, through

role-play in the ‘home corner’.

2. Demonstrates an understanding of being a member of a family/household by talking

about relationships and experiences with parents/siblings and other relatives.

3. Is aware that they are a member of a wider community within their local

neighbourhood and pre-school setting. Talks about experiences relating to those

environments.

4. Recognises that the way of life of others may be different from their own. Takes pride

in their own achievements.

5. Is aware of, and respects the way of life of others. Understands that their own way of

life should be respected by other children and adults

Page 70: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

70

Social – Communication

1. Communicates with others using single words, gestures and facial expressions.

2. Speaks using simple statements. Uses intonation to ask questions rather than

grammatically correct language.

3. Begins to combine statements to present a coherent argument or explanation.

Spoken sentences are generally a combination of ideas and not usually grammatically

correct.

4. Asks simple questions. Spoken sentences are sometimes grammatically correct.

5. Speaks fluently and coherently. Speech is generally but not always grammatically

correct. Listens attentively to the views of others and responds appropriately, taking

turns in the conversation.

Page 71: Performance Indicators in Primary Schools: A comparison of

71

© CEM Centre, Durham University 2014

Reference: DFE-RR344

ISBN: 978-1-78105-354-6

The views expressed in this report are the authors’ and do not necessarily reflect those of the

Department for Education

Any enquiries regarding this publication should be sent to us at:

[email protected] or www.education.gov.uk/contactus

This document is available for download at www.gov.uk/government/publications