88
1 PART II People and Technologies

PART_II

  • Upload
    kt8815

  • View
    215

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: PART_II

1

PART II

People and Technologies

Page 2: PART_II

2

PART II

Chapter 5. Understanding people 1: An introduction to cognitive psychologyChapter 7. Understanding people 2: Embodied, situated band distributed cognition

Page 3: PART_II

3

PART II

Goal: To consider some of the theoretical and practical foundations of designing interactive systems.Can be used individually, or as a set of three parts I-III:

to extend the material of Part I, orTo complement the method-based material in Part III.

Page 4: PART_II

4

Chapter 5:

Understanding people 1:An introduction to

cognitive psychology

Page 5: PART_II

5

Chapter 5

5.6 Visual perception5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception5.8 Depth perception5.9 Factors affecting perception5.10 Colour5.4 Memory5.5 Attention

Page 6: PART_II

6

Chapter 5 (2)

Introduces the role of psychology in the design of single-user interactive systems.⇒ Helps to understand:

The human information processing (HIP) view of human cognitionThe role of cognitive psychology in interactive systems designThe importance of memory, intention, perception and mental models to the design of interactive systems Criticism of the cognitive psychology approach

Page 7: PART_II

7

5.6 Visual perception

Can be considered as the best understood of all the forms of perception.Is concerned with extracting meaning, and hence recognition and understanding, from the light falling on our eyes.

Page 8: PART_II

8

5.6 Visual perception (2)

Sighted people

Perceiving a stable, 3-D, full colourworld filled with objects

⇒ How we perceive the world

Brain extracting and making sense of the sensory data pickup by our eyes

⇒ How it can be explained

Page 9: PART_II

9

5.6 Visual perception (3)

Visual perception

Theories of visual perception

Depth perception

Pattern recognition

DevelopmentalaspectsInterwoven

threads

Page 10: PART_II

10

5.6 Visual perception (4)

Content about the visual perception in the chapter:

Theoritical positions:Discussion of top-down visual perceptionAn account of Gibson’s direct perception theory

Application:Gestalt laws of visual perceptionTheir application to user interface design

Page 11: PART_II

11

5.6 Visual perception (5)

Understanding visual perception:19th century thinking of Helmholtz: We percrive the world by means of a serie of unconscious inferences⇒ A constructivist account of visual perception of Richard Gregory: We construct our perception of the world from some of the sensory data falling on our senses.

Page 12: PART_II

12

5.6 Visual perception (6)

Shape Shape constancyconstancy

ColourColourconstancyconstancy

Visual Visual illusionsillusions

Perceptual Perceptual constanciesconstancies

PerceptualPerceptualillusionsillusions

= =

Ability to perceive an object or a scene in an unchanged fashion illumination, viewpoint and so forth affecting the

information arriving at our senses

Page 13: PART_II

13

5.6 Visual perception (7)

Visual perception is studied because they are thought to be very revealing of how perception works by understanding what happens when perception does not work.Because the perception is seamless.

Page 14: PART_II

14

5.6 Visual perception (8)

Examples drawn by Gregory:

Müller-Lyer illusion:

Page 15: PART_II

15

5.6 Visual perception (9)

NeckerNecker cubescubes

⇒⇒ Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing= a form of unconscious inference= a form of unconscious inference

⇒⇒ We unconsciously form a hypothesis: the cube isWe unconsciously form a hypothesis: the cube is facing to the right/ leftfacing to the right/ left

But if we gaze the cube, it appears to turn insideBut if we gaze the cube, it appears to turn inside--out and back againout and back again

Page 16: PART_II

16

5.6 Visual perception (10)Gregory has produced in interesting and engaging account of visual perception.But:

How do we get started ?If visual perception relies on knowledge of the world, how do we bootstrap the process ?Because we can only acquire (visual) knowledge of the world from visual perception which relies on knowledge of the world.

Page 17: PART_II

17

5.6 Visual perception (11)

Direct perception:Gibson’s work: contrast to Gregory’s work.Ex: The pilot sitting in the fixed point experiences the world apparently flowing past him.

⇒ Optic array= flow of information : supplies unambigously all information relevant to the position, speed and altitude of the aircraft to the pilot.

⇒ No need for unconscious inferences or hypothesis testing.

⇒ The texture of the environment is expanding.

Page 18: PART_II

18

5.6 Visual perception (12)

Flow of optic arrayFlow of optic array

Page 19: PART_II

19

5.6 Visual perception (13)

Texture gradients provide important depth information.Ex:

pebbles on the beach, trees in a wood

Gibson: The environment provides all of information we required to experience it.

Page 20: PART_II

20

5.6 Visual perception (14)

So, in practice, according to psychologists, both theories merit:

Gibson: offers an account for optimal viewing conditions.Gregory: offers an account for restricted conditions.

Page 21: PART_II

21

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception

Gestaltists: a group of psychologists in the early of 20th century.Their law of perception, despite the age, map remarkbaly well onto a number of modern user interface design features.

Page 22: PART_II

22

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (2)

The laws:ProximityContinuityPart-whole relationshipSimilarityClosure

Page 23: PART_II

23

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (3)

Proximity:The objects appearing close together in space or time tend to be perceived together.Applied to auditory perception: the proximity of auditory ‘objects’is perceived as a song or a tune.

Page 24: PART_II

24

Page 25: PART_II

25

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (4)

Continuity:We tend to perceive the smooth, continuous patterns rather than disjoint, interrupted ones.

Page 26: PART_II

26

Page 27: PART_II

27

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (5)

Part-whole relationship:The whole is greater than the sum of its parts

HH

HH

HH

HHHH

HHHH

HH

HH

HH

HHHH

HHHH

HH HH HH

HHHH HH HH HH HH HHHHHHHH

HH HH HH HH HH HHHH

Page 28: PART_II

28

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (6)

Similarity:Similar figures to be tend to be grouped together.

Page 29: PART_II

29

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (7)

Closure:Closed figures are perceived more easily than incomplete or open figures.That feature is so strong that we even supply misinginformation to make a fifureeasier to perceive.

Page 30: PART_II

30

Page 31: PART_II

31

5.7 The Gestalt laws of perception (8)

Application of the Gestalt laws of perception:

Using proximity to organize buttonsUsing proximity to organize filesUsing continuity to connect disconnected elements. Ex: scrollbars of MS Windows and Mac.Using closure: We often unconsciously add missing info to close a figure Illustrating part-whole relationships: the larger figure is completed by a figure more detailed.

Page 32: PART_II

32

5.8 Depth perception

Is not particularly relevant to everyday office applicationsIs often essential to the effective design of games, multimedia applications and virtual reality systems.When designing to give the impression of three-dimensionality (a sense of depth and height), we need to understand how we pick up information from the environment which we intepret as depth and height.

Page 33: PART_II

33

5.8 Depth perception (2)

Depth perceptionDepth perception

Primary depth cuesPrimary depth cues Secondary depth cuesSecondary depth cues

Immersive Immersive

virtual reality systemsvirtual reality systems

NonNon--immersive immersive

applications: gamesapplications: games

Page 34: PART_II

34

5.8 Depth perception (3)

Cue= a means or mechanism which allows us to pick up information about environment.4 keys primary depth cues:

Retinal disparityStereopsisAccommodationConvergence

Page 35: PART_II

35

5.8 Depth perception (4)

Primary depth cuesPrimary depth cues

Retinal disparityRetinal disparity

StereopsisStereopsis

AccommodationAccommodation

ConvergenceConvergence

Make use 2 Make use 2 different retinal different retinal

images we have of images we have of the worldthe world

Rely on the muscle Rely on the muscle which control the which control the movement and movement and

focusing of our focusing of our eyseys

Page 36: PART_II

36

5.8 Depth perception (5)

Retinal disparity

Page 37: PART_II

37

5.8 Depth perception (5)

Retinal disparity: Each retina receives a slightly different image of the world when our eyes are approximately 7cm apart. This difference (Retinal disparity) is processed by the brain and interpreted as distance information

Page 38: PART_II

38

5.8 Depth perception (6)

Stereopsis: The process by which the different images of the world received by each eye are combined to produce a single 3-D experience.

Page 39: PART_II

39

5.8 Depth perception (7)

Accommodation: A muscular process by which we change the shape of the mens in our eyes in order create a sharply focussed image. We unconciously use information from these muscles to provide depth information.

Page 40: PART_II

40

5.8 Depth perception (8)

Convergence:Over distances of 2-7 metres we move our eys more and more inwards to focus on an object at these distances

⇒ To help provide additional distance information.

Page 41: PART_II

41

5.8 Depth perception (9)

Secondary depth cuesSecondary depth cues

Light and shadeLight and shade

Linear perspectiveLinear perspective

Height in horizontal planeHeight in horizontal plane

Motion parallaxMotion parallax

OverlapOverlap

Relative sizeRelative size

Texture gradientTexture gradient

Page 42: PART_II

42

5.8 Depth perception (10)

Secondary depth cues:Also called monocular depth cues

⇒ Rely only on one eye⇒ Are the basis for the

perception of depth on flat visual displays.

Page 43: PART_II

43

5.8 Depth perception (11)

Light and shade:An object with its attendant shdowimproves the sense of depth.

Page 44: PART_II

44

Page 45: PART_II

45

5.8 Depth perception (12)

Linear perspective: Using

shadow:shadow: wirewire--frame:frame:

Page 46: PART_II

46

5.8 Depth perception (13)

Height in horizontal plane:Distant objects appear higher (above the horizon) than nearby objects.

Page 47: PART_II

47

5.8 Depth perception (14)

Motion parallax (thị sai chuyển động):Depends upon movementEx: when looking out through a window in a fast-moving tain or car

⇒ nearby objects such as telegraph poles are seen to flash past very quickly

⇒ while a distant building moves muschmore slowly.

Page 48: PART_II

48

5.8 Depth perception (14b)

Page 49: PART_II

4949

5.8 Depth perception (15)5.8 Depth perception (15)

OverlapOverlap::An object which An object which obscures the sight of obscures the sight of another is understood another is understood to be nearer.to be nearer.

Page 50: PART_II

50

5.8 Depth perception (16)

Relative size:Smaller objects are usually seen as beignfurther away, particularly if the objects in the scene are approximately the same size.

Page 51: PART_II

51

5.8 Depth perception (17)

Texture gradient:Textured surfaces appear closer; irregulaties tend to be smoothed out over distance.

Page 52: PART_II

52

5.9 Factors affecting perception

Perceptual set= the things affect on how we perceive others, objects and situations.

Perceptual setPerceptual set

MotivationMotivation

Cultural factorsCultural factors

Individual differencesIndividual differencesPast experiencePast experience

AffectsAffects

ExpectationsExpectations

Page 53: PART_II

53

5.10 Colours

Page 54: PART_II

54

5.10 Colours (2)

How colour vision work:

lightlight--sensitive cellssensitive cells

Fovea of Fovea of retina retina ⊃⊃

120 millions120 millions

rodsrods

66--7 millions7 millions

conescones

Page 55: PART_II

55

5.10 Colours (2b)

conescones SensitiveSensitive

to to colourcolour

rodsrods Not sensitiveNot sensitive

to to colourcolour

Page 56: PART_II

56

5.10 Colours (3)

Red cones (64 %)Red cones (64 %)

Green cones (32 %)Green cones (32 %)

Blue cones (2 %)Blue cones (2 %)

The The colourcolour of cones of cones

reflects their reflects their

particular sensitivityparticular sensitivity

Page 57: PART_II

57

5.10 Colours (4)

The cones are also responsible for all high-resolution vision (as used in such thing as reading), which is why the eyes moves continually to keep the light from the object of interest falling on the fovea.

Page 58: PART_II

58

5.10 Colours (5)

Designing with colour:Colour is very important to us.Someone that is described colourlessseems to be considered without charcater or interest.Desiging colour into interactive systems is very difficult.

Page 59: PART_II

59

5.10 Colours (6)

Rules by Aeron Marcus, 1992:1. Use a maximum of 5 ± 2 colours2. Use foveal (central) and peripheral colours

appropriately3. Use a colour area that exhibits a minimum shift in

colour and / or size if the colour area changes in size.

4. Do not use simultaneous high-chroma, spectral colours

5. Use familiar, consistent colour codings with appropriate references

Page 60: PART_II

60

5.10 Colours (7)

• Colourconvention:– Guidelines

which may not suit every situation but should at the very least provide a sound starting point: NeutralityGrey, white

and blue

Status, background information, distance

Cool colours

Action, response required, proximity

Warm colours

Cold, water, calm, skyBlue

Go, okay, clear, vegetation, safety

Green

Caution, slow, testYellow

Danger, hot, fireRed

Page 61: PART_II

61

5.4 Memory

Has 2 major componentsShould be thought as a set of processes (ex: recall, recognition, chunking, reheasal) rather than as a database in your head. Appears to be multi-modal ⇒ remembers colours, sounds, the feel, the smellMemories in the brain are not stored as colours, sounds, the feel, the smell, but appears so when we recall them

Page 62: PART_II

62

5.4 Memory (2)

Human memoryHuman memory

WorkkingWorkking memorymemory

LongLong--term memoryterm memory

Page 63: PART_II

63

5.4 Memory (3)

Workking memory: = Short-term memory

Holding material for up to 30 sec. (short-lived nature)Very limited in size (limited capacity)Holding only 3-4 “chunk” of information (not 7 ± 2 items)

To maintain the contents of working memory ⇒ to rehearse it ⇒ to refresh mentally the contents

Page 64: PART_II

64

5.4 Memory (4)

The working memory supports 2 different modalities:

Visuo-spatial sketchpad in which the working memory can store a small amount of visual information ⇒ Visuo-spatial sketchpad= the mind’s eye ⇒ when we picture the face of someoneArticulatory loop in which the working memory can store a small amount of verbal information ⇒Articulatory loop = your inner voice ⇒ when we are signing to ourself.

Page 65: PART_II

65

5.4 Memory (5)

Long-term memory:The inverse of working memoryHave unlimited capacityCan last from a few minutes to a lifetimeHave multi-modal memories: To remember:

The smellThe tasteThe voiceThe feelThe soundThe words…

Page 66: PART_II

66

5.4 Memory (6)

Other long-term memorymemories: long-lived, but difficult to articulate. Ex:

Signing your nameRecognizing your signatureRiding a bicycleMaking a sandwichTyping…

Page 67: PART_II

67

5.4 Memory (7)

Recall and recognition:Recall= the process whereby individuals actively search their memories to retrieve a particular piece of information.Recognition:

Searching your memoryDeciding whether the piece of information matches what you have in your memory store.

Recognition is generally easier and quicker than recall.

Page 68: PART_II

68

5.4 Memory (8)

Ex: menu “Font” in Microsoft WordUse recognition rather than recallDirect manipulation (clicking on an item) rather than having to memorize sthg (ex: the name of a font)Extensive use of chunkingUse of meaningful natural mappingsUse iconsRely on visual processing

Page 69: PART_II

69

5.5 Attention

Usually defined in term of the focusing of mental resources at or on a particular task or object.Is a pivotally important human ability.Failures in attention are frequently cited reason for accidents of car, aircraft, control room, …⇒ We clearly need to be able to understand:

The mechanism of attentionIts capabilities and limitationsHow to design to make the most of these abilities while minimizing these limitations

Page 70: PART_II

70

5.5 Attention (2)

Attention can be directed at a particular task and/ or divided between a number of different tasks ⇒ We can perform:- A small number of simple tasks more or less

concurrently, or- One demanding task alone

⇒ Depending upon the characteristics of the task we are performingPractice reduces the amount of attention required by a particular task ⇒ freeing us to perform other tasks concurrently.Attention and awareness are closely linked.

Page 71: PART_II

71

5.5 Attention (3)

How attention work:Historically: 3 kinds of models developed by psychologists to account for attention ⇒ they do not agree with each another:

1. Single-chanel theory of attention2. Allocation model3. Controlled and automatic processing

Page 72: PART_II

72

5.5 Attention (4)

1. Single-chanel theory of attention:By oldest group, begin by Broadbent 1958, and then Triesman 1960, Deutsch and Deutsch 1963, Norman 1968There is a kind of mental switch or filter which select material either to be ignored or to which we pay attention.This switch or filter could be thought to be rathe rlike the tuning dial of a radio.

⇒ Not widely accepted today

Page 73: PART_II

73

5.5 Attention (5)

2. Allocation model:- Developed by Kahneman, 1973- We have a limited amount of processing power

at our disposal, and whether or not we are able to carry out a task depends on how much of this capacity is applied to the task.

⇒ More flexible and dynamic than the single-chanel models

⇒ Unable to describe how attention is chanelled or focussed.

⇒ Unable to define exactly what is meant by “capacity”.

Page 74: PART_II

74

5.5 Attention (6)

3. Controlled and automatic processing:By Schneider, 1977Distinguish between:

controlled processingautomatic attentional processing

Page 75: PART_II

75

5.5 Attention (7)

controlled processing:makes heavy demands on attenntional resourcesSlowLimited in capacityInvolves consciously directing attention towards a task.

automatic processing:makes no demands on attenntional resourcesFastUnaffected by capacity limitationsUnavoidable and diificult to modifyIs not subjetc to conscious awareness

Page 76: PART_II

76

5.5 Attention (8)

Until now:There is no one agreed account of attentionReasons:

Attetion has been studied in many different waysDifficult to define what actually constitute attention

⇒ There are 2 broad types of accounts of attention which proposed a limited capacity information processing system:- May / maynot have general purpose / specific

elements⇒ There is support for automatic tasks which do

not require attentional resources per se.

Page 77: PART_II

77

5.5 Attention (9)

Vigilance:Is an aspect of attentionRefers to detecting:

A rare event/ signal in a desert of inactivityNoise

Ex: Mariners watching for enemy submarinesRada operators watching for attacking aircrafts

The study of vigilance became importantMore usually people are required to be vigilant

⇒ Important: to understand our interaction with large complex systems, particularly with respect to the monitoring of susch systems.

Page 78: PART_II

78

5.5 Attention (10)

Designing alerts and warnings:2 different approaches:1. Unobtrusive display which expect the user

to notice the message but in their own time

2. Unobtrusive display, in contrast:• May interrupt the user’s work,• Requires intearctions- unless it is important,

urgent or life threatening• Allows the user to configure the application to

turn off such alerts

Page 79: PART_II

79

5.5 Attention (11)

Attention- grabbing techniques:

Must be used cautiously in some cases, according to more or less dramatic means.Ex: reporting the fire ≠ alerting the crew to the movie tape being jamed

Page 80: PART_II

80

5.5 Attention (12)

Wording and presentation of attention-getting alerts:

Require a little thought.Ex:

phrases containing a neagtiveexpressionPhrases in the passive voice take longer to read and understandIn an emergency situation: simple, direct and positive command

Page 81: PART_II

81

Chapter 7:

Understanding people 2:

Embodied, situated band distributed cognition

Page 82: PART_II

82

Chapter 7

7.3 Embodied interaction 3: Affordance

Page 83: PART_II

83

7.3 Embodied interaction 3: Affordance

(James Gibson, 1977) Affordance= a resource of support that the environment offers an animal; the animal in turn must possess the capabilities to perceive it and to use it.Ex:

surfaces that provide supportObjects that can be manipulatedSubsatnces that can be eatenOther animals that afford interactions of all kinds

Page 84: PART_II

84

7.3 Affordance (2)

In HCI: If we were able to design interactive systems which immediately presented their affordances to the user,

⇒ Then many, if not all, uability issues would be banished at a stroke.

⇒ ???

Page 85: PART_II

85

7.3 Affordance (3)

Donald Norman (1988):Introducing the concept of affordance to HCI:Replacing the original biological-environmental formulation with the perceived affordances.An user perceive the intended and perceived behaviours which are ususallyvery simple: sliding, pressing, rotating.

Page 86: PART_II

86

7.3 Affordance (4)

Perceived affordances tell the user:What actions can be performed on an object, andTo some extent, how to do them

Perceived affordances are often more about conventions than about reality. Ex: scrollbars.Real affordances are not nearly as important as perceived affordances.

Page 87: PART_II

87

Chapter 7

7.3 Embodied interaction: Affordance

Page 88: PART_II

88

5.6 Visual perception (2)