44
This article was downloaded by:[University of Oregon] On: 9 April 2008 Access Details: [subscription number 788774885] Publisher: Psychology Press Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Neuropsychological Rehabilitation An International Journal Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713684225 Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing individuals with neurogenic memory impairments: What have we learned in the past 20 years? Laurie A. Ehlhardt a ; Mckay Moore Sohlberg b ; Mary Kennedy c ; Carl Coelho d ; Mark Ylvisaker e ; Lyn Turkstra f ; Kathryn Yorkston g a The Teaching Research Institute-Eugene, Western Oregon University, Monmouth, USA b University of Oregon, Eugene, USA c University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA d University of Connecticut, Storrs, USA e College of Saint Rose, Albany, New York, USA f University of Wisconsin, Madison, USA g University of Washington, Seattle, USA First Published on: 26 February 2008 To cite this Article: Ehlhardt, Laurie A., Sohlberg, Mckay Moore, Kennedy, Mary, Coelho, Carl, Ylvisaker, Mark, Turkstra, Lyn and Yorkston, Kathryn (2008) 'Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing individuals with neurogenic memory impairments: What have we learned in the past 20 years?', Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1 To link to this article: DOI: 10.1080/09602010701733190 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602010701733190 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

This article was downloaded by:[University of Oregon]On: 9 April 2008Access Details: [subscription number 788774885]Publisher: Psychology PressInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Neuropsychological RehabilitationAn International JournalPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713684225

Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructingindividuals with neurogenic memory impairments: Whathave we learned in the past 20 years?Laurie A. Ehlhardt a; Mckay Moore Sohlberg b; Mary Kennedy c; Carl Coelho d;Mark Ylvisaker e; Lyn Turkstra f; Kathryn Yorkston ga The Teaching Research Institute-Eugene, Western Oregon University, Monmouth,USAb University of Oregon, Eugene, USAc University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USAd University of Connecticut, Storrs, USAe College of Saint Rose, Albany, New York, USA

f University of Wisconsin, Madison, USAg University of Washington, Seattle, USA

First Published on: 26 February 2008To cite this Article: Ehlhardt, Laurie A., Sohlberg, Mckay Moore, Kennedy, Mary, Coelho, Carl, Ylvisaker, Mark,Turkstra, Lyn and Yorkston, Kathryn (2008) 'Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing individuals with neurogenicmemory impairments: What have we learned in the past 20 years?', Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 1To link to this article: DOI: 10.1080/09602010701733190URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602010701733190

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction,re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expresslyforbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will becomplete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with orarising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Evidence-based practice guidelines for instructing

individuals with neurogenic memory impairments:

What have we learned in the past 20 years?

Laurie A. Ehlhardt1, McKay Moore Sohlberg2, Mary Kennedy3,

Carl Coelho4, Mark Ylvisaker5, Lyn Turkstra6 and,

Kathryn Yorkston7

1The Teaching Research Institute-Eugene, Western Oregon University,

Monmouth, USA; 2University of Oregon, Eugene, USA; 3University of

Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA; 4University of Connecticut, Storrs, USA; 5College

of Saint Rose, Albany, New York, USA; 6University of Wisconsin, Madison, USA;7University of Washington, Seattle, USA

This article examines the instructional research literature pertinent to teaching

procedures or information to individuals with acquired memory impairments

due to brain injury or related conditions. The purpose is to evaluate the avail-

able evidence in order to generate practice guidelines for clinicians working in

the field of cognitive rehabilitation. A systematic review of the instructional lit-

erature from 1986 to 2006 revealed 51 studies meeting search criteria. Studies

were analysed and coded within the following four key domains: Population

Sample, Intervention, Study Design, and Treatment Outcomes. Coding

included 17 characteristics of the population sample; seven intervention par-

ameters; five study design features; and five treatment outcome parameters.

Interventions that were evaluated included systematic instructional techniques

such as method of vanishing cues and errorless learning. The majority of the

Correspondence should be address to McKay Moore Sohlberg, Associate Professor/Communication Disorders and Sciences, 5285 University of Oregon Eugene, OR 97403

USA. E-mail: [email protected]

The authors gratefully acknowledge colleagues who participated in the ANCDS guidelines

peer review process. They also wish to express sincere appreciation to all the first authors

who so generously gave their time to provide edits and ensure that the review accurately

reflected their work. Finally, the authors thank Cathy Thomas and Laura Beck of the Teaching

Research Institute-Eugene and Rik Lemoncello of the University of Oregon for their assistance

in the preparation of this manuscript.

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION

iFirst, 1–43

# 2008 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business

http://www.psypress.com/neurorehab DOI:10.1080/09602010701733190

Page 3: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction.

However, issues related to the design and execution of effective instruction

lack clarity and require further study. The interaction between the target learn-

ing objective and the individual learner profile is not well understood. The evi-

dence review concludes with clinical recommendations based on the

instructional literature and a call to clinicians to incorporate these methods

into their practice to maximise patient outcomes.

Keywords: Evidence based practice guidelines; Memory impairment; Cogni-

tive rehabilitation; Instruction; Errorless learning.

INTRODUCTION

This paper is one of a series of publications by the traumatic brain injury

(TBI) Practice Guidelines Subcommittee established by the Academy of

Neurologic Communication Disorders and Sciences (ANCDS). The commit-

tee was charged with generating evidence-based practice guidelines for

cognitive-communication disorders resulting from TBI. This article examines

the instructional research literature pertinent to teaching procedures or infor-

mation to individuals with acquired memory impairments due to TBI or

related conditions. The purpose is to evaluate the available evidence in

order to generate practice guidelines on instructional methods for clinicians

working in the field of cognitive rehabilitation. (See www.ancds.org/practice.html for publications by this and other subcommittees.)

Why do clinicians need practice guidelines on instruction? Instruction is

central to a variety of cognitive intervention approaches. Whether training

the use of compensatory systems or strategies or facilitating learning of facts

or concepts, clinicians are charged with designing and delivering effective

instruction. Unfortunately, most rehabilitation professionals receive little train-

ing in instructional theory and design. In a survey of medical speech-language

pathologists working in clinical settings throughout the United States, only one-

third reported following an evidence-based instructional approach and nearly

half reported tallying data in their heads and making clinical decisions based

on intuition and experience (Lemoncello & Sohlberg, 2005).

Over the past 20 years, there has been mounting evidence that different

instructional techniques can facilitate learning in individuals with memory

impairment due to acquired neurological conditions. Also accumulating is

evidence that neuronal plasticity can occur in response to structured input

(e.g., Gonzalez-Rothi, 2001, 2006; Laatsch, Thulborn, & Krisky, 2004).

Increasing our understanding and implementation of effective methods for

structuring input via instruction and training practices can facilitate experi-

ence-dependent learning in cognitive-linguistic rehabilitation (Ducharme &

Spencer, 2001; Sohlberg, 2006). In addition to improving rehabilitation

2 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 4: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

outcomes, there are pragmatic benefits to adhering to evidence-based

instructional practices. In an era of reduced funding for cognitive rehabilita-

tion, well-designed and delivered instruction may be key to facilitating

efficient and enduring positive outcomes (Sohlberg &Mateer, 2001; Ylvisaker,

Hanks, & Johnson-Green, 2003).

Review of findings from previous meta-analyses

Special education possesses an abundant literature evaluating instructional

techniques that is highly relevant to teaching people with acquired cognitive

impairments. Several meta-analyses have attempted to parse out the most

effective instructional practices and components within the special education

literature (Adams & Engelmann, 2006; Kavale & Forness, 2000; Mastropieri,

Scruggs, Bakken, & Whedon, 1996; Swanson, 1999, 2001; Swanson, Carson,

& Sachse-Lee, 1996; Swanson & Hoskyn, 1998). Of particular interest to the

current paper are the series of meta-analyses by Swanson and colleagues

(1996–2001). Following an extensive search yielding over 900 data-based

articles, the authors selected 180 studies that met the inclusion criteria

(e.g., comparison/control group; sufficient data to calculate effect sizes).

These studies were categorised into one of four groups based on the instruc-

tional techniques that were employed. The results indicated that the

Combined Model that used both Direct Instruction and Strategy Instruction

techniques in concert produced the largest effect size. Strategy Instruc-

tion alone, Direct Instruction alone, and Nondirect/Nonstrategy instruction

showed respectively smaller effect sizes. These meta-analyses are part of a

rich experimental literature within the field of special education that supports

the use of explicit instructional techniques to effectively teach individuals

with learning disabilities (Sohlberg, Ehlhardt, & Kennedy, 2005).

Strategy instruction is a specific instructional arena that special education

has carefully evaluated. Graham and Harris (2003) conducted a meta-analysis

supporting the effectiveness of Self-Regulated Strategy Development (execu-

tive function/self-regulation scripts embedded within the curricular delivery)

for a variety of students with and without disability. Similarly, Kim, Vaughn,

Wanzek, and Wei (2004) provide a meta-analysis of graphic organisers used

with a variety of students with and without disability. The burgeoning work in

special education on current practices in strategy instruction has influenced

the brain injury field; however, this is an area that needs further attention.

The current review of instructional practices limits its consideration of specific

strategy instruction due to the lack of research involving the brain injury popu-

lation; clinicians and researchers are encouraged to examine the available

cross-disciplinary evidence. Executive function strategy and problem-

solving interventions specific to the brain injury population are detailed in a

recent practice guidelines manuscript (Kennedy et al., 2008 this issue).

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 3

Page 5: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

The field of neuropsychology has built upon the special education research

and evaluated instructional design and teaching procedures in the population

of people with acquired neurogenic memory disorders. A meta-analysis eval-

uated the effects of errorless learning and the method of vanishing cues across

11 studies (Kessels & deHaan, 2003). This analysis revealed that error elim-

ination strategies during the learning process produced a moderate effect size

of .59 when compared to trial and error strategies. This analysis also showed

a large effect size (.87) for studies specifically evaluating errorless learning

and a small effect size (.27) for those evaluating the method of vanishing

cues (.27); however, the relatively few studies in the latter category may

have affected these results. Overall, this meta-analysis encourages more

evaluations of the application of errorless learning techniques to real world

settings.

The current report provides a detailed review of the instructional literature

from 1986 to 2006 relevant to the treatment of the neurogenic population with

acquired memory disorders. Because instruction represents a broad interven-

tion domain applied to a wide variety of cognitive interventions, this paper

does not utilise the standard classification system of practice standards,

guidelines, and options (Miller et al., 1999); instead, it describes general

instructional practices based on the research evidence.

METHODS

Writing Committee process

The ANCDS TBI Practice Guidelines Committee was formulated to generate

practice guidelines for working with persons living with the effects of trau-

matic brain injury. The committee initially established a philosophy and

rationale for the purpose and use of evidence-based guidelines (Kennedy

et al., 2002) including identifying factors critical to scientific clinical

decision-making (Ylvisaker et al., 2002). Priorities for domains of practice

guidelines were also established. To date, the committee has reviewed the

literature and generated evidence-based guidelines in the following clinical

domains: use of direct attention training (Sohlberg et al., 2003); use of exter-

nal aids to manage memory impairments (Sohlberg et al., 2007); use of stan-

dardised assessment in traumatic brain injury (Turkstra et al., 2005);

behavioural interventions (Ylvisaker et al., 2007); and executive function

interventions (Kennedy et al., 2008 this issue). Each paper included Tables

of Evidence with agreed upon coding categories that allowed scrutiny and

comparison of research reports. Authors analysed the evidence in order to

develop clinical practice guidelines. Prior to submission, each paper was

sent for external review to the ANCDS membership and to first authors

4 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 6: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

whose studies were analysed in the paper. Feedback was incorporated, and the

paper was submitted to the peer review process for the relevant journal. We

followed this protocol for the current guidelines paper. We received a total of

seven peer reviews. First author (and in one case, second author) reviews

were requested from researchers who had authored two or more papers

included in the evidence base, comprising a total of six researchers. All six

review solicitations were accepted and returned with comments, thus ensur-

ing that the interpretation and coding of their studies was accurate.

The goal of this paper is to identify effective instructional practices in cog-

nitive rehabilitation that the research literature supports with a reasonable

degree of certainty. In order to be inclusive in our search and evaluate the

available evidence relevant to populations with functionally equivalent cog-

nitive-linguistic impairments, we made the decision in this paper to include

cross-population studies. Clients with traumatic brain injury, the target popu-

lation of the guidelines subcommittee, represent a complex, heterogeneous

group in which cognitive, communication, and behavioural impairments

interact and are differentially affected by changing environments. Because

memory impairments following TBI are not a uniform condition specific to

this diagnosis, it is important to consider other sources to verify treatment

methods (Ylvisaker et al., 2002). Thus, the authors of this paper reviewed lit-

erature that included the following aetiologies, all of which commonly exhibit

memory and/or learning impairments as a central symptom: acquired brain

injury (TBI plus other neurogenic aetiologies such as cerebral vascular acci-

dents, anoxic events, brain tumour, and neuro-infectious diseases), dementia,

and schizophrenia. These categories were consistent with the breakdown in

the research literature. The decision to group a variety of aetiologies into

an acquired brain injury category was made for two reasons. First, the

review of the literature did not support systematic differences in the outcomes

of intervention. Second, a number of studies evaluated mixed participant

groups, making it difficult to parse out aetiology-specific findings within

the acquired brain injury population. Schizophrenia is not a neurogenic dis-

order; however, there is increasing evidence that this population experiences

difficulties with new learning and memory similar to people with acquired

brain injury (McKenna, Clare, & Baddeley, 1995). Examination of the

instructional literature for this population reinforces a number of instructional

recommendations for people with memory impairments from a broad spec-

trum of aetiologies.

Identifying, gathering, and extracting intervention studies

The following databases were searched for articles published between 1986

and 2006: Academic Search Premier, Education Research Complete, ERIC,

Medline, Psychology and Behavioural Sciences Collection, and PsycINFO.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 5

Page 7: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

We restricted our search to the past 20 years since this period included all of

the primary instructional methods investigated with the target populations. Key

combinations of the following terms were used: brain injury, head injury,

head trauma, brain trauma, dementia, schizophrenia, amnesia, memory,

instruction, errorless learning, spaced retrieval, expanded rehearsal, distributed

practice, vanishing cues, trial and error, errorful, discovery learning, standard

anticipation, backward-forward chaining, memory, executive function, dysex-

ecutive, cognitive, computers, computer assisted instruction, and remediation.

The search methods initially yielded 2155 records narrowed to 857 records

for review. Hand searches of extant references (i.e., studies cited within an

article) were also conducted. Abstracts or full articles were then reviewed,

applying agreed upon selection criteria. Studies were included whose partici-

pants had acquired memory impairment as their primary cognitive deficit due

to a variety of aetiologies, including acquired brain injury (ABI) (e.g., TBI,

cerebrovascular accident, encephalitis), dementia (Alzheimer’s or vascular

type), or schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Only studies that evaluated

the application of instruction or training to the learning (or relearning) of

information or procedures were included. Studies also had to provide original

quantitative and/or qualitative (i.e., case study) data. When it was evident

that two different studies (whether published separately or in the same

article) evaluated the same participants, those participants were counted

twice. Studies were excluded that: (1) applied instruction or training pro-

cedures to cognitive impairments other than memory (e.g., use of errorless

learning to treat anomia); (2) reported findings from a study reviewed

earlier using the same participants; or (3) consisted of reviews of other articles

without presenting original data. Forty-eight studies met the above criteria

and were selected for final review. We incorporated three more studies

based on first author reviews for a total of 51 studies.

As part of the ANCDS practice guidelines developed for individuals with

Alzheimer’s dementia, Hopper et al. (2005) conducted a systematic review of

the spaced retrieval literature for this population; hence, those studies were

not included in this review. However, studies evaluating spaced retrieval in

the TBI population were identified and included in this review, as well as

those studies within the dementia population that were not included in the

review by Hopper and colleagues.

Reviewing and coding studies into Tables of Evidence

Tables of evidence were organised by identifying relevant parameters within

the following four key domains: population sample, intervention, study

design, and treatment outcomes. Coding included 17 characteristics of the

population sample; seven intervention parameters; five study design features;

and five treatment outcome parameters (see Table 1 for a summary of the

6 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 8: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

TABLE 1

Summary of the number and percentage of studies reporting population

characteristics, intervention parameters, design features, and outcomes

(N ¼ 51)

Number and (percent) of studies reported

Type of evidence

Class I 1 (2)

Class II 6 (12)

Class III 26 (51)

Class IV 15 (29)

Class III & IV 3 (6)

Demographic variables

Number 51 (100)

Age 50 (98)

Male:Female 41 (80)

Aetiology 51 (100)

Site of injury 20 (39)

Time post-onset 39 (76)

Neuropsychological tests 51 (100)

Premorbid IQ 16 (31)

Initial severity 12 (24)

Memory severity 41 (80)

Dual diagnosis/co-morbidity 20 (39)

Selection criteria 29 (57)

Treatment history 9 (18)

Medication 11 (22)

Education 29 (57)

Occupation 19 (37)

Living Situation 28 (55)

Intervention

Instructional approach 51 (100)

Additional components 24 (47)

Treatment targets I ¼ 33 (65) P ¼ 10 (20)

I & P ¼ 8 (17)

Treatment dosage 51 (100)

Treatment setting 25 (49)

Treatment provider 26 (51)

Measurement 51 (100)

Study design

Design 51 (100)

Statistics 41 (80)

Reliability/validity 7 (14)

Outcomes

Ecological validity 14 (27)

Immediate outcomes 100 (100)

Generalisation 22 (43)

Maintenance 30 (59)

I ¼ Information; P ¼ Procedures.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 7

Page 9: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

number and percentage of studies reporting population characteristics,

intervention parameters, design features, and outcomes). The full Tables of

Evidence are presented in three appendices. Appendix A displays the coded

population sample characteristics, Appendix B displays the coded interven-

tion characteristics, and Appendix C displays the coded parameters related

to study design and outcomes. There are three possible types of coding.

Some parameters list the relevant study content; other parameters contain a

“1” indicating the study reported the parameter or a “0” indicating the

study did not report it; and, as described later, intervention outcomes are

coded with þ, /, or – indicating positive, qualified, or negative findings,

respectively. Each appendix groups the three diagnostic categories separately

(ABI—38 studies; dementia—7 studies; and schizophrenia/schizoaffectivedisorder—6 studies) to allow for population-specific analyses. Studies

within each of the three diagnostic groups are ordered chronologically in

the appendices

Clinical practice recommendations must strongly consider the strength of

the research evidence. Therefore, in Appendix C, each study was also rated

according to the revised American Academy of Neurology (AAN) classifi-

cation system (i.e., Class I–IV evidence) (American Academy of Neurology,

2004; www.aan.org). In this system, Class I studies include prospective,

randomised controlled clinical trials with masked outcome assessment in a

representative population with qualifiers. Class II studies are prospective

matched group cohort studies with masked outcome assessment in a represen-

tative population that meet Class I criteria or randomised controlled trials that

lack one criterion for Class I evidence. Class III studies include all other con-

trolled trials where outcome assessment is independent of patient treatment in

a representative population. Class IV is evidence from uncontrolled studies,

such as case reports or expert opinion.

The first author coded all 51 intervention studies. Reliability coding with

the second author was conducted for 26 out of the total of 51 studies (51%)

for all coding categories, except for five parameters: the type of instruction

and four of the outcome components (immediate outcomes, ecological val-

idity, generalisation and maintenance). The second author coded these four

parameters for all 51 studies. There were nine points of disagreement

related to categorising types of instruction (e.g., discriminating between

errorless learning and systematic instructional packages), eight points of dis-

agreement related to determining immediate outcomes (e.g., discriminating

between a “qualified” versus “negative” outcome), one point of disagreement

related to the determination of ecological validity, and no points of disagree-

ment related to generalisation and maintenance outcomes. Inter-rater

reliability for these four parameters averaged 90%, with overall reliability

at 98% for the 26 studies reviewed in their entirety. Disagreements were

easily resolved by discussion, with components re-categorised accordingly.

8 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 10: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

RESULTS

Population sample characteristics

This section summarises relevant population sample characteristics across

the three population groups (see Appendix A for details).1

Number of participants, age, gender, aetiology, site of injury,

time post-onset

Acquired brain injury. There were 451 experimental participants with

memory impairment, 42 control participants with cognitive disabilities, and

163 non-disabled control participants across the 38 studies. Ages ranged

from 18 to 78 years, with two studies involving children 8–11 years old.

The experimental participants were predominantly adult males (.2:1). The

aetiology of memory impairment included a wide range of conditions: TBI,

CVA (including anterior and posterior communicating artery haemorrhage),

encephalitis, brain tumour, Korsakoff’s syndrome, anoxia, brain abscess,

Parkinson’s disease, toxicity, encephalopathy, and cerebral infection. Site

of injury and/or imaging findings were reported in 17/38 (45%) of the

studies. Time post-onset (TPO) was reported in 31/38 (82%) of the studies

with the majority of participants at least one year post-onset and others

several years post-onset (e.g., 15 years). Only three studies reported on par-

ticipants with symptom onset of less than one year (Dou, Man, Ou, Sheng,

& Tam, 2006; Glisky & Delaney, 1996; Wilson, Baddeley, Evans, & Shiel,

1994).

Dementia. Across the seven studies, there were 55 experimental partici-

pants and eight non-disabled control participants, ranging in age from 65

to 89 years. The participants were predominantly male, with Alzheimer’s

dementia as the most common aetiology, followed by vascular dementia.

Imaging findings were reported in 3/7 (43%) of the studies (Clare et al.,

2002; Haslam et al., 2006; Winter & Hunkin, 1999). Symptom onset date was

reported in 2/7 (29%) of the studies, and ranged from 18 months to 5 years

prior to the study (Clare et al., 2000; Haslam, Gilroy, Black, & Beesley, 2006).

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Across the six studies, therewere

203 participants with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder and memory

impairment, 21 control participants with schizophrenia/schizoaffective

1To limit manuscript space devoted to citations in the results section, the first two authors

selected and cited the two most representative studies to illustrate certain evidence parameters.

For objective evidence parameters (e.g., demographics) with more than three studies, percen-

tages were reported without citations.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 9

Page 11: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

disorder without memory impairment, and 88 non-disabled control

participants. The experimental participants were predominantly male, and

ages ranged from 18 to 55 years, with the majority of participants in the

30–40 year age range. Site of injury/imaging findings were not reported for

this population. Symptom onset date was determined by the date of first

hospitalisation relative to the start of the study, which ranged from 1 month

to 34.7 years prior.

Neuropsychological testing, premorbid IQ, initial severity, memory

severity, dual-diagnosis/co-morbidity, selection criteria

Acquired brain injury. Neuropsychological test data were available in all

38 studies; however, the type and number of measures used varied consider-

ably. Premorbid IQ was reported in 10/38 (26%) of the studies. Initial sever-

ity (length of coma and/or post-traumatic amnesia) was reported in 12/38(32%) of the studies and was not relevant to participants in several studies

given the aetiology (e.g., Korsakoff’s syndrome). Severity of memory impair-

ment was reported in 33/38 (87%) of the studies. Of these, 18/33 (55%)

reported the levelof impairment as “severe”, “profound”, “marked”, “significant”,

or “chronic, affecting everyday memory”. Ten out of 33 (30%) of the studies

included participants with a range of severity levels: “mild”, “moderate”, and

“severe”, while in other studies, severity was implied on the basis of cut-off

scores. Few studies clearly linked performance on standardised memory

assessments and screening tools to the described levels of memory impair-

ment (e.g., Page, Wilson, Shiel, Carter, & Norris, 2006; Tailby & Haslam,

2003). In addition, few studies described the specific type of memory impair-

ment. Descriptors reported typically included “anterograde” or “episodic”

memory impairments (e.g., Andrewes & Gielewski, 1999; Komatsu,

Mimura, Kato, Wakamatsu, & Kashima, 2000).

Dual-diagnosis/co-morbidity data were reported in 17/38 (45%) of the

studies. In addition to the memory impairments described above, other cog-

nitive impairments reported included: executive function and attention

impairments; disorientation; anomia and other language impairments; visuo-

perceptual disorders; motor speech impairments or motor slowing; diabetes;

and/or seizures (e.g., Glang, Singer, Cooley, & Tish, 1992; Glisky, Schacter,

& Tulving, 1986a, 1986b). For several of the remaining studies, participants

were excluded on the basis of a dual-diagnosis or co-morbidity. (Note: As so

few studies reported on vision, hearing, and motor status, these data were

included in this category when available.) Selection criteria were reported

in 16/38 (42%) studies.

Dementia. Neuropsychological test data were available across all seven

studies. Premorbid IQ was reported in 6/7 studies (86%). Two of the seven

10 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 12: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

studies included only participants with minimal or mild dementia (Clare,

Roth, Wilson, Carter, & Hodges, 2002; Clare et al., 2000), while the remain-

ing studies included those with mixed severities or moderate–severe demen-

tia only (e.g., Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005; Ruis & Kessels, 2005).

Six of the seven studies (86%) described severity of memory impairment

with participants having “significant”, “severe”, or “profound” memory

impairments. Dual-diagnosis/co-morbidity data were provided in 3/7(43%) studies (Haslam et al., 2006; Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005;

Winter & Hunkin, 1999). In addition to memory impairment, other cognitive

impairments included general decline in intellectual ability, impaired concen-

tration, disorientation, and word-finding difficulties (Haslam et al., 2006;

Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005; Winter & Hunkin, 1999). Selection cri-

teria were reported in 6/7 (86%) studies.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Neuropsychological testing was

completed in all six studies; however, the number of measures used was often

limited, thus precluding detailed descriptions of participant profiles. Premorbid

IQ was not reported for this population group. Severity of memory impair-

ment was reported in only two (33%) of the studies and determined via

cut-off scores (Kern et al., 2005; O’Carroll, Russell, Lawrie, & Johnstone,

1999). Selection criteria were reported in 6/6 (100%) studies.

Treatment history, medication status, education, occupation, living

situation

Acquired brain injury. Treatment history was reported in 9/38 (24%) of

the studies. Medication status was rarely reported – 2/38 (1%) studies

(Andrewes & Gielewski, 1999; Parkin, Hunkin, & Squires, 1998). Education

levels were reported in 21/38 (55%) of the studies with the majority of the

adult participants completing high school education, and many achieving

some level of post-secondary education as well. Occupational history was

available in 13/38 (34%) of the studies, and included reports of participant

employment post-intervention (e.g., Andrewes & Gielewski, 1999; Hillary

et al., 2003). Post-injury living situation was reported in 18/38 (47%)

studies, and included home/community dwelling, assisted living centres,

and long-term care residence.

Dementia. Treatment history was not reported. Medication status was

reported in 3/7 studies (43%) (Clare et al., 2002; Dunn & Clare, 2007;

Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005). Education status was reported in 2/7(29%) studies (Haslam et al., 2006; Ruis & Kessels, 2005), with participants

having completed up to 10 years of education. Occupational history was

reported in 4/7 (57%) studies, with participants having worked in a variety

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 11

Page 13: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

of jobs (e.g., business owners, clerical, engineers) prior to their retirement.

Living situation was reported in 6/7 (86%) studies, with the majority of par-

ticipants living with a family member.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Treatment history was not

reported. Medication status was reported in all six studies and included

anti-psychotic and neuroleptic medications. Education levels in all six

studies were reported, with the majority of participants completing at least

high school education. Occupational history was reported in only 1/6(17%) studies (Kern, Liberman, Kopelowicz, Mintz, & Green, 2002).

Living status was not consistently reported but was implied on the basis of

the described treatment setting (e.g., in-patient hospital setting; outpatient

clinic, community dwelling).

To summarise, the description of population characteristics varied widely

within and between the three population groups. The majority of the studies

provided sufficient information to develop a basic profile of study partici-

pants; however, there were notable gaps in the literature. For example,

while the majority of the studies included neuropsychological testing, clear

linkages between assessment data and the determination of both the type

and severity of memory impairment were limited. This trend in combination

with the relative lack of information in other domains such as treatment

history and medication status limit the extent to which results can be gener-

alised to individuals beyond those involved in the studies (i.e., external

validity).

Intervention

This section summarises the types of instructional methodologies, additional

training components, treatment targets, dosage, treatment settings and provi-

ders, as well as outcome measurements across the 51 studies (see Appendix B

for details).

Instructional methods

Two broad instructional categories emerged from the review: (1) systema-

tic instructional methods (e.g., method of vanishing cues, errorless learning,

and spaced retrieval); and (2) conventional methods (e.g., errorful learning/trial and error learning). The primary goal of errorless learning is to eliminate

errors during the acquisition phase of learning by providing models before

a client attempts a response; guessing is discouraged (Baddeley & Wilson,

1994). The method of vanishing cues (MVC) is a form of chaining that

provides the client with progressively stronger or weaker cues following

recall attempts of the targeted information/procedures (Glisky, Schacter, &Tulving, 1986a, b). Spaced retrieval (i.e., expanded rehearsal) is a form of

12 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 14: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

distributed practice that provides individuals with severe memory impairment

practice at successfully recalling information over expanded time intervals

(Melton & Bourgeois, 2005). Spaced retrieval incorporates errorless learning

but follows a prescribed practice regimen. In general, these systematic

instructional methods emphasise explicit, carefully faded models/prompts.

The conventional methods (i.e., trial-and-error learning, errorful learning,

standard anticipation) emphasise recall of the targeted information or skill

without prior models/prompts, with the trainer providing the models only

following errors.

Studies were grouped into the following six intervention categories and

comparison conditions:

1. Errorless learning (EL). The study evaluated errorless learning with no

comparison condition or group.

2. Errorless learning versus errorful learning (EL vs. EF). The study

compared EL vs. EF, a comparison group and/or an earlier EL training

protocol.

3. Method of vanishing cues (MVC). The study evaluated MVC with no

comparison condition or group.

4. Method of vanishing cues versus errorful or errorless learning (EF/EL). The study compared MVC with a comparison condition, such as

errorful learning or errorless learning.

5. Spaced retrieval/spaced presentations (SR). The study primarily eval-

uated spaced retrieval or spaced presentation of information.

6. Systematic instructional packages. The study evaluated a combination

of systematic techniques integrated into an instructional package and/or used a staged-learning process (e.g., Phase 1 ¼ acquisition phase,

Phase 2 ¼ application phase) with or without a comparison con-

dition/group.

The target systematic instructional methodologies varied across the three

aetiology groups as described in the following section.

Acquired brain injury. The target systematic instructional methodologies

varied for the ABI group, with comparisons of EL versus EF and systematic

instructional packages as the predominant methods (11 and 13 studies,

respectively); one study evaluated EL without a comparison condition/group (Parkin et al., 1998). There were five studies evaluating MVC with

no comparison and five with a comparison, while three studies focused

primarily on spaced retrieval/spaced presentations.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 13

Page 15: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Dementia. Three studies compared EL versus EF (Haslam et al., 2006;

Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005; Ruis & Kessels, 2005), three evaluated

systematic instructional packages (Clare et al., 2000, 2002; Dunn & Clare,

2007), and one study evaluated EL alone (Winter & Hunkin, 1999).

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Five studies compared EL

versus EF, while one study evaluated two different systematic instructional

packages with a control group (Young et al., 2002).

Across groups, documentation of specific training procedures for each

instructional methodology varied. Several studies provided sufficient detail to

allow for replication (e.g., Andrewes & Gielewski, 1999; Glisky & Schacter,

1989; Hunkin, Squires, Aldrich, & Parkin, 1998).

Additional instructional components

Several studies emphasised instructional components the researchers

hypothesised would increase the participants’ active processing of the

targeted information/procedures, while keeping errors to a minimum.

These components took the form of either a strategy (e.g., verbal elaboration,

imagery, prediction-reflection, evaluative questioning/self-generation of

responses) or an emphasis on stimulus manipulation (e.g., varied training

examples for the targeted information/task step or stimulus pre-exposure).

Acquired brain injury. Eight studies included a strategy component (e.g.,

Evans et al., 2000; Tailby & Haslam, 2003), while 12 studies emphasised

stimulus manipulation (e.g., Glisky & Schacter, 1987; Stark, Stark, &

Gordon, 2005).

Dementia. Almost half of the studies (3/7) included a strategy com-

ponent (Clare et al., 2000, 2002; Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005),

while none of the studies explicitly described stimulus manipulation.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. One out of the six studies (17%)

emphasised strategy instruction (Young, Zakzanis, Campbell, Freyslinger, &

Meichenbaum, 2002), while four of the studies described stimulus manipu-

lation (e.g., Kern et al., 2002, 2005).

Instructional targets

Instructional targets were grouped into two categories: information and

procedures. Information targets included: word lists, face–name associations,

facts, object names, definitions, concepts, and curricula content (e.g., maths,

reading). Procedures included a variety of multi-step tasks, such as: index

card filing, word processing, data entry, programming electronic aids,

14 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 16: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

external memory aid use, and route finding. Familiarity of the instructional

targets varied with both unfamiliar and/or familiar information and pro-

cedures trained across the three population groups.

Acquired brain injury. Most of the studies (24/38; 63%) focused on

training information, while 7/38 (18%) targeted procedures, and 7/38(18%) targeted both.

Dementia. The majority of the studies (6/7; 86%) targeted information

while one study (Clare et al., 2000) evaluated instruction that targeted both

information and procedures.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Half of the studies (3/6) tar-

geted information and the other half taught procedures.

Treatment dosage

Treatment dosage refers to the frequency of the treatment sessions and the

duration of treatment, both in terms of length of session and the total amount

of time participants received treatment.

Acquired brain injury. Treatment frequency ranged from one session

only to daily sessions. Treatment duration was also quite varied, with sessions

lasting a minimum of 30 minutes up to 2 hours with total duration, when

specified, from one week up to several months.

Dementia. Treatment frequency ranged from a minimum of one training

session in each condition (2 total) up to 16 sessions. Total duration, when

reported, ranged from one week up to four weeks.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Treatment frequency ranged

from one session only up to six sessions, with durations up to four weeks.

Treatment settings and providers

Treatment settings and treatment providers were not consistently reported

across the three population groups (49% and 53%, respectively).

Acquired brain injury. Half of the studies (19/38) reported treatment

settings. These studies described a range of settings, including laboratory or

clinic settings, work-sites, and home-based treatment delivered via phone

(Melton & Bourgeois, 2005). Treatment providers were reported in 20/38(53%) of the studies and included experimenters and family members, as

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 15

Page 17: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

well as computer-delivered stimuli, common to the studies by Glisky and

colleagues (e.g., 1986–1995).

Dementia. Treatment settings and providers were reported in 1/7 (14%)

of the studies (Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005) that described a combi-

nation of clinic and home-based therapy delivered by both the experimenter

and spouses.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Treatment settings and provi-

ders were reported in 5/6 (83%) of the studies, with the majority conducted

in hospital or clinic settings. Experimenters, supervisors and assistants

provided the treatment.

Measurement

Different types of intervention outcome measurements were used across

the three population groups, with the majority of studies incorporating

more than one type. The most frequently used measure was the number,

percentage, or proportion of targets correctly recalled. A total of 39/51(77%) of the entire sample included this form of measurement with the

population-specific breakdown as follows:

. Acquired brain injury: 28/38 (74%).

. Dementia: 7/7 (100%).

. Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder: 3/6 (50%).

Other outcome measures used across the three groups included documen-

tation of error responses or intrusions (e.g., Glisky & Schacter, 1987;

Komatsu et al., 2000; Leng et al., 1991), task completion times (e.g., Glisky,

1995; Glisky & Schacter, 1987; Leng, Copello, & Sayegh, 1991), levels of

independence (Andrewes&Gielewski, 1999), number of trials/sessions to cri-terion (e.g., Ehlhardt, Sohlbert, Glang, & Albin, 2005; Glisky & Schacter,

1988; Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005), behavioural checklists/questionnaires(e.g., Hunkin, Squires, Aldrich, & Parkin, 1998; Ownsworth & McFarland,

1999; Squires, Hunkin, & Parkin, 1996) and standardised assessments (e.g.,

Dou et al., 2006; Schmitter-Edgecombe, Fahy, Whelan, & Long, 1995;

Winter & Hunkin, 1999). Qualifiers to these measurement systems frequently

included whether the measure was implemented immediately following train-

ing or after a delay, whether recall was free or cued, and whether a recognition

task was also included. These qualifiers were particularly relevant to those

studies in which the determination of the different memory systems (e.g.,

implicit versus explicit memory) underlying performance outcomes was of

16 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 18: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

primary interest (e.g., Hunkin, Squires, Parkin, & Tidy, 1998; Tailby &

Haslam, 2003). It was generally unclear whether or not evaluators were

naı̈ve to the training conditions when measuring outcomes; hence, for the

majority of the studies, it was assumed that the trainer also served as the

evaluator.

To summarise, the systematic instructional methods evaluated within and

between aetiology groups varied, the majority of the studies targeting error-

less learning or systematic instructional packages. The studies also varied in

the extent to which the treatment procedures and dosages were sufficiently

detailed to allow a naı̈ve trainer to replicate the treatment, which is important

for establishing external validity. Treatment targets spanned a range of infor-

mation and procedures. A number of studies evaluated word recall, whereas

others selected functional targets, such as: face–name recall, computer task

completion, and external memory aid use. The most common outcome

measure was the number-percentage-proportion of targets correct while

other measures were specifically tailored to the treatment target (e.g., task

completion time for multi-step procedures). Lack of explicit information con-

cerning the evaluators’ knowledge of training conditions limits assessment of

internal validity. Design factors influencing internal validity will be discussed

in the next section.

Study design

This section summarises the classes of evidence, research design, statistics,

reliability-validity, and ecological validity (see Appendix C for details).

Classes of evidence

As previously stated, each study was rated according to the revised

AAN classification system (i.e., Class I–IV evidence) (American Academy

of Neurology, 2004) across population groups as follows. Table 2 shows

classification of research evidence across the three population groups.

Design, statistics, reliability-validity, ecological validity

This section summarises the methodological variables coded, including:

research design and experimental control conditions, statistics, reliability-

validity, and ecological validity. These components are analysed separately

for each population group. For this review, the authors determined that eco-

logical validity had been established when the intervention targets would be

of direct benefit to the study participants in their daily lives (e.g., teaching the

use of an external memory aid or a vocational task the person would poten-

tially utilise).

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 17

Page 19: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Acquired brain injury. Twenty-six of the 38 studies (68%) incorporated

experimental control conditions. There were three between-groups designs

with random assignment. Experimental comparison conditions included:

baseline comparisons (Ownsworth & McFarland, 1999), memory notebook

training versus support group intervention (Schmitter-Edgecombe et al.,

1995), and computer versus therapist-delivered treatment versus control

group (Dou et al., 2006). There were 12 within-subject experimental

studies. Comparison conditions included EL versus EF (e.g., Baddeley &

Wilson, 1994; Evans et al., 2000), EL versus MVC or different treatment

or presentation conditions (e.g., Glisky et al., 1986a; Komatsu et al.,

2000), and baseline comparisons (Ehlhardt et al., 2005). Eight studies

used combinations of both between and within-subject designs with com-

parison conditions similar to the above. Several studies reported counter-

balanced treatment conditions and stimuli as appropriate to the design.

Three studies used combinations of between and/or within-subject

designs and case studies (Glang et al., 1992; Glisky & Delaney, 1996;

Squires et al., 1996). Twelve studies did not use experimental control

and included case studies with and without pre–post treatment

comparisons.

TABLE 2

Classification of research evidence

Number (percent)

Acquired brain injury

(N ¼ 38) Class I 0/38 (0)

Class II 1/38 (3)

Class III 21/38 (55)

Class IV 14/38 (37)

Combinations 2/38 (5)

Dementia

(N ¼ 7) Class I 0/7 (0)

Class II 0/7 (0)

Class III 5/7 (71)

Class IV 1/7 (14)

Combinations 1/7 (14)

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder

(N ¼ 6) Class I 1/6 (17)

Class II 5/6 (83)

Class III 0/6 (0)

Class IV 0/6 (0)

Combinations 0/6 (0)

“Combination” refers to those studies that included various combinations of

Class I–IV designs.

18 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 20: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Levels of statistical significance were reported in 29/38 (76%) studies with

procedures including t-tests, ANOVAs, general linear model analyses, as well

as non-parametric procedures. Statistics were not appropriate for selected

within-subject studies that used visual analysis to determine effectiveness

(e.g., Ehlhardt et al., 2005; Glang et al., 1992). Only four (11%) of the

studies reported reliability and/or validity data related to measures (e.g.,

Ehlhardt et al., 2005; Melton & Bourgeois, 2005). Ecological validity was

considered established in 12/38 (32%) of the studies (e.g., Andrewes &

Gielewski, 1999; Glisky & Schacter, 1987).

Dementia. Four of the seven studies used within-subject designs that

included comparisons of EL vs. EF (Clare et al., 2000; Metzler-Baddeley &

Snowden, 2005; Ruis & Kessels, 2005) and use of a control set of stimulus

items (Clare et al., 2002). Haslam and colleagues (2006) used both

between and within-subjects comparisons when evaluating EL vs. EF and

control group performance. Clare and colleagues (2000) used a combination

of experimental, within-subjects (stable baseline as control), and a case study

design. Winter and Hunkin (1999) used a non-experimental, pre–post com-

parison study of EL. Levels of statistical significance were reported in 5/6(83%) of the studies and included t-tests, ANOVAs, correlational analyses,

and time-series analyses. Reliability and validity were not reported in any

of the studies. Ecological validity was established in 1/7 (14%) of studies

(Clare et al., 2000), in which participants demonstrated improved face–

name recall and use of memory strategies in naturalistic settings.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Three of the six studies (50%)

used between-group experimental designs comparing EL vs. EF, with and

without a control group, and EL vs. symptom management (Kern et al.,

1996, 2002, 2005). The three other studies used between and within-subjects

comparisons (O’Carroll et al., 1999; Pope & Kern, 2006; Young et al., 2002).

Young and colleagues (2002) compared two forms of systematic instruction:

scaffolded instruction versus direct instruction, and included a control group.

Four of the six studies (67%) used randomised assignment (Kern et al., 1996–

2005; Young et al., 2002). Levels of statistical significance were reported in

all of the studies and included t-tests, ANOVA, MANOVA, correlational pro-

cedures and non-parametric tests. Reliability and validity were reported in 3/6 (50%) of the studies (Kern et al., 2005; Kern et al., 2002; Young et al., 2002)

with one study including fidelity of implementation data (Kern et al., 2005).

Ecological validity was incorporated in one of the six studies (social problem

solving—Kern et al., 2005).

To summarise, the research designs used across the three aetiology groups

included between-group and within-subject designs. These different study

designs vary in the extent to which they establish external and internal

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 19

Page 21: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

validity. Notably, few studies documented reliability and validity procedures,

limiting the extent to which changes in the dependent variables could be con-

fidently linked to the targeted intervention (i.e., internal validity). Specific

outcomes associated with the targeted interventions are reviewed in the

next section.

Treatment outcomes

This section summarises immediate outcomes and generalisation and main-

tenance of results (see Appendix C for details).

We identified three different categories of immediate outcomes that

described the research results reported in the literature. The immediate out-

comes were coded as favourable (designated by the þ symbol in Appendix

C evidence table) if the instructional method evaluated in the study was

reported to produce positive learning in the dependent variables and/or sig-nificantly stronger learning than a control instructional condition. Outcomes

were coded as qualified (designated by the/symbol) if the instructional

method was reported to produce variable learning on the dependent measures.

Outcomes were coded as negative (using the – symbol) if the target instruc-

tional method was found to produce limited or no advantage to learning in the

dependent variables.2

Several studies, particularly those published in the 1980s and early 1990s,

focused on the basic question of whether or not a particular systematic

instructional method worked and/or resulted in better performance than

another method (e.g., MVC – Glisky et al., 1986; EL vs. EF – Baddeley &

Wilson, 1994). A trend in later studies emphasised the evaluation of enhance-

ments to previously validated methods (e.g., EL with pre-exposure – Kalla,

Downes, & van den Broeck, 2001; EL with self-generated responses – Tailby

&Haslam, 2003). Accordingly, the outcomes of these later studies were coded

by whether or not the established instructional method was again shown to be

effective as well as by the effectiveness of the hypothesised enhancement.

Immediate outcomes

The outcomes are analysed across the three population groups in order to

evaluate global trends. A broad analysis of the aggregate immediate outcomes

revealed solid evidence supporting the use of systematic instructional tech-

niques. A total of 41 out of 51 (80%) studies reported favourable learning

2Outcome decisions were based on statistically-analysed group data, individual participant

data, or both depending on each study’s research design and primary mode of analysis. The

authors acknowledge that for studies with group data only, individual variations in performance

masked by statistical analysis might have resulted in a different outcome assessment had

individual data also been available.

20 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 22: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

outcomes using the instructional methods of errorless learning, method of

vanishing cues, spaced retrieval, or systematic instructional packages.

Favourable outcomes occurred in each population group with the least

robust outcomes for the dementia population. In the ABI group, 34 out of

38 (89%) studies reported favourable immediate outcomes, four reported

qualified outcomes, and one study that included children reported negative

outcomes (Landis et al., 2006). Glang and colleagues (1992) also included

children, but reported positive findings. The two studies differed considerably

in terms of number of participants, severity levels, systematic instructional

method (errorless learning or systematic instructional package, respectively),

and study design. In the dementia group, two out of seven studies (29%)

reported favourable immediate outcomes (Clare et al., 2000; Winter &

Hunkin, 1999), while four studies reported qualified outcomes and one

study a negative outcome (Dunn & Clare, 2007). With the exception of

Kern et al. (2002) who reported qualified findings, five out of the six (83%)

studies with participants who have schizophrenia reported favourable

immediate outcomes (e.g., Kern et al., 2005; Pope & Kern, 2006).

Studies varied by type of instruction and outcome measures used to docu-

ment treatment effects. The two studies evaluating EL only reported favour-

able outcomes (Parkin et al., 1998; Winter & Hunkin, 1999). In the 20 studies

comparing EL with EF and/or a control group, 14 (56%) reported favourable

outcomes (e.g., Squires, Hunkin,&Parkin, 1997;Van der Linden,Meulemans,

& Lorrain, 1994; Young et al., 2002), with five studies reporting qualified

findings (e.g., Evans et al., 2000; Metzler-Baddeley & Snowden, 2005;

O’Carroll et al., 1999) and one study reporting negative outcomes (Landis

et al., 2006). All five studies evaluating MVC only reported favourable out-

comes (e.g., Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; Glisky & Schacter, 1988; Glisky

et al., 1986b). In the studies that compared MVC to another instructional con-

dition, three out of five reported favourable findings (Glisky et al., 1986a;

Leng et al., 1991; Riley, Sotirious, & Jaspal, 2004) and two reported qualified

findings (Hunkin & Parkin, 1995; Thoene & Glisky, 1995). All three studies

evaluating spaced retrieval/spaced presentations reported favourable outcomes

(Hillary et al., 2003; Melton & Bourgeois, 2005; Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005).

The majority of the studies (16/17; 94%) evaluating systematic instructional

packages reported favourable outcomes (e.g., Andrewes & Gielewski, 1999;

Schmitter-Edgecombe et al., 1995; Winter & Hunkin, 1999).

Generalisation

Generalisation outcomes were coded for those studies that evaluated trans-

fer of training to non-trained targets and/or to natural settings. Studies were

coded as positive (þ) if any degree of generalisation was reported and coded

as negative (–) if generalisation was measured but not achieved.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 21

Page 23: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Acquired brain injury. Generalisation measures were taken in 19 out of

38 (50%) treatment studies. Of those studies, 15/19 (79%) reported partial

or 100% generalisation of treatment targets. For example, in the study by

Ehlhardt and colleagues (2005), generalisation for learning the e-mail pro-

cedures was reported when participants were able to utilise a slightly more

complex e-mail interface. Other studies reported generalisation from labora-

tory to naturalistic settings, while in some cases training in the natural

environment also facilitated generalisation (e.g., Andrewes & Gielewski,

1999; Glisky & Schacter, 1987).

Dementia. Generalisation was reported in one of the seven studies. Clare

and colleagues (2000) reported generalisation of face–name recall to a natur-

alistic setting for two of the six participants and use of memory strategies in

new situations for one participant.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Generalisation measures were

taken in two of the six studies (33%). Kern and colleagues (2005) reported

that the experimental group generalised trained social problem-solving

skills to novel problem-solving scenarios. Young and colleagues (2002)

noted that the scaffolded instruction group demonstrated significant improve-

ment on selected generalisation assessment measures (e.g., object sorting

task).

Maintenance

Maintenance outcomes were coded when re-assessment of trained targets

occurred more than one day following the cessation of training since that was

the minimum time reported by authors as “maintenance”.

Acquired brain injury. Maintenance checks were reported in 24 out of

38 (63%) of the studies. All 24 studies reported partial or 100% maintenance

of treatment targets from a few days to nine months post-intervention

(e.g., Andrewes & Gielewski, 1999; Thoene & Glisky, 1995).

Dementia. Maintenance checks were reported in two of the seven studies

(Clare et al., 2002, 2000) and revealed generally positive findings with reten-

tion of therapy gains up to six months post-intervention.

Schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Maintenance checks were

reported in four out of six (67%) studies. Results were mixed. Kern and col-

leagues (2002) reported a decline in the performance of work-related tasks

across both the intervention groups at three months, while Kern and colleagues

(2005) reported maintenance of social problem-solving skills at three months.

22 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 24: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

One notable difference between the studies was that the Kern et al. (2002)

supplied two, 45–60 minute sessions and no opportunity to practise skills

post-training, whereas Kern and colleagues (2005) supplied six hours of train-

ing over two days with the opportunity to practice skills post-training.

To summarise, these results suggest that systematic instructional

approaches can produce durable/flexible acquisition of skills and infor-

mation. Further, the results of those studies measuring generalisation under-

score the importance of incorporating specific training components that

support “firm” skill and knowledge acquisition, maintenance, and generalis-

ation to “real-life”, personally meaningful contexts.

DISCUSSION

Globally, the research evidence provides strong support for the effectiveness

of systematic instruction. However, it is the details surrounding the design

and execution of that instruction that lack clarity and require further study.

Learning is complex, and the interaction between the target learning objective

and the individual learner profile is not well understood. While this review

demonstrates that systematic instructional techniques are helpful for individ-

uals with acquired memory impairments, identifying the most effective com-

bination of variables pertaining to the training procedures, the treatment task,

and participant characteristics requires dynamic assessment with each client.

The research does, however, suggest several themes, or practice guidelines

that can assist clinicians in designing, evaluating, and modifying their instruc-

tion based on client performance.

Key training variables

Specificity of training. A dominant theme in the research is the need to

reduce hyper-specificity of training conditions and increase effortful proces-

sing in order to facilitate flexible learning and generalisation. Stimulus var-

iance helps to reduce hyper-specificity. Strategy components (e.g., verbal

elaboration) encourage effortful processing, which can lead to improved

attention, encoding, and organisation during acquisition as well as facilitate

explicit retrieval (Riley & Heaton, 2000; Riley et al., 2004). However,

these components also run the risk of increasing recall errors. Maintaining

a balance between constraining errors and encouraging effortful processing

is thus critical to careful treatment planning (e.g., Komatsu et al., 2000;

Tailby & Haslam, 2003).

Strategies. Studies were coded as having a “stimulus variation” com-

ponent if there was an emphasis on training multiple exemplars (e.g.,

Glisky & Schacter, 1987; Stark et al., 2005) or the stimulus was enhanced

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 23

Page 25: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

in some other way (stimulus pre-exposure; Kalla et al., 2001). Studies were

coded as having a “strategy component” if the intervention included tech-

niques such as verbal elaboration, imagery, or self-generated responses

(e.g., Clare et al., 2000; Tailby & Haslam, 2003). At least 16 of the 51

studies emphasised stimulus variability, with 15/16 (94%) of these reporting

positive findings. Of the 51 total studies, 12 included a strategy component.

Of these, 9/12 (75%) reported positive findings, while three reported quali-

fied outcomes. Interestingly, two of these latter studies (Evans et al., 2000;

Thoene & Glisky, 1995) cited the use of imagery and/or verbal elaborationas contributing to better recall. Both of these are considered to contribute

to more effortful processing compared to using EL or MVC alone.

Practice. Another critical training variable is providing sufficient prac-

tice. There was a clear trend in this literature review suggesting that more

practice leads to more durable learning. For example, 14 of the 16 studies

targeting multi-step procedures (e.g., data entry, external memory aid use)

reported favourable treatment outcomes and all but one prescribed high treat-

ment dosages, ranging from at least six to 30 sessions or more (e.g., Andrewes

& Gielewski, 1999; Hunkin, Squires, Aldrich & Parkin, 1998). The two

studies that that did not report clear positive results held less than four total

training sessions (Evans et al., 2000; Kern et al., 2002).

Spacing or distribution of practice trials (i.e., spaced retrieval/expandedrehearsal) is another key training variable, a finding well supported in the

literature for non-disabled individuals (Donovan & Radosevich, 1999) and

for those with memory impairment due to dementia (Hopper et al., 2005).

Hopper and colleagues reviewed 15 studies in which all participants learned

some or all of the target information taught using spaced retrieval. In the

current review, three studies targeted spaced retrieval or spaced presentations

for individuals with TBI, all of which reported favourable outcomes (Hillary

et al., 2003; Melton & Bourgeois, 2005; Turkstra & Bourgeois, 2005). Further,

several other studies in this review that included spaced retrieval/distributedpractice as part of their errorless learning or systematic instructional

package also reported favourable outcomes (e.g., Clare et al., 2000; Ehlhardt

et al., 2005).

Both the frequency and distribution of practice trials contribute to mastering

targeted skills and information, which is essential if one is to use the skills/information in daily life. A “criterion for mastery” is the a priori determination

of the level of performance (e.g., accuracy, level of independence, time frame)

indicative of such mastery. For example, a clinician and client might determine

that mastery has been reached when a data entry task can be performed with

100% accuracy for three consecutive sessions. In this review, the majority of

the studies did not establish a criterion for mastery since the primary research

questions were concerned with whether a particular instructional method

24 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 26: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

worked and/or if it was significantly better than another method. In other

words, information/skill mastery was not always the primary goal. That

said, 13 of the 51 studies (25%) included a criterion for mastery. The majority

of these studies 11/13 (85%) reported positive outcomes, several of which

included high treatment dosages and distribution of practice trials as described

above (e.g., Ehlhardt et al., 2005; Glisky & Schacter, 1989).

Task characteristics. A number of the studies remind us of the importance

of considering task characteristics when designing intervention. For example,

multiple studies by Glisky and her colleagues (1986–1989) evaluating the

method of vanishing cues suggested that complex procedures can be learned

if they are broken into simple components, and if the knowledge relevant to

carrying out the procedures is explicitly trained. Evans and colleagues

(2000) conducted nine experiments in three study phases to compare errorless

to errorful learning. Their results suggested that tasks and recall conditions that

facilitated retrieval of implicit memory for learned material (e.g., learning

names given a first letter cue) benefit from errorless learning methods, while

those that require explicit recall of novel associations (e.g., learning a route

or programming a device) do not benefit from errorless learning.3 Similarly,

Thoene and Glisky (1995) showed a differential response to MVC based on

the type of task. They suggested that MVC was not beneficial for learning arbi-

trary associations, whereas more explicit, mnemonic techniques were shown to

be more beneficial for such material.

Ecological validity. Of most interest to clinicians is the evidence support-

ing the learning of ecological tasks. As previously stated, we defined instruc-

tional targets as being ecologically valid if the target constituted information

or skills that the study participants would use in their own daily lives. Of the

51 studies, 14 (27%) utilised ecologically valid tasks. Targets ranged from

face–name associations to recall of relevant people, training the use of exter-

nal memory aids or computer tasks, as well as academic skills. Of these

studies, 100% reported positive findings. This high percentage of positive

outcomes supports Bradely, Kapur, and Evan’s (2003) assertion that task vari-

ables, such as motivational and emotional significance, help memories to last.

Specifically, the evidence suggests learning may be facilitated when tasks or

information are inherently functional to an individual. Another interesting

trend noted in this subset of ecologically valid studies was that all reported

high treatment dosages (with a minimum of six treatment sessions), and all

incorporated task analyses to train procedures and/or were explicit in identi-

fying the target learning objective for the participant.

3Other authors would argue such procedural tasks rely on implicit rather than explicit

memory (e.g., Baddeley & Wilson, 1994; Page et al., 2006).

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 25

Page 27: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Key participant variables

No clear relationships between aetiology of memory impairment and

responsiveness to instructional methods were observed in this cross-

population analysis. At this point, we can say that individuals with memory

impairment from a variety of causes have been shown to benefit from sys-

tematic instruction, suggesting that it may be fruitful to look across neuro-

genic populations to evaluate instructional practices. However, as noted

earlier, the outcomes were less clear for the dementia population; many of

these studies had qualified treatment effects. Of the studies reporting qualified

findings across aetiology groups, most reported differential outcomes for

different memory severity levels (e.g., Evans et al., 2000; Riley & Heaton,

2000). Riley and Heaton’s (2000) study provides a clear example. They

showed that participants with poorer memories may benefit from more

gradual fading of cues while those with stronger memories may require

more rapid fading. However, a number of studies (e.g., Page et al., 2006;

Tailby & Haslam, 2003) showed benefits of errorless learning across

memory severity levels. The question of how severity of memory impairment

affects candidacy for specific instructional practices remains equivocal.

The role played by an individual’s cognitive profile in domains other than

memory (e.g., attention, executive functions, awareness) is equally unclear.

There is some evidence that clients with frontal lobe damage and concomitant

impairments in awareness and executive functions may benefit less from the

method of vanishing cues and errorless techniques than patients with less

frontal lobe dysfunction (e.g., Clare et al., 2002; Leng et al., 1991). Andrewes

and Gielewski (1999) address this issue in their detailed report of positive

outcomes following the training of vocational skills in a person with dense

amnesia resulting from herpes encephalitis. They qualify their findings,

noting that the participant’s spared executive functions, high premorbid intel-

ligence, and intact semantic memory may have allowed her to benefit from

the training and achieve successful return to work. What is evident across

studies is the need for therapists to employ instructional techniques

matched to specific client cases and measure the ongoing effects of learning

in order to adjust instructional practices as necessary. Experimental evalu-

ation of candidacy issues is an important goal for future clinical research.

RECOMMENDATIONS

The instructional literature clearly demonstrates that structuring the manner

in which target information or procedures are introduced and reviewed can

facilitate learning. By implication, careful planning of how to train and

instruct people with damaged learning systems allows clinicians to optimise

experience-dependent learning. The review of the best current evidence

26 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 28: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

suggests there are a number of key instructional practices that can promote

learning for individuals with acquired memory impairments. These include:

. Clear delineation of intervention targets and/or use of task analyses

when training multi-step procedures.

. Constraining errors and controlling client output while acquiring new or

relearning information and procedures.

. Providing sufficient practice.

. Distributing practice.

. Use of stimulus variation (e.g., multiple exemplars).

. Use of strategies to promote more effortful processing (e.g., verbal

elaboration; imagery).

. Selection and training of ecologically valid targets.

While there is substantial evidence to encourage clinicians to incorporate

and evaluate the effects of these instructional techniques, we run the risk of

being overly simplistic if we look to the evidence to identify the single,

right instructional practice(s). Learning is complex and we must recognise

that specific task, training, and learner characteristics will require different

combinations of instructional practices. Riley and Heaton (2000, p. 147)

eloquently remind clinicians and researchers that:

. . . the most effective [instructional] approach to teaching in any given

set of circumstances is likely to be a package that combines several

strategies in an attempt to facilitate a range of key processes and

which is tailored to the needs of the learning circumstances. From

this perspective, it is inappropriate to pose research questions about

whether one teaching strategy is more effective than another in some

absolute sense divorced from the circumstances.

We conclude with a call to clinicians to examine their instructional and

training practices and to be deliberate and systematic in their treatment plan-

ning. Clinicians typically do not view themselves as the designers and con-

veyors of curricula. However, this is ultimately what must occur in order to

implement the instructional procedures supported in the literature. The use

of instructional design and delivery principles that have been experimentally

validated are the key to successfully teaching or re-teaching procedures and/or knowledge to clients with memory impairments.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 27

Page 29: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

REFERENCES

Adams, G. L., & Engelmann, S. (1996). Research in direct instruction: 25 years beyond Distar,

Seattle, WA: Educational Achievement Systems.

American Academy of Neurology (AAN) (2004). Clinical practice guideline process manual.

Prepared for the Quality Standards Subcommittee and the Therapeutics and Technology

Assessment Subcommittee, by Edlund, W., Gonseth, G., So, Y., & Franklin, G. Retrieved

18 June 2006 at www.aan.org.

Andrewes, D., & Gielewski, E. (1999). The work rehabilitation of a herpes simplex encephalitis

patient with anterograde amnesia. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 9(1), 77–99.

Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem

of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32(1), 53–68.

Bradley, V. A., Kapur, N., & Evans, J. (2003). The assessment of memory for memory rehabi-

litation. In P. W. Halligan & D. T. Wade (Eds.), Effectiveness of rehabilitation for cognitive

deficits (pp.115–134). NewYork: Oxford University Press.

Butters, M. A., Glisky, E., & Schacter, D. (1993). Transfer of new learning in memory-impaired

patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(2), 219–230.

Clare, L., Roth, I., Wilson, B., Carter, G., & Hodges, J. (2002). Relearning face–name associ-

ations in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 16(4), 538–547.

Clare, L., Wilson, B. A., Carter, G., Breen, K., Gosses, A., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). Intervening

with everyday memory problems in dementia of the Alzheimer type: An errorless learning

approach. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(1), 132–146.

Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of prac-

tice effect: Now you see it, now you don’t. Journal of Applied Psychology. 84(5), 795–805.

Dou, Z. L., Man, W. K., Ou, H. N., Sheng, J. L., & Tam, S. F. (2006). Computerized errorless

learning-based memory rehabilitation for Chinese patients with brain injury: A preliminary

quasi-experimental clinical design study. Brain Injury, 20, 219–225.

Ducharme, J. M., & Spencer, T. F. (2001). Training brain injury rehabilitation therapists to use

generalized teaching skills. Brain Injury, 15(4), 333–347.

Dunn, J., & Clare, L. (2007). Learning face–name associations in early-stage dementia: Com-

paring the effects of errorless learning and effortful processing. Neuropsychological Reha-

bilitation, 17, 735–754.

Ehlhardt, L, Sohlberg, M. M., Glang, A., & Albin, R. (2005) TEACH-M: A pilot study evaluat-

ing an instructional sequence for persons with impaired memory and executive functions.

Brain Injury, 19(8), 569–583.

Evans, J. J., Wilson, B. A., Schuri, U., Andrade, J., Baddeley, A., Bruna, O., Canavan, T.,

Sala, S. D., Green, R., Laaksonen, R., Lorenzi, L., & Taussik, I. (2000). A comparison of

“errorless” and “trial and error” learning methods for teaching individuals with acquired

memory deficits. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10(1), 67–101.

Glang, A., Singer, G., Cooley, E., & Tish, N. (1992). Tailoring Direct Instruction techniques for

use with elementary students with brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation,

7(4), 93–108.

Glisky, E. (1992). Acquisition and transfer of declarative and procedural knowledge

by memory-impaired patients: A computer data-entry task, Neuropsychologia, 30(10),

899–910.

Glisky, E. (1995). Acquisition and transfer of word processing skills by an amnesic patient.

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 5(4), 299–318.

Glisky, E. L., & Delaney, E. L. (1996). Implicit memory and new semantic learning in post-

traumatic amnesia. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 11(2), 31–42.

Glisky, E. L., & Schacter, D. L. (1987). Acquisition of domain-specific knowledge in organic

amnesia: Training for computer-related work. Neuropsychologia, 25(6), 893–906.

28 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 30: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Glisky, E. L., & Schacter, D. L. (1988). Long-term retention of computer learning by patients

with memory disorders. Neuropsychologia, 26(1), 173–178.

Glisky, E. L., & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Extending the limits of complex learning in organic

amnesia: Computer training in a vocational domain. Neuropsychologia, 27(1), 107–120.

Glisky, E. L., Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1986a). Learning and retention of computer-

related vocabulary in memory-impaired patients: Method of vanishing cues. Journal of

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(3), 292–312.

Glisky, E. L., Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1986b) Computer learning by memory-impaired

patients: Acquisition and retention of complex knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 24(3),

313–328.

Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. (2001). Neurophysiologic basis of rehabilitation. Journal of Medical

Speech-Language Pathology, 9(2), 117–127.

Gonzalez-Rothi, L. (2006) The power of experience dependent re-learning after brain injury:

Hope offered to TBI rehabiliation by human rehabilitation studies in other pathophysiologic

models. Paper presented at The Federate Interagency Conference on TBI, March, Bethesda,

Maryland.

Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2003). Students with learning disabilities and the process of

writing: A meta-analysis of SRSD studies. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham

(Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 323–344). New York: Guilford Press.

Haslam, C., Gilroy, D., Black, S., & Beesley, T. (2006). How successful is errorless learning in

supporting memory for high and low-level knowledge in dementia. Neuropsychological

Rehabilitation, 16(5), 505–536.

Hillary, F. G., Schultheis, M. T., Challis, B. H., Millis, S. R., Carnevale, G. J., Glashi, T., &

DeLuca, J. (2003). Spacing of repetitions improves learning and memory after moderate

and severe TBI. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(1), 49–58.

Hopper, T., Mahendra, N., Kim, E., Azuma, T., Bayles, K. A., Cleary, S. J., et al. (2005).

Evidence-based practice recommendations for working with individuals with dementia:

Spaced-retrieval training. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 13(4),

xxvii–xxxiv.

Hunkin, N. A., & Parkin, A. J. (1995). The method of vanishing cues: An evaluation of its effec-

tiveness in teaching memory impaired individuals. Neuropsychologia, 33(10), 1255–1279.

Hunkin, N. M., Squires, E. J., Aldrich, F. L., & Parkin, A. J. (1998). Errorless learning and the

acquisition of word processing skills, Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8(4), 433–449.

Hunkin, N. M., Squires, E. J., Parkin, A. J., & Tidy, J. A. (1998). Are the benefits of errorless

learning dependent on implicit memory? Neuropsychologia, 36(1), 25–36.

Kalla, T., Downes, J. J., & van den Broeck, M. (2001). The pre-exposure technique: Enhancing

effects of errorless learning in the acquisition of face–name associations. Neuropsychologi-

cal Rehabilitation, 11(1) 1–16.

Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (2000). What definitions of learning disability say and don’t say:

A critical analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 239–256.

Kennedy, M. R., Avery, J., Coelho, C., Sohlberg, M., Turkstra, L., & Ylvisaker, M. (2002).

Evidence-based practice guidelines for cognitive-communication disorders after traumatic

brain injury: Initial committee report. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology,

10(2), ix–xiii.

Kennedy, M. R. T, Coelho, C., Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker, M., Sohlberg, M. M., Kan, P. F., &

Yorkston, K. (2008). Intervention for executive functions of problem solving, planning,

organizing, and multi-tasking after traumatic brain injury: A systematic review, meta-

analysis and clinical recommendations, Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 00, 000–000.

Kern, R. S., Green, M. F., Mitchell, S., Kopelowicz, A., Mintz, J., & Liberman, R. P. (2005).

Extensions of errorless learning for social problem-solving deficits in schizophrenia.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(3), 513–519.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 29

Page 31: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Kern, R. S., Liberman, R. P., Kopelowicz, A., Mintz, J., & Green, M. F. (2002). Applications of

errorless learning for improving work performance in persons with schizophrenia. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1921–1926.

Kern, R. S, Wallace, C. J., Hellman, S. G., Womack, L. M., & Green, M. F. (1996). A training

procedure for remediating WCST deficits in chronic psychotic patients: An adaptation of

errorless learning principles. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30(4), 283–294.

Kessels, R. P. C., & deHaan, E. H. F. (2003). Implicit learning in memory rehabilitation: A

meta-analysis on errorless learning and vanishing cues methods. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(6), 805–814.

Kim, A. B., Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., & Shangjin Wei, J. (2004). Graphic organizers and their

effects on the reading comprehension of students with LD: A synthesis of research.

Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37(2), 105–119.

Komatsu, S., Mimura, M., Kato, M., Wakamatsu, N., & Kashima, H. (2000). Errorless

and effortful processes involved in the learning of face–name associations by patients

with alcoholic Korsakoff’s Syndrome. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10(2),

113–132.

Laatsch, L. K., Thulborn, K. R., & Krisky, C. M. (2004). Investigating the neurobiological basis

of cognitive rehabilitation therapy with fMRI. Brain Injury, 18, 957–974.

Landis, J., Hanten, G., Levin, H. S., Li, X., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Duron, J., & High, W. M. (2006).

Evaluation of the errorless learning technique in children with traumatic brain injury,

Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 87, 799–805.

Lemoncello, R., & Sohlberg, M. M. (2005, November). Practicing what the instructional

research preaches: How do medical SLPs rate? Poster presentation at the annual convention

of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association: San Diego, CA, USA.

Leng, N. R. C., Copello, A. G., & Sayegh, A. (1991). Learning after brain injury by the method

of vanishing cues: A case study. Behavioral Psychotherapy, 19, 173–181.

Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., Bakken, J. P., &Whedon, C. (1996). Reading comprehension:

A synthesis of research in learning disabilities. In T. E. Scruggs & M. A. Mastropieri (Eds.),

Advances in learning and behavioral disabilities (pp. 277–303). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

McKenna, P., Clare, L., &Baddeley, A.D. (1995). Schizophrenia. InA.D.Baddeley, B.Wilson,&

F. N. Watts (Eds.), Handbook of memory disorders. Chichester, UK: John Wiley.

Melton, A. K., & Bourgeois, M. S. (2005). Training compensatory memory strategies via

telephone for persons with TBI. Aphasiology, 19(3–5), 353–364.

Metzler-Baddeley, C., & Snowden, J. S. (2005). Brief report: Errorless versus errorful learning

as a memory rehabilitation approach in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and

Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 1070–1079.

Miller, R. G., Rosenberg, J. A., Gelinas, D. F., Mitsumoto, H., Newman, D., Sufit., R., Borasio,

G. D., Bradley, W. G., Bromberg, M. B., Brooks, B. R., Kasarskis, E. J., Munsat, T. O., &

Oppenheimer, E. A. (1999). Practice parameter: The care of the patient with amyotrophic

lateral sclerosis (an evidence-based review): Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee

of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology, 52, 1311–1325.

O’Carroll, R. E., Russell, H. H., Lawrie, S. M., & Johnstone, E. C. (1999). Errorless learning

and the cognitive rehabilitation of memory-impaired schizophrenic patients. Psychological

Medicine, 29, 105–112.

Ownsworth, T. L., & McFarland, K. (1999). Memory remediation in long-term acquired brain

injury: Two approaches in diary training. Brain Injury, 13(8), 605–626.

Page, M., Wilson, B., Shiel, A., Carter, G., & Norris, D. (2006). What is the locus of the error-

less-learning advantage? Neuropsychologia, 44, 90–100.

Parkin, A. J., Hunkin, N. M., & Squires, E. J. (1998). Unlearning John Major: The use of error-

less learning in the reacquisition of proper names follow herpes simplex encephalitis.

Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(4), 361–375.

30 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 32: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Pope, J. W., & Kern, R. S. (2006). An “errorful” learning deficit in schizophrenia? Journal of

Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 101–110.

Riley, G., & Heaton, S. (2000). Guidelines for the selection of a method of fading cues.

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10(2), 133–149.

Riley, G. A., Sotirious, D., & Jaspal, S. (2004). Which is more effective in promoting implicit

and explicit memory: The method of vanishing cues or errorless learning without fading?

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14(3), 257–283.

Ruis, C., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2005). Effects of errorless learning and errorful face–name

associative learning in moderate–severe dementia, Aging Clinical and Experimental

Research, 17(6), 514–517.

Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., Fahy, J. F., Whelan, J. P., & Long, C. (1995). Memory remediation

after severe closed head injury: Notebook training vs. supportive therapy. Journal of

Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 63(3), 484–489.

Sohlberg, M. M. (2006) Evidence-based instructional techniques for training procedures and

knowledge in persons with severe memory impairment. Revista de Neuropsicologia, 1(1),

14–19.

Sohlberg, M., Avery, J., Kennedy, M. R. T., Coelho, C., Ylvisaker, M., Turkstra, L., &

Yorkston, K. (2003). Practice guidelines for direct attention training. Journal of Medical

Speech-Language Pathology, 11(3), xix–xxxix.

Sohlberg, M. M., Ehlhardt, L., & Kennedy, M. (2005). Instructional techniques in cognitive

rehabilitation: A preliminary report. Seminars in Speech and Language, 26, 268–279.

Sohlberg, M. M., Kennedy, M. R. T., Avery, J., Coelho, C., Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker, M., &

Yorkston, K. (2007). Evidence based practice for the use of external aids as a memory reha-

bilitation technique. Journal of Medical Speech Pathology, 15(1).

Sohlberg, M. M., &Mateer, C. A. (2001). Cognitive rehabilitation: an integrative neuropsycho-

logical approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Squires, E. J., Hunkin, N. M., & Parkin, A. J. (1996). Memory notebook training in a case of

severe amnesia: Generalising from paired associate learning to real life. Neuropsychological

Rehabilitation, 6(1), 55–65.

Squires, E. J., Hunkin, N. M., & Parkin, A. J. (1997). Errorless learning of novel associations in

amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35(8), 1103–1111.

Stark, C., Stark, S., & Gordon, B. (2005). New semantic learning and generalization in a patient

with amnesia. Neuropsychology, 19(2), 139–151.

Swanson, H. L. (1999). Instructional components that predict treatment outcomes for students

with learning disabilities: Support for the combined strategy and Direct Instruction Model.

Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 14(3), 129–140.

Swanson, H. L. (2001). Searching for the best model for instructing students with learning

disabilities. Focus on Exceptional Children, 34(2), 2–15.

Swanson, H. L., Carson, C., & Sachse-Lee, C. M. (1996). A selective synthesis of intervention

research for students with learning disabilities. School Psychology Review, 25, 370–391.

Swanson, H. L., & Hoskyn, M. (1998). A synthesis of experimental intervention literature for

students with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of treatment outcomes. Review of

Educational Research, 68, 277–322.

Tailby, R., & Haslam, C. (2003). An investigation of errorless learning in memory-impaired

patients: Improve the technique and clarifying the theory. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1230–

1240.

Thoene, A. I. T., & Glisky, E. (1995). Learning name–face associations in memory impaired

patients: A comparison of procedures. Journal of the International Neuropsychological

Society, 1, 29–38.

Turkstra, L. S., & Bourgeois, M. S. (2005). Intervention for a modern day HM: Errorless learn-

ing of practical goals. Journal of Medical Speech Language Pathology, 13(3), 205–212.

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 31

Page 33: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

Turkstra, L., Ylvisaker, M., Coelho, C., Kennedy, M., Sohlberg, M. M., & Avery, J. (2005).

Practice guidelines for standardized assessment for persons with traumatic brain injury.

Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 13(2), ix–xxxviii.

Van der Linden, M., Meulemans, T., & Lorrain, D. (1994). Acquisition of new concepts by two

amnesic patients. Cortex, 30, 305–317.

Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A., Evans, J., & Shiel, A. (1994). Errorless learning in the rehabilita-

tion of memory impaired people. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4(3), 307–326.

Wilson, F. C., & Manly, T. (2003). Sustained attention training and errorless learning facilitates

self-care functioning in chronic ipsilesional neglect following severe traumatic brain injury.

Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 13(95), 537–548.

Winter, J., & Hunkin, N. M. (1999). Relearning in Alzheimer’s Disease. International Journal

of Geriatric Psychiatry, 14, 983–990.

Ylvisaker,M.,Coehlho,C.Kennedy,M., Sohlberg,M.M., Turkstra, L., Avery, J.,&Yorkston,K.

(2002). Reflections on evidence-based practice and rational clinical decision making.

Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology. 10(2), ix–xxxviii.

Ylvisaker, M., Hanks, R., & Johnson-Green, D. (2003) Rehabilitation of children and adults

with cognitive-communication disorders after brain injury. ASHA Supplement, 23, 59–72.

Ylvisaker, M. M., Turkstra, L. M., Kennedy, M., Coelho, C., Yorkston, K. C., Sohlberg, M. M.,

& Avery, J. (2007). Behavioral interventions for children and adults with behavior disorders

after TBI: A systematic review of the evidence. Brain Injury, 21(8), 769–804.

Young, D. A., Zakzanis, K. K., Campbell, Z., Freyslinger, M. G., & Meichenbaum, D. H.

(2002). Scaffolded instruction remediates Wisconsin Card Sorting Test deficits in schizo-

phrenia: A comparison to other techniques. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12(3),

257–287.

Manuscript received September 2007

Revised manuscript received October 2007

First published online Month year

32 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 34: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

APPENDICES

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 33

Page 35: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

34 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 36: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 35

Page 37: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

36 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 38: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 37

Page 39: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

38 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 40: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 39

Page 41: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

40 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 42: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 41

Page 43: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

42 EHLHARDT ET AL.

Page 44: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation - ANCDSDownloaded By: [University of Oregon] At: 02:05 9 April 2008 studies reported positive outcomes in favour of systematic instruction. However,

Dow

nloa

ded

By:

[Uni

vers

ity o

f Ore

gon]

At:

02:0

5 9

Apr

il 20

08

EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE GUIDELINES FOR INSTRUCTION 43