1
General Method — All Experiments Procedure. Following Son’s (2004) procedure, word pairs were presented on a computer screen to college-student participants who then were given a choice between studying the pair again “now” (immediately), studying it again “later” (after all word pairs were shown at least once), or being “done” with it (not presented again). (See diagram.) Duration of the second presentation of each re-studied pair was held constant at 3s. After completing all study trials, participants solved arithmetic problems for 5 min. This was followed by a recall test in which the cue words were presented successively, and participants attempted to type the corresponding target word. Design. Mixed factorial with Pair Difficulty (easy, medium, hard) varied within participants and Duration of the Pairs’ Initial Presentation varied between participants. Materials. Pairs of common words (Exp. 1) or GRE-synonym pairs (Exps. 2 & 3). Item difficulty was determined a priori and manipulated experimentally. Primary dependent variable. Proportion of re-studied items chosen for later, spaced presentation (Proportion Spaced). Introduction The Question Memory is generally better when study opportunities are distributed in time (spaced practice) rather than when they occur in immediate succession (massed practice). However, when learners decide practice schedules for themselves, do they choose massed practice or spaced practice? Background Son (2004) presented pairs of GRE vocabulary words and common synonyms for 1 s each. After each pair, learners judged how well they knew it (indexing difficulty) and chose to study it again for 3 s, either now (massed), later (spaced) or never (done). For items chosen for restudy (massed or spaced), Son found a negative correlation such that the preference for spaced practice declined as judged difficulty increased. Son’s results are consistent with the Region of Proximal Learning Model (Metcalfe & Kornell, 2005) which predicts that, for items judged to be learnable, the perceived benefits of immediate study (massed practice) should be greater for more difficult items. However, Benjamin and Bird (2006), using pairs of common words with related, but different, methods, found a stronger preference for spaced practice on more difficult items. They interpreted their results in terms of the Discrepancy Reduction Model, which predicts a constant or increasing preference for the more effective strategy (spaced practice) as perceived difficulty increases. The present study We sought to resolve the conflict from these two earlier studies. Our procedure was similar to Son’s. However, within experiments, we varied item difficulty experimentally (like Benjamin and Bird) rather than correlationally (like Son), and we varied the duration of the initial presentation of each pair. Across experiments, we varied the type of item: Pairs of common words (like Benjamin and Bird) in Exp. 1, and GRE-synonym pairs (like Son) in Exps. 2 and 3. Discussion Son (2004) reported that as item difficulty increased, learners became less likely to choose spaced practice rather than massed practice. She attributed this to metacognitive judgments in which the perceived benefit of massed practice is assumed to increase with decreases in how well the pairs are judged to be learned. Her account was consistent with the Region of Proximal Learning Model (Metcalfe & Kornell, 2005). However, our findings suggest that Son’s (2004) results were largely an artifact of conditions that prevented learners from fully encoding some items, especially the most difficult ones, on the initial study opportunity. When unseen items were excluded from the analysis, the effect of difficulty on choice of spaced practice was largely eliminated. It was similarly eliminated when pairs involved common words (Exp. 1) in which length and familiarity did not vary as a function of difficulty. It appears that if learners are unable to fully encode a pair initially, they will want to see it again immediately. However, when this is not the case, learners generally seem to prefer spaced practice. When they do show a preference based on difficulty, it appears to be a preference to space the more difficult items. These findings are in accordance with both Benjamin and Bird’s (2006) results and the Discrepancy Reduction Model. Further work is necessary to determine whether the preference we observed for spaced practice actually reflects a metacognitive appreciation of its benefits. An alternate possibility is that learners who opt for spaced practice believe their choice will allow them to re-study the pairs closer to the time of the test. Thus, learners’ choices could reflect metacognitive judgments about the effect of the length of the retention interval rather than about the effect of spaced practice. This possibility is being tested. Michael S. Cohen, Meghan Davis, Amy C. Moors, Thomas C. Toppino Department of Psychology, Villanova University References Metacognitive Control Over Distribution of Practice: When is Spacing Preferred? Cue word ---------- Target word Now? Later? Done? Cue word ---------- Target word Cue word ---------- Target word x Secondary Results — Common to All Experiments Proportion of “done” responses (pairs not selected for re-study). • Decreased with increasing difficulty in all experiments • Did not vary as a function of the duration of initial pair presentation. This confirms that we successfully manipulated pair difficulty and suggests that participants used the “done” response primarily for pairs that they felt they knew from the outset. Experiment 1 Method 34 participants Pairs of common words Duration of first presentation: 1s vs. 5s Results - Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice. Significantly higher for 5s than 1 s presentation duration No significant effect of difficulty or its interaction with duration. The apparent trend for the proportion of spacing choices to increase with difficulty in the 1s condition was reminiscent of Benjamin and Bird’s (2006) findings, but was not reliable. The possibility of a similar trend in the 5s condition was obscured by a ceiling effect. Experiment 2 Son’s (2004) effect of pair difficulty was not replicated. Son’s (2004) effect of pair difficulty was replicated with a 1s presentation, but the opposite effect of difficulty was obtained with a 5s presentation. Experiment 3 - Results Method 91 participants Son’s (2004) GRE-synonym pairs Duration of first presentation: 1s vs. 5s Results - Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice. Greater for 5s than 1s presentation duration, but duration interacted with difficulty. Declined significantly with increasing difficulty in the 1s initial presentation condition (p = .035), replicating Son (2004). Increased significantly with increasing difficulty in the 5s initial presentation condition (p = .002), reminiscent of Benjamin & Bird (2006). Summary of Experiments 1 and 2 Son’s (2004) findings (declining choice of spacing as a function of increasing pair difficulty) were replicated only when we used conditions identical to hers (GRE-synonym pairs presented for a very brief 1s duration). Using pairs of common words or a long (5s) presentation duration produced either no effect of pair difficulty or a tendency for the choice of spaced practice to increase with increasing difficulty. Overall, spacing was chosen more often when the presentation duration was long (5s) rather than short (1s). Recall on final test. Spaced pairs had higher recall than massed pairs; thus, spacing was probably the superior strategy All Re-studied Pairs (Seen and Unseen) Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice Higher in the 2s and 3s conditions than in the 1s condition Declines significantly with increasing difficulty in the 1s condition (p = .026), but not in the 2s and 3s conditions. The 1s condition replicates Son (2004) and Exp. 2. How many pairs did participants see? In the 1s condition, the number of items participants saw declines significantly as a function of increasing difficulty. Although a similar trend is evident at the slower presentation durations, the magnitude of the effect is much smaller. In the 1s condition, the likelihood of seeing pairs declines as difficulty increases. This parallels the tendency to choose spaced practice less (and massed practice more) as difficulty increases. Seen Pairs Only Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice No significant decline in the likelihood of choosing spaced practice with increasing difficulty Effect of initial presentation duration is also reduced and is not significant. When results were restricted to pairs that were seen, Son’s (2004) results were not replicated. The effect of difficulty on participants’ tendency to choose spaced practice was largely eliminated. The residual trend was not statistically reliable. Experiment 3 Question -- Why are Sonʼs (2004) results replicable only with GRE-synonym pairs and a very fast presentation? Hypothesis - A 1s presentation duration strains participantsʼ ability to perceive and initially encode a pair. As GRE-synonym pairs (but not pairs of common words) increase in difficulty, the cue word tends to become longer and less familiar. Thus, increasing difficulty in GRE-synonym pairs would exacerbate any problems participants have in establishing initial encodings of the pairs. How to test the hypothesis - Ask participants whether they saw the words. Method 79 participants Son’s (2004) GRE-synonym pairs Duration of the first presentation: 1s vs. 2s vs. 3s The procedure was identical to the previous experiments with one critical exception. Immediately after participants decided whether a pair should be massed, spaced, or done, a screen appeared asking them whether they had seen both words in the pair. They responded “yes” or “no” with a keystroke. Benjamin, A.S., & Bird, R.D. (2006). Metacognitive control of the spacing of study repetitions. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 126-137. Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A Region of Proximal Learning model of study time allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 463-477. Son, L.K. (2004). Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy. JEP: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 601-604. Author Notes We thank Lisa Son for providing us with helpful information, including a copy of the items that she used in her 2004 article. For further information, contact [email protected] or [email protected]. A PDF version of this poster is available at: http://www.sccs.swarthmore.edu/~mcohen/Metacognition_and_Spacing_APS.pdf

Metacognitive Control Over Distribution of Spaced Practice ...€¦ · Son (2004) presented pairs of GRE vocabulary words and common synonyms for 1 s each. After each ... •A PDF

  • Upload
    lydieu

  • View
    215

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

General Method — All Experiments

Procedure. Following Son’s (2004) procedure, word pairs were presented on a computer screen to college-student participants who then were given a choice between studying the pair again “now” (immediately), studying it again “later” (after all word pairs were shown at least once), or being “done” with it (not presented again). (See diagram.)

Duration of the second presentation of each re-studied pair was held constant at 3s. After completing all study trials, participants solved arithmetic problems for 5 min. This was followed by a recall test in which the cue words were presented successively, and participants attempted to type the corresponding target word. Design. Mixed factorial with Pair Difficulty (easy, medium, hard) varied within participants and Duration of the Pairs’ Initial Presentation varied between participants. Materials. Pairs of common words (Exp. 1) or GRE-synonym pairs (Exps. 2 & 3). Item difficulty was determined a priori and manipulated experimentally. Primary dependent variable. Proportion of re-studied items chosen for later, spaced presentation (Proportion Spaced).

Introduction The Question

Memory is generally better when study opportunities are distributed in time (spaced practice) rather than when they occur in immediate succession (massed practice). However, when learners decide practice schedules for themselves, do they choose massed practice or spaced practice?

Background

Son (2004) presented pairs of GRE vocabulary words and common synonyms for 1 s each. After each pair, learners judged how well they knew it (indexing difficulty) and chose to study it again for 3 s, either now (massed), later (spaced) or never (done). For items chosen for restudy (massed or spaced), Son found a negative correlation such that the preference for spaced practice declined as judged difficulty increased.

Son’s results are consistent with the Region of Proximal Learning Model (Metcalfe & Kornell, 2005) which predicts that, for items judged to be learnable, the perceived benefits of immediate study (massed practice) should be greater for more difficult items.

However, Benjamin and Bird (2006), using pairs of common words with related, but different, methods, found a stronger preference for spaced practice on more difficult items. They interpreted their results in terms of the Discrepancy Reduction Model, which predicts a constant or increasing preference for the more effective strategy (spaced practice) as perceived difficulty increases.

The present study

We sought to resolve the conflict from these two earlier studies. Our procedure was similar to Son’s. However, within experiments, we varied item difficulty experimentally (like Benjamin and Bird) rather than correlationally (like Son), and we varied the duration of the initial presentation of each pair. Across experiments, we varied the type of item: Pairs of common words (like Benjamin and Bird) in Exp. 1, and GRE-synonym pairs (like Son) in Exps. 2 and 3.

Discussion Son (2004) reported that as item difficulty increased, learners became less likely to choose spaced practice rather than massed practice. She attributed this to metacognitive judgments in which the perceived benefit of massed practice is assumed to increase with decreases in how well the pairs are judged to be learned. Her account was consistent with the Region of Proximal Learning Model (Metcalfe & Kornell, 2005). However, our findings suggest that Son’s (2004) results were largely an artifact of conditions that prevented learners from fully encoding some items, especially the most difficult ones, on the initial study opportunity. When unseen items were excluded from the analysis, the effect of difficulty on choice of spaced practice was largely eliminated. It was similarly eliminated when pairs involved common words (Exp. 1) in which length and familiarity did not vary as a function of difficulty.

It appears that if learners are unable to fully encode a pair initially, they will want to see it again immediately. However, when this is not the case, learners generally seem to prefer spaced practice. When they do show a preference based on difficulty, it appears to be a preference to space the more difficult items. These findings are in accordance with both Benjamin and Bird’s (2006) results and the Discrepancy Reduction Model.

Further work is necessary to determine whether the preference we observed for spaced practice actually reflects a metacognitive appreciation of its benefits. An alternate possibility is that learners who opt for spaced practice believe their choice will allow them to re-study the pairs closer to the time of the test. Thus, learners’ choices could reflect metacognitive judgments about the effect of the length of the retention interval rather than about the effect of spaced practice. This possibility is being tested.

Michael S. Cohen, Meghan Davis, Amy C. Moors, Thomas C. ToppinoDepartment of Psychology, Villanova University

References

Metacognitive Control Over Distribution of Practice: When is Spacing Preferred?

Cue word ---------- Target word

Now? Later? Done?

Cue word ---------- Target word Cue word

---------- Target word x

Secondary Results — Common to All Experiments Proportion of “done” responses (pairs not selected for re-study).

•  Decreased with increasing difficulty in all experiments •  Did not vary as a function of the duration of initial pair presentation.

This confirms that we successfully manipulated pair difficulty and suggests that participants used the “done” response primarily for pairs that they felt they knew from the outset.

Experiment 1 Method • 34 participants • Pairs of common words • Duration of first presentation: 1s vs. 5s

Results - Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice. • Significantly higher for 5s than 1 s presentation duration • No significant effect of difficulty or its interaction with duration.

The apparent trend for the proportion of spacing choices to increase with difficulty in the 1s condition was reminiscent of Benjamin and Bird’s (2006) findings, but was not reliable. The possibility of a similar trend in the 5s condition was obscured by a ceiling effect.

Experiment 2

Son’s (2004) effect of pair difficulty was not replicated.

Son’s (2004) effect of pair difficulty was replicated with a 1s presentation, but the opposite effect of difficulty was obtained with a 5s presentation.

Experiment 3 - Results

Method • 91 participants • Son’s (2004) GRE-synonym pairs • Duration of first presentation: 1s vs. 5s

Results - Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice. • Greater for 5s than 1s presentation duration, but duration interacted

with difficulty. • Declined significantly with increasing difficulty in the 1s initial

presentation condition (p = .035), replicating Son (2004). • Increased significantly with increasing difficulty in the 5s initial

presentation condition (p = .002), reminiscent of Benjamin & Bird (2006).

Summary of Experiments 1 and 2 Son’s (2004) findings (declining choice of spacing as a function of increasing pair difficulty) were replicated only when we used conditions identical to hers (GRE-synonym pairs presented for a very brief 1s duration).

Using pairs of common words or a long (5s) presentation duration produced either no effect of pair difficulty or a tendency for the choice of spaced practice to increase with increasing difficulty. Overall, spacing was chosen more often when the presentation duration was long (5s) rather than short (1s).

Recall on final test. Spaced pairs had higher recall than massed pairs; thus, spacing was probably the superior strategy

All Re-studied Pairs (Seen and Unseen) Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice • Higher in the 2s and 3s conditions than in the 1s condition • Declines significantly with increasing difficulty in the 1s condition (p = .026), but not in the 2s and 3s conditions.

The 1s condition replicates Son (2004) and Exp. 2.

How many pairs did participants see? • In the 1s condition, the number of items participants saw

declines significantly as a function of increasing difficulty. • Although a similar trend is evident at the slower

presentation durations, the magnitude of the effect is much smaller.

In the 1s condition, the likelihood of seeing pairs declines as difficulty increases. This parallels the tendency to choose spaced practice less (and massed practice more) as difficulty increases.

Seen Pairs Only Proportion of restudied pairs selected for spaced practice • No significant decline in the likelihood of choosing spaced

practice with increasing difficulty • Effect of initial presentation duration is also reduced and is not

significant.

When results were restricted to pairs that were seen, Son’s (2004) results were not replicated. The effect of difficulty on participants’ tendency to choose spaced practice was largely eliminated. The residual trend was not statistically reliable.

Experiment 3 Question -- Why are Sonʼs (2004) results replicable only with GRE-synonym pairs and a very fast

presentation?

Hypothesis - A 1s presentation duration strains participantsʼ ability to perceive and initially encode a pair. As GRE-synonym pairs (but not pairs of common words) increase in difficulty, the cue word tends to become longer and less familiar. Thus, increasing difficulty in GRE-synonym pairs would exacerbate any problems participants have in establishing initial encodings of the pairs.

How to test the hypothesis - Ask participants whether they saw the words.

Method •  79 participants •  Son’s (2004) GRE-synonym pairs •  Duration of the first presentation: 1s vs. 2s vs. 3s •  The procedure was identical to the previous experiments with one critical exception.

Immediately after participants decided whether a pair should be massed, spaced, or done, a screen appeared asking them whether they had seen both words in the pair. They responded “yes” or “no” with a keystroke.

Benjamin, A.S., & Bird, R.D. (2006). Metacognitive control of the spacing of study repetitions. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 126-137.

Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A Region of Proximal Learning model of study time allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 463-477.

Son, L.K. (2004). Spacing one’s study: Evidence for a metacognitive control strategy. JEP: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 601-604.

Author Notes

•  We thank Lisa Son for providing us with helpful information, including a copy of the items that she used in her 2004 article.

•  For further information, contact [email protected] or [email protected].

•  A PDF version of this poster is available at: http://www.sccs.swarthmore.edu/~mcohen/Metacognition_and_Spacing_APS.pdf