55
Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and atypical children’s speech Melissa A. Redford 1 , Vsevolod Kapatsinski 1 , Jolynn Cornell-Fabiano 2 1 Department of Linguistics, University of Oregon 2 César E. Chávez Elementary School, Eugene School District 4J Running head: Classification and evaluation of child speech Key words: Autism, likeability, intelligibility, prosody, acoustics. Acknowledgments. This research was funded by Award Number R01HD061458 (PI: Redford) from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD), and made possible, in part, by the support of the Eugene School District 4J and a fellowship to the 1 st author from the European Institutes for Advanced Study (EURIAS), co-funded by the European Commission (Marie-Sklodowska-Curie Actions COFUND Programme FP7). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the views of NICHD, the Eugene School District 4J, EURIAS or COFUND. We are grateful to Aubrianne Carson, Wook Kyung Choe, and Paul Olejarczuk for significant help with data collection and processing.

Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

FinalversionacceptedforpublicationinLanguageandSpeechJune6,2017.

Laylistenerclassificationandevaluationoftypicalandatypical

children’sspeech

MelissaA.Redford1,VsevolodKapatsinski1,JolynnCornell-Fabiano2

1DepartmentofLinguistics,UniversityofOregon

2CésarE.ChávezElementarySchool,EugeneSchoolDistrict4J

Runninghead:Classificationandevaluationofchildspeech

Keywords:Autism,likeability,intelligibility,prosody,acoustics.

Acknowledgments.ThisresearchwasfundedbyAwardNumberR01HD061458(PI:

Redford)fromtheEuniceKennedyShriverNationalInstituteofChildHealth&Human

Development(NICHD),andmadepossible,inpart,bythesupportoftheEugeneSchool

District4Jandafellowshiptothe1stauthorfromtheEuropeanInstitutesforAdvanced

Study(EURIAS),co-fundedbytheEuropeanCommission(Marie-Sklodowska-CurieActions

COFUNDProgrammeFP7).Thecontentissolelytheresponsibilityoftheauthorsanddoes

notnecessarilyreflecttheviewsofNICHD,theEugeneSchoolDistrict4J,EURIASor

COFUND.WearegratefultoAubrianneCarson,WookKyungChoe,andPaulOlejarczukfor

significanthelpwithdatacollectionandprocessing.

Page 2: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

1

ABSTRACT

Verbalchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorder(ASD)oftenalsohaveatypicalspeech.In

thecontextofthemanychallengesassociatedwithASD,dospeechsoundpattern

differencesreallymatter?Thecurrentstudyaddressedthisquestion.Structured

spontaneousspeechwaselicitedfrom34children:17withASD,whosecliniciansreported

unusualspeechprosody;and17typically-developing,age-matchedcontrols.Multiword

utteranceswereexcerptedfromeachchild’sspeechsampleandpresentedtoyoungadult

listeners,whohadnoclinicaltrainingorexperience.InExperiment1,listenersclassified

bandpassfilteredandunalteredexcerptsas“typical”or“disordered”.ChildrenwithASD

wereonlydistinguishedfromtypicalchildrenbasedonunalteredspeech,buttheanalyses

indicateduniquecontributionsfromspeechsoundpatterns.InExperiment2,listeners

providedlikeabilityratingsonthefilteredandunalteredexcerpts.Again,laylistenersonly

distinguishedchildrenwithASDfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedonunaltered

speech,withtypicalchildrenratedasmorelikeablethanchildrenwithASD.InExperiment

3,listenersevaluatedtheunalteredspeechalongseveralperceptualdimensions.High

overlapbetweenthedimensionsofarticulation,clearness,andfluencywascapturedbyan

emergentdimension:intelligibility.Thisdimensionpredictedlisteners’likeabilityratings

nearlyaswellasitpredictedtheirjudgmentsofdisorder.Overall,theresultsshowthatlay

listenerscandistinguishatypicalfromtypicalchildrenoutsidethesocial-interactional

contextbasedsolelyonspeech,andthattheyattendtospeechintelligibilitytodothis.Poor

intelligibilityalsocontributestolisteners’negativesocialevaluationofchildren,andso

meritsassessmentandremediation.

Page 3: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

2

INTRODUCTION

TempleGrandin,diagnosedwithautismasachild,characterizedherearlyspeechin

thefollowingway:"Myvoicewasflatwithlittleinflectionandnorhythm.Thatalone

stampedmeasdifferent(Grandin&Scariano,1986:21).”Especiallyinchildhood,any

differencethatdeviatesfrompeer-definednormscanjeopardizeachild’saccesstopeer

friendshipsandsocialsupport.Forexample,inthedomainofspeechandlanguage,Rice

andcolleagueshaveshownthatchildrenwithspecificlanguageimpairmentandthosewho

learnEnglishasasecondlanguagehavelessaccesstopositivesocialinteractionsinUS

schoolsthantypicallydeveloping,nativeEnglish-speakingchildren(Rice,Sell,&Hadley,

1991;Gertner,Rice,&Hadley,1994).Atypicalspeechpatternsmayfunctionlikelanguage

disorderorincompletesecondlanguageacquisitionbycreatingbarrierstosocial

interactionforchildrenwithASD.Butunlikelanguagedisorderorincompletesecond

languageacquisition,theproblemmayhavelesstodowithcommunicationperseand

everythingtodowiththeconveyanceofdifference;specifically,adifferencethatresultsin

anegativesocialevaluation.ThispossibilitywasnotedevenbyAsperger(1944,as

translatedbyFrith,1991:70),whoconcludedhisdescriptionofatypicalspeechinchildren

withASDbysayingthat“thelanguagefeelsunnatural,oftenlikeacaricature,which

provokesridiculeinthenaïvelistener.”Thegoalofthepresentresearchwastotestthe

hypothesizedlinkbetweenatypicalspeechpatternsandanegativesocialevaluationof

childrenwithASD.Asecondarygoalwastoidentifythepatternsthatlaylistenersattendto

whenclassifyingchildren’sspeechastypicalornotandwhenevaluatingchildren’s

likeabilityasafunctionoftheirspeech.

Page 4: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

3

Background

Afairlysubstantialbodyofscientificworkconfirmsthatindividualswithautism

whospeakdoindeedproduceatypicalspeechsoundpatterns,andespeciallyatypical

speechprosody.Forexample,Shribergandcolleagues(Shriberg,Paul,McSweeny,Klin,

Cohen,&Volkmar,2001)usedtheversatileandcomprehensivebutunstandardized

Prosody-VoiceScreeningProfile(PVSP;Shriberg,Kwiatkowski,&Rasmussen,1990)in

theirstudyofspontaneousspeechin30individualswithASD:trainedlistenersevaluated

speechacrossavarietyofpredeterminedprosodicandvoicecategories,including

loudness,pitch,voicequality,resonancequality,phrasing,rate,andstress.Themost

consistentvoicedeficitwasinresonancequality:aninappropriatenasalqualitywasmore

oftenidentifiedinspeechproducedbyindividualswithASDthanbythecontrols.Themost

consistentprosodicdeficitwasstress;acategorythatincludedexcessive/equalstress,

prolongationandblockingtypedisfluencies,aswellasincorrectproductionoflexicalstress

patternsandaccentualpatternsatthelevelofthephrase(labeled“contrastivestress”).The

authorsreportthattheperceiveddifferencesbetweenindividualswithASDandcontrol

speakerswasduetoagreaternumberofperceiveddisfluenciesand“misplacedword

stress.”(p.1107).Insofarasanumberofotherperceptualstudiessimilarlyreportautism-

relateddeficitsintheproductionoflexicalstressandphrasalaccenting(e.g.,Baltaxe&

Guthrie,1987;McCaleb&Prizant,1985;Paul,Augustyn,Klin,&Volkmar,2005;Grossman,

Bemis,Plesa-Skwerer,&Tager-Flusberg,2010),itislikelythatdeficitsin“stress”

productionisadefiningfeatureofatypicalprosodyinASD.

Incontrasttoperceptualstudies,acousticstudiesonautisticspeechhavefocusedon

globalpatternsrelevanttocharacterizingtheprosodicdifferencesinindividualswithASD.

Forexample,Diehletal.(2009)extractedaverageF0(theacousticcorrelateofpitch)and

Page 5: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

4

itsstandarddeviation(=pitchvariability)overlargetemporalwindows(250+

milliseconds)innarrativespeechproducedbyatotalof38childrenandadolescentswith

ASDand38typicalcontrols.Theyfoundthatpitchvariabilitywasgenerallyhigherin

speechproducedbychildrenwithASDthanintypicallydevelopingchildren.Otherstudies

reportsimilarresults(Sharda,Subhardra,Sahay,…&Singh,2010;Bonneh,Levanon,Dean-

Pardon,Lossos,&Adini2011;Depape,Chen,Hall,&Trainor,2012;Nadig&Shaw,2012),

whichcouldbeconsistentwiththeabove-mentionedfindingofimpairedphrasalaccenting

inautisticspeechifpitchvariabilitycapturessomethingaboutaccentualpatterning.

Inadditiontodeficitsinlexicalstressproductionandphrasalaccenting,thereis

someevidencethatindividualswithASDproducespeechatslowerratesthantypical

controls(forareviewseeShriberg,Paul,Black,&vanSanten,2011).Forexample,Diehl

andPaul(2011)usedthesameglobalF0measuresasDiehletal.(2009)onutterances

obtainedfrom24childrenwithASDand22typicallydevelopingchildrenindifferent

subtestsoftheformallystructuredbutunstandardizedProfilingElementsofProsodyin

Speech-Communicationtest(Peppé&McCann,2003),butfoundnodifferencebetweenthe

groups.TheyalsotookmeasuresofF0rangeaswellasutterance-levelmeasuresof

acousticdurationandintensity.Onlydurationdifferencesweresignificant:childrenwith

ASDproducedlongerutterancesonaveragethanthetypicalcontrols.

Studiesthatuseperceptualevaluationormeasureglobalacousticstoidentify

prosodicdeficits/differencesassumeanexpandeddefinitionofprosodyrelativetothat

whichisassumedincarefullinguisticphoneticstudies.Butwhereasthelinguist’sinterest

isintherealizationofabstractmetricalandintonationalstructuresandthealignment

betweenthem,theclinician’sinterestisinidentifyingthosepatternsthatcouldadversely

affectanindividual’sabilitytocommunicatewith—inordertobeacceptedby—peers.The

Page 6: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

5

searchforprosodicdeficitsisthusexpandedtoincludeanysuprasegmentalpatternin

autisticspeechthatmightreliablysignaldifferenceordisorder.

Yet,evenwithitswidepurview,existingresearchonthedifferencesbetween

autisticandneurotypicalspeechreturnsfewconsistentfindings.Considerforexamplethe

contradictoryresultsonthestatusofatypicalpitchvariabilityinindividualswithASD:

Diehlandcolleagues(2009)findsignificantdifferencesbetweenchildrenwithandwithout

ASDinonetask,butDiehlandPaul(2011)findnosuchdifferencesinanothertask.Such

inconsistentresultsmaybeduetotheabsenceofdeficitsinasignificantproportionof

high-functioningindividualswithASD.Althoughsystematicdataonthedistributionof

atypicalprosodyinautismdonotyetexist,ithasbeensuggestedthatprosodicdeficitsmay

onlybeevidentinthespeechofhalfofallindividualswithASD(e.g.,Paul,Shribergetal.,

2005).

Despitecontradictoryfindingsandthepossibilitythatprosodicdisorderoccursin

onlyhalfofallhigh-functioningindividualswithASD,thetopicofprosodic

difference/deficitsinautismhasreceivedincreasingattentioninrecentyears.Whereas

McCannandPeppé(2003)foundthatbetween1980and2002only16studiesonprosody

inautism,ascholar.google.frsearchinearly2017on“prosodyautismprosodicASD”

returned1,010results.Thetop100results,whichincludedofarticles,conference

proceedingsandbookchapters,indicatedatleast57newstudies(i.e.,thosepublishedin

peer-reviewedjournals)thatwerefocusedonsomeaspectofprosodyorprosodic

processinginthespeechofindividualswithASD.Thiskindofattentionissurprisinggiven

thatautismis,afterall,characterizedprimarilybydeficitsinsocialcommunicationand

interactionaswellasbyrestrictedandrepetitivepatternsofbehaviorthataredisabling

(AmericanPsychiatricAssociation,2013).ThediagnosticcriteriasetforthintheDiagnostic

Page 7: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

6

andStatisticalManualofMentalDisorders(DSM-5)donotevenmentionatypicalspeech

soundpatterns,letaloneprosody.Theydohoweverreferencebehaviorssuchas“failureto

initiateorrespondtosocialinteractions”and“abnormalitiesineyecontactandbody

language...toatotallackoffacialexpressions”aswellasstereotypedorrepetitivemotor

movements…orspeech”(e.g.,echolaliaoridiosyncraticphrases).Giventhedisabling

natureofthecriterialbehaviorsinautism,whicharesocialorperseverativeinnature

ratherthanprosodic,isthereagoodreasontofocusevermoreresearchontheunusual

speechpatternsofindividualswithASD?Thisquestionmotivatedthecurrentstudy.

TheCurrentStudy

Thepracticedclinicianapparentlyhasnotroubleidentifyingindividualswith

autismbasedonatypicalspeechsoundpatternswhenthesearepresent(Klin,Sparrow,

Marans,Carter,&Volkmar,2000).Cliniciansintrainingcanalsoreliablydistinguish

speakerswithandwithoutautismbasedonshortconversationalspeechsamplesthatare

measurablydifferentwithrespecttopitchvariability(Nadig&Shaw,2012).Theseabilities

areperhapsnotsurprisinggiventhat,aslanguagespeakers,weareattunedtosound

patternsinspeechthatindexsocialinformation.Thissensitivityallowslistenerstoadeptly

categorizespeechbyethnolect(Lass,Almerino,Jordan,&Walsh,1980;Purnell,Idsardi,&

Baugh,1999),dialect(Clopper&Pisoni,2004;Clopper&Bradlow,2009),genderidentity

(Lassetal.,1980;Munson,2007),andsexualorientation(Munson,McDonald,DeBoe,&

White,2005;Munson,2007).Moreover,listenersdonotthemselvesneedtobeexpertin

thesocialcategoriesofinteresttoeffectivelyclassifyspeechbasedonsoundpattern

differences(Purnelletal.,1999;Clopper&Bradlow,2009).Forexample,Clopperand

Bradlow(2009)usedafreeclassificationtasktoshowthatnon-nativespeakerswereable

toclusterAmericanEnglishspeechsamplesbydialect,albeitlessaccuratelythannative

Page 8: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

7

speakersofAmericanEnglish.Otherstudiesstronglysuggestthattheperceptionofsocial

informationinspeechhassocialconsequences(vanBezooijen,1995;Purnelletal.,1999).

Forexample,Purnellandcolleaguesfoundthatlandlordsdiscriminatedagainsttenants

basedsolelyonspeechpatternsthatindexedanAfricanAmericanorChicanoidentity.

Ourautomaticidentificationofsocialinformationinspeechcoupledwith

impressionisticdescriptionsofatypicalspeechsoundpatternsinautismastooloud,shrill,

sing-songy,mechanicalandsoon(see,e.g.,Asperger,1944inFrith,1991;Klinetal.,2000)

suggestthatautisticspeechmaybenoticeabletoandsociallyevaluatedbylistenerswho

havenoknownpriorexperiencewithautism.Butthequestionremainsastohowimpactful

thesespeechdifferencesreallyare.Morespecifically,isitthespeechpersethatidentifiesa

personwithautismas“other”orisitperhapsthesuiteofunusualcommunicativeand

repetitivebehaviorstypicalofautismthatengendertheperceptionofotherness?Also,if

othernesscanbediscernedbasedsolelyonspeechpatterns,wouldsuchanidentification

resultinaninherentlynegativesocialevaluation?Tobegintoanswerthesequestions,the

currentstudytestedtheeasewithwhichlaylistenerscandistinguishchildrenwithASD

fromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedsolelyonshortsnippetsoftheirprosodically

impairedspeech.Wealsoinvestigatedhowlistenersevaluatedthisspeechabsentasocial

orlinguisticcontext.

Acousticstudiesthatfindgroupdifferencesbetweenautisticandneurotypical

speechstronglysuggestthatspeech-specificdifferencesarethereandshouldbe

perceptibleeventolaylisteners.Yet,eventhissuggestionhasyettoberigorouslytested.

TheperceptualratingstudyconductedbyNadigandShaw(2012)providessomeevidence

infavoroftheperceptualsaliencyofdifference,buttheirlistenerswerespeech-language

pathologistsintrainingwhowerealsospecificallytaughttoattendtopitchchangesinthe

Page 9: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

8

speechsamplesfromchildrenwithandwithoutASD.Inaddition,thesamplesthatNadig

andShaw’slistenersratedwereexcerptedfromconversationswithnocontrolover

languagedifferences(wordchoice,appropriatenessofresponse,etc.)thatcouldinfluence

judgmentsoftypicality.Thecurrentstudyprovidesamorerigoroustestofthehypothesis

thattheatypicalspeechproducedbychildrenwithASDandunusualprosodyis

perceptuallydistinctfromtypicalchildren’sspeech(Experiment1).Aftershowingthatitis,

wegoontoinvestigatewhetherperceiveddifferenceresultsinanegativesocialevaluation

ofthespeaker(Experiment2).Wefollowupbyidentifyingtheperceptualdimensionsthat

listenersusebothtodistinguishatypicalfromtypicalspeechandtosociallyevaluatea

speaker(Experiment3).Themethodsweusecontrolasbestaspossibleforinfluencesof

social-interactionalcontextonlisteners’classificationandevaluationofspeech:weelicited

children’sspontaneousspeechinanarrativetask,ratherthaninaconversationalone,and

furtherdecontextualizedeventhisspeechbyextractingonlyafew,fluentlyproduced

utterancesfromeachnarrative.Thestimuliandjudgmenttaskswerealsodesignedtotest

thefundamentalsaliencyofprosodicdifferences,andtocontrolforpossibleeffectsof

languageonlisteners’evaluationofspeechpatterns.Finally,weusedlaylisteners,whohad

noknownaprioriexperiencewithASDandrelativelylittleexperienceonaveragewith

children,tomaximizethepossibilitythatlistenerjudgmentswerereflectiveofbroader

societalnormsratherthandependentonpersonalhistoriesandexperiences.Intheseways,

wesoughttoanswerourmotivatingquestionofwhethertheatypicalspeechsound

patternsoftenassociatedwithASDarethemselvesimportanttoaddressdirectlygiventhe

manyothersocially-disablingbehaviorsmorecentraltoautism.

Page 10: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

9

EXPERIMENT1

ThegoalofExperiment1wastotestlaylisteners’abilitytodistinguishchildren

withASDfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedsolelyontheiratypicalspeech.The

hypothesisthatlistenerswouldbeabletodothisfollowsfromourautomaticidentification

ofsocialinformationinspeechandfromacousticstudiesthathavemeasuredandfound

differencesinspeechproducedbymanychildrenandadultswithASDcomparedtothose

withoutASD,particularlyintheprosodicdomain.Sincethesestudiesalsoshowthat

prosodicdeficitsdonotappearinallindividualswithASD,weobtainedspeechsamples

onlyfromchildrenwithASDwhowerealreadydeemedbytheirspeech–language

pathologisttohaveunusualspeechprosody.

Methods

Participants

Sixteencollege-agedadultsprovidedlayjudgmentsofdisorder.Theseadultswere

recruitedfromintroductorypsychologyandlinguisticsclasses,andreceivedcoursecredit

fortheirparticipation.AllreportednormalhearingandEnglishastheirnativelanguage.

Thelistenersevaluatedspeechobtainedfrom34children,whoalsohadnormalhearing

andEnglishastheirnativelanguage.Seventeenofthechildren(14boys)hadspecial

education(SPED)eligibilitiesforreceivingspeechandlanguageservicesintheschools

underautismspectrumdisorder(ASD).Theirmeanagewas9;0years(SD=18months).

Theother17children(13boys)weretypicallydeveloping.Theirmeanagewas8;9years

(SD=15months).

ChildrenwithASDwererecruitedforthestudythroughalocalnetworkofschool

speech–languagepathologists(SLPs)in2011.TheSLPswereaskedtoreferchildrenfrom

theircaseloadwhohad(1)aSPEDeligibilityunderASD,(2)normalcognition,and(3)

Page 11: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

10

unusualprosody.InthestateofOregon,adeterminationofautismforSPEDeligibilityis

madebytheschooldistrictandinvolvesateamofeducationprofessionalsincludinga

schoolSLPandclinicalpsychologist.Thisteammakestheirdeterminationbasedon

observationofthechildinandoutoftheclassroom,andusesassessmentinstrumentsto

evaluatetargetbehaviors,cognition,speechandlanguage(seeOregonAdministrative

RulesforSpecialEducationathttp://www.ode.state.or.us/offices/slp/spedoars.pdf).The

AutismDiagnosticObservationSchedule(ADOS;Lord,Rutter,Pamela,Dilavore,&Risi,

2008)isnotused,norisamedicaldiagnosisofautismrequiredoracceptedasasubstitute

forafullevaluationbythedistrictteam.Regardingunusualprosody,wereliedonthe

schoolSLP’sinterpretationofwhatthismightmeaninkeepingwithboththegoalsofthe

studyandtheverybroaddefinitionofwhatconstitutesprosodyinresearchonautistic

speech.

Wecollecteddatafrom18childrenwhowerethuslyreferred,butonechild’sspeech

wasaccidentallynotrecorded.Symptomseverityfortheremaining17childrenwas

measuredbyaskingthereferringSLPtocompletetheChildhoodAutismRatingScale

(CARS2;Schopler,VanBourgodien,Wellman,&Love,2010).Parentalreportwasusedto

establishwhetherornotthechildhadexperiencedlanguagedelay.Receptivevocabulary

scoreswereobtainedusingthePeabodyPictureVocabularyTest(PPVT-4;Dunn&Dunn,

2007).Table1reportsthesex,age,ASDsymptomseverity,languagedelay,and

standardizedreceptivevocabularyscoresforeachchild.

InsertTable1abouthere.

Typicallydevelopingchildrenwerenotspecificallyrecruitedforthecurrentstudy.

Instead,thesechildrenrepresentedasubsetof100childrenwhohadparticipatedina

Page 12: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

11

longitudinalstudyonthetypicalacquisitionofprosodyinschool-agedchildren,whichwas

on-goingatthetimeofthecurrentstudy.Speechsampleswereselectedfrom17ofthe

childrenwhobestmatchedtheASDgroupinageandsexatthetimeofthepresentstudy.

Typicaldevelopmentwasassessedbasedonparentreportregardingthechild’shearing,

speech,language,andmedicalhistory.Themeanageinthisgroupwasnotsignificantly

differentfromthemeanageinthegroupofchildrenwithASD.Table2reportsthesex,age,

andstandardizedreceptivevocabularyscoresofthetypicallydevelopingchildren.

InsertTable2abouthere.

ElicitationProcedure

Structuredspontaneousspeechsampleswereobtainedusingastorytellingtask.An

experimenterpresentedchildrenwith4picturebooksthatdepicteddifferentadventures

ofafrogand/oraboyandadog(i.e.,thefrogstorybooksbyMercerMayer).Childrenwere

askedtochoosethepicturebookthats/hewouldmostliketonarrate.Typicallydeveloping

childrenfamiliarizedthemselveswiththeirbookofchoiceunderexperimenter

supervision,thentoldtheirstorytoaparentorcaregivertwotimes.Storyrepetitionwas

usedtocontrolfortheeffectsoflanguageplanningonthespeechproduced(seeRedford,

2013).Asourinterestisinspeechandnotincognitiveinfluencesonlanguageproduction,

thesamplesusedinthepresentstudywereextractedfromthechildren’ssecond

storytelling.

PilotworkindicatedthatchildrenwithASDresistedtellingthesamestorytwicetoa

parentorcaregiver.Theelicitationprotocolwasthereforeadaptedinthefollowingway.

Childrenchoseabooktonarrate,thendevelopedastoryinresponsetoquestionsand

promptsfromtheexperimenterwhilelookingthroughtheirbookofchoice.Thechild’s

Page 13: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

12

caregiverwastheninvitedbackintotheroom(ortheconversationiftheyhadnotleft),and

thechildwasinstructedto“telltheirstory”tothecaregiver.Thescaffoldingprocedure

servedthesamefunctionasafirststorytellinginourstudyontypicallydeveloping

children’sspeechacquisition:itallowedchildrentoplantheirstoryandpracticethe

languagetheywouldusetotellthestory.

Allchildren’sstoriesweredigitallyrecordedontoaMarantzPMD660(witha

samplingrateof44,100Hz)usingaShureULXS4standardwirelessreceiverandalavaliere

microphone,whichwasattachedtoabaseballhatorheadbandthatthespeakerwore.

ChildrenwithASDwouldsometimesrefusetowearthehatorheadband,inwhichcasethe

microphonewasplacedeitheronthechild’sclothingoronthetablebetweenthe

experimenterandchild.

SpeechSamples

Fourmultiwordutteranceswereextractedfromthemiddleofeachchild’sstoryto

avoidthestereotypedlanguageandprosodyassociatedwithstorybeginningsandendings.

Utteranceswereselectedonthebasisofprosodiccompletenessandlength:utterancesthat

wereroughly2secondslongwereselectedoverthosethatwereshorterormuchlonger.

Thegoalwastoselectphrasesthatwouldbestrepresentphrase-levelrhythmand

intonation(hencetherelativelylongminimumlength)whilecontrollingforsignificant

juncturesduetopausesandotherdisfluencies(hencetherelativelyshortmaximum

length).SamplesfromchildrenwithASDrangedinlengthfrom1.12to3.37seconds(M=

2.21)andfrom4to14syllables(M=8.08).Samplesfromtypicallydevelopingchildren

rangedinlengthfrom1.45to3.06seconds(M=2.17)andfrom5to14syllables(M=9.15).

Onceutteranceswereextracted,filteringwasusedtocreatestimuliforapure

prosodycondition.Theunalteredutteranceswereusedtocreatestimuliforthe

Page 14: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

13

correspondingfullspeechcondition.Thegoalofthemanipulationwastotesttheextentof

prosodicdifferenceinthespeechofASDversustypicallydevelopingchildren,keepingin

mindthatfilteringalwayssubstantiallydegradesthespeechsignal.

Twodifferentbandpassfilterswereused:onepreservedlowerfrequencies(250to

750Hz)andtheotherhigherfrequencies(500to1000Hz).Thebandpassfilterthat

preservedlowerfrequencieswasusedtocapturerhythmicinformationassociatedwiththe

alternationofsonorantandnonsonorantspeechintervals.Thebandpassfilterthat

preservedhigherfrequencieseliminatedtheremaininglowfrequencyenergyassociated

withthosesonorantconsonantsproducedwithsignificantoralocclusioninonsetposition.

Thegoalofthisfilterwasthereforetopreserveinformationaboutthespacingofvowel

onsets,whichhavebeenreferredtoastheperceptualcentersofrhythmicbeatsinspeech

(Port,2003).Notethatpitch(i.e.,F0)informationwaspreservedunderbothmanipulations

becauselistenerscalculatepitchfromthefrequencyspacingbetweenpreservedharmonics

(Moore,1994).Intheend,filtertypehadnosignificanteffectonlistenerjudgmentsandso

werecombinedintheanalysesreportedbelow,whichthereforeonlyrefertofilteredand

unalteredspeechsamples.

Filteredandunalteredutteranceswereamplitudenormalizedto75dBusingthe

scalefunctioninPraat(Boersma&Weenik,2009).Individualutterancesfromeachspeaker

werethenconcatenatedinrandomordertocreatestimulithatwereblockedbyspeaker.

Utteranceswereseparatedby300millisecondsofsilenceineachblock.Blockingwas

deemednecessarybecausepilotworkindicatedthatnaïvelistenersneedtohearmorethan

2secondsofspeechfromasingleindividualtomakeareliablejudgment.Theblocks

rangedindurationfrom7.27to12.51seconds(M=9.80)forchildrenwithASDandfrom

8.18to10.86seconds(M=9.51)fortypicallydevelopingchildren.

Page 15: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

14

ClassificationTask

Listenerswereaskedtomakejudgmentsof“typical”and“disordered”basedonthe

speechsamples.Listenersalsoindicatedtheirconfidenceineachjudgmentona5-point

ratingscale.Allresponseoptionsweredisplayedaslabeledbuttonsonacomputer

monitor.Thetwoalternativejudgments,“typical”and“disordered”,wereplacedinarow

abovetheconfidenceratingscale,whichalsoappearedasarow.Listenerswouldheara

stimulusblock,makeajudgment,ratetheirjudgment,thenpress“OK”tocontinueinthe

task.Stimulusblockswerepresentedinrandomorder.Listenersheardonelow-frequency

bandpassfilteredstimulusblockandonehigh-frequencybandpassfilteredstimulusblock

perspeaker,andsomadeatotalof68typicalityjudgments(34speakersx2repetitions)in

thefilteredspeechcondition.Listenersheardthesameunalteredstimulusblocktwice,and

soalsomadeatotalof68typicalityjudgmentsinthefullspeechcondition.

Listenerinstructionsincludedinformationaboutthespeakers,abouthowthe

speechstimuliwerecreated,andtheeffectofbandpassfilteringonspeech.Specifically,

listenersweretoldthatthespeechsampleshadbeenelicitedfromchildrenbetweenthe

agesof5and11yearsold,andthathalfofthechildrenwerereceivingspeech-language

servicesintheschool.Listenerswereassuredtheresearchobjectivewastohelptarget

speech-languagedeficitsinchildrenwithdisability,andsotheyshouldnotfeelinhibitedin

assigningajudgmentof“disordered”toanyspeechsampletheythoughtwasproducedbya

childwithdisability.Listenerswerealsotoldthattheutterancesthatmadeupeachsample

weretakenfromalargerstorytellingtask,andthatthestorycontextwasnotpreserved.

Finally,listenersweretoldthatsomeofthespeechtheywouldhearwasfilteredtoremove

asmuchlanguageinformationaspossible.Theywereinstructedtomakejudgmentsbased

onwhatremainedperceptibleinthefilteredspeech;namely,rhythmandintonation.The

Page 16: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

15

experimenteralsomadesurethatlistenersunderstoodwhatspeechrhythmandintonation

were.Theseinstructionsweregiventoprovidelistenerswitheveryopportunityto

distinguishchildrenwithASDfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeerswhenprosodyalone

waspreservedinthefilteredsignal.

Finally,inordertogaininsightintothebasisforjudgmentsof“typical”versus

“disordered,”weaskedlistenerstoprovidewrittenfeedbackregardingtheirperceptual

criteria.Specifically,listenerswereaskedto:“Pleasedescribethecharacteristicsinthe

speechsamplesthatledyoutodistinguishbetween“typical”and“disordered”speech

(please,especially,notewhatyouthinkmadethespeechsounddisordered).”Listeners

respondedtothisrequesttwice:firstaftercompletingjudgmentsinthefilteredspeech

condition,andthenagainaftercompletingjudgmentsintheunalteredspeechcondition.

ControlTasks

Twoadditionaltaskswereincludedintheexperiment;bothwereusedtocontrolfor

theeffectsoflanguage(e.g.,wordchoice,syntacticstructure,samplecoherence)on

“typical”and“disordered”judgments.Thecontroltaskforthefilteredspeechcondition

investigatedtheeffectivenessofthefilteratremovinglanguageinformation:listeners

indicatedroughlyhowmanywordstheythoughttheyunderstoodinthefilteredspeech

samplesona5pointscale:none,some,half,most,orall.These5responseoptionswere

presentedinarowofboxesonthemonitor.Confidenceratingswereagaincollectedon

eachjudgmentthatthelistenermade.Thecontrolfortheunalteredspeechcondition

directlyinvestigatedeffectsofwordchoice,syntacticstructure,andotherlanguage

featuresontypicalityjudgments.Thistaskrequiredlistenerstomake“typical”or

“disordered”judgmentsbasedonwrittenversionsoftheutterancesratherthanonaurally

presentedversions.Thewrittenversionsofaspeaker’sutterancesweredisplayedfor10

Page 17: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

16

secondstoapproximatethedurationoftheaudiosamples.Listenersworeheadphones

whenmakingtext-basedjudgmentsjustasinallotherconditions,theythenclickedon

“typical”or“disordered”asintheotherclassificationtasks,andalsoindicatedtheirdegree

ofconfidenceintheirjudgment.

Theexperimentalandcontroltaskswerealwayscompletedinthefollowingorder:

filteredspeech,comprehension,text(i.e.,writtenstimuli),unalteredspeech.Thefixed

orderhadtodowiththefactthatfilteringonlyeffectivelyeliminatesaccesstolanguage

contentiflistenershavenoaprioriexpectationsaboutthiscontent.

Analyses

Thebinaryresponseswerecoded“0”for“typical”and“1”for“disordered”and

summedacrosslistenerstoobtainasinglescoreforeachsampleineachofthetaskswhere

thisresponsewaselicited.Judgmentscoresintheexperimentaltaskswerethenstrippedof

languageinfluenceinthefollowingway.Standardizedcomprehensionratingscoresfor

eachsamplewereusedtopredictthesummed“disordered”scoresinthefilteredspeech

condition(i.e.,acrossfiltertype)usinglinearregression.Similarly,summed“disordered”

scoresfromthetext-onlyjudgmenttaskwereusedtopredictthesummed“disordered”

scoresintheunalteredspeechcondition.Theresidualscoresfromtheseanalyseswere

saved,andthetwoscoresperspeakeraveragedtoobtainthedependentvariableforthe

principleanalysis,whichinvestigatedlisteners’abilitytodistinguishspeechproducedby

childrenwithASDfromspeechproducedbychildrenwithtypicaldevelopment.Listeners’

confidenceintheirtypicalityjudgmentswerealsoanalyzedasafunctionofspeakergroup.

Thesewerestandardizedwithinlistenerusingaz-transform,thenaveragedacross

listenersandwithinspeakertoobtainthemeanlistenerconfidenceratingperspeaker.

Page 18: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

17

Theprincipleanalysesusedamixed-designANOVAtoassessthebetween-subjects

effectofspeakergroup(ASDversustypical)andwithin-subjectseffectofspeechcondition

(filteredversusunaltered)onthedependentvariables:residualized“disordered”

judgmentsandstandardizedconfidenceratings.AShapiro-Wilktestindicatedthatvalues

associatedwiththesedependentvariableswerenormallydistributed.Asignificanteffectof

groupon“disordered”judgmentswouldindicatethatlistenerscandistinguishchildren

withASDfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedonspeechpatternsalone.An

interactionbetweengroupandspeechconditionwouldindicatetheinfluenceofnon-

prosodicfactorson“disordered”judgments.Speaker’sage-in-monthsandstandardized

PPVTscoreswereincludedascovariatesintheanalysestocontrolfortheireffectsonthe

dependentvariables.

Results

ListenerResponsesbyJudgmentTaskandSpeakerGroup

Theuntransformedandunstandardizedresponsesacrossthedifferenttaskswere

notthefocusofanalysis,butaresummarizedinTable3togivethereaderafullersenseof

thedata.Notethatcumulative“disordered”scoreswerehigheronaverageforchildren

withASDcomparedtotheirtypicallydevelopingpeersacrossalltasks.Notealsothatthe

meandifferencebetweenthetwogroupsofchildrenwasespeciallylargeintheunaltered

speechtask.

InsertTable3abouthere.

EffectofSpeakerGrouponResidualizedJudgmentsofDisorder

Recallthattoinvestigatetheinfluenceofspeechpatternsonjudgmentsabsentthe

influenceoflanguageweregressedthescoresobtainedfromthecontroltasks

Page 19: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

18

(comprehensionandtext-basedjudgments)againstthescoresobtainedfromthe

experimentaltasks(filteredspeechandunalteredspeechjudgments).Theanalysisonthe

residualsindicatedthatlistenersweremorelikelytojudgespeechproducedbychildren

withASDas“disordered”thanspeechproducedbychildrenwithtypicaldevelopment,F(1,

30)=4.56,p=.041,ηp2=.132.Thiseffectofgroupinteractedwithcondition,F(1,30)=

4.52,p=.042,ηp2=.131,inthatlistenerswerebetterabletodistinguishbetweenASDand

typicallydevelopingchildrenintheunalteredconditionthaninthefilteredcondition(see

Figure1).Thesimpleeffectofconditionwasnotsignificant.

InsertFigure1abouthere.

Giventhesignificantinteractionbetweenconditionandgroup,andtheseemingly

weakeffectofgroupon“disordered”judgmentsinthefilteredspeechcondition(Figure1),

followupanalyseswereconductedtodeterminewhetherlistenerswereinfactableto

distinguishchildrenwithASDfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersinthefilteredspeech

condition.Thedataweresplitbyconditionandtheeffectofgrouponresidualized

“disordered”judgmentswastestedagain.Theresultswerethatlistenersdistinguished

betweengroupsofchildrenbasedonspeechpatternsintheunalteredspeechcondition,

F(1,30)=5.99,p=.020,ηp2=.166,butnotinthefilteredspeechcondition,F(1,30)=0.70,

p>.1.Notethattheeffectofgroupon“disordered”judgmentsintheunalteredspeech

conditionissignificantevenwhenalphaiscorrectedformultiplecomparisons(i.e.,the

Bonferronicorrectedalphais.025).

WrittenFeedbackfromListenersontheirJudgments

Again,theeffectsoflanguagewerecontrolledintheunalteredspeechconditionby

partiallingoutvarianceinjudgmentsduetowordchoice,syntacticstructure,andso.Thus,

Page 20: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

19

theresultsshowninFigure1indicatethatlistenersdifferentiatedchildrenwithASDand

prosodicdisorderfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedonspeechsoundpatterns

alone.Yet,therewasnoeffectofgroupontheresidualized“disordered”judgmentsinthe

filteredspeechcondition.Thissuggestseitherthatprosodicdifferencesbetweenthetwo

groupsofchildrenweresubtleatbestandlistenersonlygainedsensitivitytothose

differencesastheexperimentprogressedorthatlistenersattendedtosomethingother

thanprosodywhenmakingjudgmentsofdisorder.Listenerfeedbacksuggestedboth

explanationscouldbetrue.Specifically,listenersreportedattendingtoarticulation,speech

clarity,fluency(=flow,rate,rhythm),andaccenting(=wordemphasis,intonation)when

makingjudgments.Fluencyandaccentingareclearlyprosodicinnature;articulationand

speechclarityreferencesegmentalcharacteristics.Table4summarizesthefeedbackwe

obtainedfromeachofthe16listenerswhoprovidedjudgmentsonthefilteredand

unalteredspeechsamples.

InsertTable4abouthere.

Listeners’ConfidenceintheirJudgments

Theanalysisonlisteners’standardizedconfidenceratingssupportthesuggestion

thatthespeechsamplesobtainedfromchildrenwithASDweresubtlydifferentfromthose

obtainedfromtypicallydevelopingchildren.Theymayalsohavebeenmorevariable.

Specifically,therewasasignificanteffectofgrouponlisteners’standardizedratings,F(1,

30)=10.75,p=.003,ηp2=.264,asshowninFigure2.Listenerswerelessconfidentintheir

judgmentsofspeechproducedbychildrenwithASDthanintheirjudgmentsofspeech

producedbychildrenwhoweretypicallydeveloping.Therewasnoeffectofconditionor

anyinteractionbetweenconditionandgroup.

Page 21: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

20

InsertFigure2abouthere.

Discussion

TheresultsfromExperiment1indicatethatlistenerscorrectlyidentifyspeech

producedbychildrenwithASDas“disordered”moreoftenthanspeechproducedby

typicallydevelopingchildren.Thesoundpatterndifferencesaresufficientlysalientthat

listenerscandistinguishthegroupsoutsidethesocial-interactionalcontextandbasedon

veryshortspeechsamples.Yet,theabsenceofagroupeffectonjudgmentsinthefiltered

speechconditionsuggeststhatthesoundpatterndifferencesaresubtleenoughtobe

obscuredindegradedspeech.Thissuggestionisfurthersupportedbylisteners’weaker

confidenceintheirratingsofatypicalchildren’sspeechcomparedtotypicalchildren’s

speech.Listeners’weakerconfidenceintheirratingsofatypicalspeechmayalsoindicate

thatthesamplesprovidedbychildrenwithASDweremorevariablethantheonesobtained

fromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeers.

EXPERIMENT2

ThegoalofExperiment2wastotestwhetherlisteners’socialevaluationofspeakers

basedonspeechsoundpatternsalsodifferentiateschildrenwithASDfromtheirtypically

developingpeers.Thisgoalisinserviceofourquestionaboutwhetheratypicalspeech

soundpatternsmatter,wherethenotionofwhatmattersisunderstoodasaquestionabout

theeffectsofdifferenceonlisteners’attitudetowardsthespeaker.Therelationship

betweenlaylisteners’judgmentsofdisorder(Experiment1)andtheirlikeabilityratings

wasalsodirectlyexplored.

Page 22: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

21

Methods

Participants

Twelvenewcollege-agedadultsprovidedlikeabilityratingsonthespeechsamples

fromExperiment1.Theseadultswererecruitedfromintroductorypsychologyand

linguisticsclasses,andreceivedcoursecreditfortheirparticipation.Allreportednormal

hearingandEnglishastheirnativelanguage.

RatingTask

ListenersratedboththefilteredandtheunalteredspeechsamplesfromExperiment

1forlikeabilityona7-pointLikertscale.Thelowvalueonthescalewasanchoredwitha

positivesocialstatement(“1=Awesome!Lovethiskid.”)andthehighvaluewithanegative

socialstatement(“7=Nah.Wouldn’tlikethiskid.”).Listenerssatinfrontofacomputer

monitorwiththeanchorsdisplayedabovetheratingscale,whichwasrepresentedasa

numberedsequenceof7buttons.Theylistenedtoaspeechsampleoverheadphones,rated

italongthelikeabilityscale,clicked“OK”andadvancedtoanewscreenandanewsample.

AsinExperiment1,listenerscompletedthefilteredspeechconditionbeforetheunaltered

speechcondition.Speechsampleswereplayedindifferentrandomorders.Listenerswere

toldonlythatthechildrenwerebetween5and11yearsold.UnlikeinExperiment1,they

werenottoldthathalfofthechildrenwerereceivingspeechandlanguagetherapyinthe

schools.

Analyses

Likeabilityratingswerestandardizedwithinlisteneracrossconditionsby

convertingtheratingscoresintoz-scores.Ratingswerethenaveragedacrosslistenersand

withinspeakers.Amixed-designANOVAwasusedtoassessthebetween-subjectseffectof

speakergroup(ASDversustypicaldevelopment)andwithin-subjectseffectofspeech

Page 23: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

22

condition(filteredversusunaltered)onlikeabilityratings.Asignificanteffectofgroupon

likeabilityratingswouldindicatethatlistenersevaluatechildrenwithASDdifferentlyfrom

theirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedonspeechpatternsalone.Aninteractionbetween

groupandspeechconditionwouldindicatetheinfluenceofnon-prosodicfactorson

listeners’ratings.Speaker’sage-in-monthsandstandardizedPPVTscoreswereincludedas

covariatesintheanalysestocontrolfortheireffectsonthedependentvariable.

Asecondanalysisusedlinearregressiontoinvestigatetherelationshipbetweenthe

residualized“disordered”judgmentsfromExperiment1andthelikeabilityratings

obtainedfromadifferentgroupoflistenersinExperiment2.Likeabilityratingswerethe

dependentvariable,andjudgmentsofdisorderthepredictorvariable.Age-in-months,

PPVTscores,andspeechconditionwereenteredascontrolpredictorvariables.

Results

EffectofSpeakeronLikeability

Theanalysisindicatedasignificantinteractionbetweengroupandspeechcondition,

F(1,30)=5.17,p=.030,ηp2=.147,whichisshowninFigure3.Neitherthesimpleeffectof

groupnorthatofspeechconditionwassignificant,thoughtheeffectofgroupapproached

significance(p=.051).

InsertFigure3abouthere.

Giventheinteraction,theeffectofgroupwasexploredfurtherinanalysessplitby

condition.Theseanalysesindicatedasignificanteffectofgrouponlikeabilityratingsinthe

unalteredspeechcondition,F(1,30)=6.79,p=.014,ηp2=.184,butnotinthefiltered

speechcondition.AsshowninFigure3,childrenwithASDwereperceivedassignificantly

lesslikeablethanchildrenwhoweretypicallydevelopingbasedontheunalteredspeech

Page 24: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

23

samples1.Notethattheeffectissignificantevenwhenalphaiscorrectedformultiple

comparisons(i.e.,theBonferronicorrectedalphais.025).

RelationshipbetweenJudgmentsofDisorderandLikeability

TheresultsonlikeabilityrecallthosefromExperiment1inthatlistenersonly

discriminatedbetweenthegroupsofspeakersintheunalteredspeechcondition.The

similaritybetweenthetwoexperimentswasconfirmedinalinearregressionanalysis,

whichshowedthattheresidualizedjudgmentsofdisordercouldbeusedtoexplain/predict

likeabilityratings,β=.59,t(67)=6.01,p<.001,evenwithage-in-months,PPVT,and

speechconditioncontrolledinthemodel.Byitself,thepredictor“disordered”judgments

accountedfor42%ofthevarianceinlikeabilityratings.Noneofthecontrolvariables

explainedasignificantproportionofthevariance.

TherelationshipbetweenperceiveddisorderandlikeabilityisshowninFigure4.

Speechsamplesthatelicitedahigher“disordered”responseelicitedamorenegativesocial

evaluation(“Nah.Wouldn’tlikethiskid.”).Whensplitbyspeechcondition,wefindthatthe

correlationbetweenthejudgmentsofdisorderandlikeabilityratingswasstrongerinthe

unalteredspeechcondition,r(34)=.72,p<.001,comparedtothefilteredspeechcondition

r(34)=.48,p=.004,thoughthisdifferencewasnotsignificant,z=1.51,p=.066(one-

tailed).

1Recallthatthe7-pointlikeabilityscalewasanchoredsuchthathigherscoresindicateamorenegativesocialevaluationthanlowerscores(“1=Awesome!Lovethiskid.”versus“7=Nah.Wouldn’tlikethiskid.”).

Page 25: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

24

InsertFigure4abouthere.

Discussion

TheresultsfromExperiment2indicatethatlaylistenersevaluatechildrenwithASD

morenegativelythantheirtypicallydevelopingpeers.Thus,likeinExperiment1,listeners

wereabletodistinguishbetweenspeakersbasedontheshortspeechsamplesprovided.

TheresultsalsoparallelthosefromExperiment1inthatlistenerswerenotableto

distinguishbetweenthedifferentgroupsofspeakersinthefilteredspeechcondition.A

regressionanalysisconfirmedthatthesimilaritiesbetweenExperiment1andExperiment

2wereduetoasignificantcorrelationbetweenlaylistenersjudgmentsofdisorderand

theirsocialevaluationofchildren.Next,weinvestigatewhetherthiscorrelationmeansthat

thesamesoundpatternstriggerboththeperceptionofdisorderandanegativesocial

evaluation.

EXPERIMENT3

InExperiment3,furtherperceptualevaluationofchildren’sspeechandacoustic

measuresofsegmentalandsuprasegmentalfeatureswereusedtoidentifythespeech

soundpatterningthatmediatedlisteners’judgmentsof“typical”versus“disordered”in

Experiment1andtheirlikeabilityratingsinExperiment2.Thegoalwastoanswerthe

questionofwhetherbothtypesofjudgmentsreferencethesamepatternsinorderto

understandwhyajudgmentofdisordermayleadtoanegativesocialevaluation.Thus,the

focusofExperiment3isonlisteners’behavior.Wedonotinvestigatethisbehaviorasa

functionofthespeakergroups,butdoconfirmthatchildrenwithASDdifferfromtypically

developingchildrenalongtheperceptualandacousticdimension(s)thatpredictjudgments

ofdisorderandtheirlikeabilityratings.

Page 26: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

25

Methods

Participants

Sixteennewcollege-agedadultsratedtheunalteredspeechsamplesalongseveral

dimensions.Theseadultswererecruitedfromintroductorypsychologyandlinguistics

classes,andreceivedcoursecreditfortheirparticipation.Allreportednormalhearingand

Englishastheirnativelanguage.

RatingTask

ListenersratedtheunalteredspeechsamplesfromExperiment1and2ona7-point

Likertscalealongthe5dimensionsderivedfromthesubjectivefeedbackacquiredin

Experiment1:articulation,speechclarity,fluency,andaccenting.Afifthdimension,

monotony,wasincludedtodifferentiateratingsthatmayhavebeenduetodifferences

appropriatelypitchaccentinglinguisticcontent(i.e.,tune-to-textalignment)fromthose

duesimplytopitchvariability.Listenerswereonlytoldthattheywouldberatingchildren’s

speechalongdifferentperceptualdimensions.Listenerswerealsotoldtheagerangeof

childspeakers,butnomentionwasmadeofthefactthatsomeofthechildrenwere

receivingspeechandlanguagetherapyintheschools.Thespeechsampleswereexplained

asbeforewithreferencetotheelicitationmethod.Onlyunalteredspeechsampleswere

usedinExperiment3becausetheseelicitedthehighestcorrelationsbetweenjudgmentsof

disorderandlikeability(seeExperiment2),andbecauseweneededtolimitthenumberof

stimulipresentedtolistenersinorderforthemtoratealldimensionswithinthe45to55

minutesallottedto“workwithbreaks”duringtheexperiment.

Listenerssatinfrontofacomputermonitorwiththeanchorsdisplayedabovethe

ratingscale,whichwasrepresentedasanumberedsequenceof7buttons.Thelowvalue

onthescalewasanchoredwithadescriptionoftypicalspeechandthehighvaluewitha

Page 27: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

26

descriptionofatypicalspeech.Thelowandhighanchorsforarticulationwere“1=ALL

soundscorrectlyproduced”and“7=NOsoundscorrectlyproduced;”forclearness,“1=

ExtremelyClear”and“7=TotallyUnclear;”forfluency,“1=SuperFluent”and“7=Really

Disfluent”.Theanchorsforaccenting2were,respectively,“1=EmphasizesMostImportant

Words”and“7=EmphasizesOnlyIrrelevantWords;”andformonotony,“1=VeryLively”

and“7=TotallyMonotonous.”Listenersheardaspeechsampleoverheadphones,ratedthe

sampleonthescalealongthespecifieddimension,clicked“OK”andadvancedtoanew

screenandanewsample.Thereweretworepetitionsofeachsampleasbefore.Allsamples

werepresentedinrandomorder.Theorderinwhichdimensionswereratedwasvaried

acrosslisteners.Fourteenlistenerscompletedratingsalongthearticulationdimension3.All

listenerscompletedratingsalongallotherdimensions.

AcousticMeasurements

Inordertobettercharacterizespeechsoundpatterningrelevanttothedimensions

ofarticulation,speechclarity,fluency,accenting,andmonotony,anumberofacoustic

segmentalandsuprasegmentalmeasuresweretakenonthe136utterancesthatwerethe

speechsamplesprovidedtolisteners.Measurementswerebasedonthehand-

segmentationoftheutterancesintoconsonantandvowelintervalsinPraatusingstandard

segmentationcriteria(see,e.g.,Redford,2014).Voweldurations,F0,F1,andF2were

automaticallyextracted.ThemeasurementintervalforF0wassetat.01secondsandthe

2Theanchorsforratingalongtheaccentingdimensionweremeanttodrawlisteners’attentiontotheintonationalaspectsofthephrase,andespeciallytotheappropriate/inappropriateuseofprosodicfocus,followingtheliteratureonprosodicdeficitsinspeechproducedbyindividualswithASD.3Listenerswererun2atatimeinthelaboratory.Ononeoccasion,theexperimentalsessionstartedlateandsothelistenerswhoparticipatedinthatsessionwerenotabletocompletethetaskintime.Sincetaskorderwasvariedacrosssessions,bothlistenersintheshortenedsessionwereunabletocompletethesame/lasttaskinthesession.

Page 28: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

27

rangesetfrom75to600Hz.ValuesthatdeviatedoneSDfromaspeaker’smean,werere-

measuredbyhand.VowelformantmeasureswerebasedonthePraatLPCalgorithmin

timestepsequalto10percentofthevowelinterval,andwiththemaximumnumberof

formantssetto5andmaximumformantfrequencyto6000Hz.Everyformanttrackwas

visuallyinspected,thenhand-correctedandre-measuredifthetrackswereoff.

Segmentalmeasures.Voweldurationandformantmeasureswereusedtoderive3

measuresofarticulation:schwaduration,meannormalizedvariabilityinschwaduration,

andF1xF2vowelspace.Voweldurationandstabilityinrepeatedvowelproductionare

indicatorsofspeechmotorcontrol,withshorterdurationsandmorestableproductionsa

featureofgreatercontrolovertargetedarticulations(seeRedford&Oh,2017).Schwa

vowelsareminimallyinfluencedbyprosodicfactorssincetheydonotreceivelexicalstress

orphrasalaccents,someanschwadurationandvariabilityindurationislesssensitiveto

contexts.Therewerealsomultipleschwaproductionsineveryspeechsample,soschwa

durationandvariabilitywassomethingwecouldcalculateforallspeakers.TheF1xF2

vowelspacewasdefinedbyfullvowelsthatwerealsomonopthongs.Thespacewas

calculatedasthemeanEuclidiandistanceofeachfullvowelfromthemeanfullvowelin

eachutterance.Thismeasurethusprovidesinformationaboutvoweldistinctivenesswithin

anutterance,whichisacorrelateofspeechclarity(see,e.g.,Lindblom,1990).

Suprasegmentalmeasures.ThedurationandF0measureswereusedtoderive

measuresofrate,rhythmandintonation.Articulationratewascalculatedasnumberof

syllablespersecondofspeech,excludinganysilencesduetopausing,whichwasextremely

rareinanycase(seespeechsamplesunderExperiment1).Thecoefficientofvariationin

voweldurations(i.e.,standarddeviation/mean)wasusedtocapturespeechrhythm.This

measurehasbeenproposedintheliteratureasaneffective,simple-to-compute,rate-

Page 29: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

28

normalizedrhythmmetric(White&Mattys,2007).Finallengthening,afeatureofprosodic

boundarymarking,wascalculatedastheratioofphrase-finalvoweldurationtothe

averagenon-finalvowelduration.Anotherfeatureofprosodicboundarymarking,final

pitchchange,wascalculatedasF0changefromthepenultimatetoultimatesyllableofthe

phrase.Pitchdeclination,acharacteristicofthewholeintonationcontour,wascalculated

astheslopeofabestfitregressionlineplottedthroughsequentiallyarrangedF0valuesfor

eachutterance.

Inadditiontothesegmentalandsuprasegmentalmeasures,severalacoustic

measurescorrespondingtovoiceweretakentodeterminewhetheroverallvoicequality

mighthavecontributedtolistenersjudgmentsofdisorder(see,e.g.,Shribergetal.,2001).

ThemeasuresweremeanF0andtwomeasuresoftheproportionoftimeduringaglottal

periodwherethereisnocontactbetweenthevocalfolds,whichisrelatedtovocalquality

rangingfromvocalfrytobreathy.These“openness”measureswerethemeanamplitude

differencebetweenthefirstharmonic(H1)andthesecondharmonic(H2)acrossallvowel

intervals,andorthemeanamplitudedifferencebetweenH1andthefirstformant(A1)(see

Garellek&Keating,2011).AswiththeF0andformantvalues,phonationmeasureswere

calculatedautomaticallyusingaPraatscript.F0andformantcalculationsweresetas

before.

DataReduction

Ratingsalongeachdimensionwerestandardizedwithinlistenerbyconvertingthe

Likertscoresintoz-scores.Ratingswerethenaveragedacrosslistenersandwithin

speakers.Inter-correlationsbetweentherateddimensionswereassessed.Ratingsalong

thedimensionsofarticulation,clearness,andfluencywereexceptionallyhighlycorrelated,

withcorrelationcoefficientsrangingfrom.92(articulationandfluency)to.96(articulation

Page 30: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

29

andclearness).Accordingly,these3dimensionswerereducedtooneusingprincipal

componentsanalysis(PCA).ScoresfromthefirstfactorofthePCAweresavedandusedin

placeofthearticulation,clearness,andfluencyratings.Foreaseofreference,wewillrefer

tothesescoresascapturingthedimensionofintelligibility.Thisdescriptivelabelwas

chosenbecausetheconstituentcomponentsofthisshareddimension(e.g.,articulation,

clearness,andfluency)areallattributesofintelligiblespeech.Intelligibility,accenting,and

monotonywerethenenteredintoamultipleregressionmodeltopredictjudgmentsof

disorder(seebelow).Thismodeldidnotsufferfromunacceptablecollinearity(VIF<3.5).

Inter-correlationsbetweentheacousticmeasureswerealsoassessed.Manydidnot

reachsignificance.AllandonlytheonesthatdidareshowninTable5.Notethenear

perfectcorrelationbetweenthetwomeasuresrelatedtovoicequalityinthetable.Given

thehighdegreeofoverlap,thesemeasureswerereducedtooneusingPCA.Scoresfromthe

firstfactorofthePCAweresavedandusedinplaceoftheindividualH1-H2andH1-A1

measures.Foreaseofreference,wewillrefertothesescoresasmeasuresofvoicequality.

InsertTable5abouthere.

StatisticalAnalyses

Stepwiselinearregressionmodelingwasusedtoinvestigatetherelative

contributionoflistener-identifiedperceptualdimensionsofspeechsoundpatterningto

residualized“disordered”judgmentsandz-scoredlikeabilityratingsonunalteredspeech

samples.Age-in-monthsandreceptivevocabularyscoreswereincludedascontrol

variablesinthemodel.Backwardeliminationwasusedtoreducethenumberofpredictor

variablestoonlythosethatcontributedsignificantlytoexplaininglisteners’responses.The

Page 31: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

30

sametechniquewasthenusedtocharacterizeinacoustictermstheperceptual

dimension(s)thatbestexplainedlisteners’behaviorinExperiments1and2.

Results

PerceptualBasisforJudgmentsofDisorder

Thefullmodelaccountedfor69%ofthevarianceinjudgmentsofdisorderandhad

anadjustedR2of.63.Thesignificantmodelwiththefewestpredictorsaccountedfor67%

ofthevarianceandhadanadjustedR2of.65.Theonlysignificantpredictorsinthismodel

wereage-in-months,β=.22,t(33)=2.10,p=.045,andintelligibility,β=.84,t(33)=7.98,p

<.001;thatis,theperceptualdimensionderivedfromtheoverlapinratingsofarticulation,

clearness,andfluency.Whereasintelligibilitybyitselfaccountedfor63%ofthevariancein

“disordered”judgments,themodelwithage-in-monthsalonewasnobetterthanthenull

modelataccountingforvariance.Figure5showstherelationshipbetweenpoor

intelligibility4and“disordered”judgments.

InsertFigure5abouthere.

Anindependentsamplest-testconfirmedthatvariancealongtheintelligibility

dimensionalsovariedsystematicallywiththegroupofspeakerswhoproducedthe

samples,MeanDifference=1.02,t(33)=3.40,p=.002(two-tailed).Althoughchildrenwith

ASDwereratedaslessintelligibleonaveragethantheirTDpeers,itisalsoevidentfrom

thedatapresentedinFigure5thatnotallchildrenwereratedasequallyunintelligiblenor

weretheyallratedasequally“disordered”.

4Recallthatthe7-pointratingscalewasanchoredsuchthathigherscoresaremorecompatiblewithdescriptionsofdisorderedspeech.(e.g.,“1=ALLsoundscorrectlyproduced”versus“7=NOsoundscorrectlyproduced”).

Page 32: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

31

PerceptualBasisforLikeabilityRatings

Thefullmodelaccountedfor57%ofthevarianceinlikeabilityratingsandhadan

adjustedR2of.49.Thesignificantmodelwiththefewestpredictorsaccountedfor49%of

thevarianceandhadanadjustedR2of.46.Theonlysignificantpredictorsinthismodel

wereintelligibility,β=.56,t(33)=4.12,p<.001,andmonotony,β=.29,t(33)=2.15,p=

.040.Intelligibilityaccountedfor42%ofthevarianceinlikeabilityratingsbyitself,and

monotonyfor21%.Thus,itwouldseemthatintelligibilityandmonotonyeachaccounted

forsomeofthesamevarianceintheregressionmodel.Figure6showstherelationship

betweenintelligibilityandlikeability,wherehighervaluesalongeachaxisindicatemore

negativeevaluations.

InsertFigure6abouthere.

AcousticCharacterizationofIntelligibilityandMonotony

Segmental,suprasegmental,andvoicemeasureswereenteredaspredictorvariables

inastepwiseregressionmodelstoprovideacousticcharacterizationsoftheperceptual

dimensions,intelligibilityandmonotony.Thefullmodelsaccountedfor46%ofthe

varianceinintelligibilityandfor68%ofthevarianceinmonotony;theyhadadjustedR2sof

.20and.55,respectively.Thesignificantmodelswiththefewestpredictorsaccountedfor

41%ofthevarianceinintelligibilityandfor56%ofthevarianceinmonotony;theyhadan

adjustedR2sof.17and.50,respectively.Articulationratewasthesolesignificantpredictor

ofintelligibility,β=–.41,t(33)=–2.55,p=.016:slowerrateswereassociatedwithpoorer

intelligibility.Bycontrast,thereducedmodelofmonotonyincludedtwosignificant

segmentalmeasures—meanschwaduration,β=.31,t(33)=2.54,p=.016,andvowel

distinctiveness,β=–.25,t(33)=–2.04,p=.050—andtwosignificantvoicemeasures—

Page 33: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

32

meanF0,β=–.37,t(33)=–2.45,p=.020,andvoicequality,β=.44,t(33)=2.91,p=.007.

Thedirectionoftheeffectofschwadurationisconsistentwiththeeffectofspeechrate:

longerschwawereassociatedwithgreaterperceivedspeechmonotonythanshorter

schwas.Theeffectofvoweldistinctivenessindicatesthatmoremonotonousspeechwas

alsolikelylessclear.Lowerpitchandlessmodal(morebreathy)voicequalitieswerealso

associatedwithmoremonotonousspeech.Giventhislistofsignificantpredictorvariables,

itwouldseemthatalthoughtheperceptualdimensionsofintelligibilityandmonotonyare

acousticallyseparable,botharewell-characterizedbyfeaturesthataremoreassociated

withspeechmotorandvoicefactorsthanwithlinguisticprosody.

Discussion

TheresultsfromExperiment3stronglysuggestthatlaylistenersattendtopatterns

relatedtomotorspeechratherthantoprosodiconeswhentheyareaskedtoclassify

children’sspeechsoundpatternsas“disordered”or“typical”andtoevaluatehowmuch

theylikeachildbasedonshortspeechsamples.Thisconclusionisbasedonthefindingthat

“accenting,”themostovertlyprosodicperceptualdimensionevaluated,wasnota

significantpredictorofjudgmentsofdisorderoroflikeability.Moreover,thestrongest

predictorofbothperceiveddisorderandlikeabilitywasintelligibility,adimensionderived

fromtheoverlapinratingsofarticulation,clarity,andfluency.Thisdimensionwasbest

characterizedbyarticulationrate.Ratemaybeasuprasegmentalfeatureofspeech,butitis

onemoretypicallyassociatedwitharticulatorytimingcontrolthanwithprosody(see,e.g.,

Redford,2014).Similarly,thedimension“monotony”wasbestcharacterizedbymeasures

thatcapturedtheaccuracyandspeedofsegmentalarticulations(schwadurationand

voweldistinctiveness)andbymeasuresrelatedtovoicecharacteristics(pitchandvoice

quality)thatarealsooutsidethedomainofprosody,strictlydefined.

Page 34: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

33

GENERALDISCUSSION

AlthoughchildrenandadultswithASDareoftenreportedtohaveatypicalspeech

patterns,manyindividualswithautismshownoprosodicdeficits(seeShribergetal.,2001;

2011;Nadig&Shaw,2012).Moreover,thebehaviorsthatarecentraltoestablishinga

diagnosisofautismaremoreobviouslysociallydisabling(e.g.,notrespondingtoone’sown

name)anddeviant(e.g.,echolalicspeech)thanwemightimagineunusualspeechpatterns

tobe.Thecurrentstudywasmotivatedbythequestionofwhetheratypicalspeechpatterns

inchildrenwithASDisimportantenoughtowarrantresearch.Inaclinicaloreducational

context,thenotionofwhatbehaviorisorisnotimportanttounderstandamountstoa

questionabouttheeffectsthatsuchbehaviorhasonpeerrelationsoronlearning.The

resultsfromthepresentstudyprovideevidenceinsupportoftheviewthatatypicalspeech

soundpatternsmarkchildrenwithASDasdifferentfromtheirTDpeers,andarelikelyto

createbarrierstopositivesocialinteractions.Weconcludefromthisthatatypicalspeech

shouldbeaddresseddirectlyinchildrenwithASD.Theresultsdonot,however,support

theviewthatprosodyshouldnecessarilybethefocusoftheseefforts.Wheninputis

limited,listeners’judgmentsareinfluencedbyspeechpatternsthatlikelyreflectdelayedor

deviantspeechmotorcontrolandunusualvoicequalityratherthanprosodicdeficitsper

se.Morespecifically,thecurrentstudyidentifiesintelligibilityastheprimaryperceptual

dimensionthatmediatesbothlisteners’judgmentsofdisorderandtheirsocialevaluation

ofthechildbasedonshortsnippetsofspeech.Thisdimensioniscloselyassociatedwiththe

perceivedgoodnessandfluency(=speed)ofsegmentalarticulation.

Intelligibilityisaconceptfundamentaltostudiesofmotorspeechdisorders(e.g.,

dysarthria),whereitisusuallymeasuredasthenumberofdistinctspeechsoundsorwords

recognizedbyapaneloflisteners(seee.g.,Kentetal.,1989).Wewouldthereforeliketo

Page 35: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

34

clarifythatnoneofthechildrenwhoparticipatedinthepresentstudysoundeddysarthric.

Theirspeechwasnotdistorted,justdifferentsounding.Thisdifferencewascharacterized

bythereferringSLPsasunusualprosody,butlisteners’behaviorsuggeststhatperhaps

speechmotorfactors,ratherthanlinguisticprosodicones,weremoreimportanttothe

perceptionofdifference.Itisforthisreasonthatwewouldarguethatthenotionof

intelligibilitybettercaptureswhatlistenersweremostsensitivetowhendistinguishing

childrenwithASDfromtheirtypicallydevelopingpeersbasedontheshortspeechsamples

provided.Additionalevidenceforthisviewcomesfromthefeedbackthatlisteners

providedinExperiment1.Forexample,Listeners1,2,and15allnotethattheirjudgments

wereaffectedbywhattheyperceivedas“slurred”or“slurring.”Listeners11and13refer

totheinfluenceofa“lisp”ontheirjudgments.Listeners3,4,5,7,8,11,and15commenton

aspectsofrate.Relatedly,articulationratewasasignificantpredictorofintelligibility.

Elsewhere,wehavefoundelsewherethatdefaultarticulationratesindextheacquisitionof

articulatorytimingcontrolmorebroadly,atleastintypicallydevelopingchildren(Redford,

2014).

Theconclusionthatlistenersmayhavefocusedonspeechmotorfactorsto

distinguishbetweenchildrenwithASDandTDisatoddswiththecharacterizationof

autisticspeechasprosodicallydisorderedifoneunderstandsspeechprosodyinlinguistic

terms;thatis,asasystemofmetricalgroupingandaccentplacementsgovernedbya

prosodicgrammarthatislinked,atsomelevel,tosyntacticstructureandsemantic-

pragmaticmeaning.Deficitsinlexicalstressplacementandcontrastivefocusmeetthis

definitionofprosodyandsoareconsistentwiththeprimarydeficitsinsemantic-pragmatic

communicationthatisassociatedwithASD,butthesewerenotfoundhere.Moreover,the

differenceswedidfindcorrespondtothemanyotherdocumenteddifferencesbetween

Page 36: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

35

autisticspeechandtypicalspeechthatarenotnecessarilyprosodic.Inparticular,acoustic-

phoneticstudiesthatcharacterizedifferencesbetweenASDandtypicalspeechintermsof

globaldifferencesinpitchrangeandvariability(Diehletal.,2009;Nadig&Shaw,2012),

longerwordandutterancedurations(Diehl&Paul,2012),andhigherratesofdisfluencies

(Shribergetal.,2001)areallcompatiblewithdifferencesinplanningandproductionand

notwithprosodyperse.

Thefindingsthatlistenersaresensitivetospeechintelligibilitywhenjudgingshort

snippetsofspeechandthatthissensitivityaffectstheirsocialevaluationofaspeakeralso

hasimplicationsforconceptualizingthesemantic-pragmaticimpairmentsinchildrenwith

ASD.Inparticular,afocusonintelligibilityemphasizestheimportanceofthesocial

interactionfordefining“impaired”,“atypical”or“disordered”,andsosupportsaviewof

pragmaticimpairmentasemergentfromtheinteractionbetweenspeakersandlisteners

(seePerkins,2007).Whenthecontributionsofbothareinbalance,communicationis

successfulandpragmaticsareintact.Whenonememberofthedyadmustadaptinan

unfamiliar,asymmetricalmannertoaccommodatetheother’sbehavior,then

communicationsuffersandpragmaticsisimpaired.Thisviewofimpairmenthasthe

advantageofallowingustounderstandwhylisteners’perceptionofdisordermightaffect

theirsocialevaluationofthespeaker.Anydecrementinspeechintelligibilityislikelyto

resultincommunicativeasymmetry;namely,anasymmetrythatcreatesextraworkforthe

listener.Iflisteners(implicitly)resentdoingextrawork,thentheymayblamethespeaker.

NotethatthisexplanationissimilartothatwhichRiceandcolleaguesproposedtoexplain

theirfindingsthatchildrenwithspecificlanguageimpairmentorwhoaresecondlanguage

speakersofEnglishhavelessaccesstopositivesocialinteractionsthantheirtypically

Page 37: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

36

developingandnativeEnglish-speakingpeersinUSschools(Riceetal.,1991;Gertneretal.

1994).

Althoughwehavesuggestedthatlistenersaremoresensitivetospeechsound

patternsthatsuggestdeficitsinspeechmotorcontrolratherthaninprosody,itis

importanttonotethatthestrengthofthisconclusionislimitedbytheexperimentaldesign

weused.Specifically,theshort,decontextualizedspeechsamplespresentedtolisteners

mayhaveforcedthemtolatchontosomethingotherthanprosodytomaketheirdecisions.

Ifunusualprosodyinautismoftenreflectsinappropriateorincorrectprosodicfocus

(Shribergetal.,2001;Paul,Augustyn,etal,2005;Peppéetal.,2007),thenaccessto

languagecontextwouldbecriticalforlistenerstoidentifytheatypicalpatterns.Infact,one

waytointerpretthefindingthatlistenerscouldonlydistinguishchildrenwithASDfrom

theirtypicallydevelopingpeersintheunalteredspeechconditionisthatonlythis

conditionprovidedlistenerswiththerelevantinformationabouttune-to-textalignment.

Moreover,somelistenersdidcommentthathowaspeakerusedaccentinginfluencedtheir

judgmentsofdisorder.Forexample,Listener4inExperiment1notedthattheirjudgment

wasaffectedbywhetherthechild“stressedtherightsyllables”andListener16was

sensitivetowhetherornotemphasiswasusedcorrectly.Futureworkshouldevaluatethe

relativeinfluencesofintelligibilityandungrammaticalprosodicfocusassignmentand

phrasingonlisteners’behaviorbyusinglongerstretchesofspeechthatprovideadiscourse

context.

Inconclusion,theresultsfromthepresentstudyshowthatlaylistenersare

sensitivetotheatypicalspeechpatternsproducedbychildrenwithASD.Thissensitivity

alsoinfluencestheirsocialevaluationofthespeaker.Speakerswhoproducedpatternsthat

wereperceivedasatypicalordisorderedandratedaslesslikeable.Thesefindingsvalidate

Page 38: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

37

effortstounderstandandtreatatypicalspeechinASD,eveninthecontextofthemany

otherdisablingbehaviorsassociatedwiththedisorder.Thepresentstudyalsoaddstoa

bodyofliteratureonlistenersensitivitytosocial-indexicalaspectsofspeech,butsuggests

thatsomenegativebiasestowardsspeakersmayemergefromthemodelingeffortthatis

neededforlistenerstoengageinsuccessfulcommunicationwithanunfamiliarspeaker.

Page 39: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

38

REFERENCES

AmericanPsychiatricAssociation.(2013).Diagnosticandstatisticalmanualofmental

disorders:DSM-5.Washington,D.C:AmericanPsychiatricAssociation.

Baltaxe,C.A.,&Guthrie,D.(1987).Theuseofprimarysentencestressbynormal,aphasic,

andautisticchildren.JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders,17,255-271.

Boersma,P.&Weenink,D.(2009).Praat,version5.1.16.Availableathttp://www.praat.org.

Bonneh,Y.S.,Levanon,Y.,Dean-Pardon,O.,Lossos,L.,&Adini,Y.(2011).Abnormalspeech

spectrumandincreasedpitchvariabilityinyoungautisticchildren.FrontiersinHuman

Neuroscience,4,2347.

Clopper,C.G.,&Bradlow,A.R.(2009).FreeclassificationofAmericanEnglishdialectsby

nativeandnon-nativelisteners.JournalofPhonetics,37,436-451.

Clopper,C.G.,&Pisoni,D.B.(2004).Someacousticcuesfortheperceptualcategorization

ofAmericanEnglishregionaldialects.JournalofPhonetics,32,111-140.

Depape,A.M.,Chen,A.,Hall,G.B.,&Trainor,L.J.(2012).Useofprosodyandinformation

structureinhighfunctioningadultswithautisminrelationtolanguageabilitiy.

FrontiersinPsychology,3,1664-1678.

Diehl,J.J.,Watson,D.,Bennetto,L.,McDonough,J.,&Gunlogson,C.(2009).Anacoustic

analysisofprosodyinhigh-functioningautism.AppliedPsycholinguistics,30,385-404.

Diehl,J.J.,&Paul,R.(2012).Acousticdifferencesintheimitationofprosodicpatternsin

childrenwithautismspectrumdisorders.ResearchinAutismSpectrumDisorders,6,

123-134.

Dunn,L.,&Dunn,D.(2007).PeabodyPictureVocabularyTest,FourthEdition(PPVT-4).

Minneapolis,MN:NCSPearson.

Page 40: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

39

Frith,U.(1991).Translationandannotationof“Autisticpsychopathy”inchildhood,byH.

Asperger.InU.Frith(Ed.),AutismandAspergersyndrome.Cambridge,UK:Cambridge

UniversityPress.

Garellek,M.&Keating,P.(2011).Theacousticconsequencesofphonationand

toneinteractionsinJalapaMazatec.JournaloftheInternationalPhoneticAssociation,41,

185-205.

Gertner,B.L.,Rice,M.L.,&Hadley,P.A.(1994).Influenceofcommunicativecompetenceon

peerpreferencesinapreschoolclassroom.JournalofSpeech,LanguageandHearing

Research,37,913-923.

Grossman,R.B.,Bemis,R.H.,PlesaSkwerer,D.,&Tager-Flusberg,H.(2010).Lexicaland

affectiveprosodyinchildrenwithhigh-functioningautism.JournalofSpeech,Language

andHearingResearch,53,778.

Kent,R.D.,Weismer,G.,Kent,J.F.,&Rosenbek,J.C.(1989).Towardphoneticintelligibility

testingindysarthria.JournalofSpeechandHearingDisorders,54,482-499.

Klin,A.,Sparrow,S.,Marans,W.D.,Carter,A.,&Volkmar,F.R.(2000).Assessmentissuesin

childrenandadolescentswithAspergerSyndrome.InA.Klin,F.R.Volmar,&S.S.

Sparrow(Eds.),Aspergersyndrome,(pp.309-339).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Lass,N.J.,Almerino,C.A.,Jordan,L.F.,&Walsh,J.M.(1980).Theeffectoffilteredspeechon

speakerraceandsexidentifications.JournalofPhonetics,8,101-112.

Lindblom,B.(1990).Explainingphoneticvariation:asketchoftheH&Htheory.InW.J.

Hardcastle&A.Marchal(Eds.),SpeechProductionandSpeechModelling(pp.403-439).

TheNetherlands:KluwerAcademic.

McCaleb,P.,&Prizant,B.M.(1985).Encodingofnewversusoldinformationbyautistic

children.JournalofspeechandHearingDisorders,50,230.

Page 41: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

40

Moore,B.C.J.(1994).Anintroductiontothepsychologyofhearing,3rdEdition.London:

AcademicPress.

Munson,B.(2007).Theacousticcorrelatesofperceivedmasculinity,perceivedfemininity,

andperceivedsexualorientation.LanguageandSpeech,50,125-142.

Munson,B.,McDonald,E.C.,DeBoe,N.L.,&White,A.R.(2006).Theacousticandperceptual

basesofjudgmentsofwomenandmen'ssexualorientationfromreadspeech.Journalof

Phonetics,34,202-240.

Nadig,A.,&Shaw,H.(2012).Acousticandperceptualmeasurementofexpressiveprosody

inhigh-functioningautism:increasedpitchrangeandwhatitmeanstolisteners.

JournalofAutismandDevelopmentalDisorders,42,499-511.

Paul,R.,Augustyn,A.,Klin,A.,&Volkmar,F.R.(2005).Perceptionandproductionof

prosodybyspeakerswithautismspectrumdisorders.JournalofAutismand

DevelopmentalDisorders,35,205-20.

Paul,R.,Bianchi,N.,Augustyn,A.,Klin,A.,&Volkmar,F.R.(2008).Productionofsyllable

stressinspeakerswithautismspectrumdisorders.ResearchinAutismSpectrum

Disorders,2,110-124.

Peppé,S.,&McCann,J.(2003).Assessingintonationandprosodyinchildrenwithatypical

languagedevelopment:ThePEPS-Ctestandtherevisedversion.Clinical

LinguisticsandPhonetics,17,345–354.

Peppé,S.,McCann,J.,Gibbon,F.,O'Hare,A.,&Rutherford,M.(2007).Receptiveand

expressiveprosodicabilityinchildrenwithhigh-functionningautism.JournalofSpeech,

Language,andHearingResearch,50,1015-1028.

Perkins,M.(2007).Pragmaticimpairment.CambridgeUniversityPress.

Port,R.F.(2003).Meterandspeech.JournalofPhonetics,31,599–611.

Page 42: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

41

Purnell,T.,Idsardi,W.,&Baugh,J.(1999).Perceptualandphoneticexperimentson

AmericanEnglishdialectidentification.JournalofLanguageandSocialPsychology,18,

10-30.

Redford,M.A.(2013).Acomparativeanalysisofpausinginchildandadultstorytelling.

AppliedPsycholinguistics,34,569-589.

Redford,M.A.(2014).Theperceivedclarityofchildren'sspeechvariesasafunctionof

theirdefaultarticulationrate.JournaloftheAcousticalSocietyofAmerica,135,2952-

2963.

Redford,M.A.&Oh,G.(2017).Therepresentationandexecutionofarticulatorytimingin

firstandsecondlanguageacquisition.JournalofPhonetics.doi:

0.1016/j.wocn.2017.01.004.

Rice,M.L.,Sell,M.A.,&Hadley,P.A.(1991).Socialinteractionsofspeech,andlanguage-

impairedchildren.JournalofSpeech,LanguageandHearingResearch,34,1299.

Schopler,E.,VanBourgodien,M.E.,Wellman,G.J.,&Love,S.R.(2010).ChildhoodAutism

RatingScale,SecondEdition(CARS2).Torrance,CA:WPS.

Sharda,M.,Subhadra,T.P.,Sahay,S.,Nagaraja,C.,Singh,L.,Mishra,R.,Singhalc,N.,Erickson,

D.,&Singh,N.C.(2010).Soundsofmelody--Pitchpatternsofspeechinautism.

NeuroscienceLetters,478,42-45.

Shriberg,L.D,Kwiatkowski,J.,Rasmussen,C.,Lof,G.,&Miller,J.(1992).TheProsody-Voice

ScreeningProfile(PVSP):PsychometricDataandReferenceInformationforChildren.

UniversityofWisconsin-Madison,WaismanCenteronMentalRetardationandHuman

Development.PhonologyProjectTechnicalReport1.

Page 43: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

42

Shriberg,L.D.,Paul,R.,Black,L.M.,&vanSanten,J.P.(2011).Thehypothesisofapraxiaof

speechinchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorder.JournalofAutismandDevelopmental

Disorders,41,405-426.

Shriberg,L.D.,Paul,R.,McSweeny,J.L.,Klin,A.,Cohen,D.J.,&Volkmar,F.R.(2001).Speech

andprosodycharacteristicsofadolescentsandadultswithhigh-functioningautismand

Aspergersyndrome.JournalofSpeech,LanguageandHearingResearch,44,1097.

Tilsen,S.,&Johnson,K.(2008).Low-frequencyFourieranalysisofspeechrhythm.Journal

oftheAcousticalSocietyofAmerica,124,EL34–EL39.

VanBezooijen,R.(1995).SocioculturalaspectsofpitchdifferencesbetweenJapaneseand

Dutchwomen.LanguageandSpeech,38,253–265

White,L.,&Mattys,S.L.(2007).Calibratingrhythm:firstlanguageandsecondlanguage

studies.JournalofPhonetics,35,501–522.

Page 44: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

43

TABLE1

CharacteristicsofparticipantswithSPEDeligibilitiesforreceivingspeechandlanguage

servicesintheschoolsunderASD.

Speaker Sex Age Symptoms Lg.Delay PPVTp109asd M 6;4 severe No 106

p110asd M 7;1 minimal Yes 119

p102asd M 7;9 severe Yes 126

p100asd M 7;11 severe Yes 83

p103asd M 8;1 moderate Yes 85

p107asd M 8;1 severe No 107

p108asd M 8;1 moderate Yes 112

p114asd M 8;1 minimal Yes 113

p115asd M 8;10 severe No 86

p101asd F 9;3 moderate No 83

p111asd F 9;4 severe No 80

p118asd M 9;6 severe Yes 104

p119asd M 9;6 severe Yes 78

p106asd M 10;3 severe Yes 94

p112asd M 10;9 minimal Yes 102

p113asd F 11;7 severe Yes 90

p116asd M 11;8 severe Yes 94

Page 45: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

44

TABLE2

Characteristicsofparticipantswithtypicaldevelopment.

Speaker Sex Age PPVTp1032td M 6;4 112

p1037td M 7;2 121

p1061td M 7;7 90

p1073td M 7;11 106

p1011td M 8;0 117

p1083td M 8;0 147

p1045td F 8;1 129

p1072td M 8;2 114

p1016td M 9;0 134

p1058td F 9;2 103

p1075td F 9;4 88

p1026td M 9;5 110

p1027td M 9;7 129

p1006td F 10;0 111

p1010td M 10;1 111

p1001td F 10;10 135

p1022td M 10;10 130

Page 46: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

45

TABLE3

Mean(andstandarddeviation)ofuntransformedandunstandardizedresponsesareshownas

afunctionofspeakergroup(TD=typicallydeveloping).Responsesofdisorderobtainedinthe

filteredspeech,unalteredspeech,andtext-basedjudgmenttaskswereaggregatedacross

samplesandlistenerstorenderaby-speakerperceiveddisorderscore(maximumscore=32

forspeech-basedjudgments;16fortext-basedjudgments).Comprehensionoffilteredspeech

wasratedona5-pointscale(nowordsunderstood=1;allwordsunderstood=5).Listeners

alsoratedhowconfidenttheywereintheirresponses(notatall=1;completelysure=5).

JudgmentTask Responsetype Childrenw/TD Childrenw/ASD

Filteredspeech(Experimental)

Cumulative“disordered” 12.53(7.08) 14.71(6.45)

confidence 3.27(0.31) 3.20(0.25)

Comprehension(Control)

ratingsfrom1to5 2.32(0.64) 2.10(0.50)

confidence 3.74(0.89) 3.61(0.91)

Unalteredspeech(Experimental)

Cumulative“disordered” 7.18(5.27) 18.35(7.25)

confidence 3.67(0.32) 3.47(0.19)

Text(Control)

Cumulative“disordered” 4.29(2.80) 7.24(3.54)

confidence 3.41(0.90) 3.38(0.93)

Page 47: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

46

TABLE4

Summaryofindividuallistener’sresponsestotherequestforfeedbackonwhatspeech

characteristicsinfluencedtheirjudgmentsofdisorderinthefilteredandunalteredspeech

conditions.Theresponsesarecategorizedbymajortheme.

Listnr Articulation SpeechClarity Fluency Accenting

1 mispronoun-ciation

slurringofwords

fluidityand…fluctuation

appropriateflux…toaccent

2 slurredwords,concisepronunciation

choppy,flow,pausingatawkwardtimes

emphasisofaword,enthusiastic

3 clarity,unclearslowerspeech,streamingwords,rhythm

4 steadyrhythm,quickly,slowly,smoothly

stressedtherightsyllables,accents

5 enunciated slower,sortofflow upanddown,placedemphasis

6

distorted“r”and“l”sounds,oddlypronouncedblends

7 mispronouncedwords,sounds speechrate stresspatterns

8 slurred,clearer,easiertounderstand littleslower,faster enthusiasm

9 pausesbetween…phrases

mid-phraseinflections

10 soundeddisjoint emphasizecertainpoints,excitement

11 lisp,“r”sounds speakeffectively draggedtheirsyllablesout

12 muffled,extremelyunclear

13 lisp,pronounciating understand…clearly

14 naturalflow,rhythm,paceofwords tone,clashy

15 clearandprecise,slurred quickly

16 disorganizedtones,emphasizedfornopurpose

Page 48: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

47

TABLE5

Allandonlysignificantpair-wisecorrelationsbetweenacousticmeasures.

SchwaDur.

Artic.Rate

F0Slope

FinalF0

MeanF0

H1–H2

H1–A1

SchwaDur. 1 –.43*

Artic.Rate 1 –.52** –.39*

F0Slope 1 –.41* –.44** –.53**

FinalF0Change 1

MeanF0 1 –.61** –.56**

H1–H2 1 .95**

H1–A1 1

*p<.05;**p<.001

Page 49: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

48

FIGURELEGENDS

Figure1.Listeners’judgmentsofdisorder,absenteffectsoflanguage,areshownasa

functionofspeechconditionandspeakergroup(TD=typicallydevelopingchildren;

ASD=childrenwithASDandprosodicdisorder).

Figure2.Listeners’confidenceintheirjudgmentsofdisorderareshownasafunctionof

speechconditionandspeakergroup(TD=typicallydevelopingchildren;ASD=

childrenwithASDandprosodicdisorder).

Figure3.Listeners’ratingsofachild’slikeability(lowanchor=“Awesome!Lovethiskid.”;

highanchor=“Nah.Wouldn’tlikethiskid.”)shownasafunctionofspeechcondition

andspeakergroup(TD=typicallydevelopingchildren;ASD=childrenwithASDand

prosodicdisorder).

Figure4.Listeners’judgmentsofdisorderbasedonfilteredandunalteredspeechsamples

predictsanothergroup’slikeabilityratingsofthespeakerswhoproducedthose

samples.Lowvaluesalongthey-axisindicategreaterlikeabilitythanhighvalues.

Figure5.Theintersectionoflisteners’ratingsofthearticulation,clearness,andfluencyof

unalteredspeechsamples(=intelligibility)predictsanothergroup’sjudgmentsof

disorderbasedonthosesamespeechsamples.Text-basedjudgmentsofdisorder

arepartialledoutfromthevaluesshownalongthey-axis.

Figure6.Theintersectionoflisteners’ratingsofthearticulation,clearness,andfluencyof

unalteredspeechsamples(=intelligibility)predictsanothergroup’slikeability

ratingsofthespeakerswhoproducedthosesamples.Lowvaluesalongthey-axis

indicategreaterlikeabilitythanhighvalues.

Page 50: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

49

FIGURE1

Page 51: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

50

FIGURE2

Page 52: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

51

FIGURE3

Page 53: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

52

FIGURE4

Page 54: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

53

FIGURE5

Page 55: Lay listener classification and evaluation of typical and ... · Final version accepted for publication in Language and Speech June 6, 2017. Lay listener classification and evaluation

Classification and evaluation of child speech

54

FIGURE6