Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

  • Upload
    raison

  • View
    226

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    1/31

    http://ltr.sagepub.com/Language Teaching Research

    http://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1191/1362168803lr127oa

    2003 7: 347Language Teaching ResearchRebecca Adams

    L2 output, reformulation and noticing: implications for IL development

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    can be found at:Language Teaching ResearchAdditional services and information for

    http://ltr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://ltr.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.refs.htmlCitations:

    What is This?

    - Jul 1, 2003Version of Record>>

    at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from at Sookmyung Women's University on October 22, 2013ltr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347http://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347http://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347http://www.sagepublications.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://ltr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://ltr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://ltr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://ltr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.refs.htmlhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.refs.htmlhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.refs.htmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.full.pdfhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.full.pdfhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347.refs.htmlhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://ltr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://ltr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.sagepublications.com/http://ltr.sagepub.com/content/7/3/347http://ltr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    2/31

    L2 output, reformulation and noticing:

    implications for IL development

    Rebecca Adams Georgetown University

    This study is a replication and extension of Swain and Lapkins (in

    press) study of the developmental effects of learners noticing

    differences between their own and native speaker output. In their

    study, task repetition, noticing and participation in stimulated recalls

    were all factors that might have contributed to more targetlike usage

    in subsequent output. The current study separated the effects of each

    of these factors. Fifty-six L2 Spanish learners were randomly assigned

    to three groups: (1) Task repetition (participants repeated the tasks

    without additional treatment); (2) Noticing (participants repeated the

    task and compared their original output to NS reformulation); and

    (3) Noticing + SR (same as Noticing group with the addition of a

    stimulated recall session). Reformulations were traced throughout the

    learners output. Analysis of the data indicates that learners noticed

    differences between their own essays and the reformulated writing, and

    that there were quantitative differences in the output of participants

    from different treatment groups, with learners who participated in

    both noticing and stimulated recall incorporating significantly more

    targetlike forms in the post-treatment output than learners from the

    other groups.

    I Introduction

    This study is an investigation of the effects of noticing and

    measures of noticing on second language learning through writing

    tasks. It is a replication and extension of Swain and Lapkins (in

    press) study of noticing through reformulated writing and its

    impact on second language learning. In addition to replicating the

    prior study, the current study seeks to tease apart the effects of

    noticing and of task repetition on the incorporation of targetlike

    forms in second language writing.

    Arnold 2003 10.1191/1362168803lr127oa

    Address for correspondence: Rebecca Adams, Linguistics Department, ICC 460, George-

    town University, Washington, DC 20057; e-mail: [email protected]

    Language Teaching Research 7,3 (2003); pp. 347376

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    3/31

    1 Writing in second language acquisition

    The Interaction Hypothesis (Long, 1996) characterizes the nature

    of second language learning through interaction and asserts that

    meaningful interaction in the second language is a necessary(although not sufficient) condition for learners to acquire second

    language competence. Longs explanation indicates that while

    interaction cannot explain the entire phenomenon of second

    language learning, it is highly unlikely if not impossible for learners

    to acquire second language communicative competence without

    engaging in meaningful interaction. The hypothesis brings together

    insights from Hatchs (1978) contention that conversation and

    comprehension are essential to the development of secondlanguage communicative competence as well as Swains (1985,

    1995) explanation of the importance of output in language

    acquisition. Swain proposed the Output Hypothesis, which suggests

    that output in the L2 is related to second language learning. Swain

    (1995) explains that output pushes learners to process language

    more deeply (with more mental effort) than does input (p. 126),

    suggesting that output is not the result of the language learningprocess, but rather a step in the process.

    Swain (1995) outlines several reasons why the act of producing

    output in the second language might lead to second language

    development. These reasons include noticing, hypothesis testing,

    and internalizing metalinguistic information. Her claims about

    noticing within the output hypothesis draw on work from Schmidt

    (1990; Schmidt and Frota, 1986), who claims that learners need to

    notice a form before they can acquire it. Noticing can take placewhen learners, in the process of generating output, perceive that

    they do not know how to express their intended meaning. A

    specific aspect of noticing, noticing the gap, occurs when learners

    receive corrective feedback and notice that it differs from their

    original output. The primary aim of Swain and Lapkin (in press),

    the study extended here, is to explore the relationship between

    noticing in writing to development in the use of targetlike forms.Swain and Lapkin hypothesized that allowing learners to compare

    their own writing with native-speaker reformulations would push

    learners to notice gaps in their second language production, which

    would in turn allow them to develop the missing linguistic

    information. The noticing hypothesis and its relationship to the

    348 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    4/31

    current study will be discussed in more detail below.

    Writing and speaking are the two modalities that require

    learners to produce output. While both writing and speaking can

    allow learners to notice differences between their language andnative speak, to test their IL hypotheses, and to internalize

    linguistic information, there are important differences between

    speech and writing that have often been overlooked in the

    discussion of output. Speech is ephemeral in order for learners

    to notice the differences between their speech and that of others,

    they presumably need sufficient processing space available to hold

    both versions in memory and compare them. Preliminary support

    for the connection between working memory and learning frominteraction has been found in recent empirical work (Mackey et

    al., 2002a; Mackey et al., 2002b) and working memory capacity has

    been included as a factor in models of forms-processing (Doughty,

    2001) and of lexical knowledge acquisition (Ellis, 2001). With the

    ongoing processing demands of conversation, it is possible that

    learners are not always able to do make this comparison. When

    learners write and receive corrective feedback, however, they areable to compare their own output with the written response of the

    NS, easing the processing demands. Conversely, speaking is an

    online activity there is little time for erasing or drafting in speech.

    Thus, when looking at writing for evidence of second language

    development, it is important to remember that learners writing

    may conceal self-corrections and other evidence of developing

    interlanguage rules or may reflect instructed rules that have not

    necessarily become part of learners internal grammar.

    2 Writing and feedback

    Feedback on second language learners writing is an issue of

    concern to both language teachers and second language

    researchers. While teacher education literature often prescribes

    certain methods of giving feedback on writing (Reid, 1982), very

    little empirical research on the effectiveness of written feedback

    techniques has been conducted to date (Carson, 2001; Hyland,

    1998; Paulus, 1999). In the growing empirical literature, many

    common methods of providing feedback for second language

    writing have been called into question. Methods such as peer

    Rebecca Adams 349

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    5/31

    review (De Guerro and Vamil, 2000; Mendoca and Johnson, 1994;

    Paulus, 1999; Zhang, 1995), teacher written corrections (Conrad

    and Goldstein, 1999; Hedgecock and Lefkowitz, 1994; Hyland,

    1998; Kepner, 1991), and teacherlearner oral writing conferences(Hedgecock and Lefkowitz, 1992, 1996; Hyland, 2000;Kassen, 1988;

    Shi, 1998) have been challenged, as have notions such as the multi-

    draft model of feedback, in which teachers give feedback first on

    content, commenting on form only in later drafts (Ashwell, 2000).

    The studies mentioned above have challenged teacher and

    researcher assumptions about second language writing. Some of

    these criticisms of writing correction are specifically relevant to the

    current study. For example, some researchers investigate thepossibility that traditional feedback can be discouraging to L2

    learners (Hyland, 1998). Papers returned covered with marks can

    seem overwhelming, and teachers are rarely able to truly balance

    positive and negative feedback (Hedgecock and Lefkowitz, 1994).

    Additionally, written corrections can be confusing to L2 learners,

    as it is often difficult for learners to determine exactly what in their

    output is non-targetlike from written comments only (Hyland,1998). Because traditional feedback methods commonly prescribe

    marking only what is incorrect in learners work, they may present

    learners with mostly negative evidence (Kassen, 1988) and lack

    sufficient positive evidence to refine IL hypotheses. Finally,

    corrections (especially from NSs and teachers) tend to be received

    passively by learners (Hedgecock and Lefkowitz, 1994; Hyland,

    2000). Such corrections therefore may not push learners to use the

    information to test their IL hypotheses and internalize the

    linguistic evidence.

    3 Reformulated writing

    The form of correction employed in this study and in Swain and

    Lapkins study, reformulated writing, avoids these potential

    problems with traditional written feedback. Reformulated writing

    is a specific sort of written feedback that may serve as a partial

    response to these issues (Cohen, 1983; Swain and Lapkin, in press).

    Cohen defines reformulation as having a native writer of the target

    language rewrite the learners essay, preserving all the learners

    ideas, making it sound as nativelike as possible (1983: 4). Rather

    350 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    6/31

    than correcting (pointing out learner errors for them),

    reformulators rewrite learner texts as a NS would write them. The

    content is left intact, but the language is made nativelike. As

    Allwright et al. (1988) explain, making a learner text nativelikeinvolves making changes at multiple levels, including syntactic,

    morphological, and lexical changes. They found that when

    reformulated writing was used in a class setting, discussion of

    reformulated texts was an important part of using the technique,

    and they called for studies of learners rationale for making

    changes in their writing. Brooks and Swain (2001) incorporated

    these ideas in a study of four pairs of adults engaged in comparing

    texts they created as dyads in a task with reformulated versions oftheir texts. They found that learners in a dyad could fill the expert

    role in discussing writing, but when neither of the dyads had

    sufficient L2 knowledge, they turned to the reformulation as the

    expert.

    In some ways, reformulations may be thought of as an extended,

    written recast, in that they provide both implicit negative evidence

    that some form was non-targetlike as well as positive evidence ofhow the idea could be expressed in a targetlike manner (for a

    review of the role of recasts as oral feedback, see Nicholas et al.,

    2001). Reformulated writing also allows learners opportunities to

    compare their own original writing with NS reformulations. In

    addition, since the errors are not pointed out for the learners, the

    use of reformulated writing can push learners to actively seek

    differences between their IL and the TL, leading to noticing, which

    in turn may lead to IL development. Qi and Lapkin (2001) found

    that the learners noticed differences between their own and

    reformulated writing. This sort of noticing facilitated by

    reformulated writing has been related to improvements in later

    revisions (Qi and Lapkin, 2001; Swain and Lapkin, in press).

    4 Noticing and learning

    Several areas of research, including research on attention (Leow,

    1997), awareness (Tomlin and Villa, 1994), and memory (Robinson,

    1995; in press) have implicated noticing as a necessary condition

    for second language development. For purposes of this study,

    Schmidts (2001) definition of noticing has been adopted. Schmidt

    Rebecca Adams 351

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    7/31

    limits noticing to a very low level of abstraction . . . assuming that

    the objects of attention and noticing are elements of the surface

    structure of utterances in the input instances of language, rather

    than any abstract rules or principles (p. 5). The purpose of this

    restricted definition is to separate metalinguistic awareness from

    noticing by limiting noticing to awareness at a very low level of

    abstraction (Schmidt, 2001: 5). Noticing can be prompted, as

    discussed above, when learners, in the context of interaction,

    realize that they do not know the necessary forms to express a

    given meaning. Noticing can also occur when learners allocate

    attentional resources to specific features in the input (Schmidt,

    1990), or when forms are made salient through interactionalfeedback (Leeman, in 2003; Long, 1996; Nicholas et al., 2001; Pica,

    1994, 1997). In both of these situations, learners are able to

    compare their own interlanguage forms with the targetlike forms

    supplied in the input and determine where discrepancies lie. This

    aspect of noticing is also referred to as noticing the gap

    (Robinson, 1995; Schmidt, 2001; Schmidt and Frota, 1986; Swain,

    1995).

    While the debate about the role of noticing in SLA theory and

    second language research methodology is on-going (Schmidt, 2001;

    Truscott, 1998), several studies indicate that noticing at least

    facilitates learning (Ellis, 1994; Lapkin and Swain, 2001; Schmidt

    and Frota, 1986; Schmidt, 1990; Swain, 1993, 1995, 1998). Mackey

    (2000) investigated the effects of learners noticing the gap through

    interactional feedback in the classroom on the development of

    morphosyntactic and lexical forms. The results indicated thatlearners who demonstrated more noticing developed more

    accurate use of certain, but not all, of the forms investigated. Izumi

    and Bigelow (2000) found that noticing led to learning as well, but

    that this was mitigated by the type of task used to promote

    noticing. Because reformulations can be used to encourage

    learners to notice differences between their own output and

    reformulated output, they can facilitate noticing the gap, which can

    in turn influence second language development.

    352 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    8/31

    5 Swain and Lapkin (in press)

    As noted earlier, this research study is a replication and extension

    of Swain and Lapkin (in press), which investigated the effect of

    reformulated writing on learner noticing and development. InSwain and Lapkins study, a pair of English L1 learners in a French

    immersion programme was asked to participate in a jigsaw task.

    Each learner was given half of a picture story and instructed to

    interact orally to put together the entire story. They were then

    instructed to write the story collaboratively. The story was given to

    a NS of French to reformulate; the original story and the

    reformulation were given to the learners and they were asked to

    find the differences between the two versions. Following thenoticing session, the learners participated in a stimulated recall

    protocol of the noticing session in order to determine whether

    noticing had actually taken place. Based on the learners discourse

    during the noticing session and their verbal reports, it was

    determined that the learners did notice differences between their

    IL and the reformulation. As a post-test, a week after the original

    task session, each learner was individually provided with theoriginal story and instructed to correct the errors.

    Swain and Lapkins study established that noticing was enabled

    by reformulated writing and that it led to incorporation of

    reformulations in the post-treatment task of correcting earlier non-

    targetlike usage. However, the post-treatment task took place after

    the learners had participated in a stimulated recall session. In this

    session, they were again exposed to the differences between their

    original output and the reformulation and were able to reflect ontheir thoughts when they first noticed these differences. While the

    intent of the stimulated recall was to determine what the learners

    had noticed about the feedback, it is possible that additional

    learning occurred during these sessions. Since the stimulated recall

    session occurred before the post-test, it is not possible to determine

    whether the stimulated recall, the noticing, or a combination of the

    two led to increased accuracy on certain forms in the post-test.Additionally, the learners dealt with very similar tasks (involving

    the same story based on the same prompts) twice. Part of their

    improvement could be related to task repetition effects. When

    learners perform a similar task more than once within a short time

    span, they can be expected to improve simply because increased

    Rebecca Adams 353

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    9/31

    task familiarity decreases the task processing difficulty (Skehan,

    1998), which can enhance the accuracy of learner output (Bygate,

    1996, 2001; Lynch and McLean, 2000, 2001). Thus, there were three

    possible sources for the learners incorporation of more targetlike

    forms in the second writing of the story: noticing the gap through

    reformulated writing, reinforcing the input through stimulated

    recall, and decreasing the processing demands through task

    repetition. Because of the exploratory nature of Swain and

    Lapkins research, teasing apart the effects of different variables

    was not the focus of the study, nor did the in-depth case study

    approach allow for comparison of different treatment effects. The

    purpose of extending this research, therefore, is to partition theeffects of the various components of their study.

    While Swain and Lapkin do not justify the use of stimulated

    recall before the post-test, other researchers (Gass and Mackey,

    2000) have raised concerns of the reactivity of stimulated recall.

    They contend that stimulated recall allows learners extra

    opportunities to reflect on and process linguistic data they receive

    from experimental treatments. Participation in the stimulatedrecall process additionally makes different demands on learners

    than the noticing treatment in the study, as the learners were not

    observing differences between their IL output and the more

    targetlike reformulation, but rather discussing the thoughts they

    had when previously noticing those differences. Since stimulated

    recall is not only another learning opportunity, but qualitatively

    different learning opportunity than the noticing treatment, its

    inclusion in the experiment before the post-test likely influencedthe conclusions inferred from the research.

    Separating the effects of noticing and stimulated recall may also

    clarify the implications for applying reformulations to second

    language learning. If comparing learner writing with reformulated

    writing leads to second language learning, this treatment could be

    investigated in a classroom setting and integrated into classroom

    learning. However, if learning is the result of both stimulated recalland noticing, it is harder to see the practical application, as

    stimulated recall does not lend itself easily to most classroom

    situations. Hence, separating noticing, stimulated recall, and task

    repetition in the experimental design can elucidate the implications

    of Swain and Lapkins findings to second language pedagogy. If

    354 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    10/31

    learning results from noticing the gap, the study could lend support

    to the noticing hypothesis. If it is the result of stimulated recall or

    task repetition, Swain and Lapkin (in press) cannot be interpreted

    as evidence for the role of noticing in second language learning.

    6 Research questions

    The current study investigates the effects of task repetition,

    noticing facilitated by exposure to reformulated writing, and

    stimulated recall by addressing the following research questions:

    1. Do learners incorporate noticed TL forms from reformulations

    in their output? (adapted from Swain and Lapkin, in press)

    2. Does noticing facilitated by exposure to NS reformulations lead

    to more incorporation of target-like forms than task repetition

    without the noticing treatment?

    3. Does participation in stimulated recall sessions in addition

    to the noticing treatment impact learners incorporation of

    reformulations in their post-treatment writing?

    II Methodology

    This experiment took place in university Spanish L2 classrooms

    and laboratories. The researcher gave instructions and monitored

    the learners to make certain that they stayed on task and

    communicated in the L2. The teachers did not participate at all in

    the data collection, nor did the researcher and the teachers assist

    learners in completing the research tasks. The study involved a pre-

    test, treatment, post-test design. In this section, the participants and

    data collection procedures will be described.

    1 Participants

    The participants for the study (N = 56) were Spanish L2 language

    learners at a North American university. All were either NSs of

    English or very highly proficient L2 speakers of English (the four

    NNSs had similar incidence of errors as the NSs of their treatment

    groups). Five classes of learners participated in the study: four

    classes of Intermediate I (third semester class in the undergraduate

    Spanish FL curriculum) and one class of Intermediate Intensive

    Rebecca Adams 355

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    11/31

    (third and fourth semester Spanish in one semester). The data were

    collected three weeks into the semester to ensure that the

    instruction in the Intermediate Intensive course had not

    progressed beyond the Intermediate I classes. Treatment groups

    consisted of learners from each of the five classes in order to

    ensure an equal distribution of learners from Intermediate I andIntermediate Intensive in all treatment groups. A description of the

    participants can be found in Table 1.

    2 Tasks

    The study follows a pre-test/treatment/post-test design, using an

    interactive task, a jigsaw task (adopted from Swain and Lapkin, in

    press), as both the pre- and post-test. The task consisted of asequence of eight pictures that together formed a story (see

    Appendix A). For the pre-test, learners were each given four

    pictures and were required to describe their pictures to each other

    in order to jointly reconstruct the story. They were then instructed

    to write out their story. While the learners were working on the

    task, they were required to speak only Spanish. For the post-test,

    learners were individually given the complete set of pictures andinstructed to write out the story. This use of the task as a pre- and

    post-task is modified from that of Swain and Lapkin in two ways.

    First, the learners in Swain and Lapkins study watched a short (5

    minute) video-taped lesson on reflexive verbs. Part of the video

    showed two students engaged in a jigsaw task. Since this study, like

    356 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

    Table 1 Learner characteristics

    Factor Learner characteristics

    Age 1822

    Gender Male 24

    Female 32

    Classes 1 (Intensive) 10

    2 15*

    3 14

    4 12

    5 5

    Note:

    *Learners performed tasks in pairs. Odd numbers of subjects indicates that oneof the pair did not take the post-test.

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    12/31

    Swain and Lapkin, did not focus solely on the use of reflexive verbs

    and since these students were accustomed to doing tasks similar

    to the research task, the video was not considered necessary.

    Secondly, learners in Swain and Lapkins study were given theiroriginal story without correction marks and asked to correct the

    errors for the post-test. Because the intention of this study was to

    determine the effects of the different treatments on the learners

    IL system, it was determined that rewriting the story from the

    pictures would induce a more valid across group comparison. Pilot-

    testing indicated that this use of the tasks was appropriate for this

    research.

    3 Data collection

    Five intermediate Spanish classes were involved in the study. The

    first stage of the study was carried out in the learners regular

    classes. Learners were divided into pairs by their teachers and one

    copy of the picture story was given to each pair of learners. Each

    learner received one-half of the pictures that made up the story;

    their partner received the other half of the pictures. Participants

    were instructed to describe their pictures to each other to

    reconstruct the story. They were instructed not to use dictionaries

    or pose questions to the researcher or the teacher. The interactions

    were audio-recorded.

    After reconstructing the story, the learners were allowed to view

    all the pictures to ensure comprehension of the story. Each dyad

    was then instructed to write out the story collaboratively. Theirdiscussion during the collaborative writing was also audio-

    recorded. During the classroom phase of the study, learners were

    required to speak only Spanish.

    The stories produced by the learners were given to either a NS

    or a near-NS of Spanish for reformulation. They were instructed

    to preserve the original meaning throughout and the original

    structure where possible, but to make the story nativelike. The

    reformulators were specifically told to change both vocabulary and

    grammar when the original did not reflect NS usage. The original

    and reformulated stories were then typed so that neither contained

    correction marks.

    The treatment phase of the study was conducted outside of class.

    Rebecca Adams 357

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    13/31

    There were three treatment groups in the study: Noticing, Noticing

    + SR, and Control. As explained previously, learners from each of

    the five classes were randomly assigned to the different groups in

    order to minimize possible effects from class membership (class

    membership was also included in the statistical models to ensure

    that it did not influence the findings). The Control group (N = 20)

    did not receive any treatment; they completed only the pre-test

    and the post-test and received no further input on the study

    material and nor feedback on their story. This group was included

    in the study to estimate the improvement based on task repetition.

    The Noticing group (N = 18) met with the researcher outside of

    class in a laboratory setting. Each dyad met individually with theresearcher. They were given a copy of their original story and of

    the reformulated version of the story and were instructed to

    compare the two versions of the story and find all the differences

    they could, discussing together why they believed the changes had

    been made. They were asked to verbalize their ideas about the

    differences between their stories and the reformulated stories in

    order to give evidence to the researcher of what they had noticed.For the sake of this study, noticing was operationalized as verbally

    stated awareness of differences between interlanguage and target

    output; Swain and Lapkin (in press) operationalized noticing in a

    similar manner. This type of noticing is commonly referred to as

    noticing the gap, and was the only type of noticing experimentally

    isolated in this study. Other noticing may have occurred under all

    experimental conditions; for example, in writing the task, learners

    may have noticed that they did not know certain key lexical items.Noticing sessions were conducted in Spanish and were audio-

    recorded. The protocol for this session closely follows that used by

    Swain and Lapkin (in press). The intent of this procedure is to

    encourage learners to notice surface level differences between

    their output and targetlike output, following Schmidts (2001)

    definition of noticing. This group was included in the study to

    isolate the effects of noticing on improvement.The final group, the Noticing + SR group (N = 18), also was

    allowed to compare their original story with the reformulated

    version, following the same instructions as the Noticing group.

    Immediately following the noticing session, the participants in this

    group participated in a stimulated recall session with the

    358 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    14/31

    researcher. Using the recording of their noticing session as a

    stimulus, the learners were asked to recall their thoughts at the

    time they made the comparisons. Learners were allowed to stop

    the tape whenever they wanted to comment on their thoughtprocesses; the researcher stopped the tape for commentary after

    each discussion of a change and after any unclarified reaction to

    the reformulations such as laughter or other non-language

    verbalizations. The stimulated recall sessions were conducted in

    English and were audio-recorded. This group was included in order

    to determine the effect of SR and noticing as opposed to noticing

    alone.

    The final phase of the study was conducted in class a week afterthe initial classroom session. Each learner was given a copy of the

    entire picture sequence from the original task and instructed to

    write out the story again on their own. It should be noted that this

    is not an exact task repetition, as the learners performed the task

    co-operatively for the pre-test and individually for the post-test.

    However, while the task conditions changed, the task form and

    objective remained the same. This was considered likely to lead toincreased accuracy in the post-test, similar to that found in more

    orthodox studies of task repetition such as Bygate (1996, 2001).

    Following the post-task, the learners filled out an exit

    questionnaire, which asked them to indicate whether they had

    looked up any unknown forms in dictionaries or grammar texts,

    and whether they had asked their teacher or other native speakers

    how to express any of the ideas they had struggled with. The

    timeline for data collection is explained in Table 2.

    4 Analysis and coding

    The changes the reformulators made on the pre-treatment texts

    produced by participants from all the groups were tallied. These

    were analysed to determine the number and type of errors the

    learners made. Reformulations were coded as directed to lexicon

    (vocabulary) or grammar; grammatical errors were further coded

    as errors of verbal morphology, preposition use, gender/agreement,

    or other. Spelling errors were checked against the tapes of the

    interactions from writing the story. If the learners used the correct

    word orally but spelled it incorrectly (or if they used a word that

    Rebecca Adams 359

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    15/31

    was plainly recognizable as the target word and not confused with

    another Spanish word), the spelling error was counted as an

    orthographic error and not included for further analysis. If the

    learners verbally used an incorrect word that was consistent with

    the spelling, it was counted as a vocabulary error. Thereformulations all were directed at non-targetlike forms. Equally

    grammatical alternatives, for example, were never included. The

    texts produced by control group pairs were also reformulated.

    Although the control group never saw the reformulations, they

    were treated as equivalent for the sake of systematic analysis. The

    reformulators were not informed which dyads were in each of the

    groups. In total, there were 746 reformulations. Of these,

    approximately half (N = 381) were directed at various grammaticalstructures and half (N = 365) at vocabulary. The proportions of

    reformulations in the data are displayed in Figure 1. The mean

    number of errors per dyad was 13.3, with a range of 620.

    Errors were then traced to the post-test. If a learner used a form

    in the post-test that had been reformulated from the pre-test, it

    was coded as either more or less targetlike (forms that remained

    unchanged were coded as less targetlike). If the learner rephrasedthe story in order to avoid a problematic form, the form was coded

    as not attempted. For the sake of this analysis, only non-targetlike

    forms from the first text were considered. Learner texts typically

    included some new non-targetlike forms these were not factored

    into the analysis for any of the groups. The total number of forms

    360 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

    Table 2 Data collection schedule

    Day 1 23 46 8

    Activities Initial in-class Reformulation: Laboratory data Final in-class

    data collection: The stories are collection: data collection:Learners reformulated by Learners Learners

    complete task highly proficient participate in individually

    and write the speakers of noticing session write out the

    story, all in Spanish in Spanish (10 picture story in

    Spanish minutes) and Spanish

    (20 minutes) stimulated recall (20 minutes)

    sessions in

    English (15

    minutes) are

    held

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    16/31

    that individual learners attempted (both more and less targetlike)

    was 445, with a mean of 7.95 per student and a range of 314. The

    distribution of reformulations in the post-tests is displayed in

    Figure 2. Grammar and vocabulary errors were fairly equally

    distributed among the more and less targetlike forms.An independent coder coded 10% of the data, and 95%

    intercoder reliability with the researchers coding was found. The

    proportion of more-targetlike forms to attempted forms was

    calculated for each learner. The scores were categorized into

    treatment groups (three levels) and into classes (five levels). Class

    membership was included to determine whether learners from

    Rebecca Adams 361

    Figure 1 Total reformulations

    Figure 2 Incorporation of reformulations in the post-test

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    17/31

    different classes would perform differently under the different

    experimental conditions. These data were submitted to a two-way

    ANOVA.

    The forms that prompted reformulation as well as the

    reformulations themselves were the topic of the noticing session.

    As mentioned previously, in these sessions, learners were instructed

    to discuss the differences they found between their original story

    and the reformulation and to try to determine why the targeted

    forms were reformulated. These discussions constituted evidence

    of noticing. While it is impossible to determine that undiscussed

    forms were not noticed (Truscott, 1998), reported noticing indicates

    that they likely were. The forms that learners from the Noticinggroup and the Noticing + SR group discussed were traced through

    to the post-test. The proportion of reported noticed forms that

    were used in a more targetlike manner in the post-test to reported

    noticed forms overall was calculated for each individual in the

    groups that participated in noticing. These data were also

    submitted to a two-way ANOVA.

    Learners in the Noticing + SR group participated in a stimulated

    recall about the noticing in addition to the noticing session. The

    recording of the noticing session served as a stimulus for these

    sessions. Learners were asked to recall their thoughts when

    comparing their own output with the reformulations. These

    sessions were transcribed and analysed. A full discussion of the

    discussions that arose from the stimulated recall is beyond the

    scope of this paper, as the focus of the research is impact of the

    stimulated recall on subsequent output. The stimulated recallsindicated that these learners, like those in Swain and Lapkins

    study, actively sought to find and understand differences between

    their own output and the reformulations.

    The coding system for this research is illustrated in Excerpt 1

    below.

    Excerpt 1:

    Original: *Ellaprepara por la escuela.

    She gets ready for school

    Reformulation: Ellasepreparapara la escuela.

    She gets herselfready for school

    362 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    18/31

    There are two errors in the original: the reflexive clitic for the verb

    prepararse to get ready is omitted, and an incorrect preposition

    por for is used. The first error is an example of one relating to

    verbal morphology (grammar) and the second as a prepositionerror (also grammar). In S1s second version of the story, she

    attempts this form as illustrated by Excerpt 5.

    Excerpt 5

    Rosa se levanta y empieza a prepararse para ir a la escuela.

    Rosa gets up and begins to get herselfready to go to school

    Both forms are attempted here, and both are used in a moretargetlike manner. These forms had been discussed in the noticing

    session and thus were coded as noticed reformulations. They were

    also discussed in the stimulated recall. Excerpt 6 is part of the

    transcript from the stimulated recall discussion of these forms.

    Excerpt 6

    S1: I didnt know that prepara shouldve been reflexive there.

    S2: Yeah, me neither.S1: And also we didnt know the difference like whether we should use por

    or para in that case.

    R: Did this did the change make sense to you? Or was it confusing?

    S1: It doesnt make sense to me. I dont know the difference.

    S2: Between por and para.

    S1: I dont get it.

    S2: But the same makes sense for the preparar for me.

    S1: Right.

    The first form, prepararse, was related to previously learned

    knowledge. The learners recognized the change and remembered

    the rule the form was based on. The second form,para, was used

    in a way that was confusing to the learners. They indicated that

    this use represents new learning from input in the reformulations,

    for which they could not rely on their current IL system. Note that

    in this excerpt, as in all of the stimulated recall interviews, the

    researcher did not supply any rule or explanation of the structures

    discussed.

    All of the forms that were reported as noticed in the noticing

    sessions were discussed in the stimulated recall sessions. The

    stimulated recall sessions did, however, make it clear that the

    Rebecca Adams 363

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    19/31

    learners noticed differences between their own and the

    reformulated output, that they used a variety of strategies to

    process the feedback, and that the stimulated recall sessions

    themselves qualitatively affected the learners acquisition of

    targetlike forms from reformulated writing.

    III Results

    The results of the analyses are presented in the order of the

    research questions stated in the first section of this article.

    1 Noticing and learningThe first research question was as follows: Do learners incorporate

    noticed TL forms from reformulations in their output? In order to

    answer this question, the results for the two groups that

    participated in noticing sessions were grouped together, and the

    percentage of noticed forms that were incorporated in the post-

    test output in a more targetlike manner was calculated. When

    learners in these groups attempted to use a reformulation that theyhad reported noticing in the post-test, they used it in a more

    targetlike manner around 61% of the time (standard deviation is

    18%). The mean number of attempted reformulations was 8.7

    forms, and the mean number of more targetlike reformulations was

    5.3. Figure 3 displays the more and less targetlike use of grammar

    and vocabulary in the post-test.

    364 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

    Figure 3 More and less targetlike usage on the post-test

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    20/31

    The percentages of incorporated grammatical forms and

    vocabulary are roughly equivalent, indicating that grammatical and

    vocabulary reformulations were equally likely to be incorporated

    in the post-test. The relatively large standard deviation, however,

    indicates that there is a great deal of variation among the learners

    in these groups.

    2 Differences among groups

    The second and third research questions dealt with the differences

    among the three treatment groups: Control, Noticing and Noticing

    + SR. It is here that the current study departs from Swain and

    Lapkins study, as their one dyad design did not allow them to

    separate out the effects of the different treatments. The point of

    directly comparing the three groups is to estimate how much of

    the incorporation of targetlike forms was related to task repetition,

    how much to noticing, and how much to the stimulated recall. The

    measurement of these effects is cumulative in the design. The

    Control groups scores represent the effects of task repetition, the

    Noticing groups scores represent the cumulative effects of taskrepetition and noticing, and the Noticing + SR groups scores

    represent the cumulative effects of task repetition, noticing, and

    stimulated recall. By directly comparing the three groups, it is

    possible to partial out the impact of each of these factors.

    The proportion of reformulations that were incorporated in a

    more targetlike form to the total number of reformulations was

    calculated for each learner. Descriptive statistics for these values

    are displayed in Table 3.

    Residual analysis indicated that there was unequal variance

    among the groups. Accordingly, the data were transformed using a

    standard transformation for proportion data ( = 2asiny), as

    suggested by Neter et al. (1996). These data were submitted to a

    two-factor ANOVA. As discussed above, two ANOVA models

    Rebecca Adams 365

    Table 3 Incorporation of more TL forms

    Group Mean Standard deviation

    Control 29.23 23.75

    Noticing 50.05 22.94

    Noticing + SR 70.97 19.34

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    21/31

    were used. The first (reported here) compared all three groups; the

    second (reported below) compared only the Noticing group with

    the Noticing + SR group using scores for learning of forms where

    learners gave overt indications of noticing. It was found that there

    were significant differences among the three groups (F = 2.84, df

    = 42, p < 0.05). There were no significant main factor effects for

    class membership (F = 1.92, df = 4, p > 0.05), and no significant

    interaction between class membership and group

    (F = 0.87, df = 6, p > 0.05). There were significant main factor

    effects for treatment group (F = 4.99, df = 2, p = 0.01), indicating

    that the different treatments significantly affected learners use of

    more targetlike forms on the post-test. The different treatmenteffects are illustrated in Figure 4. Post hoc Tukey analysis was

    conducted to determine which groups were significantly different.

    Both the Noticing group and the Noticing + SR group significantly

    outperformed the Control group. While the Noticing + SR group

    outperformed the Noticing group, this difference was not

    significant at the p < 0.05 level. It did, however, approach

    significance. Table 4 displays the contrasts between group means

    for the three treatment groups.

    Because the Control group did not receive a noticing treatment,

    in analyses that included the Control group any reformulated form

    was considered a target for reformulation. However, if only the

    Noticing and Noticing + SR groups are compared, it is possible to

    more closely examine the effects of noticing. For these two groups,

    the proportion of more targetlike incorporated reformulations to

    366 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

    Figure 4 Means by treatment group

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    22/31

    reformulations that the learners reported noticing were calculated.

    These data were submitted to a two-way ANOVA (F = 4.38,

    df = 24, p < 0.05). There was no significant interaction between

    class membership and treatment group (F = 1.62, df = 3, p > 0.05);

    main factor effects were found for both treatment group (F = 9.58,df = 1, p < 0.01) and for class membership (F = 5.55, df = 3,

    p < 0.01). Post hoc Tukey analysis indicated that the Noticing + SR

    group significantly outperformed the Noticing group. This indicates

    that stimulated recall increased the chance that learners would

    incorporate the reformulations into their subsequent output. The

    differences between the two groups are illustrated in Figure 5.

    Tukey analysis also indicated that members of one class (whether

    in the Noticing or the Noticing + SR group) incorporated

    significantly fewer reformulations in the post-test. Without further

    investigation of this class, the instructional practices, and individual

    learner differences, it is difficult to draw inferences from this

    finding. However, there was no interaction between group and

    class membership; learners from this class, like those from the

    other classes, incorporated more reformulations if they

    participated in both noticing and stimulated recall.

    Rebecca Adams 367

    Table 4 Tukey contrasts among groups

    Noticing Control

    Noticing + SR 14.09 37.19*

    Noticing 23.10*

    Note:

    *p < .05

    Figure 5 Incorporation of noticed reformulations on the post-test

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    23/31

    4 Discussion

    The first research question addressed the evidence of noticing, and

    was adapted from Swain and Lapkins study. Swain and Lapkin (in

    press) found that learners incorporated the majority of thereformulations in their subsequent output. The results of the

    current study confirm Swain and Lapkins finding that noticing

    facilitated by discussion of reformulated writing has an impact on

    learners subsequent writing. Learners who participated in the

    noticing treatments incorporated more than 60% of the

    reformulations in their output. However, there was wide variation

    in the proportion of forms incorporated. This implies that noticing

    was much more effective in prompting some learners toincorporate more targetlike forms in their output than others. It is

    possible that individual learner differences such as motivation

    (Drnyei, 1998) or aptitude (Skehan, 1998) regulate the

    effectiveness of noticing in facilitating learning.

    The finding that 60% of targeted forms were incorporated in a

    more targetlike manner overall is lower than the percentage found

    by Swain and Lapkin (in press). Both of their learners incorporatedat least 78% of the reformulations in a more targetlike manner.

    This discrepancy could stem from several different sources. Swain

    and Lapkins learners were French immersion learners, while the

    learners in this study were university Spanish learners. Because the

    learners in the current study had not learned Spanish in an

    immersion situation, but rather were studying in classes that made

    use of overt grammatical and vocabulary instruction, they were

    possibly less accustomed to learning under implicit conditions.Also, Swain and Lapkins two learners had both received the

    noticing treatment and the stimulated recall treatment, while half

    of the learners reported here had only received the noticing

    treatment. However, when learners from the Noticing group are

    removed from the analysis, the mean percentage of more targetlike

    incorporation of reformulations increases to only 64%. The

    difference in findings could also be due to the larger populationinvolved in this study. The proficiency of one of Swain and Lapkins

    two learners was rated as slightly above average by the teacher (4

    on a 7-point scale); the other was rated as highly above average

    (6/7). This study included a range of learners from several

    classrooms, not only average and above average learners.

    368 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    24/31

    The second question dealt with the effects of task repetition. The

    comparisons among the groups indicate that each of the factors

    represented task repetition, noticing, and stimulated recall

    contributed to learning facilitated by reformulated writing. The

    Control group, whose only treatment was to repeat the task, used

    approximately 30% of the non-targetlike forms from the pre-test

    in a more targetlike manner when they attempted to use those

    forms again in the post-test. Increased accuracy as a result of task

    repetition is predicted by the task-based language learning

    literature (Skehan, 1998). The first time the learners participated

    in the task, they were likely focused on expressing the meaning.

    The second time they wrote the story, they could focus more onthe forms they were using, resulting in more accurate usage. The

    increased accuracy of the task repetition group was similar to that

    found in other task-based studies, including Bygate (1996, 2001)

    and Lynch and McLean (2000, 2001). This interpretation is

    supported by the stimulated recall data from this study, in which

    learners often indicated that they immediately recognized their

    mistake and that they had previously learned the relevant form,but mistakenly had not used it when writing the story. The learners

    were better able to avoid these errors when the communicative

    burden was diminished. This finding illustrates the importance of

    conservative interpretation of treatment effects in studies without

    comparison groups, as improved accuracy on post-treatment

    measures are partially attributable to factors other than the

    treatments.

    The final research question dealt with the impact of stimulatedrecall on the findings. The difference between the Noticing + SR

    group and the Noticing group on the incorporation of noticed

    reformulations indicates that stimulated recall, in addition to

    yielding information about the learners mental processing during

    the noticing treatment, also acts as a learning experience. This

    finding underscores concerns about the reactivity of introspective

    research methods (Gass and Mackey, 2000). While such methodsallow researchers to investigate learners conscious processing,

    they can also affect the phenomenon under observation. Methods

    like stimulated recall that give learners a second opportunity to

    reflect on the treatment may reinforce previous learning or give

    learners time to process input that they have not previously

    Rebecca Adams 369

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    25/31

    processed. If such research is conducted before post-treatment

    measures are completed, they can affect the post-test outcomes,

    exaggerating the apparent effectiveness of a given treatment.

    While part of the improved accuracy on post-tests was

    attributable to task repetition, and part to the stimulated recall,

    the Noticing group still significantly incorporated reformulations

    in the post-treatment output even when the effects of task

    repetition are parcelled out. This lends support to the Noticing

    Hypothesis (Schmidt and Frota, 1986; Schmidt, 1990, 2001)

    indicating that noticing surface differences between IL and NS

    output can facilitate learning, at least in the short term. Further

    studies could determine whether this learning persists over longerperiods.

    Finally, it should be remembered that relatively large variation

    was found for all groups standard deviations of greater than 20

    were found for each of the group means. This indicates that none

    of the three treatments was equally effective for all learners. Most

    likely, individual factors beyond the scope of this study mitigated

    the effects of each treatment. Indeed, research on implicit learning

    has repeatedly indicated that individual factors including

    motivation, aptitude, learning orientation, working memory,

    grammatical sensitivity and others impact learning under implicit

    conditions (see Robinson, 2002, for discussions on these and other

    individual differences). However, the size of the groups and the

    balanced design mitigated the effects of within-group variation,

    equalizing the variation across groups. The equality of variance

    allows for making inferences about the treatments themselves. Itshould also be noted that this study did not involve any delayed

    post-testing, so it is unknown whether the use of more targetlike

    forms in the post-test was indicative of long-term changes in the

    learners interlanguage. However, these data overall support the

    conclusion that participation in a noticing treatment facilitates

    more learning than task repetition alone, and that participation in

    noticing and stimulated recall facilitates more learning than

    noticing alone.

    370 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    26/31

    5 Conclusion

    These findings bring up new questions, some of which have already

    been discussed. The role of individual factors that might cause

    the variation found among learners within groups could beinvestigated. This would help us understand how different learners

    approach and process the negative and positive feedback provided

    by reformulations. Further research could also determine whether

    the benefits of noticing facilitated by reformulated writing persist

    over time, and whether they extend to contexts beyond that of the

    task used in this research.

    This study supports Swain and Lapkins (in press) claim that

    noticing, facilitated by exposure to reformulated writing, can

    promote learning of more targetlike forms. The learners in the

    current study who participated in noticing sessions incorporated

    significantly more reformulated forms into their IL than those who

    did not. The results of this study also indicate that the use of

    stimulated recall influenced the findings in that the extra exposure

    to the reformulations given to learners during the stimulated recall

    protocol, as well as the extra time afforded them to process thosedifferences, enhanced the learning from reformulations. These

    findings imply that reformulated writing might be an effective tool

    for second language pedagogy, and supports the effectiveness of

    written output and feedback for noticing and learning forms.

    Acknowledgements

    I would like to thank Alison Mackey, Merrill Swain, Lauren Ross-Feldman and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on

    earlier drafts of this paper. I would also like to thank Yi-Young

    Kim, Cathy Stafford and Nina Moreno for assistance in data

    collection, and Ron Leow, Christina Sanz and the teachers involved

    in this study for access to classes for data collection.

    IV ReferencesAllwright, R.L., Woodley, M.-P. and Allwright, J.M. 1988: Investigating

    reformulation as a practical strategy for the teaching of academic

    writing.Applied Linguistics 9: 23656.Ashwell, T. 2000: Patterns of teacher response to learner writing in a

    multiple-draft composition classroom: is content feedback followed

    Rebecca Adams 371

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    27/31

    by form feedback the best method? Journal of Second LanguageWriting 9: 22757.

    Brooks, L. and Swain, M. 2001: Collaborative writing and expert

    feedback: how they support second language learning. Toronto:

    Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University ofToronto.

    Bygate, M. 1996: Effects of task repetition: appraising the developinglanguage of learners. In Willis J. and Willis, D., editors, Challenge

    and change in language teaching, London: Heinemann, 13646. 2001: Effects of task repetition on the structure and control of oral

    language. In Bygate, M., Skehan P., and Swain, M., editors,Researching pedagogic tasks, New York: Longman, 2348.

    Carson, J. 2001: Second Language Writing and second languageacquisition. In Silva, T. and Matsuda, P., editors, On SecondLanguage Writing, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,19199.

    Cohen, A. 1983: Reformulating second-language compositions: a potentialsource of input for the learner. Paper presented at the AnnualConvention of Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages,Toronto. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 228 866.)25 pp.

    Conrad, S.M. and Goldstein, L.M. 1999: ESL learner revision afterteacher-written comments: text, contents, and individuals.Journal ofSecond Language Writing 8: 14779.

    De Guerrero, M. and Villamil, O. 2001: Activating the ZPD: Mutual

    Scaffolding in L2 peer revision. The Modern Language Journal84,5168.

    Drnyei, Z. 1998: Motivation in foreign and second language learning.Language Teaching 31: 11735.

    Doughty, C. 2001. Cognitive underpinnings of focus on form. In RobinsonP., editor, Cognition and second language instruction, Cambridge:Cambridge University Press, 20657.

    Ellis, N.C. 2001: Memory for language. In Robinson, P., editor, Cognition

    and second language instruction, Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress, 3368.

    Ellis, R. 1994: Factors in the incidental acquisition of second languagevocabulary from oral input: a review essay. Applied LanguageLearning 5: 132.

    Gass, S. 1997:Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Mahwah,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Gass, S. and Mackey, A. 2000: Stimulated recall methodology in secondlanguage research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Hatch, E. 1978: Acquisition of syntax in a second language. In Richards,J., Understanding second and foreign language learning, Rowley,

    372 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    28/31

    MA: Newbury House, 3470.Hedgcock, J. and Lefkowitz, N. 1992: Collaborative oral/aural revision in

    foreign language writing instruction. Journal of Second Language

    Writing 1: 25576.

    1994: Feedback on feedback: assessing learner receptivity to teacherresponse in L2 composing.Journal of Second Language Writing 3:14163.

    1996: Some input on input: two analyses of student response to

    expert feedback in L2 writing. Modern Language Journal 80:287308.

    Hyland, F. 1998: The impact of teacher written feedback on individualwriters.Journal of Second Language Writing 7: 25588.

    2000: ESL writers and feedback: giving more autonomy to students.Language Teaching Research 4: 3354.Izumi, S. and Bigelow, M. 2000: Does output promote noticing and second

    language acquisition? TESOL Quarterly 34: 23978.Kassen, M.A. 1988: Native and non-NS teacher response to foreign

    language learner writing: a study of intermediate level French.Texaspapers in foreign language education 1, pp. 115. (Eric DocumentReproduction Service No. ED 345483.)

    Kepner, C.G. 1991: An experiment in the relationship of types of written

    feedback to the development of second-language writing skills.Modern Language Journal75: 30513.

    Krashen, S.D. 1985: The input hypothesis: issues and implications. London:Longman.

    Lapkin, S. and Swain, M. 2001: Focus on form through collaborativedialogue: exploring task effects. In Bygate, M., Skehan, P. and Swain,M., Researching pedagogic tasks: second language learning, teaching,and testing, London: Longman, 99118.

    Leeman, J. 2003: Records and second language development. Studies inSecond Language Acquisition 25: 3763.Leow, R. 1997: Attention, awareness, and foreign language behavior.

    Language Learning 47: 467505.Long, M. 1996: The role of the linguistic environment in second language

    acquisition. In Ritchie, W.C. and Bhatia, T.K., Handbook of languageacquisition. Vol. 2: Second language acquisition. New York:Academic Press, 41368.

    Lynch, T. and MacLean, J. 2000: Exploring the benefits of task repetition

    and recycling for classroom language learning. Language TeachingResearch 4: 22150.

    2001. A case of exercising: effect of immediate task repetition onlearners performance. In Bygate, M., Skehan, P. and Swain, M.,

    editors, Researching pedagogic tasks, New York: Longman, 14162.Mackey, A. 2000: Feedback, noticing and second language development:

    Rebecca Adams 373

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    29/31

    an empirical study of L2 classroom interaction. Paper presented atthe British Association for Applied Linguistics, October 2000,Cambridge, UK.

    Mackey, A., Adams, R., Stafford, C. and Winke, P. 2002a: Modified output

    and working memory. Paper presented at the annual meeting of theAmerican Association of Applied Linguists (April 610), Salt LakeCity, UT.

    Mackey, A., Philp, J., Egi, T., Fujii, A. and Tatsumi, T. 2002b: Individual

    differences in working memory, noticing of interactional feedback,and L2 development. In Robinson, P. and Skehan, P., Individualdifferences in L2 Learning, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 181210.

    McLeod, B. and McLaughlin, B. 1986: Restructuring or automaticity?

    Reading in a second language. Language Learning 36: 10923.Mendoca, C.O. and Johnson, K.E. 1994:Peer review negotiations: revisionactivities in ESL writing instruction. TESOL Quarterly 28: 74569.

    Neter, J., Kutner, M.H., Nachtsheim, C.J. and Wasserman, W. 1996:Applied linear statistical models. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Nicholas, H., Lightbown, P.H. and Spada, N. 2001: Recasts as feedbackto language learners. Language Learning 51: 71851.

    Paulus, T.M. 1999: The effect of peer and teacher feedback on learnerwriting.Journal of Second Language Writing 8: 26589.

    Philp, J. 2003: Constraints on noticing the gap. Studies in SecondLanguage Acquisition 25: 99126.

    Pica, T. 1994: Review article: Research on negotiation: what does itreveal about second-language learning conditions, processes, and

    outcomes? Language Learning 44: 493527. 1997: Second language research and research relationships: a North

    American view. Language Teaching Research 1: 4872.Qi, D.S. and Lapkin, S. 2001: Exploring the role of noticing in a three-

    stage second language writing task. Journal of Second LanguageWriting, 10: 277303.Reid, J.M. 1982: The process of composition. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.:

    Prentice-Hall.Robinson, P. 1995: Attention, memory, and the noticing hypothesis.

    Language Learning 45: 283331.Robinson, P., editor, 2002:Individual differences and instructed language

    learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 2003: Attention and memory during SLA. In Doughty, C. and Long,

    M.H., editors, Handbook of second language acquisition, Oxford:Blackwell. In press.

    Schmidt, R. 1990: The role of consciousness in second language learning.Applied Linguistics 11: 12958.

    2001: Attention. In Robinson, P., Cognition and second languageinstruction, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 332.

    374 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    30/31

    Rebecca Adams 375

    Schmidt, R. and Frota, S.N. 1986: Developing basic conversational abilityin a second language: a case study of an adult learner of Portuguese.In Day, R., Talking to learn: conversation in SLA, Rowley, MA:Newbury House, 237326.

    Shi, L. 1998: Effects of prewriting discussions on adult ESL studentscompositions.Journal of Second Language Writing 7: 319345.

    Skehan, P. 1998: A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford:Oxford University Press.

    Swain, M. 1985: Communicative competence: some roles of comprehen-sible input and comprehensive output in its development. In Gass,S. and Madden, C.,Input in Second Language Acquisition, Rowley,MA: Newbury House, 235253.

    1993: The output hypothesis: Just speaking and writing arentenough. Canadian Modern Language Review 50: 15864. 1995: Three functions of output in second language learning. In

    Cook, G. and Seidlhofer, B., Principles and practice in alliedlinguistics: studies in honor of H.G. Widdowson, Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press, 12544.

    1998: Focus on form through conscious reflection. In Doughty, C.and Williams,J., editors, Focus on form in classroom second languageacquisition, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 6481.

    Swain, M. and Lapkin, S. 1998: Interaction and second language learning:two adolescent French immersion learners working together.Modern Language Journal80: 32037.

    2003: Talking it through: two French immersion learners responseto reformulated writing.International Review of Applied Linguistics.

    (Special Issue on The role of interaction in instructed languagelearning.) In press.

    Storch, N. 2001: How collaborative is pair work? ESL tertiary learners

    composing in pairs.Language Teaching Research

    5: 2953.Tomlin, R. and Villa, V. 1994: Attention in cognitive science and secondlanguage acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16:183203.

    Truscott, J. 1998: Noticing in second language acquisition: a criticalreview. Second Language Research 14: 10335.

    Zhang, S. 1995: Reexamining the affective advantage of peer feedback inthe ESL writing class. Journal of Second Language Writing 4:20922.

  • 7/27/2019 Language Teaching Research-2003-Adams-347-76.pdf

    31/31

    376 L2 output, reformulation and noticing

    Appendix A: Pictures used in jigsaw task

    Reproduced from Swain and Lapkin (in press)