Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    1/20

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/Journal of Conflict Resolution

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183Theonline version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/002200277201600204

    1972 16: 183Journal of Conflict ResolutionEdward E. Azar

    Conflict escalation and conflict reduction in an international crisis: Suez, 1956

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Peace Science Society (International)

    can be found at:Journal of Conflict ResolutionAdditional services and information for

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.refs.htmlCitations:

    What is This?

    - Jun 1, 1972Version of Record>>

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183http://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183http://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://pss.la.psu.edu/http://jcr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jcr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jcr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.refs.htmlhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.refs.htmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.full.pdfhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.full.pdfhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.full.pdfhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183.refs.htmlhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://jcr.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://jcr.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://pss.la.psu.edu/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/content/16/2/183http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    2/20

    Conflict escalation and conflict reductionin an international crisis: Suez, 1956

    EDWARD E.AZAR1

    Department of Political Science, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill

    Introduction

    Conflict research has already advanced

    some very useful conceptual and empiricaldiscoveries concerning the structure and

    process of international conflict escalation.

    Some of the more basic findings suggestthat when two parties involve themselves

    in a conflict situation, they tend to escalatethe intensity, scope, and frequency of their

    negative signals; they tend to make more

    visible or even possibly compound theirareas of incompatibility; they tend to raise

    the intensity of their mutual images; and

    they tend to engage in or increase the rate

    of a potentially destructive arms race

    (Boulding, 1%1; North, 1963; Richardson,

    1960; Singer, 1962; Smoker, 1966; Wright,

    1965).Conflict reduction on the other hand is

    less well understood. However there is

    enough empirical and theoretical work on

    which further research may be based. For

    example we know that motivating an oppo-nent to clarify his signals extremely well

    may produce favorable responses, reduce

    the chances of further negative signals, and

    probably induce conflict reduction (Quester,1970). There is some evidence that the pres-sure of national and international opinionon decision-makers can induce a loweringof hostile activities

    (Randle, 1970;Rothstein,

    1970). Carrolls (1968) work on how wars

    end, Timasheffs (1965) work on the transi-

    tion from war to peace and from revolution

    to order, Osgoods (1962) work on GRIT,and the variety of gaming research on condi-tions for producing higher proportions of

    cooperative responses provide us with use-

    ful concepts, ideas, and findings about the

    movement from high levels of conflict to

    reduction of hostilities. OConnors (1969)study of modern war suggests that conflict

    reduction resides in the parties ability to

    contain their hostilities through a willful ef-

    fort to combat the pathological and other

    forces which maintain a hostile situation.

    The present study aims to contribute to

    the understanding of conflict reduction in

    international crises. It focuses on the level

    andtype

    of interaction manifested

    bythe

    signals exchanged between conflicting par-ties and the role that these signals play in

    generating or deterring future hostilities.

    1My thanks go to the Department of Political

    Science, the Institute for Research in Social

    Science, and the University Research Council atthe University of North Carolina at Chapel Hillfor their supportand to Thomas Goins, David

    Pansius, Louise Richey, Thomas Sloan, and Rob-ert Taylor for their assistance. I am indebtedto Joseph Ben-Dak, Barry Blechman, Davis Bo-brow, Nazli Choucri, Paul Conn, Robert North,

    and J. David Singer for their criticisms and help-ful comments. I am especially grateful to ClintonFink for his invaluable guidance which helpedme write this paper.

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    3/20

    184

    Some general assumptions and definitionsare introduced, and descriptive hypothesesconcerning escalation and deescalation in in-

    ternational crises are presented. These hy-

    pothesesare then tested

    usingevents data

    from the Suez Crisis of 1956. Finally a sug-gested set of correlates and determinants

    of conflict reduction are posited and dis-

    cussed.

    Inter-nation Interaction:Assumptionsand Definitions

    International actors tend to express their

    policies towards one another in the formof verbal and/or physical signals.An in-ternational signal is an inter-nation event

    which has the following characteristics: on

    a specific date a specific actor directs an

    activity towards a specific target regardingan issue of mutual concern. The date is

    the day on which the signal is reported bya reputable and publicly available source;actors and

    targetsare

    nations, organizations,or movements which have attained interna-

    tional or regional significance; activities are

    verbal or physical actions, reactions, and

    interactions; and issue-areas include the

    items about which actors and targets interact

    or signal one another (Azar, 1970). Interna-

    tional signals are units of overt behavior

    which international actors receive, interpret,and act upon. They vary in frequency and

    content intensity and are scaled either impli-citly or explicitly by an international actor

    in order to better assess his own behavior

    as well as that of his target.

    The frequency and level of friendliness

    or hostility of inter-nation signals are partly

    dependent on the frequency and intensityof previously exchanged signals, since some

    have the capability of generating others. The

    entire sequence of antecedent andconse-

    quent signals therefore establishes a patternwhich can be studied longitudinally. This

    is not to argue that one inter-nation signal

    strictly causes another but rather that cer-

    tain signals tend to flow or occur simulta-

    neously and that some nonzero level of in-

    terdependence exists between international

    signals.We do

    suggesthowever that the

    process of event-generation depends on the

    interaction situation itself as well as on the

    decisions made by the parties to the conflict

    and that a nations decisions are affected

    not only by its responses to stimuli providedby the other nation, but also by the signalswhich it or its own subsystems previouslygenerated.Over a period of time any two nations

    establish between them an interaction rangewhich they perceive as &dquo;normal.&dquo; This nor-

    mal relations range (NRR) is an interaction

    range (on a scale from very friendly to very

    hostile) which tends to incorporate most of

    the signals exchanged between that pair and

    is bound by two critical thresholds-an up-

    per and a lower threshold. The upper critical

    threshold is that level of hostility above

    which

    signalsexhibited

    byeither member

    of the interacting dyad are regarded as unac-

    ceptable to the other. Interaction above the

    present upper critical threshold (or present

    upper tolerable limit) for more than a veryshort time implies that a crisis situation has

    set in. The lower critical threshold on the

    other hand is that level of friendliness

    beyond which signals between the members

    imply that some integrative shift in their

    relations-the inverse of a crisis-has oc-

    curred.

    Changes in the level of a dyads NRR

    may occur (albeit slowly) due to changesin each actors domestic and international

    situation, attributes, or behavior.Althoughit would be useful to study the conditions

    under which these shifts occur, and the time

    it takes to bring them about, for the presentwe

    simplifyour

    task by assumingthat a

    dyads NRR does not change significantly

    during a relatively short period of time, such

    as six months to one year.

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    4/20

    185

    Conflict escalation is the movement of

    a dyad to an interaction space above the

    upper critical threshold of its present NRR.

    Conflict reduction is the movement of that

    dyad toan

    interaction space below its imme-diate upper critical threshold. This space

    can be within its previous NRR or some-

    where below it.Although it is very impor-tant to focus on how a dyad moves towardsor even below its lower critical threshold

    (i.e., toward the more friendly or coopera-tive end of the scale), we do not explorethat possibility here. This paper is con-

    cerned only with those factors which allow

    a pair of nations in a crisis to shift their

    interactions back below the upper critical

    threshold.

    The Literature on Correlates and

    Determinants of Escalation andDeescalation

    MOTIVATION

    Nations like other social organizationstend to define for themselves a preferredstate-of-affairs which includes those situa-

    tions they wish to attain as well as ones

    they wish to avoid (Miller et aL, 1960).These desired states-of-affairs or prefer-ences are established by a nations examina-tion of its strategic situation vis-a-vis its

    international targets and can involve the fol-lowing : (a) an inventory and rank-orderingof preferences; (b) an evaluation of the costs

    and chances of achieving or approximatingsuch preferences; (c) an assessment of the

    type and level of behavior necessary to

    achieve or approximate these preferences;and (d) a modification or reranking of pref-erences (preference modulation). It may be

    difficult for an observer to discern these

    steps, because decision-makers do not

    usually make such a process public and be-

    cause these are done incrementally. Also

    depending on a nations capabilities and do-

    mestic and international difficulties, it is

    conceivable that some nations might followan ad hoc approach in defining their strate-

    gic preferences.

    A conflict situation between a pair of na-tions tends to arise from their pursuance

    of directly incompatible preferences, where

    the success of one would inflict a cost on

    the other (Schelling, 1960). Thus in their

    attempt to achieve their preferences nation-

    states tend to define the saliency of the

    issues and the costs they are willing to incur

    in order to achieve them.As their prefer-ence incompatibility increases, it is likelythat these nations will intensify the volume

    and threat content of their signals in a man-

    ner which they feel will allow them to

    achieve or approximate their desired state-

    of-affairs (Pruitt, 1965).

    Wright (1965) has argued that changes in

    the level of hostilities between nations are

    influenced by the saliency of the issues over

    which conflicts arise, and that rather than

    occurring by accident war is generally usedas an instrument of policy.As hostile in-

    teractions escalate, parties tend to place

    higher values on certain issues and tend

    to marshall their domestic and international

    resources for the purpose of achieving the

    goals or preferences they associate with

    their salient issues. He found that conflict-

    ing nations tend to begin their hostile in-

    teractions by applying pressure short of mili-tary force to resolve their perceived incon-

    sistencies. If however they fail to achieve

    their goals, they tend to intervene militarilyor go to war to dictate a solution.

    It is true however that if two nations

    find that their costs have been too highwhen compared with their gains or that thereis no possible means of achieving their ob-

    jectives, they are likely to (a) lower their

    aspiration level and/or (b) reorder their

    preferences. These choices are made not

    on the basis of a nations own situation

    but on the basis of the information it has

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    5/20

    186

    about its targets preference schedule, costs,and past and anticipated behavior. Thus if

    interacting nations perceive that their prefer-ences or priorities have been achieved, it

    is probable that they will formulatenew

    preferences and raise their level of expecta-tions (see Timasheff, 1965, pp. 91-98).

    FREQUENCYAND INTENSITY OF

    INTER-NATION HOSTILE SIGNALS

    Symmetrical and asymmetrical signalling

    during conflict escalation and reduction. In

    a conflict situation interactions between a

    pair of nations tend to contain or implysome level of violence or hostility.Althoughsome inter-nation signals may express a de-

    gree of cooperation, when two nations are

    locked into an escalatory spiral they tend

    to exhibit &dquo;selective perception,&dquo; thus be-

    coming more sensitive to the relativelyhostile signals of their opponent than to the

    relatively cooperative ones.And the greater

    the sensitivity of a nation to threateningsignals in some area, the more likely it is

    that that nation will perceive a signal in

    that area and will assess the signal as hostile.This may account for the inconsistencyof a nation that perceives two equally hostile

    signals directed from two sources as signifi-

    cantly different in their hostility content.

    It is probable however that this would not

    hold true at the extremes of a continuum

    of hostile behavior.A very cooperative ac-

    tion would most likely be viewed correctlyin most instances, and similarly a highlyhostile action would probably be viewed as

    highly hostile.Although ambiguous signals

    may be exchanged between two conflictingnations, such ambiguity tends to disappearwhen two nations begin to interact outside

    the critical thresholds of their normal rela-

    tions range (NRR).In his conflict studies North (1967) has

    shown that when inter-nation hostilities

    reach crisis proportions (i.e., high levels ofviolent behavior), a tit-for-tat phenomenondevelops-thus producing an increase in the

    frequency and relative distribution of in-

    teractions. These studies show that as actorsincrease the exchange of hostile verbal mes-

    sages towards one another, they are likelyto exchange more hostile physical messagesas well. Thus in terms of our model as

    nations begin to increase the rate of their

    verbal hostile signals, they are likely to

    move towards the upper critical threshold

    of their NRR. This escalatory process

    depends upon a nations own strategic pref-erences and cost tolerance as well as its

    assessment of its opponents signals towards

    that nation (Milburn, 1971; OConnor, 1969;

    Timasheff, 1965).

    Wright has described these inter-nation

    escalatory moves and countermoves as

    &dquo;mechanical&dquo; conflict behavior or a tit-for-

    tat pattern. Thus as a nation becomes threat-

    ened due to a sharp increase in the hostile

    signals received from its opponent, then thatnation is likely to respond with similar acts.

    In other words nations that enter into a

    conflict situation are likely to escalate their

    hostile interactions symmetrically, and this

    symmetry manifests itself in terms of the

    frequency and level of hostility.Does this symmetry in hostile interactions

    manifest itself during conflict reduction as

    well as it does during conflict escalation?

    There is limited evidence to suggest that

    inter-nation interactions during conflict re-

    duction do not follow the tit-for-tat pattern

    (Azar, 1970, 1971a). On the other hand we

    feel that more knowledge about this phe-nomenon is necessary. For example doesthe asymmetrical pattern of signalling duringconflict reduction hold for nations with a

    relatively hostile NRR as well as for those

    with a relatively friendly NRR? ff yes, why?How do we explain this phenomenon in

    order to understand how conflict may be

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    6/20

    187

    reduced?Although we will not be able to

    answer these questions with the 1956 Suez

    data alone, we hope to explore some explan-

    atory hypotheses towards the end of this

    paper for the purpose of definingsome

    worthwhile areas of investigation for stu-

    dents of conflict reduction.

    The first descriptive hypothesis which

    emerges from the above discussion is that

    when two or more nations perceive them-

    selves as parties to a conflict situation and

    when these parties begin exchanging hostile

    signals such that both nations begin to move

    toward or beyond the upper critical thresh-

    old oftheir NRR, these nations will exhibit

    a symmetrical signalling pattern during the

    escalatory phase of the conflict and a slight-ly asymmetrical pattern during the deescala-

    tory phase.The temporal distance between hostile sig-

    nals in a conflict situation. McClelland

    (1961) has shown that as international actors

    escalate their hostile interactions to the

    point of crisis where they begin to exchangeviolent verbal and physical threats, the

    simple volume of their interactions increases

    substantially in comparison to their precrisisrelations. From his study of East-West in-

    teractions Corson (1969) has found that

    since 1945 periods of high tension and threat

    tended to be characterized by an increase

    in the volume of conflict events.

    Conversely Milburn (1971) has found asignificant positive correlation between a na-

    tions ability to seek a crisis settlement and

    that nations capacity to delay hostile

    response to its target nation. Both McClel-

    land and Corson have argued that noncrisis

    periods tended to be characterized by a re-

    duction in the simple volume of interactions

    as compared to the crisis periods.These studies suggest therefore that dur-

    ing escalation and deescalation there is a

    high degree of association between volume

    and intensity of hostile signals. They imply

    that as the level of signalled hostility in-

    creases, the time interval between signalsdecreases (i.e., their frequency per unit oftime increases), and that as the level of

    signalled hostility decreases, the time inter-val between signals increases. Thus as the

    conflict escalation spiral begins to develop,the length of time (temporal distance) inter-

    vening between hostile signals drops signifi-cantly, and as conflict reduction sets in,the temporal distance between hostile sig-nals rises sharply.

    In terms of our model, we posit another

    descriptive hypothesis, namely as nations

    begin to move up towards (or above) the

    upper critical threshold of their NRR, the

    temporal distance between hostile signals de-

    creases. Conversely as nations begin tomove their interactions back towards (or

    below) the upper critical threshold of their

    NRR, the temporal distance between hostile

    signals increases.

    Shifts from escalation to reduction of sig-

    nalled hostility between nation-states. Al-though conflicting nations can maintainhostile signalling for long periods (as in

    lengthy wars), we have enough evidencethat there are forces which more frequentlymotivate them to move their interactions

    below the upper critical threshold of their

    NRR or even to begin to cooperate after

    having experienced a brief period of intense

    hostility. Some of these forces or conditionsare (a) frustration or attainment of preferredsituations or responses; (b) sharp increasesin actual or perceived costs; or (c) signifi-cant changes in the domestic or international

    conditions (Carroll, 1969; Hermann, 1971;

    Milburn, 1971; OConnor, 1969; and Tima-

    sheff, 1965). These do not exhaust all the

    possibilities and they may differ from one

    conflict situation to another.Although wewill not formally test these explanatory hy-

    potheses, we will explore them in order to

    get behind the signal-exchange data that we

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    7/20

    188

    use to test the first two descriptive hypothe-ses.

    Method

    For the purpose of testing the above

    hypotheses we used 835 signals exchangedbetween Egypt and its opponents (Britain,France, Israel) from July 26, 1956 (the dayNasser nationalized the Suez Canal Com-

    pany), through January 11, 1957 (about the

    time when all occupying forces had either

    withdrawn or were about to withdraw from

    Egyptian territory). These signal events were

    gathered from eleven publicly available and

    reputable sources which comprise part ofthe set of sources we employ at the Conflictand Peace Data Bank (COPDAB) at the

    University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

    (seeAzar, 1971b).

    THE HOSTILITY-FRIENDLINESSSCALE

    At COPDAB we assume that events are

    measurable in terms of some scale and thatevery event contains some degree of &dquo;vio-

    lence,&dquo; &dquo;friendship,&dquo; &dquo;hostility,&dquo; etc. rang-ing from very low to very high. The intensityof the violence contained in each of COP-

    DABs 100,000 events has been established

    by using a 13-point scale which ranges from

    very friendly (point 1 on the scale) to veryhostile (point 13).

    In Table 1 we reproduce the thirteen

    marker-points of our scale, which was de-

    vised by conducting a battery of rankingtests and through the use of the paired

    comparison technique as described by Tor-

    gerson (1958). The reliability and validityof both the coding and scaling procedureswere evaluated and estimated through aseries of several experimental investigationsdescribed inAzar (1970).

    ESTABLISHINGTHE NORMAL RELATIONS

    RANGE

    There are a number of ways to estimate

    the NRR of a pair of nations. The problemstems from the lack of universally acceptablecriteria as to what nations will and will not

    tolerate or expect from one another. In-

    ternational allies for example and nations

    with a common cultural base tend to interact

    cooperatively and therefore establish for

    themselves an NRR towards the more coop-erative side of the scale. On the other hand

    nations which have had incompatible prefer-ences and a history of hostile relations tend

    to establish an NRR towards the more hostile

    end of the scale. Thus signals which would

    do very little to damage the relations between

    TABLE 1

    13 POINT SCALE OF MANIFEST INTER-NATION HOSTILITY-FRIENDLINESS*

    *Region II comprises points 7 to 13 and describes the high violence or more hostile region of the scale. RegionI comprises points I to 6 of the scale and describes the low violence or more friendly region.

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    8/20

    189

    nations with a normally hostile NRR would

    probably precipitate a crisis for the normally

    cooperative nation-states.

    We maintain that there are three ways

    for estimating the threshold of an NRR for

    any pair of nations:

    (1) Empirically-by content analyzingstatements of key decision-makers as they

    express acceptable or tolerable limits within

    which their target nations can behave

    towards them without a need for a reassess-

    ment of the existing relations between them;

    (2) Historically--by inventorying points

    preceding the present inter-nation crisis to

    determine the kind of event-mix which moti-vates each member of the dyad to engagein more conflictual or more friendly acts;and .

    (3) StattsttcaHy―by establishing reason-

    able normal distribution curves and continu-

    ous updating procedures.In this paper we opted for the statistical

    approach and used different time parametersfor

    computingthe NRR than were used to

    test the hypotheses. We computed the rela-

    tive distribution of international signals for

    each of the scale values for the months

    May-September 1956, and on the basis of

    these distributions we established the NRR

    for each dyad by employing an arbitrarycriterion-the scale values which comprisethe middle 85 percent of all the signals

    exchanged between any pair of nations plustwo scale points, one on each side of this

    range.A total of 320 events entered into

    these NRR computations, including 240 of

    the 835 events used for hypothesis testing.We used data from these five months for

    the following reasons:

    (1) From February-April 1956, Egypt and

    Israel were engaged in a very hostile interac-

    tion situation over the fedayeen and other

    outstanding problems, but they had movedback to their pre-February &dquo;normal&dquo; level

    beginning in May of that year. Thus in order

    not to bias the level of their NRR, we decided

    to begin in May and continue through Sep-tember or one month before large scale

    military activity had set in over Suez. We

    felt that the period from May through Sep-tember 1956 tended to portray more ac-

    curately that dyads normal pattern of in-teractions.

    (2) During these five months British-

    Egyptian relations included a variety ofevents ranging from British promises to giveEgypt aid to build theAswan Dam to the

    levelling of charges and countercharges overthe nationalization of the Suez Canal Com-

    pany. French-Egyptian relations vacillated

    between holding meetings to normalize rela-tions and discuss outstanding issues to the

    levelling of charges and countercharges over

    Egyptian support to theAlgerian Liberation

    Movement.

    (3) By computing the relative distribution

    of events at various levels of hostility duringnoncrisis periods, one can (a) reduce the

    chances of contaminating the NRR com-

    putation with data from the crisis period,and (b) allow it to detect a crisis by theshifts of the distribution towards a higher

    percentage of events at the higher levels

    of hostility.

    TESTING THE HYPOTHESES

    The 835 events for the period July 26,

    1956, to January 11, 1957, were coded,

    scaled, and grouped into subsets such as

    &dquo;Egypts signals towards Israel,&dquo; &dquo;Israels

    signals toward Egypt,&dquo; etc. In order to test

    the first hypothesis, concerning the sym-

    metry and asymmetry of signalled hostilities,we decided to separate inter-nation hostile

    signals from nonhostile signals by using the

    scale value of each signal. Signals with

    values 7 to 13 were labeled &dquo;hostile&dquo; and

    grouped in 10-day intervals.We

    thencom-

    puted a percentage score for each interval

    for each dyad based on that dyads total

    number of signals exchanged during the

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    9/20

    190

    entire 170-day period. Furthermore we de-

    cided to graph these results, visually inspectthese time-series graphs, and address our-

    selves to the data. We felt that to compute

    correlations between hostile signals ofA

    towards B and hostile signals of B towardsA would be a correct statistical procedurebut somewhat unsatisfactory for these events

    data. We were very sure that a high degreeof correlation existed because of the nature

    of the time-series events data.

    We grouped these data into 10-day inter-

    vals only as a matter of convenience. We

    wanted a reasonable spread of the data over

    time so we could detect shifts in the hostilesignalling of our actors, and we found that

    10-day intervals produced fewer empty cells

    than one-week intervals did and that they

    produced a longer spread (n = 17) than

    two-week intervals (n = 11). For the pur-

    poses of making inferences no interval

    widths is necessarily superior to the other.

    For testing the second hypothesis con-

    cerning the relationship between level of

    hostility and temporal distance between

    hostile events, we examined the frequenciesof events at each scale value for three

    distinct periods: (a) the &dquo;precrisis&dquo; periodfrom July 26, 1956, through October 29-30,1956-the three month period which pre-ceded the actual military intervention of

    Israel, Britain, and France; (b) the &dquo;acute

    crisis&dquo; period from October 29-30, 1956,

    through November 7, 1956-the approxi-mately one-week period of military interven-

    tion, ending when the British, French andto a large extent the Israelis accepted the

    UN ceasefire resolution; and (c) the periodfrom November 8, 1956, through January11, 1957-the period when conflict reduction

    was achieved.

    There may be those who would find it

    either more convenient or even more appro-

    priate to establish a different set of time

    parameters for the Suez precrisis or crisis

    periods. However the above parameters ap-

    pear very useful and satisfactory, given ournotion of the normal relations range.As

    the following discussion will show, after

    October 29 Israeli-Eqyptian hostile interac-tions had moved above the upper critical

    threshold of their NRR ~i.e_, above point10 of the scale) and had maintained them-

    selves in that region until the first part ofNovember (roughly November 7 or 8, 1956),British-Egyptian and French-Egyptianhostile relations moved above their upper

    critical threshold (above 9) on October 31,

    1956, and continued in that region until the

    end of the first week of November. While

    it is true that some subsequent interactions,such as Egyptian signals towards Britain and

    France, moved above the upper critical

    threshold, these were only sporadic signalsand did not elicit hostile reactions from either

    Britain or France.

    Finally for exploring hypotheses concern-

    ing the determinants of conflict reduction,we examined the events in relation to other

    evidence concerning the preferences, per-ceptions, and changing environments of the

    foreign policy decision-makers in these four

    countries.

    Results

    Table 2 presents the distribution of events

    exchanged between Egypt and its three

    opponents from May through September

    1956. As these results indicate, over 90percent of the Egyptian-British and Egyp-tian-French interactions during that periodranged between scale values 6 and 8 and

    86 percent of the Egyptian-Israeli interac-

    tions ranged between 6 and 9. Therefore

    both the British-Egyptian and French-Egyp-tian NRRs were between 5 and 9 on the

    13-point scale, and the Israeli-Egyptian NRR

    was from 5 to 10. Thus when French and

    British interactions with Egypt moved above

    9 and when Israeli-Egyptian interactions

    moved above 10, these countries had shifted

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    10/20

    191

    TABLE 2

    PERCENT HOSTILE SIGNALSAT EACH SCALE VALUE FOR THE PERIOD MAY-SEPfEMBER 1956*

    *Percentage figures are rounded off.-

    into a crisis situation. On the other hand

    when the Egyptian-British and French-

    Egyptian interactions moved to below 9 and

    when the Israeli-Egyptian interactions

    moved below 10, then for that time periodconflict reduction had set in.

    The first hypothesis states that hostile

    interaction during escalation will exhibit a

    symmetrical or tit-for-tat pattern, but that

    during deescalation it will exhibit a slightly

    asymmetrical pattern. The relevant data are

    shown in Table 3, which contains a summaryof the distribution of the hostile scores for

    each 10-day interval for each dyad. Thesedata are represented graphically in Figures1-3.

    The Suez conflict appears to be a very

    good case for the symmetrical exchange of

    TABLE 3

    HOSTILE SIGNALS OF NATION DYADS PER 10-DAY INTERVALSASA PERCENT OF TOTAL SIGNALSOF DYADS FROM JULY 26, 1956, THROUGH JANUARY 11, 1957*

    *Percent of hostile signals = total hostile signals for the dyad per 10-day interval divided by total signals forthe dyad in 170 days multiplied by 100.

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    11/20

    192

    FIG. 1. Hostile signals per 10-day period between Britain and Egypt as a percent of their total

    signals from July 26, 1956, through January 11, 1957.

    hostile signals hypotheses. Our data show

    that from July 26 through November 7, 1956(i.e., the escalation period), Egyptian-Brit-ish, Egyptian-French, and Egyptian-Israelibehavior tended to exhibit a tit-fot-tat pat-tern. The data show that after a series of

    verbal threats, accusations, and counterac-

    cusations, and at times small border inci-

    dents, Israeli-Egyptian interactions moved

    above the upper critical threshold of their

    NRR on October 29, 1956. This was followed

    by British-Egyptian and French-Egyptianinteractions on October 31, 1956. Thus bythe end of October the four nations were

    in a crisis situation with plans to increase

    the scope and intensity of their hostilesignals. The data show that up to November

    7, 1956, all three dyads behaved similarly,

    giving hostile responses for hostile stimuli.

    After November 8, 1956 (the approximatedate of the parties acceptance of the UN

    ceasefire resolution), British-Egyptian sig-nals maintained the same pattern exhibited

    during escalation, but the French-Egyptianand Israeli-Egyptian interactions exhibited

    a slightly dissimilar stimulus-response pat-tern. For example during the first days of

    the conflict Egypt reduced its physical hos-

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    12/20

    193

    FIG. 2. Hostile signals per 10-day period between France and Egypt as a percent of their total

    signals from July 26, 1956, through January 11, 1957.

    tile signals towards Israel by withdrawingits forces from Sinai in the face of the

    better trained and tougher Israeli armed

    forces, but Egypt continued its verbal attack

    against Israel and encouraged some unsuc-

    cessful fedayeen activities against Israel from

    the surroundingArab areas of Jordan, Syriaand Lebanon. Also Egypt continued its

    defensive actions against Britain and France,who by the first week of November had

    destroyedone

    major portion of Egypts smallair force and occupied strategic positionsaround the Suez canal area.After November

    7 both Britain and France stopped their

    military strikes against Egypt and announced

    plans to withdraw their armed forces fromthe Suez area. France was not as recep-

    tive to the idea of withdrawal before

    very clear conditions regarding navigation

    rights and compensations to Canal Companyshareholders were publicly accepted byNasser. Furthermore both Israel and France

    had other goals beyond the Suez questionto which they wanted Nasser to agree before

    bringing the hostilities toa

    full end. Francefor example wanted Nasser to stop both

    political and military aid to theAlgerians.Israel wanted Egypt to stop encouraging the

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    13/20

    194

    FIG. 3. Hostile signals per 10-day period between Israel and Egypt as a percent of their total

    signals from July 26, 1956, through January 11, 1957.

    fedayeen raids against Israel, to allow Israelishipping through Suez andAqaba, and to

    stop its anti-Israeli activities in the Middle

    East and around the world. Obviously some

    of these demands were met for the moment

    and others were denied and even com-

    pounded. Thus Israeli hostile interactionswith Egypt continued for a few days after

    British and French hostilities had stopped.In fact Israeli armed forces were the last

    forces to withdraw from Egyptian territory.We observed from the data that the pattern

    of hostile signalling was clearly symmetrical

    during the escalation period (July 26-November 7), but not during the conflict

    reduction period. We found that after Egyp-tian hostile signals towards Israel had

    dropped below the upper critical thresholdof their NRR, Israel continued its hostile

    actions against Egypt for more than three

    weeks. We also found that after France

    dropped its hostile actions against Egypt,the latter did not lower its anti-French

    hostilities in response to French signals.Instead Egypt continued a fairly high level

    of anti-French signalling. In both of these

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    14/20

    195

    cases there was a lag between offers of

    conflict reduction and responses to these

    offers.

    The second hypothesis suggests that when

    nations are in a crisis situation,they

    will

    make many more hostile acts per unit of

    time, and conversely when they begin to

    reduce the hostile content of their signals,

    they tend to increase the time intervals

    between hostile signals. In very generalterms this hypothesis says that a relationshipexists between the hostile content of signalsand the density of these signals per unitof time.

    The data in Table 4 provide a partial testof this hypothesis. The number of eventsin Period C (crisis) divided by the lengthof that period (one week) is clearly much

    higher than the same ratios for Periods P

    (precrisis) and R (reduction). The mean level

    of hostility is also higher in Period C. A

    careful inspection of the data reveals this

    hypothesis would have to be qualified for

    conflict reduction periods.The data show that as these nations cross

    the upper critical threshold of their NRR

    and establish a state of acute crisis between

    them, they begin to behave somewhat dif-

    ferently than the hypothesis suggests. For

    example Britain and Egypt seem to have

    behaved quite similarly in terms of reducingthe temporal distances between their hostile

    events during the intense crisis period and

    increasing them during conflict reduction.

    France and Egypt however behaved dif-

    ferently towards one another.As France

    began to reduce the temporal distance be-

    tween its signals, Egypt began to increasethe distance except toward the latter partof November 1956. On the other hand, from

    mid-September through the end of October1956 as Israeli hostile signals increased and

    the temporal distances between them de-creased, Egyptian hostile signals seem to

    have been reduced and the temporal dis-

    tances between the Egyptian signals in-

    creased. During the conflict reduction periodEgyptian-Israeli relations exhibited a verysimilar pattern to that of the period between

    July 26, 1956, and September 15, 1956. Inother words hostile charges and counter-

    charges were used as a bargaining tacticand a lag of two to five days tended to

    characterize the distance between an Israeli

    hostile stimulus and Egyptian hostile

    response. The following discussion is an

    exploration of the reasons for the differen-

    tiated behavior of these four nations.

    Why did the four participants in the Suez

    crisis reduce their hostilities? We have sug-

    gested earlier that nations who are involvedin a crisis situation will probably reduce

    their hostilities and move to their precrisisNRR if they achieve or frustrate their goals,if they incur heavy costs, or if they feel

    pressured by changes in the domestic and

    international conditions. The 1956 Suez crisis

    appears to be a useful case for exploringthese possibilities.

    An investigation ofa

    number of relevantsources has yielded the following rank-or-

    dering of major Israeli strategic preferences(seeAzar, 1970):

    (1) Protection of the territorial integrityand sovereignty of the Israeli nation-state,

    specifically protection of Israeli citizens

    against fedayeen attacks;

    (2) Need to strengthen Israels military

    capability and particularly her air force;

    (3) Need to end Egyptian economic boy-cott of Israel-specifically Egypts closingof the Gulf ofAqaba to hamper Israeli

    shipping;(4)Avoidance of military confrontation

    with Israeli neighbors and in particular to

    make Egypt renounce her objective of re-

    ducing Israel to impotence by force; and

    (5) Reduction of Nassers increasing pres-

    tige in the region and the world, which wouldtend to reduce the level of threat againstIsrael, whether from Egypt or the otherArabstates.

    at RYERSON UNIV on July 4, 2013jcr.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/http://jcr.sagepub.com/
  • 7/22/2019 Journal of Conflict Resolution 1972 Azar 183 201

    15/20

    196

    Aolix