Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    1/21

    Linguistics - Theory - Radford

    Chapter. 1:

    Grammar\s subdivided into:

    Morphology: the study of how words are formed out of smaller units (morphemes) and the interpretation of those words.

    Syntax: is concerned with the ways in which words can be combined together to form phrases and sentences, and the

    interpretation of those phrases and sentences.

    Grammaris the study of the principles which govern the formation and interpretation of words, phrases and sentences.

    Competence you have grammatical competence when you have tacit (subconscious) knowledge of the grammar of your

    language.

    Performance the actual use of language in concrete situations (what you actually say or understand) -misproductions and

    misinterpretations are performance errors.

    Grammar is concerned with competence rather than performance (cognitive view: language = cognitive system)

    I-language: the internalized -within the human brain mind- linguistics system which enables humans to speak and understand

    their native language.

    Traditional grammars concentrate on grammaticality (how to form grammatical phrases and sentences). Chomsky focuses

    more on ungrammaticality (why certain types of structures are ungrammatical).

    The native speaker's grammatical competence is reflected not only n intuitions about grammaticality but also in

    intuitionsabout interpretations.

    Example: Sam loves you more than Jim (ambiguous)

    Criteria; of adequacy: descriptive linguists invent/plan grammars of particular languages.

    Descriptive adequacy: a grammar of a given language is descriptively adequate if it correctly describes whether any given

    string (sequence) of words n a language is or isn't grammatical, and also correctly describes what interpretation(s) the

    relevant string has.

    Theoretical linguists (Chomsky) invent/plan a theory of grammar: a set of hypothesis about the nature of possible and

    impossible grammars of natural (human) languages.

    A theory of grammar must satisfy the following criteria:

    1.Universality: it should provide us with the tools needed to describe the grammar of any natural language

    adequately. The ultimate goal of a theory of grammar is to develop a theory of universal grammar: (UG)

    2.Explanatory adequacy: it should explain why grammars have the properties they do.

    3.Restrictive: it should provide us with technical devices which are so constrained in their expressive power that

    they can only be used to describe natural languages.

    4.Learnability: it should provide grammars which are learnable by young children in a relatively short period of time.

    5.Min imalism: it should be as simple as possible.

    Innateness hypotheses: Chomsky says that the uniformity and rapidity of first language acquisition is to suggest that the

    course of acquisition is determined by a biologically endowed innate language faculty (language acquisition program) - within

    the brain, which provides children with a (genetically transmitted) algorithm (set of procedures) for developing a grammar,

    on the basis of their linguistics experience (= speech input they receive). Evidence in support of the innateness

    hypothesis (genetic guidance):

    Species-specific: only human beings acquire a human language. Its independent of human's general intelligence (everybody

    learns to speak).

    Uniformity: it suggests that children have genetic guidance in the task of constructing a grammar of their native language;

    different speakers of the same language exposed to different input develop similar grammars. Rapidity: human beings

    learn in a relatively short period of time, once the grammar spurt has started.

    The child's linguistic experience is often degenerate (imperfect) since it is based on the linguistic performance of adult

    speakers.

    Language acquisition is an entirely subconscious and involuntary activity, and also unguided.

    Children go through three basic stages during the process of language acquisition:

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    2/21

    Single-word stage (12-18 months): children's utterances comprise single words spoken in isolation. There's no evidence

    of

    the acquisition of grammar in that children don't make productive use of inflections.

    Productive use of inflections (around 18 months): children make productive use of inflections (.e, plurals) and start

    producing elementary two and three word utterances. Here we find the first signs of acquisitions of grammar.

    Steady stage (around 30 months): children have typically acquired most of the inflections and core grammatical

    constructions used in English. They're able to produce adult-like sentences, (core grammar: all the parameters are fixed).

    Creativity: Language Acquisition is not a purely imitative process, but rather an inherently creative process. We are

    capable of producing and understanding not only sentences we have previously heard but also novel sentences which we have

    never encountered before. Acquiring a grammar involves formulating a set of generalizations about how inflections are

    used and how words are combined together to form phrases and sentences.

    (UG is Universal grammar and it is made up of a set of principles and unfixed parameters that allow the child to develop a

    grammar of any natural language.

    Experience of language > (UG (principles and parameters) > grammar of language.

    Universal principies

    1.Structure-dependence: all grammatical operations are determined by the structure of the language (inversion,

    wh-movement).

    2.Endocentricity: all phrases have a head.3.Extended projection: all sentences have a subject or all verbs take an external argument.

    4.Embedding principle: all sentences can be embedded into another sentence.

    Parameters: aspects of grammatical structure that are subject to specific language variations. They are always binary.

    1.Null-subject parameter: does/doesn't allow finite verbs to have nuil subjects. English is a non-null subject

    language.

    2.Wh-movement parameter: does/doesn't allow wh-movement. English does.

    3.Head position parameter: relative positioning of heads with respect to their complements (head-first or head-

    last). English is a head-first language,

    Parameter setting

    Each of these parameters is inherently binary and the structural learning which the child faces involves parameter setting,

    .e. determining which of the two alternative settings provided by U

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    3/21

    b- Derivation rneans that a word derives from another word (lucky-luckily). By adding the suffix LY, we change the

    category of the word (adj-adv).

    Particular derivational affixes can only be attached to words belonging to a particular category. For example, the negative

    prefixes UN- and IN- can be attached to adjectives to form a negative adj. (happy-unhappy)

    2) Syntactic evidence:

    a- Different categories of words have different distribut i ons (=different positions in a sentence/phrase).

    Examples: No + noun

    Can +verb

    Adjectives modify nouns

    Etc.

    b- Substitution

    This is a Syntactic test to determine the category of a word. You see whether the word in question can be substituted by a

    regular noun, verb, prep., adj. or adv. Example:

    "He is better at French than you"

    (more fluent) adj.

    "He speaks French better than you"(more fluently) adv.

    3) Functional categories and le xical categories

    Functional Categories = Lexical Categories

    words that carry info about number, gender, person, case, etc (test: antonym - contentive word)

    They have a functional content. content words, they carry meaning, they

    are descriptive

    Determiners -only one before a

    noun-restricted to modifying nouns (a +

    singular)

    -inflection case

    Auxiliaries -can undergo inversion

    -can be negated by a following NOT

    -can appear in sentence-final tags

    Infinitive

    particle TO-dummy (=meaningless)

    -can't be intensified by

    RI6HT/STRAI6HT -requires a

    verbal complement

    -+ infinitive

    -permits ellipsis of its complement

    (ex. aux. should)

    NOTE-1 Chomsky says that the INF.

    TO belongs to the same category of

    modals. He labelled this category

    INFLECTION (I/INFL)

    Pronouns

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    4/21

    Adjectives -more than one

    -not restricted

    Nouns -do not inflect for case

    Verbs -require DO support

    -require indirect

    negation through the use ofDO support

    -require DO tags

    Preposition

    TO -contentive

    -can be modified

    by intensifiers

    -takes a noun

    expression as its

    complement

    -+ gerund

    -doesn't

    Complementizers -FOR can't be intensified by

    (fhat-if-for) straight/right.

    -a clause introduced by FOR can

    function as a subject. -+ to infinit ive

    -IF can only introduce a

    finite clause.

    -can't introduce a clause

    which s used as the complement

    of a preposition,

    -THAT can't be substituted

    -reduced vowel

    Preposition FOR-can

    -can't be subject -+ ing

    Int. Adverbs HOW

    WHEN/WHETHER

    -can beDeterminer THAT

    substituted by another

    det. -unreduced vowel -can

    be used pronominally

    4) Parsing

    To parse is to analyze the grammatical structure of phrases and sentences. The first step is to provide the category of

    each of the words in the expression (labelled bracketing).

    5) Subcategorial features

    They are those grammatical properties which are associated with a subset of the members of a given category.

    a- There, are two different subclasses of noun:

    Common; Nouns Proper Nouns

    -can freely be modified by det. like THE.-typically denote names of people, places,

    dates, rnagazines.

    -the first letter is generally capitalized

    -have unique reference (semantic

    viewpoint)

    -can't be modified by a det.

    Both can be described in terms of a binary grammatical feature such as [ common] [ count]

    [ plural].

    b- Determiners [ Q] Referential determiners

    -Quantifying determiners - this/that/these/those - [ Q]

    many/much - [ Q]

    c- Adverbs

    They are a subclass of adjectives- quickly-[ ADVJ

    d- Verbs

    [ aspect/participle]

    [ past]

    [ AGR/3SG]

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    5/21

    example: SOES [+V, -PART, -PAST, +ASR]

    5) Cross-categorial features

    Many gramrnatical properties are cross-categorial, because they extend across more than one category,

    For example, verbs and adjectives share the morphological property that they alone permit direct UN* prefixation. (undo /

    unafraid)

    Nouns, adjectives, verbs and prepositions can be analysed as composites of just two binary grammatical features - nominal [N] and verbal [ V] - they cross classify categories

    Verb |W, -N]

    Noun [-V, +N]

    Adj. [+V, +N]

    Prep. [-V, -N]

    Supercateqory: f or example verbs and adjectives form a supercategory because they share the feature [+V]

    Auxiliarles and verbs share the core features [+V, -N], but differ in respect of some other feature. Aux. are functors and

    verbs are not.

    Aux. [+V, -N, +F]

    Vb. [+V, -N, -F] (F= functionality)

    Each contentive category has a functional counterpart:

    Lexical Categories Functional Categories

    -N- [+N,-V,-F] -PRN- [+N, -V, +F]

    -A - [+N, +V, -F] -D - [+N, +V, +F]

    -V- [-N,+V,-F] -AUX. [-N, +V, +F]

    -P- [-N, -V,-F] -C/I- [-N,-V,+F]

    Within a feature-based theory, a grammatical category is a set of elements (group of words) which nave the same value(s)

    [+ or -] for a given set of grammatical features [N, V, F]

    Chopter 3:

    Tree diagram or phrase marker: visual representation of the structure of phrases. The overall sentence is built up out of

    constituents (i.e. syntactic units or structural building blocks) of various types. Each of the category labels in a tree

    diagram is a different node (constituent) and the words are the leaves on the tree. Nodes at the bottom of the tree are

    called terminal nodes and other nodes are nonterminal nodes. The topmost node is the root.

    Forminq phrases and sentences:

    Phrases and sentences are formed by a process called merger,

    Merger: A process where two categories are joined together to form a new 'phrasal category'.

    The phrase which s formed is endocentric, because it's a projection of a head word. English s a head - f irst language

    because it positions heads before complements, but also a specifier - f irst language because it positions specifiers before

    heads.

    The result of each merger is a projection (phrasal expansion). The category label x-bar is used to designate an

    intermediate x projection, i.e. to designate an x expression which is larger than x. And the category label XP denotes an

    x

    expression which is larger than x, but which doesnt project into an even larger type of x expression. This category s

    called

    maximal projection. (being xa noun, verb, preposition, etc).

    > Sentences can be eitherCPor AgrS. (They are both maximal projection)

    The merger operation which forms projection combines two categories together in a pair wise fashion, according to the

    constituent structure principle: binarity principle

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    6/21

    Binarity principle: All non-terminal nodes are binary branching.

    Configurational relations:

    > Any given pair of nodes contained in the same p-marker will be related by one of two different types of relation;dominance (i.e. hierarchical/top-to-bottom ordering)and precedente (i.e. linear/ left-to-right ordering).

    Confiqurational definition of the term constituent: " A set of words forms a constituent of category Z if the

    terminal nodes carrying them are all dominated by the same Z node, and if there are no other terminal nodes

    dominated by the relevant Z node."

    Complementhood: A constituent X is the complement of a head H (and, by extension, of any H -bar or HP

    constituent which is a projection of H) if X and H are sisters, and the mother of X s a projection of H.

    Specifierhood: A constituent Y is the specifier of a head H (and, by extension, of any H -bar or HP

    constituent

    into which H projects) if Y is the sister of H-bar and the daughter of HP.

    Floating quantif i er: a quantifier which does not immediately precede the expression which it

    quantifies.

    Constituent testing structure:

    See photocopy

    C-command:

    > C-command is a structural relation between two categories. A constituent X c-commands a

    constituent Y, f the mother of X dominates Y, and X and Y, are disconnected.

    C-command condition on polarity expressions: A polarity expression must be c-commanded by an affective

    constituent.

    < Polarity expression: A word or phrase which has an inherent affective polarity, and is restricted to occurringwithin the

    scope of an affective constituent. (Polarity items: Any, ever, care a damn, lift a finger. Affective constituents:

    neq. words:

    no-nobody-not; interrogatives: if, whether and interrogatives in general; rf in conditionals and too as an

    adverb ).

    eg. Nobody will say anything.

    __________________________________________________________________________________________________C-command condition on binding: A bound constituent must be c-commanded by an appropriate antecedent.

    Anaphors (reflexives and reciprocals) fulfill this condition because they have the property that they can't be used

    to refer directly to an entity in the outside world, so must be bound by an antecedent elsewhere in the same

    phrase or sentence.

    All constituents in a given structure are overt. However, syntactic structures may also contain empty

    categories: (=covert=null) categories which have no overt phonetic form, and hence which are inaudible or

    silent.

    PRO Subjects

    English is not a null subject language although it allows infinitives to have null subjects. Subjectless infinitive

    clauses contain an understood null subject called PRO. It has the same grammatical properties as pronouns,

    that's why it's called

    PRO.

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    7/21

    In a sentence such as: "We would like PRO to stay."

    The null subject PRO is controlled by (refers back to) the subject we of the matrix (=containing=next

    highest) clause or equivalently, we is the controller or antecedent of PRO. Verbs (such as like) which allow

    an infinitive complement with a PRO subject are said to f unction as control predicates.

    Semantic Evidence of the existence of PRO.

    In traditional grammar it s claimed that Subjectless infinitive clauses have an understood or implicit subject-

    and positing a PRO subject in such clauses is one way of capturing the relevant intuition. The implicit subject

    becomes explicit if the relevant clauses are paraphrased by a finite clause:

    1)a- I am sorry [to have kept

    you waiting]

    b- I am sorry [I have kept

    you waiting]

    2)a- It is important [to carry your passport with you]

    b- It is important [you should carry your passport with you]

    3) a- Dumbo has promised [to come to my party]

    b- Dumbo has promised [he will come

    to my party] (covert PRO subject)

    a) from the syntax of reflexive anaphors (.e. +self/selves forms such as myself/yourself , etc.) As examples such as the

    following indicate, reflexives generally require a local antecedent.

    They want [John to help himself]

    *They want [John to help themselves]

    local antecedent means "an antecedent contained within the same bracketed clause as the reflexive.

    In a sentence such as: "John wants [PRO to prove himself]"

    ("himself" is co-referential to John) Reflexives require a local antecedent within its own bracketed clause, and this

    requirement is satisfied in the sentence above if we assume that the bracketed complement clause has a PRO subject, and

    that PRO is the antecedent of himself. Since PRO n turn is controlled by John (i.e. John is the antecedent of PRO), this

    means that himself'is correferential to (i.e. refers to the same person as) John.

    b) In relation to the syntax of predicate nominals (noun containing), nominal expressions used as the complement of a

    copular (i .e. linking) verb such as be, become, remain, etc.

    "John was my best friend."

    The property of " being my best friend''is predicated ofJohn.

    Predicate nominals in copular constructions have to agree with the subject of their own clause in number,

    They want their son [to become a millionaire/*millionaires] => clause- internal agreement requirement

    Emp i rical ev i dence

    => one's own - the possessive one's requires a local antecedent.

    => lose one's cool - the possessorone's must agree with the subject of lose.

    => each otherand togetherrequire a local antecedent.

    Radford mentions three kinds of PRO:

    a) Controlled PRO:

    PRO s controlledby the subject of the matrix clause. It refers back to the subject, which is the controlleror

    antecedent ofPRO. The controller is in the same clause.

    Verbs (such as like, want, etc) which allow an infinitive complement with a PRO subject are said to function as control

    predicates.Example:

    "John wants [PRO to use his own car]"

    b) Uncontrolled PRO:

    PRO has no antecedent within the sentence containing it; instead, the reference of PRO is discourse-determined, i.e.

    PRO denotes some specific individual or set of individuals who is talking, being talked to or being talked about in the

    relevant discourse.

    Example:

    "[PRO kill himself]?! He hasn't got the guts!"

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    8/21

    c) Arbitrary PRO:

    PRO is not controlled within the sentence, it denotes an arbitrary individual and thus has an arbitrary reference.

    Example:

    "It's wrong [PRO to blame oneself for other people's misfortune]"

    Ellipsis: A process by which a constituent is given a null phonetic form, but retains its grammatical and semanticproperties.

    In the sentence: "He could have left, and [she e have stayed.], we can see:

    1-This case is interpreted as an elliptical form of "She could have stayed." (e is an elliptical form of could)

    2-The subject is in nominative case form: "She"

    3-The perfective auxiliary have is in the uninflected infinitive form: this is because e (being an elliptical form of could)

    has the same grammatical properties as could, so that e (like could) requires a complement headed by a word (like have)

    in the infinitive form.

    Cliticization: the process by which one word attaches itself to another serves to support the existence of a null

    auxiliary. In the sentence analyzed above, the existence of a null auxiliary (elliptical form of could in this case) blocks the

    cliticization of "have" onto "she".

    Cliticization is only possible when a clitic (n't, 's, 've, 're, 'd) immediately follows the expression to which it cliticizes, i.e.

    issubject to adjacency condition, and is blocked by the presence of an intervening constituent.

    Example:

    *I wouldn't let [you've done it]

    I wouldn't let [you o have done it]

    The first example is wrong because, as we can see in the second one, there is a null to, so the clitic does not immediately

    follow the expression to which it cliticizes but, on the contrary, it is blocked by the presence ofto.

    An auxiliary occupies the head I position of IP. Finite verbs occupy the head V of a VP complement. When

    INFL is unfilled, the tense and the agreement properties of the head V of VP percolate up to INFL.

    Have can occupy two positions in the sentence.

    a)the head I position of a VP (auxiliary)

    b)the head V position of a VP complement of an unfilled INFL.

    Sentences containing (a finite form) perfective have are tagged by have, whereas sentences containing ( a finite form of )

    causative have are tagged by do.

    a) [IP She I has VP gone to Paris, hasn't she?]

    The head I position of IP is filled by the perfective auxiliary has, and so the tag contains a copy of this auxiliary.

    b) [IP She I VP has her hair styled to Paris, doesn't she?

    The head I position of IP is unfilled and hence contains no meaning-bearing constituent), and so can only be tagged by the

    dummy auxiliary does (which carries the same present-tense feature as the unfilled I constituent in the main clause.)

    Arguments: expressions which typically denote the participants in the activity or event described by a verb.

    Adjuncts: expressions providing additional information about the relevant activity/event. e.g. its location, the time at whichit took place, the manner in which it took place.

    An IP-adverb is an adverb which is positioned internally within IP (e.g.

    certainly) A VP-adverb is an adverb positioned internally within VP (e.g.

    completely)

    In the sentence: "They certainly have both completely ignored her."

    The VP-adverb completely attaches to the V-barignored her(V-bar adjunct). The IP-adverb certainly expands the I-bar

    have both completely ignored her into the extended I-barcertainly have both completely ignored her(I-bar adjunct);

    adverbial adjuncts, first merge with intermediate projections like V-bar and I-bar); and secondly they serve to expand a

    category of a given type into an extended V-bar.

    If we analyze:

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    9/21

    A: What did they do?

    B: * certainly ignored her

    .completely ignored her

    The first string is wrong because certain/yis an IP-adverb (and adverb which is positioned internally within IP), so it

    modifies the whole IP, not just the VP. Completelyis a VP-adverb, so the Second string is correct, because the VP-adverb

    is positioned internally within VP.

    Verbs like know/hear/let/watch/see select an IP complement headed by the null infinitive particle O, whereas verbs like

    expect/judge/report/consider/want;etc. select an IP complement headed by to.Example:

    I heard [him O speak]

    I expect [him to speak]

    Cliticization is disallowed in " I wouldn't let [you have done it]" because the bracketed infinitive complement is an IP headed

    by the null infinitive particle O between 'you' and "have" which blocks cliticization.

    Economy Principle: "derivations and representations are required to be minimal [as economical/short as possible] with

    no superfluous steps in derivations and no superfluous symbols in representations".

    An example for this can be the null complementizers: Null complementizers don't really exist at least in this framework.

    Evidence:

    No-contentless-projections constraint. (CP is a contentless projection)

    Coordination: complementizerless clauses cannot be coordinated with clauses containing an overt complementizer such

    as that.

    *John said [Peter left] and [that Bill kissed Mary]

    Preposing: we can prepose the subject of a clause introduced by a complementizer like that.

    5he thinks [that money can buy happiness]

    *Money she thinks [that can buy happiness]

    Cliticization: we should expect the intervening null-complementizer O to block cliticization. The fact that cliticization is

    possible suggests that the CPanalysis can't be right and that the more economical IP analysis is more plausible.

    Who do you think's helping him?

    If we analyse:

    *John said that Peter left and Bill helped Mary.

    It proves that null complementizers don't exist. This sentence is ungrammatical because we have coordinated a CPwith

    an

    IP violating the condition that only alike constituents can be conjoined. If the null complementizer really existed as a

    category, the sentence would be grammatical.

    If we analyse:

    Tom she believes Stole the money Vs *Tom she believes that stole the money

    The second sentence is ungrammatical because the subject cannot be extracted out ofCP.

    If we cnalyse:

    "Students always enjoy this class."

    We can say that all nominals are projections of a head Determiner. This implies that the bare noun Students is not simply

    an

    N, but rather a DP, headed by a null determiner.

    Semantic properties of the nuil determiner:

    The noun sugar in "Sugar is sweet" has a genericinterpretation, and hence is interpreted as meaning "sugar in general". In "I

    like sugar", the noun sugarhas an existential (= partitive) interpretation that can be paraphrased as "some sugar" ("I like

    some sugar in my coffee").

    Grammatical properties (i.e. features) of the null determiner:

    "LINGUISTS often contradict themselves,"

    The null determiner in this sentence carries quantificational, person and complement-selection properties.

    a)Person properties: the bare nominal linguists can only bind a third person reflexive like themselves and can only be

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    10/21

    tagged by a third person pronoun like they(don't they?). So, it is a third person determiner.

    b)Complement-selection properties: the null determiner can only be used to quantify a singular mass noun or a plural count

    noun as in the example above.

    In expressions such as "We syntacticians", pronouns have the categorial status of determiners.

    Pronouns are determiners used without a complement. Nominals are projections of a head determiner constituent, they

    can

    be used prenominally as in "We syntacticians" or pronominally as in "We don't....".

    Non argument nominals are N-projections.(nominals in vocative, predicative or exclamative

    use.) Ex: Do you see it, doctor? (vocative)

    Dick is head of department. (predicative position)

    Poor fool! (exclamative)

    Pronominal attributive adjectives don't overtly inflect for number but there are exceptions like the adjectives numerous

    and various, which carry an intrinsic plural feature by virtue of their meaning.

    They serve as the specifier of an empty functional category F which is positioned between D

    and N.

    Chapter 5:

    Grammar is a generative system which produces sentences. The derivation of a sentence (i.e. the series of operations by

    which it is formed) involves a set of linguistic operations which generate (i.e. produce) syntactic structures.

    These linguistic operations are: selection, merger and spell out. By an operation of selection, an individual selects a lexical

    item from a lexicon, The numeration is the set of lexical Items selected from the lexicon. Then by the process of merger,

    constituents are combined together in a pairwise fashion to form a phrase Structure tree. (with each word in the tree

    comprising a set of phonetic, semantic and grammatical features.)

    Spell out is the point at which the phrase structures generated by the process of selection and mergerfeed into two

    different components (interface levels) - a PF component which processes their phonetic features and an LF component

    which processes their grammatical and semantic features ( linguistics aspects of the meaning of the sentence).

    They are called interface levels because they are the levels at which the grammar interfaces (i.e. connects) with otherSystems which lie outside the domain of the theory of grammar.

    The PF serves as input to the articulatory-perceptual system and LF serves as input to the conceptual-intentional system.

    The phonetic, grammatical and semantic properties of words can be described in terms of sets of features.

    Principle of full interpretation: requires that PF representations should contain only phonetic features, and that LF

    representations should only contain semantic features.

    A derivation which satisfies this requirement converges and one which does not crashes.

    Some grammatical features(e.g. the person/number/gender-features of pronouns like she) have semantic content and so are

    interpretable (at LF), whereas others (e.g. the case-features of pronouns and the inflectional features of non-finite verbs)

    are uninterpretable and so must be erased in the course of the derivation.

    Words carry three sets of grammatical features: head-features (which determine their intrinsic grammatical properties),

    Specifier-feafures (which determine the kinds of complement they can take) and complement-features(^N\\\c\\ determine

    the kinds of complement they take).

    Specifier - and complement-features are uninterpretable and so must be erased by a process of checking. Only

    interpretable features survive at LF after checking.

    Checking domains: head-specifier ; Specifier head.

    In structures containing a phrasal Specifier or complement, projections carry the same head-features as their heads.

    The specifier-features of as finite nonauxiliary verb percolate from V to INFL, so that the head features of the subject

    can be checked against the specifier-features of INFL (inherited from the nonauxiliary verb). This percolation also takes

    place n order to ensure that INFL has tense properties and so is interpretable at LF.

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    11/21

    Determiners like the (and the null quantifier o) carry fixed person properties (in that they are third person determiners),

    but variable number and case properties, so that the is third person nominative plural in: "The students are revolting.", but

    third person objective singular in: "Sack the Dean!".

    One way of ensuring that determiners and attributive adjectives agree in number with the nouns they modify would be to

    suppose that the number-features carried by the noun percolates up to the head F constituent of the PF containing the

    adjective, and from there to the head D constituent of DP.

    PRO carries null case, and its case is checked by infinitival to (so that PRO carries a null-case head-feature, and infinitive

    to carries a null-case specifier-feature).

    The infinitival IP in ECAA structures, is headed by a caseless infinitive particle, and the objective case carried by the

    infinitive subject is checked by an immediately preceding transitive matrix verb or complementizer.

    The category labels carried by terminal nodes (i.e. heads) in tree diagrams can be eliminated if the relevant categorial

    properties are described in terms of head-features. The same happens with nonterminal nodes labels.

    Chapter 6:

    Head movement involves movement from one head position to another. If s also called head-to-head movement:

    Auxiliary inversin in questions in English involves an I movement operation whereby an auxiliary moves from INFL toComp...(text missing)

    serves to fill COMP. If there isnt any auxiliary we have to use the do-support. It originates in INFL because it requires a

    VP complement and then it raises to COMP

    An inverted auxiliary leaves behind a trace (i.e. a silent copy of itself) in the INFL position out of which it moves when it

    moves to COMP. The trace has the same grammatical features as the auxiliary. The moved constituent is the antecedent

    ofits trace and it binds the trace (i.e. determines its properties). It c-commands the empty trace. The moved auxiliary and

    its empty trace form a chain. The antecedent is the head ofthe chain and the trace is the foot

    Finite verbs in Early Modem English (EME) could move from V to INFL by an operation of V movement (as is shown by word-

    order facts n negative sentences like I care not for her), but this kind of movement is generally no longer possible n

    Modem Standard English (MSE).

    I movement and V movement are two different reflexes of a more general head movement operation, and head movement s

    subject to the head movement constraint which requires it to apply in a successive cyclic fashion.

    Verbs in EME had strong agreement-features (by virtue of the relatively rich agreement inflections they carried) and

    consequently allowed a null pro subject, whereas their counterparts in MSE have weak agreement-features (by virtue of

    their impoverished agreement morphology) and so do not license a pro subject.

    The strong agreement features of finite verbs in EME were checked by movement of the verb (along with its features)

    from V to INFL (this movement is motivated by considerations of greed, i.e. by the selfish desire of the verb to check its

    own morphological features), whereas the weak agreement features of finite verbs in MSE are checked by attraction (i.e.

    percolation) of the relevant agreement features from V to INFL (with the verb, itself remaining in situin the head V

    position of VP).

    Movement and attraction are two different reflexes of a common feature movement operation which moves grammatical

    features from one constituent to another, and which in the case of movement also has the effect ofpied-pipingthe

    phonetic features of the head along with its grammatical features.

    Syntax of negation:

    In EME the negative adverb not was positioned between the auxiliary and the verb in clauses containing a finite auxiliary:

    You may not deny it.

    In auxiliariless finite clauses, the main verb was positioned in front of the negative not. He heard not that.

    In MSE the negative particle notfunctions as a syntactic head NEG(ation) which projects into a NEGP (negation phrase)

    constituent.

    Not can attract the head-features of (the head verb of) its complement, but not the specifier-features of a finite verb,

    because it is an A-bar head, i.e. a nonargumental head which can't have an argument as its specifier and hence can't

    carry

    subject features. That's why the sentence She not trusts him is ungrammatical, because the [3SNom] specifier-features of

    trusts can't be checked, and so the derivation crashes at LF. (page 232)

    Nt differs from not in that not is a free form whereas n't is a bound form which can only attach to other bound

    forms.

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    12/21

    Possessive have in some varieties of English (like Baa Baa. Black Sheep) can behave like an auxiliary. It originates in the

    head V position of VP and from there can raise to INFL. It is the last vestige of V movement.

    Possessive have can be directly negated by ntbut not by not. This is because have can serve as a bound form and hence can

    adjoin to the bound negative n t (but not to the free form nof). More generally, adjunction can only adjoin one bound form

    to another, and cannot target a free form like not.

    All head movement may involve adjunction to an affix, so that V-to-I movement involves adjunction of V to an abstract

    (present or past) tense affix T: one consequence of this is that clauses are projections of a tense affix T, an hence have

    the status of TP (= tense phrase) constituents. T is a weakaffix in MSE, but was a strong affixin EME. Weak affixes havethe property that they check grammatical features, whereas strong affixes must be attached to a set of phonetic features.

    The requirements of the weak T n MSE are satisfied by merging an auxiliary with T, by attraction of the tense/agreement-

    features of a non auxiliary verb to T or by movement(where possessive have has idiosyncratic pied-piping properties) . By

    contrast, the requirements of the strong T in EME are satisfied either by mergerwith an auxiliary, or by movementof a

    nonauxiliary verb to adjoin to T.

    Questions are CP constituents headed by a C node containing a strong Q affix; the requirement for Q to be attached to a

    set of phonetic features is satisfied in main clauses by adjoining a preposed (auxiliary or nonauxiliary) verb to Q,

    (rememberdo Support. sometimes the only way to f ill COMP is by generating the auxiliary do in INFL and then raising it

    to COMP) and in complement clauses by merging the complementizer if with Q.

    Grammars contain three different types of movement operations:

    -PF movement only phonetic features move (this occurs in the PF component)-Attraction only grammatical features are adjoined to T (this occurs at LF)

    (Syntactic) Movement both phonetic and grammatical features are adjoined to T and to Q (this occurs in the syntactic

    component, prior to spellout)

    Chapter 7:

    Operator movement applies to expressions which contain an operator of some kind (negative or interrogative) and they

    move from complement position to specifier position.

    Syntax of interrogative operators:

    Wh-questions like What did you say? involve moving an interrogative wh-operator like what? into the specifier positionwithin CP, and the moved wh-operator leaves behind a trace in the position it moves out of.

    Movement of operators into spec-CP is motivated by Lasnik's principle of enlightened self-interest which says that

    constituents move in order to satisfy the morphological requirements of other constituents, and operator movement

    serves to check the [wh] specifier-feature carried by the question affix Q which heads interrogative clauses. Such an

    analysis predicts that in multiple wh-questions, only one of the wh-operator expressions will move to spec-CP, and the

    shortest movement principle also called the minimality condition (-a constituent should move the shortest distance

    possible in any single movement operation) determines that it is the nearest wh-operator expression which moves.

    (In consequence of the economy principle) movement operations affect the minimal constituent which will be enough to

    check the features triggering their movement. In simple questions like What did he say? The. wh-operatorwhat is moved

    on its own, but movement ofwhatalone in sentences like *What did he give reason? would violate Chomsky's chain

    uniformity principle (= a chain should be uniform with regard to its phrase structure status, i.e. both the head and thefoot of the chain must nave the same structure), and in consequence the complement of what(\.e.. the noun reason) has to

    be pied-piped along with what, so deriving What reason did he give?

    Preposition stranding constraint:

    This constraint against preposition stranding applies to formal styles of English. It says that a preposition can't be

    stranded (separated from its complement whom)'. With whom was Tom dancing? *Whom was Tom dancing with?

    Huang's condition on extraction domains (which allows extraction only out of complements) prevents what from being

    extracted out of a subject expression in sentences like *Which country did you say the capital of was bombed during the

    war?, and requires the whole subject constituent to be pied-piped along with which, so deriving The capital of which

    country did you say was bombed during the war?

    Syntax of embedded questions in Belfast English:

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    13/21

    These allow the head Cposition of CP to be filled either by an inverted auxiliary or by the complementizerthat, so deriving

    sentences like I asked which Street did he live in and l asked which Street that he lived in.

    Inversion takes place in non interrogative clauses from which an interrogative operator has been extracted, e.g. in

    sentences

    like Who did he hope would he see? This could be accounted for by positing that operator movement applies in a successive

    cyclic fashion (moving an operator expression into the nearest spec-CP position above it on each of its movements), and

    by

    positing that a Cwhich has an interrogative specifier is a strong (affixal) head, and hence can trigger auxiliary inversion.

    Standard English:

    C is always filled by a null interrogative complementizer o in Standard English complement-clause questions, with spec-

    CP being filled either by merging the adverb whether with C-bar, or by moving an operator expression from some

    position within TP into spec-CP.

    Whetheris directly generated in spec-CP, and thus merges with C-bar (there's no movement): He asked [whetherI was

    leaving]

    Standard English doesn't allow auxiliary inversion in embedded wh-questions, because there is a null complementizer o which

    merges with Q. This satisfies the requirement for Q to be affixed to an appropriate host, and thereby blocks any form of

    auxiliary inversion:

    [She asked [what John had done ]]

    Operator movement applies in a successive cyclic fashion, and this accounts for why wh-operators can' t be extracted out of

    wh-questions (i.e. why sentences like *How do you wonder whether he is feeling? are, ungrammatical), since successive cyclic

    movement is blocked by the fact that whetheroccupies spec-CP in the embedded clauses, and long (one-step) movementviolates the shortest movement principle.

    Subject questions like Who helped him?

    Chomsky claims that such sentences are CPs headed by the question-affix Q, and the subject who is in spec-TP, with the

    wh-feature ofwho being attracted to Q. Chomsky's CP analysis would enable us +o provide a unitary characterization of

    questions as CPs with an interrogative Q head. The [wh] feature of Q is checked by attraction, with the [wh] head-feature

    of who adjoining to the Cnode containing Q, thereby enabling the [wh] feature of Q to be checked and erased.

    Q is only a strong affix when the CP headed by Q has an interrogative operator-specifier of its own. That s why Q is weak

    here and doesn't trigger auxiliary inversion or do-support.

    However, there's an alternative characterization of questions as clauses with an interrogative specifier, and this would allow

    us to posit that subject questions like Who helped him? are TPs which are questions by virtue of the fact that they have an

    interrogative specifier (= who). In questions such as What did you say?, movement ofwhatinto spec-CP would be

    motivated

    by the need to generate a structure with an interrogative specifier.

    The interrogative specifier analysis would entail positing that yes-no questions like Is it raining?containan abstract yes-no

    question operator Op in spec-CP. There are significant parallels between the syntax of such questions and whether

    questions which make it plausible to posit that they contain a null-operator counterpart ofwhether

    a. yes-no questions can be introduced by whetherwhen they are transposed into reported speech.

    b. Root inversion questions resemble whether questions in that in both cases yes/no are appropriate answers.

    c. Main-clause yes-no questions can be tagged by or notin precisely the same way as complement-clause

    whether questions.Embedded yes-no questions headed by the complementizer if also have a null yes-no question operator-specifier (//merges

    with Q), and this assumption accounts for the fact that operators can't be extracted out of /^question clauses (*How do

    you wonder if he is feeling?), since successive cyclic movement is blocked by the null operator and long movement is

    blocked by the shortest movement principle.

    If ando are the two interrogative complementizers in English.

    Nonoperator questions: They don't contain an interrogative operator and they don't show auxiliary inversion or wh-

    movement. Besides, they can't contain polarity Items such as anybecause polarity items must be c-commanded by a

    (negative, interrogative or conditional) operator. Examples: You're leaving? He said what? You've ordered the drinks?.

    They can be analysed as TPs headed by an interrogative T, or perhaps as CPs headed by a question particle Q which is

    weak by virtue of lacking an interrogative operator in spec-CP.

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    14/21

    Chapter 8

    VP - internal subject hypothesis -> subjects originate in spec-VP and are raised into spec-TP in ail but a few

    constructions - e.g. expletive structures. This raising is called subject-to-subject raising or subject raising,

    Expletive there has the categorial status of a pronominal determiner (spec-TP), since it can occur in sentence tags and the

    auxiliary is/are can be moved in front of it (into COMP) in yes-no questions structures.

    If we analyse sentences like: "There is someone waiting for you" and its nonexpletive counterpart "Someone is waiting for

    you", we see in both "someone" originates in spec-VP; in the first, "there" serves as the specifier of the TP headed by "is"and someone remains in situ as the specifier of the VP headed by waiting. In the second, "someone" undergoes subject

    raising.

    Evidence that subjects origin a te in spec - VP

    * cliticization: ex. We wanna help you. It was claimed that to couldn't be cliticized onto want because of thepresence

    of a null-case PRO subject in spec-TP but if the PRO subject originates as spec-VP, the cliticization is possible and the

    sentence grammatical.

    "We wanna PRO help you."

    *control structures: a moved constituent leaves behind a trace in any position out of which it moves. In : "He certainly has

    compromised himself." the reflexive himselfhas as its local c-commanding antecedent the trace ofhe ;

    "[TP He certainly has [VP tcompromised himseif.1

    * predicate nominals: following the same idea the predicate nominals have to agree in number with the 'trace subject fin

    spec-VP:

    [TP They probably will [VP^ become millionaires.1

    * quantifiers: when the quantifiers are separated from the subjects they quantify, they are referred as floating

    quantifiers Following the idea above these quantifiers modify the trace of the moved subject: "[TP They are [VP both

    thelping her.]

    * idioms: (string of words which form a unitary constituent with an idiosyncratic interpretation) in an idiom like "All hell

    broke loose", the choice of subject, verb an complement is fixed (clausal idiom). This type of idioms allow auxiliaries to

    be freely inserted between the subject and the verb. To say that subjects originate in spec-VP position helps us to

    account for the fact that the unitary string "all hell.......break loose" can be separated by an auxiliary. In the sentence

    "All hell will break loose" , the QP "all hell" originates in spec-VP as the subject of the V-bar constituent "break

    loose" and is then raised across the auxiliary "will" into spec-TP, where it becomes the subject of the T-bar.

    Argument structure

    There is strong semantic evidence in support of the VP internal subject hypothesis relating to argument structure. The

    arquments (expressions denoting a participant in a relevant actiyity or event) of a verb/predicate (expression denoting an

    activity or event) are its subject and complement. The complements of verbs are positioned inside V-bar whereas their

    subject are position outside V-bar, They originate in spec-VP and typically raise to spec-VP, complements are said to be

    internal arguments and subjects external arguments. Arguments may play different semantic roles in relation to a

    predicate. They are called thematic roles,

    Basic thematic roles;

    Theme or patient-> entity undergoing the effect of some action.

    Agent/causer -> instigator of some action.

    Experiencer -> entity experiencing some psychological State,

    Recipient/possessor/locative -> entity receiving/possessing some entity.

    Goal -> entity towards which something moves.

    Arguments are assigned a theta-role by the predicates via merger with a lexical category, the complement of a verb is

    theta-marked directly by the verb, and its subject is theta marked by V-bar, i.e. verbs directly theta-mark their

    complements but indirectly theta-mark their subject.

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    15/21

    In a sentence such as : "He throw the bal!", the verb throwwill be merged with the DP the hallto form the V-barthrow

    the ball, here the DP the hallis assigned the theta-role of THEME argument ofthrow. The V-barthrow the ha/tis merged

    with pronominal determinerhe, here he is assigned the theta-role of ASENT argument ofthrow the bal!.

    Theta marking serves us to account for the fact that subjects originate in spec-VP, because if subjects were directiy

    generated in spec-TP. we couldnt say that the thematic role of the subject is determined by V-bar, And that auxiliaries

    play no role in theta marking, because they are functional categories and theta-roles are assign only by lexical categories

    Subjects raise to satisfy the predication principle, which requires that syntactic predicates (i.e. V-bar and T-bar)should have subjects. And to check their own strong case-features (checking theory). [PRO doesn't raise to spec-TP

    because it has null case, which is weak and doesn't trigger movement,1

    Case-checking

    Features can be checked via: specifier-head relation; adjunction of one head to another; and attraction (i.e. by adjunction of

    a feature to a head).

    Nominative pronouns check their case-features via spec-head relation, but n the presence of contracted auxiliaries this

    checking is done by adjunction. Because contracted auxiliaries are affixal heads which are only Pf objects if another head

    attaches to them and in this case the other head is the subject pronoun.

    The null case of PRO is checked by attraction. (i.e. by percolation of the relevant null-case-features from the PRO subject in

    spec-VP to the infinitive particle to which heads TP).

    The objective case carried by the subject of an ECM infinitive complement is checked by attraction to a matrix transitive

    head. In the sentence: "They expect him to win", the objective-case-feature carried by him is attracted to expect.

    The case properties of subjects are checked by attraction in existential structures with, expletive there. In a sentence like:

    "There are students waiting for you". the indefinite nominal students carriespartitive case , that partitive case is a weak

    case which is cheeked by attraction, so that the relevant partitive case-feature percolates up from the indefinite nominal

    The expletive "there" carries only nominative case-feature (it doesn't carry agreement features -number, person,

    gender-.) and is inserted in spec-TP in order to satisfy the strong nominative-case-feature carried by a finite T-node.

    Raising predicates and control predicates:

    verbs such as: seem, appear, happen, turn out are called subject raising verbs, they allow the subject of an

    embedded infinitive complement to be raised up to become subject of the raising verb.

    Verbs such as: try, decide, plan, choose, hope, threaten, intend manage, needs, promises, like, want, expect

    and

    have (to do sth) are called control predicates, they may take aninfinitiye complement with a controlled PRO

    subject.

    Differences between subject raising verbs and control verbs:

    Subject raising verbs

    don't theta mark their subject

    take an infinitival complement without PRO don't have restriction in subject choice

    allow an expletive there subject

    Control verbs

    theta mark their subject

    take an infinitiyal complement with PRO subjectimposes restriction in subject choice-subject:

    an expression denoting an entity capable of

    rational thought.

    dont allow an expletive there subject

    Theta criterion: Each argument bears one and only one theta-role, and each theta role is assigned to one and only

    one argument.

    'He seems to enjoy syntax", he bears the theta role of experiencer assigned by enjoy and then raises to spec-TP for

    reasons of greed to check its own nominative features.

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    16/21

    He tries to PRO enjoy." he bears the theta role of agent assigned by try and PRO bears the theta role of experiencer

    assigned by enjoy.

    passivization:

    There are four main properties which differentiate passive sentences from their active counterparts:

    Passive sentences generally require the auxiliary be

    Lexical verbs in passive sentences are in the n-participle form.

    Passive sentences may contain a by-phrase in which the complement of by seems to play essentially the samethematic

    role as the subject in the corresponding active sentence.

    The expression which serves as the complement of an active verb surfaces as the subject in the corresponding

    passive

    construction.

    Distinctive characteristics of passiye participles:

    they theta mark their complements but not their

    subjects. They are intransitive.

    UTAH: maintains that there is a uniform mapping between thematic structure and syntactic structure. Each theta-

    role assigned by a particular type of predicate is canonically associated with a specific syntactic position.

    Passivized arguments originate as complements of passive participles and are raised up to become first the subject of

    the passive participle and then the subject of the passive auxiliary be.

    Evidence:

    Floating quantifiers: "The students were all arrested."

    If we assume that a floating quantifier modifies the trace of a moved subject, we can say that the quantifier all

    modifies the trace in spec-VP of the moved DP the students. So the Students becomes the subject of the passive

    participle arrestedbefore becoming subject of the auxiliary be.

    Expletive structures: "There were several students arrested".

    The QP several Students originates as the complement of the verb arrested. Since it is positioned immediately in

    front of the passive participle arrested, and it moves into (and remains in) spec-VP and hence serves as the subject of

    arrested. (the spec-TP position is occupied by there}

    In infinitival passives such as: "He is thought to admire her", the passive subject he originates as the subject of the verb

    admire in the infinitive complement, and is raised up to become first subject ofto and finally subject ofis.

    'Passivization and raising are different manifestations of a single argument movement operation: A movement. This has the

    effect of moving a constituent from one argument position into another (more specifically, from a subject or complement

    position into a subject position.)

    I-bar movement is the movement of an argument to a non argument position (moving maximal projections into a non subject

    position. ex : operator movement

    The complement position can only be created by mergerwith a head.A specifier position can be created by mergeror by

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    17/21

    movement,

    I movement is constrained by the following US principles: the C-command condition on binding, the shortest movement

    principle, the theta-criterion and the principle of greed.

    A movement should involve movement from a lower to a higher position, this satisfies the c-command condition on binding.

    A movement should apply in a successive cyclic fashion, this satisfies the shortest movement principle.

    TheA movement passivization satisfies the theta criterion because as passive participles theta-mark their complement butlot their subject, a DP will be assigned its theta role in cornplernent position and they will be raised into subject position

    having the same theta role and not receiving another one.

    TheA movement passivization is triggered by greed, in this case the requirement of a nominative pronoun to move into a

    position where it can check its nominative case-feature.

    Chapter 9

    VP shell analysis:

    In structures where we find three-place predicates, the VP shell analysis enables the double binary structure analysis,

    which claims that VPs have a complex structure, comprising an innerVP core and an outer vp shell, and that some agentive

    arguments originate within the outer vp shell, while other (e.g. TH.EAAE) arguments originate within the inner VP core,

    The following structures containing different kinds of predicates can be accounted for in terms of the VP shell analysis or

    he light verb analysis:

    Ergative predicates

    !. Transitive phrasals

    i. V+DP+PP

    k Ditransitive predicates

    . Resultative predicates

    i, Three-place predicates with clausal complements

    '. Structures with impersonal "it'! +verb +to sby + that clause

    . Three-place predicates: verb + DP + clausal complement

    '. Object-control predicates

    0.Monotransitive predicates1.Unergative predicates

    2.Unaccusative predicates

    The VP shell analysis involves the presence of an affixal, abstract, agentive light verb. This light verb can have three

    different semantic properties: causative (it can be paraphrased "We made + roll the ball down the hill."), performative (he

    performed the act of ....) or eventive (with much the same sense as happen).

    !n this type of analysis a lexical verb merges with its complement form in a a V-bar, then it's merged with a DP to form a

    VP. The resulting VP is then merged with an abstract causative light verb o, as this light verb is affixal in nature (and so a

    ...(text missing))

    assigned the theta-role of agent by the causative light verb) to form the complex vp. This vp then merges with an abstract

    Tense affix to form a T-bar and the subject raises into spec-TP to check its nominative case.

    Ergative predicates: Verbs that can be used either as three-place predicates or as two -place predicates . The DP whichimmediately follows the verb in the three-place structure functions as the subject in the two-place structure. They both

    play the same theta-role.

    ex. [He rolled the ball down the hill.] [The ball rolled down the hill.]

    agent theme goal theme

    This analysis would be compatible with UTAH, since in both the THEME argument would originate as the subject and

    specifier of the VP headed by the lexical verb roll, because UTAH claims that two arguments which fulfill the same

    thematic function with respect to a given predicate must occupy the same underlying position in the syntax,

    Some ergative verbs: roll - f 11 - brake - withdraw - move - close

    Adverbs like "gently" may occupy two different positions in sentences:

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    18/21

    We gently rolled the ball down the hill" VS "We rolled the balI gently down the hill."

    These different positions occupied by the adverb reflect a subtle meaning difference, one means that the action which

    initiated the rolling motion was gently, whereas the other means that the rolling motion itself was gentle.

    But not all adverbs can occupy different positions in grammatical sentences. If we analyse the adverb position in

    sentences

    like:

    "He had deliberately rolled the ball down the hill"

    "He had rolled the ball deliberately down the hill"

    We find out that adverbs like deliberately are strictly vp adverbs because by virtue of its meaning, they can only be anadjunct to a projection of an agentive verb and the light verb is causative verb with an agentive subject. On the other hand

    Adverbs such as perfectly can function only as VP-adverbs.

    Transitive phrasals: In this type of structures the adverbial particles optionally adjoin to the verb, that's why we can

    have grammatical sentences such as:

    They turn down the offer". and "They turn the offer down".

    In the first, the particle down optionally adjoins to the verb turned, forming the complex verb turned down, which adjoins

    to the light verb. In the second down remains in situ.

    The VP shell analysis enables us to provide a principled account of the two positions which can be occupied by prepositional

    particles.

    V+DP+PP These verbs can't be used intransitively so they are not ergatives but they take two complements so the VP

    shell analysis is plausible. We may say that they are essentially affixal in nature (and so must adjoin to the agentive lightverb) and that they have a strong causative feature which has to be checked by adjunction to an abstract light verb. We

    may also say that they are obligatory transitive verbs (and so has to check objective case) and hence can occur in this

    structures (VP shell analysis) where they are raised into a position where they can check the objective case of the

    immediately following DP.

    ex. "They loaded [the truck] [with hay]"

    "He gave [no explanations] [to his friends.]"

    "They took [everything] [from her"]

    In this type of structures we find that VP adverbs can be positioned eitherbefore or after the verb or between its two

    Complements, it can adjoin to v-bar or V-bar.

    They carefully loaded the truck with hay." OR "They loaded the truck carefully with hay,"

    Some V+DP+PP verbs; take -load - give -blame -assure - hand - bring

    Ditransitive verbs : verbs which take two DP objects. (The lst DP is said to be the indirect object of the verb and the 2 nd its

    direct object.)

    ex. " The will get the teacher a present"

    DP DP

    "Could you pass me the salt?"

    DP DP

    i. Resultative predicates: they show the result of a process in that sth becomes sth.

    ex. "They painted the house pink."

    DP A

    "The acid turned the litmus paper green."

    DP A

    j. Three place predicates with clausal complements: verbs which take PP and CP complements.

    ex. " He remarked to her that the Senators should leave."

    PP CP

    "He agreed with her that Peter was incurable."

    PP CP

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    19/21

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    20/21

    have ion agentive subjects; they may have post verbal subjects (.e. in child language, in unaccusative imperatives in

    Belfast dialects); auxiliary "be" in earlier English; adjectival use of perfective participles.

    ex: "There carne a cry of anguish from inside." (postverbal subject)

    "Leave you now!" (imperative in Belfast dialect)

    "Go truck" (child language)

    "Is the Duke gone?" (auxiliary "be") "They arrested a man recently returned

    from France." ((adjectival use)

    some verbs of this type: there + arise - come -appear -occur - stands. Go in the other types.

    Chapter 10:

    Subject Agreement Projections

    Clauses contain three different types of Agreement Phrase.

    Split-Inflection hypothesis: it's necessary to posit the existence of a Subject Agreement Phrase (AgrSP) in addition to

    a Tense phrase (TP) in order to account for the fact that adverbs like probably (a TP adverb) and floating quantifiers likeall can be positioned either before or after a finite auxiliary. The SpIit-INFL analysis would also enable us to account for

    the fact that and adverb positioned after an auxiliary can have scope over (can C-command) the auxiliary, e.g. in a sentence

    such as "Gary can apparently lift 100 pounds."

    Since auxiliaries like have/be typically inflect for tense and agreement, finite clauses also contain an abstract agreement

    morpheme which projects into an agreement phrase. The relevant agreement head occupies a higher position than T,

    auxiliaries are generated in T and from there raise to adjoin to the separate agreement head. Subjects raise from

    specifer-vp to the specifier position in the agreement phrase to check their case and agreement features. Since the

    agreement relation in question involves subjects it is called AgrSP. Our earlier INFL head will in effect be split into two

    different heads: a T head and a AgrS head.

    Process: A TP is merged with an AgrS head which projects into AgrSP, a pronoun raises to specifier AgrS (AgrS has strong

    specifier-features so subjects always raise to check their case-and agreement -feature) to check its nominative case andhe auxiliary raises from T to adjoin to AgrS (thereby enabling it to check its agreement properties).

    Evidence:

    Scrambling in Early English -^ in sentences like: "The king your mote did see.", the DP your moter has adjoined to TP, while

    the subject the kingis in spec-AgrSP.

    Syntax of the case and agreement properties of subjects and auxiliaries in Belfast English -> in which nominative

    subjects are in spec-AgrSP and any auxiliary which checks the agreement properties of the subject is in AgrS (and so can

    move to Cin questions), whereas objective subjects are in spec-TP and any associated auxiliary is in T, and (by virtue of

    lacking agreement properties) can't move through AgrS to C in questions.

    Claims that objective DPs raise to spec-AgrOP in order to check their objective case features and their object agreement-

    features under spec-head agreement with AgrO.

    This analysis enables us to provide a unified account of case- and agreement-checking, in which all case - and agreement

    features are checked under a spec-head relation between a functional head and its specifier.

    Process: a direct object DP raises to spec-AgrOP and its associated verb adjoins to AgrO in order to check objective case

    the verb subsequently raising still further to adjoin to v)

    Evidence:

    Systematic differences between the position of CP complements and DP complements -

    T sentences such as "He reported to the police that there had been a robbery" and

  • 8/14/2019 Iummato.GrammarII.2002.Radford.Summary.gmodroff

    21/21

    "He reported the robbery to the police"

    The DP and CP play the same thematic role as the THEME ofreported. So they must originate in the same position following

    UTAH.

    DP objects are postverbal whereas CPcomplements are clause-final.

    DP complements carry objective case and hence have to be raise to Spec-AgrOP for case-checking; by contrast that-clauses

    don't carry case, so they don't move for case-checking purposes and remain in situ.

    Position of adverbs -> adverbs merge with maximal projections so to account for the grammatical sentences: "He plays

    chess well" here wellmerges with the VPplay chess, then the verb adjoins to AgrO and the DP raises from complementposition to spec-AgrOP to check the objective case feature. Then the analysis continues with v', vp, T', TP, AgrS' and ends

    in AgrSP. We may say that verbs are immediately adjacent to their objects and can't be separated by an intervening

    adverb.

    Prepositional particles position -> preposition particle can occupy three different positions: He

    poured the whisky slowly out." He poured the whisky out slowly." He poured out the whisky

    slowly."

    We may say that in transitive structures objective DPs move to spec-AgrOp and verbs move through AgrO to adjoin to v,

    he particle can remain in situ, can be incorporated into the verb and then be excorporated out of the V in AgrO position or

    the pied-piped along with it. The different positions of the particle don't interfere in the checking of the DP objective-

    feature.