9
Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness IRINA ELLIOTT 1 & SUZANNE COKER 2 1 Queensland Police Service, Central Region Office, Rockhampton, and 2 Central Queensland University, School of Psychology and Sociology, Rockhampton, Queensland, Australia Abstract A construal theory of happiness emphasises the mediating impact of cognitive and motivational processes on the individual’s perceptions of their happiness. This study investigated a path model with the two cognitive variables, self-reflection and self- rumination as mediating factors between an independent self-construal and subjective happiness. One hundred and twenty three participants completed a questionnaire designed to measure subjective happiness, independent self-construal, self- reflection, and self-rumination. Individuals’ propensity to self-reflect and self-ruminate was not found to be affected by an independent self-construal. A higher independent self-construal was associated with greater happiness. The results also indicated that self-reflection has the potential to both increase and decrease (when mediated by self-rumination) subjective happiness. This study suggests that although meaningful self-reflection may be beneficial for individuals who do not enjoy high levels of happiness, the perils of self-reflection are that it may trigger self-rumination which has detrimental consequences for happiness. The pursuit of happiness is inherent to human existence. Recently, research on happiness has been revitalised by the emergence of positive psychology with its focus on the resilience of human nature (Sheldon & King, 2001). The general consensus is that although to some degree objective situational factors (such as income, health, age, and marital status) influence people’s perceptions of their happi- ness, ultimately, happiness is a subjective phenom- enon, with subjective feelings, thoughts, perceptions, and evaluations of the situation even more important than the situation itself (Argyle, 2001; Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999; Heller, Watson, & Ilies, 2004; Myers, 2000). A construal theory of happiness The subjective perspective on happiness is reflected in one of the recent developments in research on happiness, a construal theory. According to a construal theory, happiness is determined by what people make of their experiences; that is, the way they construe and interpret the world around them (Lyubomirsky, 2001). In this respect, a construal theory of happiness emphasises the role of hedoni- cally relevant cognitive and motivational processes as mediating the effects of person and situation factors on the perception of happiness. Hedonically relevant cognitive and motivational processes (e.g., social comparison, dissonance reduction, self-reflection, and self-rumination) are those which lead to hedonic consequences; that is, they have the potential to either promote or undermine a positive view of the self. For example, the positive association between self-rumination (dwelling on the negative in relation to the self) and depressed mood is a well-documented phe- nomenon (Nolen-Hoeksema, McBride, & Larson, 1997; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1993; Nolen- Hoeksema, Parker, & Larson, 1994). From the perspective of a construal theory, self-rumination may be detrimental to happiness as focusing on the negative in relation to the self may detract from a positive view of the self, which, in turn, may undermine the individual’s perceptions of their happiness. Correspondence: Irina Elliott, Queensland Police Service, Central Region Office, Rockhampton, Queensland, Australia. E-mail: [email protected] Australian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 60, No. 3, December 2008, pp. 127–134. ISSN 0004-9530 print/ISSN 1742-9536 online ª The Australian Psychological Society Ltd Published by Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/00049530701447368

Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A pathmodel for predicting happiness

IRINA ELLIOTT1 & SUZANNE COKER2

1Queensland Police Service, Central Region Office, Rockhampton, and 2Central Queensland University, School of Psychology

and Sociology, Rockhampton, Queensland, Australia

AbstractA construal theory of happiness emphasises the mediating impact of cognitive and motivational processes on the individual’sperceptions of their happiness. This study investigated a path model with the two cognitive variables, self-reflection and self-rumination as mediating factors between an independent self-construal and subjective happiness. One hundred and twentythree participants completed a questionnaire designed to measure subjective happiness, independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination. Individuals’ propensity to self-reflect and self-ruminate was not found to be affected by anindependent self-construal. A higher independent self-construal was associated with greater happiness. The results alsoindicated that self-reflection has the potential to both increase and decrease (when mediated by self-rumination) subjectivehappiness. This study suggests that although meaningful self-reflection may be beneficial for individuals who do not enjoyhigh levels of happiness, the perils of self-reflection are that it may trigger self-rumination which has detrimentalconsequences for happiness.

The pursuit of happiness is inherent to human

existence. Recently, research on happiness has been

revitalised by the emergence of positive psychology

with its focus on the resilience of human nature

(Sheldon & King, 2001). The general consensus is

that although to some degree objective situational

factors (such as income, health, age, and marital

status) influence people’s perceptions of their happi-

ness, ultimately, happiness is a subjective phenom-

enon, with subjective feelings, thoughts, perceptions,

and evaluations of the situation even more important

than the situation itself (Argyle, 2001; Diener, Suh,

Lucas, & Smith, 1999; Heller, Watson, & Ilies, 2004;

Myers, 2000).

A construal theory of happiness

The subjective perspective on happiness is reflected

in one of the recent developments in research on

happiness, a construal theory. According to a

construal theory, happiness is determined by what

people make of their experiences; that is, the way

they construe and interpret the world around them

(Lyubomirsky, 2001). In this respect, a construal

theory of happiness emphasises the role of hedoni-

cally relevant cognitive and motivational processes as

mediating the effects of person and situation factors

on the perception of happiness.

Hedonically relevant cognitive and motivational

processes (e.g., social comparison, dissonance

reduction, self-reflection, and self-rumination) are

those which lead to hedonic consequences; that is,

they have the potential to either promote or

undermine a positive view of the self. For example,

the positive association between self-rumination

(dwelling on the negative in relation to the self)

and depressed mood is a well-documented phe-

nomenon (Nolen-Hoeksema, McBride, & Larson,

1997; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1993; Nolen-

Hoeksema, Parker, & Larson, 1994). From the

perspective of a construal theory, self-rumination

may be detrimental to happiness as focusing on the

negative in relation to the self may detract from a

positive view of the self, which, in turn, may

undermine the individual’s perceptions of their

happiness.

Correspondence: Irina Elliott, Queensland Police Service, Central Region Office, Rockhampton, Queensland, Australia.

E-mail: [email protected]

Australian Journal of Psychology, Vol. 60, No. 3, December 2008, pp. 127–134.

ISSN 0004-9530 print/ISSN 1742-9536 online ª The Australian Psychological Society Ltd

Published by Taylor & Francis

DOI: 10.1080/00049530701447368

Page 2: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Similarly, happy individuals have been found to be

less prone to engage in self-reflection, or to think

about and analyse one’s thoughts, feelings, and

outcomes of their actions (Lyubomirsky, 2001).

Moreover, in experimental studies involving indivi-

duals who display extremely high or extremely low

levels of happiness, Lyubomirsky and Ross (1999)

demonstrated that when happy people were induced

to self-reflect, their behaviour matched the behaviour

of unhappy individuals, whereas when unhappy

individuals were prevented from engaging in self-

reflection, their behaviour matched the behaviour of

happy people. This suggests that similar to the

hedonic consequences of self-rumination mentioned

above, the hedonic consequences of self-reflection

may be detrimental to the individual’s perceptions of

their happiness.

The question raised in this study is which

personality characteristics may predispose people to

self-reflect or self-ruminate. The answer to this

question may reside in differences between indivi-

duals in terms of how they see themselves (indepen-

dent from others or connected to others). These

differences are referred to as the distinction between

independent and interdependent self-construals

(Markus & Kitayama, 1991).

Independent versus interdependent self-construals

According to Markus and Kitayama (1991), those

with an independent self-construal define themselves

in terms of internal attributes such as traits, abilities,

values, and preferences. In contrast, those with an

interdependent self-construal define themselves in

terms of their relationships with others. Markus and

Kitayama argued that differences between indepen-

dent and interdependent self-concepts lead to differ-

ent consequences for a number of cognitive and

motivational processes. Subsequent research not only

supported this assumption (Choi, Nisbett, &

Norenzayan, 1999; Kurman, 2002; Poasa,

Mallinckrodt, & Suzuki, 2000) but also indicated that

these differences in cognitive and motivational

processes may be related to subjective well-being

(Cross, Gore, & Morris, 2003; Kwan & Bond, 1997).

This suggests that the distinction between inde-

pendent and interdependent self-construals can be a

source of individual differences in cognitive and

motivational processes, which, in turn, may influence

individual differences in happiness. From the per-

spective of a construal theory, the mediating impact

of cognitive and motivational processes on happiness

is related to the hedonic consequences of these

processes for the individual’s self-esteem.

In this respect, the present study was designed to

investigate the negative hedonic consequences asso-

ciated with the two cognitive variables, self-reflection

and self-rumination as mediating factors between an

independent self-construal and happiness. Another

aim of this study was to assess the direct and indirect

influences of self-reflection on subjective happiness.

For these purposes a path model as presented in

Figure 1 was constructed.

Path model and hypotheses

Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that although

independent and interdependent self-construals are

predominant in individualistic and collectivistic

cultures, respectively, individuals within a given

society can be less or more independent or inter-

dependent. (The distinction between individualistic

and collectivistic societies refers to the extent to

which the value of the individual takes precedence

over, or is sacrificed to the needs of others.)

Moreover, Hackman, Ellis, Johnson, and Staley

(1999) demonstrated that independent and inter-

dependent self-construal orientations are two sepa-

rate constructs and suggested that the predictive

capacities of these two variables should be investi-

gated separately.

Markus and Kitayama (1991) argued that for those

with an independent self-construal knowledge about

the self is more important and elaborated than

knowledge about the other; one’s internal abilities,

feelings, and thoughts are therefore highly accessible

in memory. Moreover, for those with an independent

self-construal, in comparison to those with an

interdependent self-construal, self-esteem is based

on internal attributes, which facilitates motivation to

obtain more self-knowledge. This suggests that

individuals with a highly developed independent

self-construal may be more prone to focus on

themselves when reflecting on their life. For exam-

ple, in Batson, Fultz, Schoenrade, and Paduano’s

(1987) study, the shift in perception of pro-social

behaviour as less altruistic as a result of self-reflection

was greater for individuals who emphasised the

importance of self-knowledge over relationships with

others. Therefore, a higher level of an independent

self-construal as an individual variable may lead to

greater self-reflection.

Markus and Kitayama (1991) point out

that although people with both self-construals are

Figure 1. Path model predicting subjective happiness

128 I. Elliott & S. Coker

Page 3: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

motivated to maintain a positive view of the self, they

employ different strategies to do so. For those with

an independent construal a positive view of the self

involves seeking information that enhances internal

attributes as their self-esteem is based on an ability to

express inner qualities. In contrast, having self-

attributes that are more positive should be less

central to the interdependent self, as the self-esteem

of those with the interdependent self is based on the

ability to fit in and to maintain harmony in relation-

ships with others. This suggests that individuals with

a highly developed independent self-construal will be

motivated to avoid dwelling on the negative in

relation to the self. Therefore, a higher level of an

independent self-construal as an individual variable

may lead to less self-rumination.

The role of the self-attentive process (the propen-

sity to attend to one’s thoughts and feelings) in

psychological well-being appears to be controversial

in research literature (Trapnell & Campbell, 1999).

On the one hand, studies indicate that the self-

attentive process promotes greater self-awareness

and more accurate self-knowledge (Hixon & Swann,

1993), which is considered essential for psychologi-

cal growth. On the other hand, the self-attentive

process has been found to be associated with greater

levels of depression, anxiety, and neuroticism

(Joireman, 2004). Trapnell and Campbell (1999)

labelled these contradictory findings as the ‘‘self-

absorption paradox’’ (p. 286). According to Trapnell

and Campbell, the self-absorption paradox can

be resolved by attributing negative and positive

outcomes of the self-attentive process to two

independent factors within the self-attentive process,

rumination and reflection, respectively.

Attributing clear-cut positive outcomes to self-

reflection, however, contradicts the findings of

experimental studies on happiness mentioned above.

In addition, Batson et al. (1987) demonstrated that

self-reflection can undermine altruistic behaviour.

Moreover, the clear-cut independence of reflective

and ruminative processes appears to be questionable

considering that in a number of experimental studies

connecting rumination and psychological well-being,

self-rumination was induced through generic self-

reflection (e.g., Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema,

1993). In a similar vein, a study reported by Nolen-

Hoeksema et al. (1997) suggests a possible causal

link between self-reflection, self-rumination, and

psychological well-being, with self-reflection leading

to self-rumination over time and as a result to greater

depressed mood. Therefore, greater self-reflection

may be associated with greater self-rumination.

Thus, based on the distinction between inde-

pendent and interdependent self-construals and

previous research associating self-reflection and self-

rumination with subjective happiness and well-being,

it was hypothesised that: (a) a higher independent

self-construal would be associated with greater self-

reflection and less self-rumination, (b) greater

self-reflection would be associated with greater self-

rumination and less happiness, and (c) greater self-

rumination would lead to less happiness.

Method

Participants

Participants were 123 residents of Rockhampton in

Central Queensland (female, n¼ 87; male, n¼ 34;

missing, n¼ 2). Participants ranged in age from 18 –

82 years (M¼ 38.50, SD¼ 15.31). Table I presents

the age group, education level attained, marital

status, and income level of the participants.

Materials

The Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS; Lyubomirsky &

Lepper, 1999) assesses the extent to which indivi-

duals consider themselves happy and consists of four

items with 7-point Likert type response formats.

Table I. Demographic data of participants

Demographic variable % n

Age

18 – 24 years 24.4 30

25 – 44 years 35.0 43

45 – 64 years 35.8 44

65 years and over 3.3 4

Missing 1.6 2

Gender

Males 27.7 34

Females 70.7 87

Missing 1.6 2

Education

Primary school 2.4 3

1 – 2 years secondary school 5.7 7

3 – 4 years secondary school 22.8 28

5 – 6 years secondary school 19.5 24

Technical/trade 13.0 16

Tertiary 34.1 42

Missing 2.4 3

Marital status

Single 32.5 40

Married 39.0 48

De facto 8.1 10

Separated 4.9 6

Divorced 13.8 17

Widowed 1.6 2

Income(per year)

Less than $10,000 22.0 27

$10,001 – $20,000 19.5 24

$20,001 – $30,000 15.4 19

$30,001 – $40,000 15.4 19

$40,001 – $50,000 4.1 5

$50,001 – $60,000 4.9 6

More than $60,000 17.9 22

Missing 0.8 1

Self-construal and happiness 129

Page 4: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Lyubomirsky (2001) has reported good to excellent

psychometric properties of the SHS across 14 studies.

For example, the SHS has been found to have high

internal consistency with Cronbach’s alphas ranging

from .85 to .95 in eight studies. High test-retest

reliability has been shown over a four-week period

(Pearson’s r¼ .90) and over a three-month period

(Pearson’s r¼ .71). Convergent validity has been

demonstrated between the SHS and widely used

measures of subjective well-being, including Brad-

burn’s (1969) Affect-Balance Scale (rs range from .49

to .64); Diener, Emmons, Larson, and Griffin’s

(1985) Satisfaction with Life Scale (rs range from .61

to .69); and Andrews and Withey’s (1976) Terrible-

Delighted Scale (rs range from .59 to .71). Discrimi-

nant validity has been demonstrated between the SHS

and unrelated constructs such as academic ability and

demographic variables (Lyubomirsky). In the present

study, Cronbach’s alpha was .80.

The Refined Independent Self-Construal Scale

(Hackman et al., 1999) was introduced by Hackman

et al. as a result of a validation study conducted on

two instruments which measure self-construal or-

ientation: the Independent and Interdependent Self-

Construal Scales (Gudykunst et al., 1994). The

instruments developed by Gudykunst et al. were

based on a conceptualisation of the self-construal

construct as consisting of the two orthogonal factors

of independence and interdependence. Results of

confirmatory factor analysis in Hackman et al.’s

study indicated that independent and interdepen-

dent self-construals were two separate one-factor

dimensions of the self-construal construct. These

findings provided support for the separate use of the

two self-construal orientations as predictor variables

in future research.

The Refined Independent Self-Construal Scale

(Hackman et al., 1999) assesses the independent

self-construal orientation, which is the extent to

which an individual sees himself or herself as a

unique and independent person. The Scale consists

of 11 items with 6-point Likert type response

formats. Hackman et al. reported Cronbach’s alpha

of the scale as .89 for a North American sample (high

individualism), .88 for a New Zealand sample

(moderate individualism/moderate collectivism),

.77 for a Kyrgyzstan sample (high collectivism),

and .85 for the overall sample. In the present study,

Cronbach’s alpha was .77.

The Self-Reflection Scale (SReS) and the Self-

Rumination Scale (SRuS) were developed based on

previous research specifically for this study to

measure self-reflection and self-rumination, respec-

tively. The scales are presented in the Appendix.

Although the bulk of the items of both scales were

derived from the Rumination-Reflection Question-

naire (RRQ) developed by Trapnell and Campbell

(1999), the RRQ does not address explicitly either

self-reflection or self-rumination, lacks consistency

in distinguishing reflection and rumination, and

includes emotionally laden items.

The Self-Reflection Scale consists of 12 items with

6-point Likert type response formats. The items

reflect a conceptualisation of self-reflection as con-

sisting of thinking about and analysing one’s

thoughts, feelings, and actions (Lyubomirsky,

2001). Two of the items (5 and 9), which reflect

the conceptualisation of self-reflection employed in

the present study, were taken from the RRQ without

making changes. Another six items from the RRQ

(2, 3, 4, 7, 8, and 10), which are laden with positive

emotionality, were modified to render them neutral.

Three of the items (6, 11, and 12) were developed

on the basis of tasks that were given to participants

in experimental studies to engage them in the

self-reflective process (e.g., Lyubomirsky &

Nolen-Hoeksema, 1993; Lyubomirsky & Ross,

1999). Item 1 was written specifically for this research

based on the connection between self-reflection and

self-knowledge identified in the research literature

(e.g., Hixon & Swann, 1993; Trapnell & Campbell,

1999). The items are scored in a positive direction

(except items 5, 7, and 9, which are reverse-scored),

with higher scores indicating greater self-reflection.

The possible range of scores is from 12 to 72. In the

present study, Cronbach’s alpha was .86.

The Self-Rumination Scale (SRuS) consists of 10

items with 6-point Likert type response formats. The

items reflect a conceptualisation of self-rumination

as self-focused dwelling in response to negative

events. Four of these items (1, 2, 3, and 5), which

reflect the conceptualisation of self-rumination em-

ployed in the present study, were taken without

making changes. Another four items from the RRQ

(4, 6, 8, and 10), which direct the self-reflective

process away from negative events, were modified to

fit the definition of self-rumination employed in the

present study. Item 7 and item 9 were written

specifically for this study, based on the tasks used to

induce self-rumination that were employed in

previous studies (e.g., Lyubomirsky & Nolen-

Hoeksema, 1993; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 1993).

The items are scored in a positive direction (except

for item 1 which is reverse-scored), with higher

scores indicating greater self-rumination. The possi-

ble range of scores is from 10 to 60. In the present

study, Cronbach’s alpha was .91.

Procedure

Data collection for the study was conducted using

a snow-balling technique with contacts in the

community. Individuals were approached and asked

if they would be willing to be voluntary participants

130 I. Elliott & S. Coker

Page 5: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

themselves, and also to ask their associates to

participate in the study. The overall return rate for

the 200 questionnaires distributed was 65%, with

123 questionnaires (94.62%) out of the total 130

questionnaires returned being employed in the

analysis. (The data of seven respondents were not

included in the analysis due to a large number of

missing values.)

Results

Preliminary analyses

Means and standard deviations of the four research

variables are presented in Table II. The mean

of subjective happiness (M¼ 4.90, SD¼ 0.99) in this

sample is lower than the mean reported by

Lyubomirsky and Ross (1999) in a US high school

sample (M¼ 6.11, SD¼ 0.78) for individuals who

were classified as extremely happy individuals.

Participants’ gender, level of income, marital status,

educational level, and age group had no significant

effect on subjective happiness. The results of the

reliability analyses indicated that all four measures

had acceptable levels of internal consistency (See

Materials subsection for Cronbach’s alphas).

Intercorrelations among the four research variables

were analysed to safeguard the validity of regression

analysis in case of multicollinearity, that is, highly

correlated independent variables. The results from

the correlational analyses are presented in Table III.

In terms of the three independent variables, the

results showed only one significant low bivariate

correlation: self-reflection/self-rumination¼ .31, in-

dicating that these variables may be somewhat

interrelated but still represent distinct constructs.

In addition, significant low correlations were

found among subjective happiness/independent self-

construal¼ .23 and subjective happiness/self-

rumination¼7.31.

Path analysis

As preliminary analyses indicated a significant

correlation between independent self-construal and

subjective happiness, a path connecting independent

self-construal and subjective happiness was added

to the hypothesised model presented in Figure 1.

In order to investigate whether independent self-

construal correlates with subjective happiness be-

cause of the influence of the cognitive variables

self-reflection and self-rumination, or whether

independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-

rumination have an independent influence on sub-

jective happiness, a path analysis was conducted.

Path analysis is a method in which a causal theory

and the statistical technique of multiple regression

combine to assess direct and indirect influences

between the variables of interest (Ho, 2006). To

estimate the direction and magnitude of the paths in

the model presented in Figure 1 and to test the three

research hypotheses, a series of three regression

analyses were carried out. Figure 2 presents the path

model with the estimated regression coefficients

(Beta values) associated with the hypothesised paths.

The results showed that independent self-con-

strual had only a direct effect on subjective happiness

(b¼ .21). Self-reflection had a direct effect on

subjective happiness (b¼ .25) and an indirect effect

through self-rumination (path¼ .3167.38). To

estimate whether the indirect effect was significant,

an online application of the Sobel test developed by

Preacher and Leonardelli (2001) was used. The

results indicated the significant mediating role of

self-rumination in the relation between self-reflection

and subjective happiness (Sobel test statistic¼72.75, p5 .01). The three predictor variables

accounted for 19.80% of the variance in subjective

happiness (p5 .05), with self-rumination being

the strongest predictor of subjective happiness

accounting for 9.60% of the total variance

(p5 .001). Self-reflection was a significant predictor

of self-rumination accounting for 9.40% of the

variance (p5 .01).

Table II. Means and standard deviations of research variables

Variable Mean SD

Subjective happiness 4.90 0.99

Independent self-construal 4.97 0.55

Self-reflection 4.25 0.75

Self-rumination 3.48 1.04

Table III. Intercorrelations between research variables

Variable 1 2 3 4

1. Independent self-construal –

2. Subjective happiness .23* –

3. Self-reflection .00 .14 –

4. Self-rumination 7.06 7.31*** .31** –

*p5 .05, **p5 .01, ***p5 .001.

Figure 2. Standardised regression coefficients for the path model

predicting subjective happiness

Self-construal and happiness 131

Page 6: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Discussion

The results of the study showed that independent

self-construal had only a direct influence on sub-

jective happiness, such that a higher level of indepen-

dent self-construal was associated with greater

happiness. The results also showed that self-reflection

had a direct influence on subjective happiness, such

that greater self-reflection was associated with greater

happiness. In addition, self-reflection had an indirect

influence on subjective happiness, being mediated

by self-rumination, such that greater self-reflection

led to greater self-rumination; this, in turn, was

associated with less happiness.

The findings that independent self-construal did

not account for any significant variance in self-

reflection or self-rumination indicate that the way

participants saw themselves (independent from

others or connected to others) did not influence

their tendency to self-reflect or self-ruminate. The

possible explanation for the finding that a higher level

of independent self-construal was associated with

greater happiness is that people who see themselves

as independent from others may have greater

perceptions of personal control over their lives. The

extent of perceived personal control over one’s life

has been found to be one of the significant positive

correlates of happiness (Argyle, 2001).

The finding that greater self-reflection was asso-

ciated with greater self-rumination casts doubts on

the clear-cut independence of reflective and rumina-

tive processes suggested in previous literature

(Trapnell & Campbell, 1999). It appears that self-

rumination may be a by-product of self-reflection. It

may be that people who have a tendency to self-

reflect find it difficult to disengage from this process

in the face of adverse circumstances, unfavourable

outcomes, and negative events in their lives.

The finding that greater self-reflection was asso-

ciated with increased happiness contradicts findings

of experimental studies which suggested self-

reflection to have a negative effect on happiness

(Lyubomirsky, 2001; Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1999). In

these studies, however, the negative effect of self-

reflection was demonstrated for individuals who

were classified as extremely happy individuals (with

the mean score close to the highest point of SHS). In

the present study, the mean score for the sample was

close to the middle point of SHS. This suggests that

in contrast to extremely happy people, self-reflection

may be beneficial to individuals who do not exhibit

high levels of happiness. It may be that for these

individuals self-reflection facilitates identification of

meaningful goals which have been shown to be

strong predictors of happiness (Argyle, 2001).

An alternate explanation may be related to the

experimental nature of previous studies in which

happy participants were induced to self-reflect on

trivial situations, whereas in the present study, self-

reflection was measured in a non-manipulated

environment and in a broader context. Self-reflection

on meaningful as opposed to trivial events may

facilitate having meaningful goals. From the per-

spective of a construal theory, meaningful goals have

the potential to enhance a positive view of the self.

Self-reflection may be detrimental, however, to

maintaining acquired happiness. From the perspec-

tive of a construal theory, in this respect, self-

reflection may have potential to undermine a positive

view of the self, analogous to the situation when one

finds a flaw in a perfect picture after subjecting it to

close scrutiny. For example, in Batson et al.’s (1987)

study, self-reflection on participants’ altruistic ac-

tions led them to perceive their pro-social behaviour

as less altruistic, presumably by discoveries of their

ulterior, self-oriented motives as a result of self-

reflection. Thus, the same cognitive process, self-

reflection, may have different hedonic consequences

depending on where individuals stand on the

happiness continuum as well as on the context of

the situation. The implication for practice is that

cognitive and motivational strategies employed by

extremely happy individuals may not have the same

validity when employed by individuals who do not

enjoy high levels of happiness.

As with all studies, this study has several limita-

tions. They relate to the use of a non-random sample

and the self-report nature of the measurement

instruments. In addition, the results of this study

are based on correlational and cross-sectional data.

Also, it is possible that the findings of this study are

confined to an individualistic culture (such as

Australia) where independence is valued more then

interdependence.

Nevertheless, the findings of the present study

have important implications for future research. It is

suggested that future research should investigate

whether other hedonically relevant cognitive and

motivational processes (e.g., social comparison) may

play a mediating role between self-construal and

subjective happiness. It would be interesting to

investigate the relationship between happiness and

self-reflection in a broader context than was utilised

in previous experimental studies with individuals

who exhibit extremely high levels of happiness to

ascertain if meaningful self-reflection can lead to

increased happiness for these individuals. Also,

future research should continue to explore the

possibility that other personality dimensions will

have a direct or indirect impact on the individual’s

propensity to self-reflect or self-ruminate. Finally,

the findings of this study indicate that self-reflection

has the potential to both increase and decrease (when

mediated by self-rumination) subjective happiness.

132 I. Elliott & S. Coker

Page 7: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Thus, future research should explore the possibility

of mediating factors operating between self-reflection

and self-rumination.

Conclusion

This study provides empirical support for a construal

theory of happiness in that individual differences in

hedonically relevant cognitive processes of self-

reflection and self-rumination exert a significant

influence on the individual’s perception of subjective

happiness. The individual’s propensity to self-reflect

or self-ruminate, however, was not found to be

affected by an independent self-construal as had

been predicted based on the independent versus

interdependent self-construal theoretical framework.

The results of this study suggest that meaningful

self-reflection may be beneficial for people who do

not enjoy high levels of happiness. Although self-

reflection has the potential to increase happiness, this

study highlights the perils of self-reflection in that it

may trigger self-rumination with its detrimental con-

sequences for happiness. Finally, the discovery of the

direct influence of an independent self-construal on

subjective happiness provides fuel for future research-

ers and innovators in search of more comprehensive

structural models for predicting happiness.

Acknowledgements

This study was conducted by the first author under

the supervision of the co-author in partial fulfillment

of the requirements of a Bachelor of Psychology. The

first author wishes to thank Professor Kevin Ronan at

Central Queensland University for his support over

the period in which this article was written, Margaret

Kirkwood, Desley Stehbens, Julie Mann, and Bruce

Elliott for their contribution to data collection for

this study.

References

Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-

being: America’s perception of life quality. New York: Plenum

Press.

Argyle, M. (2001). The psychology of happiness (2nd ed.). Hove,

East Sussex: Routledge.

Batson, C., Fultz, J., Schoenrade, P., & Paduano, A. (1987). Critical

self-reflection and self-perceived altruism: When self-reward

fails. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 594 – 602.

Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being.

Chicago: Alpine.

Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal

attributions across cultures: Variation and universality. Psycho-

logical Bulletin, 125, 47 – 63.

Cross, S., Gore, J., & Morris, M. (2003). The relational-

interdependent self-construal, self-concept consistency, and

well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,

933 – 944.

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larson, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985).

The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assess-

ment, 49, 71 – 75.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999).

Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological

Bulletin, 125, 276 – 302.

Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S.,

Nishida, T., Kim, K., & Heyman, S. (1994, July). Measuring

self-construals across cultures. Paper presented at the Inter-

national Communication Association Convention. Sydney,

Australia.

Hackman, M., Ellis, K., Johnson, C., & Staley, C. (1999). Self-

construal orientation: Validation of an instrument and a study

of the relationship to leadership communication style. Com-

munication Quarterly, 47, 183 – 195.

Heller, D., Watson, D., & Ilies, R. (2004). The role of person

versus situation in life satisfaction: A critical examination.

Psychological Bulletin, 130, 574 – 600.

Hixon, J., & Swann, W. (1993). When does introspection bear

fruit? Self-refection, self-insight, and interpersonal choices.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 35 – 43.

Ho, R. (2006). Handbook of univariate and multivariate data ana-

lysis and interpretation with SPSS. Florida, USA: Chapman &

Hall.

Joireman, J. (2004). Empathy and the self-absorption paradox II:

Self-rumination and self-reflection as mediators between

shame, guilt, and empathy. Self and Identity, 3, 225 – 238.

Kurman, J. (2002). Measured cross-cultural differences in self-

enhancement and the sensitivity of the self-enhancement

measure to the modesty response. Cross-Cultural Research, 36,

73 – 95.

Kwan, S. Y., & Bond, M. H. (1997). Pancultural explanations for

life satisfaction: Adding relationship harmony to self-esteem.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1038 – 1051.

Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than

others? The role of cognitive and motivational processes in

well-being. American Psychologist, 56, 239 – 249.

Lyubomirsky, S., & Lepper, H. (1999). A measure of subjective

happiness: Preliminary reliability and construct validation.

Social Indicators Research, 46, 137 – 155.

Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuat-

ing properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 65, 339 – 349.

Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1999). Changes in attractiveness of

elected, rejected, and precluded alternatives: A comparison of

happy and unhappy individuals. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 76, 988 – 1007.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self:

Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psycho-

logical Review, 98, 224 – 253.

Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith of happy

people. American Psychologist, 55, 56 – 67.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., McBride, A., & Larson, J. (1997). Rumina-

tion and psychological distress among bereaved partners. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 855 – 862.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination

and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood.

Cognition and Emotion, 7, 561 – 570.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. (1993).

Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed

mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20 – 28.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative

coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92 – 104.

Poasa, K. H., Mallinckrodt, B., & Suzuki, L. A. (2000). Causal

attributions for problematic family interactions: A qualitative,

cultural comparison of Western Samoa, American Samoa, and

the United States. Counselling Psychologist, 28, 32 – 60.

Self-construal and happiness 133

Page 8: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness

Preacher, K. J., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2001). An interactive

calculation tool for mediation tests. Retrieved 26 April 2007,

from http://www.psych.ku.edu/preacher/sobel/sobel.htm

Sheldon, K., & King, L. (2001). Why positive psychology is

necessary. American Psychologist, 56, 216 – 217.

Trapnell, P., & Campbell, J. (1999). Private self-consciousness

and the five factor model of personality: Distinguishing

rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 76, 284 – 304.

Appendix

The Self-Reflection and the Self-Rumination Scales

The Self-Reflection Scale (SReS)

Please indicate your level of agreement or disagreement with each of the following statements by recording your

rating number on the space next to each statement.

Please note that:

1¼Strongly Disagree, 2¼Disagree, 3¼Barely Disagree, 4¼Barely Agree, 5¼Agree, 6¼Strongly Agree

1_______________2_____________3_____________4______________5______________6

Strongly Disagree Disagree Barely Disagree Barely Agree Agree Strongly Agree

1. Knowing myself is very important to me. ______

2. I am self-focused by nature. ______

3. My attitudes and feelings are the focus of my attention. ______

4. I often analyse why I do things. ______

5. I don’t care much for self-analysis. ______

6. I often think what my feelings might mean to me. ______

7. Contemplating myself is something I don’t do very often. ______

8. I often explore my ‘‘inner’’ self. ______

9. I don’t really care for introspective or self-reflective thinking. ______

10. When I contemplate things that happen to me, I often focus

on my ‘‘inner’’ thoughts and feelings. ______

11. I often contemplate my current moods. ______

12. I often reflect on outcomes of my actions. ______

The Self-Rumination Scale (SRuS)

Please indicate your level of agreement or disagreement with each of the following statements by recording your

rating number on the space next to each statement.

Please note that:

1¼Strongly Disagree, 2¼Disagree, 3¼Barely Disagree, 4¼Barely Agree, 5¼Agree, 6¼Strongly Agree

1______________2______________3______________4______________5______________6

Strongly Disagree Disagree Barely Disagree Barely Agree Agree Strongly Agree

1. It is easy for me to put unwanted thoughts out of mind. _____

2. I spend a great deal of time thinking back over my

embarrassing or disappointing moments. _____

3. My attention is often focused on aspects of myself I wish I’d stop thinking about. _____

4. I tend to dwell over unpleasant things that happen to me for

a long time afterwards. _____

5. Long after an argument or disagreement is over, my

thoughts keep going back to what happened. _____

6. Often I’m playing back over in my mind how I acted in an embarrassing situation. _____

7. I often analyse my mistakes. _____

8. Sometimes it is hard for me to shut off thoughts about my faults. _____

9. I often reflect on unfavourable outcomes in my life. _____

10. I often find myself re-evaluating something I have done wrong. _____

134 I. Elliott & S. Coker

Page 9: Independent self-construal, self-reflection, and self-rumination: A path model for predicting happiness