Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    1/14

    Ideas in

    American

    PolicingNumber 16

    June 2013

    Ideas in American Policingpresents commentary and insight rom leading crimi-

    nologists on issues o interest to scholars, practitioners, and policy makers. The

    papers published in this series are rom the Police Foundation lecture series o the

    same name. Points o view in this document are those o the author and do not

    necessarily represent the ocial position o the Police Foundation. The ull series

    is available online at http://www.policeoundation.org/docs/library.html.

    2013 Police Foundation. All rights reserved.

    James J. Willis is an associate proessor in the Department o

    Criminology, Law and Society at George Mason University and

    a ormer member o the Police Foundation Research Advisory

    Committee. His research interests include police organizations,

    police reorm, police technology, and police decision making.

    POLICE

    The ly so short, the crat so long to learn.Georey Chaucer

    To put it bluntly, it is not likely that police work generally, and

    the work o individual ocers, will be appreciated at its actualvaluethat is as a service o being complex, important and

    serious, until we begin to give a damn whether it is done well.Egon Bittner

    Improving Police: Whats

    Craft Got to Do with It?By James J. Willis

    Over the last century

    or so, the police have

    been the object o

    almost continuous and intensive

    attempts at reorm. Currently,

    one o the most powerul orces

    or transorming what thepolice do and how they do it

    is the evidence-based policing

    movement, an approach that

    challenges the police to base their

    actions on scientic evidence

    about what works. This puts

    scientic research squarely in the

    drivers seat o police decision

    making, unlike past reorms that

    have aspired to proessionalism

    by ocusing on the legal and

    administrative eatures o the

    police environment (Klockars

    1988). For example, the origins

    o the proessional policing

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    2/14

    2

    model that dominated much

    o the twentieth century lay in

    the implementation o laws,

    organizational policies, and

    departmental rules. As eatures

    o bureaucratic organization,

    their purpose was to strengthen

    accountability and to infuencethe choices made by patrol

    ocers and the organizations that

    employed them (Reiss 1992).1

    Community policing, the most

    popular reorm o the last ew

    decades, then sought to reverse

    this trend by assigning a greater

    role to the needs and concerns

    o local communities or guiding

    police actions (Mastroski andGreene 1993, 80).

    Whatever the reorm

    approach, it is a common lament

    among those seeking to improve

    policing that the policing

    crat, or the culmination o

    knowledge based on hands-on

    experience, is a eature o police

    culture that poses a ormidable

    obstacle to implementing new

    policies and practices. Supporterso community- and problem-

    oriented policing, two recent

    and highly-touted reorms

    (Goldstein 1990; Skogan 2006),

    have expressed this concern,

    as have advocates o evidence-

    based policing. For example,

    Cynthia Lum (2009) notes in

    a previous Ideas in American

    Policinglecture that despiteresearch demonstrating the crime

    control benets o concentrating

    police ocers in high-crime

    areas or hot spots, there is little

    indication that police agencies

    have actually tried to reallocate

    their patrol resources accordingly.

    Here she echoes a lament made

    over twenty-ve years ago by

    Lawrence Sherman, a leader

    in the evidence-based policing

    movement, about police ocers

    general resistance to scientic

    discovery. Despite evidence

    showing that arresting batterersin misdemeanor domestic

    violence incidents in Minneapolis

    was the most eective response

    or reducing uture oending,

    patrol ocers in the department

    said they would continue to

    use their standard responses,

    including talking to both parties

    or asking one to leave (Sherman

    1984, 75).2 While proponentso evidence-based reorm are

    careul to avoid attributing a

    reluctance to embrace research to

    a single cause, it is clear that they

    consider the lower status that

    police ocers assign to science

    than to crat as a signicant

    impediment to reorm (Sherman

    1984, 1998; Weisburd 2008;

    Lum 2009).Today it appears that

    scholars are being attracted

    in increasing numbers toward

    the evidence-based movement

    and policymakers, such as the

    United States Department o

    Justice, are encouraging police

    to do the same.3 Consequently,

    1 My comments in this essayconcentrate on patrol ocers. Thus,policing and police work reer to theactivities o these ront-line practitionersunless otherwise noted.

    2 Subsequent replications o thisresearch at additional sites revealedmore complex ndings, including thecontribution o arrest to uture domestic

    violence under some circumstances(Sherman 1992).

    3 For example, the Oce o JusticePrograms Bureau o Justice Assistance

    Web site provides resources on evidence-based approaches and practices (http://

    www.ojp.usdoj.gov/BJA/evaluation/evidence-based.htm).

    Improvements in policing

    rest heavily on the shoulders

    o those who do policing atthe coalace, and patrol

    ocers have long thought

    o the way they perorm

    their work as a crat.

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    3/14

    3

    it would seem to be a good time

    to reconsider the value o the

    police crat in relationship to

    police science (Bayley and Bittner

    1984). Improvements in policing

    rest heavily on the shoulders o

    those who do policing at thecoalace, and patrol ocers have

    long thought o the way they

    perorm their work as a crat

    (Wilson 1978, 283). Thus, unless

    more attention is given to the

    crat aspects o policing within

    the context o the evidence-based

    movement, it is unlikely that

    eorts to integrate science with

    policing will deliver the resultsthat reormers desire (Weisburd

    and Neyroud 2011). Ultimately,

    any attempt to improve police

    perormance must take into

    account the views o those who

    constitute any departments largest

    resource and their understanding

    o what constitutes superior police

    work (Skogan 2008).

    Some might argue thatcrat and science already work

    well together, noting that the

    relationship between police

    researchers and practitioners has

    improved substantially over the

    past ew decades (Bayley 2008).

    This might be, but, aside rom

    a ew scholars (see Bayley and

    Bittner 1984; Mastroski 1996),

    not much attention has ocusedon examining howscientic and

    proessional knowledge might

    contribute to one another in

    mutually supportive ways in the

    context o street-level decision

    making. In this essay, I consider

    what a true marriage o crat and

    science might look like or guiding

    the decisions o rank-and-le

    ocers in two domains relevant

    to police practice: (1) advancing

    knowledge about what works,

    and (2) making decisions about

    the right thing to do. In doing

    so, I hope to illuminate somepossibilities or reorm that

    policymakers, practitioners, and

    researchers might wish to consider

    in their eorts to improve the

    police o the uture.

    Let Me Introduce

    Our Couple, Science

    and CraftFrom the perspective o evidence-

    based policing, it is social science

    that promises to revolutionize

    the use o police discretion

    (Sherman 1984, 61). A glance

    through any research methods

    textbook reveals that social

    science encompasses a range

    o methodologies. However,

    the scientic gold standard othe evidence-based policing

    movement is the experimental

    study, as it is the most rigorous

    methodological tool or

    determining whether a causal

    relationship exists between a

    particular treatment and a desired

    outcome (Sampson 2010). I you

    want to learn whether problem-

    oriented policing is more eectivethan directed patrol or reducing

    crime at hot spots (Taylor,

    Koper, and Woods 2011), or

    whether arrest is the best option

    or reducing recidivism in

    domestic violence cases (Sherman

    1992), then randomized trials

    are your best hope. This view

    o police science conjures an

    image o the police proessional

    as a technical expert, someone

    whose eorts to solve crime and

    disorder problems are infuenced

    powerully by scientic research.

    Indeed, Lawrence Sherman(1984, 7677) has envisioned

    police ocers making street-level

    decisions by accessing research

    results on laptop computers in

    their patrol cars. Having entered

    data about a particular suspect, a

    preprogrammed algorithm would

    advise the patrol ocer on the

    best course o action based on

    the likely eects o the ocersactions on the suspects behavior.

    From the perspective o

    crat, proessionalism is dened

    quite dierently. Experience,

    not scientic knowledge, is the

    oundation o eective police

    work. By encountering a variety

    o situations and people over

    time, patrol ocers learn valuable

    practical knowledge and developspecic skills. Some o these

    situations might seem clear cut,

    like dealing with a bank robbery

    or other violent crime in progress

    but others, such as domestic

    disturbances or trac stops, are

    more complex and uncertain.

    This makes the decision about

    how best to respond much more

    challenging.Under these conditions,

    what is embraced is situated

    knowledge that oers an ocer

    immediate guidance about the

    constraints and possibilities or

    responding to a specic incident

    (Thacher 2008, 51). Crat

    places a high value on fexibility

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    4/14

    4

    to t the right response to the

    particulars o the situation and

    does not necessarily demand

    orthodoxy in response. It also

    recognizes that what works

    well or one ocer might not

    work so well or another due todierences in skills and personal

    traits. Scientic knowledge

    predicting the likely outcomes o

    a specic action is certainly useul,

    but patrol ocers generally

    assign much greater importance

    to knowledge o laws and rules

    and in-depth understanding o

    people, places, and events. The

    ormer helps dene the nature othe problem and the outer limits

    o the police ocers authority

    and responsibilities (Mastroski,

    Willis, and Revier 2011, 16), and

    the latter increases an ocers

    ability to predict intention and

    behavior when conronting an

    unamiliar setting or the rst

    time (Bittner 1990, 252).

    Crat combines thisknowledge with a specic set o

    skills. For patrol ocers, these

    include the ability to remain calm

    under pressure, to talk and listen

    to people, to use orce sparingly,

    and to exercise good judgment

    by weighing up a complex set

    o actors beore coming to a

    reasoned decision (Bayley and

    Bittner 1984; Fielding 1984;Kritzer 2007, 335; Muir 1977).

    Those who have mastered these

    tools o the trade are regarded by

    their peers as master cratsmen or

    women, that is, or being cool,

    poised, inventive, careul, active,

    and nonviolentocers who

    can cope without jeopardizing

    themselves or others (Bayley and

    Bittner 1984, 5152).

    In contrast to a computer-

    driven robocop, the crat image

    o the proessional police ocer

    is o someone who thinks quickly

    on her eet to behave in waysthat are wise, compassionate, and

    air. Under some circumstances,

    especially when an ocer senses

    immediate danger, choices are

    made automatically and intuitively

    (Sherman 2012), but decision

    making also involves considerable

    observational and analytic rigor,

    and even admits the possibility

    that creativity in devising acourse o action may be valuable

    (Muir 1977, 189259). Thus,

    when police ocers make street-

    level choices, they combine

    experience-based intuition and

    science-like analytic strategies or

    thinking both ast and slow

    (Kahneman 2011).

    What Might a GoodMarriage Between

    Science and Craft

    Look Like?

    This image o science and crat

    vying or supremacy, in the

    hope that springs eternal rom

    American police reormers desire

    to improve policing, generates

    at least three distinct ways thatscience and crat may be coupled

    together. In the rst coupling,

    science is dominant and crat is

    suppressed. While this model

    is not unduly dismissive o

    experience as a guide to police

    action (Sherman 1984, 62), it

    emphasizes the limitations o

    intuitive knowledge and expresses

    alarm about its potential or

    negative consequences (Lum

    2009). From an evidence-

    based policing standpoint,

    policing as a crat projects an

    overly romantic conception o adiligent and skillul practitioner

    making judicious decisions. In

    doing so, it ails to consider

    ully that even the most well-

    respected patrol ocers have

    a limited range o experiences

    to draw upon and oten lack

    the ability to learn much about

    the long-term consequences o

    their actions (that is, beyondwhat they are able to observe

    during an encounter) (Sherman

    1984). Where science is absent,

    ocers are ree to dispense their

    own version o justice based

    on any number o problematic

    or unethical actors, including

    guesswork, personal biases, and

    oensive stereotypes (Lum 2009,

    3; Maynard-Moody and Musheno2003). Moreover, because

    ocers oten do not have reliable

    knowledge about the results o

    their actions, what they do, no

    matter how well intentioned, may

    be ineectual or even harmul to

    suspects, victims, and oenders

    (Sherman 1984, 64).

    These are important

    criticisms, but there are alsolimitations to sciences capacity

    to dictate decision making.

    Leaving aside the act that

    police may simply not trust

    scientic ndings, supporters

    o good scientic evaluations

    oten attribute the ailure o

    practitioners to embrace science

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    5/14

    5

    to their not being aware o

    relevant ndings published in

    academic journals, or to not

    understanding the orm in which

    they are delivered (Birkeland,

    Murphy-Graham, and Weiss

    2005). The obvious solution isto disseminate knowledge about

    what works more widely and

    in more digestible ormats

    (Lum, Telep, Koper, and Grieco

    2012). Clear examples o such

    an approach are the recent

    creation o the Oce o Justice

    Programs CrimeSolutions.gov

    Web site to highlight and rate

    the eectiveness o dierentcriminal justice approaches on a

    straightorward color-coded scale,

    and the Evidence-Based Policing

    Matrix summarizing the eects o

    dierent crime control strategies

    in a simple three-dimensional

    cube.4

    No doubt distrust and the

    unavailability and inaccessibility

    o scientic ndings help explainsome o the gap between

    research, policy, and practice,

    but there are other good

    reasons why crat may be dea

    to sciences exhortations. One

    is the crucial recognition that

    police organizations and the

    ocers who work or them are

    expected to accomplish multiple

    goals at once. The evidence-based movement has tended to

    identiy crime reduction as the

    primary end o policing, but

    police work is characterized by

    a swath o values or ends, such

    as equity, legitimacy, liberty, and

    eciency, which oten confict

    (Thacher 2001, 392). Under

    these conditions, scientic

    knowledge that identies the

    best means to a given end, suchas crime control, oers useul

    guidance, but it cannot resolve

    these value conficts and thus

    serve as a rm guide to action

    (Thacher 2001, 389). This

    is particularly true or patrol

    ocers working at the street

    level who must try to reconcile

    the numerous requirements that

    justice demands, say betweenbeing responsive to the needs and

    wishes o a victim while reducing

    the risk o uture oending. For

    example, in the case o a minor

    assault, should the ocer make

    an arrest when a husbandthe

    amilys only breadwinnerswears

    that he will never slap his wie

    again and she supports his claim

    while pleading or leniency?

    There are also times when

    an ocers judgment should

    override the dictates o even

    well-established scientic ndings.For example, we probably do not

    want police ocers to single out

    domestic violence oenders or

    arrest based on their employment

    status or where they live, even i

    evidence suggests that arresting

    those with jobs or who live

    in afuent areas is the most

    eective means or reducing the

    risk o uture violence (Sherman1998, 8). Finally, social science at

    best improves the odds o success

    o a given sort but, given the

    complexities o human behavior,

    it rarely guarantees it and it is

    ar rom matching the predictive

    power o the natural sciences

    (Gutting 2012). Sherman (1984,4 http://gemini.gmu.edu/cebcp/

    The evidence-based movement

    has tended to identiy crime

    reduction as the primary endo policing, but police work

    is characterized by a swath

    o values or ends, such as

    equity, legitimacy, liberty, and

    eciency, which oten confict.

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    6/14

    6

    1998) requently acknowledges

    this act, but not in the context

    o subjecting to empirical testing

    the assumption that social science

    is superior to crat in producing

    desirable outcomes.

    In the second coupling

    between crat and science, crat is

    dominant and science is missingor, i it is present, it is merely

    a presentational strategy or

    justiying traditional police

    policies and practices (Manning

    1992, 365). That is, science

    is not really driving decision

    making, at least in the ways it

    is supposed to. Compstat could

    be considered an example o

    this version o the science-crat relationship. Implemented

    in the New York City Police

    Department in 1994, Compstat is

    an inormation and management

    tool that tallies and maps crime

    statistics to hold command sta

    accountable or crime levels

    in their beats. According to

    Compstat doctrine, police are

    supposed to go beyond their

    own experiences and to apply

    innovations in crime prevention

    theory and research in order to

    solve crime problems (Bratton

    1998). In practice, however,

    in-depth research conducted

    at multiple sites has shownthat despite the availability o

    electronic maps, timely crime

    data, and crime analysis, police

    continued to rely heavily upon

    what they have learned in the

    course o their careers about

    where crime occurs and how best

    to respond. Rather than careully

    assessing a range o promising

    alternatives beore selecting themost eective crime strategy,

    district commanders requently

    used tactics they have tried in the

    past and that they believed work,

    such as saturating an area with

    patrol or increasing arrests (Willis,

    Mastroski, and Weisburd 2007).

    In this case, impressive electronic

    maps and crime statistics

    the harbingers o science

    help coner legitimacy on police

    actions while the experience-

    based aspects o police work

    continue to hold sway.

    A third model regards cratand science as a true marriage,

    where each partner bestows

    equal worth on the other and

    has a rightul place in guiding

    the decisions o the rank-and-

    le ocer. In the next section, I

    try to envision what a successul

    marriage o this sort would look

    like. Which partnercrat or

    sciencedoes what, and howmight some dierences between

    the two be resolved?

    Advancing Knowledge

    About What Works

    In the rst instance, police

    science could probably do more

    to pay attention to police crat

    in order to validate what worksand under what conditions.

    In ocusing so much o its

    energy on identiying the best

    means to preventing crime, the

    evidence-based movement has

    improved understanding about

    how police can contribute to

    public saety. In comparison,

    it has largely overlooked the

    many concerns ocers musttake into account when making

    a decision, including the many

    dierent tactics that are available

    to them (Bayley and Bittner

    1984). Egon Bittner noted long

    ago that the work o ront-line

    police ocers could not be

    adequately captured and assessed

    In the rst instance, police

    science could probably do

    more to pay attention to

    police crat in order to

    validate what works and

    under what conditions.

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    7/14

    7

    in such simple terms as ghting

    crime or enorcing laws (Bittner

    1970). In their encounters

    with the public, ocers must

    consider other important goals,

    including preventing disputes

    rom escalating, ensuring saetyat the scene, and responding

    adequately to legitimate citizen

    needs (Bayley and Bittner 1984;

    Mastroski 1996). Little is still

    known about the best treatments

    available or accomplishing

    these kinds o goals. Albert

    Reiss (1995, 103) noted with

    surprise almost twenty years

    ago that interpersonal confictsand disputes were the cause o

    much violence but that little is

    known about the eectiveness

    o police in preventing the

    occurrence o dierent kinds

    o disputes or their escalation

    into crimes when they occur.

    Little seems to have changed

    in the interim, suggesting that

    researchers need to do moreto learn rom practitioners

    themselves at close range, that

    is, through direct observations

    and interviews, about the goals

    they identiy as most important

    in any given situation and the

    specic tactics they use or their

    accomplishment. Ethnographers,

    such as Egon Bittner and William

    Muir, have provided valuableinsights into how patrol ocers

    use their discretion but not as

    part o a sel-conscious attempt to

    propose standardized treatments

    and then subject them to

    empirical evaluation.

    In the absence o scientic

    attempts to identiy, analyze,

    and validate much o what

    experience has taught patrol

    ocers, practitioners currently

    must rely on what they have

    learned by listening to old hands

    in their agency or through their

    acquisition o personal experience.As Bayley and Bittner (1984, 47)

    observed almost thirty years ago,

    those interested in improving

    the quality o police work

    stand to gain much by using

    systematic ways to identiy and

    test scientically the operational

    imperatives that ocers consider

    important in their daily work

    dealing with the public.Like a good marriage, the

    willingness o both partners to

    consider alternative perspectives

    on any given issue beore making

    judgments is an important

    criterion or success. In this

    regard, proponents o science

    or crat should not just assume

    that one is more eective than

    the other in leading to desirableoutcomes. This leads crat or

    science to merely proselytize to

    one another rather than using

    evidence and reason to try to

    resolve where their dierences

    lie. Thus, it would be ruitul or

    science and crat to consider the

    assumption that one is superior

    to the other to be a hypothesis

    worth testing, but to date thisidea has received little attention.

    To some degree, this squabble

    could be overcome by conducting

    rigorous evaluations that compare

    police discretion that is exercised

    using science to police discretion

    exercised using crat (and some

    combination o the two).

    One could envision an

    experiment that uses a variety

    o treatment conditions. Some

    ocers could be provided with

    access to scientic knowledge that

    they are prepared to use in concert

    with the kind o computer-aideddiscretion model espoused by

    Sherman and discussed earlier.

    Ocers assigned to this group

    would be required to have their

    discretion governed by scientic

    ndings. In responding to a

    particular situation, such as a

    domestic dispute, a computer

    would advise ocers about the

    preerred strategy given theiranswers to a set o relevant

    questions that might include the

    oenders prior record and the

    seriousness o the oense.

    A second treatment condition

    might be a special training and

    supervision program that brings

    out the best o what crat has to

    inorm ocers about what to do.

    Bayley and Bittner (1984, 54)propose such a model when they

    recommend that those ocers

    identied as master cratsmen or

    women are used to train their

    peers on those skills they consider

    most important to producing

    high quality police work.

    Supervision by these experienced

    and respected cratspeople might

    include regular debriengs,especially ollowing situations

    that an ocer ound particularly

    challenging. Their purpose would

    be to allow ocers to discuss and

    seek counsel on their decision-

    making process, including the

    accuracy o the initial diagnosis o

    the situation, the appropriateness

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    8/14

    8

    o the goals being pursued, and

    the strengths and limitations

    o the tactics used or their

    accomplishment.

    A third treatment condition

    could be a hybrid approach,

    where ocers are exposedto scientic evidence and are

    prepared to use it, but they are

    also exposed to the best that

    crat has to oer. These ocers

    are then given the reedom to

    ashion their actions to best t

    particular circumstances. The

    control condition would be that

    the department provides no more

    guidance than is usually availableto ocers through its existing

    rules, training, supervisory

    practices, and perormance review

    process.

    The experiment could be

    designed to measure a range o

    outcomes, including reductions

    in crime and disorder and also

    citizen satisaction or alienation.

    Thus, inormation could beprovided not just on which o

    these approaches is more likely

    to produce desirable results, but

    which o these produces more

    desirable results.

    The experiment should also

    incorporate a process evaluation

    or patrol ocers to provide

    eedback to researchers about

    how well each method workedand what were the most helpul

    or dicult aspects o doing each.

    In addition to helping illuminate

    why a particular innovation did

    or, as importantly, did not work

    (since inormation about ailures

    is equally valuable to advancing

    reorm), this eedback can open

    up potentially ruitul avenues or

    uture scholarly inquiry (Willis

    and Mastroski 2011). Moreover,

    here crat might be particularly

    useul in identiying the specic

    circumstances under which one

    response is likely to be moreor less eective. Replications o

    the Minneapolis experiment on

    domestic violence showed that

    the eects o arrest depended on

    the status o oenders and the

    degree to which they experienced

    procedural justice (Sherman

    1992), but similar insights can

    be revealed by tapping requently

    and systematically into the richvein o practitioners experiences

    and then testing them.

    But it is not just the oender

    who matters in predicting

    outcomes. Ethnographies and

    surveys reveal that ocers

    generally believe that what works

    best or one ocer might not

    work well or another based on

    the skills and characteristics othe individual ocer doing the

    job (Muir 1977; Mastroski,

    Willis, and Revier 2011). These

    characteristics include an ocers

    gender, amount o experience,

    physical size, and verbal acility.

    Identiying and then using science

    to measure these interactive

    eects might reveal that dierent

    ocer styles are equally eectiveand thereore provide a range

    o alternative responses rom

    which to choose. In turn, such

    customization o responses

    could increase the prospects that

    any research would actually be

    incorporated into ocers daily

    decision making by eschewing a

    one-size-ts-all approach (Bayley

    and Bittner 1984, 51). In short,

    validating what works, under

    what conditions, and by whom

    would help strengthen the bond

    between science and crat by

    serving the needs and perspectiveso ront-line workers directly.

    In doing so, it would redress

    a current imbalance in police

    research that tends to ocus on

    the kinds o program evaluations

    that are more relevant to

    policymakers and police managers

    than street-level decision makers

    (Thacher 2008).

    Deciding on the

    Right Thing to Do

    The marriage between science

    and crat could be urther

    improved by partisans rom

    both sides working more closely

    together to help police ocers

    deliver more justice than is

    currently on oer. To date, socialscientists have commonly ignored

    the undamental normative

    component to assessing the

    quality o work that street-

    level patrol ocers perorm,

    preerring to ocus on explaining

    and predicting variations in

    what police do and how they

    do it (e.g., use o orce, arrests).

    This trend toward separatingact rom value, or how much

    rom how well, extends back

    at least as ar as Max Weber,

    who asserted that while science

    could oer actual statements

    and causal explanations, it was

    incapable o resolving questions

    about important public values

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    9/14

    9

    or about what ought to be

    (Thacher 2006). According

    to Weber, answers to these

    questions ultimately depended

    on ones particular moral or

    political outlook and could not

    be validated empirically. Theproblem here, o course, is that

    measuring the quantity o police

    work an ocer perorms tells us

    very little about its quality, or

    whether or not an ocer uses his

    or her discretion to do the right

    thing. A consequence o this

    traditional divide is that science

    is virtually silent on those aspects

    o police work that matter mostto police leaders, their ocers,

    and the communities they serve.

    This is a major oversight. Over

    the last ew decades, many police

    leaders have demonstrated a

    clear commitment to promoting

    better policing (Bayley 2008),

    patrol ocers have expressed

    interest in more sophisticated

    approaches to assessing theirperormance than simple tallies

    o work outputs (Mastroski,

    Willis, and Revier 2011), and

    research has shown that citizens

    care mightily about the quality

    o treatment they receive in their

    personal encounters with police

    ocers (Tyler 2004; Bottoms

    and Tankebe 2012). Surely as

    researchers we can do more tolearn rom patrol ocers and

    help them make better choices.

    Take, or example, two

    patrol ocers responding to a

    dispute between two neighbors

    in an apartment building. A

    complainant is upset that the

    woman living in the adjacent

    apartment, who may or may

    not be suering rom mental

    illness, has been pounding on

    her door with a fat iron and

    physically threatening her. When

    questioned, the woman with the

    fat iron says she is rustrated

    by the complainants tendencyto slam her door when entering

    and leaving. The ocers express

    puzzlement that this should be

    the cause o so much hostility,

    but they do not explore this in

    detail with the second neighbor.

    They advise the complainant that

    she should get a summons, and

    advise the second woman that

    she could also get a summonsbut that she is not allowed to

    take the law into her own hands

    and retaliate by damaging the

    complainants door. When she

    remains deant, they tell her

    that i they have to return that

    night, she will be taken down

    to the precinct. Having given

    this warning, the ocers leave,

    disposing o the fat iron in a

    nearby trash can as they wait or

    an elevator. The entire encounter

    lasts less than ten minutes.

    This is the kind o run-o-

    the-mill dispute that characterizes

    everyday police work, but whatis the best response in this

    particular case? Is it delivering

    a sense o justice to the citizens

    involved? Resolving the

    underlying cause o the dispute?

    Giving these citizens the capacity

    to solve this problem without

    summoning the law? Making

    costly police resources available

    to those who are in greater needo police services? This dispute

    is taken rom a video clip5 o

    an actual incident, which my

    colleagues and I showed to

    The marriage between science

    and crat could be urther

    improved by partisans rom

    both sides working more

    closely together to help police

    ocers deliver more justice

    than is currently on oer.

    5 This clip is rom The Police Tapes(1976), which was produced, edited, anddirected by Alan and Susan Raymond.

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    10/14

    10

    patrol ocers as part o a studyexamining how they judge the

    quality o the work that they

    perorm (Willis et al. 2011).

    What we discovered was that

    there was little consensus about

    what constituted good police

    work, suggesting that at present

    work quality at the street level is

    let largely to the will and skill o

    the individual patrol ocer.While it is impossible to

    generalize rom a study o a

    single department, ocers

    responses to this clip appear

    to indicate that existing

    mechanisms may not be doing

    a very good job o promoting

    and advancing a common

    vision o what constitutesgood policing. There are good

    reasons or this. Available

    options or oering practical

    guidance, namely, bureaucratic

    rules and laws and, as has been

    identied here, increasing

    scientic knowledge, can oer

    only partial solutions as they

    are challenged by their general

    qualities (Marx 2006, 280).The art o street-level decision

    making is in guring how, i at

    all, these can be applied to the

    contextual richness o individual

    cases where inormation is

    oten limited, inchoate, and

    conficting, and where there is

    pressure to act quickly (Schon

    1983). The advantage o crat

    is that it provides a stock o

    knowledge acquired through

    years o handling many dierent

    situations and contingencies, and

    yet this source is seldom tapped

    systematically and made availableas a source o guidance. What

    is more, science can play an

    important role in this process.

    Take the disgruntled neighbor

    scenario described above. The

    video clip could be shown to a

    group o ocers who have been

    recruited or their skillul work.

    They could be asked to judge the

    quality o the ocers responsein the clip and to identiy the

    strengths and weaknesses o the

    approach on display. A police

    researcher could play a useul role

    in this process by helping identiy

    and clariy the major concerns

    that arise. Based on what we

    heard during our interviews,

    these might include a range o

    dimensions as shown in the tableon this page.

    Working in this collaborative

    environment, the group could

    then be asked to identiy more

    or less desirable responses, with

    the researcher oering insights

    based on scientic theory and

    evidence, particularly regarding

    potentially harmul consequences.

    It would also be necessary to tryto establish priorities among these

    criteria in order to make their

    application useul to others, a

    tough challenge but one that it is

    still possible. Studies on the crat-

    based culture o policing have

    oten examined its undesirable

    or negative eatures, such as

    Possible Dimensions for MeasuringQuality of Police Performance

    Procedural

    justice

    To what degree were the victim and

    oender treated airly and respectully?

    Problem

    diagnosis

    Did the ocers demonstrate good skills

    in identiying the nature o the problem?

    Safety and orderat the scene

    Was the potential or immediate violence

    adequately controlled?

    Lawfulness

    of response

    Was their response justied by the

    evidence?

    Distributivejustice

    Was the resolution air to allstakeholders?

    Economy Did the ocers spend too much ortoo little time on the dispute?

    Prospects for

    future risk

    Did the ocers reduce the potential or

    uture problems between the two?

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    11/14

    11

    alienation rom the department

    hierarchy or hostility toward

    the public, but ethnographies

    show that good ocers develop

    intellectual and moral virtues that

    help them weigh the exigencies

    o any given situation and makesophisticated judgments about

    the best thing to do (Muir 1977,

    189224). Providing a orum

    that encourages thoughtul

    deliberation about these

    judgments and the trade-os

    they imply would help clariy

    priorities among dierent value

    systems. Anticipating potential

    conficts ahead o time would alsohelp guide discretion in the eld,

    as trying to resolve these while

    under pressure to act quickly is

    very dicult. In the case o the

    disgruntled neighbor, while the

    ocers were certainly attentive

    and polite, a persuasive case could

    be made that their gracious and

    ecient manner took undue

    precedence over eorts tominimize the possibility o uture

    confict. In our interviews, some

    respondents were surprised that

    the ocers seemed satised with

    leaving the problem in the same

    state they had ound it. There

    will certainly be disagreements

    about how to best handle

    these kinds o disputes, but, by

    identiying what the relevantvalues are in a particular context,

    exploring their meanings, and

    clariying which should take

    priority, it should be possible

    to justiy some responses as

    superior to others. Furthermore,

    acknowledging where the

    tensions lie between dierent

    uses o discretion and identiying

    acceptable levels o compromise

    would also help inorm decision

    making.

    Police leaders, especially

    chies, rst-line supervisors,

    recruit trainers, and eld trainingocers, should play a key role

    in this process. Through their

    participation, they can help

    establish the important public

    values that the organization

    should pursue and inspire others

    to embrace them. What is oten

    lost in discussions about sciences

    role in governing practice is the

    vital contribution a coherentand enlightened philosophy can

    make to police work, and yet it

    is a valuable leadership trait or

    those in authoritative positions

    to establish the cultural tone o

    their organization by advancing

    a view o good policing that is

    transparent both to employees

    and to the public (Bass 1998).

    The next important stepwould be to subject these

    standards to empirical testing

    beore coming up with a

    strategy that allows or them

    to be applied in an operational

    setting (Mastroski 2007). It

    might be possible, or example,

    to construct a checklist that

    promotes memory recall about

    what is important and why andalso helps improve consistency, a

    key element o crat (Gatawande

    2009; Kritzer 2007). While

    guidelines cannot account or

    every contingency and will

    sometimes not work as intended,

    by providing structure they can

    increase the likelihood that patrol

    ocers will use their discretion

    in desirable ways (Kelling 1999).

    This approach to mobilizing crat

    could be used or a variety o

    encounters that are selected or

    being particularly problematic

    (like various domestic disputes),or because they are commonplace

    (trac stops, or example). The

    availability o body cameras

    easily allows or this kind o

    naturalistic observation, as well

    as opportunities or supervisors

    to give eedback on ocers

    perormance beore and ater

    the implementation o this

    discretionary tool.

    Conclusion

    I have suggested that we want

    our police ocers to act wisely

    and well, doing the kind o

    job that makes us step back

    in admiration at their capacity

    to make good judgments.

    Undoubtedly there are someocers who prompt this reaction,

    so perhaps it makes sense to

    take greater advantage o the

    insights they have to oer and to

    do more to assess their eects.

    The evidence-based movement

    has captured the attention o

    government and generated

    excitement about the possibilities

    or reorm, so this is a goodtime to use science to cultivate

    and test what accumulated

    experience has to oer. Oscar

    Wilde once remarked that the

    proper basis or marriage was

    mutual misunderstanding, and

    that happiness could not be

    ound within its bounds. This

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    12/14

    12

    does not have to be the case

    or crat and science, with one

    looking past the other and

    lamenting missed opportunities.

    A uller appreciation o the

    qualities each brings to the other

    promises a much more satisyingand enduring relationship.

    Advancing reorm in ways that

    police administrators, ocers,

    researchers, and ordinary citizens

    all care about requires that we

    ocus on what can be gained by

    strengthening this union and not

    on the dierences that divide it.

    I would like to thank David

    Weisburd and Cynthia Lum or

    their thoughtul comments on

    the science and crat o policing.

    A special note o gratitude

    goes to Stephen Mastroski,

    the voice inside and outside o

    my head always pushing me to

    think harder. His comments on

    earlier drats on this essay were

    invaluable. Finally, I appreciate theassistance o Mary Malina, Police

    Foundation communications

    director, who oversaw this essays

    production. Any errors that

    remain are my own doing.

    References

    Bass, Bernard M. 1998.

    Transormational Leadership:

    Industry, Military, and Education

    Impact. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

    Erlbaum Associates.

    Bayley, David H., and Egon Bittner.

    1984. Learning the skills o

    policing. Law and Contemporary

    Problems47: 3559.

    Bayley, David H. 2008. Police reorm:

    Who done it? Policing and Society

    18: 717.

    Birkeland, Sarah, Erin Murphy-

    Graham, Carol Weiss. 2005. Good

    reasons or ignoring good

    evaluation: The case o the drugabuse resistance education

    (D.A.R.E.) program. Evaluation

    and Program Planning28: 24756.

    Bittner, Egon. 1970. The unctions o

    the police in modern society. Chevy

    Chase, MD: National Institute o

    Mental Health.

    Bittner, Egon. 1990. Aspects o Police

    Work. Boston, MA: Northeastern

    University Press.

    Bottoms, Anthony, and JusticeTankebe. 2012. Beyond procedural

    justice: A dialogic approach to

    legitimacy in criminal justice. The

    Journal o Criminal Law and

    Criminology102: 11970.

    Bratton, William J. (with Peter

    Knobler). 1998. Turnaround:

    How Americas Top Cop Reversed

    the Crime Epidemic. New York:

    Random House.

    Fielding, Nigel. 1984. Policesocialization and police competence.

    British Journal o Sociology35:

    56890.

    Gatawande, Atul. 2009. The Checklist

    Maniesto: How to Get Things Right.

    New York: Metropolitan Books.

    Goldstein, Herman. 1990. Problem-

    Oriented Policing. New York:

    McGraw-Hill.

    Gutting, Gary. 2012. How reliable are

    the social sciences? New York Times,

    17 May. http://opinionator.blogs.

    nytimes.com/2012/05/17/how

    -reliable-are-the-social-sciences

    /?emc=eta1

    Kahneman, Daniel. 2011. Thinking,

    Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar,

    Straus, and Giroux.

    Kelling, George L. 1999. Broken

    Windows and Police Discretion.

    Washington, DC: National Institute

    o Justice. http://www.ncjrs.gov

    /pdles1/nij/178259.pd

    Klockars, Carl. 1988. The rhetoric o

    community policing. In CommunityPolicing: Rhetoric or Reality, eds.

    Jack R. Greene and Stephen D.

    Mastroski. New York: Praeger.

    Kritzer, Herbert M. 2007. Toward a

    theorization o crat. Social and

    Legal Studies16: 32140.

    Lum, Cynthia. 2009. Translating

    Police Research into Practice. Ideas

    in American Policing, no. 11,

    August. Washington, DC: Police

    Foundation. http://www.policeoundation.org/pd/Ideas

    _Lum.pd.

    Lum, Cynthia, Cody Telep,

    Christopher Koper, and Julie

    Grieco. 2012. Receptivity to

    research in policing. Justice Research

    and Policy14: 6195.

    Marx, Gary T. 2006. Forget big

    brother and big corporation: What

    about the personal uses o

    surveillance technology as seen incases such as Tom I. Voire? Rutgers

    Journal o Law and Urban Policy3:

    219286.

    Manning, Peter K. 1992. Inormation

    technologies and the police. In

    Modern Policing, eds. Michael

    Tonry and Norval Morris. Chicago:

    University o Chicago Press.

    Maynard-Moody, Steven W., and

    Michael C. Musheno. 2003. Cops,

    Teachers, Counselors: Stories rom theFront Lines o Public Service. Ann

    Arbor: University o Michigan Press.

    Mastroski, Stephen D., and Jack R.

    Greene. 1993. Community policing

    and the rule o law. In Police

    Innovation and Control o the Police:

    Problems o Law, Order, and

    Community, eds. David Weisburd,

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    13/14

    13

    Craig Uchida, and Lorraine Green

    Mazerrolle. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Mastroski, Stephen D. 1996.

    Measuring police perormance in

    public encounters. In Quantiying

    Quality in Policing, Larry T.

    Hoover, ed. Washington, DC:Police Executive Research Forum.

    Mastroski, Stephen D. 2007. Police

    organization and management issues

    or the next decade. Paper presented

    at the National Institute o Justice

    Policing Research Workshop:

    Planning or the Future,

    Washington, DC, November 2829.

    https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdles1

    /nij/grants/218584.pd

    Mastroski, Stephen D., James J. Willis,and Lauren Revier. 2011. Results

    o a survey on high quality policing

    in Manassas City, VA. Fairax, VA:

    George Mason University,

    Department o Criminology,

    Law and Society.

    Muir, William K., Jr. 1977. Police:

    Streetcorner Politicians. Chicago:

    University o Chicago Press.

    Reiss, Albert, Jr. 1992. Police

    organization. In Modern Policing,eds. Michael Tonry and Norval

    Morris. Chicago: University o

    Chicago Press.

    Reiss, Albert, Jr. 1995. The role

    o police in crime prevention.

    In Integrating Crime Prevention

    Strategies: Propensity and

    Opportunity, eds. Per-Olo H.

    Wiskstrom, Ronald V. Clarke, and

    Joan McCord. Stockholm: National

    Council or Crime Prevention.

    Sampson, Robert J. 2010. Gold

    standard myths: Observations on

    the experimental turn in quantitative

    criminology. Journal o Quantitative

    Criminology26: 489500.

    Schon, Donald A. 1983. The Refective

    Practitioner: How Proessionals Think

    in Action. New York: Basic Books.

    Sherman, Lawrence W. 1984.

    Experiments in police discretion:

    Scientic boon or dangerous

    knowledge? Law and ContemporaryProblems47: 6181.

    Sherman, Lawrence W. 1992. Policing

    Domestic Violence: Experiments and

    Dilemmas. New York: The Free

    Press.

    Sherman, Lawrence W. 1998.

    Evidence-Based Policing. Ideas in

    American Policing, no. 2, July.

    Washington, DC: Police Foundation.

    http://www.policeoundation.org

    /pd/Sherman.pd.Sherman, Lawrence W. 2012. Policing,

    theory and evidence. Paper

    presented at the annual meeting

    o the American Society o

    Criminology, Chicago, November.

    Skogan, Wesley. 2006. Police and

    Community in Chicago: A Tale o

    Three Cities. Chicago: University

    o Chicago Press.

    Skogan, Wesley. 2008. Why reorms

    ail. Policing and Society18: 2334.

    Taylor, Bruce, Christopher S. Koper,

    Daniel J. Woods. 2011. A

    randomized controlled trial o

    dierent policing strategies at hot

    spots o violent crime. Journal

    o Experimental Criminology7:

    14981.

    Thacher, David. 2001. Policing is

    not a treatment. Journal o Research

    in Crime and Delinquency38:

    387415.

    Thacher, David. 2006. The normative

    case study.American Journal o

    Sociology111: 163176.

    Thacher, David. 2008. Research or

    the ront lines. Policing and Society

    18: 4659.

    Tyler, Tom R. 2004. Enhancing police

    legitimacy. The Annals o the

    American Academy o Political and

    Social Sciences593: 8499.

    Weisburd, David 2008. Place-Based

    Policing. Ideas in American Policing,

    no. 9, January. Washington, DC:Police Foundation. http://www

    .policeoundation.org/pd

    /placebasedpolicing.pd.

    Weisburd, David, and Peter Neyroud.

    2011. Police Science: Toward a New

    Paradigm. New Perspectives in

    Policing, Executive Sessions on

    Policing and Public Saety, January.

    Cambridge, MA: Harvard Kennedy

    School. http://www.hks.harvard

    .edu/var/ezp_site/storage

    /ckeditor/le/pds/centers-

    programs/programs/criminal

    -justice/NPIP-Police%20Science

    -TowardaNewParadigm.pd

    Willis, James J., Stephen D. Mastroski,

    and David Weisburd. 2007. Making

    sense o COMPSTAT: A theory-

    based analysis o organizational

    change in three police departments.

    Law and Society Review41: 14788.

    Willis, James J., and Stephen D.

    Mastroski. 2011. Innovations in

    policing: meanings, structures,

    processes. Annual Review o Law

    and Social Science7: 309334.

    Willis, James J., Stephen D. Mastroski,

    Jillian Baird, Lauren Revier, Terri

    Hines, and Tal Jonathan. 2011.

    Results o interviews on high quality

    policing in Manassas City, VA.

    Fairax, VA: George Mason

    University, Department o

    Criminology, Law and Society.

    Wilson, James Q. 1978. Varieties o

    Police Behavior: The Management

    o Law and Order in Eight

    Communities. Cambridge: Harvard

    University Press.

  • 7/28/2019 Improving Police: What's Craft Got to Do with It?

    14/14

    1201 Connecticut Avenue, NW, Washington, DC 20036-2636(202) 833-1460 Fax (202) 659-9149 E-mail: [email protected]

    www.policefoundation.org

    POLICE

    ABOUT THE POLICE FOUNDATION

    The Police Foundation is a national, nonpartisan, nonprot organization dedicated to advancinginnovation and science in policing. As the countrys oldest police research organization, thePolice Foundation has learned that police practices should be based on scientic evidence aboutwhat works best, the paradigm of evidence-based policing. Established in 1970, the foundationhas conducted seminal research in police behavior, policy, and procedure, and works to transfer

    to local agencies the best new information about practices for dealing effectively with a rangeof important police operational and administrative concerns. Motivating all of the foundationsefforts is the goal of efcient, humane policing that operates within the framework of democraticprinciples and the highest ideals of the nation.

    DIVISION OF RESEARCH, EVALUATION,& PROFESSIONAL SERVICES

    Karen L. AmendolaChief Operating Ofcer

    David WeisburdSenior Fellow

    Garth den Heyer

    Senior Research Fellow

    Travis Taniguchi

    Senior Research Associate

    Edwin E. HamiltonProfessional Services Director

    Emily OwensResearch Fellow

    Adam KaufmanResearch Assistant

    Mary SiglerResearch Assistant

    Maria ValdovinosResearch & Administrative Coordinator

    RESEARCH ADVISORY COMMITTEE

    David Weisburd, Chair

    Hebrew University and George Mason University

    Anthony A. BragaRutgers University and Harvard University

    Robin S. EngelUniversity of Cincinnati

    Christopher KoperGeorge Mason University

    Jerry H. RatcliffeTemple University

    BOARD OF DIRECTORS

    ChairmanWeldon J. Rougeau

    PresidentJames Bueermann

    George H. Bohlinger III

    Clarence Edwards

    Dean Esserman

    Paul Helmke

    Julie Horney

    William H. Hudnut IIIJonathan Knowles

    Mark S. Mellman

    W. Walter Menninger

    Elsie L. Scott

    Andrew L. Sonner