21
Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Identity formation in multiparty negotiations Roderick I. Swaab 1 *, Tom Postmes 2 and Russell Spears 3 1 Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, USA 2 School of Psychology, University of Exeter, UK 3 Russell Spears, Department of Psychology, Cardiff University, UK Based on the recently proposed Interactive Model of Identity Formation, we examine how top-down deductive and bottom-up inductive identity formations influence intentions and behaviour in multiparty negotiations. Results show that a shared identity can be deduced from the social context through recognition of superordinate similarities. However, shared identities can also be induced by intragroup processes in which individuals get acquainted with one another on an interpersonal basis. Both top-down and bottom-up processes led to the formation of a sense of shared identity, and this in turn exerted a positive influence on behavioural intentions and actual behaviour in multiparty negotiations. The Interactive Model of Identity Formation (IMIF) suggests that there are two theoretically distinct pathways to the formation of a sense of shared (social) identity. The classic perspective on social identities is that they are inferred deductively from the broader social context within which the group members act. However, in smaller groups where members can readily interact and observe each others’ actions, a sense of shared identity can also form inductively through a reconciliation of differences and the establishment of interpersonal relations (Postmes, Haslam, & Swaab, 2005a; Postmes, Spears, Novak, & Lee, 2005b). Although this theoretical framework has received some initial support in small group settings, several questions remain unanswered. One purpose of this paper is thus to provide direct evidence of the occurrence of these two processes. The setting in which this prediction is tested is that of multiparty negotiations. A key challenge to most negotiations is to align individual and group interests. Doing so requires individual negotiators to recognize some overarching commonalties leading them to pursue outcomes that benefit themselves as well as others. We propose that a shared identity may provide just this (see also Swaab, Postmes, Van Beest, & Spears, 2007), regardless of how this shared identity was formed (i.e. deductively or inductively). The second purpose of the present paper is therefore to examine whether different paths to shared identity formation produce essentially similar consequences for group members’ collaborative behaviour in a multiparty negotiation. * Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Roderick I. Swaab, Kellogg School of Management, North-western University, USA (e-mail: [email protected]). The British Psychological Society 167 British Journal of Social Psychology (2008), 47, 167–187 q 2008 The British Psychological Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk DOI:10.1348/014466607X216124

Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations

Roderick I. Swaab1*, Tom Postmes2 and Russell Spears31Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, USA2School of Psychology, University of Exeter, UK3Russell Spears, Department of Psychology, Cardiff University, UK

Based on the recently proposed InteractiveModel of Identity Formation, we examine howtop-down deductive and bottom-up inductive identity formations influence intentions andbehaviour in multiparty negotiations. Results show that a shared identity can be deducedfrom the social context through recognition of superordinate similarities.However, sharedidentities can also be induced by intragroup processes in which individuals get acquaintedwith one another on an interpersonal basis. Both top-down and bottom-up processes ledto the formation of a sense of shared identity, and this in turn exerted a positive influenceon behavioural intentions and actual behaviour in multiparty negotiations.

The Interactive Model of Identity Formation (IMIF) suggests that there are two theoretically

distinct pathways to the formation of a sense of shared (social) identity. The classic

perspective on social identities is that they are inferred deductively from the broader social

context within which the group members act. However, in smaller groups where memberscan readily interact and observe each others’ actions, a sense of shared identity can also

form inductively through a reconciliation of differences and the establishment of

interpersonal relations (Postmes, Haslam, & Swaab, 2005a; Postmes, Spears, Novak, & Lee,

2005b). Although this theoretical framework has received some initial support in small

group settings, several questions remain unanswered. One purpose of this paper is thus to

provide direct evidence of the occurrence of these two processes.

The setting in which this prediction is tested is that of multiparty negotiations. A key

challenge to most negotiations is to align individual and group interests. Doing sorequires individual negotiators to recognize some overarching commonalties leading

them to pursue outcomes that benefit themselves as well as others. We propose that a

shared identity may provide just this (see also Swaab, Postmes, Van Beest, & Spears,

2007), regardless of how this shared identity was formed (i.e. deductively or

inductively). The second purpose of the present paper is therefore to examine whether

different paths to shared identity formation produce essentially similar consequences

for group members’ collaborative behaviour in a multiparty negotiation.

* Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Roderick I. Swaab, Kellogg School of Management, North-western University,USA (e-mail: [email protected]).

TheBritishPsychologicalSociety

167

British Journal of Social Psychology (2008), 47, 167–187

q 2008 The British Psychological Society

www.bpsjournals.co.uk

DOI:10.1348/014466607X216124

Page 2: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Negotiations and shared identityMultiparty negotiations involve three or more individuals who perceive themselves as

having opposing interests regarding scarce resources (Bazerman, Curhan, Moore, &

Valley, 2000). Research on the efficacy of different negotiation behaviours shows

consistently that the optimal strategy for individuals who face an integrative negotiation

is to make sure that their own interests are represented, while at the same time remainopen to others’ viewpoints (De Dreu, Weingart, & Kwon, 2000; Pruitt & Carnevale,

1993). How prosocial behaviour emerges in negotiations has been a key question for

negotiation and coalition researchers. For example, negotiation researchers have

focused on the impact of stable factors such as social value orientations (i.e.

psychological motives or tendencies to adopt a particular behavioural strategy) and

personality traits (Barry & Friedman, 1998; O’Connor et al., 2002; Olekalns & Smith,

1999). Theories of coalition formation, which tend to be dominated either by game

theory perspectives which assume that people behave rationally (Kahan & Rapoport,1984) or by theories that highlight the social costs associated with exclusion

(Diermeier & Gailmard, 2006; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003; Van Beest, Van

Dijk, & Wilke, 2003), have also examined the impact of prosocial value orientations, but

again, treated this as a relatively stable characteristic.

But beyond these stable characteristics, there are also contextual factors that influence

whether people act communally or not. For example, the social identity perspective has

proposed that the emergence of a shared sense of social identity is associated with prosocial

behaviour in the interest of the group (Barreto & Ellemers, 2002; Kramer & Brewer, 1984;Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). Research in the social dilemma literature

would appear to be consistent with this suggestion (see for example Van Vugt & De Cremer,

1999). But despite the potential relevance of these ideas for integrative negotiations, there

is as yet very little research examining the impacts of the formation of a (superordinate)

sense of social identity on negotiation outcomes (see Eggins, Haslam, & Reynolds, 2002;

Swaab et al., 2007 for exceptions).

One likely reason for the limited influence of social identity perspectives on the

negotiation literature is that most social identity research has concerned itself more withthe cognitive representation of large social categories than with the study of social

outcomes in small interactive groups. In other words, social identity research tends to

be more often concerned with questions of how people perceive self, in-group and out-

group in certain intergroup contexts than with actual interaction among the in- and out-

group members (see also Haslam & McGarty, 2001). This means that although the

relevance of social identity processes to negotiations has been strongly emphasized

(Haslam, 2001), the applicability of social identity perspectives to multiparty

negotiations (which do by definition entail interactions between people) remains anempirical question.

Thus, key insights from social identity research have not been incorporated into the

negotiation research and vice versa. To redress this situation, the present research draws

upon the IMIF, a recently proposed model that describes two separate pathways to the

formation of a sense of shared social identity (Postmes et al., 2005a, 2005b). In contrast to

classic work on social identity, the IMIF specifically addresses the role that interaction and

communication play in the formation of shared identities. Although past research has

confirmed that these two paths to identity formation exist, the consequences of suchidentity formation have not yet been the focus of empirical research. The object of this

paper is therefore to integrate this model with the negotiation literature, thereby testing

novel hypotheses about the consequences of shared identity for multiparty negotiations.

168 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 3: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

In the remainder of this introduction, we seek to unite the social identity

perspective, the IMIF, and the negotiation literature. We first introduce the social

identity literature and the IMIF, and then introduce their relevance to negotiations. We

then test these ideas in a pilot study and a study of an actual negotiation.

The formation of shared social identities

The social identity perspective predicts that when a social identity is salient, it will

influence the behaviour and cognitions of individuals within that group (Turner, 1982;

Turner et al., 1987). Among other things, social identity salience leads to behaviour that

is consistent with group norms (e.g. Spears, Lea, & Lee, 1990) which advances the

group’s interests (Ellemers, Kortekaas, & Ouwerkerk, 1999; Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje,

1997). However, most of the research on this phenomenon has taken place in situationsin which social identities are treated as ‘givens’, meaning that their attributes are self-

evident and relatively undisputed (e.g. because the experimenter explicitly informs the

participant about the content of that social identity and the associated norms).

This model of a social identity has proven its value, especially in large social categories

that we have reasonably consensual stereotypes about (e.g. gender, race or perhaps certain

nationalities). However, in small interactive groups, there is little evidence that such clearly

defined social identities exist, let alone that they are influential (Kerr & Tindale, 2004;

Krizan & Baron, 2006). Recently, it has been proposed that this lack of evidence may haveless to do with the relevance or irrelevance of the social identity concept for small groups

than with the faulty assumption that social identities in small groups would necessarily

operate as they do in large social categories. Inspired by distinctions between common

bond and common identity groups (Prentice, Miller, & Lightdale, 1994), interpersonal and

collective identities (Brewer & Gardner, 1996), relational and collective identities (Yuki,

2003) and dynamic and categorical groups (Wilder & Simon, 1998), Postmes and colleagues

(Postmes, Baray, Haslam, Morton, & Swaab, 2006; Postmes et al., 2005a, 2005b) proposed

that social identities can and do play a prominent role in small interactive groups. However,they suggest that an important distinction needs to be made between social identities

which are deduced and those that are induced.

Top-down deduction of shared identity in negotiationOn the one hand, the IMIF elaborates the process by which group members can infer ashared identity from knowledge of their group within the social context. This path to

shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

formation. A shared identity is formed deductively when a group of, say, negotiators

infer group-level similarities from the context they act in. The classic way to approach

this type of identity formation is when people make comparisons with relevant out-

groups and identify more highly with, or at a minimum become more aware of, their in-

group as a result. One good example of deductive identity formation in an intergroup

context is evident in political parties. Political parties can often be distinguished on aleft–right, progressive – conservative continuum so that the political ideology gives a

‘top-down’ indication of what the party line might be on any particular issue. Moreover,

it is in the nature of political parties that they compete with each other for votes and

support, so that the salience of group identity (and also the content of group identity) is

likely to be enhanced when other parties are in the frame (both left and right). However,

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 169

Page 4: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

a shared identity may also develop deductively in the absence of a directly relevant out-

group from abstract notions of togetherness such as the recognition of superordinate

commonalties and cues (Postmes et al., 2005a). For example, within many religious

groups, individual members may identify so strongly with their group that this will

impact on their way of life, regardless of the presence of other religious groups. Many

religions have doctrines that are absolute in nature and also enshrine non-competitivevalues and morals, so that the religious identity will often be evident in the absence of a

comparative context (which of course is not to say that conflicts between different

religions cannot arise). Both types of deductive identity formation, in turn, enable

individual group members to deduce properties to construct an internalized group

identity that comprises stereotypes and behavioural norms which define the group’s

identity further (e.g. by seeking distinctiveness or conformity).

Empirical evidence has supported the deductive path to identity formation in small

group interactions. In a series of studies, Haslam and colleagues (Haslam, 1997; Haslam,Turner, Oakes, McGarty, & Reynolds, 1998; Haslam, Turner, Oakes, Reynolds, Eggins, &

Nolan, 1998) divided participants into groups of three or four people and asked them to

indicate which traits they thought were most typical of Australians. In addition, they

manipulated the salience of social identity by asking participants to list three things they

and the other Australians do well. The results demonstrated that communication about

Australian stereotypes strengthened group members’ consensus and especially when a

shared social identity was salient. Research is also supportive of the idea that a deductively

inferred shared identity may develop in the absence of relevant out-groups. For example,previous research has demonstrated that communication in on-line discussion groups

provided a capacity to translate an abstract sense of togetherness into an operational

identity that informed group members about appropriate norms for decision making within

the group (Postmes, Spears, & Cihangir, 2001). By instructing participants to debate an

issue that pilot data have shown was one with which they fervently disagreed, Postmes and

colleagues promoted a critical norm that was synonymous with the group’s identity. In a

subsequent hidden profile task, this particular identity enabled the group to perform at a

higher level than those who did not develop such an identity. Throughout the experiment,no references were made to any relevant competing out-groups so as to avoid an intergroup

context, suggesting that a group identity can develop in a variety of settings which need not

be accompanied by the presence of relevant out-groups per se.

Negotiation research too, has focused on the impact of deductively inferred shared

identities on cooperative intentions and prosocial behaviour. For example, previous

research in social dilemmas (Kramer & Brewer, 1984), ultimatum bargaining (Robert &

Carnevale, 1997) and negotiation (Thompson, Valley, & Kramer, 1995), all show that

people have more negative expectations when dealing with out-group members and morepositive expectations when dealing with in-group members (Brewer, 1981). As a result,

interacting with in-group members yields stronger prosocial intentions and integrative

behaviour than interacting with an out-group member, and enables negotiators to obtain

higher joint gains (De Dreu et al., 2000). In sum, the research evidence to date suggests that

(in line with predictions of social identity theorists) deductive shared identities positively

affect negotiators’ prosocial intentions and behaviour.

However, the IMIF also proposes that identities can be deduced from a sense of unity

and similarity which is observed at the group level (i.e. in the absence of an intergroupcomparison; see also Castano, Yzerbyt, & Bourguignon, 2003). According to prevailing

theory, the impact of shared identities that are deduced in this alternative fashion should

be similarly positive. However, it remains unclear from this research or the negotiation

170 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 5: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

research, how a deductively inferred shared identity – other than through intergroup

comparisons – impacts negotiations. This prediction is tested in the present research.

Bottom-up induction of shared identity in negotiationThere is also a relatively neglected process by which individual contributions of group

members can serve as input for the induction of a shared identity. This path to identity

formation is referred to as bottom-up inductive identity formation. This process takes place

when group members construe their identityover time and develop norms that correspond

to this identity (Postmes et al., 2005a, 2005b). The possibility to observe and communicate

with other (in)group members informs individuals about certain group characteristics and

attributes that allow them to induce a shared identity which comprises stereotypes and

norms for appropriate behaviour that is distinctive to the group. An amateur band forexample, may develop its own style of music which in turn may be synonymous with the

band’s identity. In addition, the ability for group members to display unique abilities and

desires through mutual communication may also highlight a shared identity between them

and strengthen the group by enhancing its performance on certain tasks (Rink & Ellemers,

2007). Finally, and perhaps most straightforwardly, good interpersonal relations among all

group members can create the conditions under which they are willing to assimilate

behaviourally to each other (Gaertner, Iuzzini, Guerrero Witt, & Orina, 2006).

Communication, then, functions as the vehicle through which group members’interpersonal differences and commonalties, and the interpersonal relations that flow

from them, can be translated into a concrete shared identity.

Whereas the inductive route to identity formation is essentially no different from the

classic phenomenon that the observation of in-group members gives rise to the

spontaneous (and often tacit) inference of group norms (Sherif, 1935), it does suggest

intragroup communication is an essential prerequisite for this kind of shared identity

formation. In a study of email discussions for example, it was found that messages

between group members converged in both content and form over time in such a waythat intragroup interaction produced unique attributes of the group (Postmes, Spears, &

Lea, 2000). In other words, the results of this study showed that, over time, each group

converged in their use of humour and punctuation of their messages in such a way that

intragroup communication produced and accentuated attributes that were distinctive to

each individual group, effectively displaying a form of group polarization that served to

express and display a particular identity for the group. In addition, previous research has

shown that respecting mutual differences can protect group identification and

strengthen loyalty to the group (Simon & Sturmer, 2003). Although in practice theseinductive and deductive processes of identity formation are unlikely to operate

independently, the key notion here is that they have qualitatively different origins.

Negotiation scholars have acknowledged that establishing interpersonal relation-

ships and appreciating mutual differences facilitate the negotiation process and

outcome. For example, Loewenstein and colleagues showed that negotiators who were

attracted to the other party displayed more prosocial behaviour and chose alternatives

that benefited both parties (Loewenstein, Thompson, & Bazerman, 1989). Other

research revealed that getting acquainted with the counterparty or the mere ability tocommunicate fostered a shared identity and improved outcomes in social

dilemmas (Bouas & Komorita, 1996; Dawes, McTavish, & Shaklee, 1977; Dawes, Van

de Kragt, & Orbell, 1988; Kerr & Kaufman-Gilliland, 1994) as well as in integrative

negotiations (Moore, Kurtzberg, Thompson, & Morris, 1999; Morris, Nadler, Kurtzberg,

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 171

Page 6: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

& Thompson, 2002; Nadler, 2004). Hitherto, however, this research has not concerned

itself with the role played in this process by social identity induction, nor has it

examined the effects of these factors in the context of multiparty negotiations.

In sum, the IMIF suggests that the establishment of interpersonal relations can foster

the construction of a shared identity.1 The present research tests the prediction that, in

line with perspectives on the influence of shared identity in negotiations, this inductionof a shared identity gives rise to an increase in prosocial intentions and behaviour, and

thus to higher joint outcomes in integrative negotiations.

Overview

Although prior research has shown evidence that inductive and deductive paths to

social identity formation exist, it did not investigate the consequences of each. One

purpose of the present research is to develop and test manipulations of these two forms.

Having done this, the research then tests the predictions that 1) both forms of social

identity formation will foster the development of prosocial intentions, and as a result 2)

both forms of social identity formation will enhance negotiation outcomes.In order to achieve these objectives, a Pilot Study was designed to develop the

manipulation of a deductive shared identity, also testing the prediction that such a

deductive identity would be associated with more prosocial intentions. Testing this

prediction in an elegant and conclusive fashion is not all that easy – traditionally the social

identity literature has relied on subjective measures of identification as independent

variables, but these introduce a risk of circularity, with identification potentially being an

independent variable, dependent variable or process (Doosje, Spears, & Ellemers, 2002).

A further challenge was to manipulate deductive identity in the absence of any salientintergroup comparison. This is important because we did not want to confound the

inductive–deductive comparison with intra- vs. intergroup processes. Therefore, we

experimentally induced the formation of a sense of deductive identity among negotiators

by manipulating the perceived degree of identification and unity (unity and identification

being closely related, Castano et al., 2003). One key purpose of the Pilot Study was to

establish the effectiveness of this manipulation. We predicted that

Hypothesis 1: participants with a deductive identity would be more inclined to adopt prosocialintentions.

PILOT STUDY: DEDUCTIVE IDENTITY FORMATION

Method

ParticipantsOne hundred undergraduate students (54 males, 46 females, mean age ¼ 21.68 years)

of a Dutch University were asked to participate as part of a course, and were randomly

allocated to two deductive identity conditions (high deductive-identification, N ¼ 50,

vs. low deductive identification, N ¼ 50).

1 It should be noted that the inductive path to identity formation differs from the metacontrast principle because it does notrequire the presence of in-group members. Multiparty negotiations are intergroup settings in nature in which rivalry betweengroups is (oftentimes falsely) assumed. Key here is to reframe disputants’ intergroup frames into intragroup frames since thisenables them to make trade-offs. Inductive identity formation provides the means to reframe intergroup differences intointragroup opportunities.

172 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 7: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

ProcedureAt the start of the experiment, participants were informed that they would enter a

negotiation with two other parties.2 This feedback was given to provide a suitable

framework for the examination of participants’ collaborative intentions – no actual

negotiation took place. However, participants were led to expect that they would be

facing an integrative negotiation where mutual benefit could flow from collaboration. Itwas emphasized that in this negotiation they could not only look after the interests of

their own company (a baker, florist or grocer), but also look after the collective interest

of the group of negotiators (as occupants of a shopping mall). This set the stage for the

appropriate measurement of prosocial value orientations.

To manipulate a deductive identity, we substantially adapted and modified an

established manipulation of in-group prototypicality (Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1997)

– with an eye to making it applicable to tap into the cognitive component of

identification. Participants filled in an identity questionnaire which asked leadingquestions about their identification with the overarching negotiating group and about

relations between the different parties (see Appendix). The participants then generated

feedback for themselves by computing their ‘deductive identification score’. Due to the

nature of the questions, this score was likely to reinforce the impression that they

identified weakly or strongly with the (existing) shared identity inferred. It is important

to emphasize that this manipulation merely made a predefined social identity salient,

emphasized the individual’s affiliation with the group, and thus formed a robust

operationalization of deductive identity formation. It did not offer any scope for theinductive inference of social identity attributes.

In the low deductive identity condition, questions were framed such that

participants were likely to indicate that they disagreed with the identification

statements. For example, we asked participants to indicate agreement with statements

such as ‘I consider the three parties in the negotiation to be three independent

individuals’ and ‘Own interests will not be important to these negotiators’ (recoded).

Given the parameters of the expected negotiation, the first statement is logically true

and the second is false. As can be seen in Appendix A, the majority response confirmedthis. By inviting these (dis)confirmative responses, we hoped to subtly prime that the

three parties were psychologically independent. For participants in the high deductive

identity conditions, questions were more or less equivalent, in the sense that they

primed the same concepts. However, importantly, they were framed in such a way that

they invited the opposite response and also the participants were likely to agree with

statements that implied a sense of shared identity. Thus, in this condition, the equivalent

items were ‘I and the others are totally independent of each other’ (recoded) and ‘Own

interest is not the only thing which counts for this negotiating group’. It should be notedthat the main difference between these items and those in the other condition (above)

was the way they were framed, and their reference to ‘group’ instead of ‘negotiators’.

Each participant answered seven negatively and seven positively framed items.

After responding to each item, participants summed the total number of

(dis)agreements for the 14 items. This was used to reinforce the manipulation. Given

that the questionnaire led participants to maximize (high identification) or minimize

(low identification) their total scores, they ‘discovered’ that their scores were either low

or high depending on what deductive identity condition they were in (Appendix A

2 The scenario sketched a negotiation situation which was similar to the Tower’s Market case (Weingart et al., 1993).

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 173

Page 8: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

presents items and mean scores per condition). After calculating their total scores,

participants were told that prior research demonstrated that total scores from 0 to 7

indicated that people identified weakly with their group (the average score was 3.36),

whereas scores ranging from 8 to 14 indicated that they identified highly with their

group (average score 11.70). In effect, this meant that participants were given feedback

that their deductively inferred identification scores showed them to be either low orhigh identifiers (depending on condition). Participants proceeded by filling in a

questionnaire assessing prosocial value orientations and their intentions during the

group negotiation which would follow soon. In addition, they expressed how much

they identified with the superordinate group. Participants were then informed that no

actual negotiation would take place and fully debriefed.

Dependent variablesParticipants responded to several statements on 7-point scales. The manipulation check

of deductive identification tapped into the cognitive component of social identification

(Ellemers et al., 1999) and asked the participants ‘how much they identified with their

overarching group’, ‘how closely they felt connected with the others in their group with

whom they would negotiate’ and ‘how glad they were to be in that specific group’

(1, Not at all; 7, Very much, Cronbach’sa ¼ :80). Note that we only manipulatedsuperordinate identification but not subgroup identification. In their instructions,

participants were made aware of their subgroup identity by saying that they were going

to negotiate on behalf of a certain party and faced two other parties during this

negotiation.

The dependent variable was a measure of the prosocial intention (compared

with self-interested intentions). We simply asked the participants the extent to

which they personally intended to strive for their own interests (1) or for mutual

interests (7).3

Results

Manipulation checkThe manipulation of identification was successful. The manipulation check showed thatparticipants in the high deductive identity condition indicated significantly higher

identification with the superordinate category (M ¼ 4:90, SD ¼ 0:87) than those in the

low deductive identity condition (M ¼ 4:05, SD ¼ 1:07), tð99Þ ¼ 4:35, p , :001,

h2 ¼ :08.

Prosocial intentionsOur prediction that participants in the high deductive identity condition had

more prosocial intentions (M ¼ 3:96, SD ¼ 1:34) than those in the low

deductive identity condition (M ¼ 3:22, SD ¼ 1:27) was supported, tð99Þ ¼ 2:84,

p , :01, h2 ¼ :16.

3 The disadvantages of a 1-item measure are well known: this typically decreases power. However, we felt that the use of such ameasure was justified because we wanted to ask participants simply whether they intended to strive for their own or for themutual interest. Since this was such a straightforward behavioural intention (with no obvious alternative wordings or conceptsinvolved), we saw no need for additional items.

174 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 9: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

NEGOTIATION STUDY: DEDUCTIVE AND INDUCTIVE IDENTITYFORMATION

Confirming predictions inferred from the IMIF, the Pilot Study showed that adeductive identity manipulation leads negotiators to identify more strongly with a

shared identity and to become more concerned about mutual interests.4 However,

the IMIF further predicts that an inductive identity manipulation should yield similar

effects on the formation of a shared identity and that the presence of either a

deductively inferred shared identity or inductively construed shared identity is

indispensable in promoting integrative behaviour among negotiators. However, the

Pilot Study only manipulated deductive identity formation. The aim of the follow-up

study was therefore to examine both paths to identity formation and their impact onan actual negotiation.

One additional purpose of the negotiation study was to manipulate inductive

identity formation. The fact that groups have the possibility of actually interacting

with each other in the negotiation study makes it possible to manipulate an

inductively construed identity in an elegant fashion. As mentioned before, the IMIF

suggests that inductive identity formation takes place through (interpersonal)

interaction between group members (Postmes et al., 2005a). This logic implies that an

inductive identity can be construed prior to the actual negotiation by manipulatingthe nature or quality of interaction between the negotiating individuals. One effective

way to do so is by providing negotiators an opportunity to get acquainted on an

interpersonal basis because previous research has shown that self-disclosure is a

powerful factor in the formation of personal relationships (Aron, Melinat, Aron,

Vallone, & Bator, 1997; Berscheid, 1994). Hence, in order to foster the formation of an

inductively construed shared identity between negotiators prior to their actual

negotiation, we adopted a manipulation of self-disclosure (Sedikides, Campbell,

Reeder, & Elliot, 1998).Based on the IMIF, which suggests that either path to identity formation is sufficient

to establish a shared identity among a group of individuals, and previous social identity

research showing that the presence of a shared identity is correlated with constructive

behaviour among individuals, we predicted that

Hypothesis 2a and 2b: The absence of both a deductive identity manipulation as well as aninductive identity manipulation obstructs the formation of a shared identity among negotiators(a) prior to and (b) during the negotiation.

Hypothesis 3: The absence of both a deductive identity manipulation as well as an inductiveidentity manipulation negatively impacts integrative behaviour.

Hypothesis 4: Effects on integrative behaviour during the negotiation are driven by the extent towhich negotiators recognize a shared identity to be present.

4 It is important to emphasize that the manipulation was explicitly designed to be neutral with regard to the intentions it couldpossibly have primed. Where references were made to goals or outcomes (i.e. regarding own or collective profit), these werecarefully counterbalanced. A post-hoc mediation analysis confirms that the identification manipulation check mediates theeffects of condition on social value orientations and not the other way around. Our confidence in the finding that we primarilymanipulated a deductive inferred shared identity and not prosocial value orientations is further strengthened by another studydemonstrating that the presence of a shared identity before a negotiation had independent effects on prosocial behaviourduring the negotiation (Swaab et al., 2007). Thus, manipulating a shared identity via deductive routes and the subsequentdevelopment of prosocial values is qualitatively different: whereas the first refers to a cognitive process where one is led to seeoneself as part of a larger whole (or not), the latter refers to behavioural intentions individuals have.

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 175

Page 10: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Method

Participants and designUndergraduate students of a Dutch University (N ¼ 180, 98 males, 81 females, meanage ¼ 21.57 years) participated for a monetary reward (e8). They were assigned at

random to 60 groups of three persons who were randomly assigned to experimental

conditions in a 2 (inductive identity formation: low vs. high) £ 2 (deductive identity

formation: low vs. high) factorial design. The allocation of men and women to groups

was approximately the same for all conditions and, importantly, results did not vary as a

function of the gender composition of groups, p . .32.

ProcedureThe experiment consisted of four phases. In Phase 1, inductive identity formationwas manipulated. In the high inductive identity formation, participants were seated

together at a large table, where they completed a self-disclosure task (Sedikides et al.,

1998). The task consisted of talking in dyads about 29 questions which were

designed to induce interpersonal attraction through inducing self-disclosure (e.g.

‘What is your first name?’, ‘What would be a perfect way of living for you?’, ‘If you

could change one thing about yourself, what would that be?’). Participants were

instructed to work on the self-disclosure task with each individual group member,

one at a time, so that three conversations took place in each group. After theirconversations, participants were brought to separate cubicles to await instructions

for the second phase of the study. In the low inductive identity conditions,

participants did not perform the self-disclosure task and were taken to separate

cubicles straight away.

During Phase 2, participants received detailed instructions about the negotiation

task. This was an integrative negotiation in which cooperation could lead to higher joint

gains. Participants were informed that they would play managers of a bakery, florist or

grocery store. With the other parties, they would negotiate about issues such as design,distribution of rental costs and temperature in a shared shopping mall. Numbers of

points were attached to each alternative solution, indicating relative profitability

(Beersma & De Dreu, 1999; Weingart, Bennett, & Brett, 1993). The total instruction took

about 7 minutes, during which participants were allocated a specific role, were given

feedback about the scenario and their objectives within it, ensuring each payer

understood the pay-off structure.

During Phase 3, before the actual negotiation started, participants received the

deductive identity manipulation identical to the Pilot Study. They had 5 minutes to fill inthe questions, calculate their deductive identification scores and indicated how much

they agreed with the items regarding the superordinate identification measure. It is

important to note that the deductive identity manipulation followed the role allocation –

this was to ensure that a sense of superordinate identity respected inter-party

differences (cf. Hornsey & Hogg, 2000).

In Phase 4 of the experiment, participants were seated at a large negotiation table

behind plaques identifying their role. They were given 20 minutes to reach an

agreement. If they had not come to a conclusion after this time, then theexperimenter gently prompted participants to round off and settle. If no settlement

could be reached, negotiations were terminated. After finishing the negotiation,

participants calculated how many points they had earned and filled in a

questionnaire.

176 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 11: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Manipulation checksWe checked the deductive manipulation by asking participants whether their

identification score before the negotiation was low (1) or high (2). We also checked

the inductive manipulation by asking participants whether they interacted with the

group members separately before their negotiation (1) or not (2).

Dependent variablesThe questionnaire after Phase 3 (i.e. pre-negotiation) asked participants how much theyperceived a shared identity prior to their interaction and was similar to the measure

from the Pilot Study. This measure thus allowed us to test whether both inductive and

deductive manipulations had independent effects on the formation of a shared identity.

Perceptions of shared identity during the negotiation itself were measured after

Phase 4. This measure of shared identity was similar to the measure prior to the

negotiation and to the Pilot Study.

Actual behaviour was measured after Phase 4 and consisted of three measures. Our

most conservative here were the number of impasses in each condition. Second, wemeasured the total number of points achieved after negotiation. Here, a compromise (i.e.

all individuals settle on the mid-point for each negotiation issue) would yield a total of 525

points whereas a full integrative outcome (i.e. individuals trade-off issues) yielded 600

points to be divided. The number of points thus reflects the extent to which negotiators

engaged in integrative behaviour. If groups did not reach agreement (i.e. an impasse), they

were not rewarded any points. Because of this and omitting no agreement groups from

the analyses would imply a loss of valuable data (i.e. one of the goals was to examine how

manipulations affect the quality of agreements), we had to recode the outcome data inorder to avoid biased results. To correct for (negative) skew, we first subtracted all values

from the highest possible value of 600 points, added 1 and applied a square root

transformation (as recommended by Hutcheson & Sofroniou, 1999). We then multiplied

this corrected outcome by 21, so that higher scores (i.e. less negative) indicated higher

outcomes. We report analyses for the outcome data without correction and with

correction for skew. Hence, the negotiation paradigm we used was constructed in such a

way that integrative behaviour would be evidenced by the total number of points

achieved by the group, and allowed us to test that the lowest scores would be achieved inthe condition where participants had no deductive and no inductive identity.

Results

Analytic strategyTo do justice to the interplay of individual and group-level effects, we conducted

multilevel analysis (Snijders & Bosker, 1999). However, our outcome measures, the

number of points and impasses, were a group attribute and therefore analysed at

the group level only. We dummy coded a contrast (1 1 1 23) predicting that only in the

condition in which no deductive or inductive identity was manipulated, shared identity

and outcomes of integrative behaviour would be low. This contrast analysis provided usa direct test of our hypotheses. It should be noted here that testing a contrast

(as advocated by e.g. Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1985) is the recommended strategy for these

analyses according to recent APA statistical guidelines (Wilkinson & The Task Force on

Statistical Inference, 1999). As a secondary set of analyses, main effects of deductive

identity formation and inductive identity formation were dummy-coded. Contrast and

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 177

Page 12: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

main effects were entered as level 2 (group) predictors in HLM for Windows version

5.04 (Raudenbusch, Bryk, & Congdon, 2001).

In the results section, we report t- and p-values to test the strength of the relation

(gamma’s or g’s) of the predicted contrast and main effects. These parameters are

indicative for whether or not differences were significant, and for the size of the effect.

For ease of interpretation, means and standard deviations of dependent measures at thegroup level can be found in Table 1.

Manipulation checksChecks indicated that manipulations were successful. Participants had higher

identification scores in the high deductive conditions than in the low deductive

conditions, Mann–Whitney U ¼ 2; 370:00, z ¼ 25:43, p , :001. No other main or

interaction effects were found. Checks of the inductive identity formation manipulation

were also successful. Participants in the high inductive identity conditions indicated to

have had (more) interaction with others prior to their negotiation than those in the low

inductive identity conditions, Mann–Whitney U ¼ 1; 213:50, z ¼ 29:57, p , :001. Noother main or interaction effects were found.

Dependent variablesHypothesis 2a was supported: the absence of both a deductive identity manipulation as

well as an inductive identity manipulation obstructed the formation of a shared identity

among negotiators prior to the negotiation, g ¼ :15, tð58Þ ¼ 2:56, p ¼ :01, h2 ¼ :09.

Secondary analyses of main effects show that the deductive identification manipulation

failed to reach conventional levels of significance, p ¼ :17, while the inductive

manipulation was significant, p ¼ :03.

Hypothesis 2b was partially supported: although the contrast predicting that a

shared identity would not develop when both inductive identity manipulation anddeductive identity manipulation were absent was in the predicted direction, it was not

significant, g ¼ :07, tð58Þ ¼ 1:57, p ¼ :12, h2 ¼ :04. However, cross-comparison

analyses revealed a significant difference between the condition in which neither a

deductive nor inductive identity was manipulated and the condition in which both were

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of dependent variables in negotiation study

Inductive identity high Inductive identity low

Deductiveidentity high

Deductiveidentity low

Deductiveidentity high

Deductiveidentity low

Shared identity (prior to) 4.63a 4.39a 4.22ab 3.79b(0.89) (0.68) (0.94) (0.77)

Shared identity (during) 4.65a 4.39ab 4.40ab 4.21b(0.89) (0.58) (0.52) (0.57)

Impasse rates (%) .07a .13ab .13ab .36bTotal points (uncorrected) 507.50a 468.33ab 459.17ab 354.46b

(145.52) (194.50) (190.89) (275.59)Total points (corrected) 24.67 25.41 25.83 27.50

(3.28) (4.09) (3.78) (5.35)

Note. Means in a row with different subscripts differ at p , :05; Standard deviations betweenparentheses.

178 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 13: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

manipulated, p ¼ :03 (see also Table 1). Secondary analyses of main effects show that

these failed to reach conventional levels of significance, p ¼ :12 and p ¼ :13 for the

deductive and inductive manipulations, respectively.

Hypothesis 3 was supported. Analyses of the impasses – the most conservative

outcome measure – showed that the absence of both a deductive identity manipulation as

well as an inductive identity manipulation negatively impacts on integrative behaviour,Mann–Whitney U ¼ 242:00, z ¼ 22:22, p ¼ :03, h2 ¼ :06. Secondary analyses of main

effects failed to reach significance, p ¼ :14 and p ¼ :17 for deductive and inductive

manipulations, respectively. We found similar effects for Hypothesis 3 with regard to the

total amount of points: the contrast designed to test the prediction was significant for the

uncorrected measure (Fð1; 58Þ ¼ 4:09, p ¼ :05, h2 ¼ :07) and marginally significant for

the corrected measure (Fð1; 58Þ ¼ 3:03, p ¼ :09, h2 ¼ :05). Secondary analyses of main

effects failed to reach significance, p ¼ :17 and p ¼ :13 for deductive and inductive

manipulations for the uncorrected measure and p ¼ :27 and p ¼ :14 for the correctedmeasure, respectively.

Hypothesis 4 which stated that effects on integrative behaviour during the negotiation

would be driven by the extent to which negotiators recognize a shared identity to be

present required a mediation analysis and was only partially supported. We tested

whether the effect of the contrast on integrative behaviour was mediated by shared

identity during the negotiation. Since small differences were found between the different

measures of the outcome, we used group-level mediation analyses to test hypothesis 3 for

the uncorrected outcome measure and controlled for shared identity prior to thenegotiation in these analyses. First, there was a direct effect of the contrast on the

outcome (b ¼ 0:26, p , :05) and a trend for shared identity during the negotiation

(b ¼ 0:21, p ¼ :11). Importantly, when both contrast and shared identity (controlling for

shared identity prior to negotiation, b ¼ 0:00, p ¼ :98) were entered as predictors of the

outcome measure, the effect of the first became weaker and non-significant (b ¼ 0:20,

p ¼ :13), while shared identity became stronger and significantly predicted the outcome

(b ¼ 0:27, p ¼ :04). Although the Sobel test was non-significant (t ¼ 1:27, p ¼ :20), and

hence there was no evidence that shared identity fully mediated the effects of condition,the results are consistent with the conclusion that shared identity exerted a significant

influence on the negotiation outcomes.

Discussion

The results of this study broadly support our hypotheses concerning the consequences

of identity formation along deductive and inductive paths. Both were independentlymanipulated, and equally affected the development of a shared identity and

corresponding behaviour. This confirms predictions derived from the Interactive

model of Identity Formation (Postmes et al., 2005a) that there is a deductive path to

identity formation by which group members infer their identity from superordinate

similarities as well as an inductive path to the formation of social identity.

In this study, the findings for the outcomes of integrative behaviour effectively

mirrored the pattern for shared identity: the two manipulations both affected the use of

integrative behaviour and outcomes. This signals that both inductive and deductivepaths to identity formation induce negotiating partners to behave more cooperatively

and prosocially during their negotiation. Moreover, this effect of conditions on

negotiation outcomes was partially mediated by the presence of a shared identity, which

is consistent with prior studies showing that the display of constructive negotiation

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 179

Page 14: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

behaviours is influenced, among others, by the establishment of a superordinate identity

that unites the parties (see Eggins et al., 2002; Swaab et al., 2007 for exceptions).

Furthermore, the results confirm that interpersonal relations between group members

are an important predictor of negotiation outcomes (Gillespie, Brett, & Weingart, 2000;

Loewenstein et al., 1989; Thompson, Kray, & Lind, 1998).

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Taken together, the results of the Pilot Study and the Negotiation Study add to our

understanding of the social factors influencing negotiation behaviours and intentions.

Summarizing the findings, these studies demonstrate that deductively inferred identities

as well as inductively construed ones foster the development of prosocial behavioural

intentions and actual behaviour during a multiparty negotiation. They establish a clearrelationship between social identification and intentions for prosocial behaviour during

negotiations, as well as actual cooperation in the achievement of an integrative solution.

Results of the negotiation study also show that deductive and inductive processes

(and interpersonal and group-level factors) are closely entwined in their effects. On the

one hand, these two factors appear to mutually reinforce each other. Indeed, results show

that deductive and inductive manipulations both gave rise to the development of a shared

identity prior to the negotiation while originating from qualitatively different sources.

This supports the suggestion elsewhere that, in small interactive groups at least, factors atthe interpersonal level are very closely related to factors at the group level. This finding is

especially interesting given the tendency of group researchers to focus on either group-

level or on personal and interpersonal factors, but rarely on the reciprocal influence

between the two (Spears, 2001). On the other hand, both factors seem to retain an

independent influence on the formation of prosocial intentions and on actual group

behaviour. Indeed, it was notable that the establishment of an inductively construed

shared identity had effects very similar to those of deductively inferred shared identities.

ImplicationsPerhaps the most theoretically consequential finding in this research is that getting

interpersonally acquainted with other group members is far from independent of social

identity-level factors such as superordinate identification in small groups. Consistent

with the IMIF, we found that both factors, effective through deductive and inductive

manipulations, are involved in the processes of social identity formation. This is likely to

be true of related processes of identity transformation and change as well.At some level, these conclusions are obvious and intuitive. Researchers have long

championed the interactive approach to the study of social psychological phenomena,

noting the close interdependence between individual and group (e.g. Asch, 1952; Mead,

1934; Sherif, 1935). However, although this has been the word, it appears most of the

(research) money has been put elsewhere. A closer examination of the small groups’

literature shows that our understanding of the interaction between social identities and

intragroup dynamics has evolved little. In large part, this may be due to the tendency to

favour paradigms of research that do not allow for a ‘contamination’ with factors that theresearcher is not principally interested in. For instance, although social identity researchers

have long claimed to be well positioned to explain small group phenomena, social identity

research has (in line with the parameters of theory) largely focused on paradigms in which

the key variables of interest are perceptions of groups, and not interactions in-groups.

180 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 15: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

As a result, the majority of social identity research at best speaks indirectly to issues of group

dynamics (Kerr & Tindale, 2004). Similar criticism can be levelled at researchers of group

dynamics, however, who have been somewhat reluctant to explore the relevance of social

identity approaches. The present results suggest that this is an area where future advances

can be made. Ultimately, the fruitful colonization of this ‘no person’s land’ may depend on

the abandonment of some theoretical and methodological trenches.As one example of the scope for theoretical integration and growth, the negotiations

literature may benefit from a consideration of the relationships between social identity

perspectives of negotiations (Haslam, 2001) and perspectives such as dual concern

theory (Pruitt & Rubin, 1986; Pruitt & Carnevale, 1993). Both perspectives argue that

productive negotiations are the consequence of the dual consideration of own and other

party concerns. But whereas dual concern theory focuses on the role of prosocial value

orientations, social identity approaches concern themselves with the social identities at

various levels that may be involved. Moreover, whereas the focus of dual concernmodels is on the relation of self to others, dual identity models are concerned with

subgroup and superordinate group relations. Both arguments have in common the

assumption that convergence is predicated upon the recognition of commonality which

allows for the expression of self-hood. The similarity between these approaches is even

more apparent if one accepts that self may comprise group-level as well as individual

identities, and is therefore not just purely individual (Sedikides & Brewer, 2001; Tajfel &

Turner, 1979; Turner et al., 1987).

An interesting methodological implication of the current research is that one can quiteeasily foster deductive identity formation. This is encouraging because until now, few

robust manipulations of deductive identity formation in (interactive) ad hoc groups have

been developed (with some exceptions, see for example Witt & Kerr, 2002). Similarly, our

inductive identity manipulation has practical implications as well. We found inductive

identity formation – evoked by short off-topic pre-negotiation caucusing – to have a

positive influence on the group process in that it fostered the development of a shared

identity between negotiations and integrative behaviour. Although the effect of (dyadic)

off-topic pre-negotiation caucusing seems to be positive relative to no interaction, thereare risks associated with such procedures in real-life settings. For instance, when there is

inequality in the nature or amount of caucusing, not all possible dyads (negotiating parties

and or mediators involved) would have communicated with each other in similar fashion

or with similar outcomes. This may give rise to feelings of distrust which could impair the

negotiating process. Nevertheless, it would be interesting to investigate the role of (on-

topic as well as off-topic) pre-negotiation caucusing in future research.

Issues for future researchThe present research operationalized, manipulated and measured identification in terms

of its cognitive (self-categorization) component. Previous research suggests that

identification is a multifaceted construct encompassing not just cognitive aspects of

collective self-definition but also socio-emotional aspects reflecting an individual’s

affiliation with the group (Ellemers et al., 1999). A fruitful question for future research

would be to disentangle the impact that the different paths to identity formation havefor the various dimensions of social identification. For example, it could be that a

deductive identity, inferred from either intergroup comparisons or the recognition of

some superordinate similarity, is more cognitive in nature, at least initially. For instance,

knowing that you and others are competing with other groups for certain resources may

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 181

Page 16: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

urge you to operate on behalf of your group’s interest more so than when such

competition is absent (i.e. in-group favouritism). Although this need not make you more

attracted to other in-group members in the first instance, research does suggest that

such an association between (cognitive) salience and attraction can occur (Hogg &

Hardie, 1992).

In contrast, inductive identity formation requires a reconciliation of interpersonaldifferences or the establishment of interpersonal relations and it would appear logical

that this were more affective in nature, and/or related to in-group ties (Cameron, 2004).

Here, closer (inter)personal ties with other group members lead one to act on behalf of

the group’s goals. But once again, it would appear logical to assume that this form of

identification would give rise, over time, to the emergence of a sense of shared identity

of the more abstract, cognitive and kind.

Our research suggests that (within multiparty negotiations) the presence of a

deductively inferred or inductively construed shared identity is sufficient for individualsto act in the collective interest. However, it would be interesting to see whether these

findings also hold when one of the paths to identity formation has a negative valence.

In other words, are groups able to establish integrative behavioural outcomes when

their inductive identity is negative (e.g. they dislike each other) but their deductive

identity is positive and strong or vice versa? Our results suggest that they can but

additional research is needed to answer such questions more precisely.

Our results also suggest that the different paths to identity formation are potentially

(if very slightly) additive. The best negotiation outcomes were found in the conditionthat fostered both forms of identity formation. Nevertheless, future research would do

well to examine the interplay between the inductive and the deductive paths. Although

we certainly believe that, in practice, both the paths can reinforce each other, it is also

easily imaginable that there are conditions under which inductive processes would

undermine deductive identity and vice versa. Some research speaks to the latter process:

for instance research on computer-mediated interaction suggests that on-line

interpersonal interactions can undermine the perceptual unity of the group (Sassenberg

& Postmes, 2002). Furthermore, it is likely that intragroup interactions that invokeintergroup comparisons within the group would entirely undermine the capacity to

maintain a sense of superordinate identification. Understanding the ways in which

inductive and deductive identity formations interact is of key importance, given that in

most, if not all, real-world groups both processes are likely to coexist.

In sum, our research demonstrates that different paths towards social identity

formation have similar social effects on intentions and actions. These social identities are

so readily inferred from the establishment of interpersonal relations confirm that our

understanding of social identity can benefit from the study of dynamic and interactiveprocesses in-groups. Conversely, this speaks to the fruitfulness of considering the

influence of social identity concepts in small group research in general and in particular

in integrative negotiations.

Acknowledgements

This research was part of the first author’s dissertation and supported by the Amsterdam School of

Communications Research (ASCoR) as well as by grants from The Netherlands Organization for

Scientific Research (NWO, grant 425.21.009) to the first and second author and by a fellowship of

the ESRC (RES-062-23-013) to the second author. We thank Bianca Beersma and Rachael Eggins for

their helpful and insightful comments on an earlier version of this article.

182 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 17: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

References

Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental generation

of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings. Personality and

Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 363–377.

Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2002). The impact of anonymity and group identification on pro-group

behaviour in computer-mediated groups. Small Group Research, 33, 590–610.

Barry, B., & Friedman, R. A. (1998). Bargainer characteristics in distributive and integrative

negotiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 345–359.

Bazerman, M. H., Curhan, J. R., Moore, D. A., & Valley, K. L. (2000). Negotiation. Annual Review of

Psychology, 51, 279–314.

Beersma, B., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (1999). Negotiation processes and outcomes in prosocially and

egoistically motivated groups. International Journal of Conflict Management, 10, 385–402.

Berscheid, E. (1994). Interpersonal relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 79–129.

Bouas, K. S., & Komorita, S. S. (1996). Group discussion and cooperation in social dilemmas.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1144–1150.

Brewer, M. B. (1981). Ethnocentrism and its role in interpersonal trust. In M. B. Brewer &

B. E. Collins (Eds.), Scientific inquiry and the social sciences. New York: Jossey-Bass.

Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this ‘we’? Levels of collective identity and self

representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83–93.

Cameron, J. E. (2004). A three-factor model of social identity. Self and Identity, 3, 239–262.

Castano, E., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Bourguignon, D. (2003). We are one and I like it. The impact of

entitativity on social identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33(6), 735–754.

Dawes, R. M., McTavish, J., & Shaklee, H. (1977). Behavior, communication, and assumptions

about other people’s behavior in a commons dilemma situation. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 35(1), 1–11.

Dawes, R. M., Van de Kragt, A. J., & Orbell, J. M. (1988). Not me or thee but we: The importance of

group identity in eliciting cooperation in dilemma situations: Experimental manipulations.

Acta Psychologica, 68(1–3), 83–97.

De Dreu, C. K. W., Weingart, L. R., & Kwon, S. (2000). Influence of social motives on integrative

negotiation: A meta-analytic review and test of two theories. Interpersonal Relations and

Group Processes, 78(5), 889–905.

Diermeier, D., & Gailmard, S. (2006). Self interest, inequality, and entitlement in majoritarian

decision making. Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 1(4), 327–350.

Doosje, B., Spears, R., & Ellemers, N. (2002). Social identity as both cause and effect: The

development of group identification in response to anticipated and actual changes in the

intergroup status hierarchy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 57–76.

Eggins, R. A., Haslam, S. A., & Reynolds, K. J. (2002). Social identity and negotiation: Subgroup

representation and superordinate consensus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

28(7), 887–899.

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study

of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.

Ellemers, N., Kortekaas, P., & Ouwerkerk, J. (1999). Self-categorization, commitment to the group

and group self-esteem as related but distinct aspects of social identity. European Journal of

Social Psycholgy, 29, 371–389.

Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (1997). Sticking together or falling apart: Group

identification as a psychological determinant of group commitment versus individual mobility.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 123–140.

Gaertner, L., Iuzzini, J., Guerrero Witt, M., & Orina, M. M. (2006). Us without them: Evidence for an

intragroup origin of positive ingroup regard. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

90, 426–439.

Gillespie, J. J., Brett, J. M., & Weingart, L. R. (2000). Interdependence, social motives, and outcome

satisfaction in multiparty negotiation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30(6), 779–797.

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 183

Page 18: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Haslam, A., & McGarty, C. (2001). A 100 years of certitude? Social psychology, the experimental

method and the management of scientific uncertainty. British Journal of Social Psychology,

40, 1–21.

Haslam, S. A. (1997). Stereotyping and social influence: Foundations of stereotype concensus. In

R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyping

and group life (pp. 119–143). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Haslam, S. A. (2001). Psychology in organizations: The social identity approach. London: SAGE

Publications.

Haslam, S. A., Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., McGarty, C., & Reynolds, K. J. (1998). The group as a basis

for emergent stereotype consensus. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of

social psychology ( Vol. 8, pp. 203–239). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Haslam, S. A., Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Reynolds, K. J., Eggins, R. A., & Nolan, M. (1998). When do

stereotypes become really consensual? Investigating the group-based dynamics of the

concensualization process. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 755–776.

Hogg, M. A., & Hardie, E. A. (1992). Self-categorization and depersonalized attraction: Two

experiments on the relationship between self-prototypically and liking among group

members. Unpublished manuscript, University of Queensland.

Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. (2000). Assimilation and diversity: An integrative model of subgroup

relations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 143–156.

Hutcheson, G., & Sofroniou, N. (1999). The multivariate social scientist: Introductory statistics

using generalized linear models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. R. (1997). Strength of identification and intergroup

differentiation: The influence of group norms. European Journal of Social Psycholgy, 27,

603–609.

Kahan, J. P., & Rapoport, A. (1984). Theories of coalition formation. New York: Erlbaum.

Kerr, N. L., & Kaufman-Gilliland, C. M. (1994). Communication, commitment, and cooperation in

social dilemmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 513–529.

Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and decision making. Annual Review of

Psychology, 55, 623–655.

Kramer, R. M., & Brewer, M. B. (1984). Effects of group identity on resource use in a simulated

commons dilemma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 1044–1057.

Krizan, Z., & Baron, R. S. (2006). Group polarization and choice-dilemmas: How important is self-

categorization? European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(1).

Loewenstein, G. F., Thompson, L., & Bazerman, M. H. (1989). Social utility and decision making in

interpersonal contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 426–441.

Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Moore, D. A., Kurtzberg, T. R., Thompson, L., & Morris, M. W. (1999). Long and short routes to

success in electronically mediated negotiations: Group affiliations and good vibrations.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 77(1), 22–43.

Morris, M., Nadler, J., Kurtzberg, T., & Thompson, L. (2002). Schmooze or lose: Social friction and

lubrication in e-mail negotiations. Group Dynamics, 6(1), 89–100.

Nadler, J. (2004). Rapport in legal negotiation: How small talk can facilitate email dealmaking.

Harvard Negotiation Law Review, 9, 225–253.

O’Connor, K. M., de Dreu, C. K. W., Schroth, H., Barry, B., Lituchy, T., & Bazerman, M. H. (2002).

What we want to do versus what we think we should do: An empirical investigation of

intrapersonal conflict. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(5), 403–418.

Olekalns, M., & Smith, P. (1999). Social value orientations and strategy choices in competitive

negotiations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(6), 657–668.

Postmes, T., Baray, G., Haslam, S. A., Morton, T., & Swaab, R. I. (2006). The dynamics of personal

and social identity formation. In T. Postmes & J. Jetten (Eds.), Individuality and the group:

Advances in social identity (pp. 215–236). London: Sage.

Postmes, T., Haslam, S. A., & Swaab, R. I. (2005a). Social influence in small groups: An interactive

model of social identity formation. European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 1–42.

184 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 19: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Cihangir, S. (2001). Quality of decision making and group norms.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 918–930.

Postmes, T., Spears, R., & Lea, M. (2000). The formation of group norms in computer-mediated

communication. Human Communication Research, 26(3), 341–371.

Postmes, T., Spears, R., Novak, R., & Lee, T. (2005b). Individuality and social influence in groups:

Inductive and deductive routes to group identity. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 89, 747–763.

Prentice, D. A., Miller, D. T., & Lightdale, J. R. (1994). Asymmetries in attachments to groups and to

their members: Distinguishing between common-identity and common-bond groups.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 484–493.

Pruitt, D. G., & Carnevale, P. J. (1993). Negotiation in social conflict. Buckingham: Open

University Press.

Pruitt, D., & Rubin, J. (1986). Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate, and settlement. New York:

Random House.

Raudenbusch, S., Bryk, A., & Congdon, R. (2001). Hierarchical Linear and Nonlinear Modeling

( Version 5.05).

Rink, F., & Ellemers, N. (2007). Diversity as a source of common identity: Towards a social identity

framework for studying the effects of diversity in organizations. British Journal of

Management, 18, 19–29.

Robert, C., & Carnevale, P. J. (1997). Group choice in ultimatum bargaining. Organizational

Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 256–279.

Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of

variance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Sassenberg, K., & Postmes, T. (2002). Cognitive and strategic processes in small groups: Effects of

anonymity of the self and anonymity of the group on social influence. British Journal of Social

Psychology, 41, 463–480.

Sedikides, C., & Brewer, M. B. (2001). Individual self, relational self, collective self. Philadelphia,

PA: Psychology Press.

Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). The self-serving bias in

relational context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 378–386.

Sherif, M. (1935). A study of some social factors in perceptions. Archives of Psychology, 27, 1–60.

Simon, B., & Sturmer, S. (2003). Respect for group members: Intragroup determinants of collective

identification and group-serving behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29,

183–193.

Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and

advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage Publications.

Spears, R. (2001). The interaction between the individual and collective self: Self-categorization in

context. In C. Sedikides & M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Individual self, relational self, and collective self:

Partners, opponents, or strangers? (pp. 171–198). Philadelphia, PA: The Psychology Press.

Spears, R., Lea, M., & Lee, S. (1990). De-individuation and group polarization in computer-

mediated communication. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29(2), 121–134.

Swaab, R. I., Postmes, T., Van Beest, I., & Spears, R. (2007). Shared cognition as a product of, and

precursor to, shared social identity: The role of communication in negotiations. Personality

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(2), 187–199.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin &

S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–48). Monterey, CA:

Brooks-Cole.

Thompson, L., Kray, L. J., & Lind, E. A. (1998). Cohesion and respect: An examination of group decision

making and escalation dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 289–311.

Thompson, L.,Valley, K.L., & Kramer, R.M. (1995).The Bittersweet feelingof success: An examination

of social perception in negotiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 467–492.

Turner, J. C. (1982). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social

identity and intergroup relations (pp. 15–40). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 185

Page 20: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the

social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.

Van Beest, I., Van Dijk, E., & Wilke, H. (2003). The excluded player in coalition formation.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 237–247.

Van Vugt, M., & De Cremer, D. (1999). Collective action in social dilemmas: The impact of group

identification on the selection and cooperation with leaders. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 76, 587–599.

Weingart, L. R., Bennett, R. J., & Brett, J. M. (1993). The impact of consideration of issues and

motivational orientation on group negotiation process and outcome. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 78(3), 504–517.

Wilder, D., & Simon, A. F. (1998). Categorical and dynamic groups: Implications for social

perception and intergroup behavior. In J. Schopler & C. Sedikides (Eds.), Intergroup cognition

and intergroup behavior (pp. 27–44). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Wilkinson, L., & The Task Force on Statistical Inference. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology

journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 34, 594–604.

Witt, A. P., & Kerr, N. L. (2002). ‘Me versus just us versus us all’ Categorization and cooperation in

nested social dilemmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 616–637.

Yuki, M. (2003). Intergroup comparison versus intragroup relationships: A cross-cultural

examination of social identity theory in North American and East Asian cultural contexts.

Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 166–183.

Received 21 February 2006; revised version received 3 May 2007

Appendix: Deductive identity manipulation

This part of the study is about your opinion of the group of negotiators as

representatives of different parties with a common interest. In the current section, weare interested in your opinion of other negotiators and their common goal. Please fill in

the questionnaire after you have read each statement and indicate whether you agree

with it or not. Please try to empathize with your role and the fact that you are going to

negotiate with two others as good as possible during this activity.

Low deductive identity condition

Pilot (N ¼ 50) % agree

I consider the three parties in the negotiation as threeindependent individuals

86

I think each negotiator considers his or her own interests 98I think each negotiator decides for him or herself which proposal(s) to make 90In the end, an entrepreneur only has to deal with his or her own profit 100For the parties involved in this negotiation, own profit will beequally important as the attainment of a mutually satisfying agreement

90

I see myself and the others as three independent negotiatorsand a group at the same time

82

When these negotiators talk with each other, it will be toattain outcomes which are beneficial to themselves

72

How many times did you tick ‘Disagree’ with the above statements? X ¼ 1:34 SD ¼ 1:81Own interests will not be important to these negotiators 4There will not be any difference between the group of negotiating parties’ 0Own profit is guaranteed when we attain good outcomesfor the group as a whole

38

186 Roderick I. Swaab et al.

Page 21: Identity formation in multiparty negotiationsfaculty.insead.edu/roderick-swaab/documents/Paper... · shared identity formation is referred to as a process of top-down deductive identity

Copyright © The British Psychological SocietyReproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society

High deductive identity condition

(Continued)

Pilot (N ¼ 50) % agree

For these negotiators, profit for the own organization will be less relevantthan good outcomes for the collective

8

I see myself and the other negotiators as a group without any differences 6When these negotiators talk with each other, especially those outcomeswhich are good for the collective will count

32

I am sure that I and the other negotiators are going to form astrong collective

48

How many times did you tick ‘Agree’ with the above statements? X ¼ 2:00 SD ¼ 2:08Now calculate your identification score by summing the scoresyou got in the first and the second part

X ¼ 3:36 SD ¼ 3:20

Pilot (N ¼ 50) % agree

I see myself and the other negotiators as a group with common goals 82The common goal will be important in this group of negotiators 100Own interest is not the only thing which counts for this negotiating group 88A collective outcome and a profitable shopping mall contribute to theprofit of each organization separately

100

For these negotiators, good collective outcomes will berelated to individual profit

89.8

When these negotiators discuss issues with each other,collective outcomes will also count

80

I think I and the other negotiators are able to form a collective 88How many times did you tick ‘Agree’ with the above statements? X ¼ 6:30 SD ¼ 0:95I think that own interests are the only thing negotiators care about 20I think each negotiator only strives for good outcomes forhim/herself without considering others’ profit

12

These entrepreneurs only care about individual profit 55.1A collective outcome will be unimportant to the negotiators involved 16I and the others are totally independent of each other 10When the negotiators discuss with each other, it will onlybe to attain outcomes which are good for themselves

48

I see the negotiating parties only as individuals 14How many times did you tick ‘Disagree’ with the above statements? X ¼ 5:38 SD ¼ 1:38Now calculate your identification score by summing up the scoresyou got in the first and the second part

X ¼ 11:70 SD ¼ 1:99

Identity formation in multiparty negotiations 187