5
Commentary ‘‘I am worried, Doctor!’’ Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship Arnstein Finset * Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway 1. Introduction In many medical interviews patients present their hopes and uncertainties, feelings and worries. But most often patients do not express their distress as directly as indicated in the title of this article. Rather, negative feelings are often expressed as indirect hints about underlying emotions, often referred to as cues [1,2] or clues [3]. However, feelings may also be expressed more explicitly, labeled in the literature for instance as concerns [1] or empathic opportunities [4]. Fig. 1 presents a model of the consultation starting with the personality of the patient as well as the clinician (trait), the state of the participants in the encounters and characteristics of the situation. In the actual interchange of information there are many elements which could be included in the model [5], but here the emotional communication is emphasized, in terms of the clinician’s eliciting of patient emotions, the cues and concerns of the patient and the clinician responses. The model also includes a feature often neglected, namely the biological processes taking place in the consultation. Finally, the model includes the outcomes of the consultation in terms of intermediate outcomes and effects on health and quality of life of the patient as well as the clinician. In this paper we will mostly concentrate on patients’ cues and concerns and their reactions to how clinicians have responded to what patients have expressed. A crucial element of the model is the clinician’s response to the patient’s cues and concerns. In the present commentary I suggest some potential areas of future research on emotional communica- tion in medical encounters by posing four questions, all with emphasis on the responses of clinicians to patient emotion (see Fig. 1): What patient, clinician and interaction factors predict how clinicians respond to emotional cues and concerns? What happens in the body and the brain during emotional talk? Are there individual differences in patients’ responses to emotional talk in medical interviews? Do clinicians’ responses to emotion affect health outcome? In this commentary the discussion of the four questions is supported by a selective review of research literature, often with reference to papers from our own group, when relevant. A more comprehensive discussion would require a systematic review of the literature. 2. What factors predict how clinicians respond to patients’ emotional cues and concerns? Studies show large variations in how health professionals respond to patient emotion. For instance, Mjaaland et al. found in a Patient Education and Counseling 88 (2012) 359–363 A R T I C L E I N F O Keywords: Communication Doctor–patient relationship Emotions A B S T R A C T Objective: To review research on emotional communication in medical interviews regarding predictors, physiological correlates and effects of clinicians’ responses to patients’ cues and concerns and individual differences among patients. Methods: A selective review of research literature on emotional communication in medical interviews was conducted. Results: Four questions regarding emotional communication were explored: What factors predict how clinicians respond to emotional cues and concerns? What happens in the brain and the body of both patients and clinicians during emotional talk? Are there individual differences in patients’ responses to emotional talk in medical interviews? Do clinicians’ responses to emotion affect health outcome? Conclusion: Building on evidence reviewed, research on predictors of clinician responses, physiological correlates of behavior, individual differences and effects on outcome should be further pursued. Practice implications: In communication skills training programs, better understanding of the phenomena described could have implications for training clinicians to handle emotions in clinical interviews. ß 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. * Correspondence address: Department of Behavioral Sciences in Medicine, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, POB 1111, Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway. Tel.: +47 2285 1435; fax: +47 2285 1300. E-mail address: arnstein.fi[email protected]. Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Patient Education and Counseling jo ur n al h o mep ag e: w ww .elsevier .co m /loc ate/p ated u co u 0738-3991/$ see front matter ß 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2012.06.022

“I am worried, Doctor!” Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: “I am worried, Doctor!” Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

Patient Education and Counseling 88 (2012) 359–363

Commentary

‘‘I am worried, Doctor!’’ Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

Arnstein Finset *

Department of Behavioral Sciences, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway

A R T I C L E I N F O

Keywords:

Communication

Doctor–patient relationship

Emotions

A B S T R A C T

Objective: To review research on emotional communication in medical interviews regarding predictors,

physiological correlates and effects of clinicians’ responses to patients’ cues and concerns and individual

differences among patients.

Methods: A selective review of research literature on emotional communication in medical interviews

was conducted.

Results: Four questions regarding emotional communication were explored: What factors predict how

clinicians respond to emotional cues and concerns? What happens in the brain and the body of both

patients and clinicians during emotional talk? Are there individual differences in patients’ responses to

emotional talk in medical interviews? Do clinicians’ responses to emotion affect health outcome?

Conclusion: Building on evidence reviewed, research on predictors of clinician responses, physiological

correlates of behavior, individual differences and effects on outcome should be further pursued.

Practice implications: In communication skills training programs, better understanding of the

phenomena described could have implications for training clinicians to handle emotions in clinical

interviews.

� 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Patient Education and Counseling

jo ur n al h o mep ag e: w ww .e lsev ier . co m / loc ate /p ated u co u

1. Introduction

In many medical interviews patients present their hopes anduncertainties, feelings and worries. But most often patients do notexpress their distress as directly as indicated in the title of thisarticle. Rather, negative feelings are often expressed as indirecthints about underlying emotions, often referred to as cues [1,2] orclues [3]. However, feelings may also be expressed more explicitly,labeled in the literature for instance as concerns [1] or empathicopportunities [4].

Fig. 1 presents a model of the consultation starting with thepersonality of the patient as well as the clinician (trait), the state ofthe participants in the encounters and characteristics of thesituation. In the actual interchange of information there are manyelements which could be included in the model [5], but here theemotional communication is emphasized, in terms of theclinician’s eliciting of patient emotions, the cues and concerns ofthe patient and the clinician responses. The model also includes afeature often neglected, namely the biological processes takingplace in the consultation. Finally, the model includes the outcomesof the consultation in terms of intermediate outcomes and effects

* Correspondence address: Department of Behavioral Sciences in Medicine,

Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, POB 1111, Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway.

Tel.: +47 2285 1435; fax: +47 2285 1300.

E-mail address: [email protected].

0738-3991/$ – see front matter � 2012 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2012.06.022

on health and quality of life of the patient as well as the clinician. Inthis paper we will mostly concentrate on patients’ cues andconcerns and their reactions to how clinicians have responded towhat patients have expressed.

A crucial element of the model is the clinician’s response to thepatient’s cues and concerns. In the present commentary I suggestsome potential areas of future research on emotional communica-tion in medical encounters by posing four questions, all withemphasis on the responses of clinicians to patient emotion (seeFig. 1): What patient, clinician and interaction factors predict howclinicians respond to emotional cues and concerns? What happensin the body and the brain during emotional talk? Are thereindividual differences in patients’ responses to emotional talk inmedical interviews? Do clinicians’ responses to emotion affecthealth outcome?

In this commentary the discussion of the four questions issupported by a selective review of research literature, often withreference to papers from our own group, when relevant. A morecomprehensive discussion would require a systematic review ofthe literature.

2. What factors predict how clinicians respond to patients’emotional cues and concerns?

Studies show large variations in how health professionalsrespond to patient emotion. For instance, Mjaaland et al. found in a

Page 2: “I am worried, Doctor!” Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

Fig. 1. A model of the medical consultation, with reference to topics of the present paper.

A. Finset / Patient Education and Counseling 88 (2012) 359–363360

study of medical interviews in a general hospital across specialtiesthat physicians provided room for further disclosure in response toabout half of all emotional cues and concerns, but more often withreference to the medical than the affective content of the cue orconcern [6]. Similarly, Butow et al. reported that oncologistseffectively identified and responded to the majority of informa-tional cues; however, they were less effective in addressing cuesfor emotional support [7]. In another study from cancer care Pollaket al. found that oncologists responded with empathy to 29% ofpatients’ expression of negative emotion [8]. A number of studiesreport similar results, indicating that the emotional aspect ofpatients’ messages are overlooked and not responded to byphysicians [6,9–11]. Rather than follow up on emotional cues,physicians often, but not always, respond with biomedicalquestioning and information giving or rather nonspecific acknowl-edgment or premature reassurance [9–12].

The responses of nurses to cues and concerns have also beeninvestigated. The results vary. In a study from a surgical oncologyclinic Uitterhoeve et al. found that nurses explored or acknowl-edged 45% of patients’ emotional expressions, but more than half ofthe expressions were met with distancing behaviors [13]. Eideet al., on the other hand, found that 75% of all emotionalexpressions were met with implicit recognition, whereas another13% were explicitly recognized in a study of admittance interviewsto a pain clinic [14]. The differences between findings may be dueboth due methodological differences and different clinical tasksand settings in the respective studies.

There is little knowledge on what different factors actuallyinfluence clinicians’ responses to the feelings and worriesexpressed by patients in medical interviews. In earlier studiesemphasis has been more on which physician behaviors influencethe likelihood of subsequently expressed cues and concerns [15–18] than on characteristics that may influence the response. Weknow that female clinicians engage in more emotionally focusedtalk than male clinicians do [19]. Other studies have reportedassociations between patient attributes as well as situation factorsand clinician empathic behavior [8,20]. However we know littleabout the communicational dynamics, what interaction factors

predict how clinicians respond to emotions. Potential communi-cation variables include the source of the expression of concern; i.e.whether the cue or concern is initiated by the patient or by theclinician (e.g. by asking a question, or providing a facilitativeremark); characteristics of the content of the cue/concern, such asthe emotional explicitness of the expression; and the timing of theemotional expression in the consultation [1].

One recently developed approach to study emotional commu-nication in clinical encounters is the Verona Coding Definitions ofEmotional Sequences (VR-CoDES), developed by a network ofresearchers who have met annually at the University of Verona todevelop this coding system [21,22]. The VR-CoDES system includesspecifications of both the patient cue or concern and the clinicianresponse, and should be well suited for an analysis of the dynamicsbetween patient and provider expressions.

Applying the VR-CoDES we recently published a study based onanalyses of video recordings of 196 consultations in cancer care,interviewed by oncologists or nurses [23]. In that paper the unit ofanalysis was the consultation, as opposed to individual utterancesand responses. In a subsequent paper the unit of analysis was theindividual cue/concern sequence at a ‘‘turn’’ level (N = 580),making it possible to study the dynamics of the basic sequencesof cues and concerns in terms of the timing within the consultationand the associations between timing, source, explicitness andresponses [24]. The dependent variable was whether the clinicianacknowledges the cue or concern and opens up for furtherelaboration or not, referred to as providing room for furtherdisclosure, a basic dimension is several methods for classifyingclinician responses to emotions. The clinician may provide roomfor disclosure for instance by an inviting silence, by a back-channeling remark or by an explicit acknowledgment or explora-tion of the cue or concern [22].

We found that nurses in admittance interviews in in-patientunits were five times more likely to provide space for furtherdisclosure of cues and concerns than oncologists in out-patientfollow-up consultations. Moreover, both oncologists and nursesacknowledged cues or concerns initiated by themselves morefrequently than cues and concerns initiated by patients. Similarly,

Page 3: “I am worried, Doctor!” Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

A. Finset / Patient Education and Counseling 88 (2012) 359–363 361

Del Piccolo et al. found recently that psychiatrists in their study ofpsychiatric consultations provided space for further disclosure of aconcern more frequently when the concern had been initiated bythe psychiatrist in the first place [25]. The providing of space forfurther disclosure was also dependent on the explicitness andtiming of the cues and concerns [24].

These findings indicate that clinicians’ provisions of space forfurther exploration do not occur at random. A number of factors,regarding clinician characteristics as well as features with theongoing interaction, influence whether or not the clinicianacknowledges the emotion by providing room for furtherdisclosure. In future studies it might be fruitful to supplementdata regarding the quantitative analyses of cues and concerns andclinician responses with qualitative analyses of the content of thecommunication.

3. What happens in the brain and the body duringemotional talk?

All emotional behavior has obviously physiological correlates.There is a small, but growing literature on psychophysiologicalactivation during medical encounters [26]. Psychophysiologicalmethods have for instance been applied to measure cortisolresponses of medical students in difficult consultations withpatients [27,28] and a delayed cortisol response in patients 24 hafter a consultations was observed [29]. With its strong associationwith the sympathetic activity electrodermal activity (EDA) isanother physiological marker of emotion which has been appliedin studies of medical interviews [26].

In regards to clinicians’ responses to cues and concerns, we havelittle knowledge about the psychophysiological activation patternsin patients following the clinician’s response to patients’ expres-sion of emotion. Will an empathic acknowledgment of emotionsstimulate further emotional activation or will an empathicresponse rather buffer and down-regulate patient emotion? Thiswas the question we asked in a recent study on the effect ofempathic statements on psychophysiological responses. Forty-eight patients with fibromyalgia were given arranged interviewswith a research assistant trained in responding to patients’expression of emotion, who systematically varied her degree ofempathy in her responses [29]. We found that high empathy wasassociated with increased electrodermal activity in the patient,probably due to the fact that interviews with more empathicstatements also contained more expressions of patients’ emotionsdue to the eliciting effects of empathic remarks.

But emotional communication in medical interviews, includingcommunication involving empathic statements, is often associatedwith feelings in both individuals involved, not only in the patient.The contagion effect of emotions, when an individual shares theemotional experience of a person with whom he or she interacts, isassociated with activity in areas of the frontal brain known to becrucial for emotional processing and often suggested as the mainfocus for subjective experience in emotional processing, suchventromedial prefrontal cortex [30–32]. Matthews et al. havedescribed such moments of shared affect in doctor–patientrelations as ‘‘connexions’’, where the doctor feels an intenseemotion and sense of sharing with the patient [33].

Physiological correlates of reciprocal patterns of behavior interms of synchronous patterns of autonomic activation have beenreported both in interaction between mothers and infants (withmore synchrony in harmonious mother–infant interactions) [34],between spouses [35], and in a few studies also between doctorand patient [36], most often investigating concordance betweendoctor and patient heart rate during psychotherapy [37,38]. Morerecently Marci and colleagues reported a synchronous pattern inelectrodermal activation in a study of therapist–patient interaction

in psychotherapy sessions. A high degree of reported empathy inthe interaction was associated with higher electrodermal activa-tion [39].

In the studies just mentioned the expression of emotion isassociated with increased levels or synchrony of psychophysio-logical activation. But there is also indirect evidence thatverbalization of emotion may regulate psychophysiologicalactivation. In an early study of this phenomenon Hariri andcollaborators exposed their subjects to threatening and fearfulnon-face stimuli derived from the International Affective PictureSystem (IAPS) [40]. Under one condition they were given thestimuli without specific instructions. Under another condition theywere told to verbalize the emotion that was elicited from seeingthe pictures. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)revealed that whereas just seeing the stimuli was associated witha bilateral amygdala response, cognitive evaluation of these samestimuli, with a verbal labeling of the emotion, was associated withattenuation of this amygdala response and a correlated increase inresponse of the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and theanterior cingulate cortex, indicating that higher centers in thefrontal cortex overruled the more primitive affective ones.Moreover, this pattern was reflected in changes in electrodermalactivation, a marker of sympathetic activity. Other researchershave reported similar results. Applied on clinical communication,these findings could possibly indicate that by eliciting patients toexpress cues and concerns and through adequate responses thedoctor might actually help the patients to a better regulation ofemotional processing in the brain, by a downregulation ofamygdala activation and relief of stress, confirmed also by thechanges in electrodermal activation. Although it is difficult to applybrain imaging technology to clinical communication research,further studies should investigate how different clinician responsepatterns to patients’ cues and concerns could be associated withspecific activation patterns in the brain.

4. Are there individual differences in how patients respond toemotional talk in medical interviews?

People are different in how they perceive and respond toemotional stimuli. However, in the research literature there is littleemphasis on individual differences in emotional responses. Weknow little about how different patients react to clinicians’responses to patients’ cues and concerns.

We have in our group investigated responses to interviewingstyles which were quite different in terms of emotional expression.In a first study students with high and low trait anxietyvolunteered to take part in an arranged consultations with aphysician. They were instructed to bring up any medical complaintthat had in the present or had suffered in the past, or a medicalconcern regarding their parents or other close relatives. Theinterviews were performed according to two different experimen-tal conditions. During one condition, labeled as patient centered,the physician attended to psychosocial topics and respondedexplicitly to emotional concerns. In the other condition, labeledphysician centered, the physician concentrated strictly on themedical complaints and largely ignored psychosocial issues andemotional concerns. Dependent variables were affective responsesas measured with the Profiles of Mood States (PONS) and cortisolresponses. We expected stronger emotional responses to theconsultation in patients with high state anxiety. Moreover, weexpected that patients with high state anxiety would respond withattenuated emotional activation in the patient centered condition.However, in contrary to expectation, we found more arousalamong high anxiety patients in the patient centered condition [41].

In a subsequent study we looked at physician-patient interac-tion in arranged consultations with patients with fibromyalgia

Page 4: “I am worried, Doctor!” Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

A. Finset / Patient Education and Counseling 88 (2012) 359–363362

with and without difficulties in identifying and expressingemotions (alexithymia). Similarly to the study of high and lowanxiety students we found that fibromyalgia patients withalexithymia reported more confusion, less vigor and higher heartrate activation in arranged consultations with a ‘‘patient centered’’communication focus on psychosocial aspects as compared to a‘‘doctor centered’’ symptom focused communication style [42].These patients reported to be more satisfied with many empathicstatements from the doctor [43]. When the patients later viewedthe videos of the consultations, patients with alexithymiadisplayed more confusion, less vigor, increased EDA and a feelingof less control over the consultation in consultations with apsychosocial emphasis. In contrast, for patients without problemsof emotion regulation the ‘‘patient centered’’ psychosocial focus inthe communication style was associated with reduced electroder-mal activation and a feeling of more control in the consultationthan was the case for the condition with a focus on symptoms [42].The findings indicate consistent and potentially predicablepatterns of individual differences in how patients react toemotional content in medical consultations, characterized by anapparent hyperreactivity to emotional talk by some patients withproblems in emotion regulation.

5. Do clinicians’ responses to emotion affect health outcome?

In medicine the bottom line will always be effects on healthstatus. Does it really matter how clinicians respond to the cues andconcerns of patients?

The association between communication behavior and healthoutcome is a difficult one to study. Street et al. have recentlydiscussed a number of issues regarding the effects of communica-tion on outcome. They suggest careful analyses of potentialpathways of proximal, intermediate steps on the pathway to healthendpoints [44]. Yet, there are some studies which suggest thatopening up for patient emotions and providing empathicresponses may be associated with positive patient outcome interms of reduced distress [45,46], patient adherence [47,48] andsymptom resolution [49,50]. For instance, in a large Danish studydistress, self-efficacy, and perceived control were assessed prior toand after the consultation by 454 patients attending an oncologyoutpatient clinic. After the consultation, the patients also rated thephysicians’ communicative behaviors by completing a patient–physician relationship inventory (PPRI), and the physicians wereasked to estimate patient satisfaction. The overall results showedthat higher PPRI scores of physician attentiveness and empathywere associated with greater patient satisfaction, increased self-efficacy, and reduced emotional distress following the consulta-tion. [46]. Although a few studies are promising, there is need formore careful research to entangle the relationship between howclinicians respond to patient emotion and health outcome. Assuggested by Street et al. proximal outcomes should be identifiedas the first step in a pathway leading to actual health outcomes[44]. Proximal outcomes in relation to the effects of clinicianresponses to patients’ cues and concerns could be the experience ofthe patients after the consultations as to whether importantconcerns were met or not, as investigated by Eide et al. in a study ofadmittance interviews to a pain clinic [51].

6. Conclusions

I have suggested four areas of particular interest for furtherresearch on emotional communication in medical interviews.These areas concern further investigation into the predictors,physiological correlates and effects of clinicians’ responses topatients’ cues and concerns, and individual differences in have

patients react to clinicians’ responses to their emotional state-ments.

First, recent studies indicate that clinicians’ responses toemotional cues and concerns do not occur at random. Futurestudies are needed to clarify the dynamics of interaction patternsthat are associated with the different types of clinician responses toemotion.

Second, even if we know that emotional talk in the consultationevokes psychophysiological activation, we still have too littleknowledge on the specific activation patterns. More studies areneeded to better know when clinicians’ responses to patient cuesand concerns evoke or buffer patient activation and when theactivation patterns of patients and clinicians are synchronous ornot.

Third, there are individual differences in how patients respondto emotional talk. In a few studies we have seen an interactioneffect between individual traits and communication style,indicating that some patients with problems in emotion regulationreact with increased emotional activation to a communicationstyle which opens up for emotional talk. We need more research totest these associations, preferably in natural settings.

Fourth, we certainly need more research on how clinicianresponses to emotion may impact on health outcome. For instance,we sometimes seem to presume that providing room for furtherdisclosure is always the best way to respond to cues and concerns.But such assumptions need empirical testing in carefully designedstudies, which include investigation of proximal outcomes andpathways from proximal outcomes to health end points.

7. Practice implications

In the present paper the emphasis has been on suggestions forfurther research rather than practice implications. But cliniciansshould also be aware of some of the predictors, correlates andconsequences of emotional behavior in the medical interview.Moreover, in designing communication skills training programs, abetter understanding of the phenomena discussed could haveconsequences for approaches and techniques in training cliniciansto handle emotions better in clinical interviews.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

[1] Zimmermann C, Del Piccolo L, Finset A. Cues and concerns by patients inmedical consultations: a literature review. Psychol Bull 2007;133:438–63.

[2] Lussier MT, Richard C. Handling cues from patients. Can Family Physician2009;55:1213–4.

[3] Levinson W, Gorawara-Bhat R, Lamb J. A study of patient clues and physicianresponses in primary care and surgical settings. J Amer Med Assoc 2000;284:1021–7.

[4] Suchman AL, Markakis K, Beckman HB, Frankel R. A model of empathiccommunication in the medical interview. J Amer Med Assoc 1997;277:678–82.

[5] Finset A, Mjaaland TA. The medical consultation viewed as a value chain: aneurobehavioral approach. Patient Educ Couns 2009;74:323–30.

[6] Mjaaland TA, Finset A, Jensen BF, Gulbrandsen P. Physicians’ responses topatients’ expressions of negative emotions in hospital consultations: a video-based observational study. Patient Educ Couns 2011;84:332–7.

[7] Butow PN, Brown RF, Cogar S, Tattersall MHN, Dunn SM. Oncologists’ reactionsto cancer patients’ verbal cues. Psycho-Oncology 2002;11:47–58.

[8] Pollak KI, Arnold RM, Jeffreys AS, Alexander SC, Olsen MK, Abernethy AP, et al.Oncologist communication about emotion during visits with patients withadvanced cancer. J Clin Oncol 2007;25:5748–52.

[9] Bell RA, Kravitz RL, Thom D, Krupat E, Azari R. Unmet expectations for care andthe patient–physician relationship. J Gen Int Med 2002;17:817–24.

[10] Kennifer SL, Alexander SC, Pollak KI, Jefreys AS, Olsen MK, Rodriguez KL, et al.Negative emotions in cancer care: Do oncologists’ responses depend onseverity and type of emotion? Patient Educ Couns 2009;76:51–6.

Page 5: “I am worried, Doctor!” Emotions in the doctor–patient relationship

A. Finset / Patient Education and Counseling 88 (2012) 359–363 363

[11] Ryan H, Schofield P, Cockburn J, Butow P, Tattersall M, Turner J, et al. How torecognize and manage psychological distress in cancer patients. Eur J CancerCare 2005;14:7–15.

[12] Epstein RM, Hadee T, Carroll J, Meldrum SC, Lardner J, Shields CG. ‘‘Could thisbe something serious?’’ Reassurance, uncertainty, and empathy in response topatients’ expressions of worry. J Gen Int Med 2007;22:1731–9.

[13] Uitterhoeve R, de Leeuw J, Bensing J, Heaven C, Borm G, DeMulder P, et al. Cue-responding behaviours of oncology nurses in video-simulated interviews. JAdv Nurs 2008;61:71–80.

[14] Eide H, Quera V, Graugaard P, Finset A. Physician–patient dialogue surround-ing patients’ expression of concern: applying sequence analysis to RIAS. Soc SciMed 2004;59:145–55.

[15] Zimmermann C, Del Piccolo L, Mazzi MA. Patient cues and medical interview-ing in general practice: examples of the application of sequential analysis.Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc 2003;12:115–23.

[16] Langewitz W, Nubling M, Weber H. A theory-based approach to analysingconversation sequences. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc 2003;12:103–8.

[17] Bensing JM, Verheul W, Jansen J, Langewitz WA. Looking for trouble: the addedvalue of sequence analysis in finding evidence for the role of physicians inpatients’ disclosure of cues and concerns. Med Care 2010;48:583–8.

[18] Oguchi M, Jansen J, Butow P, Colagiuri B, Divine R, Dhillon H. Measuring theimpact of nurse cue-response behaviour on cancer patients’ emotional cues.Patient Educ Couns 2011;82:163–8.

[19] Roter DL, Hall JA. Physician gender and patient-centered communication:a critical review of empirical research. Annu Rev Public Health 2004;25:497–519.

[20] Neumann M, Wirtz M, Bollschweiler E, Mercer SW, Warm M, Wolf J, et al.Determinants and patient-reported long-term outcomes of physician empa-thy in oncology: a structural equation modelling approach. Patient Educ Couns2007;69:63–75.

[21] Zimmermann C, Del Piccolo L, Bensing J, Bergvik S, De Haes H, Eide H, et al.Coding patient emotional cues and concerns in medical consultations: theVerona coding definitions of emotional sequences (VR-CoDES). Patient EducCouns 2011;82:141–8.

[22] Del Piccolo L, De Haes H, Heaven C, Jansen J, Verheul W, Bensing J, et al.Development of the Verona coding definitions of emotional sequences to codehealth providers’ responses (VR-CoDES-P) to patient cues and concerns.Patient Educ Couns 2011;82:149–55.

[23] Heyn L, Ruland CM, Finset A. Effects of an interactive tailored patient assess-ment tool on eliciting and responding to cancer patients’ cues and concerns inclinical consultations with physicians and nurses. Patient Educ Couns 2012;96:158–65.

[24] Heyn L. Improving communication in cancer care. A quasi-experimental studytesting the effects of an interactive tailored patient assessment tool oncommunication between cancer patients and physicians and nurses. Doctoraldissertation. University of Oslo; 2012.

[25] Del Piccolo L, Mazzi MA, Goss C, Rimondini M, Zimmermann C. How emotionsemerge and are dealt with in first diagnostic consultations in psychiatry.Patient Educ Couns 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pec. 2012.01.010.

[26] Hulsman RL, Smets EMA, Karemaker JM, de Haes HJCJM. The psychophysiologyof medical communication. Linking two worlds of research. Patient EducCouns 2011;84:420–7.

[27] Hulsman RL, Pranger S, Koot S, Fabriek M, Karemaker JM, Smets EMA. Howstressful is doctor–patient communication? Physiological and psychologicalstress of medical students in simulated history taking and bad-newsconsultations. Intl J Psychophysiol 2010;77:26–34.

[28] Van Dulmen S, Tromp F, Grosfeld F, ten CO, Bensing J. The impact of assessingsimulated bad news consultations on medical students’ stress response andcommunication performance. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2007;32:943–50.

[29] Finset A, Graugaard PK, Holgersen K. Salivary cortisol response after a medicalinterview. The impact of physician communication behaviour, depressedaffect and alexithymia. Patient Educ Couns 2006;60:115–24.

[30] Vollm BA, Taylor AN, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, et al.Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magneticresonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage 2006;29:90–8.

[31] Farrow TF, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JF, Tarrier N, et al.Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neurore-port 2001;12:2433–8.

[32] Botvinick M, Jha AP, Bylsma LM, Fabian SA, Solomon PE, Prkachin KM. Viewingfacial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experi-ence of pain. Neuroimage 2005;25:312–9.

[33] Matthews DA, Suchman AL, Branch Jr WT. Making ‘‘connexions’’: enhancingthe therapeutic potential of patient–clinician relationships. Ann Intern Med1993;118:973–7.

[34] Beebe B, Alson D, Jaffe J, Feldstein S. Vocal congruence in mother–infant play.In: Field T, Fox N, editors. Social perception in infants. Norwood, NJ: Ablex;1988. p. 217–47.

[35] Gottman JM, Levenson RW. A valid measure for obtaining self-report of affectin marital interaction. J Consult Clin Psychol 1985;53:151–60.

[36] Levenson RW, Ruef AM. Physiological aspects of emotional knowledge andrapport. In: Ickes W, editor. Empathic accuracy. New York: Guilford; 1997. p.44–72.

[37] DiMascio A, Boyd RW, Greenblatt M. Physiological correlates of tension andantagonism during psychotherapy: a study of ‘‘interpersonal physiology’’.Psychosom Med 1957;19:99–104.

[38] Reidbord SP, Redington DJ. Nonlinear analysis of autonomic responses in atherapist during psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis 1993;181:428–35.

[39] Marci CD, Ham J, Moran E, Orr SP. Physiologic correlates of perceived therapistempathy and social–emotional process during psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis2004;5:103–11.

[40] Hariri AR, Bookheimer SY, Mazziotta JC. Modulating emotional responses: effectsof a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport 2000;11:43–8.

[41] Graugaard PK, Finset A. Trait anxiety and reactions to patient-centered anddoctor-centered styles of communication: an experimental study. PsychosomMed 2000;62:33–9.

[42] Finset A. Emotional intelligence and alexithymia and the doctor–patientrelationship. In: Koh KG, editor. Somatization and psychosomatic symptoms.Springer Verlag, in press.

[43] Graugaard P, Holgersen K, Finset A. Communicating with alexithymic and non-alexithymic patients: an experimental study of the effect of psychosocial talkand empathy on satisfaction. Psychother Psychosom 2004;73:92–100.

[44] Street Jr RL, Makoul G, Arora NK, Epstein RM. How does communication heal?Pathways linking clinician–patient communication to health outcomes. Pa-tient Educ Couns 2009;74:295–301.

[45] Duric V, Butow P, Sharpe L, Lobb E, Meiser B, Barratt A, et al. Reducingpsychological distress in genetic counseling consultations for breast cancer.J Genet Couns 2011;12:243–64.

[46] Zachariae R, Pedersen CG, Jensen AB, Ehrnrooth E, Rossen PB, von der MH.Association of perceived physician communication style with patient satis-faction, distress, cancer-related self-efficacy, and perceived control over thedisease. Brit J Cancer 2003;88:658–65.

[47] Squier RW. A model of empathic understanding and adherence to treatmentregimens in practitioner–patient relationships. Soc Sci Med 1990;30:325–39.

[48] Kim SS, Kaplowitz S, Johnston MV. The effects of physician empathy on patientsatisfaction and compliance. Eval Health Prof 2004;27:237–51.

[49] Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C, Gonnella JS.Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic patients. Acad Med2011;86:359–64.

[50] Kelley JM, Lembo AJ, Ablon JS, Villanueva JJ, Conboy LA, Levy R, et al. Patientand practitioner influences on the placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome.Psychosom Med 2009;71:789–97.

[51] Eide H, Eide T, Rustøen T, Finset A. Patient validation of cues and concerns identifiedaccording to Verona coding definitions of emotional sequences (VR-CoDES): avideo- and interview-based approach. Patient Educ Couns 2011;82:156–62.