Hudson Roxanne F

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    1/25

    Relations among reading skills and sub-skills and

    text-level reading proficiency in developing readers

    Roxanne F. Hudson Joseph K. Torgesen

    Holly B. Lane Stephen J. Turner

    Published online: 2 December 2010 Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

    Abstract Despite the recent attention to text reading fluency, few studies have

    studied the construct of oral reading rate and accuracy in connected text in a model

    that simultaneously examines many of the important variables in a multi-leveled

    fashion with young readers. Using Structural Equation Modeling, this study

    examined the measurement and structural relations of the rate and accuracy of

    variables important in early reading: phonemic blending, letter sounds, phonograms,

    decoding, single-word reading, reading comprehension, and text reading as well asreading comprehension among second grade readers. The effects from phonemic

    blending fluency and letter sound fluency to decoding were completely mediated by

    phonogram fluency, decoding fluency, single-word reading fluency, and reading

    comprehension had direct effects on the text reading fluency of the second grade

    students. Understanding the relationship among the many component skills of

    readers early in their reading development is important because a deficiency in any

    of the component skills has the potential to affect the development of other skills

    and, ultimately, the development of the child as a proficient reader.

    Keywords Decoding Reading fluency Young readers

    R. F. Hudson J. K. Torgesen

    Florida Center for Reading Research, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA

    H. B. Lane

    Department of Special Education, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA

    S. J. Turner

    Department of Educational Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA

    R. F. Hudson (&)

    Area of Special Education, University of Washington, Box 353600, Seattle, WA 98195, USA

    e-mail: [email protected]

    1 3

    Read Writ (2012) 25:483507

    DOI 10.1007/s11145-010-9283-6

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    2/25

    Modeling individual differences in decoding fluency

    in second grade readers

    Reading fluency is an important part of reading proficiency and reading a text

    fluently is critical for comprehending it (Breznitz, 2006; Daane, Campbell, Grigg,Goodman, & Oranje, 2005; Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp, & Jenkins, 2001; Samuels &

    Farstrup, 2006; Torgesen, Rashotte, & Alexander, 2001). Instruction leading to

    fluent text reading is a critical aspect of early reading instruction, and many

    researchers and practitioners have questions about the elements that make up

    reading fluency and explain the difficulties many children have developing into

    proficient readers.

    Ehris (1992) phases of word reading development provide a framework for

    understanding the development of reading fluency. Children in Ehris pre-alphabetic

    phase use cues unrelated to the letters and sounds to read or guess words. As theymove to Ehris partial-alphabetic and full-alphabetic phases they increase in their

    phonemic awareness and grasp of the correspondences between graphemes and

    phonemes. During this time they often use individual graphemephoneme recoding

    to read words, especially unfamiliar ones. As readers develop, they increasingly

    unitize, or read in larger letter units, rather than relying on individual graphemes.

    This results in increased decoding efficiency and oral reading fluency (Harn,

    Stoolmiller, & Chard,2008). It is likely that, as readers reach automaticity with an

    increasing number of units, they abandon the letter-by-letter decoding they initially

    used, instead of using rimes (McKay &Thompson, 2009; Treiman, Goswami, &Bruck,1990) and other larger letter patterns to read. This will lead to a transition to

    Ehris consolidated alphabetic phase where readers are fluent, have a large number

    of words they know by sight, and use larger letter patterns to decode unknown

    words. The number of unknown words would decrease, making analogy a much

    more useful strategy. Evidence of this pattern can be found in the longitudinal

    research of Speece and Ritchey (2005). In first grade, letter sound fluency (LSF) was

    a unique predictor of oral reading fluency level and slope after other predictors were

    in the model. By second grade, however, first grade LSF was no longer uniquely

    predictive. Thus, one would expect that early in reading development, subskills such

    as phonological awareness and letter sound fluency would be direct contributors to

    decoding and oral reading fluency, but later in reading development (e.g., second

    grade), the relationship among these variables would be mediated by other variables

    that represent more advanced reading such as automaticity with phonograms (i.e.,

    letter groups within a word that share a pattern across words such as rimes and

    suffixes) and single word reading.

    Predicting oral reading rate and accuracy

    As seen in Fig.1, text reading fluency, at least as measured by rate and accuracy of

    reading in connected text, involves parallel processes at the sub-lexical, lexical,

    sentence, text, and discourse level (Hudson, Pullen, Lane, & Torgesen, 2009;

    Torgesen et al.,2001). Automaticity in the sub-skills such as letter sound retrieval,

    484 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    3/25

    phonemic awareness, reading words by sight, and decoding processes are necessary

    for fluent text reading (Biemiller, 19771978). When these processes are not

    automatic, reading accuracy and rate suffer, as does comprehension (Perfetti, 1985;Perfetti & Hogaboam,1975). Because the reading processes share limited-capacity

    working memory, lack of efficiency in any process is likely to use more of the

    resources, starving the resource-intensive processes related to reading comprehension

    (Perfetti).

    Predictors of decoding fluency

    Beginning at the bottom of the model in Fig. 1, we predict that decoding fluency is

    explained by several lower-level within-word processes such as automaticity in

    letter sounds and phonemic blending. If any of the relevant retrieval processes

    operate slowly or inaccurately, decoding will be slowed. Better understanding of the

    role each plays can lead to the development of better tools for assessment and

    intervention.

    Fluency in phonemic blending is critical for decoding success. Over 20 years of

    research has established the importance of phonemic awareness in learning to

    decode (e.g., Adams,1990; National Reading Panel, 2000; Perfetti, Beck, Bell, &

    Hughes,1987; Rayner, Foorman, Perfetti, Pesetsky, & Seidenberg,2001; Wagner &

    Torgesen,1987). It is not enough to identify sounds associated with letters in a word

    to be a successful decoder. One must also blend those sounds together to produce

    the words pronunciation. Readers who are not automatic in this process have

    difficulty doing so while decoding unfamiliar words.

    Fluency in identifying letter sounds, or quickly and accurately producing

    the sounds represented by graphemes, is at the heart of the alphabetic principle.

    Fig. 1 Multi-level model for text reading fluency

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 485

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    4/25

    Without the knowledge of how sounds are systematically represented by letters,

    children cannot successfully decode (e.g., Adams, 1990; Ehri, 1998; Jenkins,

    Bausell, & Jenkins, 1972; National Reading Panel, 2000). Both phonological

    awareness and fluency in letter sounds in kindergarten were among the best

    predictors of first grade oral reading fluency (Speece, Mills, Ritchey, & Hillman,2003; Stage, Sheppard, Davidson, & Browning, 2001).

    Automaticity in recognition of phonograms (i.e., letter groups within a word that

    share a pattern across words) is a feature of the more advanced word recognition

    characteristic of the consolidated alphabetic reading phase (Ehri, 1992). Without

    knowledge of patterns across words, readers are not be able to move to more

    advanced, efficient decoding (Ehri, 2002). Both Treiman et al. (1990) and McKay and

    Thompson (2009) found that children read words with frequent or familiar rimes (a

    vowel plus syllable ending) more accurately than those with infrequent or unfamiliar

    ones. In addition, English is more regular at the level of rimes and larger chunks thanat the phoneme-grapheme level (Moats, 2000; Kessler & Treiman, 2003), making

    sound-symbol relationships at that level more predictable and useful in reading words.

    Readers need to develop context-sensitive mappings of relationships between

    phonemes and graphemes as well as larger units to become fluent decoders and

    readers (Berninger, Abbott, Vermeulen, & Fulton,2006; Brown & Deavers,1999).

    As evidenced by the directional arrows, we propose that the skills develop

    sequentially and predict the next set of processes. As readers develop and master

    phonemic awareness and individual letter sounds, they move to decoding through

    larger letter patterns and analogy.

    Decoding fluency

    Decoding fluency, defined here as the accuracy and rate of recoding letter sounds

    into words, plays a critical role in reading rate and accuracy. It is often considered

    an indicator of automaticity in the application of the alphabetic principle and a

    bridge to real word reading (Berninger et al., 2006). In addition, it is used by older

    and more accomplished readers as a strategy to compensate for lapses in

    automaticity in lower-level processes (Walczyk et al., 2007). In order to help

    compensate for failure in various automatic processes, decoding itself needs to be

    efficient, quick, and use few resources. Because of its role as a means to (a) decipher

    previously unseen words (Share & Stanovich, 1995), (b) learn new words (Share,

    1995; 1999), or (c) compensate for inefficiencies in other processes in reading

    (Walczyk et al.), decoding efficiency is an important area worthy of additional

    investigation. Though more frequent an occurrence for beginning readers than

    established ones, all readers encounter words that they have not seen before in print

    and need to decode. Readers do this by (a) blending together known phoneme-

    grapheme correspondences (Adams, 1990), (b) analogizing to other known words

    using rimes (Brown & Deavers,1999; Goswami,1988; McKay & Thompson,2009;

    Treiman et al.,1990), or (c) blending together amalgamated chunks (Ehri,2002). To

    measure this, however, requires something other than real words because

    researchers want to ensure the word is truly unknown and is read using letters

    and sounds, not accessed from memory.

    486 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    5/25

    Single-word reading fluency

    The degree of automaticity with which readers can identify the words in the passage

    has a large role to play in how fluently they read. The size of a readers sight word

    vocabulary, or the proportion of words in any given passage that can be recognizedby sight, plays a pivotal role in how quick and accurate a reader is (Adams, 1990;

    Compton, Appleton, & Hosp, 2004; Torgesen et al., 2001). For students who are

    below average in reading rate, this relationship is particularly important. As Ehri

    (1998) explains, sight of the written word activates its spelling, pronunciation, and

    meaning immediately in memory (p. 8). Sight word fluency is a core component of

    reading fluency and important for predicting reading comprehension (Gough,1996;

    Perfetti & Hogaboam, 1975). If a student is asked to read a passage in which a

    relatively high proportion of the words must be decoded analytically or identified by

    contextual inference, this will have an adverse effect on reading fluency, and thus,on comprehension.

    Reading comprehension and vocabulary

    There is considerable evidence to suggest that the relationship between text reading

    fluency and comprehension is reciprocal. Reading rate and accuracy has been

    identified as an important facilitator of reading comprehension (Adams,1990; Fuchs

    et al.,2001) in average and disabled readers (Breznitz, 1987,1991; Chard, Vaughn,

    & Tyler, 2002; Dowhower, 1987). More specifically, individual differences inreading rate and accuracy in third grade were found in one large study to be the

    single most important factor in accounting for differences in performance on a

    measure of comprehension of complex text (Schatschneider et al., 2004). On the

    other hand, it also appears that comprehension facilitates quick and accurate reading

    of text. For example, words in context are read faster than the same words out of

    context (e.g., Biemiller, 19771978). Jenkins, Fuchs, Van den Broek, Espin, and

    Deno (2003) found support for the view that the relationship between reading rate

    and comprehension is reciprocal. In examining fourth graders, they found that

    reading words in context explained more variance in reading comprehension than

    did reading the same words in a list (70% vs. 9%). They also found that the students

    reading comprehension score explained more variance in oral reading rate and

    accuracy in connected text than did reading the same words in a list (70% vs. 54%).

    It seems likely that the speed with which word meanings are identified would

    also affect the rate at which a passage is read. Because Perfetti (1985) suggests that

    both lexical access (word name) and semantic encoding (contextual word meaning)

    processes must be efficient, it is reasonable to think that reading fluency would be

    limited if semantic activation is not automatic (Perfetti & Hogaboam, 1975). In

    addition to finding that good comprehenders read low-frequency and nonsense

    words more quickly than poor comprehenders, Perfetti and Hogaboam also found

    that whether a participant knew the meaning of the word significantly affected the

    poor readers but not the good ones. When reading words they did not know, poor

    comprehenders were both slower and less accurate than when reading words they

    knew the meaning of while good readers were equally fast and accurate with both

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 487

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    6/25

    types of words. As long as readers are under obligation to be actively thinking about

    the meaning of what they are reading, speed of identification of word meanings may

    play a role in limiting text reading rate and accuracy.

    Rapid automatized naming

    How quickly one can access names of familiar stimuli has proven to be an important

    predictor of reading and decoding achievement. The relation between Rapid

    Automatized Naming (RAN) and reading achievement has repeatedly been

    demonstrated across various samples of typical and atypical readers, even after

    IQ, processing speed, and phonological skill have been partialed out (Denckla &

    Rudel, 1976; Kail & Hall, 1994; Manis, Doi, & Bhadha, 2000; Schatschneider,

    Fletcher, Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004; Wolf, 1997; Wolf & Bowers,1999;Wolf, Bowers, & Biddle, 2000). This relationship varies according to the stimuli

    used. Naming letters or digits is more related to reading achievement than naming of

    pictures or colors (Schatschneider et al., 2004) and RAN-digits is more related to

    reading speed than reading accuracy (Savage & Frederickson, 2005). Whether it is

    thought of as a measure of lexical access (Wagner, Torgesen, Laughon, Simmons, &

    Rashotte, 1993), a marker of orthographic processing (Manis et al., 1999; Wolf

    et al.,2000), or the speed of processing information (Catts, Gillispie, Leonard, Kail,

    & Miller,2002; Kail & Hall, 1994), RAN is more than simply naming stimuli and

    needs to be included in any model of reading fluency.

    Purpose of the study

    Despite the recent attention to text reading fluency (e.g., Fuchs et al., 2001; Hudson

    et al.,2009; Kameenui & Simmons,2001; Samuels & Farstrup,2006), few studies

    have examined the construct of oral reading rate and accuracy in connected text in a

    model that simultaneously examines many of the important variables in a multi-

    leveled fashion. Some researchers have looked at the relation between rate,

    accuracy, prosody, and reading comprehension (Daane et al.,2005; Schwanenflugel,

    Hamilton, Kuhn, Wisenbaker, & Stahl, 2004) while others have looked at the

    relation between text reading fluency and reading comprehension (Berninger et al.,

    2006; Jenkins et al., 2003; Schwanenflugel, Meisinger, Wisenbaker, Kuhn, Strauss,

    & Morris,2006). Some have looked at the predictive validity of decoding fluency to

    text reading fluency in young children (Good, Simmons, & Kameenui, 2001;

    Speece et al., 2003; Speece & Ritchey, 2005), while another examined the

    development of lexical and sub-lexical reading skills and their contributions to text

    reading fluency in beginning readers (Burke, Crowder, Hagan-Burke, & Zou,2009),

    however, to our knowledge, no one has examined the components of text reading

    fluency in children who are neither established readers nor beginners. Also, among

    the previously-cited research, Berninger et al., Jenkins et al., and Speece and

    colleagues have all focused on poor readers while the current study has a wide range

    of excellent to poor readers.

    488 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    7/25

    We had three goals in this study: (a) identify salient variables that explain

    individual differences in the text reading and decoding fluency of a sample of young

    readers, (b) create a model of the structural relations among the variables, and (c)

    empirically test the model presented in Fig. 1. We planned to determine which

    component lexical and sub-lexical skills best explain the processes underlying textreading fluency in second graders.

    First, we sought to determine the most reasonable measurement model. In

    particular, we asked whether the eight constructs of interest in this study constitute

    different factors or whether a more parsimonious model is more appropriate. In

    particular, we asked whether phonogram fluency (PGF) and decoding are two

    different factors or measures of the same construct. We hypothesized the presence

    of two separate factors because of the increasing consolidation of letter patterns as

    readers develop. Ehri (1992) suggests that first young children learn the associations

    between individual letters and sounds, and then over time, consolidate theseassociations into larger letter patterns. We see this interim step between individual

    letters and entire words as an important variable to examine. When decoding,

    children could use either single letter sounds or larger letter patterns and it is

    possible that phonogram fluency is a way to capture this transition.

    Second, we investigated the structural relations among the variables predicting

    decoding fluency. These relations are represented in Fig. 2. We predicted mediated

    effects from phonemic blending fluency (PBF) to letter sound fluency (LSF) to

    phonogram fluency (PGF) to decoding because of our proposed pattern of reading

    development. Third, we examined the structural relations among the variablespredicting text reading fluency represented in Fig.2. Based on our theoretical

    framework, we expected direct effects from single-word reading fluency (SWF) and

    reading comprehension to text reading fluency (TRF), decoding to TRF, and

    decoding to SWF.

    Fig. 2 Hypothesized structural

    model of decoding and oral

    reading rate and accuracy in

    second grade readers. Paths with

    adashed arroware hypothesizedto be close to zero. RANRapid

    automatized naming

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 489

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    8/25

    Methods

    Participants

    All second grade students in five schools in a north Florida school district wereinvited to participate in this study. The demographic characteristics of each of the

    schools are reported in Table1.

    The parents of 214 children gave permission for their children to participate; due

    to mobility, 198 students completed all of the assessments. The sample of 97 boys

    and 101 girls had a mean chronological age of 8 years 5 months. Data from a

    parental questionnaire identified 47% of the children as Caucasian, 38% as African

    American, 4% as Asian/Pacific Islanders, 3.5% as Hispanic, 4.5% as Multiracial,

    .5% as American Indian, and 2.5% did not report. About 12% of the participants

    primary caregivers (mother or grandmother) indicated that they had less than a highschool education, 44% graduated from high school or attended additional training

    beyond high school, 22% graduated from college or university, and 19% had a

    graduate education. According to school records, 75% (150) of the children had no

    identified disability label, .5% had a primary disability identification of mild mental

    retardation, 14% of speech impairment, 7% of language impairment, and 2.5% with

    a specific learning disability. The vast majority (98%) were competent speakers of

    English.

    Measures

    Measures were selected to provide assessments of each variable hypothesized to be

    important to decoding and reading fluency. Attention was paid to the reliability of

    the scores from each measure and validity for the intended purpose and sample

    (Thompson & Vacha-Haase, 2000). All measures were given at the end of second

    grade within a 3-week period.

    Table 1 School demographics in percentages

    Model School A School B School C School D School E

    Race/ethnicity

    White 76.7 17.1 61.5 4.6 58.5

    African-American 13.0 74.7 33.7 83.7 24.8

    Hispanic 2.2 3.5 1.3 7.1 9.7

    Asian 5.8 .3 .2 2.8 2.5

    Native American .2 .3 1.3 .2 .8

    Multiracial 2.0 4.1 3.1 1.6 3.7Female 47.8 47.7 48.3 48.6 50.2

    Special education 19.7 25.1 29.9 20.0 8.5

    English language learners 1.2 3.0 .6 7.8 2.4

    Free or reduced-price lunch 8.7 87.3 62.1 82.1 23.2

    490 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    9/25

    Reading comprehension

    Two measures were used to form the reading comprehension latent variable. First,

    the Gray Oral Reading Test, 4th edition (Weiderholt & Bryant, 2001) uses 14

    developmentally sequenced reading passages with five comprehension questionsfollowing each passage. Participants were asked to read progressively more difficult

    passages and answer questions about each one until the ceiling was reached,

    yielding a reading comprehension score. Second, the picture vocabulary subtest of

    the Woodcock-Johnson Test of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd edition was used. This

    measures expressive vocabulary by asking for a label for a series of pictures.

    Text reading fluency (rate and accuracy)

    Two assessments were used to measure oral reading rate and accuracy. Studentswere asked to read 3 s-grade passages from the Oral Reading Fluency subtest of the

    Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, 6th edition (DIBELS; Good &

    Kaminski, 2002a) and the number of correct words read in 1 min was used. The

    three passages were selected to have the same reading difficulty level as reported by

    the developers (Good & Kaminski, 2002b) and contain topics that second graders

    in Florida would likely be familiar with regardless of their socio-economic level.

    The Gray Oral Reading Test, 4th edition (Weiderholt & Bryant, 2001) uses 14

    developmentally sequenced reading passages with five comprehension questions

    following each passage. Participants were asked to read progressively more difficultpassages until the ceiling was reached, yielding a fluency score based on time and

    accuracy.

    Single-word reading fluency

    Two forms of the Sight Word Efficiency (SWE) subtest of theTest of Word Reading

    Efficiency(Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte,1999) were used to measure single-word

    reading fluency. Participants were asked to read as many real words as possible from

    a word list that increased in difficulty in 45 s. The alternate form reliability obtained

    in this study was .95.

    Decoding fluency (rate and accuracy)

    Two assessments were used to measure decoding fluency. In the Phonemic

    Decoding Efficiency (PDE) subtest of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency

    (Torgesen et al., 1999), participants were asked to read as many nonsense words

    presented in list format that increased in difficulty as possible in 45 s. Only fully

    blended responses were considered correct. Both form A and form B were given;

    alternate form reliability obtained in this study was .94. In the Nonsense Word

    Fluency (NWF) test of the DIBELS (Good & Kaminski, 2002a), students were

    asked to read randomly ordered VC and CVC nonsense words presented in rows.

    The difficulty of the items stayed constant. The score is the number of correct

    sounds read per minute, with a correct response consisting either of sounds read

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 491

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    10/25

    individually, partially blended, or fully blended. The median 1-month alternate form

    reliability reported by the developers was .83 (Dynamic Measurement Group,

    2008).

    Phonemic blending fluency (PBF)

    A measure consisting of three- and four-phoneme words was constructed for this

    study to measure phonemic blending rate and accuracy. Examiners orally presented

    each item sound by sound with a short pause between sounds (e.g., f-a-t) and the

    participants then blended the sounds into a whole word. Examiners timed the

    latency of response for each item using a stop watch until a minute was reached,

    yielding a score of the number of correctly blended words per minute. The obtained

    corrected Spearman-Brown split half coefficient was .78.

    Letter sound fluency (LSF)

    In order to measure rate and accuracy in graphemephoneme connections,

    researchers constructed two forms of randomly ordered lowercase single letters,

    digraphs, and r-controlled vowels (e.g.,a,f,ch,ee,ar) presented in rows. All single

    letters and digraphs in random order were represented on both forms for a total of

    48 items per form. Five practice items, including a long vowel digraph and a single

    short vowel were administered before the test and feedback given. Students were

    given 1 min to say the sound represented by each letter or digraph, yielding a scoreof correct letter sounds per minute. For single vowels, short sounds were counted as

    correct. Obtained alternate form reliability was .73.

    Phonogram fluency (PGF)

    In order to measure childrens fluency with common larger within-word letter

    patterns, researchers constructed two forms consisting of randomly ordered common

    rimes (e.g.,eed,op,um,at,unch,arp) presented in rows. To ensure that participants

    understood the task, three practice items with feedback were given using these

    directions, I am going to ask you to read as many of these phonograms as you can. A

    phonogram is a set of letters we see a lot in words, so you may have seen them before

    as a part of words youve read. Try to read each one like you would in a word, but

    dont make it into a real word if it isnt one. Ill show you how with the first one.

    Students were given 1 min to read as many phonograms as possible. Only fully-

    blended responses were coded as correct, yielding a score of correct phonograms per

    minute. The alternate form reliability obtained in this study was .89.

    Rapid automatized naming

    The Rapid Letter Naming subtest of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological

    Processing (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999) was used to measure RAN.

    Several rows of six lower case letters that repeat across the page are presented

    and participants are asked to name them as quickly as possible. The raw score is the

    492 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    11/25

    total number of seconds needed to name the letters. The alternate form reliability

    obtained in this study was .83.

    Administration

    All measures were administered in the spring of second grade and given individually

    in a quiet place in several sessions to minimize participant distraction and fatigue. In

    addition to the first author, assessors were graduate students with training in school

    psychology or education. In order to maintain assessment fidelity, assessors were (a)

    given 8 h of training, (b) required to demonstrate correct assessment procedures

    before they tested participants, (c) assessed with the first or last author before

    working independently, (d) attended weekly meetings with follow-up training, and

    (e) were observed at least twice while they assessed. Interrater reliability was

    calculated on 10% of the participants, yielding coefficients that ranged from .90 to1.0 agreement. All raw data protocols were scored a second time by the first or last

    author to ensure correct scoring. All of the data were entered twice by different

    research assistants and discrepancies corrected on a case by case basis.

    Results

    Data analysis

    Structural Equation Modeling (SEM; Kline, 2005) using AMOS 7.0 and maximum

    likelihood estimation was used to analyze the data. This occurred in two phases; first

    a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to determine the measurement model

    and second, this validated measurement model was used to test structural

    hypotheses in relation to the directional relations between phonemic blending

    fluency, letter sound fluency, RAN, phonogram fluency, single-word reading

    fluency, text reading fluency, and reading comprehension. It is important to test the

    measurement model before the structural model, since the structural relations make

    no sense if the constructs are not reasonable (Thompson, 2000). Raw scores

    uncorrected for age were used in all SEM analyses.

    Descriptive statistics and correlations

    Descriptive statistics and Pearson product-moment correlations among the indicator

    measures are presented in Table2. These statistics support the psychometric

    adequacy of the tasks for the children in our study. Correlations were in expected

    directions, with magnitudes in line with those reported in other studies (e.g.,

    Compton, 2000; Speece et al., 2003; Wagner et al., 1994).

    Measurement model

    We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis to test the adequacy of our

    measurement model. Results of the model fitting process are summarized in

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 493

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    12/25

    Table2

    Correlationsandrawscoredescriptivestatisticsforallobservedindicators

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    8

    9

    10

    11

    12

    13

    14

    15

    16

    17

    18

    1.GORT

    comprehensionSS

    2.Picturevocabulary

    .242

    3.DORFpassage

    1

    .372

    .292

    4.DORFpassage

    2

    .364

    .272

    .935

    5.DORFpassage

    3

    .357

    .304

    .950

    .9

    44

    6.GORTfluencySS

    .397

    .273

    .876

    .8

    66

    .862

    7.SWEformA

    .322

    .239

    .881

    .8

    62

    .868

    .842

    8.SWEformB

    .288

    .247

    .874

    .8

    62

    .876

    .832

    .948

    9.PDEformA

    .326

    .247

    .818

    .8

    19

    .802

    .837

    .849

    .841

    10.

    PDEformB

    .336

    .249

    .798

    .8

    09

    .789

    .820

    .831

    .822

    .940

    11.

    NWF

    .246

    .256

    .702

    .7

    01

    .690

    .702

    .709

    .696

    .793

    .791

    12.

    Phonogram

    fluencyA

    .290

    .170

    .804

    .8

    00

    .781

    .797

    .808

    .802

    .876

    .852

    .754

    13.

    Phonogram

    fluencyB

    .269

    .212

    .790

    .7

    94

    .788

    .793

    .820

    .830

    .869

    .864

    .763

    .88

    9

    14.

    Lettersound

    fluencyA

    .049

    .098

    .183

    .2

    32

    .188

    .171

    .278

    .266

    .322

    .300

    .393

    .37

    2

    .335

    15.

    Lettersound

    fluencyB

    .056

    .042

    .226

    .2

    81

    .240

    .211

    .335

    .334

    .358

    .366

    .429

    .32

    4

    .431

    .728

    16.

    Phonemic

    blendingfluency

    .171

    .303

    .145

    .1

    27

    .148

    .153

    .131

    .107

    .162

    .162

    .175

    .14

    5

    .156

    .292

    .320

    17.

    RANlettersA

    -.1

    63

    -.0

    36

    -.5

    65

    -.5

    91

    -.5

    82-.5

    12

    -.5

    96

    -.6

    10

    -.5

    26

    -.5

    17

    -.4

    88

    -.55

    9

    -.5

    76

    -.3

    21

    -.4

    05

    -.232

    18.

    RANlettersB

    -.1

    23

    -.0

    02

    -.5

    36

    -.5

    68

    -.5

    67-.4

    87

    -.5

    56

    -.6

    05

    -.4

    95

    -.5

    05

    -.4

    83

    -.55

    3

    -.5

    37

    -.3

    14

    -.3

    78

    -.212

    .817

    494 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    13/25

    Table2

    continue

    d

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6

    7

    8

    9

    10

    11

    12

    13

    14

    15

    16

    17

    18

    M

    8.60

    14.0

    6

    94.6

    102.8

    103.4

    9.6

    5

    56.0

    55.6

    26.5

    25.5

    103.5

    39.2

    39.6

    38.1

    38.6

    28.7

    22.6

    22.9

    SD

    3.97

    1.73

    37.6

    0

    40.8

    9

    38.71

    3.6

    7

    13.10

    13.8

    3

    11.5

    8

    12.3

    6

    48.3

    6

    18.62

    18.2

    4

    12.3

    4

    13.2

    0

    12.08

    6.6

    7

    7.16

    Allcorrelationsab

    ove.1

    4aresignificantatp\

    .05(2

    -tailed)

    GORT=

    GrayOralReadingTest,4thed.,

    Picturevocabulary=

    picturevocabularysubtestoftheWoodcock-JohnsonTestof

    CognitiveAbilities,

    3rded.,

    DORF

    =

    Oralreading

    fluencyfromtheD

    ynamicIndicatorsofBasicEarlyL

    iteracySkills,

    SWE=

    Sightword

    efficiencysubtestoftheTestofWo

    rdReadingEfficiency,

    PDE=

    Pho

    nemicdecoding

    efficiencysubtestfromtheTestofWordReadingEffic

    iency,NWF=

    NonsensewordfluencyfromtheDynamicIndicatorsof

    BasicEarlyLiteracySkills,

    RAN

    letters=

    Rapid

    letternamingsubtestoftheComprehensiveTestofPh

    onologicalProcessing

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 495

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    14/25

    Table3. To assess whether a newly specified model shows an improvement in fit

    over its predecessor, we examined the difference in v2 (Dv2) between the two nested

    models. A significant reduction in Dv2 (p\ .05) indicates a substantial improve-

    ment in model fit while a significant increase in Dv2 (p\ .05) indicates a substantial

    decrement in model fit. We did not use an exploratory factor analysis because we

    used theory to determine our constructs and their indicators.

    In order to evaluate the model predicted by our theory, Model 1 was fitted. The

    factor loadings were strong, ranging from .81 to .98 except those for readingcomprehension (.45 for reading comprehension, .54 for vocabulary; all regression

    estimates presented are standardized). The R2 values for the indicators were also

    large, ranging from .67 (DIBELS NWF) to .98 (DIBELS ORF 1 & 3, PDE, A),

    except again for the reading comprehension measures (.20 for GORT and .29 for

    vocabulary). The fit of this model was good, with a v932 of 148.25, a comparative

    fit index (CFI) of .986, and a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)

    of .055.

    Information from the modification indices showed that adding a path that allowed

    DIBELS NWF to load on both the Decoding and the Letter-Sound Factors was

    worth considering. Because children can earn a correct item for providing either a

    single letter sound or a blended response, this assessment does appear to measure

    both individual letter sound knowledge and decoding of nonsense words. Thus we

    added the cross-loading and Model 2 was estimated. The v2 was significantly lower,

    Dv2=11.72, Ddf = 1,p = .0006 and the model fit was good, with v92

    2 of 136.53, a

    CFI of .989, and a RMSEA of .050. The R2 for DIBELS NWF increased to .70,

    indicating that 3% additional variance was explained by the added path. The first

    order correlations between the factors are presented in Table 4.

    To test whether phonogram fluency (PGF) and decoding fluency are measures of

    two distinct factors or are the same one, a model with seven factors was fit with

    decoding predicting the indicators of phonogram fluency as well as its own

    indicators (Byrne, 2000; Kline, 2005). The resulting model (Model 3) had a

    significantly worse fit (Dv2 =52.11, Ddf =8, p\ .0001, CFI = .977,

    RMSEA = .068), indicating that they are two separate, but highly related

    Table 3 Test statistics for measurement models

    Model v2 df CFIa RMSEAb AIC Dv2c Ddfd Dp

    1. 8 Factors 148.25 93 .986 .055 [.038.071]e 268.25

    2. 8 Factors with NWFcross-loaded

    136.53 92 .989 .050 [.031.067]e

    258.53 11.72 1 =.0006

    3. 7 Factors with phonogram

    fluency, NWF, and PDE

    predicted by decoding fluency

    188.64 99 .977 .068 [.053.083]e 296.64 52.11 8 \.0001

    a Comparative fit indexb Root mean square error of approximationc Difference in v2

    d Difference in degrees of freedome

    90% confidence interval for RMSEA

    496 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    15/25

    (r = .95), factors. Thus we conclude that the eight-factor measurement model has

    the most support; it will be used in all future analyses.

    Tests of direct and mediated effects on decoding and text reading fluency

    We then addressed the next set of research questions. Results of the model

    comparisons are summarized in Table5 and the initial model is represented in

    Fig.2. The fit of Model 1, with both direct and mediated paths from all variables,

    was adequate (v1192 = 258.46, CFI = .967, RMSEA = .077). The R2 for decoding

    fluency was .91 and for ORF, .89. As predicted, the direct paths phonemic blending

    fluency (PBF) ? decoding fluency (.03), letter sound fluency (LSF) ? decoding

    Table 4 Maximum likelihood correlations among all latent variables, whole sample

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7

    1. Reading comprehension

    2. Text reading fluencya

    .666

    3. Decoding fluencya .587 .856

    4. Phonogram fluencya .486 .864 .949

    5. Letter sound fluencya .102 .273 .410 .455

    6. Single word reading fluency .553 .921 .886 .888 .370

    7. RAN -.165 -.645 -.584 -.653 -.469 -.673

    8. Phonemic blending fluency .544 .156 .180 .167 .333 .129 -.217

    All correlations above .13 are significant at p\ .05 (two tailed)a Defined as rate and accuracy

    Table 5 Test statistics for structural models

    Model v2 df CFIa RMSEAb Dv2c Ddfd Dp

    1. Initial 258.46 119 .967 .077 [.064.090]e

    2. Fixed PBF?

    PGF path to 0 258.56 120 .967 .077 [.064.090]e

    .10 1 .7523. Fixed LSF ? decoding,

    PBF ? decoding paths to 0

    259.83 122 .968 .076 [.063.089]e 1.27 2 .530

    4. Deleted all non-significant paths

    and those set to 0

    264.50 124 .967 .076 [.063.089]e 4.67 2 .100

    5. Added path from PGF ? TRF 260.68 123 .968 .076 [.063.089]e 3.82 2 .148

    6. Fixed PGF ? decoding,

    decoding ? TRF paths to 0

    603.86 124 .887 .141 [.129.152]e 343.18 1 \.00001

    PBF Phonemic blending fluency, PGF phonogram fluency, LSFletter sound fluency, RANrapid auto-

    matized naming, TRFtext reading fluency

    a Comparative fit indexb Root mean square error of approximationc Difference in v2

    d Difference in degrees of freedome 90% confidence interval for RMSEA

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 497

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    16/25

    fluency (-.04), and PBF ? PGF (-.02) were not significant (all regression

    estimates presented are standardized). The paths from RAN ? decoding fluency

    (.07) and text reading fluency (-.04) were also not significant.

    In order to test our hypothesis that there would be no unique, direct effects from

    phonemic blending (PBF) and letter sounds (LSF) to decoding fluency, the path

    PBF ? PGF was set to 0 and the model estimated (Model 2 in Table 5). The paths

    PBF ? decoding and LSF? decoding were then set to 0 and the model was

    Decoding

    Fluency

    nwf e5

    pdea e6

    pdeb e7

    Letter Sound

    Fluency

    lsfa e1

    lsfb e2

    .81

    .90

    phonfa e3

    phonfb e4

    Phonemic Blending

    Fluency

    res2

    res1

    Text Reading

    Fluency

    orf1 e8.97

    orf2 e9.96

    orf3 e10.97

    res3 GORTFluency

    e11.89

    PGF

    .94

    .95

    res4

    .75

    .97

    .96

    .95

    .25

    .18

    RAN

    rlna

    e12

    rlnb

    e13

    .92.89

    -.56

    -.41

    .20Single Word

    Reading Fluency

    sweb

    e15

    swea

    e14

    .97.97

    -.23

    .75

    .74

    res5

    -.21

    res6

    .18

    Reading

    Comprehension

    GORT

    RC e22

    vocab e33

    .55

    .44

    .18

    Fig. 3 Final structural model of decoding and text reading rate and accuracy in second grade readers.All regression weights are significant (p\ .05). lsf =Letter sound fluency; rln =Rapid letter naming

    subtest of the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing; nwf =Nonsense word fluency from the

    Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills; pde =Phonemic decoding efficiency subtest from

    the Test of Word Reading Efficiency, phonf=Phonogram fluency, orf=Oral reading fluency from the

    Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, GORT =Gray Oral Reading Test, 4th ed.,

    swe =Sight word reading subtest of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency, vocab =Picture vocabulary

    subtest from the Woodcock-Johnson Test of Cognitive Abilities, 3rd ed. RANRapid automatized naming

    498 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    17/25

    re-estimated (Model 3 in Table 5). The lack of change in fit from these two models

    demonstrated that there are indirect effects from PBF and LSF to decoding fluency,

    but no unique direct contribution once the variance due to the other variables is

    partialed out.

    Next we examined the structural relations among the variables predicting textreading fluency. In order to do this with the most parsimonious model, we removed

    all non-significant paths, including those from RAN to decoding and text reading

    fluency and those set to 0 and estimated the model (Model 4 in Table 5). The fit was

    not significantly different from the previous model (v1242

    =264.50, CFI = .967,

    RMSEA = .076). The R2 for decoding fluency was .90 and for ORF, .89. As

    predicted, the path from single-word reading fluency (SWF) ? text reading fluency

    (TRF) showed a strong association (.74). The path decoding fluency ? SWF also

    showed an equally strong association (.75) while that from decoding fluency ? text

    reading fluency was smaller (.21) as was the path from reading comprehension (RC)(.18). RAN predicted all the lower-level processes and SWR, but did not show a

    direct path to decoding fluency or text reading fluency (Fig. 3).

    In order to further examine the role phonogram fluency plays in the prediction of

    decoding fluency, we estimated a model with an additional path from phonogram

    fluency (PGF) directly to text reading fluency in addition to the path mediated by

    decoding. There was no significant change in model fit (Model 5 in Table5). We then

    set the paths from PGF ? decoding and from decoding ? text reading fluency to 0 to

    determine if PGF would account for the same variance as decoding. That is, we asked

    the question whether both variables uniquely related to text reading fluency (Model 6in Table5). This resulted in a model with significantly worse fit (Dv2 = 343.18,

    Ddf = 1,p \ .0001, CFI =.887, RMSEA = .146), indicating that both phonogram

    fluency and decoding play a unique role in explaining text reading fluency.

    Discussion

    This study examined a multi-level model of text reading fluency, which we define as

    reading rate and accuracy in connected text. In the current study, we addressed three

    goals: (a) identify salient variables that explain individual differences in the text

    reading and decoding fluency of a sample of young readers, (b) create a model of the

    structural relations among the variables, and (c) test specific theoretical hypotheses.

    Limitations

    As with any research study, the findings of this project are limited in several ways.

    One of the largest is that these findings are based on a single sample of 198 s graders

    in 5 schools. As can be seen in the descriptive statistics found in Table2, they fall in

    the average range with a large amount of variability and represent a wide range of

    readers, however another group of second graders may well produce a different set

    of findings. Second, except for reading comprehension and vocabulary, we used

    only speeded measures. It is likely that some of the predictions we saw are due to

    the fact the measures were all based on rate. Including accuracy measures would

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 499

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    18/25

    help to better sort out the relative contributions of the variables. In addition, none of

    the variables tap into orthographic processes important to reading, leaving questions

    about what other explanatory variables are not included. Our use of only rimes in

    the phonogram fluency measure added unintended complications in the interpre-

    tations of our results. Including affixes in addition to common rimes may help settlethe issue of the role larger-letter patterns plays in decoding. In addition, there were

    not multiple indicators of all variables, and the indicators had variable levels of

    reliability, which could account for some of the findings. We are also limited in our

    findings by the single age of our participants. We do not have a cross-sectional

    sample, or better yet, a longitudinal sample to study the development of these

    processes. This limits our understanding to a small snapshot in the reading of young

    children. Finally, there are other alternate models that could explain the data equally

    well that are not examined here. Despite all of these limitations, the findings of our

    study provide important insights into the nature and measurement of decodingfluency as well as variables important for text reading fluency.

    Measurement of text reading fluency and decoding fluency

    First we examined the appropriate measurement of the constructs of interest using a

    confirmatory factor analysis. In keeping with Ehris (1992) phases of development

    and other evidence that proposed an interim step between single letter sounds and

    decoding of whole words (i.e., within-word letter patterns), we hypothesized that

    there were eight separate factors. We fit the hypothesized model, which fitsignificantly well, had strong loadings in most cases, and explained a great deal of

    the variance in the various indicators. We then determined the most appropriate

    measurement model included a cross-loading of both Letter Sound Fluency and

    Decoding Fluency on the DIBELS Nonsense Word Fluency (NWF) measure. This is

    interesting because when using NWF, the first author has noticed that it does appear

    to be measuring both letter sound knowledge and blending of sounds into words.

    This measurement model confirms this observation.

    A measurement concern about the nature of decoding fluency was addressed next.

    Using our model and the hypothesized pattern of development from single letter

    sounds to within-word patterns, to reading whole words as units (Ehri, 1992), we

    thought that the automaticity of reading rimes (e.g., eet, igh, eem, otch) would be

    separate from, but highly related to, reading of nonsense words (e.g., dat, mis, stree,

    vog, tel, zul). We see the first task as measuring familiar letter units that could be read

    quickly by sight while the other two tasks are designed to facilitate recoding, or

    sounding out the nonsense words. To test this, we fit a model with the indicators of

    decoding fluency and PGF loaded onto the same factor per Byrne ( 2000) and Kline

    (2005). This model was significantly worse fitting than the full model, indicating that

    while phonogram and decoding fluency were highly related (r = .95), they are not

    the same factor. This was confirmed during the structural model analysis when

    setting the paths from decoding to text reading fluency resulted in a much worse

    fitting model. Setting the paths to zero had the effect of bypassing decoding, with a

    single route from phonogram fluency (PGF) to text reading fluency. Both appear to

    explain unique variance in the model.

    500 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    19/25

    It is possible that the high correlation between the two factors is due to an overlap

    of items between the three measures (DIBELS Nonsense Word Fluency (NWF),

    TOWRE Phonemic Decoding Efficiency (PDE), and Phonogram Fluency). We

    examined this and found little overlap between the Phonogram Fluency and

    DIBELS NWF measures (two rimes occurred twice in NWF and four occurredonce). However, there was some overlap between the TOWRE PDE and phonogram

    fluency. There were 48 occurrences of rimes in the phonogram fluency measure in

    the two forms of the TOWRE PDE, with the three rimes -at, -ip, and -inaccounting

    for 11 of them. Clearly there is some overlap, both in items and in the method of

    measurement (speeded, use of a timer, same assessors), and it is possible that the

    phonogram fluency is nothing more than a measure of non-words, however, there is

    also still unshared variance that we believe is due to the underlying differences in

    the constructs. To further examine these two factors, we conducted a CFA with just

    the two factors. We estimated a model and found that it explained the data very well(v4

    2=6.28, p = .179, CFI = .998, RMSEA = .054) with high loadings onto each

    factor. We then fixed the two factors to be equal to test whether a one-factor solution

    is truly better. This model had a significantly worse fit (Dv2 =33.75, Ddf = 1,

    p\ .0001, CFI = .972, RMSEA = .189).

    Students can approach unknown words, represented in these measures as

    nonsense words, in several ways: pronounce individual letter sounds, pronounce

    individual sounds and then blend them, blend them without the initial attempt, or

    pronounce them through an analogy. DIBELS NWF gives full credit for the first,

    second, and third options though students pay a rate penalty for the second, theTOWRE PDE gives full credit for the second through fourth options, again with a

    penalty for the second, and the Phonogram Fluency encourages the use of the fourth

    option, analogy. Perhaps it is that use of analogy that is at the heart of the unique

    variance that we are detecting in these constructs, and perhaps it is the shared nature

    of the nonsense word reading strategies that children may apply that leads to the

    high correlation.

    Mediated and direct predictors of decoding fluency

    Using two different models, we established that the lower-level skills of phonemic

    blending and letter-sound correspondences are not uniquely related to decoding

    fluency when phonogram fluency is accounted for; they are mediated by fluency in

    reading these larger letter patterns. If a reader can read using larger letter patterns,

    then it makes sense that isolated letter sounds and phonemic blending would no

    longer have a direct effect on decoding; the variation would appear in the higher

    level skill. We found evidence that phonemic blending (PBF) predicts letter sound

    fluency (LSF), which predicts phonogram fluency (PGF), which predicts decoding

    fluency. This sequence follows the expected direction of development among young

    readers (Ehri, 1992).

    Instructionally, this would suggest that teachers need to ensure their young

    students become automatic in oral blending of sounds, individual letter sounds, and

    larger letter patterns in order to be successful decoders. Instruction that provides

    enough practice to move beyond accuracy to automaticity is needed. Given the role

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 501

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    20/25

    phonogram fluency played in predicting text reading fluency, instruction in

    recognizing words with shared phonograms accurately and quickly would be likely

    to promote the development of text reading fluency. Too often, teachers focus on

    decoding accuracy at the letter level, with little or no attention devoted to the

    development of automaticity in decoding skills. Judging from the methods includedin most basal series, little instructional time is spent developing familiarity with

    phonograms. By including instruction in these component skills, teachers can play a

    more active role in their students reading fluency development.

    Mediated and direct predictors of text reading fluency

    Looking at the portion of the model directly predicting text reading fluency, it

    appears that both single-word reading fluency and decoding fluency are strong

    predictors of text reading fluency and that reading comprehension also plays animportant role. Single-word reading had a unique strong relation to text reading

    fluency, which is consistent with the findings of Compton et al. (2004) and Torgesen

    et al. (2001), who found that the percentage of sight words in a passage predicts the

    text reading fluency of elementary readers.

    Not unexpectedly, decoding fluency also had a direct relation to text reading

    fluency and single-word reading fluency, a finding consistent with Gough and Walsh

    (1991). As Ehri (2002) explained, words become sight words when they have been

    practiced sufficiently to be fully amalgamated in memory. It stands to reason, then,

    that automaticity in decoding allows for more efficient practice of words whichwould, in turn, lead to greater single-word reading fluency. In addition, these young

    readers are likely to still encounter unknown words that need to be decoded, so

    decoding continues to play a strong role in text reading fluency in addition to that

    played by single-word reading fluency.

    The findings in the current study are somewhat inconsistent with those of Burke

    et al. (2009). Like this study, Burke et al. found that decoding fluency in first grade

    predicted single-word reading fluency (SWF) in that same grade and SWF in first

    grade had a strong effect on text reading fluency in second grade. Unlike this study,

    they did not find a link between decoding fluency in first grade and text reading

    fluency. This is perhaps due to the longitudinal nature of their study; perhaps if they

    had measured DIBELS Nonsense Word Fluency (NWF) and DIBELS Oral Reading

    Fluency (ORF) concurrently, our results would have been more similar. In addition,

    perhaps the relative simplicity of the items on the DIBELS NWF measure did not

    capture as much advanced decoding as the TOWRE Phonemic Decoding Efficiency

    subtest, which has considerably more difficult items. In the current study, decoding

    fluency explains 96 and 97% of the variance in the PDE, but only 75% of the

    variance in DIBELS NWF, perhaps demonstrating this difference.

    At least among our sample, it appears that the direct effects of RAN on decoding

    and reading fluency found by many researchers (Georgiou, Parrila, Kirby, &

    Stephenson,2008; Manis et al.,2000; Savage & Frederickson,2005) are not found

    when other mediator variables are present in the model. This lack of direct effect is

    interesting because we found a rather strong and similar total effect (maximum

    likelihood correlation) of RAN on text reading fluency (-.58) and on decoding

    502 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    21/25

    fluency (-.54). It would appear that the total effect was decomposed through

    phonemic blending (PBF), letter sounds (LSF), phonogram fluency (PGF), and in

    the case of text reading fluency, single-word reading fluency. RAN uniquely

    explained 6% of variance in PBF, 15% in LSF, and 26% in PGF, even after all the

    other variance was partialed out.Our finding of a significant relation from reading comprehension to text reading

    supports the contention that reading comprehension plays a role in how quickly and

    accurately one reads connected text (Jenkins et al., 2003). The relation was small

    but significant. When the direction was reversed, and text reading fluency predicted

    reading comprehension, the relation was larger, providing support for the notion

    fluency is a necessary, but not sufficient condition for deep understanding (Breznitz,

    1987,1991; Laberge & Samuels, 1974; Perfetti, 1985).

    Implications for research

    We are left with several unanswered research questions that are raised by these

    results. There was considerable variability in the accuracy of students on the fluency

    measures, leading us to wonder if the results were due to accuracy or fluency

    differences in the childrens scores. We also wonder if some of the prediction

    provided by the lower-level skills would be found if accuracy measures were

    included. We are intrigued by the question of whether fluency in these subskills

    accounts for additional variance above and beyond accuracy in these processes. We

    also wonder if the structural relationships we observed between variables are thesame in children at different levels of reading development or achievement level.

    Given that all the variables in the current study are sound-based, we wonder about

    the role orthographic knowledge plays in the decoding and text reading fluency of

    young readers given all of these other variables. This model of decoding and text

    reading fluency will need to be replicated and validated with other samples of

    readers, especially readers at other points in their development. We wonder if first

    graders or poor readers show similar indirect effects, or would they have additional

    variance that uniquely contributes to decoding fluency.

    We chose to conduct this study with second graders because children at that age

    are typically in a steep trajectory of development in reading and thus may show a

    range of mastery of the processes under examination. Most have mastered the

    beginning skills related to phonemic awareness and letter knowledge, but few have

    reached their potential in text reading fluency. It is important to understand the

    relationship among the many component skills of readers during this phase of their

    development because a deficiency in any of the component skills has the potential to

    affect the development of other skills and, ultimately, the development of the child

    as a proficient reader. As evidenced by the wide variability in competence in the

    second graders in our sample, developmental phases in reading may be more

    important than age or grade level to the understanding of how these factors are

    related. Examination of the changes that occur in these component skills as children

    move from Ehris (1992) full-alphabetic phase to her consolidated-alphabetic phase

    could yield important findings that would further our understanding of how these

    skills lead to proficient reading.

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 503

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    22/25

    Acknowledgments The work presented in this article was supported by Grant H324N040039 from the

    US Department of Education, Office of Special Education Programs. This article does not necessarily

    reflect the positions or policies of this funding agency and no official endorsement should be inferred. We

    are grateful for the generous assistance of Richard K. Wagner and Robert Abbott in this project and the

    invaluable comments of Joseph Jenkins and anonymous reviewers on an earlier draft. We also appreciate

    Laura Snyder, Jennifer Wolvin, Jennifer Tow, Anna Ylakotola, Christan Grygas, Yi Pan, PatriciaShubrick, and Brian Mincey for their work collecting the data in this study. We especially thank the

    children, parents, teachers, and principals in the Leon County School District and Florida State University

    School who made this research possible.

    References

    Adams, M. J. (1990).Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Vermeulen, K., & Fulton, C. M. (2006). Paths to reading comprehension

    in at-risk second-grade readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 334351.Biemiller, A. (19771978). Relationships between oral reading rates for letters, words, and simple text in

    the development of reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 13, 223-253.

    Breznitz, Z. (1987). Increasing first graders reading accuracy and comprehension by accelerating their

    reading rates. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(3), 236242.

    Breznitz, Z. (1991). The beneficial effect of accelerating reading rate on Dyslexic readers reading

    comprehension. In M. Snowling & M. Thomson (Eds.), Dyslexia: Integrating theory and practice

    (pp. 235243). London: Whurr Publishing Ltd.

    Breznitz, Z. (2006).Fluency in reading: Synchronization of processes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates.

    Brown, G. D. A., & Deavers, R. P. (1999). Units of analysis in nonword reading: Evidence from children

    and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 208242.

    Burke, M. D., Crowder, W., Hagan-Burke, S., & Zou, Y. (2009). A comparison of two path models for

    predicting reading fluency. Remedial and Special Education, 30, 8495.

    Byrne, B. M. (2000). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and

    programming (2nd ed.). Florence, KY: Psychology Press.

    Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of

    processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning

    Disabilities, 35, 510525.

    Chard, D. J., Vaughn, S., & Tyler, B. J. (2002). A synthesis of research on effective interventions for

    building reading fluency with elementary students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning

    Disabilities, 35, 386406.

    Compton, D. L. (2000). Modeling growth skills in first-grade children. Reading and Writing: An

    Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 239273.Compton, D. L., Appleton, A., & Hosp, M. K. (2004). Exploring the relationship between text-leveling

    systems and reading accuracy and fluency in second grade students who are average and poor

    decoders. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19, 176184.

    Daane, M. C., Campbell, J. R., Grigg, W. S., Goodman, M. J., & Oranje, A. (2005). Fourth-grade

    students reading aloud: NAEP 2002 special study of oral reading (NCES 2006-469). US

    Department of Education. Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics.

    Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.

    Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated

    from other learning disabilities. Neurosychologia, 14, 471479.

    Dowhower, S. L. (1987). Effects of repeated reading on second-grade transitional readers fluency and

    comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 22(4), 389406.

    Dynamic Measurement Group. (2008). DIBELS 6th edition technical adequacy information (Technical

    report no 6). Eugene, OR: Dynamic Measurement Group. Accessed January 15, 2009, from

    http://dibels.org/pubs.html.

    Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to

    recoding. In P. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107143).

    Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    504 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

    http://dibels.org/pubs.htmlhttp://dibels.org/pubs.html
  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    23/25

    Ehri, L. C. (1998). Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English. In

    J. L. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 340). Mahwah,

    NJ: Erlbaum.

    Ehri, L. C. (2002). Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching. In

    R. Stainthorp & P. Tomlinson (Eds.), Learning and teaching reading. London: British Journal of

    Educational Psychology Monograph Series II.Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. D., & Jenkins, J. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading

    competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5,

    239259.

    Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Kirby, J. R., & Stephenson, K. (2008). Rapid naming components and their

    relationship with phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, speed of processing and

    different reading outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 325350.

    Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (Eds.). (2002a). Dynamic indicators of basic early literacy skills

    (6th ed.). Eugene, OR: Institute for the Development of Educational Achievement.

    Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (2002b). DIBELS oral reading fluency passages for first through third

    grades (Technical report no. 10). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.

    Good, R. H., Simmons, D. C., & Kameenui, E. J. (2001). The importance and decision-making utility of

    a continuum of fluency-based indicators of foundational reading skills for third-grade high-stakes

    outcomes.Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 257288.

    Goswami, U. (1988). Childrens use of analogy in learning to spell. British Journal of Developmental

    Psychology, 6, 2133.

    Gough, P. B. (1996). How children learn to read and why they fail. Annals of Dyslexia, 4, 320.

    Gough, P. B., & Walsh, M. (1991). Chinese, phoenicians, and the orthographic cipher of English. In

    S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy(pp. 199209). Hillsdale, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Harn, B. A., Stoolmiller, M., & Chard, D. J. (2008). Measuring the dimensions of alphabetic principle on

    the reading development of first graders: The role of automaticity and unitization. Journal of

    Learning Disabilities, 41, 143157.

    Hudson, R. F., Pullen, P. C., Lane, H. B., & Torgesen, J. K. (2009). The complex nature of readingfluency: A multidimensional view. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25, 432.

    Jenkins, J. R., Bausell, R. B., & Jenkins, L. M. (1972). Comparisons of letter name and letter sound

    training as transfer variables. American Educational Research Journal, 9, 7586.

    Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual

    differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95,

    719729.

    Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Speed of processing, naming speed, and reading. Developmental

    Psychology, 30, 949954.

    Kameenui, E. J., & Simmons, D. C. (2001). Introduction to this special issue: The DNA of reading

    fluency.Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 203210.

    Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2003). Is English spelling chaotic? Misconceptions concerning its

    irregularity.Reading Psychology, 24, 267289.Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The

    Guilford Press.

    LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.

    Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293323.

    Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness and orthographic

    knowledge in second graders.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325333.

    Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal

    prediction of reading sub-skills in first and second graders.Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129158.

    McKay, M. F., & Thompson, G. B. (2009). Reading vocabulary influences in phonological recoding

    during the development of reading skill: A re-examination of theory and practice. Reading and

    Writing, 22, 167184.Moats, L. C. (2000). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks

    Publishing Company.

    National Reading Panel. (2000). A report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read.

    Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

    Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 505

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    24/25

    Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C., & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are

    reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 283319.

    Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and reading

    comprehension skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 461469.

    Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological

    science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 3174.Samuels, S. J., & Farstrup, A. E. (2006).What research has to say about fluency instruction . Newark, DE:

    International Reading Association.

    Savage, R., & Frederickson, N. (2005). Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid

    automatic naming of digits and text-reading speed. Brain and Language, 93, 152159.

    Schatschneider, C., Buck, J., Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Hassler, L., Hecht, S., et al. (2004a).

    A multivariate study of individual differences in performance on the reading portion of the Florida

    comprehensive assessment test: A brief report. Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University, Florida

    Center for Reading Research.

    Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004b).

    Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of

    Educational Psychology, 96, 265282.

    Schwanenflugel, P. J., Hamilton, A. M., Kuhn, M. R., Wisenbaker, J. M., & Stahl, S. A. (2004).

    Becoming a fluent reader: Reading skill and prosodic features in the oral reading of young readers.

    Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 119129.

    Schwanenflugel, P. J., Meisinger, E. B., Wisenbaker, J. M., Kuhn, M., Strauss, G. P., & Morris, R. D.

    (2006). Becoming a fluent and automatic reader in the early elementary school years. Reading

    Research Quarterly, 41, 496522.

    Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition.

    Cognition, 55, 151218.

    Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-learning

    hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 95129.

    Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development:

    Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education:Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 157.

    Speece, D. L., Mills, C., Ritchey, K. D., & Hillman, E. (2003). Initial evidence that letter fluency tasks are

    valid indicators of early reading skill. The Journal of Special Education, 36(4), 223233.

    Speece, D. L., & Ritchey, K. D. (2005). A longitudinal study of the development of oral reading fluency

    in young children at risk for reading failure. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 387399.

    Stage, S. A., Sheppard, J., Davidson, M. M., & Browning, M. M. (2001). Prediction of first-graders

    growth in oral reading fluency using kindergarten letter fluency. Journal of School Psychology,

    39(3), 225237.

    Thompson, B. (2000). Ten commandments of structural equation modeling. In L. Grimm & P. Yarnold

    (Eds.), Reading and understanding more multivariate statistics (pp. 261284). Washington, DC:

    American Psychological Association.

    Thompson, B., & Vacha-Haase, T. (2000). Psychometrics is datametrics: The test is not reliable.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 60, 174195.

    Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Alexander, A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading:

    Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the

    brain. Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX:

    Pro-Ed Publishing.

    Treiman, R., Goswami, U., & Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications for reading

    development and theory. Memory & Cognition, 18, 559567.

    Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the

    acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.

    Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. (1993). Development of youngreaders phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83103.

    Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological

    processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal

    study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 7387.

    Wagner, R., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing

    (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    506 R. F. Hudson et al.

    1 3

  • 8/12/2019 Hudson Roxanne F

    25/25

    Walczyk, J. J., Wei, M., Zha, P., Griffith-Ross, D. A., Goubert, S. E., & Cooper, A. L. (2007).

    Development of the interplay between automatic processes and cognitive resources in reading.

    Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 867887.

    Weiderholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (2001). Gray oral reading test (4th ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED

    Publishing Co.

    Wolf, M. (1997). A provisional, integrative account of phonological and naming-speed deficits indyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading

    acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 6792). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias.

    Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415438.

    Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading:

    A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387407.

    Reading proficiency in developing readers 507