Hem Ma Ti

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    1/12

    The validity of the Bar-On emotional intelligence quotient

    in an offender population q

    Toni Hemmati a,b,*, Jeremy F. Mills a,b, Daryl G. Kroner c

    a Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canadab Department of Psychology, Bath Institution, P.O. Box 1500, 5775 Bath Road, Bath, ON, Canada K0H 1G0

    c Deparment of Psychology, Pittsburgh Institution, P.O. Box 4510, HNY 15, Kingston, ON, Canada K7L 5E5

    Received 21 January 2003; received in revised form 22 September 2003; accepted 6 October 2003

    Available online 19 November 2003

    Abstract

    Recent research has suggested that emotional intelligence can be quantified and is distinct from general

    intelligence. Bar-On (1997) established a self-report measure of emotional intelligence, the Emotional

    Quotient Inventory (EQ-i), proposed to reflect the potential for success in life. The current study examines

    the validity of the EQ-i in an offender sample. Results show that the EQ-i has no relationship with age, only

    a weak relationship with IQ, but a strong negative correlation with measures of psychopathology,

    depression and hopelessness. In addition, offenders as a group score higher than normals. Discussion

    centres on the suggestion that offenders interpret items differently from non-offenders, respond differently,

    and therefore require distinctive norms.

    2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Emotional intelligence; Offenders; Personality

    1. Introduction

    While the current wave of interest in measuring emotional intelligence was stimulated by

    Gardner (1983), research into emotional measurement theory has been well established for several

    qThe views expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the Correctional

    Services of Canada.* Corresponding author. Address: Department of Psychology, Bath Institution, P.O. Box 1500, 5775 Bath Road,

    Bath, ON, Canada K0H 1G0. Tel.: +1-613-351-8019; fax: +1-613-351-8347.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (T. Hemmati).

    0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.003

    Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706

    www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

    http://mail%20to:%[email protected]/http://mail%20to:%[email protected]/
  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    2/12

    decades. Eysenck (1975) offered a summary of past research into emotion measurement. Tradi-

    tionally, attempts were made to measure emotions across three distinct parameters: physiologicalconcomitants (e.g., heart rate); introspective assessment (i.e., self-report); and behavioural obser-

    vation (i.e., judging observed behaviour). Citing past research, Eysenck concluded the evidencesuggests that verbal report, far from being a throwback to pre-behaviouristic days, is in many ways

    the preferred method of measuring and indexing states of emotional arousal (p. 441).Gardner (1983) proposed that there are seven primary types of intelligence: verbal, mathe-

    maticallogical, spatial, kinesthetic, musical, intraphysic abilities (insight, inner contentment) and

    personal intelligences. The personal intelligences consist of interpersonal intelligence, the ability tounderstand others, and intrapersonal intelligence, the ability to develop an accurate model of theself and use it effectively to operate throughout life. Analogous to these personal intelligences, is

    the concept of emotional intelligence, proposed by Salovey and Mayer (1990) and popularized byGoleman (1995).

    Mayer, Salovey, and Caruso (2000) present a comprehensive review of three distinct concepts

    of emotional intelligence from the literature: The first is as a popular representation of currentculturea zeitgeist. The second consists of emotional intelligence as a component of, or synon-

    ymous with, personality. The third view, reflecting the perspective of Mayer et al., is that emo-tional intelligence is conceptualized as a mental ability. From this perspective, ability is linked to

    skill and capacity. In support of the ability model, Mayer, Caruso, and Salovey (1999) illustratedthrough factor analysis that emotional intelligence is composed of three separate factors: per-ceiving and expressing emotions; assimilating and understanding emotions; and managing those

    emotions. These authors suggest that emotional intelligence can be useful in predicting particularlife criteria such as parental warmth, life satisfaction and artistic ability.

    Rather than a measure of emotion per se, emotional intelligence is a measure of one s ability

    to recognize, use and regulate emotional, personal and social information in an adaptive way(Mayer et al., 1999). Bar-On endorses this concept but includes the personality aspects of generalmood and happiness in the Bar-On emotional intelligence quotient (EQ-i; Bar-On, 1997). He

    describes emotional intelligence as the emotional, personal, social and survival dimensions ofintelligence. Based on this definition, Bar-On developed the EQ-i as a measure of emotionalintelligence.

    1.1. Intelligence quotient

    A number of studies have examined the relationship between emotional intelligence and tra-

    ditional measures of cognitive intelligence. However, these studies have used various measures ofboth types of intelligence. For example, Bar-On (1997, p. 137) reported the absence of a rela-tionship (r 0:12) between the EQ-i and the total score of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale(WAIS) as evidence for the divergent validity of the EQ-i from IQ. However, the sample was very

    small (n 40) and only the total WAIS score was reported. Similarly, Newsome, Day, andCatano (2000) also reported no relationship (r 0:08) between the EQ-i and a measure of cog-nitive ability, the Wonderlic Personnel Test. Other research into the relationship of the EQ-i with

    a Standard Intelligence Test was conducted by Derksen, Kramer, and Datzko (2002). Theseresearchers examined the relationship of the EQ-i with the General Adult Mental Ability Scale

    (GAMA), a non-verbal measure of general intelligence, in a sample of 489 men. The authors

    696 T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    3/12

    found that the total EQ-i score was not related to GAMA but the EQ-i scales of stress and general

    mood were significantly but weakly related to GAMA (r 0:10 and r 0:12, respectively).Although the relationship was statistically significantly the overall variance accounted for was less

    than two percent.Emotional intelligence as measured by the Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS;

    Mayer et al., 1999) has also been studied with measures of cognitive intelligence. Through theirdevelopment of the MEIS, Mayer et al. (1999) found that emotional intelligence was correlated(r 0:36) with the vocabulary scale of the Army Alpha Intelligence Scale. Subsequent researchby Ciarrochi, Chan, and Caputi (2000) did not find a relationship (r 0:05) between emotionalintelligence as measured by the MEIS and cognitive intelligence as measured by the RavensIntelligence Test. Mayer, Caruso, and Salovey (2000) later argued that the low correlation be-

    tween emotional intelligence and the Ravens was due to the Ravens measurement of perfor-mance or spatial intelligence and not verbal performance.

    None of the above studies examined the relationship of emotional intelligence with both verbal

    and non-verbal (performance) measures of cognitive intelligence. The discrepancies between thesefindings may hinge upon this distinction. The current study will examine the relationship of

    emotional intelligence with cognitive intelligence as measured by both verbal and performancemeasures of cognitive intelligence.

    1.2. Age

    Research has demonstrated that IQ decreases with age in adulthood (e.g., Derksen et al., 2002).One might expect then, that emotional intelligence would also diminish as one ages, however thisis not the case: Bar-On (1997) found that EQ-i and scale scores were positively and significantly

    related to age. With age broken into 10-year blocks, the 4049 year-old age group consistently hadthe highest means across domains. This finding was replicated in a more recent study (Derksenet al., 2002) although these authors found a decrease in EQ-i scores past the age of 65 years. If the

    relationship between EQ-i and age is consistent, it might be reflected in offender populations witha broad age range representation.

    1.3. Psychopathology

    Bar-On proposed that emotional intelligence contributes to psychological well being. He sug-gested that in addition to traditional IQ tests, EQ-i can make a unique contribution to better

    understand people and their potential to succeed in various aspects of life (Bar-On, 1997, p. 4).Bar-On demonstrated that EQ-i total scores are positively related to measures of emotionalhealth, and negatively related to measures of psychopathology and neuroticism.

    Dawda and Hart (2000) found that EQ-i scores were positively correlated with emotional

    stability and negatively correlated with neuroticism and psychopathology. Parker, Taylor, andBagby (2001) also found a strong negative relationship between EQ-i scores and alexithymia.They suggested that because alexithymia is associated with illness behaviour and increased

    mortality from all causes, high emotional intelligence might convey protective factors againstmental and physical illness. They proposed that individuals high in alexithymia (therefore low in

    emotional intelligence) are intolerant of stress and possess limited adaptive resources.

    T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706 697

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    4/12

    1.4. Criminality

    Gardner (1983) stipulates in the day-to-day world, no intelligence is more important than the

    interpersonal (intelligence). If you dont have it, youll make poor choices about who to marry,what job to take and so on (p. 12). If this position is true, then those who make poor choices

    (e.g., commit offences) would be expected to have lower interpersonal intelligence than those whodo not commit offences. Little research has been conducted on the utility of emotional intelligencemeasures with criminals. Goleman (1995) stated that empathy (as defined as the capacity to know

    how another feels) is absent in criminal psychopaths, rapists and child molesters. He furtherasserted that the inability to feel the victims pain allows a perpetrator to fabricate and believelies that further facilitate their crime, for example, a child molester who believes he is expressing

    love.The literature on emotional intelligence measures in correctional facilities is limited. Bar-On

    (1997) cites an unpublished study that indicates prisoners in an American state facility scored

    significantly lower on the total and most scale scores when compared to a matched group from acommunity sample. Bar-On speculated that for this population, emotional intelligence is equated

    to success in abiding by the rules of society (Bar-On, 1997, p. 146). It is implied then that con-versely, low scores of emotional intelligence will be related to not abiding by societal laws. If

    indeed ability is related to capacity and behaviour, then the EQ-i might offer a unique insight intoforensic populations.

    The purpose of the current study is to examine the validity of the Bar-On EQ-i with an offender

    population. Consistent with prior research there are four hypotheses: First, past research com-paring emotional intelligence measures to those of general intelligence showed a positive rela-tionship between emotional intelligence and verbal components of IQ (Mayer et al., 1999). There

    were minimal relationships between emotional intelligence and non-verbal IQ measures. It istherefore hypothesized that EQ-i scores will be positively related to verbal IQ but not to per-formance IQ. Second, if the EQ-i captures the ability to deal effectively with day to day life then it

    is expected to be inversely related to measures of psychopathology. Third, studies have demon-strated a general increase in emotional intelligence with age, therefore it is hypothesized that EQ-iscores will be positively related to age among offenders. Fourth, if the EQ-i is predictive of success

    in life, then offenders scores on the EQ-i should be lower than those of the normative sample.

    2. Method

    2.1. Participants

    Participants were 119 male inmates in a medium security federal institution with an average age

    of 37.0 years (SD 11.7, range 2060). The racial composition of the sample was 70% White, 15%Black, 12% Native American, 2% Asian and 1% other. Twenty-six subjects were serving lifesentences; of the remaining subjects, the mean sentence length was 6.0 years (SD 3.6, range 216.8

    years). Offenders most serious index (confining) offences were assaultive 45%, robbery 34%,property 9%, sexual 4%, criminal negligence/driving 5% and drugs 3%. All of the participants

    volunteered and were not paid for their involvement in the study.

    698 T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    5/12

    2.2. Materials

    2.2.1. Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i; Bar-On, 1997)

    The EQ-i is a 133-item questionnaire with a 5-point Likert Response Scale. Responses to eachitem can range from 1, very seldom or not true of me to 5, very often or true of me for positively or

    negatively-keyed items. The final item is a self-report on honesty of responding and is not includedin any scale. The results are reported in four formats: the total score, the validity score, 5 scales,and 15 subscales. The scales and subscales are intrapersonal intelligence (emotional self-aware-

    ness, assertiveness, self-regard, self-actualization, independence); interpersonal intelligence(empathy, interpersonal relationships, social responsibility); adaptability (problem solving, realitytesting, flexibility); stress management (stress tolerance, impulse control); general mood (happi-

    ness, optimism). Higher scores indicate a higher level of emotional intelligence. Scores werederived by using item scales provided in the manual.

    2.2.2. Basic Personality Inventory (BPI; Jackson, 1997)

    The BPI is a 240-item instrument comprised of 12 scales: hypochondriasis, depression, anxiety,

    interpersonal problems, alienation, impulse expression, persecutory ideation, thinking disorder,self-depreciation, social introversion, denial, and deviation. Participants respond in a true or false

    format. Each of the 11 clinical scales (excluding deviation) has 20 items with balanced true/falsekeying. The deviation scale is a critical-item scale, with all items true-keyed. The BPI has dem-onstrated reliability and validity when used in an offender population (Kroner & Reddon, 1996;

    Kroner, Holden, & Reddon, 1997; Kroner, Reddon, & Beckett, 1991).

    2.2.3. Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR; Paulhus, 1994)

    The BIDR is a 40-item self-report with a seven-point Likert Response Scale. Items are scoredfrom 1, not true to 7, very true. Results are reported in two scales: Self-Deception Enhancement(SDE) and Impression Management (IM). SDE is a measure of the degree to which respondents

    answer honestly, although their answers are inflated through self-deception. IM however, rep-resents deliberate manipulation to demonstrate a better (or worse) presentation. Kroner andWeekes (1996) have demonstrated the reliability and validity of the BIDR when used with an

    offender sample.

    2.2.4. Depression Hopelessness and Suicide Screening Form (DHS; Mills & Kroner, 2002)

    The DHS is a 39-item true/false response questionnaire designed to screen for hopelessness and

    depression and flag suicide and self-harm concerns. Depression and hopelessness items are bothnegatively and positively keyed. A response of true to a positively-keyed item, is scored as 1 (e.g.,My problems dont seem to end). A false response to a negatively-keyed item is scored as 1 (e.g.,My future will be mostly happy). Higher scores on the depression scale indicate depressed affect,

    such as sadness, social withdrawal and a reduced interest in previously enjoyed activities. Elevatedscores on the hopelessness scale are suggestive of despair, for example the inability to anticipatefuture happiness and little sense of self-efficacy. The DHS includes a 13-item critical item checklist

    for self-harm and suicide ideation that is not to be summed (e.g., I recently had thoughts of hurtingmyself). Developed and normed on an offender population, the DHS has demonstrated both

    internal consistency and validity (Mills & Kroner, in press).

    T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706 699

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    6/12

    2.2.5. Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB; Jackson, 1998)

    The MAB-II is a measure of aptitude and intelligence. Scores are reported in three formats: Asa verbal IQ comprised of information, comprehension, arithmetic, similarities and vocabulary

    subscales; as performance IQ comprised of digit symbol, picture completion, spatial, picturearrangement, and object assembly subscales; and as an overall IQ score.

    2.3. Procedure

    Offenders were approached at the time they completed testing for a psychological risk assess-ment and asked if they would participate in this research. Agreement was indicated by signing aconsent form. All participants were tested for literacy and achieved at least a grade five reading

    level. All of the measures were administered within a time period of two or three days.The EQ-i manual (Bar-On, 1997, p. 43) states that item 133 is included as a validity measure

    and not summed in any scale: a response of 4, often true of me or 5, very often true of me renders

    the results invalid. This is not a reverse-scored item, therefore the indication of non-validityshould be 1, very seldom or not true of me, or 2, seldom true of me. Only one subject in the current

    study responded with 1 for this item and was dropped from analysis. No one responded with 2,therefore the total n was reduced from 119 to 118.

    3. Results

    Table 1 shows the mean, range, standard deviation, alpha and T-score equivalents for the EQ-itotal, scales, and 15 subscales. Calculation of the T-score equivalents was based upon the North

    American norms provided by Bar-On (1997). Table 2 provides descriptive statistics for the BPI,DHS, BIDR and MAB scales. All EQ-i scale intercorrelations are significant at the p< 0:01 level(Table 3). DHS scores were available for only 104 participants and only 92 participants had MAB

    scores.Table 4 shows the correlations between EQ-i total and EQ-i scales with other measures used in

    the study. The EQ-i total and scales are negatively correlated with the DHS and BPI scales with

    the exception of BPI denial that shows a positive relationship. EQ-i is also positively related to theBIDR scales. All correlations between the EQ-i and the BPI, DHS and BIDR are significant at thep< 0:01 level with the exception of the EQ-i interpersonal scale with the BPI thinking disorderscale that is non-significant. Correlations between the EQ-i and the MAB total score were sta-

    tistically significant at the p 0:05).

    Most EQ-i scale scores were higher for the current sample than those reported for the nor-mative sample by Bar-On (1997). T-tests between group means, correcting for unequal variance

    when encountered (Reddon, 1992), indicated the offender scores were significantly greater than

    700 T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    7/12

    those of the normative sample for the EQ-i total, intrapersonal, interpersonal, and adaptability

    scales (Table 5).

    4. Discussion

    Consistent with prior research (e.g., Mayer et al., 1999; Mayer, Caruso et al., 2000; Mayer,Salovey et al., 2000), the results from the current study support the hypothesis that EQ-i is weaklyrelated to verbal IQ, though there is no relationship between EQ-i and performance IQ. Theseresults are also consistent with those of Derksen et al. (2002) who found minimal but significant

    relationships between the EQ-i scales and IQ total score.Mayer et al. (1999) specified three criteria for an intelligence: First, that it can be operation-

    alized by a set of abilities; second, that these abilities should form related sets and be related to astandard pre-existing intelligence while still accounting for unique variance; and third that abil-

    ities of the intelligence will develop with age and experience from youth to adulthood. By meetingthese criteria, the EQ-i would qualify as a measure of emotional intelligence by Mayer et al. s

    (1999) standards. However, Davies, Stankov, and Roberts (1998) specify that if emotionalintelligence is to qualify as an intelligence, it must be independent from personality traits. Thisposition is also supported by Mayer et al. (1999), Mayer, Caruso et al. (2000), and Mayer, Salovey

    Table 1

    EQ-i descriptive statistics

    Scale Range Mean SD Alpha T-score equivalent

    EQ-i total 320577 481.3 57.7 0.97 104.8

    Intrapersonal 103190 164.4 20.0 0.92 105.6

    Emotional self-awareness 1740 32.6 5.5 0.83 107.9

    Assertiveness 1635 28.2 4.5 0.73 107.9

    Self-regard 1845 37.5 6.3 0.87 104.1

    Self-actualization 2345 37.7 5.3 0.75 100.0

    Independence 1835 28.3 4.0 0.59 103.1

    Interpersonal 64120 102.4 12.4 0.90 103.9

    Empathy 1940 33.6 4.8 0.75 100.4

    Interpersonal relationships 2355 46.0 6.6 0.85 104.4

    Social responsibility 2350 44.4 5.2 0.79 102.2

    Adaptability 73130 107.9 13.8 0.90 106.1

    Problem solving 2040 32.9 5.1 0.83 103.6

    Reality testing 3050 42.8 5.3 0.76 108.8

    Flexibility 1440 32.1 5.2 0.76 109.7

    Stress management 4387 69.6 9.5 0.84 102.1

    Impulse control 2343 34.5 4.5 0.61 99.8

    Stress tolerance 1945 35.1 6.0 0.82 103.8

    General mood 4185 70.0 10.2 0.89 99.1

    Happiness 2145 37.0 6.1 0.83 98.6

    Optimism 1940 33.0 5.0 0.78 100.1

    T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706 701

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    8/12

    et al. (2000). Eysenck (1975) and Bar-On (1997) though, both include personality components as anecessity in an emotional intelligence concept. Our results that show the EQ-i is more strongly

    related to psychopathology than IQ is reflective of the personality-inclusion view. It is interestingto note that although inclusive of personality, it could be argued that the EQ-i meets the first twoof the (personality-excluded) criteria for intelligence stipulated by Mayer et al. Without pre-adult

    participants however, the third criterion for an intelligence, the developmental component cannotbe fully tested.

    In the current sample, the hypothesis that EQ-i scores increase with age was not supported.

    Both correlational analysis and a one-way ANOVA failed to show a relationship between EQ-iand age. Derksen et al. (2002) and Bar-On (1997) however, found a positive relationship between

    EQ-i and age in community samples. The failure to replicate that finding in the current samplemight be unique to an offender sample. A replication with a larger offender sample would help toverify this finding.

    The third hypothesis was that there would be an inverse relationship between EQ-i and psy-chopathology. This was supported by the strong relationship between EQ-i and psychopathology

    as measured by the BPI and DHS. The size of these correlations when compared to those betweenEQ-i and IQ suggests that the EQ-i may be more strongly linked to personality constructs thancognitive intelligence. Similar correlations were found by Bar-On (1997) between EQ-i total and

    negative affect measured by the Beck Depression Inventory and the Zung Self-rating DepressionScale.

    Table 2

    BPI, DHS, BIDR, and MAB descriptives

    Scale Range Mean SD

    BPI hypochondriasis 016 3.6 3.4BPI depression 015 3.9 3.1

    BPI denial 016 7.1 3.2

    BPI interpersonal problems 117 5.7 3.4

    BPI alienation 012 3.7 2.7

    BPI persecutory ideation 015 3.8 3.1

    BPI anxiety 014 4.3 3.0

    BPI thinking disorder 07 1.6 1.7

    BPI impulse expression 017 5.0 3.6

    BPI social introversion 019 5.1 3.7

    BPI self-depreciation 014 2.0 2.5

    BPI deviation 010 3.2 1.9

    DHS totala

    022 2.4 3.8DHS hopelessnessa 09 0.5 1.2

    DHS depressiona 014 2.1 3.1

    BIDR impression management 32134 82.7 19.9

    BIDR self-deception 43123 94.9 13.0

    MAB overallb 73128 95.0 12.5

    MAB verbalb 74122 93.7 11.0

    MAB performanceb 70134 98.7 14.9

    a DHS scores based on 104 participants.b MAB scores based on 92 participants and are reported in standardized form (M 100, SD 15).

    702 T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    9/12

    Table 3

    EQ-i interscale correlations

    Scale 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 1

    1. EQ total 0.95 0.91 0.95 0.87 0.92 0.90 0.79 0.79 0.56 0.85 0.75 0.77 0.88 0.87 0.84 0

    2. Intrapersonal 0.81 0.87 0.80 0.82 0.91 0.82 0.83 0.65 0.86 0.64 0.68 0.80 0.80 0.77 03. Interpersonal 0.86 0.69 0.83 0.75 0.74 0.68 0.41 0.73 0.90 0.89 0.93 0.77 0.74 0

    4. Adaptability 0.80 0.86 0.79 0.71 0.74 0.48 0.80 0.73 0.75 0.81 0.92 0.85 0

    5. Stress

    mangement

    0.79 0.80 0.65 0.62 0.49 0.69 0.54 0.54 0.72 0.75 0.73 0

    6. General mood 0.84 0.66 0.67 0.41 0.75 0.68 0.69 0.82 0.77 0.75 0

    7. Self-regard 0.70 0.71 0.53 0.75 0.56 0.60 0.76 0.71 0.75 0

    8. Emotional

    self-awareness

    0.69 0.37 0.63 0.61 0.57 0.77 0.67 0.62 0

    9. Assertiveness 0.41 0.62 0.56 0.59 0.67 0.67 0.65 0

    10. Independence 0.46 0.31 0.34 0.41 0.45 0.49 0

    11. Self-actual-

    ization

    0.56 0.67 0.67 0.77 0.65 0

    12. Empathy 0.82 0.75 0.65 0.58 0

    13. Social

    responsibility

    0.70 0.70 0.58 0

    14. Interpersonal

    relationship

    0.71 0.76 0

    15. Reality

    testing

    0.68 0

    16. Flexibility 0

    17. Problem

    solving

    18. Stress

    tolerance

    19. Impulsecontrol

    20. Optimism

    21. Happiness

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    10/12

    The fourth hypothesis that offenders as a group would have lower scores than those of thenormative sample was not supported. If it is true that individuals who make poor life decisions(i.e., engage in criminal activity) are lower in emotional intelligence than those who make goodlife

    decisions (i.e., choose not to engage in criminal activity), then the finding that offenders havescores equal to or higher than non-offenders presents a paradox. A potential explanation is that

    offenders demonstrate a high degree of social desirability in their responses. This however, isconfounded by research that shows a significant negative relationship between the BIDR and

    criminal risk indices (Mills & Kroner, submitted; Mills, Loza, & Kroner, 2003). In other words, as

    Table 4

    EQ-i scale correlations with BPI, DHS, BIDR, IQ, and age

    Scale EQ-i total Intra Inter Adapt Stress General mood

    BPI hypochondriasis)

    0.50)

    0.47)

    0.43)

    0.47)

    0.44)

    0.47BPI depression )0.67 )0.65 )0.54 )0.60 )0.62 )0.67

    BPI denial 0.44 0.45 0.30 0.41 0.45 0.38

    BPI interpersonal problems )0.64 )0.56 )0.65 )0.61 )0.56 )0.62

    BPI alienation )0.54 )0.46 )0.51 )0.55 )0.49 )0.52

    BPI persecutory ideation )0.44 )0.44 )0.31 )0.41 )0.40 )0.42

    BPI anxiety )0.50 )0.49 )0.34 )0.43 )0.57 )0.45

    BPI thinking disorder )0.26 )0.20 )0.19 ns )0.28 )0.32 )0.26

    BPI impulse expression )0.65 )0.59 )0.52 )0.62 )0.67 )0.63

    BPI social introversion )0.56 )0.54 )0.55 )0.53 )0.38 )0.54

    BPI self-depreciation )0.57 )0.56 )0.49 )0.49 )0.50 )0.58

    BPI deviation )0.64 )0.62 )0.56 )0.60 )0.54 )0.58

    DHS total)

    0.61)

    0.57)

    0.50)

    0.61)

    0.50)

    0.61DHS hopelessness )0.47 )0.43 )0.36 )0.43 )0.41 )0.51

    DHS depression )0.59 )0.54 )0.46 )0.57 )0.50 )0.60

    BIDR impression

    management

    0.50 0.48 0.46 0.47 0.42 0.45

    BIDR self-deception 0.43 0.43 0.33 0.43 0.38 0.39

    MAB overall 0.20 ns 0.15 ns 0.20 ns 0.25 0.18 ns 0.21

    MAB verbal 0.21 0.16 ns 0.21 0.25 0.17 ns 0.21

    MAB performance 0.17 ns 0.12 ns 0.18 ns 0.20 ns 0.15 ns 0.17 ns

    Age )0.01 ns )0.01 ns )0.04 ns )0.02 ns 0.02 ns 0.00 ns

    Note: All correlations between the EQ-i, EQ-i scales, BPI scales, BIDR scales and DHS scales are significant at the

    p< 0:01 level unless otherwise indicated. Correlations with the MAB and its scales are significant at the p< 0:05 level

    unless otherwise indicated.

    Table 5

    Comparison between inmate scores and Bar-Ons (1997) normative sample

    Scale t df p

    EQ total 2.97 122.47

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    11/12

    a group, offenders who demonstrate the least amount of socially desirable responding have the

    greater risk of re-offence. This intuitively paradoxical finding suggests that these high-risk indi-viduals would respond to other measures (i.e., EQ-i) with the same low degree of socially desirable

    responding. Taken together, this suggests that correcting for socially desirable responding inoffender scores in the same manner as for non-offenders to produce corrected clinical scores will

    not yield comparable results. As suggested by Mills et al. (2003) it appears that the items mighthold different meaning for offenders in the way that they see themselves and in the way they reportthat information.

    Although different results might be obtained using a larger sample, the failure of offender scoresto reflect those of the normative sample suggests the need for offender norms distinct from non-offender norms. In regard to the type of measure used, although self-report is the most common

    and arguably the preferred method of measuring emotion (Diener, 2000; Watson, 2000), it may beinsufficient to gauge the level of intelligence governing emotion. Alternative measures that do notrely purely on self-report could be less susceptible to socially desirable responding and thereby

    yield different results when applied to an offender population. Further research in testing com-peting models of emotional intelligence that account for the potential influence of socially

    desirable responding is recommended.

    References

    Bar-On, R. (1997). EQ-i Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.

    Ciarrochi, J. V., Chan, A. Y. C., & Caputi, P. (2000). A critical evaluation of the emotional intelligence construct.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 539561.

    Davies, M., Stankov, L., & Roberts, R. D. (1998). Emotional intelligence: In search of an illusive construct. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 9891015.

    Dawda, D., & Hart, S. D. (2000). Assessing emotional intelligence: Reliability and validity of the Bar-On Emotional

    Quotient Inventory (EQ-I) in university students. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 797812.

    Derksen, J., Kramer, I., & Datzko, M. (2002). Does a self-report measure for emotional intelligence assess something

    different than general intelligence? Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 3748.

    Diener, E. (2000). Subjective well-being: The science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. American

    Psychologist, 55, 3443.

    Eysenck, H. J. (1975). The measurement of emotion: Psychological parameters and methods. In L. Levi (Ed.),

    Emotionstheir parameters and measurement. New York: Raven Press.

    Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

    Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.

    Jackson, D. N. (1997). Basic Personality Inventory. London, ON: Research Psychologist Press.

    Jackson, D. N. (1998). Multidimensional aptitude battery-II. London, ON: Research Psychologist Press.Kroner, D. G., Holden, R. R., & Reddon, J. R. (1997). Validity of the Basic Personality Inventory in a correctional

    setting. Assessment, 4, 141153.

    Kroner, D. G., & Reddon, J. R. (1996). Factor structure of the Basic Personality Inventory with incarcerated offenders.

    Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 18, 275284.

    Kroner, D. G., Reddon, J. R., & Beckett, N. (1991). Basic Personality Inventory clinical and validity scales: Stability

    and internal consistency. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 13, 147154.

    Kroner, D. G., & Weekes, J. R. (1996). Balanced inventory of desirable responding: Factor structure, reliability and

    validity with an offender sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 323333.

    Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an

    intelligence. Intelligence, 27, 267298.

    T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706 705

  • 8/2/2019 Hem Ma Ti

    12/12

    Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (2000). Selecting a measure of emotional intelligence: The case for ability

    studies. In R. Bar-On & J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), Handbook of emotional intelligence theory, development, assessment,

    and application at home, school, and in the workplace . San Franciso: Jossey-Bass.

    Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2000). Emotional intelligence as zeitgeist, as personality, and as a mental

    ability. In R. Bar-On & J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), Handbook of emotional intelligence theory, development, assessment,

    and application at home, school, and in the workplace . San Franciso: Jossey-Bass.

    Mills, J. F., & Kroner, D. G. (2002). The depression, hopelessness and suicide screening form. Unpublished user guide.

    Mills, J. F., & Kroner, D. G. (submitted). Impression management and self-report among violent offenders.

    Mills, J. F., & Kroner, D. G. (in press). A new instrument to screen for depression, hopelessness and suicide in

    offenders. Psychological Services.

    Mills, J. F., Loza, W., & Kroner, D. G. (2003). Predictive validity despite social desirability: Evidence for the robustness

    of self-report among offenders. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 13, 144154.

    Newsome, S., Day, A. L., & Catano, V. M. (2000). Assessing the predictive validity of emotional intelligence.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 10051016.

    Parker, D. A., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2001). The relationship between emotional intelligence and alexithymia.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 107115.

    Paulhus, D. L. (1994). Balanced inventory of desirable responding: Reference manual for the BIDR version 6.Unpublished manuscript. University of British Columbia.

    Reddon, J. R. (1992). A_Stat: Statistical hypotheses and utilities (version 2.0). Applied Psychological Measurement, 16,

    86.

    Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185211.

    Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford Press.

    706 T. Hemmati et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 37 (2004) 695706