head movement

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    1/17

    University of pristina

    Faculty of philosophy

    English language and literature

    Head Movement and More on Clausal Syntax

    -Presentation-

    Mentor: Student: Prof!ragana Spasic

    "o#an $ac%ovic

    &osovs%a Mitrovica'may ()*+

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    2/17

    Head Movement and More on Clausal Syntax

    ,o types of movement

    Phrasal movement: movement of a complete phrase. E.g. topicalisation:

    [DPSuch disruptions]iwe don't need tiMovement of words (called head movement head-to-head movementas all words are heads of phrases and since when heads move

    the! move to another head position". E.g.

    #asishe tileft$

    %he s!m&ol t(trace" mars the earlier position of the moved item". %he su&script iindicates that the trace is identical to other itemsmared with i. (%his identit! relation is called coindexation". %here are various different wa!s of indicating coindeation:

    ./0 a. #asisheti[)Ptread it]$ &. #as*she+t*[)Pt+read it]$

    c. #as she thas[)Ptsheread it]$ d. #as she has [)Pshe read it]$

    ,e now discuss head movement. -ther discussions: adford (*//0:ch. 1" 2arnie (+330:ch./"

    ( Su#1ect-2uxiliary 3nversion as 3-to-C Movement

    4es-no 5uestions are formed &! inverting the su&4ect and auiliar! (including dumm!5do":

    .+0She should goShould she go$

    .60

    She lies it

    She does lie it

    Does she lie it$

    '6nversion' as movement from 6nfl to 2omp (65to52 movement":

    .70

    2

    2P

    6P

    DP

    6'

    6

    )P

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    3/17

    a.

    willi

    shekti

    tk&u! a car$

    &.

    doesi

    shek

    ti

    tkeat caes$

    6nverted ver&s and complementisers compete for the same position:

    .80

    #e ased 'will she leave'

    #e ased 'did she leave'

    .90

    #e ased if she will leave

    #e ased whether she left

    .0

    7#e ased if will she leave

    7#e as'ed whether did she leave

    Illocutionary force distinctions (e.g. the 8uestion5statement contrast" involve 2 since 29s4o& is to relate 6P to a larger discourse.

    uestions demand that the discourse &e continued with an answer. Moving 6 to 2 is an instruction to the hearer to tell the speaer

    whether the proposition epressed &! 6P is true or not.

    Man! other languages have question particlesin 2:.*)0

    [2P[2 l] [6Ptu

    si

    ]]$

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    4/17

    ; (Southern ?hoisan@ Aotswana"*

    ;

    people past

    come

    BDid the people come$

    .**0

    Is I can go? Is you should eat it? Is Ben did go?

    [child descri&ed in adford

    (*//0:**" using isas a 8uestion particle]

    ,hat other languages signal with 8uestion particles English signals &! moving 6 to 2. Clternative: English also has a 8uestion particle&ut it has no phonological information associated with it so it must o&tain phonological features from the net lowest head (6nfl".

    *Dr!er M. +33Position of Polar Question Particles. 6n M. #aspelmath M. Dr!er D. il and A. 2omrie (eds."

    The World tlas of !anguage "tructures -ford Fniversit! Press.

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    5/17

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    6/17

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    7/17

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    8/17

    ,he Head Movement Constraint and the position of ver#s

    Head Movement Constraint (HMC": %he onl! place a head#can move to is the

    position occupied &! the head which selects#P as its complement.

    2onse8uence: a ver& cannot move to 2 unless it moves to 6 first. %he #M2 doesn9t for&id this t!pe of stepwise (successi$e cyclic"movement. %he net sections illustrate this.

    /* $exical ver#s in English ;uestions

    Geical ver&s don't move to 2omp &ut re8uire do5support:

    .*/0

    7Smoes she$

    7,ent she$

    7Eats she caes$

    .*+0

    Does she smoe$

    Did she go$

    Does she eat caes$

    .*60

    2

    2P6P

    DP

    6'

    6

    )P

    a.

    willi

    shekti

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    9/17

    tkgo home

    &.

    7wentishek

    tktihome$ (violates #M2 in (*+""

    c.

    7wentishek

    ti

    tktihome$

    d.

    didishek

    ti

    tkgo home$

    %he #ead Movement 2onstraint predicts that moving from ) to 2omp must involve moving to 6nfl first. Aut English leical ver&s don9t

    move to 6nfl. Since the 2omp position must &e filled &! movement of a lower head the du%%yauiliar! dois inserted under 6nfl so

    that movement from 6nfl to 2omp is possi&le. #ence the do5support in 8uestions.

    /(

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    10/17

    Ieg

    )P

    DP

    )9

    %noesti

    )

    DP

    thouk ti

    not tkti

    the answer$

    %here is independent e$idencethat leical ver&s moved to 6 in earlier English. ,e now that the negative particle (not" occupies a

    position on the left edge of the )P i.e. one &etween 6 and ). %his is true of &oth Modern English and earlier English. 6n Earl! Modern

    English notappears after the leical ver& suggesting that ) moved past not:

    .*90 2FEI% EIG6S#: #e [6did] not [)hear] her plea.

    ECGJ M-DEI EIG6S#:#e [6heardi]6not [)ti] her plea.

    /+ Multiple auxiliaries in English

    %o understand the net point we need to note that the earlier assumption that auiliaries start in 6nfl was an oversimplification since wecan have more than one auiliar!:She could have #een #eingtreated &! a decent doctor if she9d had &etter insurance. %he cluster of auiliaries is not a single head seeing it can&e &roen up &! adver&s:

    She couldpro&a&l! have #een #eingtreated &! a decent doctor.

    Each auiliar! seems to form a constituent with the material after it. %hese constituents can undergo ellipsis 4ust lie )P ellipsis:

    They said she could have been beingtreated (y a decent doctor) (ut I didn*t think+++a. +++she could ha$e (een(eing treated (y a doctor&. +++she could ha$e(een (eing treated (y a doctorc. +++she couldha$e (een (eing treated (y adoctor

    %o eplain these facts man! linguists assume that each auiliar! is a ver& forming a )P with its complement. %he highest auiliar!

    moves to 6nfl (and then to 2omp in 8uestions":

    2P

    6P

    69

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    11/17

    )P

    )P

    )P

    )P

    )P

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    12/17

    2 DP

    6

    )

    ))

    ))

    DP

    2ouldsheitt

    have&een

    &eingtreated

    ti$

    Iow the #ead Movement 2onstraint correctl! predicts that onl! the highest auiliar! can appear at the front of the sentence in

    8uestions:

    a. 2ouldithe! tihave &een &eing treated$

    &. 7#aveithe! could ti&een &eing treated$ [cf.#a$eitheyti(een (eing treated?]

    c. 7Aeithe! could have ti&eing treated$ [cf. Wereitheyti(eing treated?]

    K

    2 Draw trees for the following sentences (using triangle notation for DPs and CdvPs".

    *.

    Should we read it$

    +. Did the people get it$

    K.

    Lran could have left.

    . #e has &een reading.

    [ad4oin tooto right of lowest )P]

    .

    ?ate should have &een woring and Aill should too.

    D.

    [Earl! Modern English]: Govest thou me$

    Some older Aritish speaers sa!#a$e you a pencil?while others sa!,o you ha$e apencil$ Draw trees for &oth structures and eplain

    the difference &etween them.

    + 2 note on 3nfl and ver# inflection in English

    6n English unlie other languages ver&s don9t move to 6nfl. %wo ideas a&out how tense and agreement information in 6nfl gets

    associated with the ver&:

    2.$oering: %he inflectional features in 6 lower onto ) in a process resem&ling headmovement ecept that it moves downwards. (See

    e.g. the tet&oos &! 2arnie #aegeman". ".Feature raising: ) enters the s!nta full! inflected. %enseNagreement features moveto 6

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    13/17

    &ut )9s phonological features do not move with it. (adford "yntactic Theory)++/f"

    6P

    6')P

    )9

    DP6

    DP)

    DP

    [Present

    [Present

    Ksg]

    Ksg]

    shek

    tkunderstands

    the situation

    6n these accounts featuresmove not morphemes. Morpheme movement is implausi&le with irregular ver&s (go-&ent". -n the first

    account a&ove the morpholog! must &e postsyntactic.

    6 =egation in English

    6* >hy is there do-support ith negation?

    English leical ver&s cannot &e negated without do5support &ut auiliaries can:

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    14/17

    .(90

    a. 7She found not her e!s@7She foundn9t her e!s

    .(:0

    &. She did not find her e!s@

    She didn9t find her 'e!s

    Lrancine cannotNma! notNmightn9tNwon9t find her e!s

    C possi&le eplanation for do5support in English negative clauses (a simplification of adford "yntactic theory +K*ff":.ot-n*theads aphrase located &etween 6nfl and )P cf. (K3"a". (%he more complicated variant in (K3"&" is discussed in class."

    ./)0 a. 6P

    &. 6P

    DP6'

    DP

    6'

    6

    IegP

    6

    IegP

    Ieg)P

    not

    Ieg9

    not-n*t

    pas

    Ieg)P

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    15/17

    n/t-ne

    %he inflectional features of ver&s are incompati&le with the English Ieg head since it is not a ver&. %hus these features cannot move to

    Ieg.

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    16/17

    #ead movement and more on clausal s!nta

    %he #ead Movement 2onstraint predicts that movement of features onl! occurs &etween a head and the head of its complement. %husan!thing which moves &etween ) and 6nfl would have to move via Ieg first. 6f it can9t move to Ieg then it can9t move an! further.

    Cs a last resort dumm!5dois inserted in 6 to allow epression of inflectional features.

    6( More on =eg

    ,hat was said a&out negation onl! applies to what is called clausal negationor sententialnegation. 6n cases ofconstituent negation

    there9s no need fordo5support. (6n Minimalists!nta unnecessar! operations induce ungrammaticalit!."

    ./*0

    a. She neverfound her e!s

    &. 7She did neverfind her 'e!s (ne$erO not e$er"

    ./(0

    a. She found noe!s&. 7She did find noe!s

    a. %he! in no isefulfilled the re8uirements.7%he! did in no isefulfil the re8uirements.

    a. Dishwashers [CdvPnotonl!] save time &ut the! clean plates &etter.

    7Dishwashers do [CdvPnotonl!] save time the! also clean places &etter. #ere notnegates only not )P. not onlyis a constituent:[CdvP=otonl!] do dishwashers save time the! also clean plates &etter.

    Ione of these cases involve the Ieg head seen in the last section. ather the! involve negation inside )P ad4uncts (ne$er) not only" or

    negation inside a DP (no keys". So there9s no Ieg head &locing movement of features from ) to 6nfl.

    %he Ieg head is pro&a&l! &est seen as concerned with polarityrather than 4ust negation since the collo8uial affirmative particlessoand

    tooalso re8uire dosupport:

    ./60

    Speaer *:

    Aasil did not do that.

    Speaer +:

    #e did so@tooQ do that;

    6/ =egation in 5uestions:Notvs nt

    ./70

    a. #e did not go there

    &. #e didn9t go there

    ./80

    a. Did he not go there$

    &. 7Did he n9t go there$

    ./90

    a. 7Did not he go there$&. Didn9t he go there$

    ./t is aclitic i.e. must form a phonological unit with the auiliar!. ,hen au moves to 2 n*t must therefore move with it.

    6n can*t) don*t) &on*t a special phonological form replaces the unit auRclitic..ot isn9t a clitic so there is no reason to move it with au to the 2position.

    Sentences lie didn*t he goindicate that the do0support we see in 8uestions must involve insertion of doin 6nfl &efore moving the ver&.

    (6.e. dois not inserted directl! in 2."

  • 8/11/2019 head movement

    17/17

    $3,EA2,UAE

    Citchison H. (*//". ,ords in the Mind. Cn 6ntroduction to the Mental Geicon. +nd edition.

    2r!stalD. (*//". %he 2am&ridge Enc!clopedia of the English Ganguage.

    GipaG. (*//+". Cn -utline of English Geicolog!. +nd edition.

    IidaE. (*/0". 2omponential Cnal!sis of Meaning.

    Prcic. (*//0". TUVWXYZW Z \W^VWYZW U_Z

    %a!lorH.. (*//". Ginguistic 2ategori`ation. Protot!pes in Ginguistic %heor!. +nd edition.