6
1 2 FOOD REWARD WITHOUT A TIMING COMPONENT DOES NOT ALTER 3 THE TIMING OF ACTIVITY UNDER POSITIVE ENERGY BALANCE 4 V. VAN DER VINNE, *  J. AKKERMAN, G. D. LANTING, 5 S. J. RIEDE AND R. A. HUT 6 Chronobiology Unit, Groningen Institute for Evolutionary 7 Life Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands 8 Abstract—Circadian clocks drive daily rhythms in physiol- ogy and behavior which allow organisms to anticipate predictable daily changes in the environment. In most mam- mals, circadian rhythms result in nocturnal activity patterns although plasticity of the circadian system allows activity patterns to shift to different times of day. Such plasticity is seen when food access is restricted to a few hours during the resting (light) phase resulting in food anticipatory activity (FAA) in the hours preceding food availability. The mecha- nisms underlying FAA are unknown but data suggest the involvement of the reward system and homeostatic regula- tion of metabolism. We previously demonstrated the isolated effect of metabolism by inducing diurnality in response to energetic challenges. Here the importance of reward timing in inducing daytime activity is assessed. The daily activity distribution of mice earning palatable chocolate at their pre- ferred time by working in a running wheel was compared with that of mice receiving a timed palatable meal at noon. Mice working for chocolate (WFC) without being energetically challenged increased their total daily activity but this did not result in a shift to diurnality. Providing a chocolate meal at noon each day increased daytime activity, identifying food timing as a factor capable of altering the daily distribution of activity and rest. These results show that timing of food reward and energetic challenges are both independently suf- ficient to induce diurnality in nocturnal mammals. FAA observed following timed food restriction is likely the result of an additive effect of distinct regulatory pathways activated by energetic challenges and food reward. Ó 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd. on behalf of IBRO. Key words: activity rhythm, circadian thermo-energetics, food entrainable oscillator, nocturnality, palatable food. 9 10 INTRODUCTION 11 Circadian clocks drive daily oscillations in physiology and 12 behavior allowing organisms to anticipate daily changes 13 in their environment (Pittendrigh, 1993). The main mam- 14 malian circadian oscillator in the suprachiasmatic nucleus 15 (SCN) is synchronized with the outside light–dark (LD) 16 cycle by light input from the eye (Reppert and Weaver, 17 2002). The SCN uses neuronal and endocrine pathways 18 to orchestrate rhythmicity throughout the brain and 19 peripheral tissues (Dibner et al., 2010), leading to noctur- 20 nal activity patterns in most small mammals. 21 Plasticity in circadian organization allows for adaptation 22 to changes in the temporal organization of the environment 23 (Hut et al., 2012). Such plasticity can be observed when 24 access to food is restricted to a limited time during the light 25 phase (Mistlberger, 1994; Stephan, 2002). Food restriction 26 during the light phase results in food anticipatory activity 27 (FAA) accompanied by phase changes in body tempera- 28 ture, corticosterone (Mistlberger, 1994; Stephan, 2002) 29 and liver rhythms (Stokkan et al., 2001), while the SCN 30 remains phase locked to the LD cycle (Stokkan et al., 31 2001). FAA-related activity bouts persist during periods of 32 complete food deprivation, show transients when entraining 33 to different mealtimes and are expressed in SCN-ablated 34 animals (Stephan et al., 1979). Together, these experi- 35 ments show that FAA is driven by a SCN-independent ‘food 36 entrainable oscillator’ (FEO). 37 Time-restricted feeding protocols used to induce FAA 38 typically reduce daily food intake to 60–80% of ad libitum 39 levels to motivate animals to eat during their rest phase. 40 Increasing the severity of food deprivation also increases 41 FAA (Mendoza et al., 2005b; Gallardo et al., 2014), sug- 42 gesting that negative energy balance per se might be able 43 to induce diurnal activity in otherwise nocturnal mammals 44 (Hut et al., 2011, 2012). This idea was made explicit in 45 the circadian thermo-energetics (CTE) hypothesis, which 46 predicted that energetically challenged animals become 47 day active, because diurnality is associated with reduced 48 daily energy expenditure under natural conditions (Hut 49 et al., 2012; van der Vinne et al., 2014). The CTE hypothe- 50 sis was tested in a protocol where mice were energetically 51 challenged by letting them work for food (WFF; Hut et al., 52 2011). High ‘workloads’ (i.e. running long distances to 53 obtain a food pellet) indeed induce diurnality in mice (Hut 54 et al., 2011) and this effect is augmented by lower ambient 55 temperatures (van der Vinne et al., 2014). The WFF exper- 56 iments thus confirm that negative energy balance per se 57 induces diurnality and hence negative energy balance 58 can partly explain diurnal activity in FAA protocols. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.061 0306-4522/Ó 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd. on behalf of IBRO. * Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (V. van der Vinne).  Present address: Department of Neurobiology, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, MA, USA. Abbreviations: CTE, circadian thermo-energetics hypothesis; COG, center of gravity; ExT, external time; FAA, food anticipatory activity; FEO, food entrainable oscillator; LD, light–dark; SCN, suprachiasmatic nucleus; TCF, timed chocolate feeding; WFC, working for chocolate protocol; WFF, working for food protocol. Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.061 1 NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6 29 July 2015

Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Sistema de recompenza

Citation preview

Page 1: Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive

energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.061

NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6

29 July 2015

Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

FOOD REWARD WITHOUT A TIMING COMPONENT DOES NOT ALTERTHE TIMING OF ACTIVITY UNDER POSITIVE ENERGY BALANCE

10

11

12

V. VAN DER VINNE, *� J. AKKERMAN, G. D. LANTING,S. J. RIEDE AND R. A. HUT

Chronobiology Unit, Groningen Institute for Evolutionary

Life Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

Abstract—Circadian clocks drive daily rhythms in physiol-

ogy and behavior which allow organisms to anticipate

predictable daily changes in the environment. In most mam-

mals, circadian rhythms result in nocturnal activity patterns

although plasticity of the circadian system allows activity

patterns to shift to different times of day. Such plasticity is

seen when food access is restricted to a few hours during

the resting (light) phase resulting in food anticipatory activity

(FAA) in the hours preceding food availability. The mecha-

nisms underlying FAA are unknown but data suggest the

involvement of the reward system and homeostatic regula-

tion of metabolism.We previously demonstrated the isolated

effect of metabolism by inducing diurnality in response to

energetic challenges. Here the importance of reward timing

in inducing daytime activity is assessed. The daily activity

distribution of mice earning palatable chocolate at their pre-

ferred time byworking in a runningwheel was comparedwith

that of mice receiving a timed palatable meal at noon. Mice

working for chocolate (WFC) without being energetically

challenged increased their total daily activity but this did

not result in a shift to diurnality. Providing a chocolate meal

at noon each day increased daytime activity, identifying food

timing as a factor capable of altering the daily distribution of

activity and rest. These results show that timing of food

reward and energetic challenges are both independently suf-

ficient to induce diurnality in nocturnal mammals. FAA

observed following timed food restriction is likely the result

of an additive effect of distinct regulatory pathways activated

by energetic challenges and food reward. � 2015 Published

by Elsevier Ltd. on behalf of IBRO.

Key words: activity rhythm, circadian thermo-energetics,

food entrainable oscillator, nocturnality, palatable food.

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.0610306-4522/� 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd. on behalf of IBRO.

*Corresponding author.

E-mail address: [email protected] (V. van der Vinne).� Present address: Department of Neurobiology, University of

Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, MA, USA.Abbreviations: CTE, circadian thermo-energetics hypothesis; COG,center of gravity; ExT, external time; FAA, food anticipatory activity;FEO, food entrainable oscillator; LD, light–dark; SCN, suprachiasmaticnucleus; TCF, timed chocolate feeding; WFC, working for chocolateprotocol; WFF, working for food protocol.

1

INTRODUCTION

Circadian clocks drive daily oscillations in physiology and

behavior allowing organisms to anticipate daily changes

in their environment (Pittendrigh, 1993). The main mam-

malian circadian oscillator in the suprachiasmatic nucleus

(SCN) is synchronized with the outside light–dark (LD)

cycle by light input from the eye (Reppert and Weaver,

2002). The SCN uses neuronal and endocrine pathways

to orchestrate rhythmicity throughout the brain and

peripheral tissues (Dibner et al., 2010), leading to noctur-

nal activity patterns in most small mammals.

Plasticity in circadian organization allows for adaptation

to changes in the temporal organization of the environment

(Hut et al., 2012). Such plasticity can be observed when

access to food is restricted to a limited time during the light

phase (Mistlberger, 1994; Stephan, 2002). Food restriction

during the light phase results in food anticipatory activity

(FAA) accompanied by phase changes in body tempera-

ture, corticosterone (Mistlberger, 1994; Stephan, 2002)

and liver rhythms (Stokkan et al., 2001), while the SCN

remains phase locked to the LD cycle (Stokkan et al.,

2001). FAA-related activity bouts persist during periods of

complete food deprivation, show transients when entraining

to different mealtimes and are expressed in SCN-ablated

animals (Stephan et al., 1979). Together, these experi-

ments show that FAA is driven by a SCN-independent ‘food

entrainable oscillator’ (FEO).

Time-restricted feeding protocols used to induce FAA

typically reduce daily food intake to 60–80% of ad libitum

levels to motivate animals to eat during their rest phase.

Increasing the severity of food deprivation also increases

FAA (Mendoza et al., 2005b; Gallardo et al., 2014), sug-

gesting that negative energy balance per se might be able

to induce diurnal activity in otherwise nocturnal mammals

(Hut et al., 2011, 2012). This idea was made explicit in

the circadian thermo-energetics (CTE) hypothesis, which

predicted that energetically challenged animals become

day active, because diurnality is associated with reduced

daily energy expenditure under natural conditions (Hut

et al., 2012; van der Vinne et al., 2014). The CTE hypothe-

sis was tested in a protocol where mice were energetically

challenged by letting them work for food (WFF; Hut et al.,

2011). High ‘workloads’ (i.e. running long distances to

obtain a food pellet) indeed induce diurnality in mice (Hut

et al., 2011) and this effect is augmented by lower ambient

temperatures (van der Vinne et al., 2014). The WFF exper-

iments thus confirm that negative energy balance per seinduces diurnality and hence negative energy balance

can partly explain diurnal activity in FAA protocols.

Page 2: Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

2 V. van der Vinne et al. / Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6

29 July 2015

The argument above does not rule out the timing of

food availability as an important factor in inducing FAA.

Providing a palatable chocolate meal during the light

phase in animals with ad libitum access to regular chow

can induce FAA in rats (Mistlberger and Rusak, 1987;

Mendoza et al., 2005a; Verwey et al., 2007; Angeles-

Castellanos et al., 2008) and mice (Hsu et al., 2010).

The length and intensity of FAA, induced without chal-

lenging animals energetically, is reduced in comparison

to the FAA observed when all food access is restricted

to the light phase (Mendoza et al., 2005a; Verwey et al.,

2007; Angeles-Castellanos et al., 2008). Taken together,

these data show that the FEO controlled expression of

FAA is induced by effects of food timing combined with

negative energy balance. This suggests that both the

reward system and homeostatic regulation of metabolism

are involved in the expression of the FEO.

To test whether the reward system is also involved in

WFF-induced diurnality, we developed the ‘working for

chocolate’ (WFC) protocol in which mice with ad libitumaccess to regular chow can obtain a palatable chocolate

reward at all times of day by running in a wheel. The

daily distribution of activity observed during the WFC

protocol was compared to that of mice receiving a

chocolate reward in the middle of the light phase to

assess the importance of reward timing in re-organizing

the daily activity pattern. We expect that in the absence

of energetic challenges, daytime activity can be induced

by reward timing but not when reward timing is absent

in the WFC protocol. Furthermore, the long-term impact

of a palatable meal on the daily distribution of activity

was assessed by monitoring the daily activity pattern

after termination of daytime chocolate feeding.

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

156

157

158

159

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES

Animals and activity registration

Male CBA/CaJ mice were moved from our breeding

facility (14 h–10 h LD cycle) to the experimental rooms

(12 h–12 h LD cycle) at least one week before the start

of experiments. Mice were housed individually in

standard macrolon cages (15 � 32 � 13 cm) equipped

with a running wheel on a sawdust bedding (Lignocel

hygienic animal bedding, Rettenmaier, Rosenberg,

Germany) on experimental day 0. Standard chow food

(AM II diet rodent chow 10 mm, 17.3 kJ/g, Arie Blok,

Woerden, The Netherlands) and water were provided

ad libitum throughout the experiments. Procedures were

approved by the Animal Experimentation Committee of

the University of Groningen (DEC 5454).

Running wheel activity and activity around the feeding

place (only Experiment 2) were recorded in two-minute

bins and split in daily intervals starting at lights on

(external time (ExT) six). Activity during the light phase

was divided by the total daily activity to calculate the

percentage of daytime activity. Daily activity onset and

offset were calculated as the intersections of a short

(10 min) and a long running average (24 h; Hut et al.,

1999). Daily center of gravity (COG) was calculated as

the time of day where total activity in the preceding and

following 8 h was equal. Analyses were performed using

Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without

energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience

custom build scripts in SciLab 5.5.0, with statistics being

performed using mixed-effects general linear models in

SAS JMP 7.0. In these statistical analyses, separate anal-

yses were performed for each of the dependent variables

(daytime activity, onset, COG and offset) with treatment

as independent factor. Animal ID was included as a ran-

dom factor to our analyses to correct for the repeated

measurement of each individual. Tukey HSD post hoc

tests were performed when applicable. Data are repre-

sented as mean ± SEM in graphs and text.

Experiment 1: WFC

The WFC protocol allowed twelve ad libitum fed, five-

month-old mice to additionally earn 45-mg chocolate

pellets (Dustless Precision Pellets�, Sucrose, Chocolate

Flavor) by running in a wheel. The workload, the number

of revolutions needed to obtain a chocolate pellet, started

at 500 revs/pellet on day nine and was increased in steps

of 20 revs/pellet daily to 700 revs/pellet, where it was kept

stable (resulting in 37.3 ± 3.5 chocolate pellets per day).

This workload was chosen because it resulted in a

chocolate intake that was approximately twice the intake

observed when chocolate was provided at a fixed time,

ensuring that the lack of a phase shift during WFC was

not a result of lowered chocolate intake. The timing of the

chocolate reward in the WFC protocol was computer

controlled and the mice thus received their food reward

each time they reached the workload threshold. Reward

timing thus depended solely on the spontaneous activity

rhythm of the mice. After the WFC protocol (day 26 until

the end), each mouse was provided with 20 pellets daily

at the middle of the light phase (ExT 12) to assess the

effect of timed chocolate feeding (TCF) on the activity

rhythm. Daily checks were performed around one hour

before lights off, to confirm that all chocolate pellets had

been consumed. The daily distribution of activity was

compared for the last 5 days preceding WFC (AL), the

last 5 days of WFC (WFC) and the last 5 days of timed

chocolate feeding.

Experiment 2: TCF

Eight mice (1.5-month-old) were provided with 20 pellets

for two hours, starting at the middle of the light phase

(ExT 12). Food was given in a bowl without opening the

cage and remaining pellets were removed after two

hours to assess the number of pellets eaten. Chocolate

pellets were provided on days 9–40. On days 41–60

chocolate was replaced by chow pellets provided

between ExT 12 and 14, followed by 20 days of

undisturbed ad libitum feeding with only regular chow.

On experimental days 29–30, the acute effect of

termination of daily TCF was assessed by providing

mice with chow pellets between ExT 12 and 14.

Anticipatory activity around the feeding location was

assessed by measuring general locomotor activity using

a passive infrared detector placed directly above the

feeding place. The effects of TCF were assessed

between the last 5 days preceding TCF (pre test), the

last 5 days of TCF (Chocolate), the last 5 days of daily

provisioning of additional chow pellets (Chow pellets)

a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive

.2015.07.061

Page 3: Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

V. van der Vinne et al. / Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 3

NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6

29 July 2015

and the last 5 days without disturbance (post test). The

acute effects of termination of TCF were assessed by

comparing the changes in daily distribution of activity on

day 27–32.

RESULTS

Experiment 1: WFC

The effect of a food reward on timing of activity, without

being energetically challenged, was assessed by the

Fig. 2. Activity is phase advanced by TCF but not by WFC compared to AL. (

and TCF in 10-min bins with SEM. TCF induced a phase advance of activit

decreased during the middle dark phase during TCF compared to AL and WF

advanced by TCF but not by WFC.

Fig. 1. Mice remain nocturnal during working for chocolate (WFC) but diurnal

(A) Representative actogram of ad libitum fed mouse undergoing WFC (day

daily proportion of activity occurring during the light phase is plotted in the rig

phase is unaffected by WFC (day 21–25) but increases significantly during T

Total daily activity is increased during the last five days of WFC compared t

Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without

energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience

WFC protocol in mice with ad libitum access to regular

chow (Fig. 1A). Body mass increased significantly

throughout the experiment (F14,152 = 5.884; p< 0.0001)

without changes in growth during any of the different

feeding regimes, showing that the mice were never

energetically challenged during the experiment. WFC

induced significantly higher daily activity levels

(compared to no chocolate meal (AL) or TCF;

F2,175 = 48.66, p< 0.0001; Fig. 1C) without changing

the timing of activity. Timing of activity was only altered

A) Average relative activity profiles during the last 5 days of AL, WFC

y. Activity was increased during the middle and late light phase and

C. (B) Activity onset, center of gravity (COG) and offset were all phase

ity is induced by timed chocolate feeding during the light phase (TCF).

9–25, hatched area) followed by TCF (day 26–39, vertical line). The

ht hand graph. (B) The proportion of activity occurring during the light

CF (day 35–39), compared to ad libitum (AL; day 4–8) fed mice. (C)

o AL and TCF. *p< 0.05.

a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive

.2015.07.061

Page 4: Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

194

195

196

197

198

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

Fig. 3. Representative actograms showing running wheel and feeding place activity of a mouse provided with chocolate (days 9–28, 31–40) or

additional chow (days 29–30, 41–60) for two hours during the light phase. Chocolate feeding during the day advanced the main activity bout but did

not induce food anticipatory activity in either running wheel or feeding place activity. The daily proportion of total activity occurring during the light

phase is plotted to the right of each actogram. The total number of consumed chocolate (purple/black) and chow (green/gray) pellets eaten during

the 2-h interval is shown in the middle graph. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web

version of this article.)

4 V. van der Vinne et al. / Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6

29 July 2015

when a chocolate reward was provided in the middle of

the light phase (F2,162.1 = 222.1, p< 0.0001; Fig. 1B).

The percentage of activity occurring during the light

phase in mice undergoing AL (22.2 ± 4.1%) and WFC

(21.6 ± 4.8%) was indistinguishable from that observed

in undisturbed ad libitum fed mice (van der Vinne et al.,

2014).

The effect of TCF was also observed when the onset,

COG and offset of activity were used as phase markers of

the activity rhythm. Compared to days without access to

chocolate, WFC did not induce a phase shift in these

three phase markers while TCF produced a phase

advance of the activity rhythm (Onset: F2,165 = 23.21,

p< 0.0001; COG: F2,164 = 135.2, p< 0.0001; Offset

F2,166 = 62.25, p< 0.0001; Fig. 2B). Analysis of the

distribution of activity in 10-min bins showed increased

Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without

energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience

activity during the middle and late light phase and a

decrease during the middle of the dark phase during

TCF compared to AL and WFC (Fig. 2A).

Experiment 2: TCF

The importance of food timing for the induction of

diurnality in mice not subjected to energetic challenges

was further studied to determine the influence of

prolonged daytime food reward on diurnality. Body mass

increases throughout the experiment showed that the

mice maintained a positive energy balance

(F8,56 = 67.31; p< 0.0001) without changes in growth

between the different feeding regimes. Chocolate pellets

were provided in a 2-h interval during the light phase

and feeder approaches were measured using a passive

a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive

.2015.07.061

Page 5: Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

224

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

239

240

241

242

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

V. van der Vinne et al. / Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 5

NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6

29 July 2015

infrared detector placed directly above the feeding cup.

These measurements did not reveal a separate FAA

bout in any of the eight mice preceding the

administration of chocolate (Fig. 3). The obtained

reward increased from 6.0 ± 1.0 pellets/day in the first

5 days to 16.8 ± 0.8 pellets/day in the last 5 days of

chocolate feeding.

The importance of continued exposure to timed feeding

during the light phase for the maintenance of the phase-

advanced phenotype was assessed by replacing the

chocolate reward with regular-chow pellets. The imme-

diate effects of chocolate replacement were assessed on

experimental days 27–32 (Fig. 4B). Replacement of the

chocolate reward by regular chow did not result in a

return to a nocturnal activity profile (F5,35 = 1.724,

p=0.1548). Prolonged absence (20–40 days) of TCF

during the light phase resulted in a significant reduction in

relative daytime activity but daytime activity remained

significantly increased compared to the pre-test period

(F3,149 = 63.72, p< 0.0001; Fig. 4A). Surprisingly, the

fraction of activity occurring during the light phase stayed

nearly the same between experimental days 60 and 80,

showing a long-lasting effect of food reward during the

light phase on activity timing.

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

278

279

280

281

282

283

284

285

286

287

288

289

290

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

Fig. 4. Daytime chocolate feeding produces a slowly reversible

increase in daytime activity. (A) The proportion of activity occurring

during the light phase was significantly increased after 35–40 days of

daytime chocolate feeding (Chocolate). Relative daytime activity

reduced significantly after termination of chocolate feeding but did not

return to pre-test levels in mice provided with additional chow pellets

(Chow pellets; days 56–60) and left undisturbed (post-test; days 76–

80). (B) Relative daytime activity did not change significantly during a

two-day interruption of daytime chocolate feeding. *p< 0.05.

Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without

energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience

DISCUSSION

This study was designed to assess the influence of food

timing on the daily distribution of activity in the absence

of energetic challenges. The lack of a phase shift of

activity during WFC shows that an endogenously driven

shift to diurnality as observed during WFF (Hut et al.,

2011; van der Vinne et al., 2014) requires an energetic

challenge and cannot be induced by elevated activity

levels or the rewarding aspects of food. Although mice

increased the number of revolutions to obtain more

chocolate, these elevated activity levels did not change

the phase of the daily rhythm of activity and rest. Timing

of a palatable chocolate meal during the light phase did

however induce a phase shift in the daily timing of activity

in the absence of energetic challenges. These results

show that daytime food reward, independent of energetic

challenges, is sufficient to induce diurnality in mice.

Previous studies assessing the influence of time-

restricted palatable food access showed that the

intensity of FAA is typically lower than that observed in

response to timed food restriction (Mistlberger and

Rusak, 1987; Mendoza et al., 2005a; Verwey et al.,

2007; Angeles-Castellanos et al., 2008), while other stud-

ies failed to induce FAA (Abe and Rusak, 1992; Pecoraro

et al., 2002; Waddington Lamont et al., 2007). In the pre-

sent study, all 20 mice subjected to TCF showed a shifted

daily activity pattern although the behavioral changes

were smaller than those observed during timed food

restriction and WFF experiments (van der Vinne et al.,

2014), illustrating the importance of the metabolic state

of an animal in shaping the daily distribution of activity

(Hsu et al., 2010; van der Vinne et al., 2014).

The current study produced prolonged changes in the

daily distribution of activity and rest after daily chocolate

exposure was stopped. Although mice became

significantly less day active over the 40-day extinction

period, mice were substantially more day active during

the post-test period than pre-test. Compared to previous

measurements of the day activity observed in

undisturbed ad libitum fed mice in our laboratory (van

der Vinne et al., 2014), post-test daytime activity was also

significantly increased. Continued activity around the time

of feeding following a return to ad libitum feeding has

been reported previously (Abe and Rusak, 1992), but

not for the prolonged period seen in the present study.

The absence of energetic constraints or a reduced nega-

tive masking response in our mice might possibly provide

an explanation for the prolonged daytime activity. Overall,

the present study shows that our mice responded to time-

restricted palatable food access by robustly altering the

daily distribution of activity and rest.

The behavioral changes induced by timed palatable

food access in this study were qualitatively different

compared to the FAA observed in previous studies in

rats. Whereas the time-restricted feeding in rats induced

a separate FAA activity bout (Mistlberger and Rusak,

1987; Mendoza et al., 2005a; Verwey et al., 2007;

Angeles-Castellanos et al., 2008), our mice responded

to a timed daytime chocolate reward by phase advancing

the entire active phase. Previous studies assessing the

influence of time-restricted palatable food access in mice

a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive

.2015.07.061

Page 6: Food Reward Without a Timing Component Does Not Alter the Timing of Activity Under Positive Energy Balance

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

333

334

335

336

337

338

339

340

341

342

343

344

345

346

347

348

349

350

351

352

353

354

355

356

357

358

359

360

361

362

363

364

365

366

367

368

369

370

371

372

373

374

375

376

377

378

379

380

381

382

383

384

385

386

387

388

389

390

391

392

393

394

395

396

397

398

399

400

432

433

434

6 V. van der Vinne et al. / Neuroscience xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

NSC 16458 No. of Pages 6

29 July 2015

and hamsters did also not report a separate FAA activity

bout during the light phase (Abe and Rusak, 1992; Hsu

et al., 2010; Mendoza et al., 2010). Overall, these results

indicate a possible species difference between rats and

other rodents in their response to the weak timing signal

provided by time-restricted palatable food access, with

rats being more likely to split their total daily activity into

multiple activity bouts.

FAA induced by time-restricted feeding protocols is

likely the result of the combined influences of the

energetic challenges and daytime food reward that

occur during these protocols (Challet and Mendoza,

2010). Assuming that these two driving forces underlying

FAA are located in anatomically distinct locations may

explain why a large number of studies have been unsuc-

cessful in finding a single locus for the FEO (reviewed in

Davidson, 2009). This leads to the suggestion that the

FEO is comprised of a dispersed network of clocks

(Davidson, 2009; Verwey and Amir, 2009; Mistlberger,

2011; Blum et al., 2012). In line with this network hypoth-

esis is the observation that FAA is reduced following DMH

lesioning but reappears after SCN ablation (Acosta-

Galvan et al., 2011). Our results show that mice exposed

to the WFC protocol did not become diurnal while the

same mice shifted their active phase in response to

TCF. Because WFC simulates the rewarding aspect of

food in the previously used WFF protocol (Hut et al.,

2011; van der Vinne et al., 2014) without challenging mice

energetically, we conclude that distinct regulatory net-

works activated by either energetic challenges or by timed

food reward, are both capable of inducing diurnality.

401

402

403

404

405

406

Acknowledgments—We thank Jamey Scheepe, Nathasja Hartog

and Jenke Gorter for practical help during the experimental pro-

cedures. This work was partially funded an Ubbo Emmius

Fellowship (to S.J.R.).

407

408

409

410

411

412

413

414

415

416

417

418

419

420

421

422

423

424

425

426

427

428

429

430

431

REFERENCES

Abe H, Rusak B (1992) Anticipatory activity and entrainment of

circadian rhythms in Syrian hamsters exposed to restricted

palatable diets. Am J Physiol 263:R116–R124.

Acosta-Galvan G, Yi C-X, van der Vliet J, Jhamandas JH, Panula P,

Angeles-Castellanos M, del Carmen Basualdo M, Escobar C,

Buijs RM (2011) Interaction between hypothalamic dorsomedial

nucleus and the suprachiasmatic nucleus determines intensity of

food anticipatory behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

108:5813–5818.

Angeles-Castellanos M, Salgado-Delgado R, Rodrıguez K, Buijs RM,

Escobar C (2008) Expectancy for food or expectancy for

chocolate reveals timing systems for metabolism and reward.

Neuroscience 155:297–307.

Blum ID, Lamont EW, Abizaid A (2012) Competing clocks: metabolic

status moderates signals from the master circadian pacemaker.

Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:254–270.

Challet E, Mendoza J (2010) Metabolic and reward feeding

synchronises the rhythmic brain. Cell Tissue Res 341:1–11.

Davidson AJ (2009) Lesion studies targeting food-anticipatory

activity. Eur J Neurosci 30:1658–1664.

Please cite this article in press as: van der Vinne V et al. Food reward without

energy balance. Neuroscience (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience

Dibner C, Schibler U, Albrecht U (2010) The mammalian circadian

timing system: organization and coordination of central and

peripheral clocks. Annu Rev Physiol 72:517–549.

Gallardo CM, Hsu CT, Gunapala KM, Parfyonov M, Chang CH,

Mistlberger RE, Steele AD (2014) Behavioral and neural

correlates of acute and scheduled hunger in C57BL/6 mice.

PLoS One 9:e95990.

Hsu CT, Patton DF, Mistlberger RE, Steele AD (2010) Palatable meal

anticipation in mice. PLoS One 5:e12903.

Hut RA, Mrosovsky N, Daan S (1999) Nonphotic entrainment in a

diurnal mammal, the European ground squirrel (Spermophilus

citellus). J Biol Rhythms 14:409–419.

Hut RA, Pilorz V, Boerema AS, Strijkstra AM, Daan S (2011) Working

for food shifts nocturnal mouse activity into the day. PLoS One

6:e17527.

Hut RA, Kronfeld-Schor N, van der Vinne V, la Iglesia De H (2012) In

search of a temporal niche: environmental factors. Prog Brain Res

199:281–304.

Mendoza J, Angeles-Castellanos M, Escobar C (2005a) Entrainment

by a palatable meal induces food-anticipatory activity and c-Fos

expression in reward-related areas of the brain. Neuroscience

133:293–303.

Mendoza J, Graff C, Dardente H, Pevet P, Challet E (2005b) Feeding

cues alter clock gene oscillations and photic responses in the

suprachiasmatic nuclei of mice exposed to a light/dark cycle. J

Neurosci 25:1514–1522.

Mendoza J, ClesseD, Pevet P, Challet E (2010) Food-reward signalling

in the suprachiasmatic clock. J Neurochem 112:1489–1499.

Mistlberger RE (1994) Circadian food-anticipatory activity: formal

models and physiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev

18:171–195.

Mistlberger RE (2011) Neurobiology of food anticipatory circadian

rhythms. Physiol Behav 104:535–545.

Mistlberger R, Rusak B (1987) Palatable daily meals entrain

anticipatory activity rhythms in free-feeding rats: dependence on

meal size and nutrient content. Physiol Behav 41:219–226.

Pecoraro N, Gomez F, Laugero K, Dallman MF (2002) Brief access to

sucrose engages food-entrainable rhythms in food-deprived rats.

Behav Neurosci 116:757–776.

Pittendrigh CS (1993) Temporal organization: reflections of a

Darwinian clock-watcher. Annu Rev Physiol 55:17–54.

Reppert SM, Weaver DR (2002) Coordination of circadian timing in

mammals. Nature 418:935–941.

Stephan FK (2002) The ‘‘other’’ circadian system: food as a

Zeitgeber. J Biol Rhythms 17:284–292.

Stephan FK, Swann JM, Sisk CL (1979) Entrainment of circadian

rhythms by feeding schedules in rats with suprachiasmatic

lesions. Behav Neural Biol 25:545–554.

Stokkan KA, Yamazaki S, Tei H, Sasakki Y, Menaker M (2001)

Entrainment of the circadian clock in the liver by feeding. Science

291:490–493.

van der Vinne V, Riede SJ, Gorter JA, Eijer WG, Sellix MT, Menaker

M, Daan S, Pilorz V, Hut RA (2014) Cold and hunger induce

diurnality in a nocturnal mammal. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

111:15256–15260.

Verwey M, Amir S (2009) Food-entrainable circadian oscillators in the

brain. Eur J Neurosci 30:1650–1657.

Verwey M, Khoja Z, Stewart J, Amir S (2007) Differential regulation of

the expression of Period2 protein in the limbic forebrain and

dorsomedial hypothalamus by daily limited access to highly

palatable food in food-deprived and free-fed rats. Neuroscience

147:277–285.

Waddington Lamont E, Harbour VL, Barry-Shaw J, Renteria Diaz L,

Robinson B, Stewart J, Amir S (2007) Restricted access to food,

but not sucrose, saccharine, or salt, synchronizes the expression

of Period2 protein in the limbic forebrain. Neuroscience

144:402–411.

(Accepted 21 July 2015)(Available online xxxx)

a timing component does not alter the timing of activity under positive

.2015.07.061