5
  http://cdp.sagepub.com/ Science Current Directions in Psychological  http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/9/4/115 The online version of this article can be foun d at:  DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.00073  2000 9: 115 Current Directions in Psychological Science Susan Folkman and Judith Tedlie Moskowitz Stress, Positive Emotion, and Coping  Published by:  http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of:  Association for Psychological Science  can be found at: Current Directions in Psychological Science Additional services and information for http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions:  http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: What is This?  - Aug 1, 2000 Version of Record >> at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014 cdp.sagepub.com Downloaded from 

Folk Man 2000

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

.

Citation preview

  • http://cdp.sagepub.com/Science

    Current Directions in Psychological

    http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/9/4/115The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.00073 2000 9: 115Current Directions in Psychological Science

    Susan Folkman and Judith Tedlie MoskowitzStress, Positive Emotion, and Coping

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    Association for Psychological Science

    can be found at:Current Directions in Psychological ScienceAdditional services and information for

    http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://cdp.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    What is This?

    - Aug 1, 2000Version of Record >>

    at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • AbstractThere is growing interest in

    positive aspects of the stressprocess, including positiveoutcomes of stress and ante-cedents that dispose individu-als to appraise stressful situa-tions more as a challenge thanas a threat. Less attention hasbeen given to the adaptationalsignificance of positive emo-tions during stress or to thecoping processes that sustainpositive emotions. We reviewevidence for the occurrence ofpositive emotions under con-ditions of stress, discuss thefunctional role that positiveemotions play under such con-ditions, and present three typesof coping that are associatedwith positive emotion duringchronic stress. These findingspoint to new research ques-tions about the role of positiveemotions during stress and thenature of the coping processesthat generate these positiveemotions.

    Keywordscoping; positive emotion; chron-ic stress

    Decades of research have shownthat stress is associated with a widearray of negative outcomes, such asdepression, anxiety, physical symp-toms, disease, and even death inextreme cases. In recent years,however, there has been a growinginterest in positive aspects of thestress process, including positiveoutcomes such as personal trans-formation or growth (for review,

    see Tedeschi, Park, & Calhoun,1998) and antecedents that disposeindividuals to appraise situationsmore as a challenge than as athreat. With the exception of a fewinvestigators such as Affleck andTennen (1996), however, research-ers have not given much attentionto the actual coping mechanismsthat link the positive dispositions,on the one hand, and the positiveoutcomes of stressful situations, onthe other.

    The growing interest in positiveaspects of the stress process is par-alleled by a growing interest inpositive emotions and, of particu-lar relevance here, the possibilitythat they may have important ad-aptational significance during thestress process. Fredrickson (1998),for example, cited evidence thatpositive emotions broaden thescope of attention, cognition, andaction, and help build physical, in-tellectual, and social resources. HerBroaden and Build Model of Posi-tive Emotions, which is premisedon this evidence, raises the possi-bility that positive emotions areimportant facilitators of adaptivecoping and adjustment to acuteand chronic stress and may under-lie the beneficial effects of inter-ventions such as relaxation thera-pies (Fredrickson, 2000). Twentyyears ago, we (Lazarus, Kanner, &Folkman, 1980) suggested thatpositive emotions may have threeimportant adaptive functions dur-ing stress: sustaining coping ef-forts, providing a breather, andrestoring depleted resources. How-ever, until recently, there has beenlittle effort to provide empiricalsupport for these ideas.

    The idea that people even expe-rience positive emotions in themidst of acute or chronic stressmay at first seem counterintuitive.But people do experience theseemotions, even under the most dif-ficult of circumstances. For ex-ample, when we monitored gaymen who were the primary infor-mal caregivers of partners withAIDS for up to 5 years, the caregiv-ers reported levels of depressedmood that were more than onestandard deviation above levels inthe general population, and in-creased to more than two standarddeviations above the general popu-lations levels during periods of cri-sis. Throughout the study, with theexception of the weeks immedi-ately preceding and following theirpartners deaths, however, the care-givers also reported experiencingpositive mood at a frequency com-parable to the frequency of theirnegative mood (Folkman, 1997).

    COPING AND POSITIVEEMOTION

    Given data demonstrating thatpositive emotions occur even un-der the most dire of circumstances,the compelling question becomesnot whether people experiencepositive emotions during long pe-riods of severe stress, but rather,how they do it. In our study of care-givers, we identified three classesof coping mechanisms that help an-swer this question: positive reap-praisal, problem-focused coping,and the creation of positive events(Folkman, 1997; Folkman & Mosk-owitz, in press).

    Positive Reappraisal

    Positive reappraisal is a cogni-tive process through which peoplefocus on the good in what is hap-pening or what has happened.

    Stress, Positive Emotion, and CopingSusan Folkman1 and Judith Tedlie MoskowitzCenter for AIDS Prevention Studies, University of California-San Francisco,San Francisco, California

    115CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

    Copyright 2000 American Psychological Society at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • Forms of positive reappraisal in-clude discovering opportunities forpersonal growth, perceiving actualpersonal growth, and seeing howones own efforts can benefit otherpeople. Through the process ofpositive reappraisal, the meaningof a situation is changed in a waythat allows the person to experi-ence positive emotion and psycho-logical well-being. In our study ofAIDS-related caregiving, we foundthat positive reappraisal was con-sistently associated with positiveemotion both during caregivingand after the death of the partner.This association remained signifi-cant even when emotion at the pre-vious interview and the other typesof coping were statistically con-trolled (Moskowitz, Folkman, Col-lette, & Vittinghoff, 1996).

    Not all forms of positive reap-praisal necessarily generate posi-tive emotion. For example, a cogni-tive reappraisal through which anindividual devalues an importantgoal that has proven to be unreal-istic, such as gaining admission to aprestigious Ivy League school, maybe positive in that it lessens thepersonal significance of failing toachieve the goal, but it may domore to reduce distress than to en-hance positive emotion.

    Problem-Focused Coping

    Problem-focused coping in-cludes thoughts and instrumentalbehaviors that manage or solve theunderlying cause of distress. Ittends to be used more in situationsin which there is personal controlover an outcome and less in situa-tions in which there is an absenceof personal control. Problem-focused coping is usually consid-ered maladaptive when there is nopersonal control (Lazarus & Folk-man, 1984), but this general for-mula may be overly simplified; asituation that appears on its surfaceto be uncontrollable may still havecontrollable aspects.

    In our research, for example, wefound a significant increase inproblem-focused coping by care-givers during the weeks leading upto the partners death, a period ofprofound lack of control. A reviewof narrative data showed that dur-ing this period caregivers wereoften creating the proverbial to-dolist, usually comprising seeminglymundane tasks such as getting aprescription filled, successfully ad-ministering a medication, or chang-ing the partners bed linens. Suchlists served multiple purposes: Inattending to even the most trivialtask, the caregiver had an opportu-nity to feel effective and in control,thereby helping to combat the feel-ings of helplessness and lack ofcontrol that often characterized theoverall situation; working on taskshelped the caregiver feel mobilizedand focused, which was energiz-ing; and the successful accomplish-ment of the various tasks was oftenhelpful, in which case the caregiveroften benefited from positive feed-back from his partner or otherpeople involved in the partnerscare. An important finding wasthat this type of coping was re-sponsible for increases in positivemood (as distinct from decreasesin distress) during these weeks(Moskowitz et al., 1996).

    Creation of Positive Events

    This coping mechanism involvescreating a positive psychologicaltime-out by infusing ordinaryevents with positive meaning (Folk-man, Moskowitz, Ozer, & Park,1997), as when a person reflects ona compliment that was offered inpassing, or pauses to take note of abeautiful sunset. Such time-outsprovide momentary respite fromthe ongoing stress. In our study ofcaregivers, we were struck by thepervasiveness of this method ofcoping throughout caregiving andbereavement. Month after month,

    for example, more than 99% of thecaregivers noted and rememberedpositive events in the midst ofsome of the most psychologicallystressful circumstances people en-counter. For the most part, theseevents were ordinary events ofdaily life that in less stressful mo-ments might not even have beennoted. We believe the occurrence ofthese positive events was not ran-dom. Rather, caregivers createdthem deliberately by planningpositive events, noting positiveevents when they occurred seren-dipitously, or infusing neutralevents with positive meaning, as away of having a few moments ofrelief from the intense stress.

    Sometimes the creation of thesemeaningful events depended onthe caregivers ability to find hu-mor in the situation. Humor, whichhas long been recognized for itstension-reducing properties (e.g.,Menninger, 1963), has the addedbenefit of generating positive emo-tion in the very darkest of mo-ments, which may, in turn, helpbuild social bonds that can be ben-eficial under conditions of stress.The term gallows humor attests tothe widespread use of humor insituations that are particularly grim.Humor was common in the ac-counts provided by caregivers,even the accounts that describedpartners deaths. The humor usu-ally managed to capture positivequalities of the dying partner in aloving manner, which had the ad-ditional benefit of helping the care-giver create images of the partnerthat he wanted to remember.

    COMPELLINGUNANSWEREDQUESTIONS

    Research on coping and positiveemotions is still in its earlieststages, and each new finding raises

    116 VOLUME 9, NUMBER 4, AUGUST 2000

    Published by Blackwell Publishers Inc. at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • new questions. For example, re-searchers have only begun to un-derstand the adaptive functions ofpositive emotions in the midst ofstressful circumstances. Laboratorystudies have provided provocativesuggestions regarding the wayspositive emotions may help peopleendure stress (e.g., Fredrickson &Levenson, 1998). But because con-straints of the laboratory limit re-searchers ability to simulate themeaning or duration of seriousreal-life stressors, we strongly en-courage pursuing research underreal-life circumstances, with alltheir complexity. In our newlylaunched study of maternal care-givers, for example, we are investi-gating positive emotions and rel-evant coping processes in the dailylives of women providing care for achild with HIV or other seriouschronic illness.

    There are several issues regard-ing measurement and conceptual-ization of emotion that need tobe addressed in order to advanceknowledge of the role of positiveemotion in the stress process. Atthis point, there is little in the lit-erature about the intensity andduration of positive emotions nec-essary to activate their adaptivefunctions during stress. In ourstudy of AIDS-related caregiving,the quantitative measures assessedthe frequency of emotions, nottheir duration or intensity. Butqualitative data suggested thatpositive emotions were less intenseand less enduring than negativeemotions (Folkman et al., 1997). Itmay be that it is the frequency ofpositive emotion, and not its inten-sity or duration, that confers ben-efits on the individual.

    A related issue has to do withwhether different positive emo-tions are differentially effectivewith respect to motivating and sus-taining coping, and if so, underwhat circumstances. For example,is excitement more adaptive thanhappiness at the outset of a stress-

    ful undertaking, but less so whilethe situation unfolds?

    Answering these questions re-garding intensity, duration, anddifferential effects of positive emo-tions will require close attention tothe measurement and conceptual-ization of emotion. Recent debatesregarding whether positive andnegative emotion are independentor bipolar constructs (i.e., oppositeextremes of the same construct)have reignited interest in these is-sues (see Russell & Carroll, 1999,for a review). As Russell and Car-roll (1999) noted, when all the mea-surement issues are considered, abipolar model fits the data best. Bymaking the assertion that bothpositive and negative emotion oc-cur in chronically stressful situa-tions, we are not implying thatthey are independent and that atany given moment high levels ofnegative emotion co-occur withhigh levels of positive emotion.Rather, our point is that over a pe-riod of time, people in stressfulsituations experience not only neg-ative emotion, but also positiveemotion. Thus, in the case of ourcaregiver study, when participantsreported their emotions over theprevious week, as expected, theyindicated that they experienced fre-quent negative affect, but they alsoexperienced fairly frequent posi-tive affect.

    The coping processes that are as-sociated with positive emotion in-volve another set of questions. Arethe coping processes that generatepositive emotion truly differentfrom those that regulate distress? Ifso, how are they different? For ex-ample, many of the positive reap-praisal processes that generatepositive emotion depend on the in-dividuals ability to access deeplyheld values that enhance the per-sonal significance of ongoing cop-ing activity. In contrast, copingprocesses that people are morelikely to use to regulate distress(such as escape-avoidance or dis-

    tancing), or even strategies that areintended to reduce tension (such asrelaxation or meditation), do notseem to depend on accessing val-ues in the same way. Further workthat specifically addresses the dis-tinction between coping processesthat are associated with positive asopposed to negative emotions isclearly needed. If the ways of cop-ing that decrease negative emotiondiffer from those that increase posi-tive emotion, it may be necessaryfor researchers to expand the rep-ertoire of coping measures in orderto more fully tap into these positiveways of coping.

    Another set of questions con-cerns the importance of the dura-tion of stress, and whether thecapacity or need for positive emo-tions differs in acute versus chronicstressful situations. Does the nov-elty, immediacy, or urgency of thedemands of an acute stressful situ-ation reduce the persons capac-ity for engaging in the copingstrategies that generate positiveemotions? Or perhaps it is not asimportant to generate positiveemotions during an acute, time-limited situation as it is when astressful situation persists overtime, and the person needs to haverespite from distress to become re-dedicated to the coping efforts inorder to keep going. Studies di-rectly comparing coping withacute, relatively short-lived stress-ors and coping with chronic stress-ors are necessary to answer thisquestion.

    Finally, to what extent can peoplebe taught to generate positive emo-tions while they are also regulatingdistress? Is the ability to generatepositive emotions attached to un-derlying personality dimensions,such as optimism, that may be rela-tively immutable? Or are thereteachable skills that are indepen-dent of the underlying personalitydimensions? Lewinsohn and hiscolleagues recognized the impor-tance of pleasant events (and the

    117CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

    Copyright 2000 American Psychological Society at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

  • associated positive emotions) inthe treatment of depression yearsago (e.g., Lewinsohn, Sullivan, &Grosscup, 1980). More recent workhas pursued the idea that helpingclients identify thoughts and be-liefs that interfere with positive ex-periences is an important compo-nent of therapy (Fava, Rafanelli,Cazzaro, Conti, & Grandi, 1998).

    We have highlighted some ofthe exciting new developments inthe areas of stress, positive emo-tions, and coping. These develop-ments point the way for systematic,programmatic research that mayhelp explain the fascinating, funda-mental question, namely, why it isthat some people not only surviveadversity mentally and physically,but manage somehow even tothrive.

    Recommended Reading

    Folkman, S. (1997). (See References)Fredrickson, B.L. (1998). (See Refer-

    ences)

    Tedeschi, R.G., Park, C.L., & Cal-houn, L.G. (Eds.). (1998). (SeeReferences)

    AcknowledgmentsThis research wassupported by Grants 49985 and 52517from the National Institute of MentalHealth, and by Grant 58069 from the Na-tional Institute of Mental Health and theNational Institute of Nursing Research.

    Note

    1. Address correspondence to SusanFolkman, 74 New Montgomery, Suite600, San Francisco, CA 94105; e-mail:[email protected].

    References

    Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1996). Construing ben-efits from adversity: Adaptational significanceand dispositional underpinnings. Journal ofPersonality, 64, 899922.

    Fava, G.A., Rafanelli, C., Cazzaro, M., Conti, S., &Grandi, S. (1998). Well-being therapy: A novelpsychotherapeutic approach for residualsymptoms of affective disorders. PsychologicalMedicine, 28, 475480.

    Folkman, S. (1997). Positive psychological statesand coping with severe stress. Social Scienceand Medicine, 45, 12071221.

    Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J.T. (in press). Positiveaffect and the other side of coping. AmericanPsychologist.

    Folkman, S., Moskowitz, J.T., Ozer, E.M., & Park,C.L. (1997). Positive meaningful events andcoping in the context of HIV/AIDS. In B.H.Gottlieb (Ed.), Coping with chronic stress (pp.293314). New York: Plenum Press.

    Fredrickson, B.L. (1998). What good are positiveemotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300319.

    Fredrickson, B.L. (2000). Cultivating positive emo-tions to optimize health and well-being. Pre-vention and Treatment [On-line], 3. Available:http://journals.apa.org.prevention

    Fredrickson, B.L., & Levenson, R.W. (1998). Posi-tive emotions speed recovery from the cardio-vascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cogni-tion and Emotion, 12, 191220.

    Lazarus, R.S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, ap-praisal, and coping. New York: Springer.

    Lazarus, R.S., Kanner, A.D., & Folkman, S. (1980).Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenologicalanalysis. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.),Theories of emotion (pp. 189217). New York:Academic Press.

    Lewinsohn, P.M., Sullivan, J.M., & Grosscup, S.J.(1980). Changing reinforcing events: An ap-proach to the treatment of depression. Psycho-therapy: Theory, Research, and Practice, 17, 322334.

    Menninger, K. (1963). The vital balance: The life pro-cess in mental health and illness. New York: Vi-king.

    Moskowitz, J.T., Folkman, S., Collette, L., & Vit-tinghoff, E. (1996). Coping and mood duringAIDS-related caregiving and bereavement.Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 18, 4957.

    Russell, J.A., & Carroll, J.M. (1999). On the bipo-larity of positive and negative affect. Psycho-logical Bulletin, 125, 330.

    Tedeschi, R.G., Park, C.L., & Calhoun, L.G. (Eds.).(1998). Posttraumatic growth. Mahwah, NJ: Erl-baum.

    118 VOLUME 9, NUMBER 4, AUGUST 2000

    Published by Blackwell Publishers Inc. at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on August 29, 2014cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    /ColorImageDict > /JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict > /JPEG2000ColorImageDict > /AntiAliasGrayImages false /CropGrayImages true /GrayImageMinResolution 300 /GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleGrayImages true /GrayImageDownsampleType /Average /GrayImageResolution 150 /GrayImageDepth -1 /GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2 /GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000 /EncodeGrayImages true /GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterGrayImages true /GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /GrayACSImageDict > /GrayImageDict > /JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict > /JPEG2000GrayImageDict > /AntiAliasMonoImages false /CropMonoImages true /MonoImageMinResolution 1200 /MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleMonoImages true /MonoImageDownsampleType /Average /MonoImageResolution 300 /MonoImageDepth -1 /MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50000 /EncodeMonoImages true /MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode /MonoImageDict > /AllowPSXObjects false /CheckCompliance [ /None ] /PDFX1aCheck false /PDFX3Check false /PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false /PDFXNoTrimBoxError true /PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox true /PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfile (None) /PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier () /PDFXOutputCondition () /PDFXRegistryName () /PDFXTrapped /False

    /CreateJDFFile false /Description > /Namespace [ (Adobe) (Common) (1.0) ] /OtherNamespaces [ > /FormElements false /GenerateStructure false /IncludeBookmarks false /IncludeHyperlinks false /IncludeInteractive false /IncludeLayers false /IncludeProfiles false /MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings /Namespace [ (Adobe) (CreativeSuite) (2.0) ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK /PreserveEditing true /UntaggedCMYKHandling /LeaveUntagged /UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UseDocumentBleed false >> ]>> setdistillerparams> setpagedevice