37
Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 1 monkeys 2 Svenja Wulff 1,2 , Annalisa Bosco 3 , Katharina Havermann 1,2 , Giacomo Placenti 3 , Patrizia 3 Fattori 3 , Markus Lappe 1,2 4 1) Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany 5 2) Otto Creutzfeld Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of 6 Muenster, Muenster, Germany 7 3) Department of Human and General Physiology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy 8 9 10 11 running head: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaques 12 contact information: 13 Svenja Wulff 14 Department of Psychology 15 Fliednerstr. 21 16 48149 Muenster 17 Germany 18 Tel: +49-251-8334177 19 e-mail: [email protected] 20 21 22 23 24 25 Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (August 29, 2012). doi:10.1152/jn.00212.2012 Copyright © 2012 by the American Physiological Society.

Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 1

monkeys 2

Svenja Wulff1,2, Annalisa Bosco3, Katharina Havermann1,2, Giacomo Placenti3, Patrizia 3

Fattori3, Markus Lappe1,2 4

1) Department of Psychology, University of Muenster, Muenster, Germany 5

2) Otto Creutzfeld Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of 6

Muenster, Muenster, Germany 7

3) Department of Human and General Physiology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy 8 9 10 11

running head: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaques 12

contact information: 13

Svenja Wulff 14 Department of Psychology 15 Fliednerstr. 21 16 48149 Muenster 17 Germany 18 Tel: +49-251-8334177 19 e-mail: [email protected] 20 21 22 23 24 25

Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (August 29, 2012). doi:10.1152/jn.00212.2012

Copyright © 2012 by the American Physiological Society.

Page 2: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Abstract 26

The saccadic amplitude of humans and monkeys can be adapted using intra-saccadic 27

target steps in the McLaughlin paradigm. It is generally believed that, as a result of a 28

purely retinal reference frame, after adaptation of a saccade of a certain amplitude and 29

direction, saccades of the same amplitude and direction are all adapted to the same 30

extent, independently from the initial eye position. However, recent studies in humans 31

have put the pure retinal coding in doubt by revealing that the initial eye position has an 32

effect on the transfer of adaptation to saccades of different starting points. Since humans 33

and monkeys show some species differences in adaptation, we tested the eye position 34

dependence in monkeys. Two trained Macaca fascicularis performed reactive rightward 35

saccades from five equally horizontally distributed starting positions. All saccades were 36

made to targets with the same retinotopic motor vector. In each session the saccades 37

which started at one particular initial eye position – the adaptation position - were 38

adapted to shorter amplitude, and the adaptation of the saccades starting at the other four 39

positions was measured. The results show that saccades, which started at the other 40

positions, were less adapted than saccades which started at the adaptation position. With 41

increasing distance between the starting position of the test saccade and the adaptation 42

position, the amplitude change of the test saccades decreased with a Gaussian profile. 43

We conclude that gain-decreasing saccadic adaptation in macaques is specific to the 44

Page 3: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 45

1 Introduction 46

For an active exploration of the ambient scene, primates make rapid eye movements 47

(saccades) which shift the direction of gaze from one target of interest to another. 48

Saccades are so brief that no visual feedback is available during the saccade. Because of 49

latencies in the visual system, are so high that feedback can be processed only after the 50

saccade is finished. Therefore, the saccadic motor command has to be prepared in 51

advance to accurately aim the fovea at a new target. Since the mechanical properties of 52

the oculomotor plant can change due to growth, injury or muscle fatigue, a fixed motor 53

command could lead to saccadic targeting errors. For this reason the saccadic amplitude 54

is continuously adjusted to current requirements such that the amplitude becomes shorter 55

if the saccade consistently overshoots the target and longer if the saccade consistently 56

undershoots the target. This plasticity mechanism is called saccadic adaptation. It can be 57

mimicked in the laboratory using the McLaughlin adaptation paradigm (McLaughlin 58

1967), in which the saccade target is systematically displaced during execution of the 59

saccade. Over several trials the amplitude becomes shorter in the case that the target is 60

stepped backward and longer if the target is stepped forward along the direction of the 61

saccade. In humans saccadic adaptation is achieved in a few tens of trials (Albano 62

1996,Deubel 1987, Frens and Van Opstal 1994) whereas in monkeys a few hundred trials 63

Page 4: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

are needed (Deubel 1987, Straube et al. 1997). This indicates that the adaptive 64

mechanisms differ between humans and monkeys. 65

Our study is concerned with the reference frame of saccadic adaptation. If adaptation 66

takes place in an oculo-centric reference frame (retina referenced) it should be specific to 67

the retino-centric coordinates of the target and thus to the motor vector (i.e. direction and 68

amplitude) of the adapted saccade. Indeed, many studies found that the transfer of 69

adaptation from adapted saccades of a certain vector to saccades with different vectors is 70

incomplete, both in humans (Albano 1996, Deubel 1987, Frens and Van Opstal 1994, 71

Miller et al. 1981, Semmlow et al. 1989) and in monkeys (Deubel 1987, Noto et al. 1999, 72

Straube et al. 1997). 73

On the other hand, if saccadic adaptation takes place in an orbito-centric reference 74

frame (head referenced), it would be specific to the starting position of the saccade in 75

head-centric coordinates. In other words, the initial eye position of the saccade, i.e. the 76

eye position in the orbit, would influence the adaptation state. Early studies that 77

investigated the impact of the initial eye position on the transfer of adaptation indicated 78

nearly complete transfer of adaptation from an adapted saccade to saccades with the 79

same vector but different starting position in humans (Albano 1996, Frens and 80

Van Opstal 1994, Semmlow et al. 1989) and in monkeys (Noto et al. 1999). Thus, 81

adaptation was considered to be unspecific to the initial eye position. In consequence, the 82

plastic modulations to the visuomotor system were assumed to be coded in a purely 83

Page 5: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

retinal reference frame. 84

More recent studies, however, have demonstrated that interleaved amplitude 85

adaptation in opposite directions at different positions in space, called differential 86

adaptation, is possible in humans (Alahyane and Pelisson 2004, Shelhamer and 87

Clendaniel 2002) and monkeys (Tian and Zee 2010). This suggests that eye position 88

information, i.e. a signal representing the position of the eye in the orbit is available to 89

the saccadic adaptation mechanism. To explain this discrepancy, some authors have 90

suggested that the default of the adaptation system is to generalize the adaptation to the 91

complete saccadic operating range (i.e. all starting positions) but that in situations that 92

demand independent control at different starting positions (like in differential adaptation) 93

the eye position information is used (Hopp and Fuchs 2004, Pelisson et al. 2010). Hence, 94

the adaptation would only be specific to the initial eye position if at least two conflicting 95

modifications are applied simultaneously and the eye position signal would remain 96

unused in the normal case. However, if saccadic adaptation, for example, is needed to 97

compensate for position dependent dysmetria produced by a single paretic eye muscle, 98

the grade of required change of amplitude depends on the orbital eye position and the 99

adaptation would need to be eye position specific. 100

In fact, there have been recent studies revealing eye position specificity of saccadic 101

adaptation in humans without the differential adaptation paradigm (Havermann et al. 102

2011, Zimmermann and Lappe 2011, Zimmermann et al. 2011). For example, in the 103

Page 6: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

study of Havermann et al. (2011) subjects performed reactive saccades started at five 104

equally horizontally distributed starting positions along the horizontal median. From 105

these different initial positions saccades of a fixed vector were made. Thus, the targets all 106

had the same retino-centric coordinates when the subject was fixating the corresponding 107

fixation point. In each session the saccades starting from one selected initial eye position 108

were adapted using the McLaughlin adaptation paradigm and then the adaptation of the 109

saccades starting at the other four positions was measured. The adaptation magnitude in 110

the test saccades was found to be a linear function of the distance between the start 111

position of the test saccades and the start position of the adapted saccades. Thus, the 112

induced adaptation was not uniformly transferred to all starting position. 113

In the current study, we perform a similar experiment with macaque monkeys. In 114

each session the saccades starting in one initial eye position were adapted and the 115

adaptation of the saccades starting at four other positions was measured. We found that 116

the adaptation state was reduced at positions that are different from the adapted initial 117

eye position. Additionally, the adaptation at different test positions followed a Gaussian 118

function of the distance to the adaptation position. With these findings we confirm that 119

an eye position signal is employed in saccadic adaptation and we demonstrate that 120

adaptation of reactive saccades in monkeys is not simply generalized over the complete 121

saccadic operating range to all initial eye positions but that it is eye position specific. 122

2 Materials and Methods 123

Page 7: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Experiments were approved by the Bioethical Committee of the University of Bologna 124

and were performed in accordance with national laws on care and use of laboratory 125

animals and with the European Communities Council Directive of 24th November 126

1986(86/609/EEC), recently revised by the Council of Europe guidelines (Appendix A of 127

Convention ETS 123). The head-restraint system on the head of the trained Macaca 128

fascicularis was surgically implanted in asepsis and under general anesthesia (sodium 129

thiopental, 8 mg/Kg/h, i.v.) following the procedures reported in Galletti et al. (1995). 130

Adequate measures were taken to minimize pain or discomfort. A full program of 131

postoperative analgesia (ketorolac trometazyn, 1 mg/Kg i.m. immediately after surgery, 132

and 1.6 mg/Kg i.m. on the following days) and antibiotic care [Ritardomicina (benzatinic 133

benzylpenicillin plus dihydrostreptomycin plus streptomycin) 1-1.5 ml/10 kg every 5-6 134

days] followed the surgery. 135

2.1 Recording of eye movements and stimulus presentation 136

During the recording sessions, signals from both eyes were recorded simultaneously with 137

an infrared oculometer (ISCAN, Inc) at a sampling rate of 100 Hz. Before each 138

experimental session, the monkey was required to perform a calibration task that allowed 139

us to calibrate the signals from each eye separately. In this task, the monkey fixated 140

sequentially ten light emitting diodes (LEDs) that were mounted on a frontoparallel 141

panel at a distance of 15 cm from the eyes. In front of each eye, there were five LEDs in 142

Page 8: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

a cross arrangement with the central one being aligned with the eye’s primary position. 143

The four peripheral LEDs were located +/- 15 deg left and right and below and above of 144

the central one. Calibration factors for each eye were extracted from the eye traces 145

recorded in the calibration task. 146

During a recording session the monkey sat in a primate chair with its head restrained 147

and it faced a 17 " monitor (Acer, AL 1716 As) with a visible display size of 33,5 cm x 148

26,8 cm. The viewing distance of 32 cm from the animal’s eyes to the screen resulted in 149

a visual field of 55.3 deg x 45.4 deg. The display had a resolution of 1280 x 1024 pixels 150

and a frame rate of 60 Hz. For stimuli presentation and data analysis we used MATLAB 151

with the psychtoolbox extension (Brainard 1997). The stimuli were green and red dots 152

with a radius of 0.18 deg. 153

2.2 Behavioral task 154

In Fig. 1 the procedure of saccadic adaptation of reactive saccades is explained. The 155

sketches A) to E) show the layout of a trial in the adaptation phase of the session. During 156

the execution of the saccade in those adaptation trials the target is shifted to another 157

location and thus an error signal is induced at the end of the saccade due to the misplaced 158

saccadic landing position. Pre-adaptation trials to define a baseline of the saccadic 159

amplitude and test trials to measure the amount of adaptation in each position will be 160

explained later in detail. 161

Page 9: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

To start a new trial the monkey had to press a button near its chest, out of his visual field, 162

when the screen was all black. The button presses/releases were recorded by LABVIEW 163

with 1 ms resolution. After the button press, a green fixation point was placed at one of 164

five possible starting positions, at -12 deg, -6 deg, 0 deg, +6 deg or +12 deg horizontal 165

gaze direction (Fig. 1 F) to J)). All stimuli were presented along the screen horizontal 166

line at the animal’s eye level, that is with 0 deg vertical gaze direction. The monkey had 167

to establish and maintain fixation at this point. The monkey’s eye position was 168

monitored online by the tracker system such that the direction of gaze had to enter and 169

stay in a window of 4 deg x 4 deg centered around the fixation point. After a randomized 170

time between 1000 ms and 1500 ms the fixation point was switched off. Simultaneously, 171

a green target appeared rightwards to the fixation point. The monkeys were trained to 172

make a saccade towards the target as quickly as possible and to establish fixation at the 173

green target. During the adaptation phase in every trial the target stepped back as soon as 174

the monkey left the window centered around the fixation point. We employed slightly 175

different experimental layouts for the two monkeys because monkey B did not adapt well 176

if a target step of 5 deg was presented, which we used in the sessions of monkey A. 177

Thus, for monkey A the target was presented 22 deg rightwards from the fixation point 178

and during the saccade the target stepped back 5 deg. For monkey B a saccade of 24 deg 179

amplitude and a target back step of 2 deg were applied. It should be kept in mind that 180

different target step sizes lead to different maximal achievable adaptation states in the 181

Page 10: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

two monkeys. However, the adaptation in relation to the applied step size is expected to 182

be of comparable size. In the initial trials of the adaptation phase the saccades of the 183

monkeys landed close to the position of the first target. Due to the inward shift of the 184

target during the saccade a visual error was induced at the end of the saccade. This led to 185

saccadic adaptation and thus to a decreased amplitude of the following saccades. The 186

shifted target turned red after a randomized time between 600 ms and 1000 ms. This was 187

the signal for the monkey to release the button. If the monkey released the button within 188

a maximum time of 1000 ms, he was rewarded with a defined amount of water. In the 189

case that the monkey released the button before the turning red of the target, i. e. already 190

during the trial, or too late after the turning red, the trial was aborted, the monkey did not 191

get any reward and the screen turned black so that a new trial could be started by the 192

monkey pressing the button. Trials which were aborted were discarded from the analysis. 193

Every session consisted of 850 completed trials. The first part of each session 194

consisted of 100 so called pre-adaptation trials which did not contain a target step. The 195

pre-adaptation trials were used to measure the baseline of the saccadic amplitude in 196

every possible saccade position. Hence there were five blocks of 20 pre-adaptation trials 197

each, one block at each of the five positions. The saccadic endpoint was determined in 198

the offline analyses when the velocity of the saccade dropped under the threshold of one 199

tenth of the maximal reached velocity in that saccade. 200

Afterwards the adaptations phase started, in which 350 adaptation trials were 201

Page 11: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

performed by the monkey. During the adaptation phase all saccades were started at the 202

same starting point and all trials contained an inward target step that led to a decreased 203

amplitude. A comparison between the saccade made in the first trial and the saccade 204

made in the last trial of the adaptation phase is shown in the left panel of Fig. 2. After the 205

adaptation phase, the monkey usually had achieved a maximal amount of adaptation at 206

the adaptation position and the amplitude of the saccades did not decrease any further but 207

stayed constant. 208

After the adaptation phase ended the test phase began. It consisted of at least 20 test 209

saccades at each of the four test positions and at the adaptation position in a randomized 210

sequence. The test trials were used to measure the post-adaptation gain, see Fig. 2 right 211

panel, and to calculate the gain change in comparison to the pre-adatation trials. In a test 212

trial the target was not stepped back when the onset of the saccade had been detected but 213

instead it was switched off for 300 ms and then switched on again at the same initial 214

target position. Subsequently to the reappearance of the target it turned red after a 215

randomized time so that the monkey could fulfill its task successfully and got rewarded. 216

The target was switched off during the saccade to avoid that the monkey could see the 217

target at the end of the saccade. Hence, no visual error signal was induced in the test 218

trials. This way we tried to maintain the monkey’s adaptation as complete as possible. 219

However, to enable the monkey to accomplish the trial and to earn its reward, we needed 220

to switch the target on after 300 ms. Subsequently to the reappearance of the target, it 221

Page 12: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

turned red like in the adaptation trials. The monkey then could complete the trial 222

successfully by releasing the button. Shafer et al. showed for macaque monkeys that in 223

comparison to the conventional adaptation paradigm the achieved adaptation decreases 224

significantly if the shifted target is switched on 112 ms or 208 ms after the saccade end. 225

Nevertheless, the authors pointed out that visual errors occurring even more than 300 ms 226

after the saccade still can have an effect on saccadic gain adaptation (Shafer et al. 2000). 227

Thus, to reinforce the monkey’s adaptation during the test phase, the test trials were 228

interspersed with adaptation trials at the adaptation position. Every test trial was 229

followed by two adaptation trials. The last block of the session consisted of 100 de-230

adaptation trials to extinguish the monkey’s adaptation. 231

The whole experiment consisted of five experimental sessions, which all were 232

completed by both monkeys. Since every session consisted of 850 successful trials, each 233

monkey ran a minimum of 4,250 trials. This led to a total number of 8,500 recorded 234

successful trials. The analysis was based on the 2000 recorded successful pre-adaptation 235

and test trials. Experimental sessions were separated in time by at least 24 h between two 236

sessions to be sure that no more adaptation remained from the last session in the 237

monkeys saccadic system. In every session the saccadic amplitude was adapted at one 238

out of the five saccade positions and afterwards the amount of adaptation was tested at 239

all five positions. 240

3 Results 241

Page 13: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Fig. 3 shows the saccadic end points that were recorded in one session of monkey A. The 242

first phase of the session consists of the pre-adaptation trials at all 5 positions. These 243

trials did not contain a target step and were used to determine the baseline saccadic gain. 244

During the following adaptation phase in this session all adaptation saccades started at + 245

12 deg and the target was stepped against the saccadic direction by 5 deg. After the 246

shortening of the amplitude saturated at the end of the adaptation phase the test phase 247

started with trial 451. Test trials, to measure the post-adaptation amplitude at all 5 248

positions, were interspersed with adaptation trials that were started at the adaptation of 249

this session at +12 deg. In the end of each session 100 de-adaptation trials in which the 250

target was not stepped but kept its position were performed by the monkey. 251

For every single session we compared the adaptation induced at the adaptation position 252

to the adaptation transferred to the other positions. For that purpose, the pre-adaptation 253

trials were used to calculate an averaged baseline amplitude at each test position in one 254

session including the adaptation position. The pre-adaptation trials, like the test trials, 255

had 5 different starting positions but the target was always presented at the same retino-256

centric coordinates. Thus, the saccadic amplitude in the pre-adaptation trials did not 257

depend on the start position. Analogously, the test trials were used to calculate the 258

amplitude, i.e. the post-adaptation gain, at each of the five positions after the adaptation. 259

The amount of adaptation δA measured in a single test trial j is given as: 260

δAj = Apre,m – Apost,j 261

Page 14: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

with Apre,m being the mean pre-adaptation amplitude at that position and Apost,j the 262

post-adaptation amplitude measured in one test trial. Then the gain change achieved in 263

one test position is given by: 264

Gain change = (Apost,m) / (Apre,m) 265

Apost,m is the mean of all δAj at this position, i.e. the achieved amount of adaptation in 266

one test position. Saccades that were shorter than 20 % of the pre-adaptation amplitude 267

were discarded from the analysis. This concerned less than 2 % of all completed trials. 268

Fig. 4 shows the adaptation of the two participating monkeys in all five sessions. The 269

five panels correspond to the five experimental sessions. Each panel shows the 270

adaptation of all five test positions of one session. The position on the x-axis in every 271

panel corresponds to the initial eye position of the test-saccade in the session. The 272

outermost circle on the left in every panel depicts the adaptation of the saccades starting 273

at - 12 deg and the circle on the right depict the adaptation of the saccades starting at 274

+ 12 deg. The adaptation position of the displayed session is indicated by the filled black 275

circle. 276

The adaptation patterns show that the amplitude of the saccades in the test positions 277

are adapted, but to a lesser extent than in the saccades at the adaptation position. In 278

addition, the adaptation decreased with increasing distance of the test position to the 279

adaptation position. A two factor repeated measures ANOVA on the adaptation δA data 280

measured in all test trials with both monkeys showed a significant interaction of the two 281

Page 15: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

factors adaptation position and test position at a significance level of p < 0.05 282

(F(16,16) = 3.18, p = .01). This confirmed the existence of an eye position effect in the 283

adaptation of reactive saccades. 284

The averaged results of both monkeys are presented in Fig. 5. For each monkey the gain 285

change in every test position was normalized to the gain change that was achieved at the 286

adaptation position in the corresponding session. Thus, the data points corresponding to 287

the four test positions now directly indicate the loss of adaptation at one test position 288

compared to the adaptation that was achieved at the adaptation position. The resulting 289

adaptation patterns were fit with Gaussian functions, which are also presented in the 290

charts. The Gaussian shape of the transfer profile indicates that the transferred adaptation 291

is a symmetric function of the distance between the initial eye positions of the adaptation 292

saccade and the test saccade, no matter if the centrality of the test position in the visual 293

field increases or decreases with respect to the adaptation position. 294

We thus conclude that for reactive saccades in monkeys the amount of adaptation, which 295

is transferred from an adapted saccade with a constant retinal vector and a fixed starting 296

position to a saccade of the same retinal vector but with a different starting position, is a 297

function of the distance of the starting points of the two saccades. Thus, the initial eye 298

position of a reactive saccade affects the attained adaptation at other spatial positions. 299

Saccadic adaptation is specific to the initial eye position. 300

4 Discussion 301

Page 16: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Our study is the first study in monkeys that systematically adapted saccadic amplitude at 302

one single starting position and tested the degree to which the adaptation was transferred 303

to other starting positions. In the only other study that included transfer between eye 304

positions, monkeys adapted saccades from various intermixed starting positions in one 305

hemifield and afterwards performed saccades from similarly intermixed starting 306

positions in the other hemifield. The authors found almost complete transfer of 307

adaptation to the unadapted hemifield (Noto et al. 1999, figure 8). Tian and Zee (2010), 308

on the other hand, showed that eye position can be employed as a context cue in saccadic 309

adaptation in monkeys if differential adaptation is exerted. The seemingly opposite 310

findings of these two studies can be reconciled if one assumes that simultaneous 311

adaptation from different starting position leads to a generalization to other starting 312

positions while differential adaptation at different starting positions leads to specificity of 313

adaptation to the respective starting position (Hopp and Fuchs 2004, Pelisson et al. 314

2010). 315

In our study the monkeys adapted at only one starting position. This is a neutral 316

experimental setting that neither favors generalization nor differentiation. The results 317

show clearly that the eye position signal is part of the adaptation process. 318

4.1 Comparison with studies in humans 319

In humans, early studies saw no eye position influence (Albano 1996, Frens and 320

Page 17: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Van Opstal 1994, Semmlow et al. 1989), but later studies found eye position specificity 321

in the differential adaptation paradigm (Alahyane and Pelisson 2004, Shelhamer and 322

Clendaniel 2002). More recently, a dependence of adaptation transfer on initial eye 323

position was described. (Havermann et al. 2011, Zimmermann and Lappe 2011, 324

Zimmermann et al. 2011). 325

The results of Havermann et al. (2011) revealed a possible explanation for the 326

complete transfer of adaptation between several different initial eye positions that was 327

found in earlier studies (Albano 1996, Frens and Van Opstal 1994, Semmlow et al. 328

1989). The initial eye position influenced the transfer of adaptation strongly if the initial 329

eye position of the adapted saccade was placed in the peripheral visual field. In the case 330

of adaptation at initial eye positions in the central visual field (which the earlier studies 331

had tested) the gain change was transferred completely to peripheral, initial eye 332

positions. However, in our data, even adaptation at the central eye position transferred 333

only partially to other eye positions. Moreover, the observed transfer profile in monkeys 334

differs from that in humans. In humans, the transfer profile was linear for all adaptation 335

positions with a slope close to zero for central adaptation positions and steeper slopes for 336

more peripheral adaptation positions. In contrast, our data show a Gaussian shaped 337

transfer profile with a peak at the respective adaptation position for all 5 adaptation 338

positions. 339

Since the adaptation performance in humans and monkeys also differs in other 340

Page 18: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

aspects, it is likely that the adaptation circuitries in the two species is not identical. 341

Havermann et al. (2011) have proposed an explanation for their data in terms of eye 342

position gain fields. With modifications this explanation may also work for monkeys, as 343

detailed below. 344

4.2 A possible neural mechanism for different eye position 345

dependencies of adaptation in humans and monkeys 346

In many areas of the oculomotor pathways, such as the superior colliculus (SC), the 347

frontal eye field (FEF) or the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) neurons discharge in 348

association with a certain range of saccadic vectors, i.e. encode target information in a 349

retino-centric reference frame. However, their discharge rate is modulated by eye 350

position gain fields (Andersen and Mountcastle 1983, Zipser and Andersen 1988, 351

Campos et al. 2006, Van Opstal et al. 1995, Cassanello and Ferrera 2007). This means 352

that the activity of cells that fire before an upcoming saccade is modulated by the 353

position of the eye in the orbit. The modulation varies monotonically with the initial eye 354

position. Pouget and Sejnowski (1997) approximated the response of such a neuron by 355

the product of a Gaussian function of retinal location and a sigmoid function of eye 356

position. They proposed to use the receptive fields of such neurons as a set of nonlinear 357

basis functions for a sensorimotor transformation. Accordingly, a combination of retino-358

centric encoding with eye position modulation can form a population code that creates a 359

Page 19: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

head-centric representation. Moreover, the same population can support both retino-360

centric and head centric-representations depending on the read-out (Pouget and 361

Sejnowski 1997). 362

An eye position modulation of the cell response has also been described for some 363

single neurons in the fastigial nucleus (Fuchs et al. 1993), the NRTP (Crandall and 364

Keller 1985), area V3A (Galletti and Battaglini 1989) and area V6A (Galletti et al. 365

1995). Although such eye position modulations have not been described in the 366

cerebellum, which plays a prominent role in adaptation (Catz et al. 2008, Golla et al. 367

2008, Inaba et al. 2003, Optican and Robinson 1980), the input, which is projected to the 368

cerebellum, originates from parts of the saccadic circuitry, that commonly show eye 369

position modulation. For example, saccades evoked by microstimulation in the SC are 370

modulated by the initial eye position (Groh 2011). Hence, the signal representing the 371

initial eye position affects also directly the read-out of the SC and, consequently, the 372

input to the cerebellum. 373

Fig. 6 A) shows the input composition to the adaptive circuitry in the cerebellum 374

proposed by Havermann et al. (2011) to account for the linear transfer of adaptation to 375

other eye positions in humans. Layer I symbolizes a population of neurons with different 376

retino-centric receptive fields and eye position gain fields. All cells of this population 377

directly project to the adaptive circuitry. At each retino-centric receptive field position 378

neuronal subpopulations exist that fire more strongly for starting positions on the left 379

Page 20: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

than on the right and vice versa as a result of the gain field modulation. The 380

subpopulation which shows a strong response for saccades starting on the right side 381

contributes strongly to the saccadic drive if the initial eye positions is on the right. The 382

model then assumes that neurons with a strong response induce strong adaptation. 383

Hence, if saccades starting from the right are adapted, only the inputs from the right 384

preferring subpopulation will be adapted and saccades starting from the left will remain 385

unadapted. Fig. 6 B) shows how the linear transfer profile in humans may arise from a 386

retino-centric reference frame with such an eye position dependent modulation. 387

Fig. 6 D) shows a different composition of the input to the cerebellum. The 388

additional Layer II constructs a head-centric target representation from the collective 389

responses of Layer I (Pouget and Sejnowski 1997). Thus, for every head-centric target 390

location there is one subpopulation in Layer II, which shows a peaked response for this 391

target location (Fig. 6 E) and Fig. 6 F)). If adaptational modification is only applied for 392

active inputs into the adaptive circuitry, the adaptation state would decrease with 393

decreasing firing rate of the adapted subpopulation. Therefore, the head centric target 394

representation in Layer II explains the Gaussian shape of the adaptation transfer profile 395

that is observed in the monkey data. 396

In Fig. 6 D) the head-centric encoding is represented by a specialized Layer II of 397

head centric neurons. Such head centric neurons have been found in the parietal cortex of 398

monkeys (Galletti et al. 1993, Bremmer et al. 1998). However, following the basis 399

Page 21: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

function model of Pouget and Sejnowski 1997 such an explicit representation is not 400

always necessary. Instead, the appropriate input combination might be directly fed into 401

the adaptive circuitry, circumventing a specialized head-centric layer. 402

4.3 Trade-off between head centric and retino-centric encoding for 403

combined eye-head movements 404

The basis function representation contains the target information in multiple 405

reference frames simultaneously. Thus, the target information could be provided to the 406

adaptive circuitry in different encodings at the same time. Hence, there might also be 407

units in Layer II, which react similarly to the neurons in Layer I and thus might lead to a 408

linear transfer profile. This would allow the system to use either reference frame 409

depending on the size of the saccade. 410

In our study the monkeys performed saccades with an amplitude of 22 deg, whereas 411

in the study of Havermann et al. (2011) amplitudes of 7 deg were used. Gaze shifts are 412

usually a combination of movements of the head and the eyes (Guitton 1992, Freedman 413

and Sparks 1997, Stahl 1999). The head movement amplitude in such an eye-head 414

saccade is related to the deflection angle of the eye that would result if the head was not 415

participating in the movement. Gaze shifts ending in a central region of the visual field 416

do not involve any head movements. Hence, it is sufficient and economical that these 417

shifts are coded retino-centrically with an additional considered eye position signal 418

instead of a head centered encoding. In the case of larger gaze shifts the participation of 419

Page 22: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

the head movement in the shift requires the coding of the gaze movement in a head 420

centric reference frame. Since our setup employs larger amplitudes than the setup of 421

Havermann et al. (2011), higher deflection angles of the eye would occur in the case of 422

pure eye saccades what leads to a larger contribution of the head to the total gaze shift. 423

Therefore, the gaze shift needs to be expressed in head-centric coordinates. The input 424

provided by units with a head centric receptive field to the adaptive circuitry could gain a 425

higher weight than the input from units with retino-centric receptive fields. A peaked 426

adaptation transfer profile would be the consequence, like the Gaussian shaped profile 427

that we found in the monkey data. In contrast, the amplitude size employed by 428

Havermann et al. (2011) does not demand a head centered coding since only eye 429

movements are expected to take part in the gaze shift. Therefore, a higher weight would 430

be given to the input provided by units with retino-centric receptive fields and eye 431

position modulation. In agreement with the results of their human studies, this leads to 432

the flat adaptation transfer profiles across different initial eye positions. 433

To conclude, we have shown that saccadic adaptation in monkeys is eye position 434

specific. The specificity can be explained by employing cells with retino-centric 435

receptive fields and an eye position modulation. The peaked adaptation transfer profile 436

differs from the previously found linear transfer profile in humans. This difference might 437

be due to differences in the adaptive circuitry between the species or it can be caused by 438

amplitude dependent selection of one of several simultaneously provided representations 439

Page 23: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

of the target position in different reference frames. 440

5 Acknowledgements 441

M. Lappe is supported by the German Science Foundation DFG LA-962/3, the German 442

Federal Ministry of Education and Research project Visuo-spatial Cognition and the EC 443

Project FP7-ICT-217077-EYESHOTS. 444

P. Fattori is supported by the Fondazione del Monte di Bologna e Ravenna, MIUR, and 445

Research project Visuo-spatial Cognition and the EC Project FP7-ICT-217077-446

EYESHOTS. 447

448

449

450

451

452

453

454

455

456

457

Page 24: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

References 458

Alahyane, N. and Pelisson, D. Eye position specificity of saccadic adaptation. Invest Ophthalmol. Vis Sci 459

45(1):123–130, 2004. 460

Albano, J. E. Adaptive changes in saccade amplitude: oculocentric or orbitocentric mapping? Vis Res 461

36(14):2087–2098, 1996. 462

Andersen, R. and Mountcastle, V. B. The influence of the angle of gaze upon the excitability of the light-463

sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 3(3):532–548, 1983. 464

Brainard, D. H. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10(4):433–436, 1997. 465

Bremmer, F., Kubischik, M., Pekel, M., and Lappe, M. Selectivity for heading direction during 466

simulated eye-movements in macaque extrastriate cortex. Eur J Neurosci 11(suppl.):244, 1998. 467

Campos, M., Cherian, A., and Segraves, M. A. Effects of eye position upon activity of neurons in 468

macaque superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 95(1):505–526, 2006. 469

Cassanello, C. R. and Ferrera, V. P. Computing vector differences using a gain field-like mechanism in 470

monkey frontal eye field. J Physiol 582(Pt 2):647–664, 2007. 471

Catz, N., Dicke, P. W., and Thier, P. Cerebellar-dependent motor learning is based on pruning a purkinje 472

cell population response. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 105(20):7309 –7314, 2008. 473

Crandall, W. F. and Keller, E. L. Visual and oculomotor signals in nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis in 474

alert monkey. J Neurophysiol 54(5):1326–1345, 1985. 475

Deubel, H. Adaptivity of gain and direction in oblique saccades. In O’Regan, J. K. and Levy-Schoen, A., 476

editors, Eye movements: From physiology to cognition, pages 181–191. Elsevier Science Publishers 477

B.V., New York, Amsterdam, 1987. 478

Edelman, J. A. and Goldberg, M. E. Effect of short-term saccadic adaptation on saccades evoked by 479

electrical stimulation in the primate superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 87(4):1915–23, 2002. 480

Page 25: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Freedman, E. G. and Sparks, D. L. Activity of cells in the deeper layers of the superior colliculus of the 481

rhesus monkey: evidence for a gaze displacement command. J Neurophysiol 78(3):1669–90, 1997. 482

Frens, M. A. and Van Opstal, A. J. Transfer of short-term adaptation in human saccadic eye movements. 483

Exp Brain Res 100(2):293–306, 1994. 484

Frens, M. A. and Van Opstal, A. J. Monkey superior colliculus activity during short-term saccadic 485

adaptation. Brain Res Bull 43(5):473–483, 1997. 486

Fuchs, A. F., Robinson, F. R., and Straube, A. Role of the caudal fastigial nucleus in saccade generation. 487

I. neuronal discharge patterns. J Neurophysiol 70(5):1723–1740, 1993. 488

Galletti, C. and Battaglini, P. P. Gaze-dependent visual neurons in area V3a of monkey prestriate cortex. 489

J Neurosci 9(4):1112–1125, 1989. 490

Galletti, C., Battaglini, P. P., and Fattori, P. Parietal neurons encoding spatial location in craniotopic 491

coordinates. Exp Brain Res 96:221–229, 1993. 492

Galletti, C., Battaglini, P. P., and Fattori, P. Eye position influence on the parieto-occipital area PO (V6) 493

of the macaque monkey. Eur J Neurosci 7(12):2486–2501, 1995. 494

Golla, H., Tzidris, K., Haarmeier, T., Catz, N., Barash, S., and Thier, P. Reduced saccadic resilience 495

and impaired saccadic adaptation due to cerebellar disease. Eur J Neurosci 27(1):132–144, 2008. 496

Groh, J. Effects of Initial Eye Position on Saccades Evoked by Microstimulation in the Primate Superior 497

Colliculus: Implications for Models of the SC Read-Out Process. Front Integr Neurosci 4:130, 2010. 498

Guitton, D. Control of eye-head coordination during orienting gaze shifts. Trends Neurosci 15:174–179, 499

1992. 500

Havermann, K., Zimmermann, E., and Lappe, M. Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation. J 501

Neurophysiol 106(5):2536–2545, 2011. 502

Hopp, J. J. and Fuchs, A. F. The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement 503

plasticity. Prog Neurobiol 72(1):27–53, 2004. 504

Page 26: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Inaba, N., Iwamoto, Y., and Yoshida, K. Changes in cerebellar fastigial burst activity related to saccadic 505

gain adaptation in the monkey. Neurosci Res 46(3):359–368, 2003. 506

McLaughlin, S. Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements. Percept Psychophys 2(8):359–362, 507

1967. 508

Miller, J. M., Anstis, T., and Templeton, W. B. Saccadic plasticity: Parametric adaptive control by 509

retinal feedback. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 7(2):356–366, 1981. 510

Noto, C. T., Watanabe, S., and Fuchs, A. F. Characteristics of simian adaptation fields produced by 511

behavioral changes in saccade size and direction. J Neurophysiol 81(6):2789–2813, 1999. 512

Optican, L. M. and Robinson, D. A. Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of primate saccadic system. J 513

Neurophysiol 44(6):1058–1076, 1980. 514

Pelisson, D., Alahyane, N., Panouilleres, M., and Tilikete, C. Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye 515

movements. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34(8):1103–1120, 2010. 516

Pouget, A. and Sejnowski, T. J. Spatial transformations in the parietal cortex using basis functions. J Cog 517

Neurosci 9(2):222–237, 1997. 518

Semmlow, J. L., Gauthier, G. M., and Vercher, J. L. Mechanisms of short-term saccadic adaptation. J 519

Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15(2):249–258, 1989. 520

Shafer, J. L., Noto, C. T., and Fuchs, A. F. Temporal characteristics of error signals driving saccadic 521

gain adaptation in the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 84:88–95, 2000. 522

Shelhamer, M. and Clendaniel, R. A. Context-specific adaptation of saccade gain. Exp Brain Res 523

146(4):441–450, 2002. 524

Stahl, J. S. Amplitude of human head movements associated with horizontal saccades. Exp Brain Res 525

126(1):41–54, 1999. 526

Straube, A., Fuchs, A. F., Usher, S., and Robinson, F. R. Characteristics of saccadic gain adaptation in 527

rhesus macaques. J Neurophysiol 77(2):874–895, 1997. 528

Page 27: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Tian, J. and Zee, D. S. Context-specific saccadic adaptation in monkeys. Vis Res 50(23):2403–2410, 529

2010. 530

Van Opstal, A. J., Hepp, K., Suzuki, Y., and Henn, V. Influence of eye position on activity in monkey 531

superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 74(4):1593–1610, 1995. 532

Zimmermann, E., Burr, D., and Morrone, M. C. Spatiotopic visual maps revealed by saccadic 533

adaptation in humans. Curr Biol 21(16):1380–1384, 2011. 534

Zimmermann, E. and Lappe, M. Eye position effects in oculomotor plasticity and visual localization. J 535

Neurosci 31(20):7341–7348, 2011. 536

Zipser, D. and Andersen, R. A. A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response 537

properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature 331(6158):679–684, 1988. 538

539

540

541

542

543

544

545

546

547

548

549

550

551

552

Page 28: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

6. Figures 553

Figure 1: 554

The experimental procedure for adaptation of reactive saccades. A) At the beginning of 555

the trial the green fixation point was presented and the monkey’s gaze (circle) was 556

directed towards it. B) After a randomized time the fixation point was switched off and 557

the green target appeared. C) As soon as the onset of the saccade was detected, the target 558

was shifted to the left. In consequence of the target back step, the saccade overshot the 559

target and thus a visual error was induced. D) The monkey made a second saccade to 560

land on the target. E) After a randomized time the target became red and the monkey 561

released the button to get its reward. F) - J) After the monkey pressed the button the 562

fixation point could appear at 5 different positions. From all these starting positions 563

saccades of the same vector were initiated. 564

565

Figure 2: 566

Horizontal eye position during different trials in the session of monkey A with adaptation 567

position +12 deg. In the beginning of each trial the monkey is fixating the presented 568

fixation point. Left panel: Shown are the trajectories of the first and the last trial of the 569

adaptation phase. In the first trial (101) a saccade to the first target is initiated and a 570

corrective saccade is needed to foveate the new target due to the inward target shift made 571

during the saccade. In the last trial of the adaptation phase (450) the saccadic amplitude 572

Page 29: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

has been adapted and the saccade ends closely to the new target. Right panel: 573

Comparison between two trajectories which both started at test position -12 deg in the 574

same session. Trial 2 is a pre-adaptation trial and trial 711 is a test trial performed after 575

the adaptation of the saccades started + 12 deg. The amplitude is also shortened but to a 576

lesser extend than the saccade at the adaptation position. 577

578

Figure 3: 579

Example Session. The landing points of the saccades during the +12 deg adaptation 580

session of monkey A. Saccades started at the same fixation point are presented in the 581

same shade and the unfilled circles represent adaptation and de-adaptation trials at the 582

position +12 deg. The brackets on the left side denote the change of amplitude at the five 583

test positions, namely the difference of the mean pre-adaptation amplitude and mean 584

post-adaptation amplitude. 585

586

Figure 4: 587

The individual adaptation patterns of both monkeys of all five experimental sessions. 588

Each panel shows the gain change and its standard deviation of all five test positions of 589

one session in the spatial order of their appearance on the screen, from –12 deg to +12 590

deg in steps of 6 deg. In each session the adaptation was obtained for a different starting 591

position, this adaptation position is indicated by the filled circles. The total adaptation 592

Page 30: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

states of monkey A and monkey B differ because of the different target step sizes of 5 593

deg and 2 deg, respectively. 594

595

Figure 5: 596

Averaged results of monkey A and monkey B. The circles show the mean normalized 597

gain change in each test position of every session together with the standard deviation. 598

The adaptation position of the displayed session is again indicated by the filled circle. 599

The data has been fitted with a Gaussian function. Width of the Gaussian fits: σ-12deg = 600

10.7 deg, σ-6deg = 8.9 deg, σ0deg = 11.8 deg, σ6deg= 14.3 deg, σ12deg = 15.9 deg 601

602

Figure 6: 603

A possible neural mechanism of the eye position specificity in saccadic adaptation in 604

humans and monkeys showing how the eye position specificity of adaptation might be 605

rooted in the composition of the input to the adaptive circuitry. 606

A) - C) Based on Havermann et al. (2011): In this model the linear transfer of adaptation 607

to different initial eye positions in humans arises from gain field modulation. A) Layer I 608

consists of neurons with different retino-centric receptive fields and eye position specific 609

gain modulation. B) At each receptive field position subpopulation exist with different 610

gain field preferences (e.g. left vs. right eye positions). The subpopulation that prefers 611

the current initial eye position provides the main part of the input to the circuitry and 612

Page 31: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

thus, drives the adaptation. After successful adaptation, a shift of the initial eye position 613

to the side, which is preferred by this neuron pool, leads to a higher firing rate of this 614

pool and thus leads to an increased amplitude modulation. In contrast, a shift of the eye 615

position to the other direction leads to a decreased firing rate of that pool and thus to a 616

decreased amplitude change. C) If the adapted saccade is started at a central position, 617

two subpopulations, one preferring right and one preferring left initial eye positions, 618

contribut to the amplitude modulation. For test saccades with initial eye positions 619

deviating from the adapted one, the increase and decrease in firing rates of the two 620

subpopulations compensate each other. This leads to the complete transfer of adaptation 621

after adaptation at central eye positions that is found in human data. 622

D) - F): Sketch of an extended model of the eye position specificity of saccadic 623

adaptation to account for the Gaussian shaped transfer function. D) Layer I consists of 624

the same neurons described in A. The retino-centric receptive fields with an eye position 625

specific gain modulation form a set of nonlinear basis functions (Pouget and Sejnowski 626

1997). The units in the additional layer II use this set to constitute receptive fields that 627

code the target information in head-centric space. E) During adaptation at one position, 628

one subpopulation provides the main input to the adaptive circuitry and other 629

subpopulations are silent. F) If the initial eye position is changed now to either of the two 630

directions, the firing rate of the subpopulation that has driven the adaptation falls and 631

another (non-adapted) subpopulation drives the saccade. 632

Page 32: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

F)

G)

H)

I)

J)

Position on screen in deg viewing angle

-12 -6 0 6 12

Start position Target

A)

B)

C)

D)

E)

Page 33: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

0 200 400

10

15

20

25

30

35

Hor

izin

tal g

aze

dire

ctio

n [d

eg]

Time [ms]

Trial 101Trial 450

0 200 400

15

10

5

0

5

10

Time [ms]

Trial 2Trial 711

Target

Fixationpoint

Shiftedtarget

Hor

izon

tal p

ositi

on o

n sc

reen

Page 34: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

Sacc

adic

end

poi

nt [

deg]

0 200 400 600 800

Trial

Adaptation TrialDe−adaptation Trial

-12 deg -6 deg 0 deg

6 deg 12 deg

-12 deg - 6 deg 0 deg

6 deg 12 deg

Pre−adaptation Trials:

Test Trials:

Page 35: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

-12 -6 0 6 12- 0.1

0.0

0.1

0.2

Gai

n ch

ange

− M

onke

y A

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.1

0.0

0.1

0.2

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.1

0.0

0.1

0.2

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.1

0.0

0.1

0.2

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.1

0.0

0.1

0.2

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.05

0.0

0.05

0.1

Gai

n ch

ange

− M

onke

y B

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.05

0.0

0.05

0.1

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.05

0.0

0.05

0.1

Initial eye position [deg]

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.05

0.0

0.05

0.1

-12 -6 0 6 12 -0.05

0.0

0.05

0.1

Adapted pos.Test pos.

Page 36: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

-12 -6 0 6 12

0

0.5

1

Nor

mal

ized

gai

n ch

ange

-12 -6 0 6 12

0

0.5

1

-12 -6 0 6 12

0

0.5

1

Initial eye position [deg]

Adapted pos.Test pos.Gaussian fit

-12 -6 0 6 12

0

0.5

1

−12 −6 0 6 12

0

0.5

1

Page 37: Eye position effects in saccadic adaptation in macaque 2 ... et al 2012.pdf45 initial eye position at which the adaptation has been induced. 46 1 Introduction 47 For an active exploration

Adaptive Circuitry

Layer I

A)

C)

Population activity during adaptation at

adaptation eyeposition

Fi

ring

rat

epo

ol in

laye

r I

Am

plitu

de

chan

ge

Horizontal position of the eye

Neuron poolpreferring lefteye position

Neuron poolpreferring lefteye position

B)

Am

plitu

de

chan

ge

Horizontal position of the eye

Fi

ring

rat

epo

ol in

laye

r I

Adaptive Circuitry

Layer I

Layer II

D)

E)

Fi

ring

rat

epo

ol in

laye

r II

Am

plitu

de

chan

ge

Horizontal position of the eye

Neuron poolpreferringadaptation

eye position

F)

Fi

ring

rat

epo

ol in

laye

r II

Am

plitu

de

chan

ge

Horizontal position of the eye