28
This article was downloaded by: [Universite De Paris 1] On: 03 September 2013, At: 01:30 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK International Journal of Science Education Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tsed20 Evaluation of Students’ Understanding of Thermal Concepts in Everyday Contexts Hye-Eun Chu a , David F. Treagust b , Shelley Yeo c & Marjan Zadnik d a Natural Sciences and Science Education, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore b Science and Mathematics Education Centre, School of Science, Curtin University, Perth, Australia c School of Science, Curtin University, Perth, Australia d Department of Imaging and Applied Physics, School of Science, Curtin University, Perth, Australia Published online: 22 Feb 2012. To cite this article: Hye-Eun Chu , David F. Treagust , Shelley Yeo & Marjan Zadnik (2012) Evaluation of Students’ Understanding of Thermal Concepts in Everyday Contexts, International Journal of Science Education, 34:10, 1509-1534, DOI: 10.1080/09500693.2012.657714 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2012.657714 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

Evaluation of Students’ Understanding of Thermal Concepts in Everyday Contexts

  • Upload
    marjan

  • View
    212

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

This article was downloaded by: [Universite De Paris 1]On: 03 September 2013, At: 01:30Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of ScienceEducationPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tsed20

Evaluation of Students’ Understandingof Thermal Concepts in EverydayContextsHye-Eun Chu a , David F. Treagust b , Shelley Yeo c & Marjan Zadnikd

a Natural Sciences and Science Education, National Institute ofEducation, Nanyang Technological University, Singaporeb Science and Mathematics Education Centre, School of Science,Curtin University, Perth, Australiac School of Science, Curtin University, Perth, Australiad Department of Imaging and Applied Physics, School of Science,Curtin University, Perth, AustraliaPublished online: 22 Feb 2012.

To cite this article: Hye-Eun Chu , David F. Treagust , Shelley Yeo & Marjan Zadnik (2012)Evaluation of Students’ Understanding of Thermal Concepts in Everyday Contexts, InternationalJournal of Science Education, 34:10, 1509-1534, DOI: 10.1080/09500693.2012.657714

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2012.657714

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever orhowsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arisingout of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Evaluation of Students’ Understanding

of Thermal Concepts in Everyday

Contexts

Hye-Eun Chua∗, David F. Treagustb, Shelley Yeoc andMarjan Zadnikd

aNatural Sciences and Science Education, National Institute of Education, Nanyang

Technological University, Singapore; bScience and Mathematics Education Centre,

School of Science, Curtin University, Perth, Australia; cSchool of Science, Curtin

University, Perth, Australia; dDepartment of Imaging and Applied Physics, School of

Science, Curtin University, Perth, Australia

The aims of this study were to determine the underlying conceptual structure of the thermal concept

evaluation (TCE) questionnaire, a pencil-and-paper instrument about everyday contexts of heat,

temperature, and heat transfer, to investigate students’ conceptual understanding of thermal

concepts in everyday contexts across several school years and to analyse the variables—school

year, science subjects currently being studied, and science subjects previously studied in thermal

energy—that influence students’ thermal conceptual understanding. The TCE, which was

administered to 515 Korean students from years 10–12, was developed in Australia, using

students’ alternative conceptions derived from the research literature. The conceptual structure

comprised four groups—heat transfer and temperature changes, boiling, heat conductivity and

equilibrium, and freezing and melting—using 19 of the 26 items in the original questionnaire.

Depending on the year group, 25–55% of students experienced difficulties in applying scientific

concepts in everyday contexts. Years of schooling, science subjects currently studied and physics

topics previously studied correlated with development of students’ conceptual understanding,

especially in topics relating to heat transfer, temperature scales, specific heat capacity,

homeostasis, and thermodynamics. Although students did improve their conceptual

understandings in later years of schooling, they still had difficulties in relating the scientific

concepts to their experiences in everyday contexts. The study illustrates the utility of using a

pencil-and-paper questionnaire to identify students’ understanding of thermal concepts in

everyday situations and provides a baseline for Korean students’ achievement in terms of physics

in everyday contexts, one of the objectives of the Korean national curriculum reforms.

International Journal of Science Education

Vol. 34, No. 10, July 2012, pp. 1509–1534

∗Corresponding author: Natural Sciences and Science Education, Nanyang Technological University,

1 Nanyang Walk, Singapore 637616, Singapore. Email: [email protected]

ISSN 0950-0693 (print)/ISSN 1464-5289 (online)/12/101509–26

# 2012 Taylor & Francis

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2012.657714

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Keywords: Thermal physics; Everyday contexts; Conceptual understanding; Alternative

conceptions

Introduction

Over the last three decades, studies have revealed and proposed reasons for the exist-

ence and resilience of children’s and adolescents’ various naıve understandings about

the physical world (Duit, 2009; Osborne & Freyberg, 1985). Despite the diversity of

naıve understandings and theories, different researchers have repeatedly reported

similar results and patterns across age groups. It is also generally agreed that tra-

ditional instruction which does not take into account the existing beliefs of students

is largely ineffective in changing their naıve scientific ideas. Many students leave

school and even university with their intuitive physics understandings unchanged or

existing alongside more accepted scientific views (Meltzer, 2004; Vosniadou, 1994;

White & Gunstone, 1989). Similarly in optics, students were unable to apply scientific

concepts in different contexts even though the contexts were designed to use the same

scientific concept (Chu, Treagust, & Chandrasegaran, 2009).

With regard to students’ understanding of thermal concepts, many researchers have

indicated that students can easily be confused primarily because of the language gap

between the scientific definition and everyday terminology for heat, temperature and

energy (Clough & Driver, 1986; Kesidou, Duit, & Glynn, 1995; Tiberghien, 1985).

Students are very familiar with the terms ‘heat’, ‘temperature’ and ‘energy’ which

they encounter in their everyday lives. Consequently, students think they know the

correct meaning of scientific terminology, but their meaning of the terminology is

very different from that used by experts in science (Carey, 1991; Wiser & Carey,

1983). According to Hewitt (2006), temperature is a measure of the average kinetic

energy of the molecules or atoms. When there are temperature differences between

objects in contact, heat flows from the object at a higher temperature to the object

at a lower temperature. Heat is used mainly to indicate energy flow due to temperature

differences and heat is a process energy form, not a storage energy form in the system

(Kesidou et al., 1995). However, most students think that the objects contain heat.

Once heat has been transferred to an object or substance, it ceases to be heat and

becomes internal energy. Also, many students consider heat as an intensive quantity

and temperature as the amount of heat (Kesidou & Duit, 1993). However, after

they have learned the concept of specific heat capacity in year 11, students tend to

use the specific heat capacity concept to explain thermal equilibrium and to consider

heat as partly an extensive quantity. Many students use the specific heat capacity

concept to describe their understanding related to efficacy to conduct heat or easiness

to get to the thermal equilibrium stage without describing the rate of temperature

change or amount of heat energy transfer (Chiu & Anderson, 2009; Harrison,

Grayson, & Treagust, 1999).

Based on these studies, it is important that students have a clear scientific under-

standing about the terms temperature, heat and energy (namely, internal energy) so

1510 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

that they are able to develop their conceptions to explain the everyday phenomena

related to thermal concepts.

This approach is considered important in view of the growing emphasis on the rel-

evance of contextualised science at elementary and secondary school levels (Nentwig

& Waddington, 2005).

In this study, we were interested to learn how well Korean students were able to

respond to questions on thermal concepts that are related to everyday contexts. In

recent times, the Korean national curriculum has incorporated the movement of

science for all and science-technology-society (STS) with an emphasis on the con-

structivist classroom and scientific literacy (Ministry of Education, 1992a, 1992b,

1997). However, in the implemented curriculum, teachers continue to maintain

highly discipline-centred traditional teaching and students have little experience

with these types of physics questions based on everyday life experiences (Kim,

2001; Kwon, 2001; Oh & Kim, 2001). Consequently, this study was designed to

provide a baseline to know how well Korean students are able to answer questions

based on everyday phenomena related to thermal concepts.

Background

Three decades of research related to students’ understanding of thermal con-

cepts have indicated a variety of alternative conceptions held by students aged

12–17 years old. A summary of these findings, primarily from qualitative

studies, using interviews or questionnaires with open-ended responses, is pre-

sented in Table 1. Several factors are known to impede students’ development

of scientifically acceptable conceptions about thermal physics. Students’ accep-

tance that heat is a form of energy is a crucial prior requirement for their devel-

opment of concepts in thermal physics (Kaper & Goedhart, 2002). Also, to be

able to understand the accepted view that heat is energy that moves between

objects at different temperatures, students would first need to differentiate

between heat and ‘cold’, temperature and heat, and fully appreciate the idea

of thermal equilibrium (Arnold & Millar, 1994). Another constraint to under-

standing thermal concepts requires an ontological shift among students that

requires them to regard heat as a process instead of matter (Chi, Slotta, & de

Leeuw, 1994; Wiser & Amin, 2001).

Aims of the Study

This study was conducted to (1) determine the underlying conceptual structure of the

thermal concept evaluation (TCE), (2) investigate students’ conceptual understand-

ing of thermal concepts in everyday contexts across several school years, and (3)

analyse the variables, such as school year, science subjects currently being studied,

and science topics previously studied in thermal energy, that influence students’

thermal conceptual understanding.

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1511

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Research Methodology

Instrument

The 26-item TCE questionnaire used in this study was developed and implemented

by Yeo and Zadnik (2001) to assess the extent to which students had attained scien-

tifically acceptable understandings about basic thermal concepts like heat, tempera-

ture, heat conduction and phase changes. Students have a tendency to treat the

Table 1. Previous literature studies on thermal concepts in everyday contexts involving 12–17-

year-olds

Identified alternative conceptions Students’ age Method Author

Heat

There are two types of heat: hot heat

and cold heat

12-year-olds Interviews Erickson

(1979)

Heat is a material substance like a

wave that rises from the road

Temperature

Temperature is an extensive quantity 12-year-olds Interviews Erickson

(1979)

Temperature of boiling water can

exceed 100 8C during boiling

12–15-year-olds Interviews Andersson

(1980)

Temperature is a measure of heat 15–16-year-olds Interviews Kesidou and

Duit (1993)

Boiling

The matter inside bubbles of boiling

water is water, water vapour, heat,

air, smoke, oxygen or carbon dioxide

12-year-olds (also

including 6–11 years

old)

Open-ended

written tests

Bar and Travis

(1991)

Heat conductivity

Metals attract, hold or store heat and

cold

12–14-year-olds

(also including

adults and scientists)

Interviews Lewis and

Linn (1994)

Wool warms things up

Thermal equilibrium

The temperature of different objects

is different even though they have

been placed in the same environment

over an extended period of time

15-year-olds Interviews Clough and

Driver (1986)

17-year-olds Inquiry method

(classroom

discussion)

Harrison et al.

(1999)

12–14-year-olds

(also including

adults and scientists)

Interviews Lewis and

Linn (1994)

General thermal concepts

Students’ confusion because of

dualism of thermodynamics/lack of

understanding in the processes of

cooling (or heating) of pure

substances/understating that does not

rely on the sensations of the body

13–18-year-olds

(also including

teachers)

Open-ended

written tests

Sciarretta et al.

(1990)

1512 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

physics that they learn at school purely for the purpose of reproducing the physics on

school tests and examinations without realising the relevance of these concepts in their

everyday lives, often resulting in idiosyncratic understandings of scientific concepts.

For this reason, the items in the TCE were situated in everyday contexts with

which students would be familiar and used limited scientific language in the question-

naire items (Yeo & Zadnik, 2001).

The equivalent Korean questionnaire was back-translated into English. Three

experts in science education and two science teachers worked together to ensure

that the items were correctly translated. Also, two of the Australian authors and

one Korean author met with the Korean–English translator to discuss the back trans-

lation. The back translated items did not show different meanings from the original

items in the English version. The authors made sure that the Korean items were trans-

lated using terms with which students were familiar in everyday contexts and with

minimum use of scientific language.

An example of one of the items in the questionnaire is shown in Figure 1. The items

all involved events that students could experience in a school canteen and were

centred on students’ conversations about these experiences.

Pilot Study

A pilot study was first conducted involving 30–35 students from each school year to

determine the reliability of the items, students’ understanding of the equivalent mean-

ings of terms in translations of the items, and the time required to complete the ques-

tionnaire. Findings from the pilot study indicated that about 50 minutes was sufficient

time and the translated terms in the questionnaire did not cause understanding pro-

blems with students. So, it was confirmed that students had no difficulties in under-

standing the translated terms that were used in the final questionnaire, and the

students were given 50 minutes to complete the 26 items in the actual test.

Participants

A total of 515 Korean high school students from years 10, 11, and 12 participated in

this research. All students had already been introduced to the thermal energy concepts

that were evaluated in the questionnaire during various stages of their science lessons.

Figure 1. An example of an item from the TCE (Yeo & Zadnik, 2001)

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1513

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

In years 3–10, students were introduced in their study of integrated science to thermal

concepts like temperature change, heat transfer, states of matter, energy, heat conduc-

tivity, boiling/melting point, and thermal dynamic energy. These thermal concepts are

the main emphasised scientific concepts in this research. Some students who were

involved in this research studied physics in years 11 and 12. In year 12, gaseous mol-

ecular motion, thermal dynamic theory and specific heat are introduced, but these

concepts are not among the main scientific concepts explored in this research.

Data Analysis Procedures

Data from the 26-item questionnaire were initially examined for item difficulty (P-

value) and item discrimination index (D-value) based on Lien’s (1971) criteria, i.e.

P-values need to be in the range 30–70 and D-values 20 and over (these two values

are expressed as percentages). Items 9 and 21 failed to meet both the minimum P-

value (30) and the minimum D-value (20) and item 12 exceeded the maximum P-

value (70). Items 9 and 21 were too difficult as less than 30% of the students answered

them correctly and item 12 was too easy as more than 70% of students answered cor-

rectly. Although six additional items did not meet the minimum difficulty value, they

all had acceptable discrimination values and were retained for further analysis.

Subsequently, the remaining 23 items were analysed using SPSS software (version

18.0) beginning with a factor analysis of the items in order to investigate the under-

lying conceptual structure of the TCE. An exploratory factor analysis was performed

and a principal component analysis (PCA) was used to extract the factors followed by

oblique rotation (delta ¼ 0). The Oblique rotation method allows the factors to cor-

relate. The number of factors to be retained was guided by three decision rules:

Kaiser’s criterion (eigenvalues above 1), inspection of screeplot, and Horn’s parallel

analysis (Horn, 1965) conducted using the software developed by Watkins (2000).

The size of eigenvalues obtained from PCA was compared with those obtained

from a randomly generated data set of sample size. Only factors with eigenvalues

exceeding the values obtained from the corresponding random data set were retained

for further investigation.

After each PCA, the items were re-evaluated in the factor solution using specified

criteria. Comrey and Lee (1992) considered items with factor loadings 0.45 as fair

items and factor loadings between 0.32 and 0.45 as poor items, while Hair, Anderson,

Tatham, and Black (1998) and Kline (1994) considered a factor loading of 0.3 as

minimum loading. Moreover, Stevens (2002) considered items with a factor

loading of 0.4 to be fair items and factor loadings of 0.3 to be fair when the sample

size is greater than 300. In our research, the cut-off loading was made at 0.3

because our sample size was 515 with 13 of the 19 items showing factor loadings

greater than 0.45.

The percentage of students’ correct responses to each of the items contributing to

the underlying conceptual structure was then calculated. The correlations between

the mean scores of the conceptual groups were computed and the multivariate analy-

sis of variance (MANOVA) procedure was used to investigate the main factors that

1514 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

influenced students’ conceptual understanding of thermal concepts. The standar-

dised mean scores (50 + Z score × 10) of the four conceptual groups of items

were the dependent variables, while school year, subjects currently studied, and

physics topics previously studied relating to heat transfer, specific heat capacity,

and thermodynamics were the independent variables. To find out the impact of the

independent variables on the dependent variable (the standardised mean scores of

the four conceptual groups of items), the eta squared effect size statistics were com-

puted. The eta squared values inform us how strongly the independent variables influ-

enced the dependent variable.

Result and Discussion

Item Analysis of the TCE

Investigating the underlying conceptual structure. Inspection of the pattern matrix

showed the clear factor loadings of each item while the analysis of the structure

matrix indicated good discrimination among the four components (see Table 2).

The conceptual groups were: (1) heat transfer and temperature changes, (2)

boiling, (3) heat conductivity and equilibrium, and (4) freezing and melting. Each

conceptual group involves a particular set of related items. A description of the

items in each of the four conceptual groups and the deleted items are presented in

Table 3.

Items 3 and 20 had relatively low loadings and hence were deleted. In Items 1 and 2,

students have to consider the surroundings. For example, in item 1, the temperature

of ice in a refrigerator’s freezer compartment is decided by the temperature of the

freezer compartment. In item 3 on the other hand, the higher temperature of the sur-

rounding air may have distracted them from the temperature of the ice pieces on the

bench, leading them to believe that the temperature of the ice on the bench-top was

greater than 0 8C. item 20 was the only question in this questionnaire that concerned

air convection. Items 8 and 19 were about boiling water/soup under different press-

ures and they could not be loaded into boiling concept group 2 because items in

the boiling concept group were about the temperature of the boiling point of water

at the ground level. Item 8 showed relatively low loading, so it was deleted. Item 19

was loaded in group 1 (with factor loading 0.31, correlation 0.38 between item 19

and factor 1) and group 3 (with factor loading 0.37, correlation 0.44 between item

19 and factor 3). Item 19 was deleted because it was loaded in two inappropriate con-

ceptual groups and the factor loading values in the two conceptual groups were very

close to each other. However, item 16 was retained in conceptual group 3, heat con-

ductivity and equilibrium, even though the item was loaded in group 1 (with factor

loading 0.3, correlation 0.1 between item 16 and group1) and group 3 (with factor

loading 0.5, correlation 0.5 between item 16 and group 3). Item 16 is about conduc-

tivity in different materials, wood and metal and involved attaining temperature equi-

librium as a result of heat conduction. Item 16 was loaded in two conceptual groups

and showed relatively higher loading in conceptual group 3.

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1515

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Table 2. Factor loadings of the 19 items in the TCE with P- and D-values

Item P D

Components

1 2 3 4

Pattern

matrix

Structure

matrix

Pattern

matrix

Structure

matrix

Pattern

matrix

Structure

matrix

Pattern

matrix

Structure

matrix

Q25 46 68 0.61 0.60 20.12 20.07 20.05 0.05 0.09 0.14

Q15 58 71 0.54 0.55 20.10 20.06 0.10 0.18 20.03 0.04

Q22 52 71 0.52 0.54 20.01 0.03 20.03 0.07 0.16 0.21

Q13 36 71 0.47 0.51 0.02 0.05 0.22 0.30 0.16 0.24

Q23 23 59 0.46 0.47 0.16 0.19 0.06 0.12 20.05 0.00

Q7 62 42 0.41 0.38 0.08 0.12 20.11 0.08 20.23 0.20

Q10 42 88 0.31 0.39 0.25 0.27 0.24 20.31 0.20 20.26

Q5 36 56 20.10 20.02 0.77 0.76 0.10 0.10 0.13 0.13

Q4 22 32 0.12 20.01 0.72 0.73 20.12 20.11 20.02 20.02

Q6 28 34 20.01 0.16 0.31 0.31 20.05 20.07 20.12 20.13

Q24 18 34 20.06 20.00 20.05 0.06 0.64 0.63 20.12 0.05

Q14 23 69 20.01 0.02 0.13 20.13 0.63 0.62 0.05 20.13

Q16 33 64 0.33 0.10 20.05 20.03 0.47 0.50 20.11 20.02

Q26 31 51 0.00 0.38 20.16 20.17 0.38 0.39 0.07 0.12

Q17 24 47 0.17 0.05 20.06 20.04 0.37 0.39 20.02 0.04

Q18 17 36 20.07 0.22 0.27 0.26 0.35 0.35 0.09 0.11

Q2 58 56 20.19 0.01 0.00 20.02 0.13 0.18 0.68 0.69

Q1 50 61 0.12 0.17 20.12 20.12 20.04 0.06 0.68 0.69

Q11 46 54 0.20 20.10 0.11 0.12 20.27 20.18 0.49 0.47

Initial eigenvalues 2.5 1.4 1.3 1.3

Percentage of

variance

13.9 8.0 7.3 6.7

Conceptual groups: component 1, heat transfer and temperature changes; component 2, boiling; component 3, heat conductivity and equilibrium;

component 4, freezing and melting. Figures in bold indicate major loadings for each item.

1516

H.-E

.C

hu

etal.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Table 3. Description of TCE items in the four conceptual groups, including P- and D-values

Conceptualgroup

Itemno. Item description P D

1 Heat transfer andtemperature changes

7 Predicting temperature of mixture of water 62 4215 Comparing magnitude of ‘coldness’ resulting

from temperature decrease announced in aweather forecast

58 71

22 Inflating a bicycle tyre resulting in warming upof pump

52 71

23 Wearing sweaters in cold weather 52 5925 Producing superconductor magnets at low

temperatures46 68

10 Comparing the temperature of a cold can ofcoke with that of the bench top beneath the can

42 88

13 Transferring heat energy from hot boiled eggs tocold water

36 71

2 Boiling 5 Predicting temperature of continuously boilingwater

36 56

6 Predicting temperature of steam in contact withboiling water

28 34

4 Predicting initial temperature of boiling water 22 323 Heat conductivity

and equilibrium16 Explaining why metal ruler feels colder than

wooden ruler33 64

26 Measuring of temperature of dolls wrapped inblankets

31 51

17 Predicting room temperature based on coolingeffect of bottles wrapped with wet and drywashcloths

24 47

14 Comparing ‘warmness’ and coldness’ of plasticand metal

23 69

24 Comparing temperatures of wooden and iceparts of ice-lolly

18 34

18 Explaining why the milk carton from therefrigerator feels colder than the one from thebench top

17 36

4 Freezing and melting 2 Predicting temperature of ice water when icecubes have stopped melting

58 56

1 Predicting temperature of ice-blocks stored in afreezer

50 61

11 Comparing heat loss by equal masses of ice andice water at 0 8C in a freezer

46 54

Deleted items 12 Predicting what is in bubbles that form in

the boiling water

72 56

3 Predicting temperature of ice almost melted andlying in a puddle of water

59 61

8 Preparing tea at high altitude 50 8319 Cooking of soup in a pressure cooker 40 73

9 Predicting temperature of plastic bottle

and coke in the plastic bottle that has been

in a refrigerator overnight

26 20

20 Predicting suitable shelf in the electric oven tocook cakes well

24 24

21 Predicting why sweat cools you down 14 5

Italicised and bold items did not meet Lien’s criteria.

P, P-value; D, D-value.

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1517

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Even though the factor analysis showed four distinct conceptual groups, the

reliability of three groups was low (0.4–0.5). However, the Cronbach alpha reliability

for the 19 items was 0.59. According to Nunally and Bernstein (1994), a Cronbach

alpha reliability coefficient greater than 0.7 indicates a high reliability while values

in the range 0.5–0.7 indicate moderate reliability and are acceptable in studies of a

cognitive nature. The Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities for both the boiling, freezing

and melting conceptual groups were low because the items in the two conceptual

groups involved the thermodynamic dualism of concepts like, state and process,

and heat and temperature (Sciarretta, Stilli, & Vicentini Missoni, 1990). Several of

the items in the heat conductivity conceptual group also required background under-

standing of heat transfer to reach an equilibrium state. These kinds of conceptual con-

tinuity in the conceptual groups decreased the internal consistency reliability of the

items in the categories.

Nunally and Bernstein (1994) suggest that for good item construction, the item

content should be homogenous, and factor analysis as well as item difficulty and

item discrimination indices should be considered together when selecting items relat-

ing to particular concepts. The items in this research can be used to identify important

students’ alternative conceptions because although students were introduced to these

concepts in their earlier studies, most students were unable to make connections with

the contexts of the items involved. Consequently, these 19 items in the four concep-

tual groups were used to explain students’ conceptions in thermal concepts across the

school years 10–12.

Students’ Correct Responses

Items related to everyday contexts in the heat conductivity and the equilibrium con-

ceptual group and the boiling conceptual group elicited lower correct answers than

the other two conceptual groups. The percentages of the group means of correct

responses for these two conceptual groups were 29% and 24%, respectively. The per-

centages of students’ correct responses to each of the 19 items in four conceptual

groups across the school years are summarised in Table 4.

In general, there was no clear cut progression of conceptual development across the

school years. For example, the percentage of correct responses to items 7 and 11 for

year 10 was higher than that for years 11 and 12. Also, year 10 students showed higher

percentages of correct responses than year 11 students in items 23, 5, and 4. For items

15, 25, 10, and 6, the percentages of correct responses in year 11 were higher (or

same) than in year 12.

The statistical analysis of the influence of school year on students’ conceptual

understating is provided in Table 7 in a subsequent section.

Students’ Alternative Conceptions

Students’ alternative conceptions across school years 10–12 are summarised in Table

5. For multiple-choice items with four responses, scientifically inappropriate

1518 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Table 4. The percentage of students’ correct responses to the items in the four conceptual groups

across the school years

Item no. Correct responses

Percentage of correct responses

Y10 Y11 Y12 Over-all

Group

mean

Heat transfer and temperature changes

7 When two cups of water at 40 8C each are

added to one cup of water at 10 8C, the

resulting temperature is not the average of the

two temperatures

68 57 60 62

15 A temperature of 5 8C is not twice as cold as a

temperature of 10 8C42 69 63 58 50

22 A bicycle pump becomes hot when it is used to

pump up a tyre because energy has been

transferred from the compressed air to the

pump.

47 51 57 52

23 We wear sweaters in cold weather to reduce

heat loss from our bodies

52 47 56 52

25 Super-conducting magnets can be produced at

a temperature of 2260 8C, close to the lowest

temperature possible (i.e. 2273 8C)

42 52 44 46

10 When a cold can of coke is picked up from a

bench top, the space that was underneath the

can would be colder than the rest of the bench

top because some heat energy has been

transferred from the bench to the can

35 46 46 42

13 When boiled eggs are cooled by placing them

in cold water energy is transferred from the

eggs to the water

27 38 44 36

Boiling

5 As the water in the kettle continues to boil for

some time, the most likely temperature of the

boiling water is still 98 8C

37 28 43 36 29

6 The temperature of the steam above the

boiling water in the kettle is most likely to be

98 8C

27 32 26 28

4 The most likely temperature of water boiling in

a kettle is 98 8C (not 88 8C or 110 8C).

22 19 25 22

Heat conductivity and equilibrium

16 Metal is colder than wood when we touch it,

because metal conducts energy more rapidly

than wood

28 33 40 33

26 The constant temperature of dolls wrapped in

blankets was not due to the materials in the

blankets or the dolls

30 31 33 31 24

(Continued)

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1519

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

responses selected by more than 25% of respondents were considered as alternative

conceptions in this research, while items with five response values greater than 20%

(as in Items 11 and 26) were considered as alternative conceptions.

Heat transfer and temperature changes. The items related to everyday life in the heat

transfer and temperature changes conceptual group mainly required students to

have a clear understanding about the difference between heat and temperature.

However, students’ demonstrated confusion about these two concepts. In Table 5,

students from year 10 showed heat is proportional to temperature (item 15, 31%)

and students from all school years suggested that temperature could be transferred

Table 4. (Continued)

Item no. Correct responses

Percentage of correct responses

Y10 Y11 Y12 Over-all

Group

mean

17 When two glass bottles containing water at

20 8C were wrapped separately with a wet and

a dry washcloth, the temperature of the water

some time later in each bottle was 18 8C and

22 8C, respectively. The most likely room

temperature during this experiment was 26 8C(not 20 8C or 21 8C or 18 8C)

14 26 33 24

14 Although metal chairs may feel colder than

wooden ones, both chairs are at the same

temperature

18 22 29 23

24 The wooden stick and the ice part of an ice-

lolly taken out of a freezer are at the same

temperature because both parts are in contact

with each other

14 15 24 18

18 The colder carton of choc milk conducts heat

faster than the warmer carton of choc milk

11 20 21 17

Freezing and melting

2 The temperature of water produced by melting

ice is most likely to be 0 8C (and not 210 8C or

5 8C or 10 8C)

54 58 63 58 51

1 The temperature of ice blocks that are stored

in the freezer compartment of a refrigerator

would most likely to be 210 8C (and not 0 8Cor 5 8C)

42 48 59 50

11 When 100 g of ice at 0 8C and 100 g of water at

0 8C are both placed in a freezer, the latter

would eventually lose the greater amount of

heat energy

48 44 46 46

Y10, year 10 (n ¼ 178); Y11, year 11 (n ¼ 166); Y12, year 12 (n ¼ 171); total sample (N ¼ 515).

1520 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

from the body to another (Items 13 and 22, 14–36%). The most common alternative

conception in this group for all school years was that ‘hot and cold are different, not on

opposite ends of a continuum’ (Items 10 and 13, 21–40%).

Table 5. Frequencies of students’ alternative conceptions across the school years

Students’ alternative conceptions

Item no

(choices)

School year

Year 10

(N ¼ 178)

Year 11

(N ¼ 166)

Year 12

(N ¼ 171)

Heat transfer and temperature changes

Two different temperatures can be added up and

averaged

Q7 (b) 40 (23) 56 (34) 47 (28)

Temperature can be transferred from one body

to another

Q13 (a) 64 (36) 32 (19) 40 (23)

Q22 (b) 30 (17) 23 (14) 34 (20)

Materials like wool have the ability to warm

things up

Q23 (d) 43 (23) 52 (31) 48 (28)

‘Hot’ and ‘cold’ are different, not at opposite

ends of a continuum

Q10 (a) 72 (40) 38 (23) 46 (27)

Q13 (b) 47 (26) 45 (27) 36 (21)

There is no limit to the lowest temperature that

can be reached

Q25 (b) 46 (26) 42 (25) 55 (32)

Heat is proportional to temperature Q15 (a) 55 (31) 17 (10) 19 (11)

Boiling

Heating always results in an increase in

temperature

Q4 (c) 42 (23) 54 (33) 43 (25)

The boiling point of water is 100 8C Q4 (d) 80 (45) 76 (46) 76 (44)

The temperature at the boiling does not remain

constant

Q5 (c) 68 (38) 82 (50) 69 (40)

Steam above boiling water in a kettle is at a

temperature greater than 100 8CQ6 (c) 59 (33) 67 (40) 82 (48)

Heat conductivity and equilibrium

The amount of heat in an object depends on the

material that the object is made up of

Q14 (d) 98 (55) 94 (57) 70 (41)

Q24 (c) 63 (35) 58 (35) 51 (30)

Metal ruler feels cooler than wooden one

because metal is good radiator

Q16 (d) 39 (22) 29 (18) 38 (22)

Materials like wool have the ability to warm

things up

Q17 (b) 46 (26) 39 (24) 38 (22)

Objects at different temperatures that are in

contact with each other do not necessarily move

towards the same temperature

Q17 (c) 81 (46) 60 (36) 59 (35)

‘Hot’ and ‘cold’ are different, not at opposite

ends of a continuum.

Q18 (a) 67 (38) 48 (29) 55 (32)

Some materials are difficult to heat: they are

more resistant to heating

Q26 (c) 61 (34) 60 (36) 58 (34)

Freezing and melting

Ice is always at 0 8C Q1 (b) 88 (50) 70 (42) 57 (33)

Heat is proportional to temperature Q11 (c) 33 (19) 36 (22) 29 (17)

Water cannot be at 0 8C Q2 (c) 59 (33) 55 (33) 42 (25)

The numbers in bold indicate alternative conceptions. Percentages are denoted in parentheses.

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1521

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

These two alternative conceptions were found to be context—independent as illus-

trated in Table 6 where students displayed the same alternative conceptions in two

different items. For example, among the 64 students in year 10 who chose the alterna-

tive conception ‘Temperature can be transferred from one body to another’ in item 13,

11 students (17%) chose the same alternative conception again in item 22 (see Table 6).

Boiling. The alternative conceptions in this group were most prevalent among all

year levels. The context of the three items was about boiling water in a kettle. Students

in all the year groups were already familiar with the concepts associated with phase

changes. However, 30–50% of students indicated that ‘the temperature at the

boiling point does not remain constant’ and that ‘steam in the kettle is at a tempera-

ture higher than 100 8C’. Also, 44–46% of students from all years displayed the naive

conception that ‘the temperature of boiling water is always 100 8C’. These very basic

conceptual understandings in thermal physics are not given sufficient emphasis in the

Korean school curriculum and also there is limited inclusion of everyday life examples

that apply these basic scientific conceptions about heat and temperature.

Heat conductivity and equilibrium. The items in this conceptual group involve

context-related experiences in different everyday phenomena. Students are required

to apply the scientific concept of heat conductivity between two materials at different

temperatures until a state of equilibrium is reached. At the same time, students should

be aware that the different materials have different heat conductivities, even though

they are in a state of equilibrium at the same temperature. As many as 35–55% of stu-

dents from all school years indicated that ‘containing heat is a property of materials’ as

shown in item 14 (involving comparing the ‘coldness’ of chairs made from two differ-

ent materials, plastic and metal) and item 24 (involving comparing the ‘coldness’ of

the wooden stick and ice part of an ice-lolly). These items were context dependent.

For example, among the 98 students in year 10 who chose the alternative conception

‘The amount of heat in an object depends on the material that the object is made up

Table 6. Percentages of students who held the same alternative conceptions in different contexts

Students’ alternative conceptions Item no (choices)

School year

Y10 Y11 Y12

Temperature can be transferred from

one body to another

Q13 (a) and Q22 (b) 11 (17) 7 (22) 11 (28)

‘Hot’ and ‘cold’ are different, not at

opposite ends of a continuum

Q13 (b) and Q10 (a) 20 (28) 17 (45) 13 (28)

The amount of heat in an object

depends on the material that the

object is made up of

Q14 (d) and Q24 (c) 34 (35) 35 (37) 28 (40)

Percentages are denoted in parentheses.

1522 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

of’ in item 14, 34 students (34%) chose the same alternative conception again in item

24 (see Table 6).

In item 17 (involving heat conduction from bottles of cold water wrapped separately

with a wet and a dry washcloth) 35–46% of students from all school years suggested

that ‘equilibrium is not a necessary process for objects at different temperatures that

are in contact with each other’, that is objects at different temperature that are in

contact with each other, do not necessarily reach an equilibrium temperature. Also,

in item 18 (comparing heat conduction to a hand from a cold milk carton and from

a warm milk carton) 29–38% of students from all school years displayed that the

carton feels colder because the cold milk carton contains more coldness than the

warm milk cartoon.

Freezing and melting. Items in this conceptual group include ice blocks in the freezer

(item 1), ice blocks in water (item 2) and ice and water both at 0 8C in the freezer (item

11). This conceptual group is related to basic scientific concepts in thermal physics,

yet 33–50% of students’ from all school years indicated that ‘ice is always at 0 8C’

and 25–33% of students’ from all school years suggested that ‘water cannot be at

0 8C’. Also 22% of students from year 11 held the alternative conception in response

to item 11 that ‘heat is proportional to temperature’ so, they suggested that ‘ice at 0 8Cand water at 0 8C in the freezer both lose the same amount of heat’.

All four conceptual groups are essential for the development of students’ under-

standing of thermal physics concepts. The heat transfer and temperature changes con-

ceptual group that lays the foundation for the understanding of all other concepts;

students need to have a clear understanding of the difference between heat and temp-

erature in order to understand the other concepts. Based on correlation analysis,

involving the four conceptual groups, the heat transfer and temperature changes

group correlated significantly with (1) boiling conceptual group (r ¼ 0.1, P ≤0.05), (2) the heat conductivity and equilibrium conceptual group (r ¼ 0.3, P ≤0.005), (3) freezing and melting conceptual group (r ¼ 0.2, P ≤ 0.005). Based on

Cohen’s (1988) suggestion, the significant Pearson coefficient in this research can

be considered as a small correlation or medium correlation (range 0.10–0.29: small

correlation, range 0.30–0.49: medium correlation).

No significant correlation was found between other conceptual groups in this

research. As indicated in the Introduction, the other main reason for the existence

of students’ alternative conceptions may be due to differences in the meanings of

scientific terms when they are used in everyday language. The terms, ‘coldness’ and

‘hotness’, for example, are often used to imply heat transfer between objects.

Analysis of the Influence of the Independent Variables

The impact of several independent variables—school year, science subjects currently

studied and previously studied topics in thermal energy—on students’ understanding

of thermal energy concepts were evaluated in this research.

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1523

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

School Year

Students’ mean scores in the four conceptual groups according to their school years

are shown in Table 7. For the heat transfer and temperature changes conceptual

group and heat conductivity and equilibrium conceptual group there were no mean

difference between students in years 10 and 11 and years 11 and 12, but there were

significant mean differences in years 10 and 12. However, the eta squared values indi-

cate that only 2–4% in the variance of mean scores in these two conceptual groups can

be explained by the different year groups, suggesting that the influence of school year

on the conceptual understating of heat transfer and temperature changes and heat

conductivity and equilibrium is very limited. (Interpretation of eta squared values:

0.01 ¼ small effect; 0.06 ¼ moderate effect; 0.14 ¼ large effect). Similarly, for the

conceptual groups boiling, and melting and freezing, there were no significant differ-

ences in the mean scores across school years 10–12.

Science Subjects Currently Studied

Students in years 10–12 studied different subjects. All students studied general

science when they were in year 10. If students intend to study engineering or

science majors, they usually choose more science subjects in years 11 and 12. In

this study, year 10 students studied only general science, while students in years 11

and 12 studied one or more subjects chosen from life science, physics and chemistry.

The breakdown of subjects studied was: (1) general science—year 10 (N ¼ 178), (2)

life science—year 11 (N ¼ 76) and year 12 (N ¼ 81), and (3) physics and chemistry—

year 11 (N ¼ 90) and year 12 (N ¼ 90).

The results of MANOVA to investigate the impact of science subjects currently

studied on the four conceptual groups of thermal concepts are shown in Table 8.

Table 7. Students’ standardised mean scores in the four conceptual groups based on school years,

from MANOVA results

School year

Conceptual groups

Heat transfer and

temperature changes

Mean + SD

Boiling

Mean + SD

Heat conductivity

and equilibrium

Mean + SD

Melting and

freezing

Mean + SD

Year 10 (N ¼ 178) 48.1 + 9.2a 49.9 + 10.6 47.6 + 8.9a 49.0 + 10.0

Year 11 (N ¼ 166) 50.8 + 10.3ab 49.2 + 9.6 50.1 + 9.3ab 49.6 + 10.1

Year 12 (N ¼ 171) 51.2 + 10.3b 50.9 + 9.7 52.5 + 11.1b 51.4 + 9.8

F 4.9∗ 1.3 11.0∗∗ 2.8

Eta squared 0.02 0.01 0.04 0.01

Different superscripts indicate that there are significant differences between year levels.

∗P ≤ 0.05.

∗∗P ≤ 0.005.

1524 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Students who were studying chemistry and physics showed significantly better under-

standing than students who were studying general science or life science in the heat

transfer and temperature changes and heat conductivity and equilibrium conceptual

groups. Also, they had significantly better understanding comparing to other students

about the melting and freezing conceptual group, while students who were studying

general science showed significantly better understanding than students who were

studying life science in this conceptual area. Science subjects currently studied did

not have any influence on the understanding of boiling concepts. The impact of

science subjects currently studied on the heat transfer and temperature changes and

the melting and freezing conceptual groups displayed medium eta squared effect

size values of 0.10 and 0.11, respectively.

Previously Studied Science Topics Related to Thermal Concepts

Students were requested to select the science topics that they had previously studied

from a list of eight topics, namely heat, heat and cooling, heat transfer, temperature

scales, phase changes, specific heat capacity, homeostasis, and thermodynamics.

The corresponding MANOVA results for each of the topics concerned are summar-

ised in Table 9.

Heat topic. There was no significant difference across all four conceptual groups in

the mean scores of the responses to the TCE between students who had previously

studied the topic on heat and those who had not.

Table 8. Students’ mean scores in the four conceptual groups based on science subjects currently

studied, from MANOVA results

Current studying

subjects

Conceptual groups

Heat transfer and

temperature changes Boiling

Heat conductivity

and equilibrium

Melting and

freezing

Mean + SD Mean + SD Mean + SD Mean + SD

General Science

(N ¼ 178)

48.1 + 9.2a 49.9 + 11.0 47.6 + 8.9a 49.0 + 10.0b

Life Science

(N ¼ 157)

47.1 + 8.9a 50.2 + 8.9 49.0 + 7.9a 46.2 + 8.7a

Chemistry and

Physics (N ¼ 180)

54.3 + 10.2b 50.0 + 10.3 53.3 + 11.7b 54.3 + 9.5c

F 29.5∗∗ 0.03 16.6∗∗ 32.2∗∗

Eta squared 0.10 0.00 0.06 0.11

Different superscripts indicate that there are significant differences between year levels.

∗∗P ≤ 0.001

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1525

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Table 9. Students’ means scores in the four concept categories based on previously studied science topics related to thermal concepts, from MANOVAresults

Topic

Conceptual groups

Heat transferand temperature

changes Boiling

Heatconductivity

and equilibriumMelting and

freezing

Heat (Y ¼ 390,N ¼ 104)

Mean + SD Y 50.6 + 10.0 50.0 + 9.9 50.2 + 10.2 50.6 + 10.2N 49.0 + 9.9 50.0 + 10.1 49.7 + 9.8 48.9 + 10.0

F 2.3 0.00 0.2 2.4Eta squared 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.01

Heating andcooling (Y ¼ 313,N ¼ 181)

Mean + SD Y 52.0 + 10.0 50.1 + 10.1 51.0 + 10.3 50.5 + 9.7N 47.3 + 9.3 50.0 + 9.9 48.4 + 9.5 50.0 + 11.0

F 26.7∗∗ 0.01 7.3∗ 0.4Eta squared 0.05 0.00 0.02 0.00

Heat transfer(Y ¼ 283, N ¼ 211)

Mean + SD Y 52.8 + 9.9 50.8 + 10.2 51.5 + 11.1 51.2 + 10.1N 46.9 + 9.1 48.9 + 9.7 48.1 + 8.1 49.0 + 9.8

F 45.4∗∗ 4.6∗ 13.8∗∗ 5.5∗

Eta squared 0.09 0.01 0.03 0.01

Temperaturescales (Y ¼ 267,N ¼ 227)

Mean + SD Y 52.4 + 10.2 50.8 + 10.7 51.6 + 10.7 51.2 + 9.9N 47.8 + 9.2 49.1 + 9.4 48.3 + 9.0 49.2 + 10.1

F 29.9∗∗ 3.5 13.4∗∗ 5.0∗

Eta squared 0.06 0.01 0.03 0.01Phase changes(Y ¼ 102, N ¼ 392)

Mean + SD Y 55.2 + 10.1 50.4 + 10.5 55.2 + 12.0 53.8 + 9.4N 49.0 + 9.6 50.0 + 9.9 48.7 + 9.0 49.3 + 10.0

F 34.0∗∗ 0.2 36.5∗∗ 17.1∗∗

Eta squared 0.07 0.00 0.07 0.03

Specific heatcapacity (Y ¼ 244,N ¼ 250)

Mean + SD Y 53.5 + 10.1 50.3 + 10.3 51.9 + 11.2 52.3 + 9.9N 47.1 + 8.8 49.8 + 9.8 48.3 + 8.5 48.3 + 9.8

F 56.9∗∗ 0.3 15.9∗∗ 20.84∗∗

Eta squared 0.10 0.00 0.03 0.04

1526

H.-E

.C

hu

etal.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Homeostasis(Y ¼ 257, N ¼ 237)

Mean + SD Y 53.3 + 10.2 50.4 + 10.4 52.2 + 11.1 51.8 + 9.6N 47.0 + 8.6 49.6 + 9.6 47.8 + 8.3 48.6 + 10.2

F 55.5∗∗ 0.9 24.3∗∗ 12.9∗∗

Eta squared 0.10 0.00 0.05 0.03

Thermo-dynamics (Y ¼ 100,N ¼ 394)

Mean + SD Y 54.0 + 10.5 52.4 + 11.5 53.9 + 11.6 52.4 + 9.9N 49.3 + 9.6 49.4 + 9.5 49.1 + 9.4 49.7 + 10.0

F 17.6∗∗ 7.2∗ 18.8∗∗ 5.9∗

Eta squared 0.03 0.01 0.04 0.01

Y indicates ‘yes’; N indicates ‘no’.

∗P ≤ 0.05.

∗∗P ≤ 0.005.

Stu

den

ts’U

ndersta

ndin

gin

Every

day

Con

texts

1527

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Heating and cooling. Students who had previously studied this topic achieved signifi-

cantly higher mean scores in the heat transfer and temperature changes and heat con-

ductivity and equilibrium conceptual groups than those who had not. There was no

discernable difference in students’ achievement in the other two groups. The

medium-sized eta squared value for the heat transfer and temperature changes

group indicates a variance of 5% in the mean scores between students who had and

those who had not previously studied the topic. There was, however, only limited

influence on the heat conductivity and equilibrium conceptual group as indicated

by the small eta squared value of 0.02.

Heat transfer. There were significant differences in the mean scores of students who

had previously studied this topic and those who had not for all the four conceptual

groups, with the greatest influence being on the heat transfer and temperature

changes conceptual group (indicated by a medium-sized eta squared value of 0.09).

The influence on the mean scores was relatively small in all the other three groups.

Temperature scales. Students who had previously studied this topic achieved signifi-

cantly higher mean scores than those who had not done so in three conceptual

groups, other than the boiling group. Also, the greatest influence was on the heat

transfer and temperature changes group, indicated by the medium-sized eta

squared value of 0.06. A variance of only 1–3% in the mean scores of the other

three conceptual groups could be explained by the influence of this topic.

Phase changes. The trend was similar as in the previous topic, but this time medium-

sized eta squared values for heat transfer and temperature changes and the heat con-

ductivity and equilibrium conceptual groups indicate a variance of 7%, respectively, in

the mean scores between students who had and those who had not previously studied

phase changes.

Specific heat capacity. The differences in the mean scores were again significant for all

groups except the boiling conceptual group. The greatest influence was on the heat

transfer and temperature changes conceptual group, indicated by a medium eta

squared value accounting for 10% of the difference in the variance of the mean

scores of the two groups of students. Less than 4% in the variance of the difference

in the mean scores of the other three groups could be explained by the influence of

this topic.

Homeostasis. The trend was similar as in the three previous topics with the most sig-

nificant difference in the mean scores being for the heat transfer and temperature

changes conceptual group. The influence of the homeostasis topic on this group is

1528 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

evident from the medium-sized eta squared value accounting for 10% of the differ-

ence in the variance of the mean scores of the two groups of students.

Thermodynamics. There were statistically significant differences in the mean scores

of the responses to the TCE across all four conceptual groups between students

who had previously studied the topic on thermodynamics and those who had not.

The eta squared values for the these four conceptual groups indicate that only 1–

4% in the variance of the mean scores of students who had previously studied this

topic and those who had not could be explained by the influence of the thermodyn-

amics topic.

Conclusions

The aims of this research were to (1) determine the underlying conceptual structure of

the TCE, (2) investigate students’ conceptual understanding of thermal concepts in

everyday contexts across several school years, and (3) analyse the variables, such as

school year, science subjects currently being studied, and science subjects previously

studied in thermal energy, that influence students’ thermal conceptual understanding.

The Underlying Conceptual Structure of the TCE

A factor analysis of students’ responses to the 26-item TCE questionnaire of Yeo and

Zadnik (2001) resulted in a psychometric structure with 19 items comprising four

conceptual groups, namely, (1) heat transfer and temperature changes, (2) boiling,

(3) heat conductivity and equilibrium, and (4) freezing and melting. Seven items

were omitted because they did not distinctly evaluate the concepts in any of the

four conceptual groups. Items in two conceptual groups, heat transfer and tempera-

ture changes, and melting and freezing were found to have appropriate P-values

(item difficulties) and D-values (item discrimination index). However, items in the

other two conceptual groups (heat conductivity and equilibrium, and boiling) were

found to have low P-values but appropriate/high D-values. The items in these two

latter groups assessed understanding of fundamental thermal concepts that students

had previously learned in years 7–10, yet the students in this sample displayed limited

understanding. Subsequently, these four-item conceptual groups were used to evalu-

ate students’ understanding of thermal physics concepts in everyday contexts.

Students’ Conceptual Understanding of Thermal Concepts in Everyday Contexts

Three findings have emerged from this study:

(1) A lower percentage of students provided correct answers to the items in the heat

conductivity and equilibrium group (group mean: 24%) and the boiling group

(group mean: 29%) compared to the other two conceptual groups, heat transfer

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1529

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

and temperature changes (group mean: 50%) and melting and freezing (group

mean: 51%) groups.

(2) The students’ mean scores of the heat transfer and temperature changes concep-

tual group correlated significantly with the scores from all the other three concep-

tual groups, with the highest correlation being with the heat conductivity and the

equilibrium conceptual group.

(3) Several alternative conceptions from all four conceptual groups were held by stu-

dents in all years. A common alternative conception among students from all

school years involved the concepts of boiling, freezing and melting points. Stu-

dents generally did not associate these temperatures with phase changes that

could affect their values. Other typical alternative conceptions among students

from all school years included the inability to distinguish between heat (‘hot’

and ‘cold’) and temperature, overlooking the transfer of heat energy between

objects at different temperatures, and not realising that ‘hot’ and ‘cold’ sensations

are at the opposite ends of a continuum. These findings are consistent with

studies from other countries that used qualitative research methods. This

finding illustrates the utility of the TCE questionnaire in large-scale studies.

The Variables That Influence Students’ Thermal Conceptual Understanding

Conceptual understanding related to the four conceptual groups was found to be

influenced by students’ years of schooling, the subjects currently studied and pre-

viously studied physics topics about thermal energy. However, the influences were

limited to certain conceptual groups and also, the strengths of impact were weak,

even though the influences of school years and previous studied physics topics were

significant. Two findings have emerged from this study:

(1) The influence of students’ school years was limited to the heat transfer and temp-

erature changes and heat conductivity and equilibrium conceptual groups, but

the impact of school years on conceptual understanding in these two groups

was small. However, the currently studied science subjects had a large influence

on the heat, transfer and temperature changes as well as melting and freezing con-

ceptual groups in this research, but had a very limited impact on the heat conduc-

tivity and equilibrium conceptual group.

(2) Most of the previously studied physics topics significantly influenced thermal

conceptual understanding in all conceptual groups except the boiling conceptual

group. The reason was that the boiling concept in the TCE questionnaire was

highly related to macroscopic understanding. Students first had to understand

boiling in the normal air pressure and then consider the microscopic understand-

ing of phase changes. However, according to Luera, Otto, and Zitzewitz (2006),

some item distracters in the TCE do not indicate which concepts students do not

understand. The heat topic did not have a significant influence on any of the con-

ceptual groups and the heating and cooling topic did not show any significant

influence on the boiling, melting and freezing conceptual group. Most topics

1530 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

had a moderate effect on the heat transfer and temperature change conceptual

group (eta squared values from 0.06 to 0.10). Most previously studied physics

topics had limited effect on the heat conductivity and the equilibrium group

except for the ‘phase changes’ topic that had a moderate effect (eta squared

value ¼ 0.07). Most physics topics that students had previously studied had

limited effect on the melting and freezing conceptual group (eta squared value:

0.01–0.04).

Implications

The findings of this study have several implications for classroom physics instruction

as well as for future research in physics education, especially in Korea. At a general

level, the outcomes of the study provide indicators to teachers and curriculum devel-

opers in their planning of classroom instruction based on the needs of students in

different achieving groups and science curriculum based on students’ understanding

in different contexts. Even though students displayed a better understanding when

they had more learning opportunities in school, several alternative conceptions

seemed particularly resilient to change, e.g. boiling water in normal conditions.

Korean students continue to do extremely well in the trends in international math-

ematics and science study (TIMSS) since 1995 (Martin, Mullis, & Foy, 2008). In the

Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2006, Korean students

were also placed statistically significantly above the OECD average (OECD, 2007).

The TIMSS tests are directed towards year 8 and the PISA tests towards 15 years

old. Further TIMSS tests are more likely to include items based on the taught curri-

culum (such as items for asking scientific knowledge about knowing, applying and

reasoning) and it might be expected that the Korean students in this study would

perform well when the items are based on topics related to the taught curriculum.

As shown in Tables 2 and 3 (the boiling conceptual group and the heat conductivity

equilibrium conceptual group), this appears not to be the case with relatively low

response rates across all three years 10–12. As noted in the Introduction, the

Korean national curriculum has incorporated an STS emphasis and the constructivist

classroom has been emphasised and scientific literacy highlighted (Ministry of Edu-

cation, 1992a, 1992b, 1997). However, research about a decade ago showed that tea-

chers continue to maintain highly discipline-centred traditional teaching and provide

students with little experience of physics questions based on everyday life (Kim, 2001;

Kwon, 2001; Oh & Kim, 2001). The teachers in this sample would appear to be con-

tinuing with this same style of teaching. Consequently, this study has provided an

important baseline as to how well Korean students answer questions based on every-

day phenomena related to thermal concepts and upon which further similar studies

can be based. Using the original TCE questionnaire, similar findings of conceptual

understanding difficulties in everyday contexts have been reported by Thai secondary

students (Kruatong, Sung-ong, & Jones, 2006), by Thai and Australian university stu-

dents (Georgiou, Sharma, O’Byrne, Sefton, & McInnes, 2009), and by university stu-

dents in the USA (Luera, Otto, & Zitzewitz, 2006).

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1531

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

If the Korean Ministry of Education wishes to continue with an emphasis on STS

and the constructivist classroom highlighting scientific literacy, it may be useful to

examine the context-based curricula with extensive student materials developed in

England, the Netherlands, USA, and Germany, beginning almost 30 years ago

(Nentwig & Waddington, 2005). In research studies, the comparison is between stu-

dents’ achievement in the traditional course with a context-based course at the end of

the secondary school examination. A review of such comparisons by Bennett,

Lubben, and Hogarth (2007) showed higher achievement in context–based

courses, but with a note that the teachers involved in these courses are interested

and deem the courses to have potential benefits to their students. However, in

reviews of these context-based courses, there is no commentary of the types of ques-

tions in the examinations. Indeed, evaluation of student achievement using context-

based items seems to be less well developed and reported than the curricula. We

suspect this is part of the explanation of the less than expected achievement of the

Korean sample on this study.

Further research is planned to analyse why students in years 10–12 have difficulty

in understanding or using these thermal concepts when applied in everyday life con-

texts. An open-ended questionnaire will be developed for which students will be

required to provide explanations for their selections of responses to the items and

student interviews will also be conducted. Recently conducted studies have shown

that conceptual tests can be effective tools for improving students’ conceptual learn-

ing (Chang, Yeh, & Barufaldi, 2010; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006). In keeping with

these recent findings, further research should be considered in evaluating the efficacy

of the TCE as a means of facilitating students’ understanding of thermal concepts in

everyday contexts. These students’ understanding can be used for the context-based

programme the development and the developing teaching materials for teachers.

References

Andersson, B. (1980). Some aspects of children’s understanding of boiling point. In W.F. Archen-

hold, R. Driver, A. Orton, & C. Wood-Robinson (Eds.), Cognitive Development Research in

Science and Mathematics: Proceedings of an International Seminar (pp. 17–21). Leeds: Center

for Studies in Science Education, University of Leeds.

Arnold, M., & Millar, R. (1994). Children’s and lay adults’ views about thermal equilibrium. Inter-

national Journal of Science Education, 4(16), 405–419.

Bar, V., & Travis, A.S. (1991). Children’s views concerning phase changes. Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 28(4), 363–382.

Bennett, J., Lubben, F., & Hogarth, S. (2007). Bringing science to life: A synthesis of the research

evidence on the effects of context-based and STS approaches to science teaching. Science Edu-

cation, 91(3), 347–370.

Carey, S. (1991). Knowledge acquisition: Enrichment or conceptual change? In S. Carey & R.

Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesist of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 257–291). Hillsdale,

NJ: Erlbaum.

Chang, C.-Y., Yeh, T.-K., & Barufaldi, J.P. (2010). The positive and negative effects of science

concept tests on student conceptual understanding. International Journal of Science Education,

32(2), 265–282.

1532 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Chi, M.T.H., Slotta, J.D., & de Leeuw, N. (1994). From things to processes: A theory of conceptual

change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4, 27–43.

Chiu, G.-L., & Anderson, O.R. (2010). A multi-dimensional cognitive analysis of undergraduate

physics students’ understanding of heat conduction. International Journal of Science Education,

32(16), 2113–2142.

Chu, H.-E., Treagust, D.F., & Chandrasegaran, A.L. (2009). A stratified study of students’ under-

standing of basic optics in different context using two-tier multiple-choice items. Research in

Science & Technological Education, 27(3), 253–265.

Clough, E.E., & Driver, R. (1986). A study of consistency in the use of students’ conceptual frame-

works across different task context. Science Education, 70(4), 473–496.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Comrey, A.L., & Lee, H.B. (1992). A first course in factor analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum.

Duit, R. (2009). Students’ and teachers’ conceptions and science education. Retrieved August 13, 2009,

from http://www.ipn.uni-kiel.de/aktuell/stcse/stcse.html

Erickson, G.L. (1979). Children’s conceptions of heat and temperature. Science Education, 63(2),

221–230.

Georgiou, H., Sharma, M., O’Byrne, J., Sefton, I., & McInnes, B. (2008). University students’ con-

ceptions about familiar thermodynamic process and the implications for instruction. The Uni-

Serve Science 2008 Conference proceedings: Retrieved December 21, 2009, from http://

science.uniserve.edu.au/

Hair, J.F., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., & Black, W.C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.).

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Harrison, A.G., Grayson, D.J., & Treagust, D.F. (1999). Investigating a grade 11 student’s evolving

conceptions of heat and temperature. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(1), 55–87.

Hewitt, P.G. (2006). Conceptual physics. Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.

Horn, J.L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika,

30, 179–185.

Kaper, W.H., & Goedhart, M.J. (2002). ‘Forms of energy’, an intermediary language on the road to

thermodynamics? Part I. International Journal of Science Education, 24(1), 81–95.

Kesidou, S., & Duit, R. (1993). Students’ conceptions of the second law of thermodynamics: An

interpretive study. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30, 85–106.

Kesidou, S., Duit, R., & Glynn, S.M. (1995). Conceptual understanding in physics: Students’

understanding of heat. In S.M. Glynn & R. Duit (Eds.), Learning science in the schools: Research

reforming practice (pp. 179–198). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Kim, J.-H. (2001). The characteristics of the 7th national science curriculum of the republic of

Korea. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 21(5), 1012–1026.

Kline, P. (1994). An easy guide to factor analysis. New York: Routledge.

Kruatong, T., Sung-Ong, S., Singh, P., & Jones, A. (2006). Thai high school students’ understand-

ing of heat and thermodynamics. Journal of Kasetsart, 27, 321–330.

Kwon, J. (2001). The problems of Korean school science curriculum and suggestions for a new

direction. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 21(5), 968–978.

Lewis, E.L., & Linn, M.C. (1994). Heat energy and temperature concepts of adolescents, adults,

and experts: Implications for curricular improvements. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,

31(6), 657–677.

Lien, A.J. (1971). Measurement and evaluation in learning (2nd ed.). Dubuque, IA: William

C. Brown.

Luera, G.R., Otto, C.A., & Zitzewitz, P.W. (2006). Use of the thermal concept evaluation to focus

instruction. The Physics Teacher, 44(3), 162–166.

Martin, M.O., Mullis, I., & Foy, P. (2008). International science report. Boston, MA: TIMSS &

PIRLS International Study Center.

Students’ Understanding in Everyday Contexts 1533

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013

Meltzer, D.E. (2004). Investigation of students’ reasoning regarding heat, work, and the first law of

thermodynamics in an introductory calculus-based general physics course. American Journal of

Physics, 72(11), 1432–1453.

Ministry of Education. (1992a). The 6th national middle school curriculum. Seoul: Author.

Ministry of Education. (1992b). The 6th national high school curriculum. Seoul: Author.

Ministry of Education. (1997). The 7th national science curriculum. Seoul: Author.

Nentwig, P., & Waddington, D. (Eds.). (2005). Making it relevant: Context based learning of science.

Munster: Waxman.

Nunally, J.C., & Bernstein, I.H. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Oh, W., & Kim, J. (2001). Conceptual change via contrasting everyday and scientifically idealized

contexts. Journal of the Korean Association for Research in Science Education, 21(5), 822–840.

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2007). PISA 2006: Science competen-

cies for tomorrow’s world. Volume 1: Analysis and Volume 2: DATA. Retrieved December 20, 2009,

from http://www.oecd.org

Osborne, R., & Freyberg, P. (1985). Learning in science: The implications of children’s science.

Auckland: Heinemann.

Roediger, H.L., & Karpicke, J.D. (2006). Test enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves

long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249–255.

Sciarretta, M.R., Stilli, R., & Vicentini Missoni, M. (1990). On the thermal properties of materials:

Common-sense knowledge of Italian students and teachers. International Journal of Science Edu-

cation, 12(4), 369–379.

Stevens, J. (2002). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tiberghien, A. (1985). Part B: The development of ideas with teaching. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, &

A. Tiberghien (Eds.), Children’s ideas in science. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.

Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modelling the process of conceptual change. Learning and

Instruction, 4, 45–69.

Watkins, M.W. (2000). Monte Carlo PCA for parallel analysis (computer software). State College, PA:

Ed & Psych Associates.

White, R.T., & Gunstone, R.F. (1989). Metalearning and conceptual change. International Journal

of Science Education, 11(5), 577–586.

Wiser, M., & Amin, T. (2001). Is heat hot? Inducing conceptual change by integrating everyday and

scientific perspectives on thermal phenomena. Learning and Instruction, 11(4–5), 331–355.

Wiser, M., & Carey, S. (1983). When heat and temperature were one. In D. Gentner & A. Stevens

(Eds.), Mental models (pp. 267–297). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Yeo, S., & Zadnik, M. (2001). Introductory thermal concept evaluation: Assessing students’ under-

standing. The Physics Teacher, 39, 496–504.

1534 H.-E. Chu et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ite D

e Pa

ris

1] a

t 01:

30 0

3 Se

ptem

ber

2013