26
Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 1 Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell: Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System? Felicia Ghrist August 22, 2014 Wayne State University Advisor: Ann Stacks, Ph.D.

Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 1

Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell: Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of

Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System?

Felicia Ghrist

August 22, 2014

Wayne State University

Advisor: Ann Stacks, Ph.D.

Page 2: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 2

Abstract

Child abuse and neglect can result in the state taking custody of children and requiring

their parents to improve caregiving skills and ensure child safety. Improvements they make are

often judged subjectively by caseworkers who observe parenting time. Without an objective

measure of parenting for social workers to use, critical decisions about reunification and custody

can be based on personal opinions. The PICCOLO is an inexpensive, accessible and objective

measure that may be useful in child welfare. Unfortunately, the PICCOLO has not been tested in

a sample of maltreated dyads. This study compares parent-child interactions using the

PICCOLO and the Crowell procedure using 10 pre- and post-test video recorded parent-child

dyads (20 total videos) participating in the Wayne County Baby Court Project. Results suggest

that the PICCOLO subscales correlate with Crowell subscales in a maltreated sample. Further

research is needed to examine if the PICCOLO is indeed an alternative, objective measure for

community programs that service the child welfare system.

Page 3: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 3

Introduction

The Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observations linked to Outcomes

(PICCOLO) is a newly developed assessment tool that is becoming popular with clinicians

because of the short amount of time required to become reliable, relative low cost, and freedom

of use in a variety of settings. It has been studied with many populations such as minorities,

fathers, and children with disabilities. The PICCOLO has not been validated for use with

maltreating dyads and current practice does not include using standardized assessments of

parenting during parenting time in child welfare. Typically the foster care worker, who may not

have any formal training in assessing parent-child interactions, will report to the court the quality

of parenting. Assessing parenting in maltreating samples is important because sensitive parenting

is associated with positive developmental outcomes, while maltreatment is associated with

poorer developmental outcomes. Interruptions in child development-especially in infancy and the

toddler years-can have long lasting consequences in language, cognitive, behavioral development

and mental health. Evaluating sensitive parenting across contexts allows caseworkers an

objective way to observe and measure parenting and improvements in parenting as a result of

services. It can also help the child welfare system to document well-being.

Relational Context of Development in Infancy

Sensitive parenting includes specific behaviors such as responsiveness and emotional

support. These behaviors provide the child with an opportunity to explore, understand and

practice language in a safe, stimulating, collaborative environment (Pungello, Iruka, Dotterer,

Page 4: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 4

Mills-Koonce & Reznick, 2009) and as a result support the development of cognition, language

and behavior.

Language Development

Language development in infancy, including infants understanding of language (receptive

language) and use of words (expressive language) is associated with positive outcomes

throughout schooling. For example, oral language at age 3 is associated with early reading skills,

which are further associated with high school graduation (Hernandez, 2011; National Institute of

Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Early Child Care Research Network, 2005).

Demographic risks (low SES and parental education) and parenting are associated with language

development in infancy. To better understand how demographic risk and parenting interact to

influence language, Pungello and colleagues (2009) conducted a longitudinal study with parent-

infant dyads beginning when the infant was 12 -months and ending when the infants were 36

months. This study assessed parenting at 12- and 24-months and observed associations with

growth in language. More specifically, children with sensitive mothers had higher receptive and

expressive communication scores and showed an accelerated rate of receptive and expressive

growth from 18 to 36 months compared to children whose mothers were less sensitive when their

children were infants. This work supported earlier findings that both maternal and paternal

sensitivity, positive regard, and cognitive stimulation are associated with higher language scores

at age 2 and 3-years (Tamis-Lemonda, Shannon, Cabrera & Lamb, 2004). Parents who provide

cognitive stimulation label objects, explain the function of objects, and ask questions and expect

an answer. Pancsofar & Vernon-Feagans, (2006) showed that fathers who use a variety of word

roots and ask more open-ended questions have children with higher expressive language scores.

Page 5: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 5

Cognitive Development

Like language development, infants’ cognitive development is influenced by

demographic and parenting factors. A series of studies using the Early Head Start Research and

Evaluation Project examined the effects of demographic risk and parenting on child cognitive

outcomes across infancy. Findings suggest that maternal and paternal sensitivity, positive

regard, and cognitive stimulation are associated with higher cognitive scores at age 2 and 3-

years, after controlling for demographic risk. (Tamis-Lemonda, et al, 2004). Similarly, Lugo-Gil

and Tamis-Lemonda (2008) found that quality of parenting mediated the association between

family economic resources and children’s cognitive development at 14, 24 and 36 months. In this

study they also found that parenting remained stable over time. Provision of cognitive

stimulation when the child was 4 years old was associated with children’s cognitive ability at age

8, whereas the use of threats and coercion to obtain desired behavior was associated with lower

cognitive ability (Byford, Kuh, & Richards, 2011).

Behavior and Mental Health

A large body of literature suggests that parenting is also a strong predictor of early

behavior and mental health. The development of a secure attachment is an important social-

emotional milestone in an infant’s first-year of life; decades of research confirm the relationship

between sensitive parenting and a secure attachment (Nievar & Becker, 2008). A secure

attachment in turn is associated with children’s curiosity, persistence, self-regulation, interest in

their environment, more positive social behavior and peer competence throughout schooling

(Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006). The context in which a parent is sensitive is important. For

Page 6: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 6

example, sensitivity to a child’s distress may be more predictive of early behavior and social

outcomes than sensitivity to the infant’s neutral or positive affect. Leerkes, Blankson, &

O’Brian, (2009) found that mother’s sensitivity to their infants’ distress at 6 months of age was

associated with increased social competence at 24 months and fewer behavior problems at 24

and 36 months. Leerkes (2011) also found that only sensitivity during a distressing task

predicted attachment security. While positive parenting facilitates optimal development in

infancy, parenting that is abusive or neglectful is associated with poor infant outcomes

(Eisenburg et al., 2005).

Poor Outcomes Associated with Infant Maltreatment

Infants and toddlers who are maltreated exhibit high rates of behavioral and

developmental problems (Smyke & Breidenstine, 2008) that can result in long term negative

outcomes such as: school failure, mental health problems, and serious physical health problems

(Gilbert, et al., 2009). Child maltreatment includes physical abuse, neglect, and psychological

maltreatment. Physical abuse includes physical acts that cause or could cause physical injury to

a child, for example burning, shaking, or punching. Neglect is defined as the failure of a

caregiver to provide for the child’s basic needs, including food and shelter. Psychological

maltreatment is an act or omission in parenting, for example emotional or verbal abuse, that

causes or could cause developmental harm (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,

2012).

Baseline data from the National Survey of Child and Adolescent Well-Being II (NSCAW

II) was used to examine indicators of well-being of children who had come into contact with the

child welfare system (Casanuva, Dolan, Smith, Ringeisen, & Dowd, 2012). Results from this

study suggest that infants and toddlers reported for maltreatment have high rates of

Page 7: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 7

developmental concerns. For example, nearly half of the children in this sample of 5,873 were

under the age of 5. Children under 72 months had mean scores on Preschool Language Scale

(PLS-3) that were one standard deviation below the normative mean and 18.7% had scores that

were 2 standard deviations or more below the mean. Children under 48 months had mean scores

on the Battelle Development Inventory, 2nd addition (BDI-2) that were about one-half of one

standard deviation below the normative mean and 18.7% had scores that were 2 or more standard

deviations below the mean.

Another study using data from NSCAW II found that just over 1/3 of infants 12 to 18

months-old demonstrated social and emotional problems and just over 1/5 had low social

competence scores (McCue et al., 2012). This study supports previous findings related to social

and emotional difficulties among young children with maltreatment histories. For example, one

study found 82% of maltreated children were classified with a disorganized pattern of attachment

(Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett, & Braunwald, 1989). Abusive parenting and frequent placement

changes may contribute to this high rate of disorganized attachment. For example, frightening

parental behavior has been linked to the development of disorganized patterns of attachment in

infancy (Madigan, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van Ijzendoorn, Moran, Pederson, & Benoit (2006),

which in turn is associated with deviant levels of hostility and aggressive behavior towards adults

and peers (Lyons-Ruth, 1996) and behavior problems as toddlers (Madigan, Moran, Schuengal,

Pederson, & Otten, 2007). Furthermore, continued disruptions in primary caregiving, as is often

the case for infants in the child welfare system, can negatively impact an infant’s ability to form

a secure attachment relationship and to manage their behavior and emotions (Dozier, Zeanah,

&Bernard, 2013). These social and emotional risks put children at risk for later mental health

problems. A longitudinal analysis by Thompson & Tabone (2010) found that compared to a

Page 8: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 8

demographically at-risk sample without alleged maltreatment, child maltreatment prior to age 4

predicted increased levels of anxiety, depression and attention problems through age 10.

The Child Welfare System’s Mandate to Support Well-Being

The studies above showcase how maltreated children’s well-being is compromised

despite the fact that the child welfare system (CWS) is required to ensure children’s safety,

permanency, and well-being. Great strides have been made to ensure both permanency and

safety, but less reporting has been done in the area of child well-being (Casanueva et al., 2012).

To support infant well-being, the courts and the child welfare system must place a greater

emphasis on parenting and offer effective programming that can show measureable differences

in parenting. Most parents in the child welfare system are required to attend a parenting

intervention, but are not required to demonstrate improvement (Barth et al., 2005). In order to

recognize improvements in parenting, assessments of skills must be done before and after

intervention.

Assessments allow social workers to know their clients’ strengths and limitations in order

to target interventions and to report changes in parent-child interactions to the court (Hurlburt,

Nguyen, Reid, Webster-Stratton & Zhang, 2013). Unfortunately, the best observational methods

to assess improvement in parenting were developed for research and are too costly and require

extensive training (Roggman et al, 2013a). As a result, they are impractical for community

intervention programs where time and funding are often lacking (Beatty et al., 2011). Judges

presiding over child welfare cases want objective measures when making a decision about

whether or not it is safe to send a child home (Edwards, 1994). It is critical that an objective and

practical measure of parenting is integrated into child welfare reporting (Dozier, Zeanah, &

Bernard, 2013).

Page 9: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 9

A Promising Measure of Parenting for the Child Welfare System: The PICCOLO

A newly developed parenting assessment tool, The Parenting Interactions with Children:

Checklist of Observable Outcomes (PICCOLO), was developed for practitioners to use with

families at high demographic risk. It may be ideal for use in the child welfare system, because

training is relatively quick and cost effective and has been tested with a diverse group of over

2000 low-income families. Impoverished and minority families are over-represented in the child

welfare system. Research on this assessment suggests that scores on the PICCOLO are correlated

with secure attachment and other positive outcomes (Roggman et al., 2013a), but have not been

used with maltreated samples or been compared to well-established, gold-standard assessments

designed for research with maltreating dyads.

The purpose of this study is to further validate the PICCOLO for use with maltreating

dyads by comparing scores on the PICCOLO to those on the Crowell (Heller, Aokie, &

Schoffner (1998), and assessing whether adding a challenging task to the PICCOLO might be

advantageous. Challenging tasks are more likely to elicit negative emotions from children and

assessing parent sensitivity to negative emotions is most predictive of children’s social-

emotional outcomes (Leerkes, et al., 2009; Leerkes, 2011).

Research Hypotheses

The present study explores the correlations among subscales on the PICCOLO and

Crowell parenting assessments. It is hypothesized that:

(1) PICCOLO scores assessed during free play and teaching tasks will be correlated with

Crowell scores. More specifically, it is expected that:

Page 10: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 10

(1a) The Affection subscale on the PICCOLO will be positively correlated with the

Positive Affect and Emotional Responsiveness subscales on the Crowell.

(1b) The Responsiveness subscale on the PICCOLO will be positively correlated with the

Emotional Responsiveness Subscale on the Crowell.

(1c) The Encouragement subscale on the PICCOLO will be positively correlated with the

Emotional Responsiveness subscale on the Crowell.

(1d) The Teaching subscale on the PICCOLO will be positively correlated with the Behavioral

Responsiveness Subscale on the Crowell.

(2) Compared to PICCOLO scores assessed during free play, PICCOLO scores assessed during

the teaching task will be more strongly correlated with Crowell subscale scores.

(2a) The Affection subscale on the PICCOLO, coded during the teaching task will be most

strongly correlated with the Positive Affect and Emotional Responsiveness subscales of the

Crowell.

(2b) The Responsiveness subscale on the PICCOLO, coded during the teaching task will be most

strongly correlated with the Emotional Responsiveness Subscale of the Crowell.

(2c) The Encouragement subscale on the PICCOLO, coded during the teaching task, will be most

strongly correlated with the Emotional Responsiveness subscale of the Crowell.

(2d) The Teaching subscale on the PICCOLO, coded during the teaching task, will be most

strongly correlated with the Behavioral Responsiveness Subscale of the Crowell.

Page 11: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 11

Method

Study Description

Analyses in this study were based on 20 previously recorded videos of 10 parent-child dyads

(each dyad completed a pre- and post-test) participating in the Wayne County Baby Court

Project. The goal of Baby Court is to ensure that infants and toddlers in the child welfare system

grow up in permanent nurturing homes that support optimal development and that parents,

whenever possible, are the infant’s permanent caregiver. The project follows the Miami Child

Well-Being Model, which is anchored on the following principles: (1) the needs of vulnerable

infants and toddlers are best met by a collaborative court informed by the science of child

development, (2) young children exposed to maltreatment require evidence informed therapeutic

intervention that addressed the parent-child relationship directly and (3) the decision-making

process is improved when special insight regarding the child’s well-being, the parent-child

relationship, and the parent’s ability to protect and care for the child is made available (Miami

Child Well-Being Court Initiative, 2013).

Parent-child dyads are eligible for Baby Court if the infant is under court jurisdiction as a

result of maltreatment, and the parent is working toward reunification, and does not suffer from

serious untreated substance abuse. As part of the study evaluation participants participate in a

parenting assessment at a university research lab. This occurs shortly after the infant is placed in

foster care, and again before the court decides whether it is safe for the child to return home,

approximately 10 months after the pre-test. During the assessment, parents are videotaped

interacting with their children and are interviewed. A clinician and member of the evaluation

team, trained in the Crowell, codes the videos.

Page 12: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 12

For this project, pre- and post-test videos were re-coded using the PICCOLO so that PICCOLO

scores could be compared to already assigned Crowell scores. The PICCOLO coders were blind

to study IDs, pre- and post-test status, and Crowell scores.

Sample Characteristics

Ten parent-infant dyads (2 fathers, 8 mothers) participated in this study. Parents ranged in

age from 15-23 years (M =19.7 years), 100% were African-American, and the majority were

single, never married (80%). Parents’ highest grade completed ranged from 9th grade to two

years in college (M=10.9), in general parents were very low income; and the mean annual

income was $8,602.50 (range $0-$20,040). Among the children, just over half were female

(60%), and they ranged in age from 11 to 32 months (M= 20.5), all were African-American. At

the time of the pretest 20% of the infants were living with their biological parent, 30% were

living with a relative, and 50% were living in a foster home.

Measures

Demographic data, including parent and infant age when the petition was filed, race, level

of education, and history in the child welfare system were collected via interview during the pre-

test laboratory visit and a court records review after the case was closed.

The Crowell Procedure (Heller et al., 1998) was used to assess parent-child interactions

in the Baby Court Project. The Crowell is a 40 minute parent-child interaction task that is

comprised of 8 episodes/tasks. First, parents are asked to play as they would at home for 10

minutes with previously selected toys that promote pretend play (free play). Next, they are asked

to have their child clean up the toys (clean up task). In the third task, the parent is asked to blow

bubbles and have the child pop them. Next, they engage in four time-limited teaching tasks that

Page 13: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 13

get progressively harder so that the assessor can see how the parent responds when the child is

frustrated. The interaction is video-taped and each episode is scored along seven child domains

and five parent domains. These episode scores yield one overall score for each domain. Scores

for each domain and the overall score range from 1 (poor) to 7 (excellent). Only the parent

domains will be used in this study. These domains are: Behavioral Responsiveness, Emotional

Responsiveness, Positive Affect, Withdrawn/Depressed, and Irritability/Anger. Behavioral

Responsiveness assesses whether the parent’s responses to the child’s needs are developmentally

appropriate. Emotional Responsiveness assesses emotional support and attention given to child

regarding assigned tasks. Positive Affect assesses parent’s positive interaction with the child.

Withdrawn/Depressed assesses parent’s negative interaction with the child. Irritability/Anger

assesses parent’s irritability, anger, and/or hostility displayed toward the child.

Each Crowell videotape was re-coded using the PICCOLO (Roggman et al., 2013b),

which assesses four domains of parent behavior during a 10 minute parent-child free play.

Following the work of Leerkes and colleagues (2009, 2011) in this study the PICCOLO coding

was also used on the most difficult teaching task to assess parenting in a challenging task that

pulls for child negative emotion. The PICCOLO contains twenty-nine items that are evaluated

on a 3-point scale ranging from 0 (behavior is absent) to 2 (behavior is clearly seen) and then

summed for a total domain score; higher scores indicate more of the behavior. These domains

are: Affection (7 items), Responsiveness (7 items), Encouragement (7 items), and Teaching (8

items). Affection scores represent the degree to which the parent displays warmth, physical

closeness, and positive expressions toward the child. The Responsiveness subscale indicates the

parent’s response to child’s cues, emotions, words, interests, and behavior. Encouragement

assesses the degree to which the parent actively supports explorations, effort, skills, initiative,

Page 14: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 14

curiosity, and play shown in child. The Teaching subscale looks at shared conversation and play

between the parent and child, the parent’s provision of cognitive stimulation and explanations

and whether the parent asks the child questions.

Results

Analysis Plan and Descriptive Statistics

Prior to any analyses all study variable were inspected for normality. The

Withdrawn/Depressed scale on the Crowell was skewed and a log transformation corrected the

skew. The transformed variable was used in the correlational analyses. Descriptive statistics for

Crowell and PICCOLO subscale scores are reported in Table 1

Correlations between the PICCOLO and the Crowell

Correlations were used to assess the relationship among subscales of the PICCOLO and

the Crowell. It was expected in hypothesis 1a that Affection subscale of the PICCOLO

measured during free play and teaching would be positively correlated with the Positive Affect

and Emotional Responsiveness on the Crowell. This hypothesis was partially supported. There

was a significant relationship between Affection coded in free play and Positive Affect (r = .54,

p < .05). There was a significant relationship between Affection coded in teaching and

Emotional Responsiveness (r = .56, p < .01) and Positive Affect ( r = .69, p < .001). Contrary to

the hypothesis, Affection coded in free play was not significantly correlated with Emotional

Responsiveness.

Page 15: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 15

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Parenting Variables

Subscale Name N Mean SD Range Possible Range

PICCOLO

Affection FP 19 10 2.03 7-14 0-14 Affection TT 20 8.5 3.43 2-13 0-14

Responsiveness FP 19 7.89 2.62 5-14 0-14 Responsiveness TT 20 7.15 2.91 2-11 0-14 Encouragement FP 19 6.89 2.23 3-10 0-14

Encouragement TT 20 7.10 3.83 2-13 0-14 Teaching FP 19 8.53 2.41 5-12 0-16

Teaching TT 20 4.65 2.98 1-11 0-16 Crowell

Behavioral Resp. 20 3.65 .93 2-5 0-7

Emotional Resp. 20 4.25 .97 3-6 0-7 Positive Affect 20 4.95 .95 3-6 0-7

Withdraw/ Depressed 20 1.55 .83 1-3 0-7 Irritability/Anger 20 1.80 1.11 1-4 0-7

*Note FP= Free Play; TT=Teaching Task

It was expected in hypothesis 1b that Responsiveness subscale on the PICCOLO and the

Crowell Emotional Responsiveness subscale would be positively correlated. The hypothesis was

partially supported. The only significant relationship was between Responsiveness coded during

the teaching task and Emotional Responsiveness (r = .62, p < .01). It was expected in

hypotheses 1c that the Encouragement subscale on the PICCOLO and the Crowell Emotional

Responsiveness subscale would be positively correlated. This hypotheses was partially

supported. The only significant relationship was between Encouragement coded in the teaching

task and Emotional Responsiveness (r = .54, p < .05). It was expected in hypotheses 1d that

Teaching subscale on the PICCOLO and the Crowell Behavioral Responsiveness subscale would

be positively correlated. These hypotheses were not supported. Correlation statistics for

Crowell and PICCOLO subscale scores assessed in free play are reported in Table 2.

Page 16: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 16

Table 2. Correlations among Coded PICCOLO Free Play and Crowell Domains

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Affection --

2. Responsiveness .63** --

3. Encouragement .49* .62** --

4. Teaching .53* .52* .60** --

5. Behavioral Resp. -.09 -.13 .14 .28 --

6. Emotional Resp. .34 .07 .37 .54* .80** --

7. Pos. Affect .54* .46* .54* .46* .40 .65** --

8. Withdraw/Dep -.33 -.13 -.29 -.39 -.56* -.71** -.71** --

9. Irritability/Anger -.49* -.35 -.72** -.22 -.17 -.44 -.62** .42 --

Note. *p < .05, ** p < .01. Variable Definitions: 1.Affection 2. Responsiveness 3. Encouragement 4. Teaching 5. Behavioral

Responsiveness 6. Emotional Responsiveness 7. Positive Affect 8. Withdrawn/ Depressed 9. Irritability/Anger

Table 3. Correlations among Coded PICCOLO Teaching Task and Crowell Domains

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1. Affection -- 2. Responsiveness .84** --

3. Encouragement .72** .79** -- 4. Teaching .65** .70** .72** --

5. Behavioral Resp. .49* .53* .43 .37 -- 6. Emotional Resp. .56** .62** .54* .56** .80** --

7. Pos. Affect .69** .58** .43 .54* .40 .65** -- 8. Withdraw/Dep -.61** -.61** -.41 -.43 -.56* -.71** -.71** --

9. Irritability/Anger -.71** -.70** -.71** -.68** -.17 -.44 -.62** .42 --

Note. *p < .05, ** p < .01. Variable Definitions: 1. Affection 2. Responsiveness 3. Encouragement 4. Teaching 5. Behavioral Responsiveness 6. Emotional Responsiveness 7. Positive Affect 8. Withdraw/Depressed 9. Irritability/Anger

Page 17: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 17

Parenting Assessed during Free Play versus a Teaching Task

The next set of hypotheses addressed whether it is important to assess parenting in a

variety of contexts. Hypothesis 2a, that Affection subscale on the PICCOLO coded during the

teaching task would be more strongly correlated with Positive Affect and Emotional

Responsiveness than the Affection subscale coded during free play and this was supported. The

Affection subscale coded during teaching was more strongly correlated with Emotional

Responsiveness and Positive Affect (r = .56, p < .01; r = .69, p < .001, respectively) than it

was when assessed in a free play episode (r = .34, NS; r = .54, p < .05, respectively). It was

expected in hypothesis 2b that the Responsiveness subscale on the PICCOLO coded during the

teaching task would be more strongly correlated with the Emotional Responsiveness subscale of

the Crowell than Responsiveness coded during free play. This hypothesis was supported. For

the Responsiveness subscale, scores for the teaching task were more strongly correlated with

Emotional Responsiveness (r = .62, p < .01) than those coded during the free play episode (r =

.07, NS). It was expected in hypothesis 2c that the Encouragement subscale on the PICCOLO

coded during the teaching task would be more strongly correlated with the Emotional

Responsiveness subscale of the Crowell than the Emotional Responsiveness coded during free

play. This hypothesis was supported. The Encouragement subscale coded during the teaching

task was more strongly correlated with Emotional Responsiveness (r = .54, p < .05) than it was

when coded during free play episode (r = .37, NS). It was expected in hypothesis 2d that the

Teaching subscale of the PICCOLO coded during the teaching task would be more strongly

correlated with the Behavioral Responsiveness subscale of the Crowell than the Behavioral

Responsiveness coded during free play. This hypothesis was not supported. For the teaching

subscale in both the teaching task and free play there were no significant correlations (r = .37,

Page 18: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 18

NS; r = .28, NS, respectively). Correlation statistics for Crowell and PICCOLO subscale scores

assessed during the teaching task are reported in Table 3.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate if the PICCOLO, a brief measure of

parenting interactions, is a valid assessment of parenting in maltreating samples by comparing it

to the Crowell, a more in-depth research measure. As expected, many of the PICCOLO scores

were highly correlated with the Crowell scores and compared to the PICCOLO scores assessed

in free play, those assessed in the teaching task scores were more strongly correlated with

Crowell overall scores.

It is important that offending parents of maltreated children be objectively observed to

measure positive parenting behaviors and the PICCOLO appears to be a useful measure of

parenting for maltreating dyads. Using the PICCOLO to assess strengths and changes in

parenting as a results of services may help case workers and judges to incorporate parenting, an

indicator of well-being, into their permanency decisions. It is important to remember that no cut-

off has been established on the PICCOLO for maltreating dyads so PICCOLO scores alone

should not be used as a basis to make permanency decisions.

The findings also suggest that adding a teaching or challenging task to the protocol may

be beneficial. The teaching task is designed to challenge the dyad by asking the parent to have

the child complete a task that is above his or her developmental level. These tasks often elicit

negative emotion from the child. Parent support of children’s negative emotions has important

implications for development (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998). The findings from this

study which suggest stronger correlations between teaching tasks and overall Crowell scores and

Page 19: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 19

findings by Leerkes and colleagues (2011) which suggested that parent’s sensitivity measured in

a challenging task verses sensitivity in free play was more predictive of child well-being indicate

that for parents in the child welfare system.

One advantage of the PICCOLO is that it assesses teaching behaviors, which are

associated with children’s cognitive, language, and socioemotional development (Roggman et

al., 2013a). This is important because of the high rates of development delays in infants and

toddlers reported for maltreatment. Understanding and providing additional support for parents

in this area may be an important intervention for reducing developmental delays and supporting

children’s development when they are returned home. It was expected that Teaching would be

correlated with Behavioral Responsiveness on the Crowell, which includes similar measures of

parent behavior in regards that support to the child is developmentally helpful (Heller et al.,

1998). While these subscales were not correlated, Teaching was associated with other Crowell

subscales including Emotional Responsiveness and Positive Affect during both the free play and

teaching tasks and Irritability/Anger during the teaching task. This suggests that there is a

component to Teaching that includes positive affect during difficult situations rather than

measure their ability to give helpful hints or strategies that contribute to completing a task.

One of the strengths of the Crowell is that it assesses negative parenting behaviors like

withdrawal, anger and hostility. While no hypotheses was made about relationships between

these subscales and PICCOLO subscales, there were interesting negative correlations between

anger as assessed in the Crowell and several dimensions on the PICCOLO, especially when

parenting was assessed during the teaching task. The Affection subscale was negatively

associated with Anger (r = -.71, p < .01) during the teaching task. Parents who are high on

Affection are consistently warm and regard their child positively throughout the dyad whereas

Page 20: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 20

parents who are high on Anger more often exhibit hostility towards their child. Parents who

regard their child positively may have children that are more compliant and better adjusted which

may cause less overall stress on the parent and reduce the frequency of difficult child behavior.

Responsiveness on the PICCOLO was also negatively associated with Anger on the Crowell (r =

-.70, p < .01) during the teaching task. Parents who are highly responsive are able to anticipant or

better understand their child’s needs and respond positively. When parents endorse higher Anger

scores their irritation with the child may distract them from what their child’s behaviors are

trying to communicate, instead the parent interprets behaviors negatively and is unable to fulfill

child’s expectations or wants. This may lead to a strained parent-child relationship that promotes

miscommunication and creates unnecessary challenges which may lead to developmental delays

in the child.

The Encouragement subscale on the PICCOLO was also negatively associated with

Anger assessed by the Crowell (r = -.71, p < .01) during the teaching task. Encouraging parents

support their children’s efforts, independence, and creativity. Hostile parents may discourage a

child from acting independently and instead try to use force or manipulation to change child’s

behaviors or direct child in making decisions based on parent’s needs. This may cause

disruptions is a child’s ability to regulate emotions in conflicts and cognitive functions in later

childhood.

Teaching on the PICCOLO was also negatively associated with Anger (r = -.68, p < .01)

during the teaching task. Parents who are rated high in Teaching share conversation with their

child in play, give explanations to questions, and provide cognitive stimulation that supports a

range of developmental outcomes. Parents who are angry with their children may not be as

vocal or interactive in play and may tend to ignore or dismiss child questions and are unable to

Page 21: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 21

guide their child’s attention to completing a challenging task. This may cause a child to abandon

difficult cognitive tasks that affect later development and school readiness.

While these findings are promising they should be considered preliminary because of the

very small sample size (N=20). Future studies may want to collaborate with community agencies

and universities in varied geographical locations to produce a more representative and larger

sample.

Page 22: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 22

References

Barth, R. P., Landsverk, J., Chamberlain, P., Reid, J. B., Rolls, J. A., Hurlburt, M. S., ... & Kohl,

P. L. (2005). Parent-training programs in child welfare services: Planning for a more

evidence-based approach to serving biological parents. Research on Social Work

Practice, 15(5), 353-371. Doi: 10.1177/1049731505276321.

Beatty, J. R., Stacks, A. M., Partridge, T., Tzilos, G. K., Loree, A., & Ondersma, S. J. (2011).

LoTTS parent–infant interaction coding scale: Ease of use and reliability in a sample of

high-risk mothers and their infants. Children and Youth Services Review, 33(1), 86-90.

Doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.08.016

Byford, M., Kuh, D., & Richards, M. (2011). Parenting practices and intergenerational

associations in cognitive ability. International journal of epidemiology. Doi:

10.1093/ije/dyr188.

Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/disoriented

attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmental psychology, 25(4), 525.

Doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.4.525.

Casanueva, C., Dolan, M., Smith, K., Ringeisen, H., & Dowd, K. (2012). Indicators of well-

being among children in the United States child welfare system. Child Indicators

Research, 5(3), 547-565. Doi: 10.1007/s12187-012-9148-4.

Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in infants in

maltreating families through preventive interventions.Development and

psychopathology, 18(03), 623-649. Doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060329.

Dozier, M., Zeanah, C. H. and Bernard, K. (2013), Infants and Toddlers in Foster Care. Child

Development Perspectives, 7, 166–171. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12033

Page 23: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 23

Edwards, L. P. (1994), Improving Implementation of the Federal Adoption Assistance and Child

Welfare Act of 1980. Juvenile and Family Court Journal, 45, 3–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-

6988.1994.tb01469.x.

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations

Among Positive Parenting, Children's Effortful Control, and Externalizing Problems: A

Three‐Wave Longitudinal Study. Child Development, 76(5), 1055-1071. Doi:

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00897.x

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion.

Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241-273. Doi: 10.1207/s15327965plo0904_1.

Gilbert, R., Widom, C. S., Browne, K., Fergusson, D., Webb, E., & Janson, S. (2009). Burden

and consequences of child maltreatment in high- income countries. The

Lancet, 373(9657), 68-81. Doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61706-7.

Heller, S., Aoki, Y. & Schoffner, K. (1998) Caregiver-Child Structure Interaction Procedure

(Crowell) Administration and Coding: Clinical and Research. Unpublished Coding

Manual New Orleans: LA: Tulane University Health Sciences Center Institute of Infant

and Early Childhood Mental Health.

Hernandez, D. J. (2012). Double Jeopardy: How Third-Grade Reading Skills and Poverty

Influence High School Graduation. Baltimore, MD: Annie E. Casey Foundation.

Hurlburt, M. S., Nguyen, K., Reid, J., Webster-Stratton, C., & Zhang, J. (2013). Efficacy of the

Incredible Years group parent program with families in Head Start who self-reported a

history of child maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(8), 531-543. Doi:

10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.10.008.

Page 24: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 24

Leerkes, E. M. (2011). Maternal sensitivity during distressing tasks: A unique predictor of

attachment security. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(3), 443-446. Doi

10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.066.

Leerkes, E. M., Blankson, A. N., & O’Brien, M. (2009). Differential Effects of Maternal

Sensitivity to Infant Distress and Nondistress on Social‐Emotional Functioning. Child

Development, 80(3), 762-775. Doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01296.x.

Lugo‐Gil, J., & Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Family resources and parenting quality: Links to

children’s cognitive development across the first 3 years. Child development, 79(4),

1065-1085. Doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01176.x.

Lyon-Ruth, K. (1996). Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior

problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 64-73. Doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.64

Madigan, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., van IJzendoorn, M.H., Moran, G., Pederson, D.R., &

Benoit, D. (2006). Unresolved states of mind, anomalous parental behavior, and

disorganized attachment: A review and meta-analysis of a transmission gap. Attachment

and Human Development, 8(2), 89–111. Doi: 10.1080/14616730600774458.

Madigan, S., Moran, G., Schuengel, C., Pederson, D. R., & Otten, R. (2007). Unresolved

maternal attachment representations, disrupted maternal behavior and disorganized

attachment in infancy: links to toddler behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 48(10), 1042-1050. Doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01805.x

Miami Child Well-Being Court Initiative. (January 2013). The Miami Child Well-Being Court

Model: A Handbook For Clinicians. Miami, FL.

Page 25: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 25

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (Ed.). (2005). Child care and child development:

Results from the NICHD study of early child care and youth development . Guilford Press.

Nievar, M. A., & Becker, B. J. (2008). Sensitivity as a privileged predictor of attachment: A

second perspective on De Wolff and van IJzendoorn's meta‐analysis. Social

Development, 17(1), 102-114. Doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00417.x

Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young

children: Contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental

Psychology, 27(6), 571-587. Doi: 10.1016/jappdev.2006.08.003.

Pungello, E. P., Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., Mills-Koonce, R., & Reznick, J. S. (2009). The

effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early

childhood. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 544. Doi: 10.1037/a0013917.

Roggman, L.A., Cook, G.A., Innocenti, M.S., Jump-Norman, V. & Christiansen, K. (2013a),

Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to Outcomes

(PICCOLO) in diverse ethnic groups. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34(4), 290-306. Doi:

10.1002/imhj.21389.

Roggman, L.A., Cook, G.A., Innocenti, M.S., Jump-Norman, V., Christiansen, K., Anderson, S.

(2013b). Parenting Interactions with Children: Checklist of Observations Linked to

Outcomes: PICCOLO User’s Guide. Baltimore, Maryland: Paul H. Brookes Publishing

Co.

Smyke, A.T., & Breidenstine, A.S. (2009). Foster Care in Early Childhood. In C. H. Zeanah

(Ed.), Handbook of Infant Mental Health (pp. 500-515) New York, NY: Guilford

Publications

Page 26: Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Parents of Maltreated Children in the Child Welfare System

Ghrist/ Use of the PICCOLO 26

Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., & Lamb, M. E. (2004). Fathers and

mothers at play with their 2‐and 3‐year‐olds: contributions to language and cognitive

development. Child development, 75(6), 1806-1820. Doi: 10.1111/j.1467-

8624.2004.00818.x

Thompson, R., & Tabone, J. K. (2010). The impact of early alleged maltreatment on behavioral

trajectories. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34(12), 907-916. Doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.06.006.

US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families,

Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Children’s Bureau. (2013). Child

Maltreatment 2012. Available from:

http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/cb/cm2012.pdf