Upload
phungduong
View
221
Download
2
Embed Size (px)
Shame, depression and self-care in emerging adults with type 1 diabetes
By Alice Turland
Submitted for the Degree of
Doctor of Psychology(Clinical Psychology)
School of PsychologyFaculty of Health and Medical Sciences
University of SurreyGuildford, SurreyUnited KingdomSeptember 2016
1
Abstract
Objective: Consistent self-care is vital to maintaining good health and quality
of life for people with type 1 diabetes (T1D), as well as avoiding life-limiting
conditions. Evidence suggests depression impacts on T1D self-care, but research into
what might influence this is lacking. The aim of the current study was to explore
whether shame-proneness predicts diabetes self-care in an emerging adult population
and whether this relationship is mediated by depression. Method: 292 participants
between the ages of 16 and 25 and with T1D took part in an online survey. They
completed measures of shame-proneness, depression and adherence to diabetes self-
care activities, and provided their most recent HbA1c reading. Results: As
hypothesised, all four shame subscales showed a significant correlation between high
shame and low self-care. All four types of shame evidenced some partial mediation by
depression, though this was only marginal for diabetes shame. There was evidence of
stronger mediation by depression in the models of behavioural and character shame.
For the models assessing diabetes-related and body shame on self-care outcomes, the
direct effect of shame generally remained significant. HbA1c levels in the sample
were high (mean=64 mmol/mol) and uptake of attendance at structured diabetes
education courses was low (40%). Conclusion: Preliminary support was found for a
model whereby diabetes and body shame directly affect T1D self-care in emerging
adults. Depression remains an important factor in T1D self-care and for some may be
influenced by character and behavioural shame. Potential ways of reducing shame for
people with T1D are discussed. Further research is needed to establish causality and
measure factors which may play a role in triggering and maintaining shame.
2
Acknowledgments
Countless individuals have supported me on my journey to become a Clinical
Psychologist, and unfortunately naming each individual is beyond the scope of these
acknowledgements. I would, however, like to acknowledge the groups of people who
have helped to shape my training experience and without who I would not be in this
position today.
Firstly, and most importantly, I would like to thank the service users, patients and
clients (however they wish to define themselves) with whom I have worked across
each of my placements. I have borne witness to stories of the best and worst of
humanity, have spoken with people at their lowest ebb and been amazed and humbled
by the resilience that people can summon even in the darkest of places. I have learnt,
from you, how to be a better Psychologist and also how to be a better person and I am
forever grateful for the opportunity to work with you. I hope that you are all able to
find and sustain some level of happiness and meaning in your lives, and that our brief
time together played some part in helping you towards this.
Secondly, I would like to thank my Supervisor’s for supporting me through my
placements and teaching me new ways of working. I have taken something from each
of you and hope I can continue to combine this, once qualified, into my own way of
working. In particular, I would like to thank those Supervisor’s, and colleague’s, who
modelled to me how to achieve a work life balance and the importance of recognising
when to lower ones expectations of oneself in order to sustain a healthy approach to
the profession.
3
Thirdly to Cohort 42, with whom I have shared the ups and downs of the last three
years of training. We got through it as a group, with hard work and a healthy sense of
humour.
With specific regard to my research, which takes up a significant part of this portfolio,
I would like to thank Dr Laura Simonds for her supervision, critique and
encouragement throughout the entire process; from discussing ideas to writing up the
final portfolio. I would also like to thank Dr Mary John and Dr Debbie Cooke for
their thoughtful input and advice.
The study would not have been possible without the support of the Diabetes Online
Community, many of whom not only participated but also shared with their peers and
offered advice about further avenues for recruitment. The charity Diabetes UK were
also invaluable in supporting and advertising on my behalf.
Lastly I would like to extend a more personal thanks to my family and friends for the
support and encouragement they have shown me over the years. To my parents, who
always showed a limitless belief in what I could achieve. And finally, I would like to
thank my fiancé, Jak Swain, for his support, patience and well-timed texts of
inspirational quotes. I certainly would not be in this position without you and I cannot
wait to begin the next chapter of our lives together.
4
Research
1 MRP Empirical Paper (including Abstract)
2 MRP Empirical Paper Appendices
3 MRP Proposal (without appendices)
4 MRP Literature Review (with appendices)
Clinical Experience A brief overview of my 3 years’ of clinical experience.
Assessments Titles of all your academic assessments
Contents of portfolio
5
Empirical Paper
Shame, depression and self-care in emerging adults with
type 1 diabetes
Alice Turland
Word Count (excluding reference list, appendices and abstract)
9628
Journal Choice
Journal of Health Psychology (APA).
“Health Psychology” is an international APA journal. It.was chosen as the target journal to reflect the papers key focus on engagement with diabetes self-care and the psychosocial factors involved in this. Further detail about the journal can be
found in Appendix Q.
6
Abstract
Objective: Consistent self-care is vital to maintaining good health and quality
of life for people with type 1 diabetes (T1D), as well as avoiding life-limiting
conditions. Evidence suggests depression impacts on T1D self-care, but research into
what might influence this is lacking. The aim of the current study was to explore
whether shame-proneness predicts diabetes self-care in an emerging adult population
and whether this relationship is mediated by depression. Method: 292 participants
between the ages of 16 and 25 and with T1D took part in an online survey. They
completed measures of shame-proneness, depression and adherence to diabetes self-
care activities, and provided their most recent HbA1c reading. Results: As
hypothesised, all four shame subscales showed a significant correlation between high
shame and low self-care. All four types of shame evidenced some partial mediation by
depression, though this was only marginal for diabetes shame. There was evidence of
stronger mediation by depression in the models of behavioural and character shame.
For the models assessing diabetes-related and body shame on self-care outcomes, the
direct effect of shame generally remained significant. HbA1c levels in the sample
were high (mean=64 mmol/mol) and uptake of attendance at structured diabetes
education courses was low (40%). Conclusion: Preliminary support was found for a
model whereby diabetes and body shame directly affect T1D self-care in emerging
adults. Depression remains an important factor in T1D self-care and for some may be
influenced by character and behavioural shame. Potential ways of reducing shame for
people with T1D are discussed. Further research is needed to establish causality and
measure factors which may play a role in triggering and maintaining shame.
7
Introduction
It is estimated that 3.2 million people in the UK have a diagnosis of diabetes;
around 10% of these have type 1 (T1D). T1D is most commonly diagnosed between
the ages of 10 and 14, meaning that the majority of sufferers have added pressure in
their adolescent years of adapting to managing their condition (Diabetes UK, 2014).
Self-care for people with T1D requires a high level of responsibility on the part of the
individual. Self-care routines can involve daily dietary and lifestyle changes,
carbohydrate calculations, multiple blood glucose tests and self-injecting of insulin
(National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, 2015a).
Typically, when first diagnosed, parents take on at least partial responsibility
for T1D self-care. The age at which full responsibility shifts to the young person
differs depending on the individual. In one qualitative interview study, adolescents
described transitioning to independent self-care as a complex and gradual process.
Many factors played a role in successful transitioning; maturity, peer acceptance, and
support and validation of the young person making their own decisions, both by
parents and healthcare professionals (Karlsson, Arman, & Wikblad, 2008).
To add to the complexity, the transition to independent diabetes self-care is
embedded within the context of typical adolescent developmental processes such as
developing and exploring self-identity (Erikson, 1968). Learning to adjust to
independent self-care is likely to take place at a time when young people are
exploring some activities for the first time such as using alcohol and drugs and buying
food when out with friends. Each of these activities can lead to poor glucose control,
perhaps by two different routes. Firstly, a young person’s lack of experience with new
activities may mean they lack the knowledge and skill to effectively adjust their
8
insulin and/or carbohydrate intake to accommodate these activities. Secondly, even if
they possess the knowledge around what should be avoided and what can be adjusted
for, they may ignore this advice in favour of wanting to fit in amongst their peers.
Adolescents with diabetes are likely facing the tension between fitting in with peers
and achieving optimum health on a daily basis.
The HbA1c blood test is the measurement most widely used by healthcare
professionals to indicate average glycaemic control over the preceding 2-3 months.
When mapped over time, a pattern emerges of worsening glycaemic control during
early adolescence, peaking and stabilising between the ages of 17 and 19, followed by
a gradual improvement into young adulthood (Bryden et al., 2001; Clements et al.,
2015). However, despite this promising improvement in early adulthood, Clements et
al (2015) found that, amongst their sample, the average HbA1c of 26 year olds
remained above recommended levels. There was also a large degree of variation,
suggesting that individual differences play a vital role in individual trajectories of
glycaemic control during this sensitive time (Clements et al., 2015).
Difficulties arising from poor glycaemic control extend beyond the day-to-day
challenges of headaches, tiredness and poor concentration resulting from mild
hypoglycaemia and hyperglycaemia (Diabetes UK, 2015). Complications vary from
immediate and life-threatening conditions such as diabetic ketoacidosis, to chronic
health problems leading to heart disease, stroke and retinopathy (NHS Choices, 2013;
NHS Choices, 2015).
A review of the literature highlighted a number of commonly cited reasons for
not adhering to diabetes regimens. This included fear of inadvertently causing
hypoglycaemia, a dislike of needles, and a lack of understanding of complex self-care
9
regimens (Odegard & Capoccia, 2007). One study found that 35% (n=87) of 11-25
year olds reported purposefully omitting insulin in order to lose weight (Peveler et al.,
2005). Understanding more about the cognitive and emotional factors that play a role
in self-care is vital in order to offer appropriate support for people struggling with
self-care. The current study aimed to explore the role of shame as a potential
contributing factor towards variability in diabetes self-care, and consequently
glycaemic control, for emerging adults with T1D.
Emerging Adulthood
Arnett (2000) defined the concept of “emerging adulthood” as the transitional
period from adolescence to adulthood. In Western cultures this stage represents the
time following compulsory schooling, where young people may be continuing into
further/higher education/training independent of adult supervision, but not yet
burdened by full adult responsibility. Arnett argues that whilst identity exploration
typically begins in adolescence, it is usually not resolved until the emerging adulthood
stage, when increased independence from the family of origin opens up exposure to a
broader range of world-views and opportunities. Palladino et al (2013) suggest that
this period generates particular problems for those with T1D. Parental supervision of
self-care routines most likely cease completely and further challenges to the treatment
regimen are likely to arise, such as cooking for oneself for the first time and travelling
independently. Of particular concern is the finding that, despite the considerably
greater risk to health, in one study, there was no difference in self-reported binge
drinking or smoking between an emerging adult sample with and without diabetes
(Palladino et al., 2013).
10
In 2011 the American Diabetes Association (ADA) released a position
statement highlighting the challenges facing emerging adults transitioning from
paediatric to adult healthcare services. They noted the challenges of moving to adult
health services which usually offer shorter appointments and expect patients to act
more autonomously in their decision-making regarding treatment and lifestyle. In the
US there is the additional issue at this age of changes in health insurance, sometimes
leading to dangerous gaps in provision (Peters, Laffel, & American Diabetes
Association Transitions Working Group, 2011).
Similar issues are likely a factor in the UK. In a recent multi-site UK
qualitative study young people and their parents frequently reported less support in
adult services, often seeing a different consultant on each visit (Kime, 2014). There is
a recognition of transition issues in the recently updated UK National Institute for
Health and Care Excellence (NICE) guidelines which advises flexibility in the age of
transition, to allow for individual differences in emotional maturity and general life
stability (NICE, 2016).
The period of emerging adulthood is problematic to define precisely, as the
development of independence can be a gradual process, which begins and ends at
different times depending on the individual and their social and cultural context.
Although Arnett defines emerging adulthood as occurring between the ages of 18-25,
Clements et al’s (2015) results suggest that, for some, a stabilisation of glycaemic
control begins at around the age of 16. However this is by no means the case for
everyone, and an understanding of which individual factors differ between those who
begin to stabilise their glycaemic control, and those who do not, is vital in targeting
support to those who need it.
11
The increasing independence that defines the emerging adult period is likely to
begin before the transition of moving home. This is at a time when those in late
adolescence may still live with parents but begin to make decisions about their futures
and venture out independently with friends. Indeed many new situations that may
affect glycaemic control may begin to occur before an adolescent leaves home, such
as drinking alcohol and purchasing food independently. Additionally, the transition to
adult services in developed countries is encompassed during this time period. It is for
these reasons that the current study focussed on 16-25 year olds.
Diabetes and mental health
Diabetes has been linked to an increased prevalence of eating disorders (Jones,
Lawson, Daneman, Olmsted, & Rodin, 2000) and depression (Mommersteeg et al.,
2013) as well as directly causing anxiety issues that people may otherwise never have
had to face, such as needle phobias and fears of hypoglycaemia. Depression is the
mental health problem most commonly linked to diabetes, with current estimates
placing those with diabetes at roughly twice the risk of those without for developing
depression (Mommersteeg et al., 2013). However despite widespread occurrence, the
reason for this increased risk is not clear-cut.
Qualitative studies provide an insight into the experiences of young people
with type 1 diabetes and a starting point to consider the psychosocial factors that may
be involved in the link between depression and diabetes. Feeling isolated from peers
(Kime, 2014), noticing fear in parents and other people, disliking authoritarian stances
from health care professionals and the increased complexity of negotiating
independence from parents (Scholes et al., 2013) were all raised as themes by young
people when asked about their experience of diabetes. Interpersonal conflict is a
12
commonly cited trigger for depression (Frone, 2000; Sheeber, Hops, Alpert, Davis, &
Andrews, 1997), and diabetes certainly has the potential to create conditions in which
conflict could occur. The fear noticed in parents may develop into overprotectiveness,
and eventually conflict, when the young person either purposefully or accidentally
makes an unwise condition self-care decision.
A large scale qualitative study across adults with both type 1 and type 2
diabetes also reported, alongside experiences of fear related to risk of hyperglycaemic
or health complications, a general sense of hopelessness related to their condition
(Stuckey et al., 2014). It may be that the fear of future complications, in conjunction
with repeated past experiences of difficulties controlling the condition, leads to a
sense of hopelessness, and subsequently depression, in some people with diabetes.
Another possibility for the increased prevalence of depression is that the
physical symptoms of low or high blood sugar in people with diabetes, such as
tiredness and irritability combine to trigger experiences of low mood and, over a
prolonged period, depression. Biological explanations go one step beyond this to
highlight similar neurological and endocrinological mechanisms potentially
underlying both conditions and thus the presence of one automatically increases the
likelihood of developing the other (Korczak, Pereira, Koulajian, Matejcek, & Giacca,
2011).
A recent study found that although people with diabetes scored more highly on
depression measures than healthy controls, the proportion of somatic to
cognitive/affective symptoms was no higher in those with diabetes (Stewart et al.,
2011). Furthermore, Seigel, Golden, Gough, Lashley and Sacker (1990) found a larger
difference in reported psychological symptoms than somatic symptoms in a diabetic
13
group compared to a healthy comparison group, supporting a substantial role for
psychosocial factors.
The factors underpinning the co-occurrence of diabetes and depression are
likely multifaceted and require a biopsychosocial model. Of course, for some people
diabetes may play little or no role in their depression, the latter instead being related
to other difficult life experiences. However, whatever the cause, depression is of
particular importance in diabetes research, because it has frequently been associated
with hyperglycaemia (Lustman et al., 2000).
Diabetes self-care and mental health
Mental health issues such as needle phobia and eating disorders not only link
directly to T1D but also point to relatively straightforward mechanisms by which
glycaemic control is affected (i.e. through avoidance of injecting insulin or overusing
insulin in order to lose weight). The way in which depression links to poorer
glycaemic control is less straightforward as avoidance of self-care could be for a
variety of reasons inherent in depression. These include low motivation, being
unwilling to inject in public, denial, poor memory, or in an attempt at suicide. From a
biological perspective, there is also an argument that the same underlying mechanisms
may occur in both diabetes and depression and therefore it is possible that those with
poorer glycaemic control are likely to experience more severe depression irrespective
of engagement in self-management (Korczak et al., 2011).
Support for the behavioural and motivational impact of depression on self-
management were shown in a review that highlighted a significant negative
association between depression and diabetes self-care across a number of studies
14
(Gonzalez et al., 2008). It follows that the decrease in motivation that so often forms
part of depression would affect the degree to which an individual could adhere to a
complex and intensive daily self-management routine. A cognitive factor that
potentially exacerbates this relationship is self-efficacy (i.e. the belief that one is
capable of instigating change). If an individual lacks a sense of self-efficacy in
relation to their diabetes, motivation is likely to be reduced, further reducing the
likelihood of attempting to engage with condition self-management. Hopelessness
about the future is also likely to impact on self-efficacy. Evidence for the role of self-
efficacy in diabetes self-care was shown in one study amongst a group of emerging
adults. Self-efficacy specifically related to diabetes partially mediated the link
between impulse control and diabetes self-care (Stupiansky, Hanna, Slaven, Weaver,
& Fortenberry, 2013).
There is a wealth of research on cognitive and biological factors in depression
and diabetes but relatively less attention has been paid to emotional factors. Given the
powerful effect that negative emotional states can have on memory, behaviour and
thinking abilities, further exploration of the role of emotion and its interaction with
cognition is important.
Shame
Within society, the word “shame” is often used interchangeably with other
self-conscious emotions such as guilt or embarrassment (Tangney, Miller, Flicker, &
Barlow, 1996) or in the case of biblical shame, to imply a severe wrongdoing that in
some way damages the internal self and requires the individual to repent. Within the
15
social sciences, however, shame is seen as a complex negative experience, often
linked with reactions to trauma, experience of anxiety and depression (Feiring, Taska,
& Lewis, 2002; Gilbert, 2000).
Shame can be described as both a self-conscious and socially conscious
emotion: entirely linter-woven with ones sense of self, yet experienced through the
eyes of an internalised other (Tracy & Robins, 2007b; Lewis, 1971; Gilbert, 2011) .
This experience involves not only affect but also a negatively laden cognitive
appraisal of the self in relation to the situation at hand (Tracy & Robins, 2007b). In
other words, the individual is aware of not being as they wish to be, or as others
which them to be in a situation, and the reason for this is perceived as an internal,
stable defect.
In traditional Beckian cognitive theory, repeated negative appraisals stem from
underlying stable, negative beliefs about the self (Beck, 1991). Shame-related sets of
beliefs have been theorised as one type of early maladaptive schema, developed
during childhood through the prolonged or repeated experience of rejection (Young,
Klosko, & Weishaar, 2003). When an individual experiences a current situation as
similar to these early experiences, there is a risk the schema is activated, with the
emotional experience of shame as one consequence of this.
A key feature of shame is the motivation to hide the perceived defective self
through avoiding others (Tangney et al., 1996). Gilbert (2011) proposed that the sense
of threat appraisal inherent in shame has evolutionary roots. As humans evolved as
social beings, belonging to a group was advantageous and offered safety, whereas
becoming aware of being rejected from the group and being seen as unacceptable by
others was potentially life threatening. Gilbert describes external shame as that which
16
is perceived as coming from the social group, interacting with an internal sense of the
self as inherently shameful.
Several theorists construe shame-proneness as a general propensity to feel
shame. Andrews, Qian and Valentine (2002) argue that shame may not be felt
globally by an individual, but rather in relation to certain aspects of themselves. On
the basis of this assertion, they developed the Experience of Shame Scale to assess
body, character and behavioural shame as three separate, but linked, domains of the
shame experience.
For an adolescent or emerging adult coming to terms with a chronic condition
that marks them out as different from their peers, and limits what they can do, there
may be a vulnerability to experiencing shame. A social risk may arise where an
emerging adult with diabetes may be forced to choose between activities congruent
with peer acceptance but incongruent with diabetes self-management, such as
drinking excessive alcohol. It may be that this social risk results in direct avoidance of
diabetes self-management.
It may also be that shame directly impacts on self-care following repeated
difficulties in maintaining HbA1c at recommended levels, as is common in
adolescence. This may be brought to the foreground by well-meaning and concerned
diabetes healthcare professionals during consultations. In one qualitative study that
interviewed patients with hypercholesterolemia, shame emerged as a theme in relation
to the emotions experienced by some when clinicians commented on their diet or
cholesterol test results (Frich, Malterud, & Fugelli, 2007). Furthermore Archer
(2014) discusses how Clinicians may inadvertently create a context for future shame
by praising people with T1D who manage their diabetes well and appear to be “ideal”
17
patients; an accolade that may be immensely difficult to maintain. In observing GP
consultations in Denmark, Guassora, Reventlow and Malterund (2014) commented on
the patients’ discourse of achievement in relation to lifestyle factors, which they
labelled on a continuum from honour to shame. It may be that repeated exposure to
healthcare consultations experienced as shameful results in an escape or avoidance
reaction, with the individual beginning to avoid aspects of self-care which have
become associated with shame. Furthermore Terry and Leary (2011) argue that self-
compassion (incongruent with shame) mediates self-regulation of health behaviours at
every stage from being able to set more realistic goals, to an increased ability to
recover from a lapse. By implication, shame is likely to interfere with self-regulation.
Shame has consistently been associated with depression (Andrews et al., 2002;
Cheung et al., 2004). As noted above, both are promoted by similar attribution styles.
One longitudinal study highlighted shame-related schemas as being predictive of
severity of depression nine years later, suggesting that a belief about oneself that
evokes shame may pre-date depression (Halvorsen et al., 2010). Therefore, as well as
shame having a potentially direct effect on condition self-management as noted
above, it may also be that depressive affect mediates the relationship between shame
and self-care. For example, shame-related depressive affect might decrease
motivation and produce problems with memory. It might also increase risk of
hyperglycaemia through the physiological effects of stress (Diabetes.co.uk, Diabetes
Digital Media ltd, 2014). For those who are already prone to experiencing shame,
adjusting to diabetes self-management may be a perfect storm, inducing shame related
affect and cognitions, which in turn enhances negative affect and reduces, both
directly and indirectly the chances of engaging in condition self-management.
18
Summary
Consistent self-care is vital to maintaining good health and quality of life for
people with diabetes, as well as avoiding life-limiting conditions. The emerging adult
years are important for laying down the foundations for lifelong management.
Independent diabetes self-care can be challenging for young people. Evidence
suggests depression impacts on diabetes self-care, but research into why this might be,
or what might influence this is lacking. Shame is a complex emotional and cognitive
experience that has been consistently linked with depression, and may arise in young
people with diabetes who perceive themselves as different from their peers, or who
experience advice about their condition self-management as shame inducing. The aim
of the current study was to explore whether shame-proneness predicts diabetes self-
care and whether this relationship is mediated by depression.
Main Hypotheses
1. High shame-proneness will be related to poorer diabetes self-care (i.e. lower
adherence to self-care activities and higher Hba1c level)
2. Depression will partially mediate the relationship between shame and self- report
indices of diabetes self-care.
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited from several sources. Advertisements were posted
on a number of diabetes specific social media groups, from the UK, USA, Australia,
New Zealand and Ireland, as well as being shared through the author’s own social
19
network. The charity Diabetes UK advertised the study through their magazine,
website and social media accounts. Posters and flyers were distributed throughout the
University of Surrey campus, in public places and on the noticeboards of local
businesses (Appendix A). Lastly participants were encouraged to share the study with
further potentially eligible parties. The inclusion criteria stipulated that participants be
between the ages of 16 and 25 (inclusive) and have a diagnosis of Type 1 diabetes.
Due to the online format of the study and the form of the questionnaires used,
participants were also required to be able to read and understand English.
Procedure
The University of Surrey Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences Ethics
Committee granted the study a favourable ethical opinion (Appendix B).
Participants accessed the online survey by following a link placed in the
advertisements, leading to a comprehensive information screen (Appendix C). This
detailed the broad nature of the study (emotions and diabetes self-care) including the
general requirement for participants to look inwards and consider their own
potentially difficult emotions. Following this, the consent screen specified how
personal information would be used and participants were prompted to indicate their
consent in order to continue (Appendix D).
The survey then began with a statement normalising non-adherence to self-
care routines, and encouraging participants to answer honestly (Stenner et al, In
preparation; Appendix E). Initial demographic and diabetes-specific questions (such
as age at diagnosis) provided information necessary to describe the sample (Appendix
F). Two questions were taken from Stenner et al (In preparation) following
20
consultation with an author (Appendix G). Participants were then prompted to answer
the specific outcome measures in the order stated below. The opportunity was given
to receive a summary of the study findings once complete. Email addresses were
gathered for this purpose only and were stored in a separate data file to ensure
participant anonymity.
Following the demographic section of the survey, a prompt was given at the
end of each page asking participants if they wanted to continue. If they clicked “no”
to this question, they were taken to the standard debrief screen (Appendix H). This
gave further details about the study, normalised the experience of low mood within
diabetes and encouraged participants to contact their healthcare professional if they
noticed a problem with mood or self-care. This was followed by a list of mental health
and diabetes resources. The same screen appeared for participants who completed the
survey.
Measures
Predictor variables: Shame and Depression
Experience of Shame Scale
(ESS; Andrews, Qian & Valentine; Appendix I)
The ESS is a 25-item questionnaire that measures shame-proneness across
three domains: body, character and behavioural. Character shame focusses on shame
associated with who the individual is as a person. For example, the respondent is
asked about shame associated with their “manner with others”. The behavioural
domain looks specifically at shame associated with what one does, such as “doing
something wrong”. The body shame domain refers to shame about one’s body (or part
21
thereof). For each subscale, questions are asked that address the affective, cognitive
and behavioural (e.g. concealment) aspects of the shame experience. The respondent
is asked to consider how they have felt over the last year and rate each item on a likert
scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Higher scores indicate higher shame. Due to
the variability in number of items per subscale, subscale item means are presented for
comparison.
Good construct validity and internal reliability have been shown for both the
total scale and the three subscales with an undergraduate student sample. Furthermore
the high test-retest reliability, over an 11-week period, suggests that the ESS is likely
to be measuring shame-proneness rather that transient states of shame (Andrews et al.,
2002).
With the author’s permission, a further 7-item diabetes shame scale was
developed for the purpose of this study (Appendix J). This asked questions in the
same format of affect, cognition and behaviour, in relation to two aspects of diabetes:
engaging in self-care and experience during healthcare consultations.
Depression Subscale of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale
(DDASS; Lovibond & Lovibond 1995; Appendix L)
The DASS-21 is a 21-item, self- report scale comprising of three subscales
measuring depression, anxiety and stress. Each of these subscales are designed to
measure on a continuum, rather than publishing cut-off points for diagnosis. A
separate score is given for each of the subscales. Factor analysis has confirmed the
existence of the three factor structure.
22
In the present study, only the depression scale was used comprising 7 items
(DDASS). Good convergent validity has been shown between the depression subscale
and the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995).
Responses for each item were on a likert scale ranging from 0 (did not apply to me at
all) to 4 (applied to me very much or most of the time). Scores can range from 0 to 21,
with higher scores indicating higher depressive affect. The DDASS has the benefit of
focussing on the affective and cognitive elements of depression, thus reducing the
likelihood that somatic symptoms commonly associated with diabetes, such as poor
sleep and fatigue, would affect the results.
Criterion Variables: Self-care and glycaemic control
Self-Care Inventory- Revised (SCI-R; Weinger, Butler, Welch & La Greca,
2005; Appendix K)
This 15-item measure asks participants to rate their diabetes self-care on a
series of 5-point likert scales, varying from 1- “never do it”, to 5- “always do it as
recommended, without fail”. Scores are presented and used in analysis as average
item means. This is to account for the 4 questions which had a “not applicable”
option; scoring these as zero would otherwise bias the results.
Items measure the main aspects of diabetes self-care such as diet, exercise,
glucose monitoring and insulin administration. Weinger et al. (2005) developed the
SCI-R from the original SCI, the latter being developed for use with adolescents and
their parents. The adult-focussed SCI-R was chosen for use in the current study as the
age range under investigation (16-25) more closely represented a young adult
population as opposed to an adolescent one. Given the SCI-R is for completion by
23
adults, parental reports are not included. The SCI-R showed high internal consistency
(α =0.87) at initial validation and concurrent and convergent validity with existing
measures in the expected directions (Weinger et al., 2005).
Self-reported Glycolated Haemoglobin levels (Hba1c)
Hba1c is routinely assessed by blood tests at diabetes check-up appointments.
HbA1c gives diabetes clinicians an index of average plasma glucose concentration
over the preceding two or three months and, therefore by implication, an estimation of
the individuals engagement in diabetes self-care. In 2011 the UK transitioned to
reporting Hba1c in the worldwide standard International Federation of Clinical
Chemistry (IFCC) units of mmol/mol. However reporting using the prior percentage
system may still occur (Abbott Diabetes Care, 2015).
In practice patients are often given an individual numerical Hba1c target,
particularly if they are at risk of hypoglycaemia: very low blood glucose levels.
However, in order to avoid long term diabetic complications associated with sustained
hyperglycaemia (high blood glucose levels), achieving a value of less than 48
mmol/mol is considered to be indicative of good diabetes control and is the general
recommended clinical target for adults (National Institute for Health and Care
Excellence, 2015b). However any reductions are generally considered to reduce the
risk of complications associated with hyperglycaemia (Diabetes UK, 2016a).
Therefore HbA1c was presented as a continuous variable in the form of HbA1c scores
(mmol/mol).
HbA1c is widely used in research as an indicator of diabetes self-care.
However, given that Hba1c may be influenced by other factors, such as
24
appropriateness of medication and additional illness (Diabetes Digital Media ltd,
2014), it is used in conjunction with self-reported self-care in the current study.
Data Analysis
Hypothesis 1, which focused on assessing the relationship between measures
of shame proneness and self-care indices (Hba1c levels, SCI-R scores; both
continuous variables) was tested using Pearson’s correlations. Hypothesis 2 was
tested using mediation analysis. The method of bootstrapping was chosen as a more
valid alternative to Baron and Kenny’s (1986) traditional causal steps analysis,
yielding both a lower chance of Type 1 error, and higher power (Hayes, 2009).
Hayes’ PROCESS macro was used to test simple mediation.
The first model contained diabetes shame as the predictor, self-care (SCI-R) as
the outcome (continuous), and depression (continuous) as the mediator. Mediation
analysis was then repeated with the remaining shame subscales (body, character and
behaviour) as the predictors. All models were then analysed with self-reported HbA1c
(continuous) as the outcome. Figure A below represents the relationships tested:
Figure A – Mediation paths tested
Depression (DDASS)
Results
25
Self-care (SCIR/HbA1c)
Shame
(ESS subscales)
Overview
Firstly, total participant numbers at each stage of analyses are described and
the criteria by which participants were included clarified. An overview of participant
demographic characteristics, first generally and then diabetes specific, is given. This
is followed by an overview of means, distribution and reliability for all measures. The
analysis regarding Hypothesis 1, relating to the potential association between shame
and self-care, is then presented. Each ESS subscale is considered, firstly with SCIR
scores as the dependent variable, followed by Hba1c level. Hypothesis 2, which
suggests depression as a mediating factor between self-care and shame is then
addressed through mediation.
Participants and missing data
In total, 354 participants began the survey. Ten were screened out based on the
exclusion criteria for age (n=8 >26 years old) and type 1 diabetes diagnosis (n=2
without T1D). In order to be included in some analyses, participants needed to have
completed a self-care measure (either the SCI-R or HbA1c value) in addition to a
predictor variable (DDASS or ESS subscale). Fifty two participants did not meet this
minimum completion rate, so were excluded on this basis. This left a total of 292
participants.
Some participants dropped out before the end of the survey (n=11) but the
decision was taken to include the data for completed measures as consent for use of
data up to that point had not been withdrawn. Two hundred and four participants
indicated they knew their latest HbA1c value and reported it. Of these, three HbA1c
26
values were excluded as they were accompanied by language indicating that the
respondent was not sure (e.g. “about”). This left 201 HbA1c values.
In order to avoid biasing the data towards those who had completed the survey
with great care, individual subscale mean replacement was used for two cases that had
one missing item each. These occurred on the character shame subscale and the SCI-
R.
Between 90 and 196 participants is sufficient to detect a medium effect size in
a mediation analysis (alpha = .05) at 80% power (Fritz & Mackinnon, 2007), so the
final participant number for all analyses yielded good power.
27
Table 1
Summary of participant demographicsAge % of sample (n)
20.76 (2.78) a
Range 16-25
GenderMale 18.8 (55)
Female 81.2 (237)Occupation
Student 52.4 (153)Employed full time 32.5 (95)Employed part time 6.8 (20)
Self-employed 1 (3)Occasional/ casual work 2.1 (6)
Other 5.1 (15)Country of residence
UK 80.5 (235)Non-UK 19.5 (57)
Live with parents
60.6 (177)
EthnicityWhite 92.8 (270)
Mixed/multiple ethic groups 4.1 (12)Asian/ Asian British 1.7 (5)
Black/African/Caribbean/British 0.7 (2)Other ethnic group 0.7 (2)
Mental health
diagnosis bCurrent diagnosis 19.5 (57)
Past diagnosis (not including current)
10.3 (30)
Anxiety/ depression 18.6 (54)Eating disorder 2.05 (6)
Personality disorder, psychosis or trauma
2.05 (6)
Neurodevelopmental 0.68 (2)a =Mean (SD), b=Some participants had several diagnoses, the percentages represent diagnoses in the over-all sample, not cumulative individuals.
28
The participants were predominantly white, female and UK residents. As
recruitment involved advertising on non-UK based social media groups, a minority of
participants resided outside the UK. Around half of the participants indicated that they
were students. This is to be expected given the target age range of the study.
Around one fifth of the sample indicated that they had a current diagnosis of
anxiety or depression. Several participants indicated they had been given more than
one mental health diagnosis.
Table 2 shows diabetes-specific demographics.
Table 2
Diabetes-specific demographic variablesRegimen % of sample (n)
MDI* 64.7 (189)Pump 34.9 (102)Other .3 (1)
Age diagnosed a11.57 (5.82)
Range (years) <1 – 25Diabetes duration a
9.20 (6.06)Range (years) <1 – 25
Manage diabetes independently from parents
74 (216)
Parents always attend appointments 19.5 (57)
HbA1cKnown 71.2 (208)Not known 27.7 (81)Not measured 1 (3)Above recommended level 83.1 (167)At or below recommended level
16.9 (34)
Attended a structured diabetes education course
39.7 (116)
Nb= Where missing data was present, valid percentages were used. *MDI= Multiple Daily Injection,
a = Mean years (SD)
29
Only two fifths of participants indicated that they had attended a structured
diabetes education course, despite this being highlighted as a key priority for
implementation in recent UK guidance (National Institute for Health and Care
Excellence, 2015b). Despite two thirds of participants indicating that they lived with
their parents, three quarters said they managed their diabetes independently. Of those
that stated they knew their HbA1c level and provided it, 83% were above the
recommended level of 48 mmol/mol.
Variable distribution and reliability
Table 3:
Descriptive statistics, distribution and reliability of variables
Measure (N) Total Mean (SD)
Item Mean* (SD)
Cronbach’s alpha
Skew z Kurtosis z
DASS (292) 6.85 (6) .98 (.85) .94 5.43 -1.47SCI-R (292) n/a 3.50 (.6) .82 -3.308 -.151ESS total (281) 41.3 (23) 1.40 (.76) .97 1.593 -3.22ESS – Chara (287) 14.5 (10) 1.20 (.87) .95 3.35 -3.22ESS – Behav (282) 12.3 (7) 1.37 (.81) .93 1.97 -3.08ESS – Body (281)ESS – Diab (281)
7.01 (3)7.62 (5)
1.97 (1)1.27 (.94)
.93
.90-3.742.07
-3.58-3.80
Hba1c (201) 63.72 (19.8)
n/a n/a 9.87 13.62
HbA1c extreme outliers excluded (198)
62.47 (17.1)
n/a n/a 6.60 7.38
*ESS scale: from 0 to 3 with 3 indicating high shame. SCIR scale: from 1 to 5 with 5 indicating good self-care. DASS: from 0 to 3 with 3 indicating low mood.
DDASS
Individual scores on the DDASS spanned the full range; from 0 to 21. The
score distribution showed some positive skew which reflects a substantial group who
reported no symptoms of depressive affect. Alongside the mean, this suggests that
overall depression levels in the sample were low, with a minority reporting maximum
30
scores. As such, kurtosis was negative and indicated a flatter distribution with scores
extending to either end of the scale. When also considering a visual inspection of the
histogram (Appendix M), the distribution does appear somewhat non-normal but not
excessively so. Cronbach’s alpha indicated good reliability. ‘Alpha if item deleted
analysis’ and ‘item-total correlation analysis’ supported the inclusion of all items in
the scale.
ESS
Given the ESS is rated 1 to 5, the average shame level was low in this sample.
A comparison of subscale item means indicated that body shame was the highest in
the sample and character the lowest. Histograms revealed approximately normal
distribution for the ESS total. Body shame subscale scores were slightly skewed
towards higher shame whilst character, diabetes and behavioural shame subscale
scores were slightly skewed towards lower shame. Kurtosis was negative for all
subscales, indicating flatter distributions (Appendix M). However when considered in
conjunction with skewness and kurtosis z scores, the distribution of all ESS subscales
were not considered to be substantially non-normal.
Given that the diabetes shame scale was developed specifically for the current
study a maximum likelihood factor analysis with direct oblimin rotation was
performed on these items to investigate if there may be a 2-factor solution
(consultation-related shame and diabetes self-care related shame.) However, this was
not supported due to the emergence of only one factor with an eigenvalue over one
and a significant chi-square goodness of fit test for the 2-factor model. This suggested
a poor fit. In addition one item loaded on both factors. The diabetes self-care shame
subscale was, therefore, analysed as a single subscale. Cronbach’s alpha was high for
31
this subscale (.9) suggesting very good internal reliability. It also correlated highly
with the ESS total (r=.782, p<.001).
The ESS total scale demonstrated excellent internal reliability in this sample.
Internal reliability was also high for all subscales. ‘Alpha if item deleted’ analysis
indicated that the inclusion of one question (“Have you avoided looking at yourself in
the mirror”) lowered the body shame subscale alpha (from .93 to .91). However, as
this value of alpha was still very high and because the item had a strong correlation
with the total (.65) it was included.
SCIR
The distribution of self-care scores was slightly skewed towards the higher
end of the scale indicating better diabetes self-care. Kurtosis was low and, taken in
conjunction with the histogram, the distribution of scores was considered to be
approximately normal (Appendix M). Cronbach’s alpha indicated good internal
reliability of scores in this sample.
HbA1c
Self-reported HbA1c values were reported for two thirds of the sample.
HbA1c’s were given in both % and mmol/mol units and converted into mmol/mol for
ease of comparison. Participants were asked to indicate whether values were given in
% or mmol/mol, but answers were often contradictory. For this reason, scores above
20 were rated as mmol/mol and below 20 as % converted to mmol/mol (Diabetes
Digital Media ltd, 2016). Skew and Kurtosis z scores, along with a visual inspection
of the histogram (Appendix M) revealed a positive skew for HbA1c scores, with a
slightly long tail of values at the higher end of the scale.
32
A boxplot revealed 8 statistical outliers and 3 extreme outliers at the higher
end of the scale (Appendix O). No published maximum possible HbA1c values could
be found to warrant the removal of these statistical outliers. However based on range
information given on two well-known patient information websites (Diabetes Digital
Media ltd, 2016; Diabetes UK, 2016b) it was deduced that the 3 extreme outliers were
unlikely to be true HbA1c values. Removal of these 3 reduced the skew and kurtosis
to an approximately normal level, especially when considering the shape of the
histogram (Appendix M). As such, Pearson’s correlation was deemed appropriate to
use. As these values could not be ruled out completely as extremely high scores, all
subsequent analyses involving HbA1c values were conducted on the full sample, in
addition to the sample minus the three extreme outlying values (HbA1c-EO). Both
sets of results are presented.
Hypothesis 1 – Shame-proneness and self-care
Scatterplots revealed that there were no substantial violations of the
assumptions of linearity and homoscedasticity for each ESS subscale and self-care
outcome pairing (Appendix N). Skew and kurtosis values along with visual
inspection of the histograms (Appendix M) indicated approximately normal
distribution for the SCIR. There was some slight skew for some of the shame-
proneness subscales, and some remaining skew for HbA1c-EO. However, given the
large sample size and the robustness of Pearson’s correlation to moderate non-
normality (Bishara & Hittner, 2012; Field, 2000) the Pearson product-moment
correlation was selected.
33
Table 4
Pearson’s correlations between shame-proneness and diabetes self-care measures
ESS Total ESS Character
ESS Body ESS Behaviour
ESS Diabetes
SCI-RN 281 287 281 292 281Pearson’s r -.327 -.275 -.270 -.170 -.470Size of correlation
moderate small small small moderate
p-Value* p<.001 p<.001 p<.001 p=.002 p<.001
HbA1cN 194 198 194 195 194Pearson’s r .294 .229 .260 .173 .376Size of correlation
small small small small moderate
p-Value* p<.001 p=.001 p<.001 p=.008 p<.001
HbA1c (mmol/mol) with extreme outliers removedN 191 195 191 192 191Pearson’s r .280 .193 .253 .177 .385Size of correlation
small small small small moderate
p-Value* p<.001 p=.003 p<.001 p=.007 p<.001*One-tailed
All shame-proneness subscales were significantly negatively correlated with
diabetes self-care, with coefficients above a .15 level. They were also all significantly
positively correlated with HbA1c (higher HbA1c generally indicates poorer diabetes
self-care). Diabetes-related shame showed the largest coefficient across all three
indicators of self-care, and behavioural shame showed the smallest. This suggests that
as shame-proneness increased, particularly diabetes shame, diabetes self-care
decreased. This provides support for hypothesis 1 that high shame-proneness is
associated with poorer diabetes self-care. This also suggests that shame associated
specifically with diabetes self-care and interactions with healthcare professionals has
a stronger relationship with self-care behaviours than more general shame
experiences.
34
Hypothesis 2 – Depression as a mediating factor between shame and self-care
Shame and depression
Table 5
Pearson’s correlations between depression and shame
DDASS ESS Total ESS Character
ESS Body ESS Behaviour
ESS Diabetes
n 281 287 281 282 281
Pearson’s r
.624 .615 .418 .520 .513
Size of correlation
large large medium large large
p-value p<.001 p<.001 p<.001 p<.001 p<.001
Scatterplots revealed that no substantial violations of linearity and
homoscedasticity occurred (Appendix N). The DDASS scores were positively
skewed, but as they were paired with approximately normally distributed shame
scores, in the context of a large sample, the parametric Pearson’s correlation was
used. All shame subscale scores were positively correlated with depression scores
with a large effect size, with the exception of body shame which showed a medium
effect.
Depression and Self-care
Pearson’s correlation was used to assess the relationship between depressive
affect (DDASS) and the self-care variables (SCIR, HbA1c, HbA1c-EO). There was a
significant, moderate, negative correlation both between depression scores and SCI-R
(r=-.34, p<.001). This indicates that higher depressive affect was associated with
lower levels of self-reported self-care activities. There was also a significant,
moderate positive correlation between depression scores and self-reported HbA1c
(outliers included, r=.371, p<0.001, outliers excluded, r=.317, p<0.001).
35
Mediation analyses
To further assess normality for the purpose of mediation, histograms were
generated for the standardised residuals for each pairing of variables included in the
models (Appendix N). These indicated approximately normal distribution for all
pairings, with the exception of HbA1c. However, as before, removal of the three
statistical outliers resulted in the distribution becoming approximately normal. Models
including both HbA1c and HbA1c-EO are included.
Separate mediation analyses were computed using ESS subscales as predictor
variables. Total ESS score was not used as a predictor as the subscales held enough
reliability to stand alone and provided more useful information as such. In each
mediation analysis, depression score was the mediator. This analysis was repeated
using the SCIR, HbA1c and or HbA1c-EO as dependent measures of self-care.
36
Table 6 Mediation analysis
Predictor Mediator Outcome IV on M M on DV Direct Effect Indirect Effect Total Effect
(IV) (M) (DV) (a path) (b path) (c’ path) (a x b) 95% CI (c path)Diabetes Shame Depression Self-care .54*** -.01 (p=.038) -.04*** -.007 (-.015, .000) -.049***
HbA1c .55*** .83 (p=.002) .90 (p=.001) .459 (.084, .962) 1.357***
HbA1c-EO .54*** .49 (p=.038) .93*** .260 (-.020, .649) 1.193***
Body Shame Depression Self-care .64*** -.03*** -.02 (p=.01) -.017 (-.028, -.009) -.041***
HbA1c .64*** 1.09*** .63 (p=.097) .703 (.277, 1.309) 1.332***
HbA1c-EO .62*** .77*** .63 (p=.059) .478 (.147, .926) 1.108***
Behav Shame Depression Self-care† .42*** -.034*** .0004, (p=.95) -.01 (-.021, -.008) -.014 (p=.004)
HbA1c† .40*** 1.3*** -.043 (p=.84) .525 (.222, .888) .482 (p=.016)
HbA1c-EO .40*** .92*** .054 (p=.78) .366 (.156, .655) .419 (p=.014)
CharacShame
Depression Self-care .35*** -.027*** -.006 (p=.13) -.009 (-.015, -.005) -.016***
HbA1c† .35*** 1.27*** -.007 (p=.97) .446 (.180, .737) .439 (p=.001)
HbA1c –EO .34 *** .960*** -.004 (p=.98) .324 (.124, .572) .32 (p=.007)
***p<.001; 95%, CI in bold suggest evidence that the indirect effect is different from zero, †program flagged that bias corrected confidence intervals were untrustworthy so percentiles were used (Hayes, 2013)
37
38SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
The conditions that need to be met to demonstrate mediation are:
1. Shame-proneness significantly predicts depression (path a)
2. Depression significantly predicts self-care (path b)
3. Shame significantly predicts self-care before depression is entered (path c)
4. The indirect effect of depression must be significantly different from zero. This is
assessed by examining whether the 95% CI encompasses zero.
Table 6 indicates that the first three conditions for mediation were met in all
models. Inspection of the 95% Cis indicates evidence that the indirect effect of
depression is significantly different from zero in all but two models (diabetes shame
on SCI-R scores and HbA1c without extreme outliers). In all other models,
comparison of c and c’ indicates the mediation effects were partial. There is evidence
of stronger mediation effects in the models of behavioural and characterological
shame on SCI-R/HbA1c (both with and without extreme outliers) through depression.
For the models assessing diabetes-related and body shame on self-care outcomes,
comparison of c and c’ reveals the direct effect of shame remains either significant, or
marginally significant.
Additional Findings
Independent sample t-tests were conducted to ascertain whether there were
any differences between those who knew or did not know their HbA1c levels.
Levene’s test for equality of variances was non-significant for all tests, indicating
equality of variance could be assumed, unless otherwise indicated.
39SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Table 7
t-tests to determine difference on measures based on knowledge of HbA1c
Mean (SD) t(df) Sig (2-tailed)
Measure Known Not KnownDepression 6.2(5.7) 8.4(6.3) -2.8(287) p=.006Diabetes shame 6.8 (5.4) 9.7(5.7) -3.9(276) p=<0.001Character shame
13.6(10.2) 16.9 (10.9) -2.4(282) P=.017
Behaviour shame*
11.9(7) 13.6(8) -1.6(123) P=.107
Body shame 6.6(3.9) 8(3.8) -2.7(276) P=.006Self-care (SCIR)
3.6 (0.58) 3.3 (0.59) 3.7(287) p=<0.001
*Equal variance’s not assumed
These tests indicated that those who did not know their HbA1c level had
significantly higher levels of depression compared to those who did not. They also
had significantly higher shame scores in all areas except for behavioural shame and
significantly lower SCIR scores. This suggests that people who have high depressive
affect and shame and low self-care are less likely to be aware of their HbA1c level.
Discussion
Summary of main findings
Hypothesis 1 was supported: each of the subscales of shame-proneness
showed a significant correlation of high shame-proneness with poorer self-care.
Diabetes shame-proneness showed the strongest correlation (moderate-large) out of
each of the shame subscales.
The findings confirm that shame is a relevant area of research for emerging
adults with T1D, both in relation to wellbeing and their ability to engage in vital self-
40SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
care. The findings also suggest that different types of shame may predict self-care via
differing pathways.
There was a significant association between both depression and poorer self-
care and shame and depression, supporting the direction of existing research findings
(Cheung et al., 2004; Gonzalez et al., 2008)
However Hypothesis 2 was only partially supported. All four types of shame
evidenced some partial mediation by depression, though this was only marginal for
diabetes shame. There was evidence of stronger mediation by depression in the
models of behavioural and character shame. For the models assessing diabetes-related
and body shame on self-care outcomes, the direct effect of shame generally remained
significant.
Discussion of main findings
There was evidence that diabetes shame and body shame directly predicted
extent of engagement in self-care. One explanation of this link could be the role of
concealment. Concealment is a key aspect of shame, raised by multiple shame
theorists (Andrews et al., 2002; Gilbert, 2011) and supported by research (Tangney,
1993; Tangney et al., 1996). As such, it was directly captured as part of all shame
subscales. Shame is inherently rooted in the fear of the self being held in the mind of
others with denigration (Gilbert, 2011). Concealment, therefore is the behavioural
response to this fear, and an attempt to avoid it becoming a reality. This fear of
disapproval may be directed at those invested in the individual improving their self-
care, such as parents and healthcare professionals. They may also be peers whom the
individual fears may ridicule or bully them for the overt self-care behaviours that
mark them out as different. This may result in avoidance of self-care activities where
41SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
there is a social risk of disapproval; whether that disapproval relates to engaging or
not engaging in self-care in the ideal way.
Body shame has also been associated with behaviours that would directly
counter self-care. Research has shown that body shame is more predictive of
disordered eating than global shame (Burney & Irwin, 2000). The direct link between
body shame and lowered self-care could be explained by weight control behaviours
directly impacting on self-care. This includes skipping meals and the dangerous
practice of restricting insulin in an effort to lose weight. Research suggests that over a
third of female 11-25 year olds with T1D have experimented with insulin omission
for weight loss (Peveler et al., 2005).
High body shame is also at odds with self-care, which involves active
engagement in caring for the body. The urge may be to avoid this, if engaging with a
shameful body has become too painful, or individuals lack the motivation to care for a
body which they see as unacceptable.
It follows that an urge to conceal, whether that be the body or the self-care
aspects of diabetes, would leave people less able to ask for help with aspects of self-
care they are struggling with. Thus without appropriate support; both practical and
emotional, self-care may continue to decline.
This is supported by Terry and Leary’s (2011) proposal that low self-
compassion (and thus high shame) likely prevents people from seeking medical
attention. They explain this through Neff and Dahm’s (2014) concept of common
humanity; those who see their distressing experiences as unique to themselves, as
opposed to routine problems shared by many people, are less able to be self-
compassionate (and by implication high in shame) as they expect others to view them
negatively. This could be extended to displaying self-care amongst peers. Those high
42SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
in self-compassion may be more accepting of their diabetes as just one aspect of
themselves, and although they may not know others with diabetes, could hold on to
the notion that everyone suffers to some extent and has some understanding and
acceptance of difference. Similarly, those with high self-compassion may be more
accepting of having an imperfect body, and thus more able to engage in self-care.
Character and behavioural shame both showed a much stronger mediation by
depression. Shame and concealment related to general behaviour and personality are
unlikely to influence diabetes self-care directly. Instead, this type of shame may point
more towards isolation and depressive rumination about the self. It may be that for
some people with T1D, as in the general population, there is a vulnerability to shame
related depression, perhaps fuelled by shame-related early maladaptive schema’s
(Young et al., 2003). The additional aspects of depression, such as hopelessness and
self-efficacy may then lead to the resultant deleterious effect on self-care. For people
with T1D who experience more general shame related depression, additional support
is still needed. However this may focus on reducing symptoms of depression, and
tackling any existing shame may be one route to doing this.
Secondary findings
Despite NICE guidance stating HbA1c as the gold standard measurement for
glycaemic control over time (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence,
2015b), it was noteworthy that only two thirds of participants were able to state theirs.
In addition to this, there were some levels that fell outside of usually reported ranges
(Diabetes Digital Media ltd, 2016; Diabetes UK, 2016b) suggesting possible errors.
When asked to state if the value they had given was measured in mmol/mol or %
many participants gave answers that were not compatible. This suggests that either
43SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
HbA1c levels are not widely understood amongst this age group, or many individuals
are not motivated to remember it.
Interestingly, those who did not know their HbA1c levels had significantly
higher levels of depression and shame, and lower levels of self-care. This could be for
a number of reasons. One interpretation is that those with higher levels of emotional
problems (such as depression and shame) are less likely to remember HbA1c, in the
context of multiple possible stressors in their lives. Another interpretation is that the
concealment and avoidant aspect of shame may lead people to engage less with
tangible markers of poor glycaemic control. This finding does suggest that knowledge
of HbA1c and ability to engage with it may be intertwined with emotional factors, as
are other aspects of self-care.
Additionally, only 40% of participants indicated that they had attended a
structured diabetes education programme. This is at odds with UK guidance, which
advises all people with T1D are encouraged to attend such a programme to enable
them to improve their self-care (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence,
2015b). This is concerning, given that T1D is a long-term condition that requires a
high level of patient autonomy and engagement to maintain health. The sample was
also predominantly from the UK. A lack of knowledge of HbA1c may prevent people
with T1D from being able to take a longer-term view of their diabetes (i.e. over the
course of a few months) to effectively self-monitor over time. Not attending a
structured course also removes a normalising opportunity to see others struggling with
similar aspects of self-care.
44SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Strengths, Limitations and Future Directions
A key strength of this study was the large number of participants recruited.
Given the constructs under investigation, sufficient numbers of participants was
essential to capture a range of experience and the possible associations between these.
This was made possible by advertising through diabetes related social media groups
popular with this age group. The large sample size indicates that this was an issue of
concern to people with T1D and the completion rate of the study was high, indicating
acceptability of the survey and a resonance with individual experience.
One limitation born out of the recruitment strategy was the lack of
demographic variability of the sample, which was predominantly white, female and
British. It may be that shame is experienced differently in differing cultures, or
amongst men. Previous research suggests that gender differences in investment in
social relationships may result in women experiencing higher levels of shame than
men (Gross & Hanson, 2000). Further research could explore this potential difference
in a sample with T1D and ascertain whether and how this may impact on self-care.
The main limitation of the current study was that self-care indices were given
by retrospective self-report only. For HbA1c levels this meant that the numbers for
inclusion in analyses were lower than those in the SCI-R analyses. As discussed
earlier, there were notable differences between those who reported and those who did
not report their HbA1c level, which may have affected the result obtained. It was also
not clear whether HbA1c values which fell at the extreme end of the scale were valid
or represented mistakes. However, by removing the statistically extreme outliers, the
relationship between diabetes-related shame and HbA1c more closely resembled the
relationship with the SCIR, lending support to these being valid indicators.
Notwithstanding this limitation, the findings relating to self-care measures (e.g.
45SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
correlations with shame and depression) were largely consistent, suggesting some
internal consistency in the sample in their ratings of self-care.
It is important to note that the results of the present study are correlational in
nature, and thus causation cannot be assumed. An alternative explanation of the
findings is that people feel diabetes-related shame in particular because they know
they are not meeting standards of self-care. However, even if this is the case, it is
important to acknowledge, as shame is a powerful and distressing emotion.
The current study does, however, provide evidence that shame and self-care
are linked, and suggests that some types of shame may be mediated by depression.
Further research should assess the potentially circular relationship between self-care
and shame. For example, for someone who is high in shame-proneness, navigating
initial life adjustments following diagnosis may be particularly difficult as they fail to
control their blood glucose levels. This may then begin to result in avoidance of self-
care (which has begun to evoke experience of shame), which in turn leads to further
experience of shame in healthcare appointments.
Longitudinal research could address the question of how self-care behaviour
and shame-proneness change over time. This would also allow for modelling of
factors which may trigger or maintain experiences of shame, such as differing
experiences with peers, healthcare professionals and family.
Clinical Implications
For an emerging adult with a vulnerability to experiencing shame, they may be
caught in a double bind of being ashamed to adhere to self-care around some
members of their peer-group, but also feeling shame at not having improved their
glycaemic control at healthcare appointments.
46SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Self-compassion is often discussed as the adaptive opposite of shame (Gilbert
& Procter, 2006b) because it involves a focus on understanding that failure and
suffering are universal (Neff & Dahm, 2014). Cultivating self-compassion is therefore
a potential avenue for reducing shame. A self-compassionate narrative within a
diabetes healthcare appointment would be one that focussed on individualised self-
care targets rather than continually aiming for below the recommended 48mmol,
which some may never achieve. Wolpert and Anderson (2001) highlight the increased
risk of perpetuating perfectionism within the clinician-patient relationship in the case
of T1D, particularly when guidelines for HbA1c levels are not personalised and may
be very challenging to reach. Individualised targets would weigh up the medical costs
and benefits alongside the psychosocial, ensuring that the goal is always quality of
life.
Archer (2014) highlights the inter-relational way that shame can play out
within the relationship between the Clinician and person with T1D and how shame
may well also arise for Clinicians who feel de-skilled in the face of a complex
presentation. He suggests that Clinicians reflect on their own experiences of shame
and consider sharing an example with their patients. Whilst many Clinicians may feel
uncomfortable doing this directly, conversations could at least openly acknowledge
the widespread difficulty that young people in particular face in maintaining their self-
care routines, in order to reinforce the concept of common humanity.
Compassion could also be usefully cultivated within peer groups. Emerging
adults are increasingly turning to social media platforms for T1D peer support
(Greene, Choudhry, Kilabuk, & Shrank, 2011). Encouraging the use of these
platforms, and other face-to-face peer support when possible, could be an effective
47SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
way of enhancing self-compassion amongst emerging adults with T1D through a
focus on the sharing of common difficult experiences and mutual understanding.
Conclusion
This study provides preliminary support for a model whereby diabetes and
body shame directly affect T1D self-care in emerging adults. Conversely, character
and behavioural shame showed evidence of almost complete mediation by depression.
Further research is needed to explore experiences of diabetes and body shame over
time alongside factors which may play a role in triggering and maintaining the
experience of shame. Cultivating a self-compassionate narrative in healthcare
consultations may be one potential avenue for further research to counteract
experiences of shame amongst this population.
48SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
References
Abbott Diabetes Care. (2015). 3 ways to help manage your numbers. Retrieved from
https://freestylediabetes.co.uk/freestyle-thinking/post/manage-your-numbers
Andrews, B., Qian, M., & Valentine, J. D. (2002). Predicting depressive symptoms with a
new measure of shame: The experience of shame scale. The British Journal of
Clinical Psychology / the British Psychological Society, 41, 29-42.
Archer, A. (2014). Shame and diabetes self‐management. Practical Diabetes, 31(3), 102-106.
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens
through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55(5), 469-480.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.
Beck, A. T. (1991). Cognitive therapy: A 30-year retrospective. American Psychologist,
46(4), 368-375.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for
measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Bishara, A. J., & Hittner, J. B. (2012). Testing the significance of a correlation with non
normal data: Comparison of pearson, spearman, transformation, and resampling
approaches. . Psychological Methods, 17, 399-417. doi:10.1037/a0028087
Bryden, K. S., Peveler, R. C., Stein, A., Neil, A., Mayou, R. A., & Dunger, D. B. (2001).
Clinical and psychological course of diabetes from adolescence to young adulthood:
A longitudinal cohort study. Diabetes Care, 24(9), 1536-1540.
Burney, J., & Irwin, H. J. (2000). Shame and guilt in women with eating‐disorder
symptomatology. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56(1), 51-61.
49SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Cheung, M., Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2004). An exploration of shame, social rank and
rumination in relation to depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(5),
1143-1153.
Clements, M. A., Foster, N. C., Maahs, D. M., Schatz, D. A., Olson, B. A., T salikian, E., . . .
Chen, V. (2015). Hemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) changes over time among adolescent and
young adult participants in the T1D exchange clinic registry. Pediatric Diabetes,
doi:10.1111/pedi.12295
Diabetes Digital Media ltd. (2014). Guide to HbA1c. Retrieved from
http://www.diabetes.co.uk/what-is-hba1c.html
Diabetes Digital Media ltd. (2016). Guide to HbA1c: HbA1c as an indicator of diabetes
control. Retrieved from http://www.diabetes.co.uk/what-is-hba1c.html
Diabetes UK. (2014). Diabetes: Facts and stats. ( No. 3).Diabetes UK.
Diabetes UK. (2015). Hypos and hypers. Retrieved from https://www.diabetes.org.uk/Guide-
to-diabetes/Complications/Hypos-Hypers/
Diabetes UK. (2016a). Testing: HbA1c. Retrieved from http://www.diabetes.org.uk/Guide-
to-diabetes/Monitoring/Testing/#glucose
Diabetes UK. (2016b). Testing: HbA1c results. Retrieved from
https://www.diabetes.org.uk/Guide-to-diabetes/Monitoring/Testing/
Diabetes.co.uk, Diabetes Digital Media ltd. (2014). Diabetes and stress. Retrieved from
http://www.diabetes.co.uk/diabetes-destress.html
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.
Feiring, C., Taska, L., & Lewis, M. (2002). Adjustment following sexual abuse discovery:
The role of shame and attributional style. Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 79-92.
Field, A. P. (2000). Discovering statistics using SPSS for windows. London: Sage
Publications.
50SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Frich, J. C., Malterud, K., & Fugelli, P. (2007). Experiences of guilt and shame in patients
with familial hypercholesterolemia: A qualitative interview study. Patient Education
and Counseling, 69(1), 108-113.
Fritz, M. S., & Mackinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect.
Psychological Science, 18(3), 233-239. doi:PSCI1882 [pii]
Frone, M. R. (2000). Interpersonal conflict at work and psychological outcomes: Testing a
model among young workers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(2), 246-
255.
Gilbert, P. (2000). The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: The role of the
evaluation of social rank. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 7(3), 174-189.
Gilbert, P. (2011). Shame in psychotherapy and the role of compassion focused therapy. In R.
L. Dearing, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Shame in the therapy hour.
325-354. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12326-014
Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high shame
and self‐criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach. Clinical
Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13(6), 353-379.
Gonzalez, J. S., Peyrot, M., McCarl, L. A., Collins, E. M., Serpa, L., Mimiaga, M. J., &
Safren, S. A. (2008). Depression and diabetes treatment nonadherence: A meta-
analysis. Diabetes Care, 31(12), 2398-2403. doi:10.2337/dc08-1341 [doi]
Greene, J. A., Choudhry, N. K., Kilabuk, E., & Shrank, W. H. (2011). Online social
networking by patients with diabetes: A qualitative evaluation of communication with
facebook. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 26(3), 287-292.
51SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Gross, C. A., & Hansen, N. E. (2000). Clarifying the experience of shame: The role of
attachment style, gender, and investment in relatedness.Personality and individual
differences, 28(5), 897-907.
Guassora, A. D., Reventlow, S., & Malterud, K. (2014). Shame, honor and responsibility in
clinical dialog about lifestyle issues: A qualitative study about patients’ presentations
of self. Patient Education and Counseling, 97(2), 195-199.
doi:10.1016/j.pec.2014.08.003
Halvorsen, M., Wang, C. E., Eisemann, M., & Waterloo, K. (2010). Dysfunctional attitudes
and early maladaptive schemas as predictors of depression: A 9-year follow-up study.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(4), 368-379.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Frequently asked questions about my macros. Retrieved from
http://afhayes.com/macrofaq.html
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond baron and kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new
millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408-420.
Jones, J. M., Lawson, M. L., Daneman, D., Olmsted, M. P., & Rodin, G. (2000). Eating
disorders in adolescent females with and without type 1 diabetes: Cross sectional
study. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.), 320(7249), 1563-1566.
Karlsson, A., Arman, M., & Wikblad, K. (2008). Teenagers with type 1 diabetes—a
phenomenological study of the transition towards autonomy in self-management.
International Journal of Nursing Studies, 45(4), 562-570.
Kime, N. (2014). 'Join us on our journey': Exploring the experiences of children and young
people with type 1 diabetes and their parents. Practical Diabetes, 31(1), 24-28.
doi:10.1002/pdi.1823
52SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Korczak, D., Pereira, S., Koulajian, K., Matejcek, A., & Giacca, A. (2011). Type 1 diabetes
mellitus and major depressive disorder: Evidence for a biological link. Diabetologia,
54(10), 2483-2493.
Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. Psychoanalytic Review,
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states:
Comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression
and anxiety inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335-343.
Lustman, P. J., Anderson, R. J., Freedland, K. E., de Groot, M., Carney, R. M., & Clouse, R.
E. (2000). Depression and poor glycemic control: A meta-analytic review of the
literature. Diabetes Care, 23(7), 934-942.
Mommersteeg, P. M. C., Herr, R., Pouwer, F., Holt, R. I. G., & Loerbroks, A. (2013). The
association between diabetes and an episode of depressive symptoms in the 2002
world health survey: An analysis of 231 797 individuals from 47 countries. Diabetic
Medicine, 30(6), e208-e214. doi:10.1111/dme.12193
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2015a). Diabetes (type 1 and type 2) in
children and young people: Diagnosis and management. NICE guideline [NG18].
Retrieved from https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng18/resources
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2015b). Type 1 diabetes in adults:
Diagnosis and management. NICE guidelines [NG17]. Retrieved from
https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng17
Neff, K. D., & Dahm, K. A. (2014). Self-compassion: What it is, what it does, and how it
relates to mindfulness. Mindfulness and Self-Regulation.New York: Springer,
NHS Choices. (2013). Type 1 diabetes - complications. Retrieved from
http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Diabetes-type1/Pages/Complications.aspx
53SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
NHS Choices. (2015). Diabetic ketoacidosis - causes. Retrieved from
http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/diabetic-ketoacidosis/Pages/Causes.aspx
NICE. (2016). NICE pathways: Diabetes in children and young people, transition to adult
care. Retrieved from http://pathways.nice.org.uk/pathways/diabetes-in-children-and-
young-people
Odegard, P. S., & Capoccia, K. (2007). Medication taking and diabetes: A systematic review
of the literature. The Diabetes Educator, 33(6), 1014-1029. doi:33/6/1014 [pii]
Palladino, D. K., Helgeson, V. S., Reynolds, K. A., Becker, D. J., Siminerio, L. M., &
Escobar, O. (2013). Emerging adults with type 1 diabetes: A comparison to peers
without diabetes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38(5), 506-517.
doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jst002 [doi]
Peters, A., Laffel, L., & American Diabetes Association Transitions Working Group. (2011).
Diabetes care for emerging adults: Recommendations for transition from pediatric to
adult diabetes care systems: A position statement of the american diabetes association,
with representation by the american college of osteopathic family physicians, the
american academy of pediatrics, the american association of clinical endocrinologists,
the american osteopathic association, the centers for disease control and prevention,
children with diabetes, the endocrine society, the international society for pediatric
and adolescent diabetes, juvenile diabetes research foundation international, the
national diabetes education program, and the pediatric endocrine society (formerly
lawson wilkins pediatric endocrine society). Diabetes Care, 34(11), 2477-2485.
doi:10.2337/dc11-1723 [doi]
Peveler, R. C., Bryden, K. S., Neil, H. A., Fairburn, C. G., Mayou, R. A., Dunger, D. B., &
Turner, H. M. (2005). The relationship of disordered eating habits and attitudes to
54SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
clinical outcomes in young adult females with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care, 28(1),
84-88. doi:28/1/84 [pii]
Scholes, C., Mandleco, B., Roper, S., Dearing, K., Dyches, T., & Freeborn, D. (2013). A
qualitative study of young people's perspectives of living with type 1 diabetes: Do
perceptions vary by levels of metabolic control? Journal of Advanced Nursing, 69(6),
1235-1247 13p. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2012.06111.x
Seigel, W. M., Golden, N. H., Gough, J. W., Lashley, M. S., & Sacker, I. M. (1990).
Depression, self-esteem, and life events in adolescents with chronic diseases. Journal
of Adolescent Health Care, 11(6), 501-504. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=c8h&AN=1991125059&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Sheeber, L., Hops, H., Alpert, A., Davis, B., & Andrews, J. (1997). Family support and
conflict: Prospective relations to adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 25(4), 333-344.
Stenner, K., Grant, L., Bond, R., Elliott, J., Hopkins, D., Pouwer, F., . . . Cooke, D. (In
preparation for submission to Diabetologia.). Assessment of Self-care behaviours
related to glycaemic control in type 1 diabetes: Development and validation of the
SCB-T1D questionnaire. . Unpublished manuscript.
Stewart, S. M., Simmons, A., & White, P. C. (2011). Somatic items in the assessment of
depressive symptoms in pediatric patients with diabetes. Journal of Behavioral
Medicine, 34(2), 112-119. doi:10.1007/s10865-010-9289-1
Stuckey, H. L., Mullan-Jensen, C., Reach, G., Kovacs Burns, K., Piana, N., Vallis, M., . . .
Peyrot, M. (2014). Personal accounts of the negative and adaptive psychosocial
55SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
experiences of people with diabetes in the second diabetes attitudes, wishes and needs
(DAWN2) study. Diabetes Care, 37(9), 2466-2474. doi:10.2337/dc13-2536
Stupiansky, N. W., Hanna, K. M., Slaven, J. E., Weaver, M. T., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2013).
Impulse control, diabetes-specific self-efficacy, and diabetes management among
emerging adults with type 1 diabetes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38(3), 247-
254. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jss110 [doi]
Tangney, J. P. (1993). Shame and guilt. . In G. Costello (Ed.), Symptoms of depression.
wiley series on personality processes (pp. 161-180). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and
embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
70(6), 1256-1269.
Terry, M. L., & Leary, M. R. (2011). Self-compassion, self-regulation, and health. Self and
Identity, 10(3), 352-362.
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Self-conscious emotions: Where self and emotion meet.
The Self in Social Psychology, , 187-209.
Weinger, K., Butler, H. A., Welch, G. W., & La Greca, A. M. (2005). Measuring diabetes
self-care: A psychometric analysis of the self-care inventory-revised with adults.
Diabetes Care, 28(6), 1346-1352. doi:28/6/1346 [pii]
Wolpert, H. A., & Anderson, B. J. (2001). Metabolic control matters: Why is the message
lost in the translation? the need for realistic goal-setting in diabetes care. Diabetes
Care, 24(7), 1301-1303.
Young, J. E., Klosko, J. S., & Weishaar, M. E. (2003). Schema therapy: A practitioner's guide
Guilford Press.
56SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
57SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
List of Appendices
Appendix A Example advertisement used online and in poster form–
wording was changed slightly across different mediums
depending on audience and character limit.
Appendix BEthics approval letter
Appendix C Information screenAppendix D Consent screenAppendix E Normalising statementAppendix F Demographic and diabetes
specific questions
Appendix G Additional diabetes specific questions
Appendix H Debrief screenAppendix I ESSAppendix J ESS Diabetes shame scale
Appendix K SCI-RAppendix L DDASSAppendix M HistogramsAppendix N ScatterplotsAppendix O Box-plotsAppendix P Histograms for residuals
Appendix Q Health Psychology Instructions for Authors
58SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix A- Advertisement Poster
Do you have type 1
diabetes?Self-care for people with type 1 diabetes, such as taking medication and adjusting diet, is essential to staying healthy. However many people struggle to keep up with this complex routine.
We’re looking for people between the ages of 16 and 25 with a diagnosis of type 1
diabetes to take part in a 15 minute online questionnaire, looking at aspects of self-care and
emotional wellbeing. This research could lead to new guidelines or psychological interventions for people
struggling with self-care, helping them to manage their diabetes and health long term.
To take part please scan the code below or visit :
59SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
http://surveys.fahs.surrey.ac.uk/Self_Care_in_Diabetes/
Thanks for your time.
Study end date: October 2015
Appendix B – Statement of ethical approval
60SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix C- Participant Information Screen
Self-care and emotions in young people with type 1 diabetes
Information ScreenIntroductionI am a Trainee Clinical Psychologist and as part of my Doctorate I am conducting a research study investigating self-care in diabetes. Before you decide whether you would like to participate, you need to understand why the research is being done and what it will involve for you. Please take the time to read the following information carefully and talk to others about the study if you wish.
What is the purpose of the study?Self-care through taking the right medication at the right time, as well as making lifestyle adjustments is vital for people with type 1 diabetes. However, many struggle with their self-care and it is important to find out more about this in order to know how best to support people to stay healthy. This research aims to look at the role of emotions in self-care.
Who can take part?To take part you should be between the ages of 16-25, able to read and understand English and have a diagnosis of type 1 diabetes.
Do I have to take part?No, you do not have to participate. You can also choose to withdraw at any time without giving a reason.
What will my involvement require? You will be asked to complete a short online questionnaire. This asks questions about your emotions along with a few demographic questions about yourself (e.g. age, gender). We also ask for your most recent Hba1c level reading (though you can still take part if you don’t know it), along with a series of questions about
61SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
medication routine and lifestyle. Participation will take around 15 minutes.
What will I have to do?If you would like to take part, please read the consent statements on the next page. If you agree with these, please indicate this by clicking “Yes” and the questionnaire will appear on the next page. If you would like more time to decide whether you want to participate, you can come back to this page when you feel ready.
What are the possible disadvantages or risks of taking part?As the research is looking at emotions, some questions require you to “look inwards” and think about your own mood and state of mind. It is possible that some people may find this upsetting. You are encouraged to only take part if you are happy to think about, and answer questions about your emotions. If after filling out the questionnaire you do have concerns about your emotional wellbeing, or diabetes self-care we would see this as an opportunity for you to seek support, by speaking to your regular healthcare specialist. Advice on how to find additional services that can offer support are included on the debrief screen, which will appear after you have completed the questionnaire. At the end of each section of questions you will be asked if you would like to continue; by clicking “No – withdraw from the study now” you can choose to withdraw from the study and skip straight to the debrief screen.
What are the possible benefits of taking part? It is unlikely that you will benefit directly but your participation may give us vital information that contributes towards helping others with their self-care.
What happens when the research study stops?No further participation is required after the online questionnaire. The questionnaire is for research purposes only, and as such you will not be able to access your individual scores. However, interested participants can opt to be emailed a summary of the study, highlighting its findings, once it is complete. Email addresses will not be linked with individual questionnaire answers and will be destroyed once we have sent out summaries.
What if there is a problem?Any complaint or concern about any aspect of the way you have been dealt with during the course of the study will be addressed; please contact the research supervisor: Dr Laura Simonds:[email protected].
Will my taking part in the study be kept confidential?Yes. All of the information you give will be rganizing so that those
62SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
reading reports from the research will not know who has contributed to it.All participants will be assigned a number in place of their name to ensure confidentiality. All research data will be stored securely in a database and then destroyed after 10 years. Data collection and retention will be in accordance with the Data Protection Act (1998).
Contact details of researcher and supervisor:Principal Researcher: Alice TurlandEmail: [email protected] Supervisor: Dr Laura SimondsEmail [email protected]: School of PsychologyUniversity of SurreyStag Hill CampusGuildford, GU2 7XHPhone number: 01483 686936
Who is rganizing and funding the research?This research is being completed as part of the PsychD training in Clinical Psychology at the University of Surrey. It will form part of the researcher’s portfolio, and efforts will also be made to publish in an academic journal, and through the charity Diabetes UK.
Who has reviewed the project?The study has been reviewed and received a Favourable Ethical Opinion (FEO) from the University of Surrey Faculty of Arts and Human Sciences Ethics Committee.
Thank you for taking the time to read this Information Screen.
PLEASE CLICK HERE TO START THE SURVEY
63SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix D
Consent Screen
I voluntarily agree to take part in the study on Self-care and emotions in young people with Type 1 Diabetes
I have read and understood the Information Screen. I have been given a full explanation by the investigators of the nature, purpose, location and likely duration of the study, and of what I will be expected to do. I have been advised about any discomfort and possible ill-effects on my health and well-being which may result.
I consent to my personal data, as outlined in the accompanying information sheet, being used for this study and other research. I understand that all personal data relating to volunteers is held and processed in the strictest confidence, and in accordance with the Data Protection Act (1998).
I understand that I am free to withdraw from the study at any time without needing to justify my decision and without prejudice.
I confirm that I have read and understood the above and freely consent to participating in this study. I have been given adequate time to consider my participation and agree to comply with the instructions and restrictions of the study.
By clicking the “YES” button you consent to the above statements.
Appendix E – Opening normalising statement, (Stenner et al, In preparation)
64SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
“Some people tell us that managing their diabetes can be challenging. We would like to know about the things you are doing to look after your diabetes, and
about your emotional wellbeing. If we understand how people manage, we can offer better care and support. We realise that you may not be doing what you have been advised to do all of the time. This is not a test. Please be as honest as you
can when you are filling this in. Your responses will be confidential. “
Appendix F – Demographic and diabetes specific questions
65SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
What is your gender?
Male
Female
Transgender
Other
Please indicate your occupational status
Employed full time
Employed part time
Self-employed
Occasional/ casual work
Student
Other
What is your ethnic group?
Choose one option that best describes your ethnic group or background
White
1. English/Welsh/Scottish/Northern Irish/British
2. Irish
3. Gypsy or Irish Traveller
4. Any other White background, please describe
Mixed/Multiple ethnic groups
5. White and Black Caribbean
6. White and Black African
7. White and Asian
8. Any other Mixed/Multiple ethnic background, please describe
Asian/Asian British
9. Indian
10. Pakistani
66SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
11. Bangladeshi
12. Chinese
13. Any other Asian background, please describe
Black/ African/Caribbean/Black British
14. African
15. Caribbean
16. Any other Black/African/Caribbean background, please describe
Other ethnic group
17. Arab
18. Any other ethnic group, please describe
ONS ethnicity question, retrieved from: http://ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/measuring-equality/equality/ethnic-nat-identity-religion/ethnic-group/index.html
Do you currently live with parents / carers?
Yes
No
Do you currently live in the United Kingdom?
Yes
No
(If no) Please state country of residence
Approximately at what age were you diagnosed with type 1 diabetes?
Do your parents / carers attend Diabetes related healthcare appointments with you?
Always
Often
67SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Occasionally
Never
Please indicate the extent to which you manage your diabetes independently
I manage my diabetes independently 1 2 3 4 5 6 7My parent / carer manages my diabetes
How easy do you find it to return to your diabetic regimen following a lapse?
Very easy Easy Difficult Very Difficult
Do you currently have a diagnosed mental health problem(s)?
Yes
No
Please state current diagnosed mental health problem(s)
Have you previously been diagnosed with a mental health problem(s)?
Yes
No
Please state previously diagnosed mental health problem(s)
68SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix G – Additional diabetes specific questions, (Stenner et al, In preparation)
How do you manage your diabetes?
Multiple Daily Injections (MDI) – injecting with every meal and using long-acting insulin once or twice a day.
Insulin Pump
Other
Have you attended any diabetes structured education courses?
Yes
No
(If yes) In which year did you attend the course?
Appendix H
69SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Debrief Screen
You have now reached the end of the study. Thank you for taking the time to participate. The questionnaire was specifically looking at low mood,
experience of shame and how much self-care young people engage in. Everyone experiences shame so it is not unusual. However, some people
experience it more than others. People can feel shame when they think they are not what they would like to be or if they think other people disapprove of
them. For young people, this is often about seeing yourself as different to your friends. We are interested in knowing whether young people with diabetes feel shame and if this relates to low mood. We also want to know if shame and low
mood relate to how much young people with diabetes engage in self-care activities. If it looks like this is the case, it could lead to offering talking
therapies focusing specifically on addressing shame and low mood, for those that struggle with diabetes self-care.
As stated in the information screen, we are unable to provide you with your
questionnaire scores as they are designed for research purposes only. However if, after completing the questionnaires, you have any
concerns about your diabetes self-care, mood, or shame please seek advice from your regular healthcare practitioner. Below are links to
further resources for people who have concerns about their emotional or diabetic wellbeing.
Lastly, If you know anyone else between the ages of 16 and 25, who has type 1 diabetes, we would be very grateful if you could pass on the details of this study to them.
Details of where to get help to if you are concerned about your mental health
or diabetes self-care
More information www.depressionalliance.org – This website contains useful information for anyone who thinks they may be depressed. You can learn about the symptoms, find self-help advice, and access an online support forum. Diabetes UK - http://www.diabetes.org.uk/About_us/ - Diabetes UK provide information, help and peer support to help people with diabetes manage their condition effectively.
Access to professional support
70SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
If you think you may be depressed, or suffering with any other mental health problems, and would like to explore treatment options, please visit your local GP. Psychological therapy and counselling is usually available on the NHS, but this will vary depending on where you live. These approaches can be incredibly helpful in allowing you some space to explore the problems in your life, and learn techniques to begin to feel like your old self again. Your GP will be able to let you know what’s available in your area, or discuss medication options with you.
It is common to find diabetes self-care a challenge. If you are concerned about any aspects of your diabetes self-care, please contact your GP or diabetes healthcare professional to discuss how they can support you. Helplines Samaritans –24-hour support line. Talk to a trained supporter in complete confidence about whatever is troubling you.08457 90 90 90 MIND – A mental health charity that can provide information and advice about accessing support. Lines are open 9am to 6pm, Monday to Friday (except for bank holidays).0300 123 3393 Access to urgent support If you’ve reached crisis point and are worried that you may harm yourself, please call 999 or visit your local Accident and Emergency. There you will be able to talk to an experienced professional who can make a safety plan with you, and put you in touch with the right support services.
Appendix I: Experience of Shame Scale (ESS) (Andrews et al., 2002)
71SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Everybody at times can feel embarrassed, self-conscious or ashamed. These questions are about such feelings if they have occurred at any time in the past year. There are
no 'right or 'wrong answers. Please select the response which applies to you.
Not at all
1
A little
2
Moderately
3
Very Much
4
1. Have you felt ashamed of any of your personal habits?
2. Have you worried about what other people think of any of your personal habits?
3. Have you tried to cover up or conceal any of your personal habits?
4. Have you felt ashamed of your manner with others?
5. Have you worried about what other people think of your manner with others?
6. Have you avoided people because of your manner?
7. Have you felt ashamed of the sort of person you are?
8. Have you worried about what other people think of the sort of person you are?
9. Have you tried to conceal from others the sort of person you are?
10.Have you felt ashamed of your ability to do things?
11.Have you worried about what other people think of your ability to do things?
12.Have you avoided people because of your inability to do things?
13.Do you feel ashamed when you do something wrong?
72SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
14.Have you worried about what other people think of you when you do something wrong?
15.Have you tried to cover up or conceal things you felt ashamed of having done?
16.Have you felt ashamed when you said something stupid?17.Have you worried about what other people think of you when you said something stupid?
18.Have you avoided contact with anyone who knew you said something stupid?
19.Have you felt ashamed when you failed in a competitive situation?
20.Have you worried about what other people think of you when you failed in a competitive situation?
21.Have you avoided people who have seen you fail?
22.Have you felt ashamed of your body or any part of it?
23.Have you worried about what other people think of your appearance?
24.Have you avoided looking at yourself in the mirror?
25.Have you wanted to hide or conceal your body or any part of it?
73SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix J: ESS Diabetes shame subscale (created for purposes of current study)
U Not at all
1
A little
2
Moderately
3
Very Much
4
26. Have you felt ashamed of your diabetes
self-care activities (e.g. insulin, medication,
diet and lifestyle changes)?
27. Have you worried about what other people
think about your diabetes self-care activities?
28. Have you avoided diabetes self-care activities
because of a worry about what others think?
29. Have you felt ashamed when Healthcare
professionals have spoken to you about diabetes
self-care activities?
30. Have you worried about what Healthcare
professionals think about your diabetes self-care?
31. Have you concealed aspects of your
diabetes self-care from your Healthcare
professionals?
74SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix K - Self Care Inventory-Revised Version (SCI-R) (Weinger et al., 2005)
This survey measures what you actually do, not what you are advised to do. Howhave you followed your diabetes treatment plan in the past 1-2 months?
Never: 1 Rarely: 2 Sometimes: 3 Usually: 4 Always: 5
1 2 3 4 5
1. Check blood glucose with ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) monitor
2. Record blood glucose results ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
3. If type 1: Check ketones when ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )glucose level is high or Have type 2 diabetes ( )
4. Take the correct dose of ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )diabetes pills or insulin or Not taking diabetes pills or insulin ( )
5. Take diabetes pills or insulin at ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )the right time orNot taking diabetes pills or insulin ( )
6. Eat the correct food portions ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
7. Eat meals/snacks on time ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
8. Keep food records ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
9. Read food labels ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
10. Treat low blood glucose with ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )just the recommended amountof carbohydrate
Never had low blood glucose ( )
75SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
1 2 3 4 511. Carry quick acting sugar to treat low blood glucose ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
12. Come in for clinic ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )appointments
13. Wear a Medic Alert ID ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
14. Exercise ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )
15. If on insulin: Adjust insulin ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( )dosage based on glucosevalues, food, and exercise
Not onInsulin ( )
76SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix L
DDASS 7 – Depression subscale
Please read each statement and circle a number 0, 1, 2 or 3 which indicates how much the statement applied to you over the past week. There are no right or wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any statement.
The rating scale is as follows:
0 Did not apply to me at all
1 Applied to me to some degree, or some of the time
2 Applied to me to a considerable degree or a good part of time
3 Applied to me very much or most of the time
1. I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all 0 1 2 3
2. I found it difficult to work up the initiative to do things 0 1 2 3
3. I felt that I had nothing to look forward to 0 1 2 3
4. I felt down-hearted and blue 0 1 2 3
5. I was unable to become enthusiastic about anything 0 1 2 3
6. I felt I wasn’t worth much as a person 0 1 2 3
7. I felt that life was meaningless 0 1 2 3
77SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix M – Histograms to show distribution of variables
78SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix N – Scatterplots to show correlations between all variables in main analysis
SCI-R
79SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
HbA1c
80SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Depression and Shame
81SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix O – Boxplot to show extreme outliers in HbA1c sample
82SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix P – Standardised residual graphs
Dependent variable: depression
83SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
IV: diabetes shame IV: body shame
IV: behavioural shame IV: character shame
Dependent variable: Self-care (SCI-R)
IV: Diabetes shame IV: Body shame
IV: body shame
84SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
IV: Behavioural shame IV: Character Shame
IV: depression
Dependent Variable: HbA1c
IV: diabetes shame
IV: behavior shame IV: character shame
IV: depression
85SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Dependent variable: HbA1c-EO
IV: Diabetes shame IV: body shame
IV: behaviour shame IV: character shame
86SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
IV: Depression
Appendix Q – Guidelines for Authors
Instructions for Authors- Health Psychology
ManuscriptsThe manuscript title should be accurate, fully explanatory, and no longer than 12 words. The
title should reflect the content and population studied. If the paper reports a randomized clinical trial, this should be indicated in the title. The title of brief reports should start with the words "Brief Report".
The title page should include the names of all authors and their affiliations at the time the research was done. This information will be masked to ensure a blind peer review process by the editorial office. Authors should make sure that all other identifying information in the text of the paper is masked/removed prior to submission.
All manuscripts must include a structured abstract containing a maximum of 250 words with the following sections:
Objective (brief statement of the purpose of the study); Methods (summary of the participants, design, measures, procedure); Results (primary findings); and Conclusions (specific statement of the implications of the data).
87SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Please supply up to five keywords or brief phrases after the abstract. The Introduction should not exceed 3–4 pages in length. The paper should be referenced appropriately but excessive citations should be avoided.
All research involving human participants must describe oversight of the research process by the relevant Institutional Review Boards and should describe consent and assent procedures briefly in the Methods section.
All statistical tests should include effect size whenever possible.First person language ("I", "we") should be avoided. Terminology should be sensitive to the
individual who has a disease or disability. The journal endorses the concept of "people first, not their disability." Terminology should reflect the "person with a disability" (e.g., children with diabetes, persons with HIV infection, families of people with cancer) rather than the condition as an adjective (e.g., diabetic children, HIV patients, cancer families). Nonsexist language should be used.
It is important to highlight the significance and novel contribution of the work. The translation of research into practice must be evidenced in all manuscripts. Authors should incorporate a meaningful discussion of the clinical and/or policy implications of their work throughout the manuscript, rather than simply providing a separate section for this material.
Health Psychology publishes a broad array of types of papers. Authors of qualitative and measure development papers should read the guidelines for these types of papers, noted below.
Reference
American Psychological Association (2014). Health Psychology: Instructions for
Authors: Manuscripts. Retrieved 08/06 2014. Retrieved from:
http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/hea/.
PSYCHD CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Major Research Project Proposal
88SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
An exploration of shame in young adults with Diabetes, and its
relationship with depression and treatment adherence.
Word count: 2868
Background and Theoretical Rationale
There are currently 3.2 million people in the UK with a diagnosis of diabetes;
around 10% of these have type 1 (Diabetes UK). Type 1 diabetes is most commonly
diagnosed between the ages of 10 and 14, meaning that the majority of sufferers have
added pressure in their teenage years of adapting to managing their condition
independently. Management of type 1 diabetes requires a high level of responsibility
on the part of the patient given that management regimens involve daily dietary and
lifestyle changes, carbohydrate calculations, multiple blood glucose tests and self-
89SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
injecting of insulin. This is likely to be even more difficult in the context of
adolescents exploring some activities for the first time, with limited prior experience
of how their condition may be affected; such as a variety of sports, cooking for
oneself, and drinking alcohol. It is therefore perhaps not surprising that low adherence
to condition self-management is a widespread issue, with adolescents often reporting
less adaptive condition self-management than children or adults (Bryden et al., 2001;
Johnson, Freund, Silverstein, Hansen, & Malone, 1990). When researched
longitudinally, problems with condition self-management present in adolescence have
also been shown to extend into adulthood (Kovacs, Goldston, Obrosky, & Iyengar,
1992) suggesting that this is a key period for targeted intervention. This research aims
to explore the role of shame as a potential contributing factor towards variability in
condition self-management for adolescents and young adults with type 1 diabetes.
Condition self-management in diabetes consists of four main aspects: insulin
administration, blood glucose testing, dietary adjustments and exercise. Many studies
examining condition self-management in diabetes have used HbA1c levels as an
objective index. HbA1c, or glycosylated haemoglobin, is measured routinely by blood
test at diabetes healthcare appointments. The test gives an indication of the patient’s
average blood glucose levels for the preceding 2-3 months (Diabetes Digital Media
ltd, 2014). Condition self-management can also be measured subjectively by asking
patients to estimate their adherence to certain aspects of their diabetes management
over a set period of time. A review of the literature highlighted a number of
commonly cited reasons for not adhering to diabetes regimens including fear of
inadvertently causing hypoglycaemia, a dislike of needles, fear of weight gain and a
lack of understanding of a complex regimen (Odegard & Capoccia, 2007). However
90SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
psychosocial factors such as experience of critical parenting style (Grabill et al., 2010)
and, most commonly, depression (Gonzalez et al., 2008) have also shown associations
with lower condition self-management.
Relatively little empirical work has been undertaken examining psychosocial
factors predicting variability in condition self-management. One potential factor is
low self-esteem. Rosenberg (1965) describes self-esteem broadly as “the direction of
self-attitude”. Leary and Macdonald (2005) conceptualise it as an “affectively laden
self-evaluation” (p.401) and distinguish it from related concepts such as self-concept
and self-efficacy on the basis that one can have high self-esteem whilst being aware of
one’s flaws. Self-esteem in the face of personal flaws might increase an adolescent’s
resilience whilst navigating potentially several unsuccessful initial attempts to self-
manage their condition, and give them the psychological strength to continue.
To date, 7 studies have used a validated global measure of self-esteem to
compare adolescents with diabetes with a healthy control group, but the findings were
surprisingly mixed. Three studies showed no significant difference in the mean self-
esteem score of adolescents with diabetes compared to those without (Jacobson et al.,
1984; Kellerman et al., 1980; Sullivan, 1978), one showed higher self-esteem in the
diabetic group (Zashikhina & Hagglof, 2013) and only one showed lower self-esteem
in the diabetic group (Seigel et al., 1990).
Furthermore, studies have found no link between self-esteem and HbA1c
levels either through the course of an intervention (de Wit et al., 2008), or as a
predictor of later HbA1c levels in a longitudinal study (Bryden et al., 2001).
91SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Rather than prematurely concluding that self-esteem is not an important
underlying factor in either the psychological experience of young people with
diabetes, or condition self-management, a more detailed and nuanced ways of
conceptualising self-evaluative mechanisms should be explored. Self-esteem
measures such as the Coopersmith Self-esteem inventory (Coopersmith, 1967) only
measure the degree to which an individual largely likes or dislikes him/herself, and
are often contaminated by questions that overlap with perceptions of competence. By
instead considering the emotional states that underlie global self-evaluation, a
qualitative distinction can be made between those who recognise flaws in their
competence, and those who experience the intensely painful emotion of shame, and
the self-derogatory internal dialogue that accompanies it.
Tracy and Robins (2007a) suggest that for self-esteem, the opposing emotions
of shame and pride are likely to be crucial. Low self-esteem could be conceptualised
by a tendency to experience affective states of shame, and a lack of experience of
affective states of pride. This link is supported by findings that 83% of variance on the
Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) could be accounted for by self-
reported ratings of pride and shame (Brown & Marshall, 2001). Tracy and Robins
(2007) go on to explain the functional aspect of shame in the context of social
relationships by way of Sociometer theory (Leary & Baumeister, 2000). This theory
proposes that low self-esteem serves as an evolutionarily “warning” state which
motivates an individual to act to increase their social standing, and thus increase the
likelihood of reproductive success. Shame may be how we experience this warning
affectively.
92SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Gilbert and Procter (2006a) highlight that shame relates not only to how we
feel about ourselves, but how we believe others view us, and believing that you exist
as fundamentally unacceptable in the minds of others is a painful belief to hold. For
an adolescent coming to terms with a chronic condition that marks them out as
different from their peers, and limits what they can do, there may be a vulnerability to
experiencing shame. This is supported by evidence that shame is more commonly
experienced in adolescence compared to later adult life (Orth, Robins, & Soto, 2010).
In addition, research has shown that in a group of adolescents, higher scores on a
measure of “personal fable”, that is, seeing your experiences as unique and separate
from others, was associated with decreased self-compassion (Neff & McGehee,
2010). Self-compassion has been routinely cited as the adaptive opposite to shame,
employing a differing and competing biological system, the enhancement of which,
through compassion focussed therapy, has shown some effectiveness in reducing
shame (Gilbert & Procter, 2006a).
Given the role of perceived perceptions of others inherent in experiences of
shame, it follows that some adolescents with diabetes may try to protect against
feelings of shame by avoiding condition self-management, in an effort to appear
“normal” and acceptable to others.
Terry and Leary (2011) argue that self-compassion mediates self-regulation of
health behaviours at every stage from being able to set more realistic goals, to an
increased ability to recover from a lapse. The second point is demonstrated in a study
by Adams and Leary (2007) who found that dieters who had received a self-
compassionate message prior to eating a high calorie snack not only reported less
subsequent distress but were able to compensate by eating less than those who did not
93SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
receive this message. This type of dietary decision is likely one that arises frequently
for those with diabetes, and suggests that those low in self-compassion, and thus high
in shame, may struggle to re-engage with positive condition self-management
following a lapse. In one qualitative study that interviewed patients with
hypercholesterolemia, shame emerged as a theme in relation to the emotions
experienced when given health messages by clinicians about diet and exercise. As
diabetes is another condition that requires patients to be engaged in regular
conversations about their condition self-management, it may be that they also
experience feelings of shame in these situations.
Shame has also consistently been associated with depression (Andrews, Qian,
& Valentine, 2002; Cheung, Gilbert, & Irons, 2004). One longitudinal study
highlighted shame related schemas as being predictive of depression nine years later,
suggesting that a belief about oneself that evokes shame may pre-date depression
(Halvorsen, Wang, Eisemann, & Waterloo, 2010). Therefore, as well as shame having
a potentially direct effect on condition self-management as noted above, it may also
be that depressive affect mediates the relationship between shame and self-
management. For example, shame-related depressive affect might decrease motivation
and produce problems with memory. It might also increase risk of hyerglaecemia
through the physiological effects of stress (Diabetes.co.uk, Diabetes Digital Media ltd,
2014).
Summary
Consistent self-care is vital to maintaining good health and quality of life for
people with diabetes. The mid-teenage and early adulthood years are important for
laying down the foundations for lifelong management. Independent condition self-
94SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
management can be challenging for young people and evidence suggests depression
might impact on self-management.
Self-esteem is one area that has been explored in those with diabetes and in
predicting condition self-management, but this has yielded mixed findings. An
alternative approach to studying global self-evaluation is to examine potential specific
underlying emotions that directly and indirectly influence condition self-management.
This research aims to explore the link between shame, depressive affect and condition
self-management in adolescents with type one diabetes.
Main Hypothesis
1. Higher shame will be associated with poorer condition self-management as measured (a) by subjective reports of diet, exercise, insulin administration and blood glucose monitoring; and (b) objective Hba1c levels.
2. Depressive affect will partially mediate the relationship between shame and
subjective and objective indices of condition self-management.
Participants
The inclusion criteria are as follows:
Young people between the ages of 16 and 25 (as this represents
a period of increasing independence in condition self-management)
Have a formal diagnosis of type 1diabetes
Have the ability to provide consent
The exclusion criteria:
The presence of any other diagnosed chronic physical illnesses
Proposed recruitment routes
95SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Clinical Psychologists working in local Diabetes teams have been contacted.
Through liaison with the charity Diabetes UK, who offer assistance in
recruitment for diabetes related research.
Measures/ Interviews/ Stimuli/ Apparatus
Validated measures will be used as follows:
Experience of Shame Scale (ESS) (Andrews et al., 2002)
The ESS is a 28-item questionnaire that measures shame across three domains:
body shame, charactorological shame and behavioural shame. Good construct validity
and internal reliability have been shown for both the total scale and the three
subscales with an undergraduate sample (Andrews et al., 2002).There is insufficient
prior research to make specific hypotheses as to which of these subscales is likely to
be most strongly related with condition self-management. Therefore, the subscale
scores will each be utilised to address the hypotheses.
Depression Inventory of the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS)
(Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995)
The DASS is a 42-item, self- report scale, split into 3 subscales which measure
depression, anxiety and stress. Each of these three areas are conceptualised as existing
on a continuum, rather than with certain cut-off points for diagnosis and a separate
score is given for each of the subscales. Factor analysis has confirmed the existence of
the 3 factor structure, and good convergent validity has been shown between the
depression subscale and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961). In
96SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
the present study, only the depression scale will be used. The DASS depression scale
has the benefit of focussing on the affective and cognitive elements of depression,
thus reducing the likelihood that somatic symptoms commonly associated with
diabetes, such as poor sleep and fatigue, would affect the results (Lovibond &
Lovibond, 1995).
Self-Care Inventory- Revised (SCI-R) (Weinger, Butler, Welch, & La
Greca, 2005)
This 15-item measure asks participants to rate their condition self-
management on a series of 5-point likert scales, varying from 1- “never do it”, to 5-
“always do it as recommended, without fail”. Items measure diet, exercise, glucose
monitoring, insulin administration, low glucose levels, and preventative aspects of
self-care. This measure has been adapted from the original SCI which was aimed at
adolescents and their parents. The updated, adult version was chosen as the age range
under investigation (16-25) more closely represents a young adult population as
opposed to an adolescent one. As such, parental reports are not included in the SCI-R.
The SCI-R shows high internal consistency (α =0.87) and concurrent and convergent
validity with existing measures in the expected directions (Weinger et al., 2005).
Glycolated Haemoglobin levels (Hba1c)
This is taken routinely by blood test at diabetes check-up appointments.
HbA1c gives a numerical value to glucose control over the preceding two or three
months, typically. Good control is generally considered to be at 6.5%, and levels for
non-diabetics range from 4-5.9 HbA1c can provide an additional means of measuring
self-care behaviours, as those who are adhering to their recommended diabetes self-
97SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
care are likely to have better control, and for this reason it is widely used in research.
However, it also may influenced by other factors, such as appropriateness of
medication and additional illness, so it will be used in conjunction with self-report
(Diabetes Digital Media ltd, 2014).
Participants are likely to have a copy of their latest reading. For those
participants who do not know their reading, this could serve as evidence of low
condition self-management, in as far as they are not aware of their current level of
control.
A series of demographic questions will be asked so that potential confounds
may be highlighted and further explored. These will be as follows:
Current Age
Age diagnosed with diabetes
Gender
Parental attendance at clinics (as a marker of parental
involvement in treatment)
Estimation of ownership of control (measured on a visual
analogue scale):
I manage my diabetes independently-------------------My parent(s)/carer
manages my diabetes
Presence and/or history of diagnosed mental health problems
Procedure
98SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
The study will be advertised to potential participants via psychologists in local
teams, and through Diabetes UK. Information and consent forms will be developed.
The inclusion of an incentive in the form of a prize draw will be considered to aid
participation. An online battery of the above questionnaires will be developed to
allow ease of completion for participants.
Once the data has been collected, analysis will be conducted, as discussed
below.
Contingency planning
It is a possibility that some patients may not know their HbA1c levels. If the
majority of participants are not aware of their HbA1c levels, absence of knowing this
could be coded dichotomously (know it/don’t know it) and be used as an additional
indicator of self-management using logistic regression analysis. This will still be
interpreted alongside the self-report measure outcome variable.
Project Costing
All measures are freely available from the authors. Permission will be sought
to use the measures in a secure, password protected online format.
£200 – prize draw to encourage participation
Ethical considerations
99SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
The study will invite participants to think about how they feel about
themselves and their condition management. This has the potential to be upsetting.
This will be made clear in the information sheet so that potential participants can
make an informed decision about whether they wish to do this, and if not, can of
course decline to participate at this stage.
The study also has the potential to alert participants to existing depressive
symptoms and/or experiences of shame. Details will be given in the debrief sheet
about mental health provision, a crisis telephone line number and the route to
accessing psychological therapy (e.g. by self-referral to their local IAPT or via GP).
This will be worded in a way that normalises depression/shame in diabetes and
highlights the benefits of seeking additional support.
All participant data will be assigned a number, and given that names/contact
details will not be taken anonymity is assured. All study information will be stored
securely and retained for 10 years as per University regulations.
R&D considerations
R & D departments of the Trusts in which recruitment is taking place will be
contacted and formal approval sought prior to commencing the study. This will be
done in parallel with NRES application.
Proposed Data analysis
100SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
A mediation analysis will be used. The first model will contain total shame
score as the predictor, HbA1c as the outcome, and depression score as the mediator.
As HbA1c represents a dichotomous outcome, logistic regression will be used. A
mediation analysis will be repeated with each of the ESS shame subscales (body,
character and behaviour) as the predictor. All models will then be repeated with the
SCI-R as the outcome, and multiple regression used to represent the continuous nature
of this outcome variable. The model below demonstrates this:
Shame total and subscales (ESS) HbA1c/ SCI-R
Depressive affect
(DASS depression subscale)
Between 90 and 196 participants should be sufficient to detect a medium
effect size in a mediation analysis (alpha = .05) at 80% power (Fritz & Mackinnon,
2007).
Dissemination strategy
The aim will be to publish in a peer-reviewed journal such as “Health
Psychology”, or a diabetes specific medical journal.
Involving interested parties
The most relevant group to consult is young adults living with type 1 Diabetes.
The charity Diabetes UK can provide access to contact details of some of their
members for the purposes of research. The charity has been contacted for advice on
contacting a small group of advisors to provide qualitative feedback on this proposal.
101SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
References
Adams, C. E., & Leary, M. R. (2007). Promoting self-compassionate attitudes
toward eating among restrictive and guilty eaters. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 26(10), 1120-1144.
Andrews, B., Qian, M., & Valentine, J. D. (2002). Predicting depressive
symptoms with a new measure of shame: The experience of shame scale. The British
Journal of Clinical Psychology / the British Psychological Society, 41(Pt 1), 29-42.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An
inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001). Self-esteem and emotion: Some
thoughts about feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(5), 575-584.
Bryden, K. S., Peveler, R. C., Stein, A., Neil, A., Mayou, R. A., & Dunger, D.
B. (2001). Clinical and psychological course of diabetes from adolescence to young
adulthood: A longitudinal cohort study. Diabetes Care, 24(9), 1536-1540.
Cheung, M., Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2004). An exploration of shame, social
rank and rumination in relation to depression. Personality and Individual Differences,
36(5), 1143-1153.
Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. . San Francisco:
Freeman.
102SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
de Wit, M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A., Bokma, J. A., Haasnoot, K.,
Houdijk, M. C., Gemke, R. J., & Snoek, F. J. (2008). Monitoring and discussing
health-related quality of life in adolescents with type 1 diabetes improve psychosocial
well-being: A randomized controlled trial. Diabetes Care, 31(8), 1521-1526.
doi:10.2337/dc08-0394
Diabetes.co.uk, Diabetes Digital Media ltd. (2014a). Diabetes and stress.
Retrieved from http://www.diabetes.co.uk/diabetes-destress.html
Diabetes.co.uk, Diabetes Digital Media ltd. (2014b). Guide to HbA1c.
Retrieved from http://www.diabetes.co.uk/what-is-hba1c.html
Fritz, M. S., & Mackinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the
mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18(3), 233-239. doi:PSCI1882 [pii]
Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with
high shame and self‐criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach.
Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13(6), 353-379.
Gonzalez, J. S., Peyrot, M., McCarl, L. A., Collins, E. M., Serpa, L., Mimiaga,
M. J., & Safren, S. A. (2008). Depression and diabetes treatment nonadherence: A
meta-analysis. Diabetes Care, 31(12), 2398-2403. doi:10.2337/dc08-1341 [doi]
Grabill, K. M., Geffken, G. R., Duke, D., Lewin, A., Williams, L., Storch, E.,
& Silverstein, J. (2010). Family functioning and adherence in youth with type 1
diabetes: A latent growth model of glycemic control. Children's Health Care, 39(4),
279-295. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
103SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=psyh&AN=2010-22641-003&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Halvorsen, M., Wang, C. E., Eisemann, M., & Waterloo, K. (2010).
Dysfunctional attitudes and early maladaptive schemas as predictors of depression: A
9-year follow-up study. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(4), 368-379.
Jacobson, A. M., Hauser, S. T., Powers, S., & Noam, G. (1984). The
influences of chronic illness and ego development on self-esteem in diabetic and
psychiatric adolescent patients. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 13(6), 489-507.
doi:10.1007/BF02088595
Johnson, S. B., Freund, A., Silverstein, J., Hansen, C. A., & Malone, J. (1990).
Adherence-health status relationships in childhood diabetes. Health Psychology, 9(5),
606-631. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.9.5.606
Kellerman, J., Zeltzer, L., Ellenberg, L., Dash, J., & Rigler, D. (1980).
Psychological effects of illness in adolescence. I. anxiety, self-esteem, and perception
of control. The Journal of Pediatrics, 97(1), 126-131. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=6966685&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Kovacs, M., Goldston, D., Obrosky, D. S., & Iyengar, S. (1992). Prevalence
and predictors of pervasive noncompliance with medical treatment among youths with
insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(6), 1112-1119.
104SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-
esteem: Sociometer theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
psychology (32nd ed., pp. 1-62). San Diago, CA: Academic Press.
Leary, M. R., & MacDonald, G. (2005). Individual differences in self-esteem:
A review and theoretical integration. In M. R. Leary, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.),
Handbook of self and identity (pp. 401-418). New York: The Guildford Press.
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative
emotional states: Comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the
beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3),
335-343.
Neff, K. D., & McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and psychological
resilience among adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity, 9(3), 225-240.
Odegard, P. S., & Capoccia, K. (2007). Medication taking and diabetes: A
systematic review of the literature. The Diabetes Educator, 33(6), 1014-29; discussion
1030-1. doi:33/6/1014 [pii]
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Soto, C. J. (2010). Tracking the trajectory of
shame, guilt, and pride across the life span. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 99(6), 1061-1071. doi:10.1037/a0021342
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, New
Jersey: Princeton University Press.
105SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Seigel, W. M., Golden, N. H., Gough, J. W., Lashley, M. S., & Sacker, I. M.
(1990). Depression, self-esteem, and life events in adolescents with chronic diseases.
Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 11(6), 501-504. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=c8h&AN=1991125059&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Sullivan, B. J. (1978). Self-esteem and depression in adolescent diabetic girls.
Diabetes Care, 1(1), 18-22. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=554782&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Terry, M. L., & Leary, M. R. (2011). Self-compassion, self-regulation, and
health. Self and Identity, 10(3), 352-362.
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Self-conscious emotions: Where self
and emotion meet. In C. Sedikides & S. Spence (Ed.), The self in social psychology .
New York: Psychology Press.
Weinger, K., Butler, H. A., Welch, G. W., & La Greca, A. M. (2005).
Measuring diabetes self-care: A psychometric analysis of the self-care inventory-
revised with adults. Diabetes Care, 28(6), 1346-1352. doi:28/6/1346 [pii]
Zashikhina, A., & Hagglof, B. (2013). Self-esteem in adolescents with chronic
physical illness vs. controls in northern russia. International Journal of Adolescent
Medicine and Health, , 1-7.
106SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix A- Gant Chart to show proposed timeline of MRP
Jul-14
Aug-14
Sep-14
Oct-14
Nov-14
Dec-14
Jan-15
Feb-15
Mar-15
Apr-15
May-15
Jun-15
Jul-15
Aug-15
MRP Proposal
Ethics
R&DCollect dataData analysis
Intro
Method
Results
Discussion
Draft to Svr
1SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
PSYCHD CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY
Major Research Project Literature Review
Self-esteem in adolescents with diabetes:
A review
Word count: 7977
2SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Abstract
Objective: The increased incidence of depression in people with diabetes is
well documented, though less is known about the psychological mechanisms
mediating this relationship. The current review considers the existing research
regarding the nature of self-esteem in people with diabetes, during the stage at which
comparisons of oneself to the peer group become heightened: adolescence. Method:
Following a search of several data-bases, 14 studies met the inclusion criteria of the
review which consisted of a mixture of designs including cross-sectional and
randomized controlled trials. A total of 1985 adolescents participated in the studies,
ranging in age from 12 to 19. Results: The findings of the review suggest no clear
evidence for adolescents with diabetes having lower global self-esteem than their
peers. Preliminary evidence suggests that those with more severe illness or longer
illness duration may be at greater risk of developing low self-esteem. Associations
were also found between a lower self-esteem and poorer diabetes related adjustment
to a number of social domains. Furthermore, three out of the four targeted
interventions considered showed a significant improvement in self-esteem pre to post.
Conclusions: Overall the findings are encouraging and suggest that diabetes does not
globally have a negative effect on an adolescent’s self-esteem. There are also
preliminary findings as to factors that may increase the likelihood of a low self-
esteem developing. However the usefulness and conceptualisation of global self-
esteem as a meaningful measure in this client group is considered, along with
recommendations for future research.
3SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Statement of Journal Choice
This review article is intended for consideration for publication in the APA
journal “Health Psychology”. This journal publishes research, including reviews, that
examines the combination of biological, behavioural and psychosocial factors in
illness. This review, whilst mainly coming from a psychosocial perspective, also
touches on the interaction with biological factors.
Health Psychology also lists “child and adolescent health” and “lifespan
approaches” under a list of example topic areas. This complements the current review
as it looks at the developmental stage of adolescence and highlights the factors
specific to this stage that warrant developmentally specific research.
4SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Introduction
This year, Diabetes UK (2014) estimated that there are around 35,000 children
and young people under the age of 19 in the UK currently diagnosed with diabetes.
Although Type II diabetes is on the rise within young people, 96% of people with
diabetes under the age of 19 have a diagnosis of Type I (Diabetes UK, 2014).
Type 1 Diabetes is a chronic illness that usually requires daily management
through monitoring and adjustment of insulin levels by injection or pump, as well as
careful dietary planning. Complications arising from poorly managed diabetes range
from immediate hyper or hypoglycaemia to longer term conditions such as
retinopathy and heart disease (NHS Choices, 2012a; NHS Choices, 2012b)
Given the seriousness of the disease, it is perhaps not surprising that a person
with the diagnosis of diabetes is approximately twice as likely to develop depression
as someone without diabetes (Mommersteeg, Herr, Pouwer, Holt, & Loerbroks,
2013). This co-morbidity presents additional risks and challenges as depression co-
morbidly occurring with diabetes, in adulthood, has been consistently associated with
both poorer glycaemic control (Lustman et al., 2000) and poorer treatment adherence
(Gonzalez et al., 2008).
One explanation for this increased prevalence of depression is that the
physical symptoms of low or high blood sugar in people with diabetes, such as
tiredness and irritability (NHS, 2012) link directly to experiences of low mood and
over a prolonged period, depression. Another possibility is that depression is over-
reported in people with diabetes because of the inclusion of somatic symptoms in self-
5SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
report measures such as the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Ward, Mendelson,
Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) or the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (Kroenke & Spitzer,
2002).
However a recent study found that although people with diabetes scored more
highly on depression measures than healthy controls, there was no difference in the
pattern of cognitive and somatic symptoms across the two groups (Stewart, Simmons,
& White, 2011). Furthermore, Seigel, Golden, Gough, Lashley and Sacker (1990)
found a larger difference in reported psychological symptoms than somatic symptoms
in a diabetic group compared to a healthy comparison group. This lends support to the
existence of a psychosocial mechanism mediating the onset of depression in diabetics.
One potential such mechanism, which has shown significant correlations with
depression in a general population, is low self-esteem (Watson, Suls, & Haig, 2002).
Rosenberg (1965) described self-esteem in its broadest sense as simply “the direction
of self-attitude” (p. 15). He acknowledged that self-attitude can be measured across a
number of domains of a person’s life, but self-esteem is the global direction of that
persons feeling toward the self. Leary and Macdonald (2005) described self-esteem as
an “affectively laden self-evaluation” (p.401) and distinguish it from related concepts
such as self-concept and self-efficacy on the basis that one can have a sense of high
self-esteem whilst still being aware of their flaws . As Rosenberg (1965) described,
high self-esteem is not about regarding yourself as good, rather as “good enough”.
Sociometer Theory (Leary & Baumeister, 2000) describes self-esteem
developing as part of an evolutionarily adaptive process. By holding an internal
gauge of our relational worth to others, if we notice that it is low, we are then
motivated to increase our social standing. This provides the evolutionary advantage of
6SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
subsequent group protection and reproductive opportunities. Leary and Macdonald
(2005) therefore describe the sense of current self-esteem that any one individual has,
as “the extent to which an individual has been accepted or rejected by people over
time” (p. 404).
Rosenberg (1965) describes adolescence as a key period in the development of
self-esteem as the mind’s eye is turned inward to evaluate the self in order to make
decisions about suitability of, for example, future occupations or romantic partners.
Rosenberg also noted that it is a time of “status ambiguity” (p. 4) where an
individual’s identity of being a child no longer fits, but they do not yet have the
independence that comes with adulthood.
Given this increased self-focus during adolescence, the question arises of the
effect of having a chronic disease such as diabetes on an adolescent’s self-esteem. In
addition, research has shown that in a group of adolescents, higher scores on a
measure of “personal fable”, that is, seeing your experiences as unique and separate
from others, was associated with decreased self-compassion (Neff & McGehee,
2010). Although this is a slightly different concept, it would fit that if an adolescent
sees themselves as fundamentally different from their peers and this affects their
ability to show self-compassion, there may well be a concurrent decrease in self-
esteem.
There has been much research into depression in diabetes, but there is limited
research that looks specifically at self-esteem in this population, with much of it
failing to clearly define self-esteem as a separate construct to either “self-concept” or
“self-evaluation”.
7SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
The main aim of this review was to further explore the existing literature on
the nature of self-esteem in adolescents with diabetes. Given that the transition from
primary to secondary education represents a period of increased autonomy and a shift
towards spending increased amounts of time with the peer group as opposed to the
family, the current review was focussed on research with the lower age range of
participants starting at the beginning of secondary education (11) and spanning to
roughly the end of adolescence (19). 11 is also the usual age of transition to middle
school in the US, where fifty percent of the studies were conducted. The initial aim
was broad, but following evaluation of the existing literature, it was possible to refine
the questions to the following:
1. Is a chronic disease such as diabetes associated with a lowered
self-esteem in adolescents?
2. Does diabetes differ, with regard to self-esteem, from other
chronic diseases experienced in this age range?
3. What interventions have successfully addressed self-esteem in
this population?
4. What other factors are likely to mediate the likelihood of an
adolescent with diabetes developing low self-esteem?
Method
Search terms were arrived at firstly by defining self-esteem as the construct of
interest. Although the review aimed to avoid inclusion of studies measuring other
“self” terms that differ from self-esteem, “self-confidence” was added to capture
further papers as it closely overlaps with Rosenberg’s (1965) definition of self-
8SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
esteem. However, as discussed below, the criteria of including a validated self-esteem
measure was still required, and in reality “self-confidence” did not return many
additional papers. Only studies looking at self-esteem in relation to diabetes were
considered so the search term “diabet*” was also added to capture studies mentioning
different phrases for the disease such as: diabetes, diabetic, diabetes mellitus or type 1
diabetes.
Finally the “NOT” criterion were used to eliminate what was initially an
expansive search result, largely consisting of unrelated studies looking at the medical
management of diabetes and associated medical issues. The final search terms “self-
esteem OR “self-confidence” AND diabet* NOT insulin, medical, “medical
intervention”, “eating disorder*”, gene*” were used to arrive at the first set of studies.
The data-bases Psyc-Info, Medline, Cinhal and Web of Science were all searched
between 12/02/14 and 14/02/14.
On all but one database, the age criteria of adolescence (13-17) was added and
for Web of Science, which did not have this option, the following additional terms
were used instead: adolescen* OR teen* OR "Young adult*" OR "Young person*"
OR youth. Although the inclusion criteria for the review was slightly broader than
ages 13-17, this search would still have found studies where the age range included
but also went beyond this bracket. Diabet* was tagged as a subject term on all
databases other than Web of Science (which does not have this option) to limit the
search to studies which focussed on diabetes as a main subject.
This first search yielded a total of 135 potentially eligible studies from the
data-bases searched (See Figure 1). These studies were screened at title and abstract
level and studies that were obviously unrelated to diabetes and self-esteem in
9SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
adolescence were excluded. All studies that were not from a peer-reviewed journal or
were not written in English were also excluded at this stage, along with duplicates.
It was subsequently decided that relevant studies looking at people with
diabetes and eating disorders should not be excluded. Although the studies in this area
that the search initially returned were unrelated to self-esteem, excluding the term
completely may result in missed relevant articles, particularly as self-esteem is a well-
recognised factor in eating disorders (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003). Therefore,
an additional search was carried out with the same phrasing as before, removing the
NOT eating disorder* criteria and adding an AND criteria of; “eating disorder* OR
anorexia OR bulimia. 19 further articles were retrieved in total from the same
databases as originally searched, and although one met the inclusion criteria in an
initial title and abstract screen, it was later removed for not meeting the criteria at full
text screen.
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
38 studies were left after the initial screening for full text consideration.
Studies were included that reported primary data of the level of and/or contributing
factors towards the self-esteem of adolescents with diabetes. There is some debate in
the self-esteem literature about the overlap between the varying conceptualisations of
self-regard (Leary & MacDonald, 2005) and this issue will be discussed further in the
Discussion section of this review. To prevent dilution of results by potentially
including a number of overlapping but slightly differing concepts, the criteria was set
such that the studies must include a validated measure stating that it specifically
measures global self-esteem. As a result, a number of studies were excluded that
10SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
claimed to be measuring self-esteem but instead used “self-concept” or “self-
perception” measures, as were qualitative studies.
A subset of studies also included a broad age range of children and
adolescents, for example “under 18” or “8-15 years”. In order to avoid variation in
developmental stage confounding results, studies including participants outside of the
11-19 age bracket were excluded.
Lastly, studies were excluded that selected participants on the basis of any
other factor which could systematically affect self-esteem, for example; short stature.
11SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Figure 1: Flowchart summarising the searching process
Potentially eligible records identified through first database search: 135
Medline: 30Psyc Info: 15Web of Science: 73Cinhal: 17
Exclusion of duplicate records:
38
Potentially eligible records: 116
Excluded: following title/ abstract screen: 81
Full text articles assessed for eligibility: 38
Exclusion of articles after full text screening: total n= 26
Reasons:Inclusion participants outside
of age range 11-19 (n=12)Does not use a validated self-esteem measure or subscale
(n=8)No primary data (n=3)
Looked specifically at Diabetes and short stature (n=1)Population did not have
diabetes (only at risk of) (n=1)Obesity as an additional factor
(n=1)
Additional potentially
relevant articles that were
highlighted as relevant from the text of full screen
articles: 3
Articles included in the review: 14
Relevant articles found from search of reference
lists of all final papers and google scholar “citing articles” searches: 2
Additional search of all above databases including eating disorders: 19
12SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Quality Criteria
The Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for evaluating primary research
papers (Kmet, Lee, & Cook, 2004) were used as a reference point for broad
comparisons between papers. These criteria were developed for use when making
comparisons across a range of quantitative designs and include consideration of
sample selection, study design and control of potential confounds (Kmet et al, 2004).
Results
An overview of the 14 included studies is presented in Table 1. The dates
published range from 1978 to 2013, with 86% published more than five years ago.
The studies contain a total of 1985 participants, which has been adjusted to allow for
the four studies which use overlapping samples (de Wit et al., 2008; de Wit et al.,
2010; Sullivan, 1979b; Sullivan, 1978). The overall reported age ranges were between
11 and 19, though the exact age range was not reported for two papers, with one
giving average ages of 14.9 (diabetic) and 15.8 (oncology) and stating that all
participants were adolescents (Török, Kökönyei, Károlyi, Ittzés, & Tomcsányi, 2006)
and another stating that diabetic participants may have occasionally exceeded high-
school age, due to the scope of the clinic they were recruited from (Kellerman,
Zeltzer, Ellenberg, Dash, & Rigler, 1980). Additionally, given the longitudinal nature
of Bryden et al (2001), the second measurement was taken when the participants were
in the 20-28 age range. However this study still provides relevant information as to
the potential longer term course of self-esteem, with a baseline measured in
adolescence.
13SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Fifty percent of the studies were conducted in the United States. The
remainder were conducted in various European countries, all only appearing once,
with the exception of de Wit (2008) and de Wit (2010) who report findings from the
same sample in the Netherlands.
In terms of design, seven studies employed a cross-sectional design in which
the self-esteem of a diabetic group was compared with a sample of either adolescents
who were healthy, or adolescents with other chronic diseases. One study used a cross-
sectional design, but just looked at factors within a sample of adolescents with
diabetes, without reference to a comparison group. However, as the same sample was
used in another study by the same author, with a comparison group, some
comparisons can be made (Sullivan, 1979b; Sullivan, 1978). Five studies reported the
outcomes of interventions designed to improve psychological functioning in
adolescents with diabetes. Due to the scope of the review, only changes in self-
esteem and other factors related to this are reported. Of these five studies, three made
comparisons with a control group, of which two involved random allocation to
groups. The remaining two reported the pre and post measures of the treatment group.
The remaining study employed a longitudinal design only measuring self-esteem at
time one, but then using this as part of a model to assess whether factors such as
Hba1c levels1, and mental health status could be predicted from baseline scores.
Half of the studies measured self-esteem using the Rosenberg self-esteem
scale (RSES) (Rosenberg, 1965), with one additionally using the “I think I am”
(ITIA) questionnaire (Ouvinen-Birgerstam, 1985). A further four used the
Coopersmith Self-esteem Inventory (CSEI) (Coopersmith, 1967) and another used
1 Hba1c refers to the level of glucose in the blood during the preceding 2-3 months, as tested by a blood sample. It is routinely used as an indication of longer term diabetes control (Diabetes UK, 2016a).
14SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
form b of the CSEI, a shorter version of the same scale (Coopersmith, 1967). The
remaining two papers used the self-esteem subscale of the Children’s Health
Questionnaire-87 (Landgraf, Abetz, & Ware, 1996). As a result of the range of
European countries included in the studies, three used translated versions of the above
measures.
It is of note that out of the papers that used the RSES, only four used the
original scoring criteria as set out by Rosenberg (1965) with Guttman scale scores out
of ten and a higher number indicating lower self- esteem (Cavusoglu, 2001; Seigel et
al., 1990; Sullivan, 1979b; Sullivan, 1978). The remaining papers that used the RSES
used an additive scoring system, with higher scores indicating higher self-esteem.
This creates an added level of complication when it comes to comparing across
studies, and brings into question the validity of the studies diverging from Rosenberg
(1965)’s original scoring system. However Sinclair et al (2010) recently analysed
properties of the RSES using the additive scoring system and found high internal
consistency (Cronbach’s alpha coefficient: 0.91) as well as favourable item
convergent validity and item discriminant validity. Rosenberg (1965) also found high
reproducibility (93%) and correlations in the expected directions with existing
measures of, for example, depression and answers to questions such as “what do
others think of you?” It may be that both scoring systems are valid, and the second
has at least been more rigorously tested with a broader sample.
Table 1. Overview of included studies
15SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
ReferenceTotal N Country
Range of age
Design Measure used
Bryden et al. (2001) 76 UK 11 – 18* L CSEI
Cavusoglu (2001) 55 Turkey 13-18 CS RSES (Turkish)Colquhoun et al. (1998) 14 Ireland 14-16
PPCG CSEI form b
de wit et al. (2008) 41 Netherlands 13-17 RCT CHQ-87 S
de wit et al. (2010) 81 Netherlands 13-19 RCT CHQ-87 S
Hauser et al. 1979 216 USA 11 to 19 CS CSEI
Herskowitz (1990) 6 USA 15-19 PP CSEIJacobson et al. (1984) 390 USA 12-16 CS CSEIKellerman et al. (1980) 517 USA 11 - 18 CS RSES
Seigel et al. (1990) 180 USA 12 to 18 CS RSES
Sullivan (1978) 205 USA 12 -16 CS RSES
Sullivan (1979) 105 USA 12-16 CS RSES
Torok et al. (2006) 97 Hungary no data PP RSES (Hungarian)Zashikina et al (2013) 148
Northern Russia 13 - 16 CS
RSES and ITIA (Russian)
Note: RSES: Rosenberg self-esteem scale (Rosenberg, 1965);CSEI: Coopersmith self-esteem inventory (Coopersmith, 1967); CSEI form b: Shortened version of CSEI (Coopersmith, 1967); ITIA: I Think I AM (Ouvinen-Birgerstam, 1985); CHQ- 87 S: Child Health Questionnaire-87subscale (Landgraf et al., 1996); CS: Cross-sectional; PPCG; Pre-post control group design; PP: Pre-post, no control group design; RCT: Randomised control trial; L: Longitudinal; *age 20-28 at follow up.
16SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Self-esteem of diabetic adolescents compared to healthy controls
Table 2 shows the six studies that compared the self-esteem of adolescents
with diabetes to that of healthy adolescents. The findings were mixed. Effect sizes
were computable from four out of the seven reported results and ranged from a
moderate effect in the direction of adolescents with diabetes having higher self-
esteem (-0.456) to a small effect in the direction of adolescents with diabetes having
lower self-esteem (0.251). Only one study reported the self-esteem of the diabetic
group to be significantly lower than that of the healthy controls. Three studies
reported no significant difference between the average self-esteem scores of the two
groups, whilst another study reported significantly higher self-esteem in the diabetic
group on two separate measures. A final study reported a “slightly higher” average
self-esteem of the diabetic group but as no significance tests were reported, this was
assumed to be non-significant (Hauser et al., 1979a).
Demographic and disease related data for the studies which used comparison
groups are shown in Table 3. Of the studies which reported no significant difference
in self-esteem of adolescents with diabetes compared to that of healthy adolescents,
there were some additional between-group differences, for example a larger
proportion of participants of high socio economic status in the healthy group
(Jacobson, Hauser, Powers, & Noam, 1984; Sullivan, 1978). Kellerman (1980), also
reported that the diabetes group, along with two other disease groups, had
significantly less white participants than the healthy group. Although repeated
analyses excluding black participants and found no significant change to the results,
this did not account for the Hispanic participants. It may be that, given the age of the
study “black” referred to all ethnic minorities, but it is unclear if this is the case. It
17SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
would be purely speculative to comment on whether group differences resulted in
non-significant findings, but nonetheless an additional difference between comparison
groups introduces the possibility of confounds, whether that represents a potential
difference in self-esteem across ethnic groups, or different conceptualisations of what
self-esteem is.
One notable detail in Jacobson et al (1984), who also found no significant
difference between groups, is that the diabetic group were “Insulin Independent” and
those that were recruited from hospital were admitted for intensive education and
treatment adjustment and were “not acutely ill”. This contrasts with Seigel et al
(1990)’s diabetic sample (in which the diabetic group had lower self-esteem than the
healthy group) whom had been selected on the basis of having been hospitalized at
least twice in the previous year, and thus experiencing increased severity. It may be
that the diabetes of the adolescents in Jacobson et al (1984)’s study was sufficiently
mild to cause minimal impact on their life, and their self-esteem. This suggests that
disease severity may be one factor which influences the likelihood of an illness group
reporting a lower self-esteem than a healthy group.
There were large disparities in terms of sample size; Sullivan (1978) had the
most balanced group numbers with 105 adolescents in the diabetic group, and 100 in
the healthy group. Jacobson et al (1984) was the only study to have considerably more
diabetic participants than healthy participants and the remaining four all had a much
larger number of healthy participants than diabetics. Given that studies with a range of
results fell into the latter category, it doesn’t appear as if this systematically affected
the results in one particular direction.
18SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
The majority of studies gave no particular comment on, overall, whether the
groups showed high or low self-esteem, and no standardised cut off points were set by
Rosenberg (1965). However one can assume that the healthy controls provide a local
norm, and if the disease groups do not differ significantly from these (as the majority
did not) they could be considered to have average self-esteem. This does assume that
the healthy group was a representative sample, however, and in many studies these
population came from a single school, so this may not be the case. The one study that
found a lower average self-esteem in the adolescents with diabetes (Seigel et al.,
1990) did report that across all disease groups 79% of participants were classified as
having low self- esteem, compared to only 19% of healthy controls.
None of the studies reviewed specifically recruited participants who suffered
with depression, and thus no comparisons of those with clinically significant
depression and those without were made. Instead, self-esteem means were averaged
across groups and these Means formed the basis of the comparison. The implications
of this will be discussed further in the Discussion section of this review.
19SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Table 2: Mean self-esteem scores of adolescents with diabetes compared to that of healthy adolescents
ReferenceDiabetes alone or pooled with other
diseases*Healthy controls Mean S-E (SD)
Diabetic groups mean S-E (SD)
Measure Result Effect size
Hauser et al (1979) Diabetes Alone. Not reported Not reported CSEID group slightly higher***
N/A
Jacobson et al (1984) Diabetes Alone. 70.9 69.5 CSEI No sig difference N/A
Kellerman et al (1980) Diabetes Alone 30.2 (4.3) 30.4 (5.2) RSES No sig difference d= -0.042
Seigel et al (1990) Pooled Not reported Not reported RSES D group sig lowera N/A**
Sullivan (1978) Diabetes Alone 2.12 (2.07)† 2.63 (1.99)† RSES No sig difference d= + 0.251
Zashikina et al (2013) Diabetes Alone 30.28 (4.10) 32.04 (4.31) RSES D group sig higherb d= - 0.418
Zashikina et al (2013) Diabetes Alone 54.15 (32.03) 69.04 (33.29) ITIA D group sig higherb d= -0.456
Note: D= diabetes group; S-E= self-esteem; RSES: Rosenberg self-esteem scale (Rosenberg, 1965);CSEI: Coopersmith self-esteem
inventory (Coopersmith, 1967); CSEI form b: Shortened version of CSEI (Coopersmith, 1967); ITIA: I Think I AM (Ouvinen-Birgerstam,
1985)a p<0.001; b p<0.01; *only results of pooled analyses included where additionally no sig difference was shown between diabetes and
other disease groups;** The relevant information to compute effect sizes was not reported;***No significance statistics reported; † higher
score indicates lower self-esteem; + d score indicates healthy controls have higher self-esteem than diabetic group and – d score indicates
20SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Study Group NSampl
e%
maleMean age
% white SES Severity
Seigal (1990)
Healthy 100 LC
No data- states groups did not differ significantly in
gender, age or race
Mainly low
N/A
Asthma 40 OCMainly low
Recently severe*
Sickle Cell Disease 20 OC
Mainly low
Recently severe*
Diabetes 20 OCMainly low
Recently severe*
Jacobson (1984)
Healthy 62 LS 42 14 no data
68% High SES
N/A
Diabetes 271I***&
SC 49.8 14 no data
24% High SES
IIDM*
Hauser (1979)
Healthy53 +n
no data 35.8 13-14 no data
mainly middle class N/A
Diabetes 163 SC/C 50.3 13 no data
mainly middle class
no data
Sullivan (1978)
Healthy 100 LS 0 13.8 no datamiddle class
N/A
Diabetes 105 SC 0 13.7 no data rangeno data
Table 3: Demographic and health variables across studies utilising cross-
sectional design
21SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Zashikina et al (2013)
Healthy 301 LS 44 14.64 no data
measured but not
reported for
individual groups
N/A
Asthma 50 OC 54 14.29 no data 50% severe
Epilepsy 48 OC 40 14.97 no data 27.1% severe
Diabetes 50 OC 56 14.15 no data 42% severe
Kellerman et al (1980)
Healthy 301 LS 46.8 15 79 no data N/A
Oncology 30 C 60 15.3 73.3 no data severe:26.7
%
Cardiology 30 C 50 16.3 36.7 no data Severe:3.6%
Cystic Fibrosis
23 C 65.2 15.9 91.3 no data Severe:17.4
%
Nephrology 25 C 30.4 30.4 34.8 no data Severe: 12%
Rheumatology
30 C 40 40 63.3 no data Severe:
6.7%
Diabetes 30 C 56.7 15.4 43.3 no data Severe:33.3
%
Cavusoglu (2001)
Oncology (Leukemia)
22 OC 66.7 14.2 no datano
overall SES data
75.8% treatment
Diabetes 22 OC 59 14.7 no datano
overall SES data
100% check-ups
Torok et al (2006)
Oncology 52 SC 42.3 15.9 no data no data no data
Diabetes 45 SC 46.7 14.8 no data no data no data
* at least 2 hospitalizations in the last year; ** Insulin Independent Diabetes Mellitus; *** admitted for intensive education and alterations in treatment rather than acute illness; LS- local school; OC- outpatient clinic; C- clinic (does not specify in or outpatient); SC- specialist diabetes camp; I- inpatient for management of diabetes; +n- norms data.
22SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Self-esteem in adolescents with diabetes, compared to other disease groups
Six studies compared the self-esteem of adolescents with diabetes to that of
adolescents with other chronic diseases (Table 4). Nine chronic diseases were
represented. A non-psychotic adolescent psychiatric inpatient population was also
included as a comparison group in one study (Jacobson et al., 1984). This finding is
not reported in the review as this group is not comparable on the basis of physical
illness.
No significant difference was found in the self-esteem levels of adolescents
with diabetes compared to adolescents with cancer. Effect sizes ranged from a
negligible difference in the direction of lower self-esteem in those with diabetes
(d=0.21) to moderately, but not significantly in the direction of lower self-esteem in
the oncology group (0.525).
The latter study (Cavusoglu, 2001) reported that the mean diabetes group self-
esteem score was “high” and the cancer group mean “moderate” though a small
sample size of only 22 in each group may have prevented the difference reaching
significance. In this study, there were additional differences in terms of severity as
only 18.2% of the participants with cancer were able to attend school, compared to
72.7% in the diabetic group. Additionally, all of the participants with diabetes were
23SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
attending the clinic for routine medical check-ups, whereas 75% of those with cancer
were actively receiving treatment at the clinic. It may be that the severity of the illness
is the more important factor than the diagnosis, as in contrast, the only effect size in
the direction of the oncology group having higher self-esteem was in Kellerman et al.
(1980)’s study (effect size +0.21 ns) in which the illness of 63.3% of the oncology
group were indicated as “mild” in comparison to only 29.6% of the diabetic group.
The average self-esteem in the epilepsy group was shown to be significantly
lower than the diabetic group on two measures (RSES and ITIA) used with the same
population (Zashikhina & Hagglof, 2013). Interestingly, the effect sizes shown by the
two measures differed substantially with the RSES showing a moderate effect size of
d= -0.630 and the ITIA showing a much stronger d= -0.924. The ITIA also appeared
to be more sensitive when comparing asthma to diabetes. Participants with asthma
scored significantly lower on it whereas no significant difference was found with the
RSES. The ITIA data was included on the basis that Zashikhina and Hagglof (2013)
describe it as measuring self-esteem but Norwicka et al. (2009) describes it as being
derived from a mixture of questions from the RSES and Piers-Harris Self-Concept
scale (Piers & Harris, 1967). Unfortunately it is not possible to examine the questions
as an English version was unavailable, but it could be that this scale measures a
cognitive appraisal of abilities, as opposed to global liking of oneself, which in turn
may show more discrepancies between the disease group based on the specific
physical limitations of the disease.
None of the other reported chronic disease group comparisons yielded any
significant mean differences in self-esteem.
24SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Table 4: Self-esteem in other chronic disease groups, as compared to those with diabetes.
Reference Comparison groupComparison group Mean and
(SD)Diabetic groups mean
SEMeasur
e Result Effect size
Seigel (1990) Sickle cell 75% low† 80% low† RSES No sig difference N/A
Asthma 80% low† 80% low† RSES No sig difference N/A
Zashikina et al (2013)
asthma 31.36 (4.16) 32.04 (4.31) RSES No sig difference d= -0.160
epilepsy 29.71 (2.95) 32.04 (4.31) RSES D group sig higher * d= -0.630
asthma 56.13 (26.46) 69.04 (33.29) ITIA D sig higher* d= -0.429
epilepsy 42.6 (23) 69.04 (33.29) ITIA D sig higher ** d= -0.924
Kellerman et al (1980)
Oncology 31.4 (4.3) 30.4 (5.2) RSES No sig difference d= + 0.21
Cardiology 29.9 (3.9 30.4 (5.2) RSES No sig difference d= -0.109
Cystic Fibrosis 30.9 (3.8) 30.4 (5.2) RSES No sig difference d= + 0.110
Nephrology 32.4 (2.8) 30.4 (5.2) RSES No sig difference d= +0.479
Rheumatology 30.4 (5.1) 30.4 (5.2) RSES No sig difference d= 0
Cavusoglu (2001) Oncology (Leukemia) 2.82 (0.92) 2.27 (1.16) RSES No sig difference d= - 0.525
Torok et al (2006) Oncology 27.19 (3.65) 28.61 (4.26) RSES No sig difference d= - 0.358
25SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Note:*p<0.05; **p<0.001:, +comparison group had higher self-esteem;- diabetic group had higher self-esteem; †mean not given, reported as percentages of group who fell into low self-esteem category.
26SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
The effect of targeted self-esteem interventions
Table 5 shows the results of four studies that assessed the effect of a
psychosocial intervention on self-esteem levels. None of the interventions were
designed specifically to improve self-esteem; rather self-esteem was measured as part
of a battery of psychosocial measures to evaluate existing specialist camping
programmes. Alternatively self-esteem improvement was cited as a broad aim of
therapeutic discussion based interventions amongst addressing other “psychosocial
issues”.
Three of the four studies showed a significant improvement in self-esteem
either at post-intervention or follow-up. Two of these employed fully randomized
controlled designs and showed significantly higher self-esteem at either post-test or
follow up in the treatment group, taking into account baseline scores. Both of these
interventions were discussion based; one consisted of four weekly groups, in which
video footage of an adolescent talking about their diabetes preceded an open
discussion about the groups coping (Colquhoun, Drury, Cregan, & Keenan, 1988).
The other involved three extended clinic appointments over the course of a year, in
which, the adolescents completed “health related quality of life” questionnaire would
serve as an opening into a discussion of any psychosocial areas they felt their diabetes
was impacting upon (de Wit et al., 2008; de Wit et al., 2010).
The remaining two studies described outdoors based “camps” which both
aimed to foster a sense of achievement through engagement in novel outdoors
activities. Torok et al. (2006) did show a significant mean increase in self-esteem for
participants at the end of the camping programme, but without a control group it
cannot be assumed that their self-esteem would not have naturally increased at this
27SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
time. Given that the camp only lasted eight days, this is unlikely, but cannot be ruled
out.
The remaining camp-based study which showed no improvement in self-
esteem failed to re-administer the self-esteem measure until 7-11 months later, in
which time any number of confounding events could have occurred to influence self-
esteem (Herskowitz, 1990).
De Wit et al. (2008) found that, during the first year their study, improvement
in self-esteem showed no association with an improvement in A1C levels (an
indicator of glycaemic control, and thus control of diabetes). A main aim of
psychosocial diabetes interventions is to indirectly improve diabetic self-care, but in
this case it appears that this intervention did not achieve this aim. There was a return
to baseline self-esteem scores one year after the intervention sessions (deWit et al.
2010). This suggests that perhaps an intervention with this level of low intensity is not
enough to cause lasting change to self-esteem.
28SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Table 5 The effect of Interventions on self-esteem level
Study N IPre-test Mean
(SD)Post-test Mean
(SD) Result. Follow-up ResultSelf-esteem Measure used
TG CG TG CGCG- pre-post
TG pre-post
post T and C
TG Mean(SD)
CG Mean (SD)
TG pre-follow-up
TG post-follow-up
Colquhoun (1988) 14 GD CSEI-
form b
measured and included in analysis
but not reported18.71† 18.29† N/A N/A
Borderline sig
difference*
20.57 (SD: 4.07)
not reported N/A
self-esteem
sig higher*
*
Herskowitz (1990) 6 OB CSEI. 83 (4) 75 (3) N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
7-11 months: 84
(5)76 (3) not
sig N/A
de wit et al (2008 & 2010)
81 Q &D
CHQ-87
subscale
74.84 (10.43)
75.97 (10.63
)
79.94 (10.44)
75.97 (8.71)
non-sig
TG self-esteem sig
improved***
TG self-esteem sig higher **
73.72 (12.77) N/A N/A
Self-esteem
sig lower*
Torok 97 SC RSES 28.61 (4.26) NC 30.20
(4.50) NC NCPost-test
Sig higher ***
NC 2 months: 29.20 (4.52)
no control N/A N/A
Note: N: Total number of participants in treatment group (and control if applicable), I: Intervention; GD: Group Discussion; OB: Outward bound course; Q & D: questionnaire and discussion; TG: treatment group; CG: control group; NC: No Control group;* P=0.06**p<0.05; ***p<0.01; † adjusted mean taking into account pre -treatment scores.
29SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Other factors linked to self-esteem
Table 6 summarises other variables reported to be associated with self-esteem.
Only variables which were reported in three or more papers are depicted in the tables,
the remainder will be discussed in the text.
The most common reported factor was gender, which was reported in four
papers. The findings were mixed, with two studies reporting that females with
diabetes had significantly lower self-esteem than males, and the remaining two
finding no significant difference. However of these latter two studies, one pooled the
results with oncology patients (Török et al., 2006) and the other reported that twice as
many males had “high self-esteem” than females (Cavusoglu, 2001) but perhaps due
to low numbers, no significant difference was found. Interestingly, although
Kellermen et al. (1980) found lower self-esteem in females, they found that a group x
sex interaction effect was non- significant, suggesting this pattern is no different to
that of healthy adolescents.
Only one study (Zashikhina & Hagglof, 2013) found a significant association
between increased age and lower self-esteem. The exact opposite result was found in
the same study within the epileptic group, suggesting a role for disease-specific
variables on the course of self-esteem. Two other studies failed to find any significant
association with age, however, implying that conclusions regarding age are
premature. Furthermore, Sullivan (1979b) looked at age of onset (i.e. in childhood
through to adolescence) in relation to self-esteem, but no significant association was
found.
30SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Perhaps a more useful way to conceptualise the age effect observed is in terms
of illness duration. Two studies found an association between longer illness duration
and lower self-esteem, though effect size was reported for only one of these and was
small (Hauser et al., 1979b). It is of note that the latter study reported a positive
coefficient, but described the association in the discussion section as a negative one.
This review has therefore tentatively interpreted the association as negative, and the
coefficient has been altered to represent this in Table 6. Zashikhina and Hagglof
(2013) indicated that they included disease duration in their general linear modelling,
but as results are not reported, it may be assumed that they were non-significant.
Another factor that may mediate the relationship between age and self-esteem
is ego-development level, which can differ between adolescents of the same age.
Hauser et al (1979a) found that adolescents with diabetes who were also in the pre-
conformist stage had significantly lower self-esteem (p < 0.01) than the other two ego
development groups measured (conformist and post-conformist). These findings are
supported by Jacobson et al. (1984) who found that in a multi-way ANOVA, ego
development level emerged as having a significant main effect on self-esteem
(p=0.0006) in which self-esteem increased linearly from pre-conformist, to
conformist and post-conformist. This was despite finding no effect for age.
An interesting finding reported by Kellerman et al. (1980) was that those with
higher self-esteem were more likely to report a lower level of anxiety and an internal
locus of control. This was reported globally across disease and healthy groups, not
specifically for those with diabetes, but does suggest a potentially important
mediating factor.
31SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Severity was only measured explicitly in relation to self-esteem in two studies,
revealing conflicting findings. Zashikhina and Hagglof (2013) found that severity was
the most significant predictor of self-esteem in the diabetic group. In contrast,
Kellerman et al. (1980) found no significant association. However taken with the
finding that the only study that found adolescents with diabetes had significantly
lower self-esteem than healthy adolescents was the one which specifically selected
those with increased severity (Seigel et al., 1990), severity does appear to have a role.
Sullivan (1979b) looked at self-esteem in relation to psychological and social
adjustment to diabetes across a number of domains during the development of the
Diabetes Adjustment Scale (DAS). The DAS is a 37-item questionnaire which
assesses adjustment to diabetes across six different life domains, for example family
and school. This generates scores relating to each of these domains, as well as an
over-all adjustment score.
When adjustment scores were spit into “good” and “poor” categories, not
only did those with “poor” over-all adjustment have significantly lower self-esteem
than those with “good” adjustment (p<0.001), but this pattern extended across the
subcategories of adjustment in peer relationships (p<0.001) negative attitudes towards
diabetes and dependence-independence issues (p<0.01).
Strong correlations were also found between depression and self-esteem
scores. However, when self-esteem and DAS scores were included in an ANOVA
alongside depression scores, as measured by the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et
al., 1961) there was neither a significant effect of self-esteem on adjustment (F=3.3,
p=ns) nor an interaction effect between self-esteem and depression (F=0.43, ns).
32SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
However within this ANOVA there was still a significant main effect for depression
on adjustment (F=8.01, p<0.01).
Another finding of Sullivan (1979b) was that participants who reported an
“excellent” relationship with their mother had significantly higher self-esteem
(p<0.01) than those who reported a “good, fair or poor” relationship. The same
pattern was even stronger for reported relationships with fathers (p<0.001).
Sullivan’s (1979b) findings show a promising insight into the factors that may
influence and interact with self-esteem in adolescents with diabetes. However
causality cannot be assumed from a cross-sectional study, and therefore conclusions
cannot be drawn as to whether poor diabetic adjustment lead to lower self-esteem, or
an existing lower self-esteem contributed to poor adjustment following diagnosis.
Additionally, in the absence of an equivalent general social adjustment questionnaire
administered to matched healthy adolescents, it cannot be assumed that this pattern is
unique to those with diabetes. Poor diabetic adjustment in the context of, for example,
peer relationships, could be just one element of over-all poor adjustment to peer
relationships for that individual.
Finally, Bryden et al. (2001) reported that self-esteem was linked to
"concurrent psychological adjustment" seemingly measured by a global score of
depression anxiety and aggression. Self-esteem was not one of the psychological
variables that predicted later Hba1c levels, but unfortunately self-esteem was not
measured at time 2, so a longitudinal picture of self-esteem course was not possible.
33SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Table 6. Other factors associated with self-esteem
Reference Gender Age Illness duration
P Direction P Direction r or p Direction
Bryden et al. (2001)
p<0.03
Females sig lower than males
Cavusoglu (2001)
p=0.36 No significant difference*
de wit et al. (2010) =0.90 0.029 Lower self-esteem predicted by
longer illness duration†
Hauser et al. (1979)
r = -0.15a
p<0.05
Self-esteem negatively associated with duration
Jacobson et al.
(1984)NR No significant effect NR NR No significant effect
Kellerman et al.
(1980)NR Females sig lower than
males*** NR No significant effect
Torok et al. (2006) NR No significant difference**
Zashikina et al.
(2013)
p<0.01
Older age predicted lower self esteem
NR= Not Reported; *Twice as many males had high self-esteem;**grouped with oncology patients;***group x sex interaction effects non-significant;† at follow up only; a: reported as r=0.15 but described and interpreted in the paper as a negative association. It has thus been adjusted in the table to represent this.
34SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Discussion
This review aimed to explore what is known about the status of self-esteem in
adolescents with diabetes. Fourteen studies were reviewed which reported findings on
a range of aspects of self-esteem which fell into four main categories;
1. Comparisons of global self-esteem of adolescents with diabetes and:
a) Healthy controlsb) Adolescents with other chronic diseases
2. The effect of targeted psychosocial interventions on self-esteem in adolescents
with diabetes
3. Other factors that are associated with self-esteem level in adolescents with
diabetes
The studies reviewed did not provide consistent evidence for the argument that
adolescents with diabetes suffer with lower self-esteem than their healthy peers. In
fact the majority of studies showed no significant difference between the two groups,
with available effect sizes spanning both directions.
Sociomoter theory describes self-esteem as an indicator of how much value
the individual perceives they hold for other people. It may be that asking whether
individuals with diabetes have lower self-esteem than their healthy peers is too
simplistic. If viewed on a continuum, rather than categorically, it may be that the
more severe, and thus more debilitating a disease is for an adolescent, the more likely
they are to perceive themselves as having limited value to others. Findings relating to
severity were limited and mixed, with one study finding no significant association
between severity and self-esteem (Kellerman et al., 1980) and another finding that
severity was the most significant predictor of self-esteem amongst the diabetic group
35SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
(Zashikhina & Hagglof, 2013). Further support is found for this idea, however, as the
only study to find a significantly lower self-esteem in the adolescents with diabetes, in
comparison to healthy adolescents, specifically selected participants who had been
hospitalized at least twice during the previous two years, and thus represented a group
who had current, or at least recent experience of being severely ill (Seigel et al.,
1990).
The lack of difference in self-esteem between adolescents with diabetes and
adolescents with other chronic diseases is also surprising. In particular, no difference
was found between those with cancer and those with diabetes. Again, severity within,
not across diseases may account for this as, the only effect size in the direction of the
oncology group having higher self-esteem was in Kellerman et al. (1980) (effect size
+0.21 ns) in which the illness of 63.3% of the oncology group were indicated as
“mild” in comparison to only 29.6% of the diabetic group.
It is a positive finding that despite the additional adversity that being
diagnosed with diabetes in adolescence inevitably brings, there is no evidence that
diabetes alone universally reduces an adolescent’s self-esteem to a lower level than
that of their peers. Cognitive Adaptation Theory (Taylor, 1983) proposes that
individuals going through life-threatening events experience an initial drop in self-
esteem, but are then motivated towards self-enhancement. Taylor (1983) found that,
among cancer patients, one way of doing this is through downward social
comparisons; i.e. (“Well at least I’m better off than X”). This could be one
explanation of why, in some studies, the mean self-esteem of adolescents with
diabetes was in fact higher.
36SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
It would be premature to conclude that these results show that diabetes does
not affect, or interact with adolescent’s self-esteem. It may be that in measuring the
global self-esteem of a large group of adolescents, participants with especially low or
high self-esteem in effect cancel each other out. This would certainly be supported by
the larger standard deviations reported for the diabetic groups in all but one study,
especially in comparison to other disease groups. This is also supported by Sullivan’s
(1979b) findings that suggest that those who report poor adjustment to diabetes across
a number of social contexts are more likely to experience lower global self-esteem.
This result was found within a participant population who were found to have no
significant differences in their global self-esteem in comparison to a group of healthy
adolescents (Sullivan, 1978), indicating that when measured globally, these subtle
patterns were lost.
There were mixed results for the association between gender and self-esteem,
but on the whole there was more evidence weighted on the side of females with
diabetes having generally lower self-esteem than their male counterparts. However
one study found this pattern was similar in healthy adolescents (Kellerman et al.,
1980).
There was some evidence that self-esteem deteriorates with age in adolescents
with diabetes, with the opposite trend found in an epileptic population. Perhaps a
better way to conceptualise this is in terms of duration of illness, for which there was
mixed, but slightly stronger evidence for. This goes against Taylor’s (1983) theory of
an initial drop in self- esteem followed by a subsequent rise, as it points towards a
possible gradual deterioration in self-esteem following diagnosis. This pattern lends
support to Fennel’s (1997) conceptualisation of low self-esteem as being interwoven
37SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
with negative core beliefs. It is thought that if an individual has a negative core belief
about their self-worth, any situation that is directly relevant to self-worth will be
interpreted through the lens of their core belief (Beck, 1991). This is then maintained
through a mixture of avoidance and cognitive biases, thus creating a self-perpetuating
downward spiral (Fennell, 1997). It would then logically follow that if the diagnosis
of diabetes represents a congruent event that confirms an existing negative core belief
(such as “I’m abnormal”) adolescents who had had diabetes for longer would be
further into the spiral that maintains a low self-esteem. This would fit with
longitudinal evidence that in one sample, HbA1c levels were at their highest in late
adolescence, indicating poorer diabetic control at this age (Bryden et al., 2001).
From the perspective of Sociometer theory, Jacobson’s (1984) finding about
locus of control might suggest a mediating factor between adolescents who use the
cue to low-self-esteem as a motivator to increase it, and those for whom it becomes a
self-perpetuating cycle. An internal locus of control may provide the impetus needed
to increase perceived value, whereas an external locus of control, perhaps concurrent
with core beliefs about other people holding more power than oneself, may see their
self-esteem as purely in the hands of others and the world, and therefore are unable to
independently seek out ways to improve it.
The findings for Ego Development would also support this. Jacobson et al.
(1984) highlight that later ego development stages represent a move to relying on
internal conceptualisations of the self, as opposed to external. This is likely to foster
an increased self-esteem as the individual can navigate situations based on their own
set of consistent standards, as opposed to potential varying ones from others. Ego
development stages do not correlate exactly with age, but even so, it would be
38SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
expected, based on this idea, that self-esteem would increase in later adolescence,
which contradicts with the previous discussed findings.
Sullivan (1979b) found that adolescents with diabetes that reported a better
relationship with their parents were more likely to have high self-esteem. This
supports Fennel’s (1997) conceptualisation of self-esteem, which sees the messages a
child receives from their parents as a key factor in the development of core beliefs.
However, in the absence of a comparison with healthy controls, there is nothing to
suggest that this pattern is unique to adolescents with diabetes.
The results for interventions were largely positive and showed a significant or
almost significant increase in self-esteem in three out of the four interventions
reported. It is not possible to draw firm conclusions as to the most effective style of
intervention, as only four were reviewed, and methodology varied between the
studies, but it is of note that both discussion based interventions were associated with
significant improvement. Five week follow-up of the group discussion intervention
was promising, with a further increase in self-esteem recorded compared to post test
(Colquhoun et al., 1988). Unfortunately de Wit (2010) showed a return to baseline
levels of self-esteem at one year after the end of an individual discussion intervention.
It is possible that the group format was more effective than the individual
intervention, and the fact that self-esteem scores were continuing on an upwards
trajectory at five week follow up would support this. The group intervention may have
had the added benefit of peer normalisation, which in the context of self-esteem being
socially relevant, particularly in adolescence (Rosenberg, 1965) may account for
improved gains. However, as the data is not available, it cannot be ruled out that the
39SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
participant’s self-esteem in Colquhourn et al.’s (1988) study did not return to baseline
one year later, as did de Wit et al.’s (2010).
The pattern of an initial rise, followed by a fall in self-esteem, observed in de
Wit (2010) implies either no change to self-esteem at the core belief level, or an initial
bolstering of an alternative self-schema, followed by the more negative schema
returning. The CHQ-87 questionnaire used in de Wit (2010) does report a global
measure of self-esteem but the questions themselves ask specifically about how “good
or bad” they have felt across a number of domains such as social, academic and
health. This doesn’t seem to tap into a general global sense of like or dislike of the
self in the way that RSES does, and may be confounded by actual achievement and
current mood state. It may be that following the individual discussion sessions, mood
and perceived self-competence were increased, but without continued support
fostering the opportunity to test out an alternative self-schema, whether explicit or
not, this did not result in a change at the core belief level.
A measure such as the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Beck & Weissman,
1978) may tap into self-esteem at the core belief level more effectively and provide a
more accurate picture of self-esteem and self-esteem change than measures such as
the CHQ-87 and CSEI which appear to overlap with self-competence.
The discussion groups appear to be more effective than camp based programs.
It may be that through discussion of diabetes related insecurities, cognitive biases are
naturally challenged, especially in the context of a group of adolescents with the same
condition. Camp based programs, may provide active opportunities for “evidence
against” negative automatic thoughts. However according Fennell’s (1997) model, in
40SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
people with low self-esteem, cognitive biases may prevent disconfirmation of
negative beliefs if they are not explicitly discussed.
Limitations
As highlighted earlier, self-esteem is not a universally defined concept. The
RSES most accurately captures Rosenberg’s (1965) conceptualisation of self- esteem
being a global measure of self-liking. However other measures such as the Piers
Harris Self-Concept Scale also claim to measure self-esteem, but do so in a way that
blurs overall liking and acceptance of the self with a cognitive evaluation of
competence across domains. As such, only measures stating they were specifically
measuring global self-esteem were included. This has inevitably limited the breadth of
papers included and potentially missed relevant findings that captured some element
of self-esteem. There was also not a clear cut-off between two distinct categories of
measures and included measures such as the CSEI and CHQ subscale did touch on
aspects of self-concept, but were included based on their stated remit and inclusion of
a global self-esteem scale.
The review also focussed explicitly on adolescents and papers including the
results of children under 11 or adults over 19 were excluded. This may have resulted
in a longitudinal picture of the course of self-esteem from childhood to adolescence
being missed, however as most studies did not separate into age categories, it was
thought that broad age ranges may mask significant findings.
41SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Implications for practice and research
The results of this review suggest that low self-esteem may not be a universal
problem amongst adolescents with diabetes, however this must be tempered by the
limited evidence base and varying concepts of self-esteem. This still indicates a
hopeful position for recently diagnosed adolescents; diabetes does not necessarily
have a negative impact on self-esteem. The finding that brief discussion based
interventions could also provide at least a temporary raise in self-esteem is also
encouraging, and may reflect the kinds of conversations that many health
professionals are already having with their clients.
Although the reviewed studies compared self-esteem of adolescents with
diabetes to self-esteem of adolescents with a variety of other chronic conditions, and
those with none, none specifically looked at the population for whom low self-esteem
is most significant; those with diagnosed depression. Strong associations have been
found between depression and self-esteem in general populations (Watson, 2002) and
self-esteem has been shown to predict negative affect (Lightsey, 2006). Additionally,
Sullivan (1979b) found strong correlations between adolescent girls with diabetes
scores on the BDI and their scores on the RSES. However these girls were not chosen
on the basis of a diagnosis of depression, and many may have simply represented non-
clinical fluctuations in mood. By measuring the self-esteem, and other demographic
and psychological variables, of a group of adolescents with diabetes and concurrent
depression, along with a group of adolescents with diabetes and no depression, a more
consistent pattern of predictor variables may become apparent.
The same would useful in terms of severity, perhaps measured by Hba1c
levels. There was some evidence that pointed to an association of lower self-esteem
42SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
with increased severity of illness, but this effect would be better tested by specifically
selecting participants who represented a broad range of severities and subsequent
impact on their quality of life.
Kellerman et al.’s (1980) finding that a stronger internal locus of control was
related to lower anxiety and higher self-esteem provides a potential focus for
intervention. Diabetes requires close monitoring and adjustment of glucose levels, and
it may be that those who perceive they have a greater level of control over their
diabetes experience less anxiety and a subsequent more positive self-esteem,
compared to those who have either poor control, or perceive their efforts at control as
futile. A potential focus for intervention would be on supporting an adolescent to
improve their internal locus of control, perhaps by offering more flexible treatment
options and encouraging them to take ownership of the management of their diabetes.
However other research has shown, in a population of children and adolescents (aged
10-17), a higher external locus of control is associated with healthier hba1c levels
(Evans & Hughes, 1987) and healthier hba1c levels are in turn associated with a rigid,
more controlling family structure. This suggests that navigating the age at which an
adolescent takes control of their insulin management involves a delicate balance
between potentially enhancing self-esteem and independence, and risking a
destabilisation of hba1c levels. Skinner suggest negotiating this transition with the
adolescent in an individual basis, and as Kellermen et al.’s (1980) findings were from
a group of adolescents with mean ages between 15 and 16, and the mean age in Evans
and Hughes, (1987) study was 13, there is tentative evidence that mid-adolescence is
a good time for this, in terms of self-esteem enhancement. Further research is
necessary to confirm this hypothesis.
43SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
It also may be that self-esteem is too broad and multifaceted a concept to
capture useful information on. Conceptualising self-esteem as a core belief, and thus
using a measure such as the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale would allow a more
specific, individualised picture of the adolescent’s views about themselves, and thus a
clearer focus for subsequent Psychological treatment, and a clearer measure of
change.
It may also be that self-esteem in its current conceptualisation is not a
significant mediating factor between the diagnosis of diabetes and the onset of
depression. Despite strong correlations between self-esteem and depression, Sullivan
(1979b) still found a stronger link directly between depression and diabetes
adjustment. Furthermore, global self-esteem may be too blunt a measure to capture
meaningful differences. Drawing on Becks cognitive model, an existing negative core
belief about the self may become activated in the context of a diagnosis of diabetes if
the adolescent views it as a personal failing or defect. This may become particularly
exacerbated in patients with complex diabetes that involves a high level of adjustment
of lifestyle and diet. In this context, measuring “shame-proneness” may capture the
likelihood of that adolescent going on to conceptualise their diabetes as something
shameful, representing a personal failing, thus translating to a negative view of the
self.
44SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
References
Beck, A. T. (1991). Cognitive therapy: A 30-year retrospective.
American Psychologist, 46(4), 368.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J.
(1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Bryden, K. S., Peveler, R. C., Stein, A., Neil, A., Mayou, R. A., &
Dunger, D. B. (2001). Clinical and psychological course of diabetes from
adolescence to young adulthood: A longitudinal cohort study. Diabetes Care,
24(9), 1536-1540.
Cavusoglu, H. (2001). Self-esteem in adolescence: A comparison of
adolescents with diabetes mellitus and leukemia. Pediatric Nursing, 27(4),
355-361. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=12025273&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Colquhoun, E., Drury, M. I., Cregan, D., & Keenan, P. (1988). Group
work with diabetic adolescents. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 5(1),
37-40. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=psyh&AN=1989-09386-001&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. . San
Francisco: Freeman.
45SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
de Wit, M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A., Bokma, J. A., Haasnoot,
K., Houdijk, M. C., Gemke, R. J., & Snoek, F. J. (2008). Monitoring and
discussing health-related quality of life in adolescents with type 1 diabetes
improve psychosocial well-being: A randomized controlled trial. Diabetes
Care, 31(8), 1521-1526. doi:10.2337/dc08-0394
de Wit, M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A., Bokma, J. A., Haasnoot,
K., Houdijk, M. C., Gemke, R. J., & Snoek, F. J. (2010). Follow-up results on
monitoring and discussing health-related quality of life in adolescent diabetes
care: Benefits do not sustain in routine practice. Pediatric Diabetes, 11(3),
175-181. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00543.x
Diabetes UK. (2012). Testing. Retrieved June, 2014, Retrieved from
http://www.diabetes.org.uk/Guide-to-diabetes/Monitoring/Testing/#glucose
Diabetes UK. (2014). Diabetes: Facts and stats. ( No. Version
3).Diabetes UK.
Evans, C. L., & Hughes, I. A. (1987). The relationship between
diabetic control and individual and family characteristics. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 31(3), 367-374. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=3625590&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour
therapy for eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment.
46SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(5), 509-528.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8
Fennell, M. J. V. (1997). Low self-esteem: A cognitive perspective.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25, 1-25.
Gonzalez, J. S., Peyrot, M., McCarl, L. A., Collins, E. M., Serpa, L.,
Mimiaga, M. J., & Safren, S. A. (2008). Depression and diabetes treatment
nonadherence: A meta-analysis. Diabetes Care, 31(12), 2398-2403.
doi:10.2337/dc08-1341 [doi]
Hauser, S. T., Pollets, D., Turner, B. L., Jacobson, A., Powers, S., &
Noam, G. (1979). Ego development and self-esteem in diabetic adolescents.
Diabetes Care, 2(6), 465-471.
Herskowitz, R. D. (1990). Outward bound, diabetes and motivation:
Experiential education in a wilderness setting. Diabetic Medicine: A Journal
of the British Diabetic Association, 7(7), 633-638. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=2146073&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Jacobson, A. M., Hauser, S. T., Powers, S., & Noam, G. (1984). The
influences of chronic illness and ego development on self-esteem in diabetic
and psychiatric adolescent patients. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 13(6),
489-507. doi:10.1007/BF02088595
47SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Kellerman, J., Zeltzer, L., Ellenberg, L., Dash, J., & Rigler, D. (1980).
Psychological effects of illness in adolescence. I. anxiety, self-esteem, and
perception of control. The Journal of Pediatrics, 97(1), 126-131. Retrieved
from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=6966685&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Kmet, L. M., Lee, R. C., & Cook, L. S. (2004). Standard quality
assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of
fields. Alberta, Canada: Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research.
Kroenke, K., & Spitzer, R. L. (2002). The PHQ-9: A new depression
diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatr Ann, 32(9), 1-7.
Landgraf, J. M., Abetz, L., & Ware, J. E. (1996). The CHQ user’s
manual. . New England Medical Center, Boston: The Health Institute.
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of
self-esteem: Sociometer theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
experimental social psychology (32nd ed., pp. 1-62). San Diago, CA:
Academic Press.
Leary, M. R., & MacDonald, G. (2005). Individual differences in self-
esteem: A review and theoretical integration. In M. R. Leary, & J. P. Tangney
(Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 401-418). New York: The Guildford
Press.
48SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Lustman, P. J., Anderson, R. J., Freedland, K. E., de Groot, M.,
Carney, R. M., & Clouse, R. E. (2000). Depression and poor glycemic control:
A meta-analytic review of the literature. Diabetes Care, 23(7), 934-942.
Mommersteeg, P. M. C., Herr, R., Pouwer, F., Holt, R. I. G., &
Loerbroks, A. (2013). The association between diabetes and an episode of
depressive symptoms in the 2002 world health survey: An analysis of 231 797
individuals from 47 countries. Diabetic Medicine, 30(6), e208-e214.
doi:10.1111/dme.12193
Neff, K. D., & McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and
psychological resilience among adolescents and young adults. Self and
Identity, 9(3), 225-240.
NHS Choices. (2012a). Diabetes, type 1- complications. Retrieved
05/27, 2014, Retrieved from
http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Diabetes-type1/Pages/Complications.aspx
NHS Choices. (2012b). Diabetes, type 1-symptoms. Retrieved 05/27,
2014, Retrieved from
http://www.nhs.uk/Conditions/Diabetes-type1/Pages/Symptoms.aspx
Nowicka, P., Höglund, P., Birgerstam, P., Lissau, I., Pietrobelli, A., &
Flodmark, C. (2009). Self‐esteem in a clinical sample of morbidly obese
children and adolescents. Acta Paediatrica, 98(1), 153-158.
Ouvinen-Birgerstam, P. (1985). I think I am: Manual [Jag tycker jag
är]. Stockholm: Sweden: Psykologiforlaget.
49SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Piers, E., & Harris, D. (1967). Piers-harris childrens self-concept scale.
Nashville: Counselor recordings and tests.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image.
Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Seigel, W. M., Golden, N. H., Gough, J. W., Lashley, M. S., & Sacker,
I. M. (1990). Depression, self-esteem, and life events in adolescents with
chronic diseases. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 11(6), 501-504.
Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=c8h&AN=1991125059&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Sinclair, S. J., Blais, M. A., Gansler, D. A., Sandberg, E., Bistis, K., &
LoCicero, A. (2010). Psychometric properties of the rosenberg self-esteem
scale: Overall and across demographic groups living within the united states.
Evaluation & the Health Professions, 33(1), 56-80.
doi:10.1177/0163278709356187
Stewart, S. M., Simmons, A., & White, P. C. (2011). Somatic items in
the assessment of depressive symptoms in pediatric patients with diabetes.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 34(2), 112-119. doi:10.1007/s10865-010-
9289-1
Sullivan, B. J. (1979b). Adjustment in diabetic adolescent girls: II.
adjustment, self-esteem, and depression in diabetic adolescent girls.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 41(2), 127-138. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
50SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=psyh&AN=1980-25981-001&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Sullivan, B. J. (1978). Self-esteem and depression in adolescent
diabetic girls. Diabetes Care, 1(1), 18-22. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?
direct=true&AuthType=ip,shib&db=cmedm&AN=554782&site=ehost-
live&custid=s4121186
Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of
cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38, 1161-1173.
Török, S., Kökönyei, G., Károlyi, L., Ittzés, A., & Tomcsányi, T.
(2006). Outcome effectiveness of therapeutic recreation camping program for
adolescents living with cancer and diabetes. Journal of Adolescent Health,
39(3), 445-447. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.018
Watson, D., Suls, J., & Haig, J. (2002). Global self-esteem in relation
to structural models of personality and affectivity. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 83(1), 185-197. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.185
Zashikhina, A., & Hagglof, B. (2013). Self-esteem in adolescents with
chronic physical illness vs. controls in northern russia. International Journal of
Adolescent Medicine and Health, , 1-7.
51SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Appendix A
Instructions for Authors- Health Psychology
ManuscriptsThe manuscript title should be accurate, fully explanatory, and no longer than 12 words. The
title should reflect the content and population studied. If the paper reports a randomized clinical trial, this should be indicated in the title. The title of brief reports should start with the words "Brief Report".
The title page should include the names of all authors and their affiliations at the time the research was done. This information will be masked to ensure a blind peer review process by the editorial office. Authors should make sure that all other identifying information in the text of the paper is masked/removed prior to submission.
All manuscripts must include a structured abstract containing a maximum of 250 words with the following sections:
Objective (brief statement of the purpose of the study); Methods (summary of the participants, design, measures, procedure); Results (primary findings); and Conclusions (specific statement of the implications of the data).
Please supply up to five keywords or brief phrases after the abstract. The Introduction should not exceed 3–4 pages in length. The paper should be referenced appropriately but excessive citations should be avoided.
All research involving human participants must describe oversight of the research process by the relevant Institutional Review Boards and should describe consent and assent procedures briefly in the Methods section.
All statistical tests should include effect size whenever possible.First person language ("I", "we") should be avoided. Terminology should be sensitive to the
individual who has a disease or disability. The journal endorses the concept of "people first, not their disability." Terminology should reflect the "person with a disability" (e.g., children with diabetes, persons with HIV infection, families of people with cancer) rather than the condition as an adjective (e.g., diabetic children, HIV patients, cancer families). Nonsexist language should be used.
It is important to highlight the significance and novel contribution of the work. The translation of research into practice must be evidenced in all manuscripts. Authors should incorporate a meaningful discussion of the clinical and/or policy implications of their work throughout the manuscript, rather than simply providing a separate section for this material.
Health Psychology publishes a broad array of types of papers. Authors of qualitative and measure
52SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
development papers should read the guidelines for these types of papers, noted below.
Reference
American Psychological Association (2014). Health Psychology: Instructions for
Authors: Manuscripts. Retrieved 08/06 2014. Retrieved from:
http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/hea/.
53SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
54SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
55SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Summary of Clinical Experience
Year 1
Adult Community Eating Disorder Service- November 2013- September 2014 (1.5
days p/w for 1st 6 months, then 2.5 days p/w).
This role involved assessment, formulation and collaborative treatment with adults
presenting with diagnosed eating disorders. The main model used was Fairburn's
Transdiagnostic CBT model for eating disorders, and I also used aspects of DBT.
As part of this placement I delivered a presentation for staff on diabetes and eating
disorders, and developed an adapted version of Fairburn's model to incorporate the additional
patterns and risks that can arise for those with diabetes
Adult Rehabilitation Ward – November 2013 – March 2014 (1 day p/w)
The placement gave me the opportunity to work as part of a ward MDT to support the
rehabilitation of adults moving out of acute mental health units. It involved taking a holistic
approach to consider preparing the clients for independent living and joint working with other
MDT professionals.
Year 2
Older Adult Community Mental Health Team and Memory Clinic – March 2015 –
October 2015
This placement involved working with individuals facing complex health problems,
dementia, and end of life issues. I completed two full dementia assessments and two extended
screenings. I jointly ran a 12 week CBT group with my Supervisor. I completed consultations
to carers (both paid and family members) and presented a teaching session on strengths and
values based approaches to working with older adults.
56SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service - September 2014 – March 2015
I worked with young people aged 6-17 on difficulties such as bullying, food phobia,
ASD, OCD and anger as well as cognitive assessments. I developed my skills in integrating
systemic factors into my formulations, and managing the often competing goals of those
surrounding the young person. Liaison and consultation with local schools was vital in some
cases to support children to continue to attend and seek support from teachers when needed.
I mostly used a Narrative therapy approach, and enjoyed the flexibility that this
approach allows in creatively incorporating a young person's strengths and interests into the
treatment. I also developed and ran a group for teenagers experiencing tics.
Year 3
Community Team for People with Learning Disabilities - October 2015- March
2016
This involved working with adults at all levels of learning disabilities providing
therapy, cognitive assessment and consultation to supported living placements for clients with
behavior that challenges. The consultation work taught me the importance of appreciating
staffs limited time, and adapting complex formulation into simple steps that they could take.
Paediatric Psychology Team – General Hospital setting- March 2016- September
2016
This placement involved a mixture of outpatient and inpatient work within the
Paediatric Psychology Team at a busy General Hospital. My work included outpatient
therapeutic work with children of a variety of ages with conditions including Chronic Fatigue,
Cerebral Palsy, Obesity and Diabetes. Much of this work included linking in with schools,
social services and other professional groups within the hospital. I also worked with several
57SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
parents whose young children were admitted to PICU- consisting of initial supportive meetings
and basic psychological first aide. I completed one general and one complex
neuropsychological assessment under the supervision of a Neuropsychologist. Lastly I
developed and co-ran a workshop for Parents of children with Enuresis.
58SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
PSYCHD CLINICAL PROGAMMETABLE OF ASSESSMENTS COMPLETED DURING TRAINING
Year I AssessmentsASSESSMENT TITLE
WAIS-IV Short report of WAIS-IV data and practice administration
Service-Related Project Adult Eating Disorders NICE Guidelines Service Evaluation Phase two: The Content and Usability of Care Plans.
Practice Case Report A Case report of the assessment process with a female presenting with Anorexia Nervosa and Obsessive Compulsive Symptoms
Problem Based Learning – Reflective Account
Problem Based Learning Reflective Account: The Relationship to Change
Major Research Project Literature Review
Self-esteem in adolescents with diabetes:A review
Adult – Case Report 1 A case report of an intervention with a young adult presenting with Anorexia Nervosa and Low Self-Esteem.
Adult – Case Report 2 A case report of a Neuropsychology assessment with a young man with Psychosis
Major Research Project Proposal
An exploration of shame in young adults with Diabetes, and its relationship with depression and treatment adherence.
Year II AssessmentsASSESSMENT TITLE
Professional Issues Essay
In relation to yourself critically explore the statement that clinical psychologists should “move away from psychiatric diagnoses…., which have significant conceptual and empirical limitations, and develop alternative approaches which recognise the centrality of the complex range of life experiences…” (Time for a Paradigm Shift, BPS, 2013).
Problem Based Learning – Reflective Account
Reflective Account 2 – The Stride Family
Child and Family – Case Report
An Integrative case report about a 14-year old girl presenting with OCD and family tensions. It draws mostly on Narrative and CBT ideas, and aims to integrate these along with other relevant theories.
59SELF-CARE IN TYPE 1 DIABETES
Personal and Professional Learning Discussion Groups – Process Account
PPLDG Process Account
Older People – Oral Presentation of Clinical Activity
Using and integrating strengths and values based models with clients and teams
Year III Assessments ASSESSMENT TITLE
Major Research Project Empirical Paper
Shame, depression and self-care in emerging adults with type 1 diabetes
Personal and Professional Learning – Final Reflective Account
On becoming a clinical psychologist: A retrospective, developmental, reflective account of the experience of training
Case report 5 - People with Learning Disabilities
An integrative exploratory case report of a woman in her 20's with a learning disability and complex trauma history.