240
How do service users’ interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and perceptions of staff coercion impact upon self - reported anger and rates of aggression in secure-care settings? Jessica Holley Submitted for the Degree of Doctor of Psychology (Clinical Psychology) School of Psychology Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences University of Surrey 1

epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

How do service users’ interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and perceptions of staff coercion impact upon self -reported anger and rates of aggression in secure-care

settings?

Jessica Holley

Submitted for the Degree of

Doctor of Psychology

(Clinical Psychology)

School of PsychologyFaculty of Health and Medical Sciences

University of SurreyGuildford, SurreyUnited KingdomSeptember 2019

1

Page 2: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Abstract of empirical paper

Background: Coercive practices – which are used as means to manage violent/aggressive

behaviour in secure forensic settings – have come under scrutiny in recent years due to their

paradoxical effects on provoking further service user aggression and violence. Previous

research has found relationships between increased service user aggression with both service

users’ interpersonal styles and perceptions of staff coercion (i.e. staff limit setting). There is,

however, a gap in the literature which looks at the way in which forensic service users

perceive such coercive practices in relation to interpersonal sensitivities and whether this too

has an impact upon service user aggression.

Aims: To investigate whether forensic service users’ levels of interpersonal sensitivity to

dominance increases levels of self-reported anger and rates of aggression towards staff

through perceptions of staff coercion.

Methods: In a cross-sectional quantitative study design, 70 service users were recruited from

one high and two medium secure forensic hospitals. Standardised measures were completed

by service users and recorded incident data was collected within the past year. Correlation

and mediation analyses were run to investigate the relationship between study variables.

Results: A significant relationship was found between service users’ interpersonal sensitivity

to dominance and self-reported rates of anger, where forensic service users’ who had higher

levels of interpersonal sensitivity to others’ dominance were likely to report higher rates of

anger. No significant relationships were found between all other study variables.

Conclusion: The findings from this study contradict previous research where coercive

practices may not necessarily increase rates of aggression towards staff but, in the context of

service users’ interpersonal sensitivities to dominance, it may be more useful to consider the

way in which coercive practices are implemented. The findings have, however, been

2

Page 3: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations have been made

for clinical practice and future research.

3

Page 4: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Acknowledgements

I wish to express my gratitude to the course team for their support during the three years of

training. A special thank you to my clinical tutor, Dr Catherine Huckle, and my associate

clinical tutor, Dr Heinz Kobler, for their invaluable advice and guidance. I would also like to

thank each of my placement supervisors who have provided me with a wealth of clinical

experience – their time and patience both in and outside of supervision was greatly

appreciated and valued.

Thank you to my supportive family, friends, and fellow trainees who, over the past few years,

have provided me with an endless stream of moral support and encouragement. A special

thank you to Matúš who listened patiently and gave me hope during times when I felt just

ever-so-slightly depleted.

Finally, I would like to thank the many service users that I have had the pleasure of working

alongside during these past three years. I have felt humbled by your willingness to share

some of your most difficult of experiences with me whilst also inspired by your resilience

and stories of hope. You are and will continue to drive me to be the best Clinical Psychologist

that I can be.

4

Page 5: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Contents

Part 1: Research – MRP Empirical Paper................................................................................................6Abstract.......................................................................................................................61.0 Introduction...........................................................................................................72.0 Main hypotheses and research questions.........................................................133.0 Method.................................................................................................................154.0 Results..................................................................................................................285.0 Discussion............................................................................................................386.0 Conclusion...........................................................................................................49References..................................................................................................................50MRP Empirical Paper Appendices.........................................................................58Part 2: Research - MRP Literature Review...........................................................95Abstract.....................................................................................................................951.0 Introduction.........................................................................................................962.0 Methods.............................................................................................................1043.0 Results................................................................................................................1094.0 Discussion..........................................................................................................135References................................................................................................................143Part 3: Clinical Experience....................................................................................152Part 4: Assessments..................................................................................................155

5

Page 6: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Part 1: Research – MRP Empirical Paper

How do service users’ interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and perceptions of staff coercion impact upon self -reported anger and rates of aggression and in

secure-care settings?

Abstract

Background: Coercive practices – which are used as means to manage

violent/aggressive behaviour in secure forensic settings – have come under scrutiny

in recent years due to their paradoxical effects on provoking further service user

aggression and violence. Previous research has found relationships between

increased service user aggression with both service users’ interpersonal styles and

perceptions of staff coercion (i.e. staff limit setting). There is, however, a gap in the

literature which looks at the way in which forensic service users perceive such

coercive practices in relation to interpersonal sensitivities and whether this too has an

impact upon service user aggression.

Aims: To investigate whether forensic service users’ levels of interpersonal

sensitivity to dominance increases levels of self-reported anger and rates of

aggression towards staff through perceptions of staff coercion.

Methods: In a cross-sectional quantitative study design, 70 service users were

recruited from one high and two medium secure forensic hospitals. Standardised

measures were completed by service users and recorded incident data was collected

within the past year. Correlation and mediation analyses were run to investigate the

relationship between study variables.

Results: A significant relationship was found between service users’ interpersonal

sensitivity to dominance and self-reported rates of anger, where forensic service

users’ who had higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity to others’ dominance were

6

Page 7: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

likely to report higher rates of anger. No significant relationships were found

between all other study variables.

Conclusion: The findings from this study contradict previous research where

coercive practices may not necessarily increase rates of aggression towards staff but,

in the context of service users’ interpersonal sensitivities to dominance, it may be

more useful to consider the way in which coercive practices are implemented. The

findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations.

Recommendations have been made for clinical practice and future research.

Word Count (excluding Tables, Figures, References and Appendices): 10000

1.0 Introduction

Forensic psychiatric inpatients are often deemed as “dangerous, violent or having

criminal propensities” (Mason, 1993, p. 413) and who have usually “interfaced with

the law at one level or another” (Mason, 2006, p. 3). In Bowers and colleague’s

review (2011) of 424 international studies, the frequency of violent incidents in

secure-care settings were significantly higher compared to general mental health

hospitals (Bowers et al., 2011). For example, in a survey of an independent secure

care facility, 2,137 violent incidents involving 56.4% of service users were recorded

(Dickens et al., 2013). The consequences of workplace violence have been known to

lead to staff absenteeism due to illness, injury and disability (Holmes, 2012). In

addition, these workplace violent incidents can also lead to high staff turnover,

decreased productivity, decreased work satisfaction, and a lack of staff commitment

to work (Holmes, 2012). Not only do these violent incidents have a negative impact

upon staff well-being, but also puts other service users at risk of physical and

7

Page 8: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

psychological harm (The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE),

2015). For those service users committing violence whilst in residing in secure

settings, their stay in secure care can often be extended (e.g. Castro et al., 2002).

Longer lengths of stay in secure care does not only result in high economic burden

on these services (e.g. Vӧllm et al., 2017), but may also impact negatively upon

forensic service users’ quality of life (e.g. Shaw et al., 2001).

Aggressive behaviour in secure care has centrally been managed by the

implementation of coercive measures (NICE, 2015). Although coercive measures are

most commonly associated with short-term management methods of aggression and

violence (through seclusion and restraint), more long-term coercive measures are

also used. Szmukler and Appelbaum (2008) outlined in their ‘sliding scale’, how

coercive measures can take on various forms in forensic secure settings including:

persuasion (e.g. efforts to influence service users’ behaviour by emotional

reasoning); interpersonal leverage (e.g. where service users’ relationship with staff is

used to put pressure on them, such as pointing out dissatisfaction in service user’s

behaviour); inducement (e.g. conditioning ‘good’ behaviour through the use of

positive rewards); threats (e.g. to lose particular benefits); compulsory treatment (e.g.

service users having choice taken away and treatment carried out against their will);

and physical security features (i.e. locked doors and barred windows).

More recently, the use of coercive practice has been scrutinised, particularly with re-

gards to the impact it has upon service users’ personal autonomy and human rights

(Hui et al., 2013). These issues of concern with regards to the use of coercive prac-

tice were raised following several widely reported investigations and scandals that

8

Page 9: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

had taken place in mental health care (e.g. Blom-Cooper, 1992; Fallon, 1999; Bubb,

2014). A number of international and national documents have, as such, called for a

review and reduction in restrictive practices in secure and general mental health care

settings (e.g. American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Nurses Asso-

ciation & National Association of Psychiatric Health Systems, 2003; Queensland

Government, 2008; MIND, 2013; Department of Health, 2014; NICE, 2015). The

need to reduce coercive practices in secure-care settings in order to support service

user’s personal recovery may, however, be more problematic than in general mental

health inpatient settings. As highlighted in a national briefing paper on “making re-

covery a reality in forensic settings”, there appeared to be a more complex balance

between the reduction of service users’ risk towards themselves and others whilst

also ensuring they have autonomy and choice over their own recovery (Drennan and

Wooldridge, 2014).

Staff having the ability to understand the way in which service users make sense of

coercive practice within an inpatient setting (i.e. how they react to the demands

placed upon them) may be critical for not only the staff-service user therapeutic rela-

tionship, but may also help to inform more effective ways of reducing and managing

aggressive behaviour (Cookson et al. 2012). As highlighted in previous empirical

evidence, staff placing restrictions on service users through, for example, directing

them to do something and/or being inflexible/rigid with rules, was one of the most

commonly reported triggers of service user aggression (e.g. Bjøkly, 1999; Daffern et

al., 2008; Daffern et al., 2003; Hornsveld et al., 2014; Meehan et al., 2006). Accord-

ing to Kiesler’s (1987) interpersonal theory, in our interactions with others (our inter-

personal behaviour), we are inherently predisposed to establish relationships that re-

9

Page 10: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

inforce our sense of self; this is done through our attempts to manoeuvre others’ re-

actions through our own behaviour towards them. All people’s interactions can be

characterised by two dimensions: affiliation (hostile to friendly) and control (domin-

ance to submission). Each person is likely to align more towards one end of these di-

mensions. For example, an individual may more likely have a friendly rather than

hostile disposition whilst be more likely to submit than dominate others; this is

known as their interpersonal style. In addition, an individual’s interactions tend to

complement the interactions of others (i.e. match across the affiliation dimension but

oppose on the control dimension). For example, hostile interpersonal styles may typ-

ically be met with hostility from others whereas submissive interpersonal styles may

typically be met with dominance from others. At times, an individual’s personal

characteristics can often lead to difficulties in their interactions with others. For ex-

ample, individuals with low self-esteem may feel frustrated by those who attempt to

dominant them and in turn, may become overly dominant and rarely submissive

(BjØrkvik et al., 2009). This may be even more problematic for those with personal-

ity disorders with more extreme interpersonal styles who are likely to become ‘stuck’

at one end of the dimensions (e.g. Blackburn, 1998).

Previous research with offenders, confirms the principles of complementarity (i.e.

individuals attempt to manoeuvre others in a way that reinforces their own position)

which reveals how, when committing a violent offence, an offender’s dominant

interpersonal style is likely to elicit victim submission (e.g. Porter and Alison, 2004).

This notion is further supported by previous research studies that have reported

correlations between interpersonal style and aggression where service users in

forensic settings who were more violent were likely to have more dominant,

10

Page 11: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

coercive, and hostile interpersonal styles (e.g. Daffern et al., 2010b; Doyle et al.,

2006; Vernham et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2013). According to Cookson and

colleagues (2012), service users with dominant and hostile interpersonal styles are

more likely to encounter problems with psychiatric inpatient treatment. More

specifically, maladaptive interpersonal functioning often reflects persistent and

problematic interpersonal styles and are often associated with aggression and

treatment non-compliance (Daffern et al., 2008).

The rules and regulations of the secure care setting (also known as coercive meas-

ures) “may challenge a service user’s dominance; they may also activate competitive

drives where service users seek to reassert control and mastery over their environ-

ment” (Daffern et al., 2008, p483). In the context of interpersonal functioning, ser-

vice users may react in an acomplementary (e.g. dominant) rather than complement-

ary (e.g. submissive) manner to assert their interpersonal dominance; this can lead to

staff typically responding to aggression by attempting to improve control and order

thus ensuring the integrity and security of the facility (Daffern et al., 2010b). It is

possible that, in turn, service users perceive staff’s attempts to restore order as threat-

ening and exploitative, which thus leads to preventative actions by service users to

restore dominance (Lillie, 2007); this is also known as the ‘aggression-coercion

cycle’ (Goren et al., 2003). Previous research has indicated how conflictful staff–ser-

vice user interactions is a factor leading to aggression on psychiatric wards (Papado-

poulos et al., 2012; Whittington & Richter, 2005). Previous studies have reported

how nurses and other ward staff who, given the time exposed to service users and the

nature of their role, were most likely to be doing the limit setting and were therefore

most likely to be the victims of service user aggression (e.g. Daffern et al., 2010b;

11

Page 12: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Meehan et al., 2006). More specifically, Winje and colleagues (2018) found that ser-

vice users in a secure care setting were more likely to be aggressive due to irritability

that is caused by staff making restrictions over their behaviour. According to Horow-

itz and colleagues (2006), the acomplementary behaviour displayed by service users

can be explained in the context of them viewing staff behaviour as irritating as it

frustrates the service users’ own interpersonal motives. For example, those who

value being in control are likely to be most frustrated by others who are bossy and

act superior (Henderson & Horowitz, 2006). This theoretical notion suggests that

people are differentially sensitive to specific forms of others’ interpersonal behaviour

due to a variance in their own interpersonal styles/motives, also known as interper-

sonal sensitivities. Therefore, it may be possible that some service users residing in

secure care settings may be less sensitive to, for example, the rules and regulations of

the hospital and/or staff limit setting. This, in turn, may result in service users being

less likely to display aggressive behaviour towards staff as there may not be a need to

assert their interpersonal dominance.

As highlighted above, previous research indicates that service users residing in

forensic settings are more likely to have dominant interpersonal styles and, in turn,

may react in an acomplementary manner (aggressively) to staff implementing the

more long-term coercive measures (i.e. persuasion, limit setting). However, there

seems to be a gap in the literature which looks at the way in which service users

make sense of such coercive practices in the context of their interpersonal sensitivit-

ies towards others, and in turn, whether they are more likely to respond aggressively

to such coercion.

12

Page 13: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

2.0 Main hypotheses and research questions

2.1 Research question

Do forensic service users’ levels of interpersonal sensitivity to dominance affect

levels of self-reported anger and rates of aggression towards staff through

perceptions of staff coercion?

2.2 Research hypotheses

Hypothesis 1

Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance (sensitivity to control) and cold dominance

(sensitivity to antagonism – which is specifically related to service users’ potential to

feel belittled/provoked by staff as noted by Meehan and colleagues, (2006)) is related

to self-reported anger and recorded rates of aggression towards staff, more

specifically:

1a) Service users who have higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

are likely to have higher levels of self-reported anger;

1b) Service users who have higher level of interpersonal sensitivity to dominance are

likely to have higher rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff;

1c) Service users who have higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity to cold

dominance are likely to have higher levels of self-reported anger;

1d) Service users who have higher level of interpersonal sensitivity to cold

dominance are likely to have higher rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards

staff.

Hypothesis 2

13

Page 14: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and cold dominance is related to perceptions

of staff coercion, more specifically:

2a) Service users who have higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

(sensitivity to control) are likely to have higher levels of perceived staff coercion;

2b) Service users who have higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity to cold

dominance (sensitivity to antagonism) are likely to have higher levels of perceived

staff coercion.

Hypothesis 3

Perceptions of staff coercion are related to self-reported anger and rates of recorded

aggression, more specifically:

3a) Service users who have higher levels of perceived staff coercion are likely to

have higher levels of self-reported anger;

3b) Service users who have higher levels of perceived staff coercion are likely to

have higher rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff.

Hypothesis 4

The relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and cold dominance

and self-reported anger and higher rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards

staff are mediated by perceptions of staff coercion, more specifically:

4a) The relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and self-reported

anger are mediated by perceptions of staff coercion;

4b) The relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and rates of

recorded aggressive incidents towards staff are mediated by perceptions of staff

coercion;

14

Page 15: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

4c) The relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and self-

reported anger are mediated by perceptions of staff coercion;

4d) The relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and rates of

recorded aggressive incidents towards staff are mediated by staff coercion.

3.0 Method

3.1 Study design

A cross-sectional quantitative study design was used to examine whether the

relationship between service users' levels of interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

and self-reported anger and rates of aggression is mediated through perceptions of

staff coercion. The data were collected through completion of standardised outcome

measures from within a representative sub-sample of service users residing in high

and medium secure-care forensic settings at a specific point in time.

Early in the development of the research proposal, it was discovered that the current

research study was investigating a related/similar research topic to another trainee’s

project on the Clinical Psychology Doctorate. As the two research studies were going

to be undertaken in the same setting and with the same participant sample, it felt

appropriate to share out the task of recruitment and data collection. As such, a joint

ethics research application was conducted whereby the participant information sheet

and consent forms were designed together (with information on both research

projects) (see Appendices B and C for a copy of the participant information sheet and

consent form). Going forward in the conduct of the research, the two researchers

shared equal responsibility of recruitment and data collection (see section 3.5 for

further details).

15

Page 16: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

3.2 Setting

Research was conducted at one high-secure and two medium secure forensic

hospitals within a London NHS Trust. The forensic hospitals were specialist services

that provided assessment, treatment, and care for people with mental illness and

personality disorders who had either been convicted of an offence or who was seen

as high risk of harm to staff or public, who had specialist rehabilitation needs, whose

behaviours were challenging, and, in the case of the high-secure service users, who

presented a high degree of risk to themselves and to others. One of the medium-

secure hospitals was a female enhanced medium secure service whereas the high-

secure and other medium-secure hospital housed male forensic service users only.

3.3 Sampling and recruitment

3.3.1 Participants

The sample consisted of male and female adults (18> years) who were forensic

service users detained under the Mental Health Act (Her Majesty’s Stationery Office

[HMSO], 1983) at a U.K. high-security or medium-security hospital.

Service users were recruited using a convenience sampling approach. This

sampling approach was deemed appropriate as it aimed to recruit service users who

were accessible, available and willing to take part in the research study. Due to the

challenging nature of recruiting service users within secure-care settings, this

sampling approach was deemed the most feasible and practical way to achieve the

target sample size.

A priori power analysis was conducted using G*Power (3.1.9.2) to determine the

estimated sample size (Faul et al., 2007). As there was limited information on the

power size calculations used in previous related research studies, a pragmatic

16

Page 17: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

approach was used. Assuming we needed a power of 0.8 to detect a medium effect

size of F2=0.15 with alpha = 0.05, a total sample size of 70 service users needed to be

obtained.

3.3.2 Recruitment procedure

An email with participant information sheets attached was sent to the lead

responsible clinician/s, lead clinical psychologist and ward manager from each ward

within the two units (see Appendix B for a copy of the participant information sheet).

A list of all service users was provided in the email with a request for permission to

approach service users to invite them to take part in the research. The lead

responsible clinician or the lead clinical psychologist provided a response detailing

who was appropriate to approach from their ward and who was deemed too unwell

and/or unstable at present to take part in the research. Of those identified (and if there

are no concerns of risk), one of the two researchers followed up the email by visiting

the ward and liaising with the ward manager or nurse in charge before approaching

each of the service users. Service users were provided with a participant information

sheet and were given a brief description of the research. As service users were given

24 hours to decide whether they wanted to take part or not (and assuming the service

user was willing to take part in principle), the researchers liaised with the service

user and the nurse in charge to arrange an appropriate time to visit the service user on

another day to take written informed consent and to complete the questionnaires (see

Appendix C for a copy of the consent form). The written informed consent procedure

ensured that service users were not only willing to participate in the study, but to

confirm that they had: 1) read and understood the participant information sheet; 2)

been given the opportunity to ask further questions about the study; 3) understood

17

Page 18: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

their participation was voluntary; 4) the right to withdraw at any time; 5) given

permission for us to have access to their clinical notes. If the service users made a

clear refusal (e.g. “No, I do not want to take part”) no further contact was made and

they were not included in the study. However, some service users declined on the

day (which may have been due to feeling unwell or being too busy) but were open to

taking part some other time in future. As such, these service users were re-

approached on another day and if they declined on this second approach then no

further contact was made.

From a total sample of 267 forensic mental health service users (182 male service

users from 14 wards in a high-secure hospital, 85 male and female service users from

4 male wards and 2 female wards in one medium-secure hospital), 222 were

approached to take part in the study. The 44 not approached (36= high-secure, 9=

medium secure) were deemed by their responsible clinician as either too mentally

unwell or too risky to take part in the research study. Out of the 222 who were

approached, 152(68.5%) declined, leaving 70(31.5%) service users in the final

sample.

3.4 Participant description

The 70 service users who took part in the study were predominantly male (94%) with

a mean age of 38.19 years (SD=11.0, range=23-66 years). Most service users were

White British (41.4%) or Black/Caribbean/African/Black British (27.1%), and a

majority had an index offence of violence including GBH/assault (50%) or homicide

(22.9%). The most prevalent International Statistical Classification of Diseases and

Related Health Problems 10th Revision (ICD-10) (World Health Organisation, 1992)

18

Page 19: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

primary diagnoses were paranoid schizophrenia or delusional disorder (44.3%),

personality disorder (antisocial and borderline: 24.3%), or a dual diagnosis of

paranoid schizophrenia/delusional disorder and personality disorder (21.4%).

Fifty-four (77.1%) of the study sample were residing in a high-secure hospital. With

regards to ward types, 40(57.1%) were based in assertive rehabilitation wards,

13(18.6%) were in admission wards, 10(14.3%) were in high dependency wards, and

7(10%) were in hybrid wards. The mean overall length of stay in the current unit was

452.44 days (SD=410.88; range= 15-1909 days, median=303 days). See Table 1 for

full details of the sample’s descriptive statistics.

The sample described in this study is comparable to the general population of service

users residing in high and medium secure units based in the UK, as reported in

previous studies (e.g. Harty et al., 2004; Vӧllm et al., 2017). Most service users

described by both Harty et al. (2004) and Vӧllm et al. (2017) were male (85%) with a

mean age of 40 and 45 years old, respectively. With regards to ethnicity, the samples

were predominantly from a White British background (70%) (Harty et al., 2004) or

were reported to have been born in the UK (91.7%) (Vӧllm et al. (2017). The most

prevalent single diagnosis of service users in both studies was schizophrenia (61%

and 57.9% respectively) followed by personality disorder (45% and 46.7%

respectively). Most service user’s index offence involved some form of violence

against another person (42% and 57.9% respectively) which included homicide and

GBH. However, it is important to note that one study was conducted over ten years

(Harty et al., 2004) and the other study focused specifically on long stay service

users (10 years or more in high secure, 5 years or more in medium secure, or 15

19

Page 20: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

years + in a combination of both high and medium secure) (Vӧllm et al., 2017) and

therefore the comparability of these samples to the current study’s sample needed to

be taken with caution.

20

Page 21: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Table 1: Sample description

High-secure unit (52) unit Medium-secure unit (18) Total (70)

Sex (%) Male 52 (100) Male 14(77.8)Female 4(22.2)

Male 66(94)Female 4(6)

Age Mean=37.3SD=10.6Range=23-65

Mean=40.13SD=12.6Range=25-66(Missing=3)

Mean= 38.19SD=11.0Range 23-66(Missing=3)

Ethnicity (%)

White British 27(51.9)Black/African/Caribbean/Black British 12(23.9)Asian/Asian British 2(3.8)Mixed/Multiple ethnic groups 6(11.5)White Other 1(1.9)Other ethnic group 1(1.9)Missing 3(5.8)

White British 2(11)Black/African/Caribbean/Black British 7(38.9)Mixed/Multiple ethnic groups 3(16.7)White Other 1(5.6)Other ethnic group 2(11.1)Missing 3(16.7)

White British 29 (41.4)Black/African/Caribbean/Black British 19(27.1)Asian/Asian British 2 (2.9)Mixed/Multiple ethnic groups 9(12.9)White Other 2(2.9)Other ethnic group 3(4.3)Missing 6(8.6)

Diagnosis (%)

Schizophrenia/Psychosis 21(40.4)Personality Disorder 17(32.7)Mixed PD and Schizophrenia 11(21.2)Mixed with LD 2(3.8)Missing 1(1.9)

Schizophrenia/Psychosis 10(55.5)Mixed PD and Schizophrenia 4(22.2)Depression 1(5.6)Missing 3(16.7)

Schizophrenia/Psychosis 31(44.3)Personality Disorder 17(24.3)Mixed PD and Schizophrenia 15(21.4)Mixed with LD 2(2.9)Depression 1(1.4)Missing 4(5.7)

Index Offence (%)

GBH/assault 25(48.1)Manslaughter 2(3.8)Homicide 14(26.9)Sexual Offence 3(5.8)Damage to Property 3(5.8)Other (robbery)/mixed 4(7.7)Missing 3(5.8)

GBH/assault 10(55.5)Homicide 2(11.1)Damage to Property 3(16.7)Missing 3(16.7)

GBH/assault 35 (50)Manslaughter 2(2.9)Homicide 16(22.9)Sexual Offence 3(4.3)Damage to Property 6(8.6)Other (robbery)/mixed 2(2.8)Missing 6(8.6)

Ward Type (%)

Admissions 9(17.3)High dependency 10(19.2)Assertive rehabilitation 26(50)Hybrid 7(13.5)

Admissions 4(22.2)Assertive rehabilitation 14(77.8)

Admissions 13(18.6)High dependency 10(14.3)Assertive rehabilitation 40(57.1)Hybrid 7(10)

Length of stay in current hospital (days)

0-6 months – 18(34.6)6months-1 year – 9(17.3)1-2 years – 7(13.5)2-3 years – 0(0)3-4 years – 8(15.4)4-5 years – 4(7.7)5-6 years – 1(1.9)Missing – 5(9.6)

0-6 months – 4(22.2)6months-1 year – 3(16.7)1-2 years – 5(27.8)2-3 years – 0(0)3-4 years – 1(5.6)4-5 years – 1(5.6)5-6 years – 0(0)Missing – 4(22.1)

0-6 months – 22 (36.1)6months-1 year – 12 (19.7)1-2 years – 12 (19.7)2-3 years – 0(0)3-4 years – 9(14.8)4-5 years – 5(8.2)5-6 years – 1(1.6)Missing – 9(12.9)

21

Page 22: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

3.5 Data collection

Before completing the questionnaires, one of the two researchers went through the

participant information sheet with the service users, providing opportunity for them

to ask any questions about the research. After written informed consent was

obtained, each participant completed six structured questionnaires, two of which

were used specifically for the current research project, three were used specifically

for the other research project, and one was used in both projects (see details of the

three measures used in this research project in the section below). Those service

users who had difficulties with literacy were offered support by the researchers with

reading and writing during the completion of the questionnaires (for example,

reading through the questionnaires with them, helping them to write their response

and/or writing their responses for them). Approximately half of the study sample had

support from the researcher with completing the questionnaire; it was, however,

unclear as to how many of these service users had literacy difficulties as opposed to

how many just wanted the researcher to read the questionnaire out to them.

Completion of questionnaires took between 30-60 minutes. Service users were paid

£5 for their participation in the research study. The money was credited into their

hospital account after they had completed the questionnaires. The two researchers

collected approximately half of the data each (i.e. researchers conducted

questionnaires with around 35 participants each).

With consent from the service users, information was sought from each participant’s

electronic-based clinical notes with regards to age, gender, ethnicity, diagnosis, index

offence, and length of stay in current unit. Permission was also sought to obtain

information from the hospital incident forms (IR1’s) on the amount of times they had

22

Page 23: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

been verbally or physically aggressive to staff in the past 12 months working back

from the date in which the participant completed the questionnaire (see outcome

measures for further details). These notes were accessed by the researcher who had

an honorary contract with the Trust for the purposes of data collection.

3.6 Measures

Service users’ interpersonal sensitivities to dominance were measured using the

Interpersonal Sensitivity Circumplex (ISC) dominant (sensitivity to the control) and

cold dominant (sensitivity to antagonism) octant scales (Hopwood et al., 2011).

Ratings of service users’ aggression (in general situations) was measured by asking

service users to complete ‘The Brief Aggression Questionnaire’ (BAQ; Webster et

al., 2014). Service users’ perceptions of staff coercion were rated using ‘The Basic

Psychological Needs Satisfaction ‘Autonomy’ sub-scale (BPNSS; Deci & Ryan

2000). Additionally, data on aggressive behaviour within the past year (this was

calculated from the point of data collection) was collected from online incident

reports and was categorised as either physical, attempted physical, or verbal.

3.6.1 Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction Scale (BPNSS) (Autonomy subscale)

– This is a 21-item self-report questionnaire, which includes 3 subscales to assess

autonomy (7 items, α = .69), relatedness (6 items, α = .86) and competence (8 items,

α = .71), with the three subscales also forming an index of general need satisfaction

(α = .89) (Deci & Ryan, 2000). General needs satisfaction is innate, psychological

and essential for well-being and all three basic needs of autonomy, relatedness, and

competence must be fulfilled for it to occur (ibid). For the purposes of this research,

autonomy was the only sub-scale used which refers to the need to feel that one’s

23

Page 24: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

behaviour and outcomes of the behaviour are self-determined as opposed to being

influenced or controlled by outside forces. Although not explicitly measuring

perceptions of staff coercion (for which there was no measure available), the BPNSS

autonomy sub-scale was thought to be the most appropriate measure to capture the

more long-term coercive measures used in day to day practice (i.e. persuasion,

interpersonal leverage). For example, it would be assumed that those who scored

lower on autonomy would feel more controlled/influenced by outside forces within

their current environment.

Service users were asked to rate how true a statement is for them currently (e.g. “In

my daily life, I frequently have to do what I am told”). These statements were a rated

on a 7-point Likert scale from (1) “Not at all true” to (7) “Very True”. This

questionnaire has not been validated with forensic populations however it has been

used across a wide variety of participant groups and shown to have good external

validity (Johnston & Finney, 2010).

3.6.2 Interpersonal Sensitivity Circumplex (ISC) (Hopwood et al., 2011)

(dominant and cold dominant octant scales) – The Interpersonal Sensitivity

Circumplex is a 64-item self-report questionnaire with content that represents

behaviours that would bother most people to some extent (e.g., “It bothers me when a

person is hostile”). These behaviours were rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale

ranging from (1) “not at all, never bothers me” to (7) “Very much, bothers me most

of the time.” Items cohere into eight 8-item scales (i.e., octant scales) that each

represent an interpersonal sensitivity (e.g., sensitivity to control (dominance) and

antagonism (cold dominance)). The scale has been found by the study authors to

24

Page 25: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

show adequate internal consistency (α = .89, Range = .72–.92, dominance: α=.84,

Mean=6.04, SD=1.06; cold dominance: α=.86, Mean=6.17, SD=1.10).

3.6.3 Brief Aggression Questionnaire (BAQ) (Webster et al., 2014) is a brief

version of the Buss-Perry Hostility inventory used to measure trait aggression and

includes 4 sub-scales to assess physical aggression (e.g. “If I have to resort to

violence to protect my rights, I will”), verbal aggression (e.g. “when people annoy

me I may tell them what I think of them”), anger (e.g. “I have trouble controlling my

temper), and hostility (e.g. “When people are especially nice, I wonder what they

want”). These traits were measured on a 7-point response scale ranging from (1)

“extremely uncharacteristic of me” to (7) “extremely characteristic of me”. This

questionnaire has not been validated with forensic populations however has been

used across a wide variety of participant groups. Test–retest reliability correlations

were found by the study authors to show strong and significant total score reliability

(α = .81) with scores ranging from .68 to .81 among the four subscales.

3.7 Ethical considerations

NHS Ethics Approval was obtained from the East of England – Essex Research

Ethics Committee [18/EE/0028]. See Appendix D for the ethics approval letter.

Confidentiality and anonymity of service users were maintained by assigning each

person a study code representing their personal details (example, P1, P2) at the point

of consent. All data was identified by participant study codes from the point of

consent onwards. Hard copies of the data (including consent forms) was stored in

locked filing cabinets at the hospital site. All electronic data was stored under

25

Page 26: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

password protected files on an encrypted USB stick. In accordance with the

University policy, all project data related to the administration of the project, (e.g.

consent forms) will be held for at least 6 years and all research data for at least 10

years.

Service users had to have capacity to consent to take part in the research and could

not be of high risk which was the reason for consent to approach each service user

being sought from their responsible clinician first. Some questions included in the

questionnaires could be of a sensitive nature for service users. As such, service users

were offered breaks if necessary and were given an opportunity to discuss any

sensitive and/or difficult issues that may have arisen during the completion of the

questionnaires. In addition, as part of the introduction we explained that we had an

obligation to pass on information regarding any risk or safeguarding concerns raised

during the interview. If any issues raised during the interview indicated that the

participant or others were at risk, the ward manager and/or named nurse linked with

the service user’s care would be informed after completion of the questionnaires.

Given that the research was in a secure-care setting, each participant’s care team

would be available to them 24/7 if they were to become distressed by the research.

3.8 Data analysis

Data was entered into the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version

25 (IBM Corp, 2017). Descriptive statistics were carried out on demographic (age,

gender, ethnicity), clinical (diagnosis), and forensic (index offence, length of stay in

current unit) characteristics of the study sample. Descriptive statistics were also used

to calculate the mean, standard deviation, and coefficient alpha (Cronbach, 1951)

26

Page 27: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

scores for all study variables. In addition, prior to analysis of study variables, the raw

data was inspected for the presence of non-normal distribution and potential outliers

by a Shapiro-Wilk’s test (p = <0.00) and a visual inspection of histograms and box

plots.

Preliminary associations between variables were investigated through correlation

analysis (hypotheses 1-3). Bivariate Pearson Correlational statistical tests (or non-

parametric equivalents) were used to test for associations between: 1) interpersonal

sensitivity to dominance or cold dominance and perceived staff coercion (levels of

perceived autonomy); 2) interpersonal sensitivity to dominance or cold dominance

and self-reported rates of aggression; 3) interpersonal sensitivity to dominance or

cold dominance levels and rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff 4)

perceived staff coercion (levels of perceived autonomy) and self-reported rates of

aggression; 5) perceived staff coercion (levels of perceived autonomy) and rates of

recorded aggressive incidents towards staff. Effect sizes were also determined using

Cohen’s r (1988) standard correlation coefficient.

Mediation analysis was conducted using PROCESS (Version 31) (Hayes, 2012)

through SPSS to investigate the relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to

dominance and self-reported anger and rates of recorded aggressive incidents, with

perceived staff coercion as a mediator (hypotheses 4). For each sub-hypothesis,

mediation analysis was used to look at the direct effect, indirect effect, and the total

effect. The direct effect looks at the effect of the independent variable on the

dependent variable (also known as Pathway c’). The indirect effect looks at the effect

of the independent variable on the dependent variable through the mediating variable

27

Page 28: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

(also known as pathway ab). The indirect effect was tested using a bootstrap

estimation approach based on 10000 samples (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Lastly, the

total effect looks at the sum of the direct effect and the indirect effect (also known as

pathway c (ab + c’). See Figure 1 for an example of a basic mediation model.

Figure 1: Diagram of a basic mediation model (taken from Field, 2017)

4.0 Results

4.1 Description of outcome data

Descriptive statistics and tests of normality were run on all study variables. These

findings have been summarised below. See Table 2 for further details on this data for

each of the variables. See also Appendices E- G for the SPSS outputs for the

descriptive statistics, internal consistency, and tests of normality.

4.1.1 Interpersonal sensitivity

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for each of the ISC dominant and cold dominant

domains ranged from .79 to .87 which indicates adequate internal consistency.

28

Independent Variable (IV)

Mediator Variable

(MV)

Dependent Variable

(DV)

Pathway a Pathway b

Pathway c’= Direct effectPathway ab= Indirect effect

Pathway c= Total effect (ab + c’)

Page 29: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Participants were most sensitive to antagonism (ISC cold dominance domain,

mean=4.89, SD=1.75, α=.87) which was closely followed by sensitivity to control

(ISC dominance domain, Mean=4.59, SD=1.50, α=.79). A Shapiro-Wilk’s test (p =

>0.05) and a visual inspection of histograms and box plots suggested that dominant

and cold dominant domains were normally distributed.

4.1.2 Perceptions of staff coercion (perceived autonomy)

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the BPNSS Autonomy sub-scale was .66 indicating

questionable internal consistency. A visual scan of item-total statistics (See

Appendix G) indicated that it would not be possible to increase the alpha coefficient

to an adequate level by the removal of any individual items. The mean BPNSS

Autonomy score in the participant sample was 4.23 (SD=.84). Although the Shapiro-

Wilk’s test (p = <0.05) rejected the null hypothesis, a visual inspection of histograms

and box plots suggested that perceived autonomy scores were normally distributed.

4.1.3 Self-reported anger

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for BAQ total score reliability was .69 and scores

ranged from .49 to .80 among the four subscales indicating questionable internal

consistency. A visual scan of item-total statistics (see Appendix F) indicated that it

would not be possible to increase the alpha coefficient to an adequate level by the

removal of any individual items. The mean BAQ total score in the participant

sample was 3.33 (SD=.84). With regards to the sub-domains, participants scored

highest on verbal aggression (mean=3.71, SD=1.3, α=.51) which was followed by

physical aggression (mean=3.32, SD=1.7, α=.80) and hostility (mean=3.17, SD=1.5,

α=.60). Participants scored lowest on anger (mean=2.50, SD=1.1, α=.49). Although a

29

Page 30: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Shapiro-Wilk’s test (p = <0.05) rejected the null hypothesis, a visual inspection of

histograms and box plots suggested that self-reported anger mean total scale scores

were normally distributed.

4.1.4 Rates of aggression towards staff

A total of 308 aggressive incidents were recorded against staff within the past year.

A total of 224 (72.7%) of the incidents recorded were verbal assaults against staff, 66

(21.4%) were physical assaults against staff, and 18 (5.8%) were attempted physical

assaults against staff. These offences were committed by 28 (40%) of the 70 service

users who took part in the study. Twenty-one (30%) participants had been the

assailant for more than one incident and one participant was responsible for 81

(26.3%) of the incidents recorded. Incident data was missing for 7 (10%) participants

due to them not giving permission for the research team to access this information. A

Shapiro-Wilk’s test (p<0.001) and a visual inspection of histograms and box plots

suggested that rates of recorded incidents of aggression towards staff were positively

skewed.

30

Page 31: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Table 2: Descriptive statistics of study variables

Measure Domains/ sub-domains

Normative mean scores (SD)

n Mean score (SD)

Range (Median)

Alpha α

ISC (8 items for each sub-domain)

Dominance Cold dominance

Hopwood et al. (2011) n=1,3366.04 (1.06)6.17 (1.10)

70 4.59 (1.50) 4.89 (1.75)

1-8 (4.50)1-8 (4.88)

.79

.87

BAQ (12 items)

PhysicalVerbalAngerHostilityTotal score

Webster et al. (2013) n=1,0003.63 (2.15)1.70 (1.70)3.19 (1.69)3.93 (1.61)4.02 (1.21)

70 3.32 (1.7)3.71 (1.3)2.50 (1.1)3.17 (1.5)3.33 (.84)

1-7 (3.00)1-7 (3.67)1-6 (2.33)1-7 (3.00)2-6 (3.33)

.80

.51

.49

.60

.69BPNSS Autonomy

(7 items)Wei et al. (2005) n=299 5.05 (.82);

Gagne (2003) n=1185.16 (1.05)

70 4.23 (.84) 2-6 (4.14) .66

Rates of aggression towards staff in past year

Verbal Physical Attempted Total

----

63 3.56 (10.53)1.05 (2.59)0.29 (.79)4.89 (12.33)

0-74 (.00) 0-14 (.00) 0-4 (.00)0-81 (.00)

----

4.2 Correlational analysis

Preliminary relationships between variables were investigated through Bivariate

Pearson Correlational statistical analysis. Non-parametric statistical tests

(Spearman’s Rank correlation coefficient) were run on the correlations that involved

the rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff due to the non-normal

distribution of the data. See table 3 for the key findings from the correlational

analyses. The findings of these correlational tests have also been summarised below,

31

Page 32: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

responding to hypotheses 1-3 outlined in the methods (see section 2.2). See also

Appendix H for the SPSS outputs for the correlational analyses.

4.2.1 Hypothesis 1: Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance, anger and aggression

1a) Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and self-reported anger – There was a

medium (Cohen, 1988), statistically significant positive correlation between

interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and self-reported anger, r = .42**, p=<.01.

The results suggested that those who were more sensitive to others’ dominance were

likely to report higher levels of anger.

1b) Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and rates of recorded aggressive incidents

towards staff – There was no correlation between interpersonal sensitivity to

dominance and rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff, r = -.05, p=.72.

1c) Interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and self-reported anger – There was

a small, but not significant, positive correlation between interpersonal sensitivity to

cold dominance and self-rated anger, r = .16, p= .18.

1d) Interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and rates of recorded aggressive

incidents towards staff – There was a small, but not significant, negative correlation

between interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and rates of recorded aggressive

incidents toward staff, r =-.11, p= .39.

4.2.2. Hypothesis 2: Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance or cold dominance and

perceptions of staff coercion

2a) Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and perceptions of staff coercion – There

was no correlation between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and perceptions of

staff coercion, r = -.08, p= .49.

32

Page 33: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

2b) Interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and perceptions of staff coercion –

There was no correlation between interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and

perceptions of staff coercion, r = -.04, p= .78.

4.2.3 Hypothesis 3: Perceptions of staff coercion, anger and aggression

3a) Perceptions of staff coercion and self-reported anger – There was no correlation

between perceptions of staff coercion and self-reported anger, r = -.04, p= .77.

3b) Perceptions of staff coercion and rate of recorded aggressive incidents towards

staff – There was no correlation between perceptions of staff coercion and recorded

rates of aggressive incidents toward staff, r = -.08, p= .54.

Table 3: Bivariate correlations between variables

Variables Self-rated anger

Rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff

Perceptions of staff coercion

Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

.42** -.05 -.08

Interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance

.16 -.11 -.04

Perceptions of staff coercion

-.04 -.08 -

Note: **P<.01

4.3 Mediation analysis

Table 4 presents the key findings from the mediation analyses. The findings of the

mediation have also been summarised below, responding to the fourth hypothesis

outlined in the methods (see section 2.2). See also Appendix I for the SPSS outputs

for the mediation analyses.

33

Page 34: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

4.3.1 Results for hypothesis 4a: In order to test the mediating effect of perceptions

of staff coercion on the relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

and self-rated anger, model 1 (hypothesis 4a) was tested (see Figure 1). Interpersonal

sensitivity to dominance did not significantly predict perceptions of staff coercion

and perceptions of staff coercion did not predict self-rated anger. Interpersonal

sensitivity to dominance predicted self-rated anger when perceptions of staff

coercion was or was not in the model, b=.24, 95% CI (.11, .36), p<.001. The indirect

effect confidence interval contained zero (-.02 and .02) which indicates that there

was no evidence to support the idea that perceptions of staff coercion mediates the

relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance on self-rated anger. The

R² value tells us that interpersonal sensitivity to dominance explains 18% (R²=.18) of

the variance in self-rated anger. The positive coefficient (b value) tells us that as

interpersonal sensitivity to dominance increases, self-rated anger also increases.

Figure 2: Mediation model for hypothesis 4a

4.3.2 Results for Hypothesis 4b: In order to test the mediating effect of perceptions

of staff coercion on the relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

and rates of recorded aggressive incidents toward staff, model 2 (hypothesis 4b) was

34

Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

Perceptions of staff

coercion

Self-rated anger

(Pathway a) b= -.05, p=.49

(Pathway b) b= -.00, p= .99

(Pathway c’) Direct effect, b= .24, p= .00***(Pathway ab) Indirect effect, b= .00, 95% CI

[-.02, .02]

(Pathway c) Total effect, b= .24, p= .00***

Page 35: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

tested (see Figure 2). Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance did not significantly

predict perceptions of staff coercion and perceptions of staff coercion did not predict

rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff. Interpersonal sensitivity to

dominance did not predict rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff when

perceptions of staff coercion was or was not in the model.

Figure 3: Mediation model for hypothesis 4b

4.3.3 Hypothesis 4c: In order to test the mediating effect of perceptions of staff

coercion on the relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and

self-rated anger, model 3 (hypothesis 3c) was tested (see Figure 3). Interpersonal

sensitivity to cold dominance did not significantly predict perceptions of staff

coercion and perceptions of staff coercion did not predict self-rated anger.

Interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance did not predict self-rated anger when

perceptions of staff coercion was or was not in the model.

35

Interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

Perceptions of staff

coercion

Recorded rates of

aggression towards staff

(Pathway c’) Direct effect, b= -.69, p= .51(Pathway ab) Indirect effect, b= -.02, 95% CI [-.32, .35]

(Pathway a)b= -.06, p= .38 (Pathway b)

b= .31, p= .87

(Pathway c) Total effect, b= -.71, p= .49

Page 36: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Figure 4: Mediation model for hypothesis 4c

4.3.4 Hypothesis 4d: In order to test the mediating effect of perceptions of staff

coercion on the relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and

rates of recorded aggressive incidents toward staff, model 4 (hypothesis 4d) was

tested (see Figure 4). Interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance did not significantly

predict perceptions of staff coercion and perceptions of staff coercion did not predict

rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff. Interpersonal sensitivity to cold

dominance did not predict rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff when

perceptions of staff coercion was or was not in the model.

Figure 5: Mediation model for hypothesis 4d

36

Perceptions of staff coercion

Interpersonal sensitivity to

cold dominance

Self-rated anger

Perceptions of staff coercion

Interpersonal sensitivity to

cold dominance

Rates of recorded

aggression towards staff

(Pathway a) b= .02, p= .77 (Pathway b)

b= .49, p= .79

(Pathway c’) Direct effect, b= -.55, p= .55(Pathway ab) Indirect effect, b= .01, 95% CI

[-.19, .32]

(Pathway a) b= .02, p= .77 (Pathway b)

b= -.04, p= .73

(Pathway c’) Direct effect, b=.08, p=.18 (Pathway ab) Indirect effect, b= -.00, 95% CI

[-.04, .02]

(Pathway c) Total effect, b= .08, p= .18

(Pathway c) Total effect, b= -.54, p= .56

Page 37: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

Table 4: Mediation analysis results

Sub-Hypotheses

Regression coefficients(for each pathway)

Measure of indirect effect

Pathway a Pathway b Pathway c’ (Direct effect)

Pathway c(Total effect)

LCI UCI Pm R²

4a) IV: ISD DV: SRA MV: PSC

-.05 -.00 .24 .24 -.02 .02 .00 .18

4b) IV: ISD DV: RRIS MV: PSC

-0.6 .31 -.69 -.71 -.32 .35 .03 .01

4c) IV: ISCDDV: SRA MV: PSC

.02 -0.4 .08 .08 -0.4 .02 -.01 .03

4d) IV: ISCD DV: RRISMV: PSC

.02 .49 -.55 -.54 -.19 .32 -.02 .01

Note: IV=dependent variable; DV= dependent variable; MV= mediating variable; ISD= interpersonal sensitivity to dominance; ISCD= interpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance; SRA= self-rated anger, RRIS= Rates of recorded aggressive incidents towards staff; PSC= perceptions of staff coercion; Pm= the proportion of the direct effect that is explained by the indirect effect.

37

Page 38: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

5.0 Discussion

5.1 Summary of study results

The results from this study indicated a significant relationship between interpersonal

sensitivity to dominance and self-reported rates of anger, where forensic service

users’ who had higher levels of interpersonal sensitivity to others’ dominance were

likely to report higher rates of anger. There were, however, no significant

relationships found between all other study variables. Furthermore, the relationship

between forensic service users’ interpersonal sensitivity dominance/cold dominance

and self-reported anger/recorded rates of aggression towards staff were not found to

be mediated by their perceptions of staff coercion. I will first discuss these findings

in relation to relevant literature. I will then move on to discuss the findings in the

context of strengths and limitations of the chosen methodology and methods

employed to collect and analyse the data.

5.2 Contextualising the findings within previous research

The significant relationship found between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance

(control of others) and self-reported anger may be associated with the findings of

previous qualitative research whereby the controlling nature of staff was a factor

perceived by service users to contribute to repetitive acts of aggression (Meehan et

al., 2006). These findings simultaneously support and contradict previous empirical

evidence where, although dominant and coercive service user interpersonal styles

have been shown to significantly correlate with aggressive and violent behaviour

(e.g. Daffern et al., 2010a; Doyle et al., 2006; Vernham et al., 2016), in the context of

interpersonal sensitivities, we would also expect to see those who value personal

authority and being in control, to be more frustrated by the coercive behaviours of

38

Page 39: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

others (Henderson & Horrowitz, 2006). Contrary to sensitivity to dominance, there

was no relationship found between sensitivity to cold dominance (antagonism) and

self-rated anger. Perhaps in the context of secure-care settings, a cold dominant

interpersonal style may be necessary to assert/protect one’s status, particularly when

under threat to others who are competing for a less admiring cold dominant position

(Hopwood, 2018). If we consider this notion in the context of complementarity

(Kiesler, 1987), we would expect to see those who display more cold dominant

interpersonal styles to also be less sensitive to others displaying cold dominance.

Furthermore, it may be that those who align with more cold dominant interpersonal

styles may show anger in a more passive, covert manner (moody disagreeableness,

hostility) (Hopwood et al., 2009) which is perhaps captured less so by the BAQ

compared to those more overt displays of anger (i.e. hitting out at others, losing one’s

temper).

Aside anger and aggression towards staff, in the context of a forensic service user

population, there may be a need to further consider how service users’ maladaptive

behaviours can manifest in such coercive environments. For example, given the re-

percussions for service users who do display increased aggression (i.e. increased re-

strictions such as loss of escorted or unescorted leave), perhaps there is a need to re-

store a sense of control by directing aggression elsewhere, such as bullying towards

other service users who may be considered more weaker and/or vulnerable (e.g. Ire-

land, 2006) or even through acts of self-neglect and/or self-harm (e.g. Jeglic et al.,

2005). This aligns with the current study’s findings where there was no significant

relationship found between actual rates of aggression towards staff and other study

variables; it is therefore possible that actual aggressive incidents may have been dir-

39

Page 40: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

ected outside of the staff-service user relationship. In addition, aside coercive prac-

tices, perhaps aggressive incidents are provoked by other aversive stimuli such as,

for instance, uncontrollable noises on the ward, and interactions with other service

users (Winje et al., 2018) or the distress caused by delusions or hallucinations (e.g.

Barlow et al., 2000).

The fact that perceptions of staff coercion was not found to be a mediator between

interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and cold dominance and self-reported anger

and/or rates of aggression, challenges previous notions that coercive practices can

have a counter-therapeutic effect upon increased cycles of aggression and violence

(Goren, 1993). The current findings also contrast with previous quantitative research

which have suggested that forensic service users are more likely to respond to coer-

cive practices in an assertive (acomplementarity) manner as opposed to a submissive

(complementary) manner (Daffern et al., 2010b). Consistent with Hopwood and col-

leagues (2011) research into interpersonal sensitivities, service user participants may

have been more sensitive to interpersonal styles that are opposite to their own. For

example, those who have more dominant interpersonal styles may more likely be

sensitive to or bothered by the passivity and submissiveness of others (Hopwood et

al., 2011). As suggested in the previous section, this may be due to the questionable

validity of the questionnaire used to measure perceived coercion (autonomy sub-

scale in the BPNSS) or it may be perhaps that there is no mediating relationship to be

found. Perhaps, as suggested by Lorem and colleagues (2015), it may also be im-

portant to consider service users’ perceptions of ‘good coercion’ which coincide with

their own best interests that acts as an important measure to protect them from their

own impulses.

40

Page 41: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

5.3 Strengths and limitations of the research process

The alpha coefficients for both the BPNSS and the BAQ both did not meet satisfact-

ory criteria for internal consistency in the current research study. This means that

there was a lack of coherence between individual questionnaire items in being able to

measure a given construct. Perhaps the items were not worded appropriately or were

not suitable to use in a forensic setting. It is therefore questionable as to how reliable

the measures were in being able to capture service users’ self-reported anger and per-

ceived autonomy.

Interestingly and unexpectedly, the mean score of perceived autonomy in the current

environment was not that dissimilar than those from previous research based within

non-forensic settings (e.g. current study: 4.23 (.84); previous studies: 5.05 (.82) (Wei

et al. 2005); 5.00 (.80) (Gagne, 2003)). Perhaps it is important to consider service

users’ perceptions of autonomy as relative to their experiences within forensic

settings. For example, those who were in assertive rehabilitation wards (wards where

service users work towards lesser security) – which was over half of the sample –

may have perceived higher levels of autonomy due to an increased sense of freedom

compared to when they were first admitted into the secure care system. A large

proportion of the study sample had been residing in secure care where just over one

third (36.1%) of the sample had been in the current unit for under 6 months. As

suggested by Vernham and colleagues (2014), it is possible that those participants

who had more recently been admitted in to secure-care have limited interpersonal

strategies and may more likely act out aggressively as a means of coping compared

to those who had stayed in secure-care for longer. In addition, mean BAQ scores

were similar in current and previous study samples and it is therefore possible that

41

Page 42: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

forensic service users may be less likely to respond honestly about their levels of

anger due to concerns that this may impact upon their progress whilst in hospital. It

is also important to consider that, compared to the general population, forensic

service users may have limited insight into their aggressive traits, hence the

similarity in mean scores.

Although the target sample size of 70 participants was reached, in consideration of

the small effect sizes shown through some of the statistical outputs (e.g. between in-

terpersonal sensitivity to cold dominance and self-reported anger; between interper-

sonal sensitivity to cold dominance and rates of recorded aggressive incidents), a big-

ger sample size was required to detect statistical power between study variables. In

addition, while common place in inpatient settings, there was a high attrition rate

with only 31.5% of service users approached taking part in the research. It is there-

fore important to consider the representativeness of the final sample. For example,

perhaps those who chose not to take part were less happy within their current envir-

onment due to feeling more coerced and/or restricted. Furthermore, perhaps those

who were more aggressive and/or problematic with members of staff were deemed as

too risky to be included in the study and therefore not approached.

A researcher remained present with the service users whilst they completed the

questionnaires. Although this was done for the purposes of providing support to

service users if required (e.g. outlining instructions and clarifying questions), it also

resulted in all questionnaires being completed fully and as such, there was no

missing data. Over half of the service users requested for the researcher to read out

aloud and complete the questionnaires with them. It was uncertain as to whether

42

Page 43: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

requests to have the questions read out to them was due to needing extra support with

literacy or whether the service user simply preferred to be more interactive with the

researcher (which was explicitly stated by some service users). This raises issues of

potential interviewer bias where, for example, service users may have been more

suggestible to responding in a desired way when responding directly to the

researcher compared to those who completed the questionnaires by themselves.

Conversely, there may have been a higher chance of service users who completed the

questionnaires on their own misinterpreting and/or making errors when responding to

some of the questions. Furthermore, service users would have needed to accurately

hold in mind the nature of the questionnaire whilst answering each item measure, for

instance, this may have been more problematic when completing some of the longer

outcome measures (e.g. ISC).

The retrospective nature of the recorded aggressive incidents towards staff needs to

be considered with caution. Stated as a limitation in previous research studies (e.g.

Daffern et al., 2008; Vernham et al., 2016), the data collected on the specific nature,

severity and direction of aggression perpetrated by service users may have been

reliant on the limited details of the incident forms and on the member of staff

completing such forms (although Vernham and colleagues did find a good degree of

inter-rater reliability). Furthermore, the aggressive incident may have been indirectly

targeted at the member of staff, in other words, there may have been multiple causes

outside of the staff-service user interaction that led to the aggressive behaviour (see

section below for further discussion). The fact that the recorded incidents were used

alongside service users’ self-reported anger meant that observational (incident) data

could circumvent the problems of impression management – an approach lacking in

43

Page 44: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

previous studies. Although not reported in the main results, a significant positive

correlation was found between self-reported anger (BAQ mean scores) and rates of

recorded incidents (see Appendix J). Even though the focus of the BAQ was on self-

reported anger in general rather than self-reported anger towards staff, it can still be

assumed that there is some accuracy in the recorded incidents (i.e. verbal aggression

had the highest mean score on the BAQ and was the highest reported incident

towards staff).

When inspecting the raw data, there were some outliers with regards to some

measures (e.g. a small number of service users scored much higher on self-reported

anger and much lower on perceived autonomy than others). Unsurprisingly, these

service users had also been involved in substantially more aggressive incidents

towards staff in the last year, hence the non-normally distributed incident data. As

such, it did not feel appropriate to retract these service users’ data from the data set

as these responses seemed to coincide to the retrospective data collected. As

suggested above, perhaps including those service users who were in seclusion and/or

were more riskier, may reduce the chances of these outliers as it would be anticipated

that their BAQ mean scores (self-rated anger) and their involvement in aggressive

incidents would be higher than the majority of the participants who were included in

the sample who had been involved in no aggressive incidents in the past year. In

addition, those residing in the high dependency (more restricted) wards would also

expect to score lower on perceived autonomy in their current environment compared

to most of our sample who were residing in assertive rehabilitation wards.

44

Page 45: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

5.4 Implications and Recommendations for policy and practice

As there was a lack of significant findings from the current study, the subsequent im-

plications and recommendations for future clinical practice have been drawn upon

from the wider literature that surrounds interpersonal style and coercive practices in

forensic settings. It is important to stress that this wider literature has been used to

make tentative suggestions of what the study’s findings might mean for clinical prac-

tice.

The findings of this study indicate that, although there was a significant relationship

between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and self-reported anger, there was no

relationship between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and cold dominance and

actual acts of aggression towards staff. In future practice, there may be a need to con-

sider the types of behaviours that are recorded as acts of aggression and violence and

furthermore, to try and record more subtle maladaptive behaviours that capture how

service user’s react to coercive practices (i.e. become more demanding towards staff,

bullying other service users, self-harm).

Recommendation: In future clinical practice, it may be useful to consider

the need to collect more detailed information from multiple stakeholder perspectives

about untoward incidents that have taken place. For example, building in to existing

models of practice (e.g. The ‘Safewards’ model) to ask service user/s, and more than

one member of staff on what led to the incident (Royal College of Nursing (RCN),

2016). For example, earlier events that acted as a trigger and possible rationales for

the behaviour.

45

Page 46: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

In future clinical practice, it may be important to consider autonomy as relative to

service users’ experiences within the secure-care environment where for example,

those residing on assertive rehabilitation wards are likely to feel much less restricted

than when first admitted into hospital. Perhaps coercive practices are respected by

service users where, although they may be interpersonally sensitive to others’ domin-

ance, in the context of staffs’ duty of care, it is understood that rules, boundaries, and

restrictions are in the secure-care environment for a purpose. As suggested by Dren-

nan and Wooldridge (2014), staff and service users need to work together to develop

an organisational culture in which there is a balance between safety and recovery.

Recommendation: Rather than coercive practice being an antithesis to service

users’ autonomy and human rights (Hui et al., 2013), perhaps a more dialectical

stance needs to be taken when conceptualising coercive practices within policy and

practice. There is therefore a need to focus on how coercive practice in secure

settings is not only something that is of benefit for service users but that it also a

responsibility that they can share with staff in ensuring that themselves and others are

kept safe whilst having autonomy in the secure care environment.

Echoing previous research (Hopwood et al., 2011), service users’ interpersonal

sensitivities may be context specific. Therefore, building upon this idea, service users

may only feel bothered by coercion when it does not feel necessary and/or relevant.

For instance, the difference between a member of staff who used coercive practices

for the purposes of service users and staffs’ safety versus a member of staff who was

overly dominant on a consistent (and perhaps unnecessary) basis. The latter may be

what can lead to repetitive acts of aggression and violence by service users as this

type of coercion may be perceived be a way to belittle them. As suggested by the

46

Page 47: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

findings of this research, whereby most service users perceived some sense of

autonomy in their current environment, it may be that the use of coercive practices

within a secure-care setting is not seen by service users to unnecessarily restrict them

on a day to day basis.

Recommendation: As suggested in a document on ‘Positive and Proactive

Care’ (Royal College of Nursing, 2016), “avoiding assumptions, threats and

provocations adds to positive outcomes” (pp. 7). This approach may respond to the

interpersonal motives of those service users who may be more interpersonally

sensitive to others’ dominance whereby the implementation of coercive practices

within secure-care settings should take place in the context of mutually respected and

positive relationships between staff and service users (DH, 2014). Furthermore, with

collaborative empowerment as central to care, psychosocial interventions such as

persuasion and negotiation should be made a transparent part of everyday

communication between staff and service users. As suggested by Winje and

colleagues (2018), perhaps enhanced methods for communicating restrictions on

service users’ behaviour (e.g. remaining calm with self-assured expressions) needs to

be incorporated into staff training. This coincides with previous research that

suggests good clinical practice cannot be separated from the formal, moral evaluation

of coercion (i.e. was it necessary and was it implemented with open communication

and empathy) (Lorem et al., 2015).

5.5 Recommendations for future research

Future research should attempt to widen the participant sample to those who are pos-

sibly seen as riskier, such as those in seclusion or who are residing on higher depend-

ency wards. This may help to provide more variance and/ or extremities in service

47

Page 48: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

users’ views of their current environment where, for example, there are lower per-

ceptions of autonomy and higher levels of self-rated anger. Assuming interpersonal

relationships between service user and staff are more problematic (particularly con-

sidering that 40% of the sample had displayed violent behaviour in the past year), it

would also be assumed that interpersonal sensitivities would be more extreme and

possibly directed to their current circumstances.

As indicated in the current study and in previous research (e.g. Chambers et al.,

2009), there is a lack of standardised outcome measures that have been validated

within forensic populations and as such the internal validity of what is being meas-

ured is questionable given their use in non-forensic populations. Therefore, there is a

need for more measures to be validated specifically for forensic service users who

are residing in secure-care settings. More specifically, there is no outcome measure

that adequately captures service users’ perception of staff coercion. As such, the de-

velopment of an outcome measure that explicitly captures either perceptions of coer-

cion and/or autonomy which is specifically designed and validated within a forensic

setting would be beneficial for future research studies. For example, perhaps to

measure actual levels of restriction instead of perceived lack of autonomy, an ordinal

scale of “restrictiveness” could be developed to look at, for instance, perceptions of

seclusion, ward placement, and ground access. Furthermore, it would be useful to

measure self-reported anger specifically towards staff as opposed to general self-re-

ported anger as this may give a more specific indicator as to the extent to which ser-

vice users experience anger towards staff and in turn, whether this is related to per-

ceptions of coercion.

48

Page 49: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Given that individuals may be more sensitive to interpersonal styles that are opposite

to their own (Hopwood et al., 2011), future research would benefit from looking

more explicitly at the relationship between interpersonal styles and interpersonal

sensitivities. For example, it would be useful to find out whether, in a forensic

service user population, if service users’ reactions to staff coercion are more

submissive (complementary) or dominant (accomplementary). The complementarity

of their reactions to coercion therefore needs to not only consider whether others’

controlling behaviour is bothering to the person (their interpersonal sensitivity) but

also how they are then predisposed to react to the coercion of others (their

interpersonal style).

In addition, as interpersonal theory relies on the way in which individuals establish

relationships with others (Kiesler, 1997), it may be useful to investigate staff’s own

interpersonal styles and sensitivities. Perhaps some staff members have more

dominant interpersonal styles than others and may be therefore seen as more

coercive. Aside the implementation of hospital rules and regulations that encapsulate

restrictive practice, this would help to shed light on how staff characteristics interact

with service users own sense making and reactions to such coercive measures (i.e. it

may be more about the way in which coercive practices are implemented).

6.0 Conclusion

In conclusion of the study’s findings, perceptions of staff coercion (perceived

autonomy) was not found to mediate the relationship between interpersonal sensitiv-

ity to dominance/cold dominance and self-reported anger and rates of aggression to-

wards staff. Furthermore, although there was a significant relationship found

49

Page 50: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

between interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and self-reported anger, there was no

relationship between interpersonal sensitivity and actual rates of aggressive incidents

towards staff. It may be important to consider some of the implications that these

non-significant findings have upon clinical practice, where it may not be coercive

practices that are necessarily increasing rates of aggression (as indicated by previous

research) but may be the way in which coercive practices are implemented. These

non-significant findings may also, in part, be due to some of the study’s limitations.

For example, the use of measures which had not been validated in a forensic popula-

tion, the unavailability of measures that accurately captured study variables (i.e. per-

ceptions of staff coercion), and the possible need for a larger sample size. With these

limitations in mind, the conclusions that were drawn from the current study’s find-

ings should be considered under a tentative lens. The conduct of future related re-

search (as discussed in the previous section) should be considered in context of the

study’s limitations and recommendations.

References

American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Nurses Association & National Association of Psychiatric Health Systems. (2003). Learning from each other: Success stories and ideas for reducing restraint/seclusion in behavioural health. Retrieved from WorldCat Website: https://www.worldcat.org/title/learning-from-each-other-success-stories-and-ideas-for-reducing-restraintseclusion-in-behavioral- health/oclc/52758580

Barlow, K., Grenyer, B., & Ilkiw-Lavalle, O. (2000). Prevalence and precipitants of aggression in psychiatric inpatient units. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34(6), 967-974. DOI: 10.1080/000486700271

Bjørkly, S. (1999). A ten‐year prospective study of aggression in a special secure unit for dangerous patients. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 57-63. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10216464

BjØrkvik, J., Biringer, E.V.A., Eikeland, O.J., & Nielson, G.H. (2009). Self-esteem and interpersonal functioning in psychiatric outpatients. Scandinavian

50

Page 51: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Journal of Psychology, 50(3), 259-265. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00709

Blackburn, R. (1998). Relationship of personality disorders to observer ratings of interpersonal style in forensic psychiatric patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 12(1), 77-85. DOI: 10.1521/pedi.1998.12.1.77

Blom-Cooper, L. (1992). Report of the Committee of Inquiry into Complaints about Ashworth Hospital, Cm 2028-1. London: HMSO.

Bowers, L., Stewart, D., Papadopoulos, C., Dack, C., Ross, J., Khanom, H., & Jeffery, D. (2011). Inpatient violence and aggression: a literature. Report from the Conflict and Containment Reduction Research Programme. Retrieved from http://www.kcl.ac.uk/iop/depts/hspr/research/ciemh/mhn/projects/litreview/Li

tRevAgg .pdf

Bubb, S. (2014). Winterbourne view–time for change: transforming the commissioning of services for people with learning disabilities and/or autism. Retrieved from British Library Website: https://www.bl.uk/collection-items/winterbourne-view--time-for-change-transforming-the-commissioning-of-services-for-people-with-learning-disabilities-andor-autism

Castro, M., Cockerton, T., & Birke, S. (2002). From discharge to follow-up: a small-scale study of medium secure provision in the independent sector. British Journal of Forensic Practice, 4, 31-9. DOI: 10.1108/14636646200200019

Chambers, J. C., Yiend, J., Barrett, B., Burns, T., Doll, H., Fazel, S., Jenkinson, C., Kaur, A., Knapp, M., Plugge, E., Sutton, L., & Sutton, L. (2009). Outcome measures used in forensic mental health research: a structured review. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 19(1), 9-27. DOI: 10.1002/cbm.724

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analyses for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cookson, A., Daffern, M., & Foley, F. (2012). Relationship between aggression, in-terpersonal style, and therapeutic alliance during short‐term psychiatric hospi-talization. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 21(1), 20-29. DOI:10.1111/14470349.2011.00764

Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297-334. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02310555

Daffern, M., Mayer, M. M., & Martin, T. (2003). A preliminary investigation into patterns of aggression in an Australian forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 14(1), 67–84. DOI: 10.1080/1478994031000074306.

51

Page 52: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Daffern, M., Duggan, C., Huband, N., & Thomas, S. (2008). The impact of interpersonal style on aggression and treatment non-completion in patients with personality disorder admitted to a medium secure psychiatric unit. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14(6), 481-492. DOI: 10.1080/10683160801948717

Daffern, M., Duggan, C., Huband, N., & Thomas, S. (2010a). Staff and Patient’s Perceptions of Each Other’s Interpersonal Style Relationship with

Severity of Personality Disorder. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 54(4), 611-624. DOI: 10.1177/0306624X09335111

Daffern, M., Tonkin, M., Howells, K., Krishnan, G., Ijomah, G., & Milton, J. (2010b). The impact of interpersonal style and perceived coercion on aggression and self-harm in personality-disordered patients admitted to a secure psychiatric hospital. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 21(3), 426-445. DOI:10.1080/14789940903505951

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R.M. (2000). The “what” and the “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behaviour. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268. DOI: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Department of Health. (2014). Positive and proactive care: reducing the need for restrictive interventions. Retrieved from Department of Health Website: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/

attac hment_data/file/300293/JRA_DoH_Guidance_on_RP_web_accessible.pdf

Dickens, G., Picchioni, M., & Long, C. (2013). Aggression in specialist secure and forensic inpatient mental health care: incidence across care pathways. Journal of Forensic Practice, 15, 206-17. DOI: 10.1108/JFP-09-2012-0017

Doyle, M., & Dolan, M. (2006). Evaluating the validity of anger regulation problems, interpersonal style, and disturbed mental state for predicting inpatient violence. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 24(6), 783-798. DOI: 10.1002/bsl.739

Drennan, G., & Wooldridge, J. (2014). Making recovery a reality in forensic services. Implementing Recovery through Organisational Change (ImROC) briefing paper. Retrieved from the Centre for Mental Health and Mental Health Network NHS Confederation Website: file:///C:/Users/jess/Downloads/ImROC-briefing-10-Making-Recovery-a-Reality-in-Forensic-Settings-final-for-web%20(1).pdf

Fallon, P. (1999). Report of the Committee of Inquiry into the Personality Disorder Unit, Ashworth Specialised Hospital, Cm 4194-1. HMSO, London.

52

Page 53: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03193146

Field, A. (2017). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics. London: Sage Publications Ltd.

Gagne, M. (2003). Autonomy support and need satisfaction in the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15(4), 372-390. DOI: 10.1080/714044203

Goren, S., Singh, N. N., & Best, A. M. (1993). The aggression-coercion cycle: Use of seclusion and restraint in a child psychiatric hospital. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 2(1), 61–73. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF01321301

Harty, M. A., Shaw, J., Thomas, S., Dolan, M., Davies, L., Thornicroft, G., Carlisle, J., Moreno, M., Leese, M., Appleby, L., & Jones, P. (2004). The security, clinical and social needs of patients in high security psychiatric hospitals in England. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 15(2), 208-221. DOI: 10.1080/14789940410001703967

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modelling [White paper]. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/ public/process2012.pdf

Henderson, L., & Horowitz, L. M. (2006). Social allergens and frustrated interpersonal motives, presented at the Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research Conference, Philadelphia, 2006. Philadelphia, PA: Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research.

Her Majesty’s Stationery Office (HMSO). (1983). Mental Health Act 1983, c12. Retrieved from https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/1983/20/contents

Holmes, D., Rudge T. & Person, A. (2012). Rethinking violence in health care settings. Ashgate, Farnham: Routledge.

Hopwood, C. J. (2018). Interpersonal dynamics in personality and personality disorders. European Journal of Personality, 32(5), 499-524. DOI: 10.1002/per.2155

Hopwood, C. J., Ansell, E. B., Pincus, A. L., Wright, A. G., Lukowitsky, M. R., & Roche, M. J. (2011). The circumplex structure of interpersonal sensitivities. Journal of Personality, 79(4), 707-740. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00696

Hopwood, C. J., Morey, L. C., Markowitz, J. C., Pinto, A., Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Zanarini, M. C., Shea, T., Yen, S., McGlahsan, T.H., Ansell, E.B., Grilo, C.M., Sanislow, C. A. (2009). The construct validity of

53

Page 54: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

passive-aggressive personality disorder. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 72, 256–268. DOI: 10.1521/psyc.2009.72.3.256

Hornsveld, R. H., Kraaimaat, F. W., Bouwmeester, S., Polak, M. A., & Zwets, A. J. (2014). Behavior on the ward of personality-disordered inpatients and chroni-cally psychotic inpatients during a three-year stay in a Dutch forensic psychiatric hospital. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 25(6), 684-704. DOI: 10.1080/14789949.2014.947308

Horowitz, L. M., Wilson, K. R., Turan, B., Zolotsev, P., Constantino, M. J., & Henderson, L. (2006). How interpersonal motives clarify the meaning of interpersonal behavior: A revised circumplex model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 67–86. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr10014

Hui, A., Middleton, H., & Völlm, B. (2013). Coercive measures in forensic settings: findings from the literature. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 12(1), 53-67. DOI: 10.1080/14999013.2012.740649

IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Ireland, J. L. (2006). Bullying among mentally‐ill patients detained in a high‐secure hospital: an exploratory study of the perceptions of staff and patients into how bullying is defined. Aggressive Behavior: Official Journal of the International Society for Research on Aggression, 32(5), 451-463. DOI: 10.1002/ab.20145

Jeglic, E. L., Vanderhoff, H. A., & Donovick, P. J. (2005). The function of self-harm behavior in a forensic population. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 49(2), 131-142. DOI: 10.1177/0306624X04271130

Johnston, M. M., & Finney, S. J. (2010). Measuring basic needs satisfaction: Evaluating previous research and conducting new psychometric evaluations of the Basic Needs Satisfaction in General Scale. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(4), 280-296. DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.04.003

Kiesler, D. (1987). Manual for the Impact Message Inventory. Mountain View, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Kiesler, D. J. (1997). Contemporary interpersonal theory and research: Personality, psychopathology and psychotherapy. The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 6(4), 339-341. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3350312/

Lillie, R. (2007). Getting clients to hear: Applying principles and techniques of Kiesler's Interpersonal Communication Therapy to assessment feedback. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 80(1), 151-163. DOI: 10.1348/147608306X115198

54

Page 55: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Lorem, G. F., Hem, M. H., & Molewijk, B. (2015). Good coercion: Patients' moral evaluation of coercion in mental health care. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 24(3), 231-240. DOI: 10.1111/inm.12106

Mason, T. (1993). Seclusion as a cultural practice in a special hospital. Educational Action Research, 1(3), 411–423. DOI: 10.1080/0965079930010307

Mason, T. (2006). Introduction. In T. Mason (Ed.), Forensic psychiatry: Influences of evil (pp.1– 14). New Jersey, NJ: Human Press.

Meehan, T., McIntosh, W., & Bergen, H. (2006). Aggressive behaviour in the high‐secure forensic setting: the perceptions of patients. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 13(1), 19-25. DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-

2850.2006.00906

Mezey, G. C., Kavuma, M., Turton, P., Demetriou, A., & Wright, C. (2010). Perceptions, experiences and meanings of recovery in forensic psychiatric patients. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 21(5), 683-696. DOI:10.1080/14789949.2010.489953

Mind (2013). Physical Restraint in Crisis: A report on physical restraint in hospital settings in England. Retrieved from Mind Website: https://www.mind.org.uk/media/197120/physical_restraint_final_web_version.pdf

NICE. (2015). Violence and Aggression: short-term management in mental health, health and community settings. Retrieved from National Institute of Clinical Excellence Website: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng10

Papadopoulos, C., Ross, J., Stewart, D., Dack, C., James, K., & Bowers, L. (2012). The antecedents of violence and aggression within psychiatric in‐patient settings. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(6), 425-439. DOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01827.x

Porter, L.E., & Alison, L.J. (2004). Behavioral coherence in violent group activity: An interpersonal model of sexually violent and gang behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 449–468. DOI: 10.1002/ab.20047

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879-891. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

Queensland Government. (2008). Policy statement on reducing and where possible eliminating restraint and seclusion in Queensland mental health services. Retrieved from http://www.health.gld.gov.uk/mentalhealth/docs/sandrpolicy 081030.pdf.

55

Page 56: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Royal College of Nursing. (2016). Proactive and Positive Care: Reducing the need for Restrictive Interventions. London: RCN.

Shaw, J., Davies, J., & Morey, H. (2001). An assessment of the security, dependency and treatment needs of all patients in secure services in a UK health region. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 12(3), 610-637. DOI: 10.1080/09585180127380

Smith, S. T., Edens, J. F., & McDermott, B. E. (2013). Fearless dominance and self-centered impulsivity interact to predict predatory aggression among forensic psychiatric in patients. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 12(1), 33-41. DOI: 10.1080/14999013.2012.760186

Szmukler, G., & Appelbaum, P. S. (2008). Treatment pressures, leverage, coercion, and compulsion in mental health care. Journal of Mental Health, 17(3), 233-244. DOI: 10.1080/09638230802052203

Vernham, Z., Tapp, J., & Moore, E. (2016). Observer ratings of interpersonal behavior as predictors of aggression and self-harm in a high-security sample of male forensic inpatients. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31(9), 1597-1617. DOI: 10.1177/0886260515569060

Völlm, B., Edworthy, R., Holley, J., Talbot, E., Majid, S., Duggan, C., Weaver, T., & McDonald, R. (2017). A mixed-methods study exploring the characteristics and needs of long-stay patients in high and medium secure settings in England: implications for service organisation. Health Service and Delivery Research, 5(11). Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK424815/

Webster, G. D., DeWall, C. N., Pond, R. S., Jr., Deckman, T., Jonason, P. K., Le, B. M., & Bator, R. J. (2014). The Brief Aggression Questionnaire: Psychometric and behavioural evidence for an efficient measure of trait aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 120–139. DOI:10.1002/ab.21507

Wei, M., Shaffer, P. A., Young, S. K., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, shame, depression, and loneliness: The mediation role of basic psychological needs satisfaction. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(4), 591. DOI: 10.1037/0022- 0167.52.4.591

Whittington, R., & Richter, D. (2005). Interactional aspects of violent behaviour on acute psychiatric wards. Psychology, Crime & Law, 11(4), 377-388. DOI: 10.1080/10683160500255372

Winje, M., van Mastrigt, S., Gjestad, R., Urheim, R., & Rypdal, K. (2018). Staff members’ evaluation of inpatients’ motivation for aggression–the roles of staff restrictions and aggression severity. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 29(3), 419-433. DOI: 10.1080/14789949.2017.1410563

56

Page 57: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

World Health Organisation. (1992). ICD-10 Classifications of Mental and Behavioural Disorder. Geneva: World Health Organisation.

57

Page 58: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

MRP Empirical Paper Appendices

List of appendices to the empirical paper

Appendix A – Glossary of terms

Appendix B – Participant information sheet

Appendix C – Consent form

Appendix D – Ethics approval letter

Appendix E – SPSS Outputs: Descriptive statistics

Appendix F – SPSS Outputs: Internal consistency tests

Appendix G – SPSS Outputs: Tests of normality

Appendix H - SPSS Outputs: Correlation analyses for hypotheses 1-3

Appendix I – SPSS Outputs: Mediation analyses

58

Page 59: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendices to the empirical paper

Appendix A – Glossary of terms

Forensic psychiatric hospitals: secure facilities that treat and rehabilitate individuals who have come in conflict with the law and are deemed unfit to stand trial or not criminally responsible due to mental illness.

High-secure services: high secure services cater for service users who pose a grave and immediate danger.

Medium secure services: medium secure services cater for those service users who present serious danger to the public.

Index offence: Also known as an index crime which includes: murder and non-negligent homicide, rape, robbery, aggravated assault (or grievous bodily harm), burglary, larceny-theft, arson, and auto theft.

The Mental Health Act (1983): main piece of legislation that covers the assessment, treatment, and rights of people with a mental health disorder.

Mental Health Act Section: a case when someone can be detained under the Mental Health Act (1983) and treated without their agreement. People detained under the Mental Health Act and need urgent mental health treatment and are at risk of harm to themselves or others.

Assertive rehabilitation wards: wards where service users are likely to spend most of their time in secure care and is for the purposes of working towards a place of lesser security.

Admission wards: wards set up specifically for service users’ arrival in to hospital. There is a higher ratio of staff to service users on these wards. Service users spend their time being assessed on these wards and depending upon progress, will move to another acute treatment or rehabilitation wards.

High dependency wards: wards set up for service users who are at immediate risk higher level of staffing and physical security.

Hybrid wards: wards set up for service users who have dual diagnosis (personality disorder and mental health).

Risk management: involves the development of flexible strategies aimed at preventing any negative event from occurring or, if this is not possible, minimising the harm caused.

Anger: a strong emotion of annoyance, displeasure, or hostility.

Aggression: feelings of anger that result in hostile or violent behaviour (whether verbal or physical) or readiness to attack or confront.

Violence: behaviour involving physical force intended to hurt, damage, or kill someone or something.

Coercion: the action or practice of persuading someone to do something by using force or threats.

59

Page 60: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Coercive/restrictive practice (in mental health settings): including the short-term (e.g, seclusion and restraint) and long-term (e.g. limit setting, persuasion, interpersonal leverage) management of aggression and violence.

Manual restraint: direct contact with service users with an intention to prevent or restrict a person's movement. Used when a person poses an imminent risk of physical harm to self or others and it intended to be the least restrictive intervention that achieves safety.

Rapid tranquilisation: when medicine is given to a person who is very agitated or displaying aggressive behaviour to help quickly calm them.

Seclusion: a type of restraint that involves confining a person in a room from which the person cannot exit freely.

Long-term segregation: a situation where, in order to reduce risk of harm posed by service users to others (which is a continuous feature of their presentation), service users should not be allowed to mix freely with other service users on the ward or unit on a long-term basis.

Paradoxical effects: an effect of treatment which is opposite to the effect that would normally be expected.

Dominance: power or influence over another.

60

Page 61: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix B – Participant information sheet

[NHS Trust logo]

Participant information sheet(Version number: 4.0 25/05/2018)

How do service users’ interpersonal style and perceptions of the therapeutic environment impact upon rates of aggression and treatment readiness in secure-

care settings?

IntroductionOur names are [name of trainee] and [name of trainee] and we are Trainee Clinical Psychologists working in the NHS and studying at the University of Surrey. We would like to invite you to take part in a research project. Before you decide you need to understand why the research is being done and what it will involve for you. Please take the time to read the following information carefully. Talk to others about the study if you wish.

Why are we doing the research?We are interested in exploring how people staying in secure care settings view their relationships with staff and in turn, how this impact on rates of aggression and treatment motivation.

Why have I been invited?You have been invited to take part in this research as you are staying in either a medium or high secure hospital.

What will happen to me if I agree to take part? Before you decide to take part you will have the opportunity to ask any questions relating to the study. If you decide to take part we will ask you to sign a consent form, you will receive a copy of this and we will keep a copy in the research file. You will be provided with opportunity to have an Independent Mental Health Advocate or a member of staff present during your participation in the study.

During your participation you will be asked to complete five questionnaires, three will ask about relationship experiences, one will ask about the causes of aggression, and two will ask about treatment motivation. We expect that completion of the questionnaires will take approximately 45 minutes – 1 hour in total, in which you can take a break or discontinue if required. This will be a one-off occasion.

The research team will also ask for your permission to review your clinical notes as part of this research to obtain some additional information on 1) the number (if any) of untoward incidents that you have been involved in over the past month and 2) your engagement in treatment programmes over the past 12 months. We will also collect some basic demographic information (such as age, ethnicity and gender). In

61

Page 62: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

reviewing your clinical notes, we will only record information about the above things, nothing more. In addition, you will be paid to £5 for your participation in the research study. This money will be credited in to your hospital account after you have completed the questionnaires.

Do I have to participate?No, you do not have to participate. Your participation is voluntary, and you are free to withdraw at any time, without giving any reason, and without your legal rights or clinical care being affected.

Having said this, if you do decide to participate this will also not contribute to any further progression in your care. You have the right to withdraw your data from the project up to the point of data analysis. After this time frame, we may not be able to remove the data from the analysis process.

Who has reviewed the study?All research in the NHS is looked at by independent group of people, called a Research Ethics Committee, to protect your interests. This study has been reviewed and given favourable opinion by the [name of research ethics committee] (18/EE/0028).

Will my Participation be kept confidential?All information which is collected about you during the course of the research will be kept strictly confidential, hard copies of questionnaires and consent forms will be scanned and uploaded on to an encrypted USB stick and the paper copies will be kept in a locked filing cabinet at the hospital site in the office of the research supervisor. Any information about you will be removed (anonymised) and a unique code will be used so that you cannot be recognised from it.

We will let your clinical team know that you have agreed to take part in this research project, but we will not share details with them. However, if you tell us about any criminal offences or things that suggest there is a risk of harm to yourself or others during the interview, we will need to share these with your clinical team, and possibly with other authorities (such as the police). We will talk to you about this if this occurs, so you are aware of what we are sharing.

All project data related to the administration of the project, (e.g. consent form) will be held for at least 6 years and all research data for at least 10 years in accordance with University policy. Your personal data will be held and processed in the strictest confidence, and in accordance with current data protection regulations.

Who is organising and funding this research?This research is a part of our Clinical Psychology Doctorate training programme and is funded by the Psychology department at the University of Surrey.

What will happen to the results of this research?A written report will be prepared with the view to publishing the results. If you would like a copy of the published report please indicate this on the consent form and

62

Page 63: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

a copy will be sent to you. We will contact your primary nurse about sending this to you, which can be via the post or email. Please note that you will not be identified in any report or publication that arises as result of this study. We may use anonymise direct quotes in reports or publications. We will also be producing a summary of the results which we can send to you after the study is completed.

What if there is a problem?Any complaint or concern about any experience during the course of this will be addressed. Your primary nurse can contact [name of research supervisor], who is supervising this research. He will be able to contact the researchers, who will then arrange to come to the ward to discuss any concerns and answer any questions you may have about the research. If you have a complaint, he will also be able to contact the Programme Director at the University (contact details below). You would also be able to put your complaint into writing (with some help from your primary nurse or the advocacy service if needed) and your primary nurse will be given details of how and where to send this.

Who is handling my data?As researchers on behalf of the University of Surrey will collect information about you for this research study from you directly and your service user notes. We will not provide any identifying information about you to The University of Surrey. We will use this information as explained in the ‘What will happen to me if I agree to take part’ section above. This information will include age, ethnicity, gender, index offence, and length of stay and health information like MHA section which is regarded as a special category of information. We will use this information as explained in the ‘What will happen to me if I agree to take part’ section above.

What will happen to my data? As a publicly-funded organisation, we have to ensure when we use identifiable personal information from people who have agreed to take part in research, this data is processed fairly and lawfully and is done so on the basis of public interest This means that when you agree to take part in this research study, we will use your data in the ways needed to conduct and analyse the research study.

Your rights to access, change or move your information are limited, as we need to manage your information in specific ways in order for the research to be reliable and accurate. If you decide to withdraw your data from the study, we may not be able to do so. We will keep the information about you that we have already obtained. To safeguard your rights, we will use the minimum personally-identifiable information possible.

You can find out more about how we use your information https://www.surrey.ac.uk/information-management/data-protection and/or by contacting [email protected]

What if I want to complain about the way data is handled?

If you wish to raise a complaint on how we have handled your personal data, you can contact our Data Protection Officer [name of officer] who will investigate the matter.

63

Page 64: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

If you are not satisfied with our response or believe we are processing your personal data in a way that is not lawful you can complain to the Information Commissioner’s Office (ICO) (https://ico.org.uk/). For contact details of the University of Surrey’s Data Protection Officer please visit: https://www.surrey.ac.uk/information-management/data-protection

Further Information

Researchers: [contact details of trainee researchers]

Research supervisor:[contact details of research supervisor]

The University has in force the relevant insurance policies which apply to this study.  If you wish to complain, or have any concerns about any aspect of the way you have been treated during the course of this study then you should follow the instructions given above.

Alternative Contact details

[name of programme director]

Independent Mental Health Advocacy ServiceAddress: PO Box 14043, Birmingham, B6 9BLTelephone number: 0300 456 2370Email Address: [email protected] 

64

Page 65: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix C – Consent form

[NHS Trust logo here]

CONSENT FORM (Version number: 3.0 25/05/18)

Title of Study: Interpersonal style, attachment and perceptions of the secure care environment.

Name of Researcher: Name of Participant:

1. I confirm that I have read and understand the information sheet version number …………dated...................................... for the above study and have had the opportunity to ask questions.

2. I understand that my participation is voluntary and that I am free to withdraw at any time, without giving any reason, and without my medical care or legal rights being affected. I understand that should I can ask for information to be withdrawn at a point before October 2018. After this point, information cannot be erased as it will have been used in the project analysis.

3. I understand that anonymised data collected in the study may be looked at by authorised individuals from the research group where it is relevant to my taking part in this study.

4 I give permission for these individuals to have access to these records and to collect, store, analyse and publish information obtained from my participation in this study. This may include anonymised direct quotes.

5. I understand that my personal details will be kept confidential.

6. I understand that should I disclose any information that puts me or others at imminent risk, my clinical team and/or Responsible Clinician will be informed of this.

7. I agree to my notes being accessed by the research team.

8. I agree to take part in the above study.

9. I would like a summary of the study results.

10. I agree for my special category data (ethnicity) to be collected for the purposes stated in the information sheet.

__________________ ______________ ____________________Name of Participant Date Signature______________ ______________ ____________________Name of Person taking consent Date Signature

65

Please initial box

Page 66: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix D – Ethics approval letter

66

Page 67: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

67

Page 68: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

68

Page 69: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix E – SPSS Outputs: Descriptive statistics

DescriptivesStatistic Std. Error

DOM_mean Mean 4.59 .17995% Confidence Interval for Mean

Lower Bound

4.23

Upper Bound

4.95

5% Trimmed Mean 4.59Median 4.50Variance 2.236Std. Deviation 1.495Minimum 1Maximum 8Range 7Interquartile Range 2Skewness .147 .287Kurtosis -.558 .566

CD_Mean Mean 4.89 .20995% Confidence Interval for Mean

Lower Bound

4.48

Upper Bound

5.31

5% Trimmed Mean 4.90Median 4.88Variance 3.049Std. Deviation 1.746Minimum 1Maximum 8Range 7Interquartile Range 3Skewness -.092 .287Kurtosis -1.005 .566

BAQ_Mean Mean 3.33 .10195% Confidence Interval for Mean

Lower Bound

3.13

Upper Bound

3.53

5% Trimmed Mean 3.29

69

Page 70: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Median 3.33Variance .708Std. Deviation .842Minimum 2Maximum 6Range 4Interquartile Range 1Skewness .527 .287Kurtosis .261 .566

Autonomy_Mean

Mean 4.23 .10195% Confidence Interval for Mean

Lower Bound

4.03

Upper Bound

4.43

5% Trimmed Mean 4.21Median 4.14Variance .708Std. Deviation .841Minimum 2Maximum 6Range 4Interquartile Range 1Skewness .494 .287Kurtosis .401 .566

Appendix F – SPSS Outputs: Internal consistency tests

70

Page 71: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

ISC dominance sub-domain

Reliability Statistics

Cronbach's Al-

pha

Cronbach's Al-

pha Based on

Standardized

Items N of Items

.794 .795 8

Item-Total Statistics

Scale Mean if

Item Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Al-

pha if Item De-

leted

Dominant 32.16 116.830 .384 .235 .790

Dominant 31.44 109.149 .537 .406 .765

Dominant 31.30 113.865 .507 .334 .770

Dominant 32.43 112.133 .515 .394 .769

Dominant 33.23 112.295 .537 .322 .765

Dominant 31.33 111.122 .506 .289 .770

Dominant 32.09 107.500 .655 .474 .747

Dominant 33.03 119.275 .381 .285 .789

ISC cold dominance sub-domain

Reliability Statistics

Cronbach's Al-

pha

Cronbach's Al-

pha Based on

Standardized

Items N of Items

.870 .869 8

Item-Total Statistics

71

Page 72: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Scale Mean if

Item Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Al-

pha if Item De-

leted

Cold Dominant 34.29 149.251 .711 .576 .844

Cold Dominant 34.61 165.603 .424 .357 .873

Cold Dominant 33.64 150.320 .672 .578 .848

Cold Dominant 33.49 156.137 .545 .405 .862

Cold Dominant 35.06 146.576 .701 .547 .845

Cold Dominant 34.16 146.453 .740 .587 .841

Cold Dominant 34.36 153.334 .556 .382 .862

Cold Dominant 34.40 149.809 .648 .581 .851

BAQ (self-reported anger)

Reliability Statistics

Cronbach's Al-

pha

Cronbach's Al-

pha Based on

Standardized

Items N of Items

.685 .682 12

Item-Total Statistics

Scale Mean if

Item Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Al-

pha if Item De-

leted

Physical 35.10 94.700 .396 .395 .654

Physical 34.74 88.571 .526 .519 .630

Physical 34.54 89.991 .466 .566 .640

Anger 35.01 104.420 .153 .179 .690

Anger 35.89 98.103 .390 .576 .657

Anger 35.94 103.504 .251 .523 .676

Verbal 33.29 108.845 .032 .455 .706

Verbal 34.07 95.835 .347 .392 .662

Verbal 35.84 98.917 .367 .337 .660

Hostility 34.73 97.766 .296 .249 .670

Hostility 35.63 95.251 .386 .570 .655

Hostility 34.47 98.253 .246 .629 .679

72

Page 73: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

BPNSS (Autonomy sub-scale) Internal consistency

Reliability Statistics

Cronbach's Al-

pha

Cronbach's Al-

pha Based on

Standardized

Items N of Items

.661 .666 7

Item-Total Statistics

Scale Mean if

Item Deleted

Scale Variance if

Item Deleted

Corrected Item-

Total Correlation

Squared Multiple

Correlation

Cronbach's Al-

pha if Item De-

leted

Autonomy 26.46 41.585 .439 .257 .604

Autonomy 25.66 50.895 .166 .075 .686

Autonomy 25.43 49.582 .328 .315 .638

Autonomy 27.01 47.116 .366 .232 .627

Autonomy 25.99 43.464 .517 .476 .582

Autonomy 25.59 46.101 .453 .407 .604

Autonomy 26.01 45.840 .361 .279 .629

73

Page 74: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix G – SPSS Outputs: Tests of normality

Tests of NormalityKolmogorov-Smirnova Shapiro-Wilk

Statistic df Sig. Statistic df Sig.DOM_mean .070 70 .200* .982 70 .401CD_Mean .085 70 .200* .971 70 .098BAQ_Mean .088 70 .200* .963 70 .035Autonomy_Mean

.127 70 .007 .963 70 .036

*. This is a lower bound of the true significance.a. Lilliefors Significance Correction

DOM_mean

74

Page 75: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

CD_Mean

75

Page 76: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

BAQ_Mean

76

Page 77: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Autonomy_Mean

77

Page 78: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Tests of NormalityKolmogorov-Smirnova Shapiro-Wilk

Statistic df Sig. Statistic df Sig.total number of incid-ents on staff

.346 63 .000 .447 63 .000

a. Lilliefors Significance Correction

total number of incidents on staff

78

Page 79: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

79

Page 80: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix H - SPSS Outputs: Correlation analyses for hypotheses 1-3

Correlations

Correlations

DOM_mean CD_Mean BAQ_MeanAutonomy_Me

anDOM_mean Pearson Correlation 1 .778** .419** -.084

Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .000 .489N 70 70 70 70

CD_Mean Pearson Correlation .778** 1 .162 .035Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .180 .775N 70 70 70 70

BAQ_Mean Pearson Correlation .419** .162 1 -.036Sig. (2-tailed) .000 .180 .766N 70 70 70 70

Autonomy_Mean Pearson Correlation -.084 .035 -.036 1Sig. (2-tailed) .489 .775 .766N 70 70 70 70

**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

80

Page 81: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Nonparametric Correlations

Correlations

DOM_mean CD_Mean

total number of incidents on

staffAutonomy_Me

an BAQ_MeanSpearman's rho DOM_mean Correlation Coefficient 1.000 .773** -.046 -.054 .401**

Sig. (2-tailed) . .000 .718 .659 .001N 70 70 63 70 70

CD_Mean Correlation Coefficient .773** 1.000 -.110 .022 .188Sig. (2-tailed) .000 . .392 .857 .119N 70 70 63 70 70

total number of incidents on staff

Correlation Coefficient -.046 -.110 1.000 -.079 .256*

Sig. (2-tailed) .718 .392 . .537 .043N 63 63 63 63 63

Autonomy_Mean Correlation Coefficient -.054 .022 -.079 1.000 -.017Sig. (2-tailed) .659 .857 .537 . .890N 70 70 63 70 70

BAQ_Mean Correlation Coefficient .401** .188 .256* -.017 1.000Sig. (2-tailed) .001 .119 .043 .890 .N 70 70 63 70 70

**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

*. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)81

Page 82: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

82

Page 83: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Appendix I – SPSS Outputs: Mediation analyses

Hypothesis 4 a

Run MATRIX procedure:

**************** PROCESS Procedure for SPSS Version 3.1 ******************

Written by Andrew F. Hayes, Ph.D. www.afhayes.com Documentation available in Hayes (2018). www.guilford.com/p/hayes3

**************************************************************************Model : 4 Y : BAQ_Mean X : DOM_mean M : Autonomy

SampleSize: 70

**************************************************************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: Autonomy

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0841 .0071 .7130 .4843 1.0000 68.0000 .4888

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 4.4478 .3279 13.5633 .0000 3.7934 5.1021DOM_mean -.0473 .0680 -.6959 .4888 -.1830 .0884

83

Page 84: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

**************************************************************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: BAQ_Mean

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .4186 .1752 .6018 7.1156 2.0000 67.0000 .0016

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 2.2490 .5799 3.8781 .0002 1.0915 3.4066DOM_mean .2356 .0627 3.7583 .0004 .1105 .3607Autonomy -.0009 .1114 -.0085 .9932 -.2233 .2214

************************** TOTAL EFFECT MODEL ****************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: BAQ_Mean

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .4186 .1752 .5930 14.4434 1.0000 68.0000 .0003

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 2.2448 .2991 7.5065 .0000 1.6481 2.8416DOM_mean .2356 .0620 3.8005 .0003 .1119 .3593

************** TOTAL, DIRECT, AND INDIRECT EFFECTS OF X ON Y **************

Total effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI

84

Page 85: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

.2356 .0620 3.8005 .0003 .1119 .3593

Direct effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI .2356 .0627 3.7583 .0004 .1105 .3607

Indirect effect(s) of X on Y: Effect BootSE BootLLCI BootULCIAutonomy .0000 .0095 -.0168 .0242

*********************** ANALYSIS NOTES AND ERRORS ************************

Level of confidence for all confidence intervals in output: 95.0000

Number of bootstrap samples for percentile bootstrap confidence intervals: 10000

NOTE: Variables names longer than eight characters can produce incorrect output. Shorter variable names are recommended.

------ END MATRIX -----

Hypothesis 4b

85

Page 86: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Run MATRIX procedure:

**************** PROCESS Procedure for SPSS Version 3.1 ******************

Written by Andrew F. Hayes, Ph.D. www.afhayes.com Documentation available in Hayes (2018). www.guilford.com/p/hayes3

**************************************************************************Model : 4 Y : TAI_S X : DOM_mean M : Autonomy

SampleSize: 63

**************************************************************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: Autonomy

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .1128 .0127 .7394 .7855 1.0000 61.0000 .3790

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 4.5258 .3471 13.0400 .0000 3.8318 5.2198DOM_mean -.0633 .0714 -.8863 .3790 -.2060 .0795

**************************************************************************OUTCOME VARIABLE:

86

Page 87: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

TAI_S

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0909 .0083 155.8063 .2498 2.0000 60.0000 .7798

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 6.7764 9.8049 .6911 .4922 -12.8364 26.3892DOM_mean -.6923 1.0429 -.6638 .5094 -2.7783 1.3938Autonomy .3094 1.8586 .1665 .8683 -3.4083 4.0272

************************** TOTAL EFFECT MODEL ****************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: TAI_S

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0883 .0078 153.3229 .4796 1.0000 61.0000 .4913

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 8.1769 4.9977 1.6361 .1070 -1.8168 18.1705DOM_mean -.7118 1.0279 -.6925 .4913 -2.7673 1.3436

************** TOTAL, DIRECT, AND INDIRECT EFFECTS OF X ON Y **************

Total effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI -.7118 1.0279 -.6925 .4913 -2.7673 1.3436

Direct effect of X on Y87

Page 88: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Effect se t p LLCI ULCI -.6923 1.0429 -.6638 .5094 -2.7783 1.3938

Indirect effect(s) of X on Y: Effect BootSE BootLLCI BootULCIAutonomy -.0196 .1550 -.3159 .3523

*********************** ANALYSIS NOTES AND ERRORS ************************

Level of confidence for all confidence intervals in output: 95.0000

Number of bootstrap samples for percentile bootstrap confidence intervals: 10000

NOTE: Variables names longer than eight characters can produce incorrect output. Shorter variable names are recommended.

------ END MATRIX -----

Hypothesis 4c

88

Page 89: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Run MATRIX procedure:

**************** PROCESS Procedure for SPSS Version 3.1 ******************

Written by Andrew F. Hayes, Ph.D. www.afhayes.com Documentation available in Hayes (2018). www.guilford.com/p/hayes3

**************************************************************************Model : 4 Y : BAQ_Mean X : CD_Mean M : Autonomy

SampleSize: 70

**************************************************************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: Autonomy

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0349 .0012 .7172 .0827 1.0000 68.0000 .7745

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 4.1484 .3031 13.6877 .0000 3.5437 4.7532CD_Mean .0168 .0584 .2876 .7745 -.0997 .1333

**************************************************************************

89

Page 90: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

OUTCOME VARIABLE: BAQ_Mean

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .1673 .0280 .7092 .9644 2.0000 67.0000 .3865

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 3.1178 .5840 5.3384 .0000 1.9521 4.2836CD_Mean .0788 .0581 1.3560 .1797 -.0372 .1947Autonomy -.0419 .1206 -.3471 .7296 -.2826 .1988

************************** TOTAL EFFECT MODEL ****************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: BAQ_Mean

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .1620 .0262 .7000 1.8319 1.0000 68.0000 .1804

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 2.9442 .2994 9.8325 .0000 2.3467 3.5417CD_Mean .0781 .0577 1.3535 .1804 -.0370 .1932

************** TOTAL, DIRECT, AND INDIRECT EFFECTS OF X ON Y **************

Total effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI .0781 .0577 1.3535 .1804 -.0370 .1932

90

Page 91: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Direct effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI .0788 .0581 1.3560 .1797 -.0372 .1947

Indirect effect(s) of X on Y: Effect BootSE BootLLCI BootULCIAutonomy -.0007 .0086 -.0200 .0174

*********************** ANALYSIS NOTES AND ERRORS ************************

Level of confidence for all confidence intervals in output: 95.0000

Number of bootstrap samples for percentile bootstrap confidence intervals: 10000

NOTE: Variables names longer than eight characters can produce incorrect output. Shorter variable names are recommended.

------ END MATRIX -----

91

Page 92: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Hypotheses 4d

Run MATRIX procedure:

**************** PROCESS Procedure for SPSS Version 3.1 ******************

Written by Andrew F. Hayes, Ph.D. www.afhayes.com Documentation available in Hayes (2018). www.guilford.com/p/hayes3

**************************************************************************Model : 4 Y : TAI_S X : CD_Mean M : Autonomy

SampleSize: 63

**************************************************************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: Autonomy

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0375 .0014 .7479 .0857 1.0000 61.0000 .7707

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 4.1381 .3438 12.0378 .0000 3.4507 4.8255CD_Mean .0189 .0644 .2927 .7707 -.1100 .1477

**************************************************************************92

Page 93: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

OUTCOME VARIABLE: TAI_S

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0827 .0068 156.0297 .2065 2.0000 60.0000 .8140

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 5.6196 9.1225 .6160 .5402 -12.6281 23.8674CD_Mean -.5540 .9311 -.5950 .5541 -2.4164 1.3084Autonomy .4898 1.8493 .2648 .7921 -3.2095 4.1890

************************** TOTAL EFFECT MODEL ****************************OUTCOME VARIABLE: TAI_S

Model Summary R R-sq MSE F df1 df2 p .0753 .0057 153.6513 .3482 1.0000 61.0000 .5573

Model coeff se t p LLCI ULCIconstant 7.6463 4.9272 1.5518 .1259 -2.2064 17.4990CD_Mean -.5448 .9233 -.5900 .5573 -2.3910 1.3015

************** TOTAL, DIRECT, AND INDIRECT EFFECTS OF X ON Y **************

Total effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI -.5448 .9233 -.5900 .5573 -2.3910 1.3015

93

Page 94: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Direct effect of X on Y Effect se t p LLCI ULCI -.5540 .9311 -.5950 .5541 -2.4164 1.3084

Indirect effect(s) of X on Y: Effect BootSE BootLLCI BootULCIAutonomy .0092 .1200 -.1866 .3241

*********************** ANALYSIS NOTES AND ERRORS ************************

Level of confidence for all confidence intervals in output: 95.0000

Number of bootstrap samples for percentile bootstrap confidence intervals: 10000

NOTE: Variables names longer than eight characters can produce incorrect output. Shorter variable names are recommended.

------ END MATRIX -----

94

Page 95: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Part 2: Research – MRP Literature Review

Abstract

Background: Coercive practices have become integral to managing

violent/aggressive behaviour in secure forensic settings. In more recent years, the use

of coercive practice has come under scrutiny due to its paradoxical effects on

provoking further service user aggression and violence. In addition, coercive

practices can also be counter-therapeutic to the staff-service user relationship,

particularly the way in which service users relate and react to such practices used by

staff (also known as service users’ interpersonal style).

Aims: To investigate the extent to which coercive practices and service user

interpersonal style predict service user aggression in secure settings.

Methods: A literature review was conducted using the following databases:

CINAHL; Psychinfo; Psyarticles; P&BSC; Medline. Peer-reviewed journals were

reviewed. Search terms included: “forensic”, “interpersonal”, “coercion”, and

“aggression” were used.

Results: Nine studies were included in the review. All studies included in this

literature review indicated that either interpersonal style (particularly dominance and

coercion) or coercive measures (such as staff limit-setting) were predictors of service

user violence and aggression. Only one study explicitly focused on interpersonal

style, coercion, and aggression, and this was based in the context of service user

admissions.

Conclusion: There is little research that has explicitly looked at the relationship

between both service users’ interpersonal style and coercive practice as possible

predictors of further aggression/violence. Future research should seek to understand

the way in which service user’s make sense and react to coercive practice,

95

Page 96: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

particularly in relation to their interpersonal styles. This research may help staff to

find alternative ways in engaging with service users that reduces the use of coercive

practice whilst simultaneously promoting service users’ recovery and maintaining a

safe environment.

Key words: Forensic, interpersonal style, coercive measures, aggression.

Word count: 7822

1.0 Introduction

Despite a shortage of beds, service users admitted into acute mental health wards in

England have a longer length of stay compared to several other developed countries

(Care Quality Commission (CQC), 2017). According to the Care Quality

Commission (2017), there was an extreme variation in the length of stay on NHS

high dependency rehabilitation wards, ranging from 45 to 1,744 days in 2015/16,

with an average length of stay of 341 days. This growing attention is due to

increasing pressure for containment of costs within psychiatric care and an ongoing

focus on psychiatric care being delivered in the least restrictive settings possible

(Compton et al., 2006). The Centre for Mental Health UK (2011) noted forensic

beds are more expensive and treat a smaller number of service users compared to

other general psychiatric admission beds. For those residing in high and medium

secure forensic hospitals, concerns have been raised that service users are staying for

too long in too high levels of security (Shaw et al., 2001; Harty et al., 2004).

Extended length of stay in secure care does not only result in high economic burden

on these services (e.g. Vӧllm et al., 2017) but can also have a negative impact on

forensic service users’ quality of life (e.g. Shaw et al., 2001). There have been

several research studies over the past few decades that have investigated possible

96

Page 97: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

predictors/determinants of longer length of stay in both non-forensic and forensic

settings (e.g. Taylor et al., 1996; Compton et al., 2006; Davoren et al., 2015; Vӧllm

et al., 2016). A major factor found to predict length of stay was the number of

untoward incidents (mainly violence and aggression) that the service user had been

involved in whilst in the institutional settings (e.g. Castro et al., 2002; Mellesdal et

al., 2003; Tulloch et al., 2008; Vӧllm et al., 2016).

1.1 Violence, aggression, and coercive practice in forensic settings

A review of 424 international studies found that the prevalence of violent incidents in

forensic settings was significantly higher compared to general inpatient psychiatric

hospitals (Bowers et al., 2011). A recent survey of a large independent secure care

facility found that 2,137 incidents involving 56.4% service users were reported

(Dickens et al., 2013). In terms of high-secure settings, Uppal and McMurran (2009)

reported 3,565 violent incidents over a 16-month period in just under 400 service

users.

Coercive measures have become integral to managing violent/aggressive behaviour

by use of severe containment methods such as manual restraint, rapid

tranquillisation, seclusion, long-term segregation and withholding parole (NICE,

2016). The definition of coercion is typically seen as an ‘action or practice of

persuading someone to do something by using force or threats’ (Oxford Dictionaries,

2010). As highlighted in the literature, the term ‘coercive measures’ has multiple

definitions that can lead to confusion and difficulty for those who wish to examine it

(e.g. Jarrett et al., 2008). Although coercive measures are mostly thought of in

context of short-term management of aggression and violence (through seclusion and

97

Page 98: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

restraint), there are also more long-term coercive measures used (Hui et al., 2013).

As outlined by Szmukler and Appelbaum’s (2008) ‘sliding scale’, coercive measures

may take on various forms in forensic psychiatric settings including: persuasion (e.g.

attempts to influence service users actions by emotional reasoning); interpersonal

leverage (e.g. where the service user-professional relationship is used to put pressure

on the service user, such as pointing out disappointment in service user’s behaviour);

inducement (e.g. the use of positive rewards if action suggested by clinician is

accepted by the service user); threats (e.g. to lose particular benefits); compulsory

treatment (e.g. locked doors, bars on windows, choices taken away and treatment

administered against service users’ wishes).

In more recent years, the use of coercive practice has come under scrutiny, particu-

larly with more emphasis having been placed on personal autonomy and human

rights (Hui et al., 2013). Much of this current pressure has come about due to investi-

gations and scandals which have been widely reported (e.g. Blom-Cooper, 1992; Fal-

lon, 1999; Bubb, 2014). Coercive practice has even been found to have paradoxical

effects on provoking further aggression and violence (Daffern et al., 2003; Goren,

Singh, & Best, 1993). For example, Goren (1993) considered how high levels of se-

clusion and restraint used in child psychiatric hospitals to manage aggression and

non-compliance may have led to a pattern of behaviour characterised by an aggres-

sion-coercion cycle exhibited by both staff and service users. Several national and in-

ternational documents have called for review and reduction in restrictive practices in

general and secure-care psychiatric settings (e.g. American Psychiatric Association,

American Psychiatric Nurses Association & National Association of Psychiatric

Health Systems, 2003; Queensland Government, 2008; MIND, 2013; Department of

98

Page 99: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Health, 2014; NICE, 2015). Reducing coercive practice in forensic settings to sup-

port service user’s personal recovery may, however, be more problematic than in

general psychiatric settings. As highlighted in the Centre for Mental Health’s brief-

ing paper on “making recovery a reality in forensic settings”, there seems to be a

more complex balance between providing service users with autonomy and choice

over their own care when the reduction of service users’ risk towards themselves and

others is imperative whilst residing in secure-care services (Drennan and

Wooldridge, 2014). It is therefore recommended that staff and service users need to

work together to develop an organisational culture in which there is a balance

between safety and recovery (Drennan and Wooldridge, 2014).

1.2 Research on coercion in forensic settings

There is some empirical literature on coercive practice in forensic settings, but more

often, research is based in general psychiatric settings. In a systematic review

conducted by Hui and colleagues (2013), ten out of the fifteen studies included

conducted retrospective analyses of forensic hospital records to examine the use of

coercive measures in relation to various service user demographic indicators (such as

age, gender, and ethnicity), and clinical indicators (such as diagnosis and length of

admission). Findings suggested that younger service users tended to be secluded over

older service users (e.g. Ahmed & Lepnurm, 2001; Beck et al., 2008). In addition,

service users were likely to be secluded or restrained in their first two months of

admission and that after this period the use of such coercive measures were likely to

reduce (Beck et al., 2008). Findings related to gender, ethnicity and service user

diagnosis were equivocal in the review’s included studies. A smaller number of

qualitative studies included in the review conducted semi-structured interviews with

99

Page 100: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

staff and/or service users to explore attitudes, perceptions, and experiences of

coercive measures (e.g. Keski-Valkama et al. 2010; Sequiera & Halstead, 2004). In

one study, nursing staff reported feelings of anger and anxiety, low morale, and

conflict between the use of restraint and seclusion techniques and their role as a

nurse (Sequiera & Halstead, 2004). In another study, forensic service users were

likely to view seclusion negatively and as a form of punishment and suggested that

limitations in staff-service user interactions during seclusion often increased feelings

of anxiety and confusion (e.g. Keski-Valkama et al. 2010).

Some research studies have focused on situational/environmental determinants of

aggression in secure-care settings (e.g. Kelly et al. 2015). Coercive practice has not

been explicitly examined in this body of research but has instead, emerged from the

findings as a determinant of aggressive behaviour. For example, in a cross-sectional

survey of service user assaults against staff in a forensic psychiatric hospital, a

sample of three hundred and forty-eight staff participated in an online survey about

their workplace experiences, psychosocial characteristics and well-being (Kelly et

al., 2015). Ninety nine percent of staff reported experiencing conflict with service

users and eighty percent of staff reported using seclusion, restraint, or unscheduled

medication within the last six months. Frequency of service user assaults were

substantially correlated with the frequency of service user-staff conflict (r = 0=54)

concluding that ‘staff who regularly engage in […] in service user containment

procedures, have the highest likelihood of experiencing service user-on-staff assault’

(Kelly et al., 2015: p 1118).

100

Page 101: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

1.3 Coercion and interpersonal style

It has been suggested that aggressive behaviour may be a result of a complex

interaction between situational factors and individual factors (Anderson & Bushman,

2002). In psychiatric hospital settings, we may therefore need to consider that it is

not just the coercive practices (i.e. the implementation of hospital rules and

regulations (Alexander and Bowers, 2004)) that predicts rates of aggression, but it is

also how these situational factors interact with individual service user characteristics.

According to Kiesler’s (1987) interpersonal theory, when interacting with others (our

interpersonal behaviour), we are inherently predisposed to establish relationships that

reinforce our sense of self, and this is done through the reactions of others. Every

individual's interactions can be characterized by two main dimensions: affiliation

(hostile to friendly) and control (dominance to submission). Our interactions

complement the interactions of others (i.e. usually matching across the affiliation

dimension but opposing on the control dimension). For example, friendly

interpersonal styles are typically met with friendliness from others whereas dominant

interpersonal styles are typically met with submission from others. At times,

individual characteristics may lead to maladaptive behaviours in our interactions

with others where lower self-esteem may be linked to increased frustration that

manifests as an over reliance in becoming overly dominant and rarely submissive

(BjØrkvik et al., 2009). For example, when staff confiscate certain privileges off

service users who act out aggressively (i.e. ground leave taken away), some service

users may stop the aggressive behaviour (complementarity across the control

dimension) whereas others may react with further aggression (acomplementarity

across the control dimension).

101

Page 102: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

According to Cookson et al. (2012), service users with dominant and hostile

interpersonal styles are more likely to encounter problems with psychiatric inpatient

treatment. Service users’ attempts to maintain their dominance are more often met by

a dominant response from nursing staff, which in the context of coercive practice, is

an attempt to maintain the integrity of the treatment regime and to ensure safety of

service users and staff (Daffern et al., 2010a). Staff’s attempts to restore order may

be perceived by service users as threatening and exploitative, which thus leads to

preventative actions by service users to restore dominance (Lillie, 2007). Therefore,

in the context of some service user’s interpersonal styles, the use of coercive

practices may have a counter-therapeutic effect through escalating cycles of attempts

to secure dominance by staff and service users (Goren, 1993). This coincides with

previous research in psychiatric hospital settings which has suggested that

hostile/dominant/coercive personality styles, in particular, are strong predictors of

service user aggression and violence (e.g. Morrison, 1992; Dolan & Blackburn,

2006; Harris et al., 2014). This highlights that the implementation of coercive

practice, does not take place in a vacuum, but also concerns the way in which service

users make sense of and react to staffs’ use of such practices.

Previous research has indicated that the formation of a therapeutic relationship

between staff and service users is fraught with difficulties in forensic settings

compared to non-forensic settings (Mason et al., 2008). Furthermore, as highlighted

above, previous research has indicated that forensic settings not only have a

significantly higher prevalence of violent incidents (Bowers et al., 2011) but consists

of a more complex balance between providing both a therapeutic and a safe/secure

environment (e.g. Burrows, 1991; Drennan and Wooldridge, 2014). This echoes the

102

Page 103: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

works of Goffman (1968), where staff working in institutions find themselves

simultaneously trying to care whist enforcing strict regulations that threaten this

caring process. The complex interaction between individual and situational factors in

predicting aggressive behaviour may be further complicated within a forensic setting

where therapeutic efficacy may well be related to issues of maintaining control of a

population of service users who, more often than not, wish to disrupt a hated system

(Peternelj-Taylor & Johnson, 1995).  As such, it may be important to investigate the

current evidence base on how service users residing explicitly in secure forensic

settings make sense of coercive practices used by staff, particularly in context of

service users’ interpersonal styles and in turn, how these factors play a role in the

incidence of violence/aggression.

1.4 Literature survey aims

1.4.1 Research question

What is known from the existing literature about the extent to which service users’

interpersonal style and coercive practices predict incidents of aggression and

violence in secure forensic settings?

1.4.2 Research objectives

A common theme that arose from the current empirical evidence, is the paradoxical

effects of coercion on provoking further service user aggression and violence. As an

alternative to focusing on factors that are associated with the use of coercive practice

(e.g. Hui et al., 2013), this literature review aims to identify empirical evidence on:

1) the extent to which coercive practices predict outcomes of service user aggression

in secure forensic settings;

103

Page 104: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

2) the extent to which service users’ interpersonal style predicts outcomes of service

user aggression in secure forensic settings;

3) the extent to which both coercive practice and interpersonal style predict service

user aggression in secure forensic settings.

2.0 Methods

2.1 Identifying the relevant studies

Peer reviewed journal papers were identified via the relevant health and social care

databases available at Surrey University through a keyword search using terms

relating to coercive practice, service user interpersonal style and aggressive

behaviour in secure forensic settings. In total, five databases were searched:

CINAHL; Psychinfo; Psycharticles; P & BSC; Medline. These databases were

deemed most appropriate as they specifically included articles related to the subjects

of psychology and healthcare. These database searches were run in 01/11/2017 and

re-rerun on 26/01/19.

Searches for ‘grey literature’ were also conducted via online searches (Google

Scholar, Open Grey) and by liaising with authors of included papers. Searching of

key journals (i.e. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology) and examination of

retrieved articles’ reference and citation lists (including relevant literature reviews)

were also undertaken.

2.1.1 Search terms

104

Page 105: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

To search for studies focusing on service user interpersonal style, coercion (either

perceived coercion or coercive practice), and service user aggression in secure

forensic settings the following search terms were used:

Forensic AND (secure OR inpatient)

AND

Aggress* OR Violen*

AND

Interpersonal

OR

Coerci* OR Seclusion OR restraint OR Restrict* OR Constrain* OR force OR threat

Search fields: Set to default: searches in Abstract, Titles and Key Words. No

restrictions over year of publication.

2.1.2 Rationale for search terms

The main search term ‘forensic’ was used in all searches. As suggested by Hui and

colleagues (2013), although distinctions are made between low, medium, and high

secure services in the UK, there is less of a distinction in other countries. As such,

‘forensic’ (and ‘secure’ or ‘inpatient’) felt most appropriate to use as search terms.

Search terms ‘aggression’ or ‘violence’ were included as they were used

interchangeably in the literature and ‘interpersonal’ was used to cover all

eventualities related to interpersonal style. A related review that looked at coercive

measures within a forensic setting (Hui et al., 2013) only used search terms specific

to short-term coercive measures (e.g. coercion, restraint, seclusion, involuntary

105

Page 106: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

medication). In this review, it was also important to identify articles that focused on

long-term coercive measures as described by Szmukler and Applebaum’s (2008)

‘sliding scale’ (see section 1.1). It therefore felt appropriate to use more broader

terms which captured coercion in forensic settings. As such, an iterative approach

was adopted whereby keywords of relevant retrieved papers were looked at, and any

new relevant keywords were added to the search terms.  

2.2 Study selection

2.2.1 Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Inclusion and exclusion criteria were used to eliminate studies that did not answer the

research area of interest. Rather than adhering to a hierarchy of evidence approach,

based on methodology, empirical studies were included (quantitative and qualitative)

most likely to be relevant to the research area of interest (Aveyard, 2014).

Inclusion criteria:

a. Peer-reviewed empirical studies published in peer reviewed journals

addressing the areas in the key research questions;

b. Empirical studies based in forensic, secure-care settings;

c. Service user sample includes adults (18-65 years old);

d. Empirical studies that focus on either service users’ interpersonal style

and/or coercion (whether that be perceived coercion or coercive

/restrictive practices/measures);

e. Empirical studies that focus on service user violence and aggression

as an outcome measure;

106

Page 107: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

f. Conference papers addressing the areas in the key research questions

published in peer reviewed journals or as ‘grey literature’ conference

proceedings;

g. Both quantitative and qualitative research studies;

h. English language publications.

Exclusion criteria:

a. Duplications;

b. Studies that are not based in forensic psychiatric settings;

c. Studies concerning children, young people (0-18) and older age (65+);

d. Empirical studies that do not focus on either service users’

interpersonal style or coercion (whether that be perceived coercion or

coercive /restrictive practices/measures);

i. Empirical studies that do not focus on service user violence and

aggression as an outcome measure;

e. Commentary pieces; policy and guidance documents; material

published in ‘trade press’ and magazines; conference abstracts;

f. Papers and reports published in a language other than English.

2.2.2 Study screening process

Titles, abstracts, and key words of the retrieved studies were initially screened. Those

that did not meet the inclusion criteria were excluded. The full text of the remaining

articles were reviewed for eligibility. Those studies that did not meet the inclusion

criteria were excluded. A record was kept reporting the reasons why each study was

excluded (e.g. wrong setting). A PRISMA chart has been used to illustrate this

selection process (see Figure 1).

107

Page 108: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

2.3 Data extraction

Relevant data was extracted from each of the included studies following a similar

approach to Arksey and O’Malley (2005). By using a ‘data extraction table’ (see

Table 1), the following key information was recorded about each study:

Author(s), year of publication, study location

Setting

Study population

Methods (including study design, data collection, data analysis)

Key findings

2.4 Collating, summarizing and reporting the results

By referring to the data extraction table (see Table 1), an initial list of key findings

from each of the included studies were created. The review’s three objectives (see

section 1.4.2) were used to create a ‘thematic framework’ (Ritchie et al., 2003). This

framework was used to classify and organise data from the included studies into key

categories, each comprising of overarching categories subdivided into related sub-

categories. The findings sections of each of the nine original articles were re-checked

to ensure that extracts of data reported on the findings had not been missed thus

further refining the emergent categories and sub-categories within the thematic

framework. The emerging categories/sub-categories from the included studies were

presented to the research supervisor (with examples from the included studies data)

to assess credibility of the coding process. The supervisor read through the results

section of a selection of studies to cross-check whether the categories I had pulled

108

Page 109: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

from these papers were aligned with their own identified emerging categories/sub-

categories.

2.5 Quality assessment

The quality of the included studies was assessed using appropriate critical appraisal

tools for both quantitative and qualitative methodologies. Quantitative studies were

assessed using the STROBE’s (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies

in Epidemiology) for cohort studies (Von Elm et al., 2007). This checklist was

deemed the most appropriate checklist for the quantitative studies included in the

review as all were cohort study designs. Qualitative studies were assessed using the

critical appraisal skills programme (CASP) qualitative appraisal tool (CASP, 2013).

The checklists were used to assess whether each of the included studies met the

criteria with a rating scale of ‘yes’ (1 point), ‘partly’ (0.5 points), and ‘no’ (0 points).

Each study’s points were added up to give a total quality score which was out twenty

for the quantitative studies (total scores were deducted where quality criteria were

not applicable for the study) and out of eight for the one qualitative study.

3.0 Results

The database, grey literature and previous relevant literature review searches yielded

a total of 573 papers, reducing to 357 after duplicates were removed. 250 articles

were excluded as they did not meet inclusion criteria. A total of 107 full text copies

were obtained and 9 relevant papers were included in the final review (see Figure 1

for the selection process flow diagram).

109

Page 110: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Figure 1: PRISMA flow diagram of selection process

110

Titles and abstracts identified through

electronic database search (n = 566)

Screening

Included

Eligibility

Identification

Titles and abstracts identified through other

sources (i.e. grey literature, literature reviews, and

citation and reference lists of relevant articles)

(n = 7)

Duplicates removed(n = 216)

Records screened (titles and abstracts)(n = 357) Records excluded

(n = 250)

Full-text articles retrieved and assessed for eligibility

(n = 107)

Full-text articles excluded, with reasons

(n = 98)

Wrong setting (non-forensic): n=40

Did not look at interpersonal style or coercive/restrictive practice n= 31

Wrong outcome measures (did not look at aggression/violence) n=27

Studies included (n =9)

Page 111: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

3.1 Description of included studies

Study focus: All nine studies investigated how interpersonal style and/or coercive practices

may have influenced service users’ aggressive behaviour in secure care. Four of these studies

elicited data surrounding the impact of coercive practices on service user aggression (e.g.

limit setting by staff and service user’s responses to these limitations/measures of control)

(Harris & Varney, 1986; Bjøkly, 1999; Meehan et al., 2006; Winje et al., 2018). Four studies

investigated the relationship between service user interpersonal style and aggression (Daffern

et al., 2008; Doyle et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2013; Vernham et al., 2016). The remaining

study looked at the impact of both interpersonal style and perceived coercion on aggression

and self-harm (Daffern et al., 2010b).

Study design/methods: All studies drew on a quantitative methodology, except for Meehan

and colleagues (2006) who used a qualitative methodology. Seven out of the eight

quantitative studies used a range of standardised outcome measures assessing service users’

aggressive behaviour (e.g. Report Form for Aggressive Episodes (REFA); Aggressive

Incident Motivation Evaluation Scale (AIMES); Staff Observation Aggression Scale – Revised

(SOAS-R)) (Bjøkly, 1999; Doyle et al.,2006; Winje et al., 2018) symptom/diagnostic

characteristics (e.g. PCL-R; BPRS) (Daffern et al., 2010b; Smith et al., 2013; Doyle et al.,

2006); personality traits (e.g. TCI; PCIR) (Smith et al., 2013); interpersonal styles (e.g. IMI;

CIRCLE) (Daffern et al., 2008; Daffern et al., 2010b; Doyle et al., 2006; Vernham et al.,

2016); and perceived staff coercion (MAES) (Daffern et al., 2010b). Four out of the eight

quantitative studies retrospectively reviewed incidents of violent and aggressive behaviour in

a past time period (Daffern et al., 2008; Daffern et al., 2010b; Vernham et al., 2016; Winje et

al., 2018). Four studies adopted a prospective approach where incidents were recorded at

baseline and at specific incidents in future time points (Harris & Varney, 1986; Bjøkly, 1999;

111

Page 112: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Doyle et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2013). In three out of the eight quantitative studies, outcome

measures were completed by staff and service users (Bjøkly, 1999; Daffern et al., 2010b;

Doyle et al., 2006), in four quantitative studies only staff completed outcome measures on

service users (Daffern et al., 2008; Daffern et al., 2010b; Vernham et al., 2016; Winje et al.,

2018), and in one study, only service users completed outcome measures (Smith et al., 2013).

In one quantitative study, no outcome measures were used but service users and where

possible, staff and witnesses were interviewed about the time, cause and actual events that

had transpired from an assaultive incident (Harris & Varney, 1986). Meehan and colleague’s

(2006) qualitative studies conducted focus groups with service users.

Study settings: In terms of study setting, four out of the nine studies were conducted in a

high-secure forensic hospital (Harris & Varney, 1986; Daffern et al., 2010b; Meehan et al.,

2006; Vernham et al., 2016), two in medium-secure units (Daffern et al., 2008; Doyle et al.,

2006), and three studies conducted outside of the UK were set in ‘specialised secure units’

(Bjøkly, 1999; Smith et al., 2013; Winje et al., 2018). In addition, two out of the nine studies

conducted research in a specialist personality disorder unit (Daffern et al., 2010b; Daffern et

al., 2008).

Study participants: With regards to participant demographics, sample sizes ranged from

nineteen participants to two hundred and fifty-three participants. Five studies samples were

predominantly male (Bjøkly, 1999; Meehan et al., 2006; Doyle et al., 2006; Smith et al.,

2013; Winje et al., 2018) and the sample in the remaining four studies were solely male

(Harris & Varney, 1986; Daffern et al., 2010b; Daffern et al., 2008; Vernham et al., 2016).

The age range of participants in most studies were between 18-65 years old with the mean

age falling between 30-43.45 years old. For those studies that included details on ethnicity,

112

Page 113: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

most participants were White Caucasian (Doyle et al., 2005; Daffern et al., 2010b; Daffern et

al., 2008; Bjøkly, 1999; Smith et al., 2013; Vernham et al., 2016). Most participants in the

samples of six studies had a diagnosis of schizophrenia or other psychotic-related disorder

(e.g. psychosis) (Harris & Varney, 1986; Meehan et al., 2006; Doyle et al., 2006; Bjøkly,

1999; Vernham et al., 2016; Winje et al., 2018) whereas two studies’ samples only included

those with a diagnosis of personality disorder (due to the studies being set in Personality

Disorder units) (Daffern et al., 2010b; Daffern et al., 2008). See Table 1 for the review’s

extraction table.

113

Page 114: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Table 1: Data extraction table

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 1

Bjøkly, S. (1999)

Norway

To identify, classify and measure the occurrence of aggressive behaviour, as well as the frequency of events preceding such behaviour.

Setting: Special secure unit, Norway.

Participants: 19 service users who were treated during a 10-year period in the unit.

White, Native Norwegian; 6 females and 13 males; mean age of 30.5 years old; 13 paranoid schizophrenia, 4 paranoid PD, 3 substance abuse, 1 antisocial PD.

Average Length of stay in secure unit during 10-year study was 49.5 months; 6 were still in secure unit after 10-year period.

Design: 10-year prospective study using hospital records (cohort study)

Data collection: Incidents of aggressive behaviour recorded on the Report Form for Aggressive Episodes (REFA) by nursing staff.

After service users had calmed, they were asked for their motives for the aggressive behaviour.

A routine injury form (rating severity) was also completed as close in time to the incident.

Outcome measures/instruments:

The REFA- rating scales - measures aggressive behaviour towards other persons (Bjorkly, 1996). A list of 30 situations/interactions, grouped in seven main categories, to help determine the situations/interactions that precipitated the aggressive episode. The seven main categories are: Physical contact; Limit-setting; Problems of communication; Changes/readjustments; Persons; High risk contact; and Drugs/stimulants.

Data analysis: Descriptive statistics were used to report the data. No specific details on how this was done.

Rates of aggression: During ten-year study period, a total of 2021 aggressive episodes occurred (Mean=106 per service user, Range= 0–694, SD=179) (See Table 1). Twenty-three per cent of the aggressive episodes were physical assaults, 25% were physical threats and around half were verbal.

Precipitants of aggression: Sixty-four per cent of aggressive incidents were precipitated by situations that involved limit-setting. A relatively higher proportion of physical threats (73%) than verbal threats (63%) and physical assaults (58%) had limit-settings as precipitant of aggression. The relative proportion of physical contact that resulted in physical assaults (7%) was higher than in verbal threats (3%) and physical threats (2%).

Victims of aggression: Nursing staff were the main target of service user aggression. There were 229 incidents of service user-to-service user aggression out of a total of 2021 episodes (11%).

114

Page 115: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 2Daffern et al. (2008)

UK

To examine the impact of interpersonal style and psychopathy on treatment non-completion and aggressive behaviour.

Setting: Personality Disorder Unit (PDU), a 12-bed service for male service users with personality disorder. Participants: Service users ranged in age from 17 to 41 years old (M=26.82, SD = 6.68) and most were described as Caucasian (49; 96.1%). Their length of stay varied between 30 and 1515 days (M=411.27, SD = 326.15).

Design: Retrospective study using hospital records (cohort study)

Data collection: An electronic database of incident forms was reviewed in September 2006 to determine whether participants had been aggressive during their stay. Incidents categorised as either physical, verbal, or damage to property.

Outcome measures/instruments completed by staff:Psychopathy Checklist – Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991)-Two factor scores: Interpersonal and affective characteristics.

International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE; Loranger et al., 1994); The Impact Message Inventory (IMI, Kiesler, 1987) The IMI Items are grouped into one of 15 interpersonal style scales (six items per scale); a total scale score is the sum of the six items. This was completed by staff.

Data Analysis: Continuous data compared with independent t-tests using an initial significance level of p<0.1. Logistic regression used to check for possible confounding and effect modification.

Aggressive incidents: Twenty-nine (56.9%) service users were aggressive during their stay on the PDU and twenty-one were not. For those who were aggressive, the mean number of incidents was 3.66 (SD 2.57). Individuals ranged from one to nine aggressive incidents overall.

PCL-R- Total score ranged from 6 to 30 (M=18.09, SD = 5.93). The PCL-R Factor 1 score ranged from 1 to 17 (M=5.74, SD = 3.03). The PCL-R Factor 2 score ranged from 2 to 20 (M=10.80, SD = 3.71).

The relationship between psychopathy and interpersonal style and aggression: Service users who were aggressive during the time period scored higher on the Competitive and Dominant sub-scales of the IMI (0.48, p=0.055 and 0.47, p=0.08, respectively). However, PCL-R total scores showed no association between aggressive and non-aggressive service users.

Study 3 Setting: Rampton high- Design: Retrospective study (cohort study). Perceived coercion: The mean MPCS score in the current sample

115

Page 116: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Daffern et al. (2010b)

UK

To explore the impact of interpersonal style and perceptions of staff coercion on aggression and self-harm.

secure hospital; PD service.

Participants: 39 residents (DSPD n =15, PD n = 24) consented to participate in the study.

Mean age 37.95 years. Predominant ethnic group was White-British (n =32). Remaining seven service users were a mixture of White-Other (n = 3), Mixed-White and Black African (n =1), Mixed-White and Asian (n= 1), and Caribbean (n=1).

Data collection: Service users participated in a semi-structured interview using the MacArthur Admission Experience Survey: Short Form 1 (MAES: SF; Gardner et al., 1993).

Two independent members of staff familiar with the service user then completed CIRCLE ratings.

A register of incident forms on an electronic database was reviewed six months after initial interview. Incidents were coded by a research assistant.

Outcome measures: The chart of interpersonal reactions in closed living environments (CIRCLE; Blackburn & Renwick, 1996) – determined service users’ interpersonal style. The MacArthur Admission Experience Survey: Short Form 1 (MAES: SF; Gardner et al., 1993)- an interview that focuses on service users’ perceptions of coercion during hospital admission.

Data Analysis: The hypotheses addressed using descriptive statistics and tests of association (i.e. multiple regression, nonparametric statistics were used wherever continuous MPCS and incident data were included in the analyses).

was 3.03 (SD = 1.87; median =3.00; minimum =0; maximum= 5). Most service users felt they had little influence (n = 27 or 69.2%), control (n= 24 or 61.5%), choice (n=19 or 48.7%) or freedom (n= 25 or 64.1%) over their admission.

What is the relationship between interpersonal style, perceived coercion and subsequent aggression and self-harm?

96 incidents of aggression and 31 incidents of self-harm were recorded. Most aggressive incidents were categorised as verbal aggression (n =70, 72.92%), however, there were several physically aggressive incidents directed at others (n =23, 23.96%). Staff were predominantly the victims of aggressive acts (68.75% of aggressive incidents).

There was a correlation between a coercive interpersonal style and aggression approached statistical significance (p=0.002).

With regards to the overall regression model, coercive interpersonal style made the only statistically significant contribution to the prediction of self-harm and aggression (ß=0.63, F=22.79*). Perceived coercion did not (ß=0.15, F=0.76).

116

Page 117: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 4Doyle et al. (2006)

UK

To assess the predictive validity of brief assessment scales with forensic service users to test the hypotheses that anger regulation problems, interpersonal style, and disturbed mental state would be linked to increased violence risk in a forensic hospital during a hospital stay.

Setting: Three adult forensic mental health medium secure units.

Participants: 94 mentally disordered service users. Sample predominately male (91.5%, n = 86). Mean age of the sample was 38 years (SD=9.03 years; range= 21 to 63 years). 83% of the sample were white Caucasian, 16% (n = 15). The majority (90.4%, n =85) had a psychotic-related disorder. Four (4.3%) had a primary diagnosis of personality disorder. Had an index offence of homicide, 31.9% (n = 30) index offence of wounding or grievous bodily harm, and 10.6% (n = 10) had an index offence of a sexual nature.

Design: Prospective study using hospital records (cohort study)

Data collection: Baseline assessments were conducted with participants and their key workers interviewed. Subsequent institutional violence was recorded weekly up to 12 weeks post baseline.

Outcome measures: The Ward Anger Rating Scale (WARS) (Novaco & Renwick, 1998) rating verbal and physical behaviours relating to anger and aggression. And then rating the behaviours’ affective–behavioural attributes (completed by staff).The Novaco Anger Scale (NAS) (Novaco, 2003) - measures cognitive, arousal, and behavioural correlates of anger (completed by service user); Chart of Interpersonal Reactions in Closed Living Environments (CIRCLE) (Blackburn & Renwick, 1996)- assesses interpersonal styles based on ward observations.

Data analysis: Mean scores of the scales were compared to determine whether the violent group scored higher; Mann–Whitney u statistics used to test the significance of the differences; Spearman’s r coefficient used to compare associations between different scales, and between scales and frequency of violence.

Prevalence of violence: Only 5.3% (n = 5) of the sample were physically aggressive in the 12 weeks following baseline assessment, committing a total of 10 acts of physical violence.

The self-report NAS and subscales were only predictive of physical violence (r=.82*, p<0.05).

WARS scales, rated based on behaviour in the preceding week, had relatively high predictive validity for physical violence (r=.79*, p<0.05) and physical and verbal threats of violence (r=.86***, p<0.001).

Interpersonal style and violence: The CIRCLE scale mean scores revealed that violent participants were more likely to have a dominant (r= .69**, p<0.01), coercive (r=.83***, p<0.001), and hostile interpersonal style (r=.71**, p<0.01).

117

Page 118: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 5Harris & Varney (1986)

USA

To examine some of the temporal characteristics of physically assaultive behaviour over an extended period in a maximum-security psychiatric unit.

Setting: Maximum security psychiatric unit.

Participants: Forensic service users.

Over a five-year period of the study, only 45 out of 980 service users committed more than 4 assaults over the five-year period. Characteristics of assaulters were compared to 44 non-assaulters.

Characteristics of reported assaulters: All male; mean age 32.07 years old; 53% diagnosed as psychotic.

Design: Prospective study using hospital records (cohort study)

Data collection: Researchers collected data on assaults that occurred in the maximum-security unit between January 1975 to December 1984.

Incidents were classed as assaults if they involved service user- initiating intentional physical contact of a forceful nature.

Researchers examined ward reports of significant incidents daily. Service users, staff and witnesses interviewed about the time, cause and actual events that had transpired from an assaultive incident.

Outcome measures: Number of assaultive incidents between 1975-1984 taken from hospital records.

Analysis: Assaultive behaviour was analysed using descriptive statistics including types of assault (from actual to attempted assault on either staff or service user victims) and other assaultive characteristics (e.g. location of assault; reasons for assault; and details on the assaulters in comparison to an equal sample of non-assaulters).

Prevalence of assaultive behaviour: Attempted assaults on staff was the highest throughout the 10 years.

Key findings from the more detailed 5-year data set on assaults: The vast number of assaults in the ward occurred in the ward corridors (59%) whereas fewer occurred in the service users’ room (13%) (possibly because staff were discouraged from entering upset service users’ rooms).

Reasons for assaults: three categories which were: No reason, Staff/program reasons (e.g. “ordered to do something”, “staff refused a request”, “patient upset by rules”, “staff provocation”, and “other”), and service user reasons (“teased by another patient”, “crowded”, “voices or delusional orders”, “reaction to homosexual advances”, and “building tension”).

Service users were most likely to attribute the assaultive behaviour with being teased or bugged by other service users (20.5%), being provoked by staff (12%), that there was no reason (15.4%) or that they gave no reason/no comment (23.8%). Ward staff were most likely to attribute assaultive behaviour to: No reason (24.6%), unknown reasons (58.6%) followed by the fact that service users had been ordered to do something (16.8%).

Lowest agreement on reasons for assaults were most apparent in which staff were the victims (p<0.001).

118

Page 119: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 6Meehan et al. (2006)

Australia

To elicit service user perceptions of 1) the factors leading to aggressive behaviour; and 2) strategies to reduce the risk of such behaviour.

Setting: High Secure Forensic Unit (HSFU) situated on the campus of a large tertiary mental health facility.

Participants: 22 male and 5 female clients. Eighty-five per cent had a diagnosis of schizophrenia and 73% were found not guilty of their crime due to insanity.

Those service users who had spent less than 3 months in either unit and those who were experiencing an exacerbation of their illness at the time of the study were excluded.

Design: Qualitative research

Data collection: Focus groups were conducted with to elicit perceptions of (1) factors that lead to aggressive behaviour; and (2) strategies that can reduce the risk of such behaviour.

Five discussion groups (ranging from 35 to 75 min) with between four and seven participants were conducted.

Data analysis: Content analysis (Morse & Field 1996) was used to guide analysis of the transcripts.

Environment- The lack of personal space was raised frequently and unfavourably. The impact of living in a confined area for long periods of time could lead to aggressive behaviour.

Staff interactions- the way staff interacted with service users was a major source of dissatisfaction. Staff were perceived to adopt superior attitudes and controlling behaviours to enforce a strict sense of authority rather than to care for service users.

Ward procedures -Staff were portrayed as ignoring frequent requests for assistance as ‘you’ve got to beg these people 26 [swear word] times before you get anything’. The inflexible and strict way in which staff enforced ward ‘rules’ was also perceived to cause aggressive behaviour. Frustration was expressed by service users when staff appeared to enforce rules and withdraw service users’ privileges for no real reason.

119

Page 120: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 7Smith, Edens & McDermott (2013)

USA

To examine the psychopathic personality inventory subscales’ (fearless dominance (FD) and self-centred impulsivity (SCI) prospective relationship with predatory, impulsive and psychosis-motivated aggression in an inpatient forensic psychiatric facility.

Setting: Forensic hospital

Participants: 200 forensic service users consisting of: 171 men and 29 women; 65% White, 20.5% Black, 7.5 % Hispanic, 7% other; mean age of 43.45 years old (SD-9.83); 67.5% had a diagnosis of schizophrenia spectrum disorder and 26.5% had diagnosis of Antisocial Personality disorder.

Design: Prospective follow-up study with a review of hospital records (cohort study).

Data Collection: Service users voluntarily enrolled in the study. They completed various outcome measures (see below). Aggressive incidents were tracked on each participant prospectively for an average of 4.39 years (SD- 0.99) after initial enrolment. Reports were completed by nurses following an aggressive incident. Researchers screened reports and categorised the aggressive behaviour to either: predatory, impulsive, psychotic, and unclear.

Outcome measures used:

The Psychopathic Personality Inventory Revised (PPIR; Lilienfeld & Widows, 2005) and Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCLR; Hare, 2003) were used to measure aspects of the participants’ personality that were thought to be linked to aggressive behaviour.

Data analysis: Bivariate relationships examined between criterion measures and FD and SCI, as well as for the PPI-R total score for comparative purposes. Hierarchical logistical regression tests used to examine moderator effects between sub-scales of the PPI-R and the PCL-R.

Incidents of aggression: 36.5% of participants (n = 73) engaged in at least one documented incident of physical aggression during the study’s prospective follow-up period. Only 9.5% of the total sample committed at least one predatory act of aggression, 24.5% committed at least one impulsive aggressive act, and 9% acted aggressively at least once due to psychotic symptoms.

Key findings: Fearless dominance was marginally related to predatory aggression, AUC = .60, SE = .08, p = .07 (one-tailed), 95% CI [.45, .75] but unrelated to impulsive aggression, psychotic aggression and any aggression (AUCs ranging from .42 to .50, SEs from .04 to .07, all p values = ns).

Study 8 Setting: Male forensic inpatient unit in a high-

Design: Retrospective quasi-experimental and Aggressive incidents: Verbal (n=53) and physical aggression (n=41) were the most commonly reported incidents within 12

120

Page 121: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Vernham et al., (2016)

UK

To investigate the predictive validity of the Chart of Interpersonal Reactions in Closed Living Environments (CIRCLE) for incidents of verbal and physical aggression, and self-harm recorded from 204 high-secure forensic service users. 

secure hospital.

Participants: 262 male service users with CIRCLE ratings (58 were excluded because they only had one rating completed). 204 male service users therefore included in the final sample.

Mean age 41.53 years (SD=10.03). Majority were White British (60.8%), and most had an index offence of homicide (62.7% or sexual offence 13.2%). Most prevalent diagnoses were paranoid schizophrenia or delusional disorder (48.0%) and personality disorder (antisocial and borderline 28.5%). Shortest length of stay (below 53.26 months); and longest (above 53.26months).

correlational study design. (cohort study)Procedure: Sample identified from the Centralised Groupwork Service (CGS) based at UK high secure hospital. IR1 used to identify incidents of verbal and physical aggression and self-harm for the study sample between 2004 and 2009. Details collated: date and location, incident category.

Measures: CIRCLE (the chart of interpersonal interactions) (Blackburn and Glasgow, 2006). Data analysis: Descriptive statistics used to collate info on demographics, clinical diagnosis and forensic characteristics. Means comparisons between service users with and without reported incidents of verbal aggression, physical aggression, and self-harm were conducted for dominant, hostile, and coercive scales of the CIRCLE, as directed by the hypotheses. Correlations were conducted between the CIRCLE total scores on the dominant, hostile, and coercive scales and the total frequency of incidents. All statistical analyses were conducted on incident data collected at 12, 24, and 48 months to investigate whether the predictive validity of the CIRCLE scales changed over time.

months.

Significant positive associations between the dominance and coercion scores and the frequency of verbal aggression (DOM: r=.234, p<.001; COER: r= .289, p<.001), and physical aggression (DOM: r=.192, p=.003; COER: r= .245, p<.001). No association found between aggression and violence with hostility.

All associations were consistent at 24 months, whereas at 48 months a further significant association was found between hostility scores and the frequency of verbal aggression (p=.050) and physical aggression (p=.034).

121

Page 122: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Author/Date/ Country/ Aims

Setting/ study populations

Methods (including relevant outcome measures) Key findings

Study 9Winje et al. (2018)

Norway

This study investigates staff members’ evaluation of motivation for aggressive incidents, and how such evaluations are influenced by staff restrictions and aggression severity.

Setting: Secure psychiatric unit. One high and one medium security ward.

Participants: Convenience sample used. 108 service users were admitted in the two wards. A total of 67 service users were included in the study.

57 were male (85.1%). Mean age of service users at the first registered aggressive incident was 32 years (SD = 9.63, range 15–62). Mean length of stay for service users was 22.42 months (SD = 3.29, range 0–120). Most common diagnosis was schizophrenia (59.7%).

Design: A cross sectional study.

Procedure: As soon as aggressive incident occurred, one staff member involved in or witnessing the incident was asked to fill out a report form. This data was collected over the period 2006–2015 of all incident reports with information about aggression motivation.

Measures: The Aggressive Incident Motivation Evaluation Scale (AIMES) (Urheim et al., 2014) - assessed motives and mental/emotional states associated with each motivational dimension.The Staff Observation Aggression Scale – Revised (SOAS-R) (Nijman et al., 1999; Palmstierna & Wistedt, 1987) – used to collect information about provocation.The Visual Analog Scale (VAS) - used to assess staff members’ rating of aggression severity (Nijman et al., 1999).Data Analysis: Report forms were registered as individual episodes (according to Participant study ID). Descriptive statistics and internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s α) tests ran. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with corrected standard errors was specified to confirm the three-factor structure of the AIMES. Multilevel modelling with random intercepts and cross-level interaction used to predict outcome variables.

Key findings: On average service users were involved in 39 (SD = 58) aggressive incidents, with a range from 1 to 292. Approximately 40% of service users had ten or fewer aggressive incidents. In total, 1900 incidents were reported with provocation scored as ‘no observable provocation’ (53.2%) or ‘staff placing restriction on service users’ behaviour’ (46.8%) in the SOAS-R.

Mean rating of aggression motivation- Irritable motivation (47.1% of incidents) instrumental motivation (39.9% of incidents) and defensive motivation (7.5% of incidents).

Staff members placing restrictions on service users’ behaviour was associated with higher levels of irritable (ß=.45, p<.001) and instrumental motivation (ß=.31, p<0.01). Service users with more incidents provoked by staff restrictions were rated higher on irritable (ß=.72, p<.001) and instrumental motivation (ß=.72, p<.001) across aggressive incidents.

122

Page 123: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

3.2 Overview of studies key findings

Overview of service user aggression and violence: Incidents reported in all nine

studies included verbal aggression, physical threats, physical assaults/violence. In

three of the studies, more than half of the incidents recorded were verbal aggression

or physical threats (Bjøkly, 1999; Daffern et al., 2010b; Doyle et al., 2006).

Furthermore, in one study sample, 36.5% (n=73) of participants engaged in at least

one incident of physical aggression (Smith et al., 2013). Although Winje and

colleagues (2018) did not report the type of aggressive incident, they did report

whether the aggressive behaviour was motivated by either irritable (47.1% of

incidents), instrumental (39.9% of incidents), or defensive (7.5% of incidents)

means. In four studies, staff (particularly nursing staff) were found to most often be

the victims of service user aggression (Harris & Varney, 1986; Bjøkly, 1999; Daffern

et al., 2010b; Meehan et al., 2006).

Coercive practice and aggression: In relation to coercion, limit setting by staff was

one of the most commonly reported precipitants of service user aggression (Harris &

Verney, 1986; Bjøkly, 1999; Daffern et al., 2008; Meehan et al., 2006; Winje et al.,

2018). For example, Bjøkly (1999) found that 64% of reported incidents of

aggression were preceded by staff limit setting. Staff limit-setting was defined in

several ways which included, for example, demands for activities/ordering service

users to do something, denial or ignoring service user requests, staff provocation,

inflexibility of rules put in place by staff. Furthermore, Winje and colleagues (2018)

found that, out of 1900 incidents recorded, 46.8% were reported to be provoked by

staff placing restrictions on the service users’ behaviour on the SOAS-R measure. In

123

Page 124: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

the one qualitative study, service user participants described how staff’s limit-setting

was, at times, demeaning (Meehan et al., 2006).

One study reported service users’ interactions with staff as being a major source of

dissatisfaction and a factor that often leads to service users’ aggression (Meehan et

al., 2006). In Meehan and colleagues’ (2006) qualitative study, service users

described staff as adopting a superior attitude and controlling behaviour.

Furthermore, service users described how staff lack of understanding and non-caring

attitude often led service users to retaliate in an aggressive manner.

Interpersonal style and aggression: Four studies reported a correlation between

service user interpersonal styles and aggressive behaviour (Daffern et al., 2010b;

Doyle et al., 2006; Vernham et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2013). Daffern and colleagues

(2010b) found that a coercive interpersonal style correlated with aggression, this

approached a statistical significance (p= .002). Another study found that violent

service users were more likely to have dominant, coercive and hostile interpersonal

styles (Doyle et al., 2006). Vernham and colleagues (2016) also found a significant

relationship between dominant and coercive interpersonal styles with verbal and

physical aggression (dominant and verbal, p=<.001; dominant and physical, p=.003;

coercive and verbal, p=<.001; coercive and physical, p=<.001), but unlike Doyle and

colleagues, they found that hostile interpersonal styles did not significantly predict

either verbal or physical aggression. Contrary to the significant relationship between

dominant interpersonal style and aggression, Smith and colleagues (2013) found that

fearless dominance was marginally related to predatory aggression (p=0.07) and

unrelated to impulsive aggression and psychotic aggression.

124

Page 125: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

With regards to moderator effects, in Daffern and colleagues (2010b) study neither

hostility nor dominance were independently related to aggression whereas an

elevation in both interpersonal styles was found to increase likelihood of service user

aggression. Similarly, Smith and colleagues, found significant interactive effects

between high levels of fearless dominance and self-centred impulsivity in increasing

the likelihood of predatory aggression (p=<0.05) whereas high levels of one of either

characteristic independent of the other did not increase likelihood of predatory

aggression. Furthermore, Smith and colleagues (2013) found no significant

interactive effects between fearless dominance and self-centred impulsivity in forms

of impulsive or psychotic aggression. Nearly three times more of the included sample

in Smith and colleagues (2013) study took part in impulsive aggressive incidents

compared to predatory aggressive incidents which, in the context of this study’s

findings, suggests that rates of aggressive incidents in forensic settings may not be

related to those who display more dominant interpersonal behaviours.

Doyle and colleagues (2006) findings indicated that compliant interpersonal styles

were a protective factor towards aggressive behaviour. Furthermore, the strongest

inverse relationship was found between dominance and submission (where those

who have dominant interpersonal characteristics are least likely to show submissive

interpersonal characteristics) (Doyle et al., 2006).

With regards to longitudinal follow up, Vernham and colleague’s study (2016), there

was a consistent (but not an increased) association between dominant and coercive

interpersonal styles and aggression from when service users were first admitted into

125

Page 126: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

the unit, and at 12 and 24 months follow up. There was, however, a significant

positive association between hostile scorings with recorded incidents of verbal and

physical incidents recorded at 48 months follow up (verbal, p=0.05; physical,

p=.034). Furthermore, although not formally assessed, this study found that

predictive validity of interpersonal styles was only applicable to service users who

had been recently admitted who were more likely to see hospital processes as more

coercive compared to those that had been residing in hospital for longer.

Interpersonal style, coercion and aggression: Only one study investigated the

relationship between interpersonal style, perceived coercion and incidents of

aggression during admission (Daffern et al., 2010b). The study’s findings concluded

that there was no statistical significance in the relationship between perceived

coercion and service user interpersonal style. And although coercive interpersonal

style was correlated to aggression, service users’ perceptions of coercion during the

admission process was not related to likeliness of aggression. For example, service

users’ perceptions of the control and restrictions put in place when first admitted into

hospital did not correlate to future aggressive behaviour. There was no attempt to

analyse how interpersonal style interacted with perceived coercion to impact on

aggression and self-harm due to the non-significance in the above findings.

3.3 Quality assessment results of the included studies

Following from the methodological quality assessment, this section will summarise

the main strengths and limitations of the review’s included studies.

With regards to total quality scores, the highest scoring study met 82.5% of

quality criteria (Vernham et al., 2016), four studies met between 60-70% of quality

126

Page 127: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

criteria (BjØkly, 1999; Doyle & Dolan, 2006; Smith et al., 2013; Winje et al., 2018),

and the remaining four studies scored 50% or lower (Meehan et al., 2006; Daffern et

al., 2008; Daffern et al., 2010b; Harris & Varney, 1986). All nine studies provided in

full or in part, an appropriate/relevant scientific background. In addition, all studies

provided clear study objectives, whereas only two out of the eight quantitative

studies provided study hypotheses (Smith et al., 2013; Vernham et al., 2016).

Two out of the nine studies provided a rationale for the study design (Smith

et al., 2013; Vernham et al., 2016). All studies provided in full or in part, a

description of the study setting. Only one study provided a clear, full description of

the study sampling criteria and recruitment strategy (Smith et al., 2016) and

furthermore, only three out of the eight quantitative studies provided full details on

how study sample size was calculated (BjØkly, 1999; Vernham et al., 2016; Winje et

al., 2018). All, aside one of the eight quantitative studies (Harris & Varney, 1986),

either in full or partly provided details on the methods of assessment/outcome

measures. Only one quantitative study (BjØkly, 1999) did not include any details on

the statistical methods used to analyse the data. The remaining eight quantitative

studies only partly fulfilled the criteria for methods of statistical analysis as they did

not include any details on, for example, sensitivity analyses. The one qualitative

study (Meehan et al., 2006) provided a clear description on data collection and data

analysis (e.g. how focus groups were conducted and how content analysis was used

to analyse the data). This study did not, however, provide detail on researcher

reflexivity (how researcher’s own position impacted upon the collection and analysis

of the data).

All eight quantitative studies only partly met criteria for descriptive data; this

was because, for example, there was no information provided on missing data. Only

127

Page 128: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

two quantitative studies provided full details of the main results (e.g. including

adjusted estimates for confounding variables) (BjØkly, 1999; Doyle & Dolan, 2006).

The one qualitative study provided a clear description of the five identified themes

(Meehan et al., 2006).

All studies either fully or partly discussed key results in the context of study

objectives, previous research evidence, and study limitations. Those who partly met

these criteria were missing information on, for example, clinical implications (e.g.

Harris & Varney, 1986) or provided no discussion on how confounding variables

may have impacted upon the study findings (e.g. Daffern et al., 2008).

128

Page 129: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Table 2: Quality assessment table for quantitative studies

Criteria BjØkly (1999)

Daffern et al. (2008)

Daffern et al. (2010b)

Doyle et al. (2006)

Harris & Varney (1986)

Smith et al. (2013)

Vernham et al. (2016)

Winje et al. (2018)

Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N

Titl

e an

d ab

stra

ct

(i) Was the study’s design a commonly used term in the title or the abstract?

Yes No Partly Partly Yes Partly Partly No

(ii) Did the abstract provide an informative and balanced summary of what was done and what was found?

Yes Yes Partly No Partly Yes Yes Yes

Intr

oduc

tion

i) Background/rationale: Was an appropriate/relevant scientific background and rationale for the investigation reported?

Partly Yes Yes Partly Partly Yes Yes Yes

ii) Objectives: Were specific objectives stated and prespecified hypotheses specified?

Partly Partly Yes Partly Partly Partly Yes Partly

Met

hods

i) Study design: Were key elements of study design presented early in the paper?

No No No No No Yes Yes Partly

ii) Setting: Was there a clear description of the setting, locations, and relevant dates, including periods of recruitment, exposure, follow-up, and data collection?

Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Yes Yes

129

Page 130: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Criteria BjØkly (1999)

Daffern et al. (2008)

Daffern et al. (2010b)

Doyle et al. (2006)

Harris & Varney (1986)

Smith et al. (2013)

Vernham et al. (2016)

Winje et al. (2018)

Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/Niii) Participants: (a) Was eligibility criteria, and the sources and methods of selection of participants provided? Were methods of follow-up described? (b) For matched studies, was matching criteria and number of exposed and unexposed given?

n/a Partly Partly Partly No Yes Partly Partly

iv) Variables: Were outcomes (DV), exposure (IV), predictors (experimental/manipulated variable), potential confounders, and effect modifiers clearly described?

Partly No No Yes Partly Yes Yes Yes

v) Data sources/ measurement: For each variable of interest, were sources of data and details of methods of assessment (measurement) given? Was comparability of assessment methods if there is more than one group described?

Partly Partly Partly Yes No Partly Yes Partly

vi) Bias: Were any efforts to address potential sources of bias provided?

No No No Partly No Partly Yes Partly

130

Page 131: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Criteria BjØkly (1999)

Daffern et al. (2008)

Daffern et al. (2010b)

Doyle et al. (2006)

Harris & Varney (1986)

Smith et al. (2013)

Vernham et al. (2016)

Winje et al. (2018)

Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/Nvii) Study size: Was there an explanation on how the study size was arrived at?

Yes n/a No Partly n/a No Yes Yes

Viii) Statistical methods: (a) Were all statistical methods, including those used to control for confounding described? (b) Was there a description of the methods used to examine subgroups and interactions? (c) Was there an explanation for any missing data? (d) If applicable, was there an explanation as to how loss to follow-up was addressed? (e) Were sensitivity analyses included and described?

No Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly

Res

ults

i) Participants: (a) Were numbers of individuals reported at each stage of study? —eg numbers potentially eligible, examined for eligibility, confirmed eligible, included in the study, completing follow-up, and analysed (b) Were reasons given for non-participation at each stage? (c) Was a flow diagram used?

No No Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly

131

Page 132: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Criteria BjØkly (1999)

Daffern et al. (2008)

Daffern et al. (2010b)

Doyle et al. (2006)

Harris & Varney (1986)

Smith et al. (2013)

Vernham et al. (2016)

Winje et al. (2018)

Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/Nii) Descriptive data: (a) Were characteristics of study participants (eg demographic, clinical, social) and information on exposures and potential confounders given? (b) Were number of participants with missing data for each variable of interest indicated? (c) Was follow-up time (eg, average and total amount) summarised?

Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly Partly

iii) Outcome data: Were numbers of outcome events or summary measures over time reported?

Yes No n/a n/a Yes Partly Partly Yes

iv) Main results: (a) Were unadjusted estimates and, if applicable, confounder-adjusted estimates and their precision (eg, 95% confidence interval) given? Was it clear which confounders were adjusted for and why they were included? (b) Were category boundaries when continuous variables were categorized reported? (c) If relevant, was translating estimates of relative risk into absolute risk for a meaningful time period considered?

Yes No No Yes No Partly Partly Partly

132

Page 133: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Criteria BjØkly (1999)

Daffern et al. (2008)

Daffern et al. (2010b)

Doyle et al. (2006)

Harris & Varney (1986)

Smith et al. (2013)

Vernham et al. (2016)

Winje et al. (2018)

Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N Y/P/N

Dis

cuss

ion

i) Key results: Were key results with reference to study objectives summarised?

Yes Yes Yes Yes Partly Yes Yes Yes

ii) Limitations: Were limitations of the study considering sources of potential bias or imprecision discussed? Were both direction and magnitude of any potential bias discussed?

Partly Yes Partly Yes Partly Yes Yes Yes

iii) Interpretation: Was a cautious overall interpretation of results considering objectives, limitations, multiplicity of analyses, results from similar studies, and other relevant evidence given?

Yes Yes Yes Yes Partly Yes Yes Yes

iv) Generalisability: Was the generalisability (external validity) of the study results given?

Partly Partly Partly Yes No Yes Yes Yes

Total score 11/19(60.5%)

8.5/19(44.7%)

9/19(47.4%)

12/19(63.2%)

7.5/19(39.5%)

14/20 (70%)

16.5/20(82.5%)

11.5/19 (60.5%)

Note: Y= yes, met criteria; P= partly met criteria; N= no, not met criteria.

133

Page 134: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Table 3: Quality assessment table for the qualitative study

Criteria Meehan et al. (2006)

Y/P/N1) Was there a clear statement of the aims of the research?

Partly

2) Is the qualitative methodology appropriate? Yes

3) Was the research design appropriate to address the research aims?

Partly

4) Was the recruitment strategy appropriate to the aims of the research?

Partly

5) Was the data collected in a way that addressed the research issue?

Partly

6) Has the relationship between researcher and participants been adequately considered?

No

7) Was the data sufficiently rigorous? Partly

8) Is there a clear statement of findings? Partly

Total quality score 4/8 (50%)

Note: Y= yes, met criteria; P= partly met criteria; N= no, not met criteria

134

Page 135: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

4.0 Discussion

In this section, the main findings of the included studies will be discussed in the

context of current theory, practice and research whilst presenting potential areas of

focus or specific questions for future research, and potential implications for policy

and practice. Before discussing the findings of the literature review in more detail,

the methodological limitations of the study (following from the quality assessment)

will be discussed. These limitations will provide a critical framework in which to

consider the reliability and validity of the current evidence base on coercive practice,

service user interpersonal style and aggressive behavior in secure forensic settings.

4.1 Methodological limitations of the included studies

In some of the included studies, service user participants may have completed

questionnaires in a socially desirable way or may have been less ‘risky’ than other

service users who refused to participate. In addition, some of the included studies

collected data from specialist secure units which only took those service users who

displayed more challenging behaviour and therefore the sample may have been

skewed by, for example, certain personality traits and interpersonal styles which are

more likely to be correlated with higher incidents of violence and aggression. Aside

service user participants, seven out of the eight quantitative studies used only one

member of staff to complete measures about, for example, service user interpersonal

style, with most studies not reporting who the staff were and/or the nature of their

relationship with the service user. Furthermore, in Meehan and colleague’s (2006)

qualitative study, presumably participants signed themselves up to taking part in the

focus groups (although there is no information reported on this) and it is therefore

questionable as to whether there may have been a biased/skewed view towards staff.

135

Page 136: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

For example, service users may have been motivated to take part in the research as

they had negative issues, they wanted to vent about staff behaviour, although it is

also important to highlight that this does not make their experience any less real.

Future research should provide more details on how characteristics relevant to the

service user population are represented within the sample and furthermore, consider

the impact of self-completed measures (by staff and service users) on the validity of

the findings.

All eight quantitative studies collected data on violent and aggressive incidents over

specific periods of time. As these incidents are recorded as routine procedure in

hospital settings, the data collected on the specific nature, severity and direction of

aggression perpetrated by the service users may have been reliant on the limited

details of the incident forms. Further to this, most of the studies did not report

specific details on missing data and it was therefore unclear as to which participants’

data was and was not included in the follow up periods. For example, in Harris &

Varney’s (1986) study there was a likelihood that those who were discharged from

the units during the study (possibly to lower secure units) were service users who

were less likely to be involved in violent/aggressive incidents and it is therefore

understandable why incidents of violence and aggression would reduce at follow up.

Future research should consider ways to triangulate routine collected data with other

sources of information. For example, the findings in the only qualitative study

included in the survey, were complementary to and provided further insight into the

quantitative findings on coercive practice and aggression (Meehan et al., 2006). This

suggests a potential need to conduct mixed methods research thus, dovetailing

qualitative service user and/or staff accounts on to the quantitative data. In specific

136

Page 137: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

reference to the only study that looked at the relationship between both interpersonal

style and perceived coercion on rates of aggression and violence (Daffern et al.,

2010b), it is important to consider that there was a limited variability in perceived

coercion and a narrow and extreme range of interpersonal styles within the study

sample. which may just reflect the nature of those who reside in such services (e.g.,

Blackburn, 1998). This may have been due to the study’s small sample size which

focused only on those who had been recently admitted into hospital. It may have

therefore been useful to include a larger, more varied sample size which included

service users who had been in the secure care system for longer and who’s

perceptions of coercion may be somewhat different to service users who had been

recently admitted.

All included studies reported significant findings that mapped on to the main

research objectives and existing evidence base whilst providing implications and

recommendations for future practice. However, the significance of the findings from

the included studies should be considered with caution due to the limited detail on

the methods of data collection (e.g. sampling and statistical analysis) and the

relatively small sample sizes that were often recruited from specialised units within

one or two hospital settings. For example, Vernham and colleagues (2016) was the

only study included in the review that had included a power size calculation while

also had a large sample size (which reached statistical power). It is therefore

important to consider that the statistical significance of the other studies findings

may have been more vulnerable to Type I errors.

137

Page 138: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Lastly, five studies were conducted outside the UK (USA, Norway, Australia) whilst

half the included studies were published over a decade ago. It is therefore important

to consider differences and changes in policies and procedures in secure forensic

hospitals within and between the study settings and the impact that this may have

upon the relevance of the study’s findings for current UK forensic secure-care.

4.2 Discussion of the literature review’s findings

As highlighted in the findings, only one study explicitly looked at the relationship

between coercive practice, interpersonal style and aggression and no significant

relationship was found between perceived coercion of staff and interpersonal style

(Daffern et al., 2010b). However, there was no information about how the sample

size was arrived at and/or whether a power calculation was conducted and therefore

it was not known as to whether there was an appropriate sample size in which a

precise and accurate conclusion could be drawn upon. The authors concluded that

perceived coercion when first admitted into hospital does not correlate to future

aggressive behaviour (Daffern et al., 2010b). Therefore, there is a need for research

that focuses on the use of coercive measures and service users’ perceptions of these

measures that runs outside of the period of admission.

Coercive practice has become integral to managing violent/aggressive/non-compliant

behaviour by use of extreme coercive measures through manual restraint, rapid

tranquillisation and seclusion (NICE, 2014). Surprisingly, although included in the

search terms, none of the studies looked explicitly at seclusion or restraint, which are

the more commonly known coercive measures. Furthermore, studies that have

focused on seclusion, restraint and service user aggression did so in general

138

Page 139: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

psychiatric inpatient settings and were therefore excluded. This mirrors Hui’s review

of coercive measures in forensic settings, who noted that there was a distinct lack of

research on coercive measures in forensic inpatient settings (Hui et al., 2013).

Dominant and coercive service user interpersonal styles were significantly correlated

with aggressive and violent behaviour (Daffern et al., 2010b; Doyle et al., 2006;

Smith, Edens & McDermott et al., 2013; Vernham et al., 2016). In Doyle and

colleagues (2006) research, an inverse relationship between submission and

compliance with more dominant service user interpersonal styles was found. In the

context of Kiesler’s interpersonal theory (1987) and the complementarity in our

interactions with others, we would expect that service users who scored highly on

dominant and coercive interpersonal styles as being more likely to clash with

coercive practices (which aim to coerce service users into submission). However, as

suggested by Daffern and colleague’s (2010b) findings, there was no significant

relationship between dominant and coercive interpersonal styles and perceptions of

staff coercion (during the period of admission into hospital).

Putting interpersonal style aside, four out of nine studies included in this review

reported staff limit-setting as the most common predictor of service user aggression

(Harris & Varney, 1986; Bjøkly, 1999; Daffern et al., 2008; Meehan et al., 2006;

Winje et al., 2018). This illustrates the relevance of longer-term measures of coercion

used in secure-care settings such as, persuasion, interpersonal leverage and

inducement, as well as the shorter-term, more extreme coercive measures, such as

seclusion and restraint (Applebaum & Szmukler, 2008). Aside the types of status

challenges that arise between staff and service user in the implementation of coercive

measures, and as suggested by the one qualitative study included in the review

139

Page 140: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

(Meehan et al., 2006), perhaps service users’ vulnerabilities to low self-esteem and

emotional deprivation (e.g. feeling as if staff do not understand or care about them)

may also trigger acts of aggression and violence amongst more dominant/coercive

service users. This notion is supported by previous literature which indicates that

those with lower self-esteem may be more vulnerable to experiencing increased

frustration and often display overly dominant behaviour as a way to cope (BjØrkvik

et al., 2009). There is therefore a need to consider that, in the context of secure-care

settings where coercive measures are being used by staff on a day-to-day basis (i.e.

limit setting/restricting rules), those service users who are vulnerable to low self-

esteem and feelings of emotional deprivation may resort to acts of aggression and

violence (become overly dominant) as a maladaptive coping strategy. Therefore, as

highlighted by the studies included in this review, it may not just be

dominant/coercive interpersonal styles that are more likely to predict service user

aggression but, may also be about the way in which service users perceive the

behaviour of others. Perhaps service users’ perceptions of staff coercion are linked to

individual differences in their interpersonal sensitivities to others where “some

patients may be less likely to be sensitive to others' irritating behaviour” (Hopwood

et al., 2011: p 708). For example, those who value personal authority and being in

control should be most frustrated by others who are arrogant, bossy, and act superior

(Henderson & Horowitz, 2006). This would suggest that people may be differentially

sensitive to specific forms of aversive behaviour of others because their interpersonal

motives vary. Therefore, interpersonal sensitivities may act as a “bridge” between

service users’ interpersonal styles and their perceptions of staff coercion and is an

area which needs further exploration.

140

Page 141: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Given the high hits and the small final number of relevant papers, it may be

important to consider the sensitivity and specificity of the review’s search strategy.

This might suggest that the search did not only retrieve the relevant literature but lots

of irrelevant literature as well. In addition, the review only included studies based in

forensic/secure settings and therefore may have excluded several studies from non-

forensic settings which may looked at the link between coercive practices,

interpersonal style, and service user violence/aggression. Furthermore, although the

search terms were carefully selected for the search strategy, they will in turn

ultimately influence those articles retrieved from the literature. For example, Hui and

colleagues (2013) noted that different definitions of coercive measures exist, which

made the selection of search terms for this review even more complex in nature. It is

therefore questionable as to how adequate the criteria were in capturing all measures

of coercion used in forensic settings. With these implications in mind, it would

therefore be advisable for this search strategy to be reviewed in future research.

All studies included in this literature review indicated that either interpersonal style

and/or coercive measures (such as staff limit-setting) were linked to service user

violence and aggression. There was, however, no research that explicitly looked at

the relationship between both service users’ interpersonal sensitivities and perceived

staff coercion as possible predictors of further aggression/violence. A better

understanding in to the way in which service users make sense of coercive practices

in context of their interpersonal sensitivities and in turn, how these impact upon

aggressive incidents, may help to reduce the number of untoward incidents that occur

because of service users feeling frustrated/restricted by staff limit setting. For

example, for staff to have an awareness that some service users may be more

141

Page 142: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

bothered by inducement (e.g. the confiscation of items) and may act out aggressively

compared to other service users who are not bothered by such coercive measures. In

order to reduce the likelihood of aggressive incidents, alternative approaches to

manage challenging behaviour may need to be considered. Perhaps if staff had a

better understanding of the types of interpersonal behaviours that service users are

sensitive to, this would help aid in their communication with service users with the

aim of maximising service user’s recovery whilst reducing and managing risk. For

example, by interweaving interpersonal sensitivities into the service user’s care plans

(i.e. service user may act out with further aggression when staff make threats to

confiscate items), clinical teams may be more aware of specific triggers related to

their interactions with service users that could precede service user aggression and

furthermore, to consider more appropriate/alternative communication styles. This

may help to break the ‘aggression-coercion cycle’ (Goren, 1993) between staff and

service users and in turn, reduce aggressive incidents, thus helping service users to

move forward in their recovery and through the secure care system. As stated

previously, aggressive behaviour may be a result of a complex interaction between

situational factors and individual factors (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). For example,

some service users may be particularly sensitive to being controlled by others which

can be exacerbated further when placed in a restrictive environment. On a policy

level, perhaps there may be a need to consider not just what coercive measures need

to be in place (i.e. hospital rules and regulations) but how they are implemented, and

how they may or may not predict further acts of aggression and violence in the

context of specific service user characteristics.

142

Page 143: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

References

Fallon P (1999) Report of the Committee of Inquiry into the Personality Disorder Unit, AshworthSpecial Hospital, Cm 4194-1. London: HMSO.Fallon P (1999) Report of the Committee of Inquiry into the Personality Disorder Unit, AshworthSpecial Hospital, Cm 4194-1. London: HMSO.Fallon P (1999) Report of the Committee of Inquiry into the Personality Disorder Unit, AshworthSpecial Hospital, Cm 4194-1. London: HMSO.Henderson, L., & Horowitz, L. M. (2006, June) Social allergens and frustratedinterpersonal motives. Paper presented at the Society for Interpersonal Theoryand Research, Philadelphia, PA.Henderson, L., & Horowitz, L. M. (2006, June) Social allergens and frustratedinterpersonal motives. Paper presented at the Society for Interpersonal Theoryand Research, Philadelphia, PA.Henderson, L., & Horowitz, L. M. (2006, June) Social allergens and frustratedinterpersonal motives. Paper presented at the Society for Interpersonal Theoryand Research, Philadelphia, PA.Ahmed, A. G. and Lepnurm, M. (2001). Seclusion practice in a Canadian forensic

psychiatric hospital. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 29(3), 303–309. Retrieved from https://europepmc.org/abstract/med/11592458

Alexander, J. & Bowers, L. (2004). Acute psychiatric ward rules: A review of the literature. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 11, 623-631. DOI: 10.1111/j.1365 2850.2004.00770

American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Nurses Association & National Association of Psychiatric Health Systems. (2003). Learning from each other: Success stories and ideas for reducing restraint/seclusion in behavioural health. Retrieved from WorldCat Website: https://www.worldcat.org/title/learning-from-each-other-success-stories-and-ideas-for-reducing-restraintseclusion-in-behavioral-health/oclc/52758580

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B.J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27-51. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231

Arksey, H., & O'Malley, L. (2005). Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 8(1), 19-32. DOI: 10.1080/1364557032000119616

Aveyard, H. (2014). Doing a literature review in health and social care: A practical guide. London: McGraw-Hill Education.

143

Page 144: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Beck, N.C., Durrett, C., Stinson, J., Coleman, J., Stuve, P., & Menditto, A. (2008). Trajectories of seclusion and restraint use at a state psychiatric hospital. Psychiatric Services, 59(9), 1027–1032. DOI:

10.1176/ps.2008.59.9.1027

Blackburn, R. (1998). Criminality and the interpersonal circle in mentally disordered offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 25(2), 155-176. DOI: 10.1177/0093854898025002001

Blackburn, R., & Renwick, S.J. (1996). Rating scales for measuring the interpersonal circle in forensic psychiatric patients. Psychological Assessment, 8, 76–84. DOI: 10.1037/1040- 3590.8.1.76

Blom-Cooper, L. (1992). Report of the Committee of Inquiry into Complaints about Ashworth Hospital, Cm 2028-1. London: HMSO.

Bjorkly, S. (1996). Report Form for Aggressive Episodes: Preliminary report. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 1139-1152. DOI: 10.2466/pms.1996.83.3f.1139

Bjørkly, S. (1999). A ten‐year prospective study of aggression in a special secure unit for dangerous patients. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 57-63. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10216464

BjØrkvik, J., Biringer, E.V.A., Eikeland, O.J., & Nielson, G.H. (2009). Self-esteem and interpersonal functioning in psychiatric outpatients. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(3), 259-265. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00709

Blackburn, R., & Glasgow, D. V. (2006). Manual for the Chart of Interpersonal Reactions in Closed Living Environments (CIRCLE). Unpublished manuscript, University of Liverpool, UK.

Bowers, L., Stewart, D., Papadopoulos, C., Dack, C., Ross, J., Khanom, H., & Jeffery, D. (2011). Inpatient violence and aggression: a literature. Report from the Conflict and Containment Reduction Research Programme. Retrieved from http://www.kcl.ac.uk/iop/depts/hspr/research/ciemh/mhn/projects/litreview/LitRevAgg.pdf

Bubb, S. (2014). Winterbourne view–time for change: transforming the commissioning of services for people with learning disabilities and/or autism. Retrieved from British Library Website: https://www.bl.uk/collection- items/winterbourne-view--time-for- change-transforming-the-commissioning-of-services-for-people-with-learning-disabilities-andor-autism

Burrows S. (1991). The special hospital nurse and the dilemma of therapeutic custody. Journal of Advances in Health and Nursing Care, 1, 21–38.

144

Page 145: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Care Quality Commission. (2017). The state of care in mental health services 2014 to 2017. Retrieved from Care Quality Commission Website: https://www.cqc.org.uk/publications/major-report/state-care-mental-health-services-2014-2017

Castro, M., Cockerton, T., & Birke, S. (2002). From discharge to follow-up: a small-scale study of medium secure provision in the independent sector. British Journal of Forensic Practice, 4, 31-9. DOI: 10.1108/14636646200200019

Centre for Mental Health. (2011). Pathways to unlocking secure mental health care. Retrieved from Centre for Mental Health Website: https://www.centreformentalhealth.org.uk/sites/default/files/2018-09/Pathways_to_unlocking_secure_mental_health_care.pdf

Compton, M. T., Craw, J., & Rudisch, B. E. (2006). Determinants of inpatient psychiatric length of stay in an urban county hospital. Psychiatric Quarterly, 77(2), 173-188. DOI: 10.1007/s11126-006-9005-z

Cookson, A., Daffern, M., & Foley, F. (2012). Relationship between aggression, interpersonal style, and therapeutic alliance during short‐term psychiatric hospitalization. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 21(1), 20-29. DOI:10.1111/1447-0349.2011.00764

Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP). (2013). Qualitative Research Checklist. Retrieved from Critical Appraisal Skills Programme Website: http://www.casp-uk.net/wp-content/uploads/2011/11/casp-qualitative-

research-checklist-31.05.13.pdf#!casp-tools-checklists/c18f8

Daffern, M., Mayer, M. M., & Martin, T. (2003). A preliminary investigation into patterns of aggression in an Australian forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 14(1), 67–84. DOI: 10.1080/1478994031000074306

Daffern, M., Howells, K., & Ogloff, J. R. (2007). What’s the point? Towards a methodology for assessing the function of psychiatric inpatient aggression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 101-111. DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.01.011

Daffern, M., Duggan, C., Huband, N., & Thomas, S. (2008). The impact of interpersonal style on aggression and treatment non-completion in patients with personality disorder admitted to a medium secure psychiatric unit. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14(6), 481-492. DOI: 10.1080/10683160801948717

Daffern, M., Duggan, C., Huband, N., & Thomas, S. (2010a). Staff and Patient’s Perceptions of Each Other’s Interpersonal Style Relationship with

Severity of Personality Disorder. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 54(4),611-624. DOI:10.1177/0306624X09335111

145

Page 146: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Daffern, M., Tonkin, M., Howells, K., Krishnan, G., Ijomah, G., & Milton, J. (2010b). The impact of interpersonal style and perceived coercion on aggression and self-harm in personality-disordered patients admitted to a secure psychiatric hospital. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 21(3), 426-445. DOI:10.1080/14789940903505951

Davoren, M., Byrne, O., O’Connell, P., O’Neill, H., O’Reilly, K., & Kennedy, H. G. (2015). Factors affecting length of stay in forensic hospital setting: need for therapeutic security and course of admission. BMC psychiatry, 15(1), 301. DOI: 10.1186/s12888-015-0686-4

Department of Health. (2014). Positive and proactive care: reducing the need for restrictive interventions. Retrieved from Department of Health Website: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/

attac hment_data/file/300293/JRA_DoH_Guidance_on_RP_web_accessible.odf

Dickens, G., Picchioni, M., & Long, C. (2013). Aggression in specialist secure and forensic inpatient mental health care: incidence across care pathways. Journal of Forensic Practice, 15, 206-17. DOI: 10.1108/JFP-09-2012-0017

Dolan, M., & Blackburn, R. (2006). Interpersonal factors as predictors of disciplinary infractions in incarcerated personality disordered offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(5), 897-907. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.003

Doyle, M., & Dolan, M. (2006). Evaluating the validity of anger regulation problems, interpersonal style, and disturbed mental state for predicting inpatient violence. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 24(6), 783-798. DOI: 10.1002/bsl.739

Drennan, G., & Wooldridge, J. (2014). Making recovery a reality in forensic services. Implementing Recovery through Organisational Change (ImROC) briefing paper. Retrieved from the Centre for Mental Health and Mental Health Network NHS Confederation Website: file:///C:/Users/jess/Downloads/ImROC-briefing-10-Making-Recovery-a-Reality-in-Forensic-Settings-final-for-web%20(1).pdf

Fallon, P. (1999). Report of the Committee of Inquiry into the Personality Disorder

Unit, Ashworth Specialised Hospital, Cm 4194-1. London, UK: HMSO.

Gardner, W., Hoge, S. K., Bennett, N., Roth, L. H., Lidz, C. W., Monahan, J., & Mulvey, E. P. (1993). Two scales for measuring patients' perceptions for coercion during mental hospital admission. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 11(3), 307-321. DOI: 10.1002/bsl.2370110308

Goffman, E. (1968) Asylums Essays on the Social Situation of Mental Patients and Other Inmates. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

146

Page 147: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Goren, S., Singh, N. N., & Best, A. M. (1993). The aggression-coercion cycle: Use of seclusion and restraint in a child psychiatric hospital. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 2(1), 61–73. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF01321301

Hare, R.D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist Revised (PCL-R). Toronto, Ontario: MultiHealth Systems.

Harty, M. A., Shaw, J., Thomas, S., Dolan, M., Davies, L., Thornicroft, G., Carlisle, J., Moreno, M., Leese, M., Appleby, L., & Jones, P. (2004). The security, clinical and social needs of patients in high security psychiatric hospitals in England. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 15(2), 208-221. DOI: 10.1080/14789940410001703967

Harris, G. T., & Varney, G. W. (1986). A ten-year study of assaults and assaulters on a maximum security psychiatric unit. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1(2), 173-191. DOI: 10.1177/088626086001002003

Harris, S. T., Oakley, C., & Picchioni, M. M. (2014). A systematic review of the association between attributional bias/interpersonal style, and violence in schizophrenia/psychosis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19(3), 235-241. DOI: 10.1016/j.avb.2014.04.009

Henderson, L., & Horowitz, L. M. (2006). Social allergens and frustrated interpersonal motives, presented at the Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research Conference, Philadelphia, 2006. Philadelphia, PA: Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research.

Hopwood, C. J., Ansell, E. B., Pincus, A. L., Wright, A. G., Lukowitsky, M. R., & Roche, M. J. (2011). The circumplex structure of interpersonal sensitivities. Journal of Personality, 79(4), 707-740. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00696

Hui, A., Middleton, H., & Völlm, B. (2013). Coercive measures in forensic settings: findings from the literature. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 12(1), 53-67. DOI: 10.1080/14999013.2012.740649

Jarrett, M., Bowers, L., & Simpson, A. (2008). Coerced medication in psychiatric inpatient care: Literature Review, Journal of Advanced Nursing, 64(4), 538-548. DOI: 10.111/j.1365-2648.2008.04832.x

Kelly, E.L., Subica, A. M., Fulginiti, A., Brekke, J. S., & Novaco, R.W. (2015). A cross-sectional survey of factors related to inpatient assault of staff in a

forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 71(5), 1110–1122. DOI: 10.1111/jan.12609

Keski-Valkama, A., Koivisto, A.M., Eronen, M. and Kaltiala- Heino, R. (2010). Forensic and general psychiatric patients’ view of seclusion: A comparison

147

Page 148: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

study. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 21(3), 446–461. DOI: 10.1080/14789940903456569

Kiesler, D. (1979). An interpersonal communication analysis of relationship in psychotherapy. Psychiatry, 42, 299–311. DOI: 10.1080/00332747.1979.11024034

Kiesler, D. (1987). Manual for the Impact Message Inventory. Mountain View, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Kiesler, D. J. (1997). Contemporary interpersonal theory and research: Personality, psychopathology and psychotherapy. The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 6(4), 339-341. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3350312/

Lillie, R. (2007). Getting clients to hear: Applying principles and techniques of Kiesler's Interpersonal Communication Therapy to assessment feedback. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 80(1), 151-163. DOI: 10.1348/147608306X115198

Lilienfeld, S. O., & Widows, M. R. (2005). Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: PAR, Inc.

Loranger, A.W., Sartorius, N., Andreoli, A., Berger, P., Buchheim, P., Channabasavanna, S.M., Coid, B., Dahl, A., Diekstra, R., Ferguson, B., Jacobsberg, L., Mombour, W., Pull, C., Ono, Y., & Regier, D.A. (1994) The International Personality Disorder Examination. The World Health Organization/Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration International Pilot Study of personality disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 215-224. DOI: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950030051005

Mason, T., Lovell, A., & Coyle, D. (2008). Forensic psychiatric nursing: skills and competencies: I role dimensions. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 15(2), 118-130. DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2850.2007.01191

Meehan, T., McIntosh, W., & Bergen, H. (2006). Aggressive behaviour in the high‐secure forensic setting: the perceptions of patients. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 13(1), 19-25. DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-

2850.2006.00906

Meissner, W. (2007). Therapeutic alliance: Theme and variations. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 24, 231–254. DOI: 10.1037/0736-9735.24.2.231

Mellesdal, L. (2003). Aggression on a psychiatric acute ward: a three-year prospective study. Psychological Reports, 92, 1229-1248. DOI: 10.2466/pr0.2003.92.3c.1229

Mind (2013). Physical Restraint in Crisis: A report on physical restraint in hospital settings in England. Retrieved from Mind Website:

148

Page 149: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

https://www.mind.org.uk/media/197120/physical_restraint_final_web_version.pdf

Morrison, E. F. (1992). A coercive interactional style as an antecedent to aggression in psychiatric patients. Research in Nursing & Health, 15(6), 421-431. DOI: 10.1002/nur.4770150604

Morse J.M. & Field P.A. (1996). Nursing Research: The Application of Qualitative Approaches, 2nd edn. London: Chapman and Hall.

National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2005). Violence: The short-term management of disturbed/violent behaviour in inpatient psychiatric settings and emergency departments. Retrieved from NICE Website: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng10/resources/violence- and aggression-short-term-management-in-mental-health-health-and-community settings-pdf-1837264712389

NICE. (2015). Violence and Aggression: short-term management in mental health, health and community settings. Retrieved from National Institute of Clinical Excellence Website: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng10

Nijman, H., Muris, P., Merckelbach, H. L. G. J., Palmstierna, T., Wistedt, B., Vos, A. M., Rixtel, A.V., & Allertz, W. (1999). The staff observation aggression scale-revised (SOAS-R). Aggressive Behavior, 25(3), 197–209. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.ez.statsbiblioteket. dk:2048/docview/619431198?accountid=14468

Novaco,R. W. (2003). The Novaco Anger Scale and Provocation Inventory (NAS-PI). Los Angeles, LA: Western Psychological Services.

Novaco, R. & Renwick, S. (1998). Anger predictors of the assaultativeness of forensic hospital patients. In E. Sanavio (Ed.), Behavior and cognitive therapy today: Essays in honor of Hans J. Eysenck (pp. 199–208). Oxford: Pergamon.

Oxford Dictionaries. (2010). ‘Coercion’, Oxford Dictionary of English, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Palmstierna, T., & Wistedt, B. (1987). Staff observation aggression scale, SOAS: Presentation and evaluation. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76(6),

657– 663. DOI: 2048/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1987.tb02936

Peternelj‐Taylor C. & Johnson R. (1995). Serving time: psychiatric mental health nursing in corrections. Journal of Psychosocial Nursing, 33, 12–19. DOI: 10.3928/0279-3695- 19950801-06

Queensland Government. (2008). Policy statement on reducing and where possible eliminating restraint and seclusion in Queensland mental health services. Retrieved from http://www.health.gld.gov.uk/mentalhealth/docs/sandrpolicy 081030.pdf.

149

Page 150: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Ritchie, J., Spencer, L. & O'Connor, W. (2003). Ritchie, J., and Lewis, J. (eds.) Carrying out Qualitative Analysis, in Qualitative Research Practice: A Guide for Social Science Students and Researchers. London: Sage.

Sequeira, H. & Halstead, S. (2004). The psychological effects on nursing staff of administering physical restraint in a secure psychiatric hospital: “When I go home, it's then that I think about it.”. British Journal of Forensic Practice, 6(1), 3–15. DOI: 10.1108/14636646200400002

Shaw, J., Davies, J., & Morey, H. (2001). An assessment of the security, dependency and treatment needs of all patients in secure services in a UK health

region. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 12(3), 610-637. DOI: 10.1080/09585180127380

Smith, S. T., Edens, J. F., & McDermott, B. E. (2013). Fearless dominance and self-centered impulsivity interact to predict predatory aggression among forensic psychiatric inpatients. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 12(1), 33-41. DOI: 10.1080/14999013.2012.760186

Szmukler, G., & Appelbaum, P. S. (2008). Treatment pressures, leverage, coercion, and compulsion in mental health care. Journal of Mental Health, 17(3), 233-244. DOI: 10.1080/09638230802052203

Taylor, P. J., Maden, A., & Jones, D. (1996). Long-term medium secure hospital units: service gap of the 1990s? Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 6, 223–239. DOI: 10.1002/cbm.96

Tulloch, A. D., Fearon, P., & David, A. S. (2008). The determinants and outcomes of long-stay psychiatric admissions. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(7), 569-574. DOI: 10.1007/s00127-008-0332-2

Uppal, G., & McMurran, M. (2009). Recorded incidents in a high‐secure hospital: A descriptive analysis. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 19, 265-76. DOI: 10.1002/cbm.741

Urheim, R., Rypdal, K., Melkevik, O., Hoff, H. A., Mykletun, A., & Palmstierna, T. (2014). Motivational dimensions of inpatient aggression. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 24(2), 141–150. DOI: 2048/10.1002/cbm.1894

Vernham, Z., Tapp, J., & Moore, E. (2016). Observer ratings of interpersonal behavior as predictors of aggression and self-harm in a high-security sample of male forensic inpatients. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31(9), 1597-1617. DOI: 10.1177/0886260515569060

Vӧllm, B. & Nedopil, N. (2016). The use of coercive measures in Forensic Psychiatric Care: Legal, ethical and practical challenges. Switzerland: Springer.

150

Page 151: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Völlm, B., Edworthy, R., Holley, J., Talbot, E., Majid, S., Duggan, C., Weaver, T., & McDonald, R. (2017). A mixed-methods study exploring the characteristics and needs of long-stay patients in high and medium secure settings in England: implications for service organisation. Health Service and Delivery Research, 5(11). Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK424815/

Von Elm, E., Altman, D. G., Egger, M., Pocock, S. J., Gøtzsche, P. C., Vandenbroucke, J. P., & Strobe Initiative. (2007). The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. International Journal of Surgery, 12(12), 1495-1499. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.01

Winje, M., van Mastrigt, S., Gjestad, R., Urheim, R., & Rypdal, K. (2018). Staff members’ evaluation of inpatients’ motivation for aggression–the roles of staff restrictions and aggression severity. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 29(3), 419-433. DOI: 10.1080/14789949.2017.14105

151

Page 152: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Part 3: Clinical Experience

Community Mental Health Recovery Service (CMHRS) (Year 1: November 2016-

September 2017) – this was a community service offering psychological assessment and

intervention to people (16 years old +) with severe and enduring mental health problems who

were eligible for Secondary Services. In this placement, I conducted 1:1 therapeutic work

with a diverse range of clients, primarily employing disorder-specific cognitive behaviour

therapy (CBT) (e.g. CBT for Psychosis, Bi-Polar Affective Disorder, Depression, and Post-

Traumatic Stress Disorder). Aside the 1:1 psychological work, I co-led a family intervention

for psychosis. With regards to psychological group work, I co-delivered a drop-in CBT skills-

based psychoeducational group and a 10-week dialectical-behaviour therapy group

intervention for those with emotional unstable personality disorder.

Older People’s Recovery & Support Team / Memory Assessment Service (Year 2:

October 2017 – March 2018) – I obtained experience in three different settings on this

placement working alongside older adults (65 years old +). The first setting was an older

people’s recovery and support team where I provided psychological interventions (e.g. CBT

and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT)) for older adults experiencing depression

and/or anxiety. The second setting was a memory assessment service whereby I undertook

neuropsychological assessments on older adults as well as supporting clients and their

families adjusting to a dementia diagnosis. The third setting was a rehabilitation unit working

with older adults who had been referred to the unit for physical health difficulties (i.e. having

experienced frequent falls/mobility issues and Parkinson’s disease) and who were also likely

to benefit from brief psychological therapy. In addition, I worked within the Care Home

Liaison Service supporting staff of local care homes who were experiencing difficulties

managing the behaviour of residents, predominantly those with advanced dementia.

152

Page 153: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Child and Adolescent Community Mental Health Team (Year 2: April 2018-September

2018) –I worked alongside children and young people (from the age of 6-18 years old) with

their families and often, their schools, dividing my time between a tier 2 and tier 3 service. In

the tier 3 service I worked alongside young people with more acute presentations who often

needed both psychological and psychiatric (i.e. medication) input. I provided a range of

psychological interventions including, exposure therapy for panic and developing a PBS plan

for an individual with Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD)). In the tier 2 service I

predominantly worked in the single point of access (SPA) where my role was to conduct

initial psychological assessments, including risk assessments, with young people and their

families and to signpost these individual on to appropriate services (i.e. tier 2 or tier 3 mental

health services, parenting workshops). I also provided brief CBT interventions for young

people with mild mental health difficulties (i.e. narrative therapy for a child with separation

anxiety).

Low and medium-secure forensic unit (Year 3: October 2018- March 2019) – I chose to

work in a low and medium secure forensic unit for my specialist placement and gained

experience working alongside male and female offenders (aged 18-65 years old) presenting

with mental health difficulties (primarily psychosis-related disorders). Complex

comorbidities that were common in this client population included substance misuse and

personality disorder in addition to difficulties caused by biological factors (e.g. ASD),

psychosocial factors (e.g. traumatic childhood/past experiences), and coping/adaptation to

stressful life events (e.g. bereavements, difficult transitions from adolescence to adulthood).

A main part of my work was conducting psychological assessments which enabled me to

develop psychological formulations for not only the individual’s mental health difficulties but

also their offending behaviour/risk history (i.e. arson, grievous bodily harm) and personality

153

Page 154: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

and interpersonal functioning. I conducted both 1:1 psychological therapy (i.e. CBT for

Psychosis and solution-focused therapy) whilst also lead on an adapted family therapeutic

intervention. With regards to group work, I co-led an ‘Understanding Mental Health’ group

which was primarily a CBT skills-based psycho-educational intervention for those residing in

the medium secure wards. This also required the supervision of psychology assistants in the

design and delivery of group material.

Community Learning Disability Healthcare Team: (Year 3: April 2019- September

2019) – in this placement I worked with male and female adults (18-65 years old) who had a

diagnosed learning disability typically alongside other comorbidities such as Autism

Spectrum Disorder, Downs Syndrome and Cerebral Palsy. The Psychology team sat within a

wider physical health care team that included, for example, speech and language therapists,

occupational therapists, nurses, and physiotherapists. Reasons for referral to the psychology

team included mild mental health problems (i.e. anxiety), challenging behaviour and life

changes and transitions (i.e. bereavements, moving to independent living). There was a lot of

indirect work with systems, for instance, gathering information and presenting psychological

formulations to social workers and care staff from residential settings. The main

psychological approach/model used in this placement was behavioural, for example,

developing Positive Behaviour Support plans through formal observations and the monitoring

of ABC (antecedent, behaviour, consequence) charts. In addition, I conducted cognitive

assessments (i.e. Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)-IV) and social functioning

assessments (i.e. Adaptive Behaviour Assessment Systems (ABAS)) to assess whether an

individual had a diagnosis of a learning disability in order to access appropriate services.

154

Page 155: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Part 4: Assessments

Year I AssessmentsAssessment Title

WAIS WAIS Interpretation (online assessment)

Practice Report of Clinical Activity CBT assessment and formulation of Simon (pseudonym), a male in his 50’s experiencing low mood and rumination.

Audio Recording of Clinical Activity with Critical Appraisal

Audio Recording and Critical Appraisal of Ac-ceptance and Commitment Therapy for Depres-sion and Anxiety.

Report of Clinical Activity N=1 Cognitive Behaviour Therapy with a woman in her early forties with Bipolar Affective Disorder.

Major Research Literature Survey Coercive practice and patient interpersonal style as predictors of patient aggression and violence in secure-care forensic settings: A literature Survey.

Major Research Project Proposal The relationship between interpersonal sensitivity and perceived staff coercion in predicting levels/rates of patient aggression in a secure forensic setting.

Service-Related Project Evaluation of a Trauma and Dissociation Aware-ness Training program for staff in multidisciplin-ary adult mental health teams.

Year II AssessmentsAssessment Title

Report of Clinical Activity – Formal Assessment A neuropsychological assessment with a lady in her early 70s for a suspected Dementia within a Memory Assessment Clinic.

PPD Process Account Personal and Professional Development Group Process Account.

Presentation of Clinical Activity CBT for Depression with a White British Adolescent Boy.

155

Page 156: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations

URN: 6455433

Year III AssessmentsAssessment TitleMajor Research Project Literature Review Coercive practice and service user interpersonal

style as predictors of service user aggression and violence in secure-care forensic settings: A literature review.

Major Research Project Empirical Paper How do service users’ interpersonal sensitivity to dominance and perceptions of staff coercion impact upon self -reported anger and rates of aggression and in secure-care settings?

Report of Clinical Activity Adapted family therapy with a man in his 30s and his father in the context of a forensic setting.

156

Page 157: epubs.surrey.ac.ukepubs.surrey.ac.uk/852329/1/E-thesis_JHolley 6455433… · Web viewThe findings have, however, been interpreted tentatively due to various study limitations. Recommendations