13
Copyright 1985 by Rosemead School of Psychology, Biol a University, 0091-6471/4107-3000 Journal of Psychology and Theology 1985, Vol. 13, No. 2, 104-115 Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature JOHN R. FINNEY and H. NEWTON MALONY, JR. The Graduate School o f Psychology Fuller Theological Seminary Pasadena, California Christian prayer is a central religious practice which has received scant attention in psychological research. The two major types of prayer are verbal and contem- plative prayer. A review of the empirical studies of prayer suggests that the work done in this area can be divided into four categories: (a) developmental studies of conceptions of prayer; (b) research on motivations for praying; (c) studies of the effects of verbal prayer; and (d) studies of the effects of contemplative prayer. The studies on the development of the concept of prayer generally have found patterns consistent with Piaget’s stages of moral and cognitive development. Regarding motives for prayer, Welford (1947) supported the hypothesis that prayer is not just a neurotic flight from anxiety. Elkins et al. (1979) found verbal prayer to be generally ineffective in anxiety reduction. Parker and St. Johns (1957) demonstrated that a program of spiritual development that includes verbal prayer can be effective as a therapeutic intervention. The findings of Sacks (1979) suggest that contemplative prayer may facilitate ego development. Opportunities for further research on prayer are discussed. ceived as similar to all social relationships in that an imaginative social process takes place mentally in which an idea of oneself and an idea of the other are related (Strong, 1909; Sunden, 1974). The nature of prayer differs from religion to religion depending upon the way the divine is conceived. In Christianity God is seen to be essentially all-loving and all-powerful, the one who has revealed himself in Jesus, the Christ. In the Christian tradition prayer is, thus, intimate communication with a benevolent deity. This article will focus on Christian prayer although it will, at times, review studies which have wider implications. Heiler’s (1932/1958) phenomenological analysis of prayer in general, is probably the most comprehensive. He concludes that there are two types of prayer, mystical and prophetic. Mystical prayer in Heilers Nowhere is the longstanding breach be- tween psychology and religion more evident than in the lack of research on prayer. Only a few studies of prayer exist in spite of the fact that prayer is of central religious impor- tance. This article develops a conceptual model for prayer and reviews research re- lated to this religious practice. Definitions of Prayer Prayer can be defined as “every kind of inward communion or conversation with the power recognized as divine” (James, 1902/ 1962, p. 464). As such, prayer can be con- This article is based on a dissertation written by the first author, chaired by the second author and submit- ted to the Graduate School of Psychology, Fuller Theo- logical Seminary. Requests for reprints should be sent to John R. Fin- ney, MDiv, 2261 N. Villa Heights, Pasadena, Cali- fomia 91107. 104

Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    3

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

Copyright 1985 by Rosemead School o f Psychology, Biol a University, 0091-6471/4107-3000

Journal o f Psychology and Theology 1985, Vol. 13, N o . 2, 104-115

Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature

JOHN R. FINNEY and H. NEWTON MALONY, JR.The Graduate School o f Psychology Fuller Theological Seminary Pasadena, California

Christian prayer is a central religious practice which has received scant attention in psychological research. The two major types of prayer are verbal and contem- plative prayer. A review of the empirical studies of prayer suggests that the work done in this area can be divided into four categories: (a) developmental studies of conceptions of prayer; (b) research on motivations for praying; (c) studies of the effects of verbal prayer; and (d) studies of the effects of contemplative prayer. The studies on the development of the concept of prayer generally have found patterns consistent with Piaget’s stages of moral and cognitive development. Regarding motives for prayer, Welford (1947) supported the hypothesis that prayer is not just a neurotic flight from anxiety. Elkins et al. (1979) found verbal prayer to be generally ineffective in anxiety reduction. Parker and St. Johns (1957) demonstrated that a program of spiritual development that includes verbal prayer can be effective as a therapeutic intervention. The findings of Sacks (1979) suggest that contemplative prayer may facilitate ego development. Opportunities for further research on prayer are discussed.

ceived as similar to all social relationships in that an imaginative social process takes place mentally in which an idea of oneself and an idea of the other are related (Strong, 1909; Sunden, 1974). The nature o f prayer differs from religion to religion depending upon the way the divine is conceived. In Christianity G od is seen to be essentially all-loving and all-powerful, the one who has revealed himself in Jesus, the Christ. In the Christian tradition prayer is, thus, intimate communication with a benevolent deity. This article will focus on Christian prayer although it will, at times, review studies which have wider implications.

H eile r’s (1932/1958) phenomenological analysis of prayer in general, is probably the most comprehensive. He concludes that there are two types of prayer, mystical and p ro p h e tic . M ystica l p ray e r in H e i l e r s

Nowhere is the longstanding breach be- tween psychology and religion more evident than in the lack o f research on prayer. Only a few studies of prayer exist in spite o f the fact that prayer is of central religious impor- tance. This article develops a conceptual model for prayer and reviews research re- lated to this religious practice.

Definitions of Prayer

Prayer can be defined as “ every kind of inward communion or conversation with the power recognized as divine” (James, 1902/ 1962, p. 464). As such, prayer can be con-

This article is based on a dissertation written by the first author, chaired by the second author and submit- ted to the Graduate School of Psychology, Fuller Theo- logical Seminary.

Requests for reprints should be sent to John R. Fin- ney, MDiv, 2261 N. Villa Heights, Pasadena, Cali- fomia 91107.

104

Page 2: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

J.R. FINNEY AND H.N. MALONY

Prayer: Functions and Processes

A number o f writers have considered the function of prayer. Some have viewed it as basically an expression o f need (Heiler, 1932/1958; Pratt, 1930, pp. 318-320; Selbie, 1924, p. 220; Johnson, 1945, p. 112, Hodge, 1931). Others have concluded that prayer is essentially an affirmation of faith (Ellens, 1977; Phillips, 1965).

Some theorizing has been devoted to the psychological processes active in prayer. The one that has received the most attention is hypnotic suggestion (Coe, 1900, 1916; Cutten, 1908; Hodge, 1931; Horton, 1931; James, 1902/1963; Pratt, 1930; Relton, 1925; Selbie, 1924; Strunk, 1959; Thouless, 1923/ 1956; Valentine, 1929). Theoretical dis- eussions of mysticism and meditation are grounds for speculating tha t additional p sy ch o lo g ica l p ro c esse s invo lved in contem plative prayer include regression (F reud , 1929/1961; A llison, 1966, 1968; Arieti, 1967; Committee on Psychiatry and Religion, 1976; Fingarette, 1958; Kris, 1936; Maupin, 1962, 1965; Owens, 1972; Prince & Savage, 1972), a deautomatization of per- ception and cognition (Carrington, 1977; Deikman, 1966; Goleman, 1971; Om stein, 1971), a conditioning o f a lower level o f arousal through induction o f a relaxation state (Shapiro, 1980), and desensitization (G olem an, 1971; Carrington & Ephron, 1975; Otis, 1974).

B eyond s tud ies on the frequency o f prayer (Strommen, 1971, pp. 553-554, 560), the empirical work that has been done can be divided into four groups: (a) Develop- mental studies of conceptions of prayer;(b) research on the motivations for praying;(c) studies of the effects o f verbal prayer; and (d) studies o f the effects of contem- plative prayer.

Empirical Studies on Prayer

Developmental Studies o f Conceptions o f Prayer

Regarding the first category of the re- search on prayer, six studies have been done

schema seeks union with G od and is charac- terized by silent attentiveness to G od rather than by words. Prophetic prayer is a spon- taneous expression of emotion. Verbal peti- tion to the divine is the usual form that prophetic prayer takes. Heiler argues that mystical prayer is incompatible with the tenets of Christianity, implying that it is a form of do-it-yourself salvation.

Although Heiler’s (1932/1958) distinction between verbal and mystical prayer has stood the test of time, rejection of mystical prayer as a Christian practice has been chal- lenged (von Balthasar, 1981, 1982, pp. 333- 343; Bouyer, 1963, pp. 406-420; Simpson, 1965, pp. 112-113; Tillich, 1967, Vol. 2, pp. 83-84; Tinsley, 1969).

Verbal and mystical prayer appear to be d iffe ren t p h en o m en a , h o w ev er. Som e Christian theologians (M acquarrie, 1966, pp. 437-438; Tillich, 1967, Vol. I, pp. 112- 113, 126, 267; Vol. I l l , pp. 120, 192-193), argue that both are the work of the Holy Spirit on the basis of such texts as Romans 8:15-16. This view perceives both verbal and mystical prayer as G od within us relating us to G od above us. Thus, both can be seen as the same in essence.

More detailed classifications of the types of prayer have been developed both by those speaking from within the Christian tradition (Magee, 1957) and spokespersons for psy- chology (Clark, 1958, pp. 312-325). Heiler’s (1932/1958) basic distinction has been af- firmed and refined. Verbal prayer has been subdivided into such categories as petition, intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known as “ contemplative” prayer, which can be defined as giving one’s full attention to relating to G od in a passive, nondefensive, nondemanding, open, non- verbal way. Thus, the essence o f contem- plative prayer is not a technique but a par- ticular type o f interpersonal response to God. The purpose of contemplative prayer is to wait on G od to deepen one’s confidence in his power and love so that one can grow in Christlikeness.

105

Page 3: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

PRAYER REVIEW

Welford (1947) surveyed 63 male students between 18 and 25 years of age who were all church-goers. T he subjects were given descriptions of six situations, three of which were pleasant and three unpleasant. They were told to imagine themselves in the situa- tions and to rank order the anecdotes on three bases. The first ranking was the order in which the situations would stir their emo- tions and was used as an indicator of affect. The measure of frustration was based on the extent to which the situations would be beyond their power to handle them, except by praying. The third ranking was the likeli- hood that they would pray in the situation and was the prayer measure.

Correlating rank orders, he found signifi- cant relationships (p < .01) between prayer and affect (r = .51 ), prayer and frustration (r = .59, and affect and frustration (r = .38). When frustration was partialled out from the correlation of prayer and affect, the mean correlation was still significant (r = .33, p < .01). When affect was partialled from the correlation of prayer and frustration, it, too, was significant (r = .47, p < .05). Thus, his hypothesis was confirmed.

The subjects were also asked why they were more likely to pray in some situations and not others. These reasons for prayer seemed to divide into two groups. One group saw prayer as a means of changing the situation, while the other group tended to view it as a way of altering their subjective response to the situation. These qualitative reports also stressed that prayer was a means of adjustment to be used when no other course of action seemed viable.

W elford’s (1947) research provides an em pirical basis for understand ing peti- tionary p ray e r as a re sp o n se to need. Considering the independent correlation of both affect and frustration with prayer Wei- ford concluded, 4‘Any simple hypothesis which regards prayer as a response merely to distressing, threatening forces in the environment is inadequate” (p. 317). His findings suggest that petitionary prayer is motivated both by the need to reduce frus­

on the development of the concept o f prayer (Brown 1966, 1968; Godin & Van Rooey, 1959; G oldm an, 1964; Long, Elkind, & Spilka, 1967; Thouless & Brown, 1964). All of these deal with the period of childhood through adolescence. These studies have been reviewed elsewhere in detail (Godin, 1971, pp. 134-135; Elkind, 1971, pp. 673- 675). P a tte rn s co n s is ten t w ith P iag e t’s stages of moral and cognitive development were generally observed (Godin & Van Rooey, 1959; Goldman, 1964; Long, Elkind, & Spilka, 1967). It was also frequently found that with increasing age, there is a decline in the belief that prayer will have material consequences (Brown, 1966, 1968; Godin & Van Rooey, 1959; Goldman, 1964; Thouless & Brown, 1964). A third conclusion of the research on the development of the concept of prayer in childhood and adolescence is that a child’s concept of prayer develops from vague assoc ia tions with the term “ G o d ” to a private conversation with G od in which very intimate things are shared (Long, Elkind, & Spilka, 1967). Finally, it has been observed that as a child matures chronologically there is a movement in the conten t o f petitionary prayer from ego- centric wish fulfillment for such things as candy and toys to altruistic desires for such things as peace on earth (Long, Elkind, & Spilka, 1967, p. 103).

Motivations fo r Praying

Few studies have explored the motives for prayer. Welford (1947) investigated the question o f whether petitionary prayer in adulthood was a positive means of adjust- ment to unusual or baffling situations or just a n eu ro tic flight from fru s tra t io n . H e hypothesized that if petitionary prayer was correlated with both positive and negative em otion in addition to frustra tion , this would support the hypothesis that peti- tionary prayer is more than a reaction to frustration.

106

Page 4: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

J R. FINNEY AND H.N. MALONY

Galton noted that members of royal houses had the lowest average life expectancy. This was in spite of the tradition of praying for the sovereign. He went on to point out that the same group of statistics showed that emi- nent clergy fell behind lawyers and physi- cians in average life expectancy. He also made reference to the chapter, “ Divines,” in his work, Hereditary Genius in which a pattern of poor health was observed among leading clergy of the day. In G u y ’s statis- tics, clergy as a whole had one of the longest average lifespans of the privileged groups examined, but Galton dismissed this as due to the easy country life of many local clergy.

Galton (1872) also cited other support for his conclusion that there is no objective effi- cacy to prayers of petition and intercession. He claimed that mortality rates for mission- aries were not any better than for the general population and implied that they might be worse. He said that although one would ex- pect parents to be praying for the health of babies soon to be bom , the distribution of still-births is the same as that for all deaths. He noted that nobility were particularly sub- ject to insanity and that religious madness was quite common.

Although Galton rejected the hypothesis that there is objective efficacy to petitionary and intercessory prayer, he concluded his article by saying it is of subjective value as a means of ventilating emotional pain and as a way of experiencing a comforting sense of communion with God.

To the modern scientist, it is obvious that G alton’s evidence, while intriguing, does not justify the definitive tone of his con- elusion. In many cases, hard empirical data were lacking, and where statistics were pro- vided, they were obviously open to other interpretations. The most glaring of these is the dismissal o f the exceptionally long life- span of allclergy combined. The assumptions about who prays and is prayed for and in what manner are also grounds for challenge. Nevertheless, G alton’s study is noteworthy because of his pioneering courage in doing empirical study of a central religious prac-

tration and by the need to adjust to unusual situations. Thus, petitionary prayer is not necessarily just a neurotic flight from the unpleasant. It can also be a positive means of adjustment which reflects certain meta- physical assumptions.

Johnson ( 1945) has proposed a number of psychological effects of prayer which also can be construed as motivations for prayer. He suggests that they include awareness of needs, emotional catharsis, peace of mind, broader perspective on problems, decisions, emotional renewal, social responsiveness, joy, gratitude, acceptance of one’s losses, loyalty and perseverance, and integration of the personality (pp. 122-123). John so n ’s speculations have not yet been empirically investigated.

The Effects o f Verbal Prayer

The third group of studies contains re- search on the effects of verbal prayer. The first of these studies dates back to the begin- nings of psychology as a formal discipline. Francis Galton (1872) wrote an article in the Fortnightly Review entitled “ Statistical In- quiries Into the Efficacy of Prayer.” Gal- ton’s study of petitionary and intercessory prayer grew out of the research he did on clergy for the chapter on divines in his book Hereditary Genius. The artic le’s publi- cation sparked a controversy: Advocates of the objective efficacy of prayer were out- raged and the clerical community was of- fended. G alton’s article was reprinted in the first edition o f his book Inquiries into Human Faculty, but he bowed to ecclesias- tical pressure and consented to allow his publishers to omit it from the second edition.

Galton (1872) concluded that there is no statistical evidence for the objective value of petitionary and intercessory prayer. He based this conclusion on an examination of measures of well-being of various groups that one would expect to pray more or for whom many people would be likely to pray. After examining G u y ’s table of the average lifespan of various English affluent groups,

107

Page 5: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

PRAYER REVIEW

any guilt, hate, feeling of inferiority, or fear was encouraged.

The second part of the therapy empha- sized the limitless power and love of God. Partic ipan ts w ere urged to identify the inadequacies in their conceptions of God and abandon them in favor of the notion of an ever-present G od of love.

These first two portions of the therapy were apparently the warp and woof of a weekly two hour group therapy session. There seems also to have been an emphasis on obedience to the Law of Love in these therapy sessions, but the report is quite un- clear on this point.

A third aspect of the Prayer Therapy was personal prayer according to very specific guidelines. Based on the psychological pre- testing, each week a sealed envelope was given each member of the Prayer Therapy group. The envelope contained a slip of paper on which was written one detrimental personality aspect revealed by the psycho- logical tests. Each person’s homework was the elim ination or im provem ent o f this specific shortcoming through daily prayer. Four guidelines were given for this prayer. F irst of all, it was to be regular. Secondly, it was to be an act of surrender. The individual was to pray with the attitude of giving up his or her own desires and demands, seeking only to do G o d ’s will. Thirdly, the prayers were to be positive, affirmative statements rather than desperate begging or negative statem ents. While praying, the subjects were to visualize themselves as they desired to be. Finally, the prayer was to be recep- tive. Participants in the prayer therapy were asked to pray believing that they had already received what they requested of God.

T he experim ental design used in this study m akes causal inferences inappro- priate. There was no random assignment to treatment groups. Since the prayer therapy was a multifaceted program, no conclusions about verbal prayer itself can be drawn. The positive outcome of the Prayer Therapy group does, however, encourage further investigation o f a program o f religious

tice in spite of the church’s strong resistance to such scrutiny.

The next major study of the effects of verbal p rayer was published m ore than eighty years later. In 1957 William Parker’s experim ent was reported in Prayer Can Change Your L ife , a book written in a non- technical style and co-authored with Elaine St. Johns.

Parker’s subjects were forty-five volun- teers ranging in age from 22 to 60. They were all suffering from either psychosom atic sym ptom s or experiencing considerable subjective emotional distress. The subjects were assigned to one of three groups, each containing 15 persons. G roup 1 received weekly individual psychotherapy sessions. The people in this group expressed either a preference for psychotherapy or had been advised by their physicians to seek this treatment. G roup 2 was labeled the “ Ran- dom Pray-ers.” The subjects in this group were practicing Christians who agreed to pray daily that their specific problems would be overcome. They prayed in their accus- tomed manner. G roup 3 was the Prayer Therapy group which followed a specified program for religious growth.

The length o f the experiment was nine months. Pre-testing and post-testing was done using the following battery of psycho- logical tests: Rorschach, Szondi, Thematic Apperception Test, sentence completion, and w o rd a s so c ia tio n . A n im p artia l psychometrist compared the scores of each person and arbitrarily assigned a percentage of improvement to each of them. G roup 1, the psychotherapy group, showed an aver- age improvement of 65%. G roup 2, the Ran- dom Pray-ers, showed no improvement. G roup 3, the Prayer Therapy group, had an average improvement of 72%

The popular style in which Parker’s study was reported makes it difficult to know pre- cisely w ha t w as done w ith the P ray e r T h e ra p y g roup . In b ro ad o u tlin e , the therapy seems to have consisted of three parts. The first stressed honestly examining one’s mental life. The acknowledgement of

108

Page 6: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

J.R. FINNEY AND H.N. MALONY

each subject was asked to pray in their accustom ed way and to concentra te on adoration and praise. The E E G s of these six subjects, which were recorded during the prayer sessions, showed a shift to shorter duration half-waves. Thus, the E E G s were faster, disconfirming the hypothesis. The E E G s w ere exam in ed fo r p a t te rn s o f drowsiness or sleep and none were found. The subject whose E EG showed the largest shift was also the person who exhibited the most devout behavior. She often got up at 5a.m. in order to have an hour for prayer. During the laboratory prayer session she reported a flow of tears which was not un- common for her when she prayed.

In the fifth study of verbal prayer effects, Carson and Huss (1979) examined the use of prayer and Bible reading by the nursing staff in the treatment of chronic undifferentiated schizophrenics who had not responded to psychotherapy. Unfortunately, the study has serious design deficiencies which make the results only suggestive. The treatment group was comprised of volunteers whereas the control group was not. The lack of ran- dom assignm ent to treatm ent conditions makes it impossible to infer causality. Also, the tool for assessing change was a non- standardized instrument on which no relia- bility or validity information were reported. N either were any statistical tests o f signifi- cance used.

The experimental group was comprised of ten Christian schizophrenics who met indi- vidually w ith nursing s tu d en ts fo r ten w eeks. E ach m eeting w as opened and closed with a written prayer. A passage of Scripture was also read each time. T he prayers and Scriptures focused on the love oT G od and the worth of each individual to God. Thus, this study can be conceived as an investigation of prayer of adoration and praise. After the Scripture reading the stu- dents talked with their clients about any- thing they wished to discuss. The treatment of the control group was simply a thera- peutic relationship. Change was assessed with an inventory adapted from “ Elements

development which includes positive peti- tionary prayer as a therapeutic technique.

The third study of verbal prayer was a brief single-subject design by Sajwaj and Hedges (1973). It was a study of the effects of mealtime prayer on a six year old boy who was moderately retarded. The child was oppositional and an extreme behavior prob- lem in the home. For fifteen days, disrup- tions and appropriate table behaviors were recorded for ten-second intervals during dinner. The table grace had an adverse ef- feet on the child’s behavior. For the eight days when dinner began with prayer, the median was 63% of the mealtime being dis- rupted. This median fell to 15% on the no- grace days. Appropriate dinner behavior showed a similar pattern. A comparison of means was much less contrasting, however. On the average, dinners started with grace were only somewhat more disrupted than no-grace mealtimes.

The researchers theorized that the meal- tim e p ra y e r ac ted as a d isc r im in a tiv e stimulus, signaling to the child that dis- ruptive behavior would evoke parental at- tention. Qualitative observation supported this thesis. Sajwaj and Hedges (1973) con- eluded that further research on the stimulus functions of prayer is in order.

Surwillo and Hobson (1978) reported a fourth study on the effects of verbal prayer. They analyzed brain electrical activity dur- ing prayer which was primarily adoration and praise. The researchers hypothesized that there would be a slowing of electro- cortical rhythms as measured by electro- encephalograms (EEGs).

The subjects in the study were six adults, an equal number of each sex, whose ages ranged from 24 to 70 years. They were all members o f the Church of G od and regu- larly attended worship. O n a scale o f one to four, all subjects gave the maximum rating to the d eg ree to w hich p ra y e r w as a meaningful part o f their lives. The regular length of daily prayer for the various sub- jects ranged from 10 to 60 minutes.

During a twenty-minute prayer session

109

Page 7: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

PRAYER REVIEW

iety in the prayer group as measured by scores on the ST A I. T he prayer group partic ipan ts subjectively experienced a greater amount o f tension reduction than was indicated physiologically. There were no relationships between tension reduction in the prayer group and type of prayer style, intercessory or reflective. However, those prayer group members who rated prayer m ost im portant showed greater tension reduction on the EM G than did those who rated prayer less important. The researchers concluded that prayer was not shown to be effective in tension reduction although a trend in this direction was observed.

T o sum m arize , th e re have been six studies of the effects of verbal prayer re- ported in the literature. Two of these are particularly notew orthy. Parker and St. Johns (1957) pioneered in utilizing tech- niques of spiritual development, including positive petitionary prayer, as therapeutic interventions. Elkins et al. (1979) investi- gated the anxiety-reducing effects of verbal prayer of both an intercessory and a reflec- tive style and found that such verbal prayer is inferior to muscle relaxation and probably ineffective as a means to relieve anxiety.

The E ffects o f Contemplative Prayer

The last category of empirical research on prayer consists o f those studies investi- gating the effects of contemplative prayer.

Techniques of meditation common to the w orld’s major religions are often used as vehicles o f contemplative prayer. Research on meditation (Shapiro, 1980) and mysticism (Hood, 1977; Hood, et al., 1979; Strunk, 1965) provide further elucidation of the con- cept of contemplative prayer but a detailed discussion of these is beyond the scope of this article.

Only two studies of the effects of contem- plative prayer have been carried out. The first, by M arilyn M allory (1977) was a m ulti-faceted study o f m em bers o f the Discalced Carmelite O rder whose central enterprise is contem plative prayer. The

for a Psychological A ssessm ent,” an article by Snyder and Wilson (cf. Carson & Huss, 1979).

In co n tra s t to the contro l group, the experimental group grew in their ability to express anger and aggression. They also be- came more hopeful about changing their lives, exhibited more appropriate affect, and decreased in somatic complaints.

The last study on the effects of verbal p ra y e r co m p ared the re la tive anx ie ty - reducing effects of prayer and muscle relax- ation. It was carried out by Elkins, Anchor and Sandler (1979). In comparison to a con- trol group, the muscle relaxation caused significant reductions in objective and sub- jective measures of anxiety whereas prayer did not.

The 42 subjects were composed of almost equal numbers of males and females, and were members o f a Baptist church. M ost of them held at least a baccalaureate degree. T h e an x ie ty m ea su res w ere e le c tro - myogram (EM G ) recordings o f the frontalis muscle and Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxi- ety Inventory (STAI).

Subjects were randomly assigned to one o f three groups, a prayer group, a relaxation group, or a control group. The prayer group was subdivided into two subgroups. There was an in tercessory group in which the partic ipants favored p rayer that sought objective divine intervention. T he other subgroup preferred a reflective style o f p ra y e r in w hich the focus w as on the communication o f private feelings to God. Both groups prayed about events in their personal lives which were o f concern to them.

The subjects were tested before and after a ten day training period. Analysis o f co- variance show ed the m uscle re laxation group to have significantly reduced muscle tension compared to the controls. Analysis o f the STA I state anxiety scores revealed a similar pattern. The prayer group did not show significantly greater muscle relaxation as compared to the control group. However, there was a trend toward less subjective anx­

110

Page 8: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

J.R. FINNEY AND H.N. MALONY

chological/sociological variables, a canoni- cal correlation significant at the .01 level show ed the sum scores o f the m ystical prayer factors, 1 and 8 together, correlated with the psychological variables of extra- version (r = .23) and happy emotionality (r = .41) as well as a number o f other vari- ables. This finding suggested that mysti- cism is associa ted with positive mental health.

Mallory (1977) also performed a canonical correlation with the sumscores of factors 3,6, and 10, which were active visual prayer, active rational prayer, and troubled/dis- tracted prayer. These together correlated (p < .05) with the following psychological vari־ ables: neuroticism (r = .43), psychosomatic neuroticism (r = .32), introversion (r = .43) and unhappy emotions (r = .45). These correlations suggest that prayers involv- ing rational processes are associated with mental distress.

A third major finding of M allory’s study was that factors 1 and 8, the mystical fac- tors, together correlated with the mild views o f ascetic ism but not the s tric t views. Mild asceticism means trusting G od and a passive receptiv ity to G o d ’s love. F o r prayer, it implies a rejection of rational ac- tivity as a vehicle to divine communion. Severe asceticism implies such things as self-inflicted physical pain. The canonical correlation of the sumscores of the mystical prayer factors 1 and 8 and the mild asceti- cism items of the questionnaire was, how- ever, marginally significant (p < .20) and so cannot be considered very meaningful.

From the original 53 subjects, 14 were enlisted for E E G recordings during prayer. A significant reduction in alpha abundance during prayer was found (p < .04). How- ever, since this finding was based on a visual assessment of the E E G s and also because the laboratory facilities were grossly inade- quate, the results can only be considered suggestive.

It is most unfortunate that the statistical procedures used by Mallory make her re- suits untrustworthy. The factor analysis of

teaching of St. John of the Cross is a primary theological resource for this group. Mai- lory’s central hypothesis was that asceti- cism would not correlate with mystical development whereas psychological and re- ligious factors would do so.

Participants in the study were 53 nuns and friars from Holland and Dutch-speaking Belgium. T here were 44 females and 9 males. Their ages ranged from 24 to 77.

The subjects were given an 187 item ques- tionnaire on spirituality which contained six sections: prayer experiences, distractions in prayer, ascetical views, attitudes toward John of the Cross, ideas of God, and per- sonal assessment of one’s level of advance- ment in contemplation. Responses were on a six point scale except in the ideas of God section in which an eight point scale was used. A large number of the items in the questionnaire were drawn directly from the writings of John of the Cross.

Participants in the study were also given a number of psychological tests: the Amster- damse Biographische Vrangenlijst by G. J. Wile, which is a Dutch adaptation of H. J. E ysenck’s M audsley Personality Inven- tory; an adapted version of Barron’s Ego- Strength Scale; The Scale of Inter-Personal Values which has subscales for degree of conformity, degree o f independence, al- truism, leadership qualities, need for group support, and need for recogn ition ; the Vrangenlijst over Positief Innerlijk Welbe- vinden by H. J. Hermans which measures happy and unhappy emotionality; and the hysteria scale of the M M PI. The E E G s of a portion of the participants were also re- corded during prayer.

The first section of the questionnaire, which was on prayer experiences, contained 68 items. It was factor analyzed with an oblique rotation. Fifteen factors were identi- fied and named by two neutral judges.

Factors 1 and 8, which were labeled emo- tionally pleasurable mystical experience and emotional contemplation, were used as indi- cators of mysticism.

Correlating prayer experiences and psy­

111

Page 9: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

PRAYER REVIEW

The mean age was 22. All subjects had at least a high school education. Eighteen had an undergraduate college degree, and one had completed a m aster’s degree. Four of the subjects dropped out and were omitted from the analysis.

L oevinger’s test o f ego developm ent, which has a sentence completion format, was used in modified form. The altered inventory had a test-retest reliability of r = .92. The questionnaire was administered two months prior to the beginning of the retreat, two weeks before it, and one month after the end of the retreat. During the time of the retreat, novices were isolated from the outside world.

The mean scores for the three testings were 5.50, 5.48, and 5.78 in chronological order. The difference between the latter two means bordered on statistical significance (/ = 1.63, (45),/? < .058). Sacks noted that it is impossible to identify which aspect of the retreat was responsible for the results. In particular, the God-self interaction and the new social structure might both be major contributors to the increase in integration of the self-system.

Conclusions

This article has reviewed the empirical studies of prayer since 1872, about the time of the birth of psychology. They have been meager, to say the least, both in number and substantial findings. Nevertheless, several aspects of the work done to date on prayer are noteworthy. First, the studies of the development of the concept of prayer pro- vide the most conclusive findings since their results were convergent. From childhood through adolescence patterns consistent with Piaget’s stages of moral and cognitive development were observed (Godin & Van Rooey, 1959; Goldman, 1964; Long, Elkind & Spilka, 1967). Second, Welford (1947) gave empirical grounds for considering ver- bal petitionary prayer as more than just a neurotic flight from anxiety. His work also dem onstra ted one creative approach to studying motives for prayer with his rank

the first section of her spirituality question- naire is the prime deficiency. She factor analyzed 68 items with only 53 subjects. Nunnally (1967, p. 421) has recommended at least ten times as many subjects as van- ables in order to guard against taking advan- tage of chance, while G orsuch (1983, p. 148) requires 5 to 10 times as many subjects as variables and at least several hundred sub- jects. Mallory did not even have a 1:1 ratio o f su b jec ts to va riab les , and the to ta l number was far less than even 100.

F urther criticisms o f M allory’s (1977) statistical procedures are these: She failed to consider the prodigious instability of canon- icals; she seems to have treated factors as scales without giving any consideration to the general tendency for the internal consis- tency o f factor scales to reduce as one moves from the initial to later factors; no Eigenvalues are given to know how far the major factor analysis of the study was car- ried; no justification was given for doing an oblique rotation.

M allory’s (1977) work must also be criti- cized because her conclusions do not follow from her data in some cases. F or instance, she argued support of a particular chron- ology of prayer development from the inter- correlation of factors (pp. 40-41). Corre- lations are indications of association r nd imply nothing about sequence of events . She also stressed the correlation of mystical prayer with mild asceticism even though the canonical correlation was nonsignificant at the .05 level.

Howard Sacks (1979) also studied the ef- fects of contemplative prayer. He examined the effect o f Ignatius L oy o la ’s spiritual exercises on the integration of the self- system. He hypothesized that the thirty day retreat required of those joining the Jesuit order would result in increased cognitive integration. An increase in integration was observed, but it only bordered on being statistically significant.

Sacks (1979) studied 50 male subjects who made their retreats in six Jesuit centers throughout the United States and Canada.

112

Page 10: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

J.R. FINNEY AND H.N. MALONY

porary researcher. To some degree prayer is a sacrosanct subject. Some religious per־ sons are reluctant to subject it to empirical investigation. Nevertheless, the subject is of such import that prayer research should proceed.

R E F E R E N C E SAllison, J. (1966). Recent empirical studies of religious conversion experiences. Pastoral Psychology, 17, 21-33.Allison, J. (1968). Adaptive regression and intense reli- gious experiences. Journal o f Nervous and Mental Disorders, 145, 452-463.Arieti, S. (1967). The intrapsychic self. New York: Basic Books.Balthasar, H. U. von. (1981). Christian and non- Christian meditation. Word and Spirit, 2, 147-166.Balthasar, H. U. von. (1982). The Balthasar reader (M. Kehl & W. Loser, Eds.). New York: Crossroad.

Bouyer, L. (1963). The spirituality o f the New Testa- ment and the fathers (M.P. Ryan, Trans.). London: Bums and Oates. (Original work published 1960)Brown, L. B. (1966). Egocentric thought in petitionary prayer: A cross-cultural study. Journal o f Social Psy- chology, 68, 197-210.Brown, L. B. (1968). Some attitudes underlying peti- tionary prayer. In A. Godin (Ed.), From cry to word (pp. 65-84). Brussells: Lumen Vitae Press.Carrington, P. (1977). Freedom in meditation. New York: Anchor/Doubleday.Carrington, P., & Ephron, H. (1975). Meditation as an adjunct to psychotherapy. In S. Arieti, (Ed.), New dimensions in psychiatry: A world view (pp. 261-292). New York: WileyCarson, V., & Huss, K. (1979). Prayer, an effective therapeutic and teaching tool. Journal o f Psychiatric Nursing, 17, 34-37.Clark, W. H. (1958). The psychology o f religion. New York: Macmillan.Coe, G. A. (1900). The spiritual life. New York: Eaton & Mains.Coe, G. A. (1916). The psychology o f religion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Committee on Psychiatry and Religion. (1976). Mysti- cism: Spiritual quest or psychic disorder? Group for the Advancement o f Psychiatry, 9, 718-822.Cutten, G. B. (1908). The psychological phenomena o f Christianity. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

Deikman, A.J. (1966). Deautomatization and the mystic experience. Psychiatry, 29, 324-338.

ordering o f situations as to the extent to which they would evoke em otion, frus- tration, and prayer. Third, a beginning has been made to empirically assess the poten- tial o f verbal and contemplative prayer to enhance psychological health. Significant research on this issue has been done by El- kins et al. (1979), Parkerand St. Johns (1957) and Sacks (1979). Elkins and associates found that verbal intercessory and reflective prayer are probably not effective in reducing anxiety. Parker’s research raised the possi- bility that verbal positive petitionary prayer could form an effective core for group therapy. From Sacks it can be inferred that perhaps contemplative prayer may facilitate integration of the self-system.

There is opportunity for much further empirical investigation of prayer. O f course, replication and extension of the work done so far is needed. This would include investi- gation of the motives for prayer, the differ- ential effects of the various kinds of prayer, and the psychological processes active in prayer. A second research area is the effects o f p ray e r on re lig iosity and sp iritua l development. There seems to be no pub- lished w ork in th is a rea . F in a lly , the relationship of Christian prayer to prayer of the other major world religions is waiting to be done. All of the studies identified to date have been of prayer in the Judeo-Christian tradition. Prayer of the other world religions has been virtually ignored unless one is wil- ling to think of the meditation studies as investigations of prayer. This seems in- appropriate, however, because no consider- ation has been given to the techniques of meditation as being viewed by the partici- pants as means of relating to the divine. Prayer is a core religious practice and should not be neglected any more as a subject of research by those interested in the psy- chology of religion and the integration of psychology and Christian theology.

It should be rem em bered that G alton (1872) evoked a great deal of resistance from the religious community in studying prayer. This may still be the fate of the contem­

113

Page 11: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

PRAYER REVIEW

Macquarrie, J. (1966). Principles of Christian theology. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

Magee, J. B. (1957). Reality and prayer. Nashville: Abingdon.Mallory, M. (1977). Christian mysticism: Trans- cending techniques. Amsterdam: Van Gorcum Assen.Maupin, E. (1962). Zen Buddhism: A psychological review. Journal o f Consulting Psychology, 4, 362-378.Maupin, E. W. (1965). Individual differences in re- sponse to a Zen meditation exercise. Journal o f Con- suiting Psychology, 29, 139-145.Nunnally, J. C. (1967). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.Omstein, R. E. (1971). The techniques of meditation and their implications for modern psychology. In C. Naranjo & R. E. Ornstein (Eds.), On the psychology o f meditation (pp. 137-234). New York: Viking Press.Owens, C. M. (1972). The mystical experience: Facts and values. In J. White (Ed.), The highest state o f consciousness. Garden City: Anchor/Doubleday.Otis, L. S. (1974, November). If well-integrated but anxious, try TM. Psychology Today, pp. 45-46.Parker, W. R., & St. Johns, E. (1957). Prayer can change your life. Carmel, NY : Guideposts.Phillips, D. Z. (1965). The concept o f prayer. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Pratt, J. B. (1930). The religious consciousness. New York: Macmillan.Prince, R., & Savage, C. (1972). Mystical states and the concept of regression. In J. White (Ed.), The highest state o f consciousness. Garden City: Anchor/Double- day.Relton, H. M. (1925). The psychology of prayer and religious expereince. InO. Hardman (Ed.),Psychology and the church (pp. 55-102). London: Macmillan and Co.Sacks, H. L. (1979). The effect of spiritual exercises on the integration of self-system. Journal for the Scientific Study o f Religion, 18, 46-50.Sajwaj, T., & Hedges, D. (1973). A note on the effects of saying grace on the behavior of an oppositional re- tarded boy. Journal o f Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 711-712.Selbie, W. B. (1924). The psychology o f religion. Lon- don: Oxford University Press.Shapiro, Jr., D. H. (1980). Meditation: Self-regulation and altered states o f consciousness. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.Simpson, R. L. (1965). The interpretation o f prayer in the early church. Philadelphia: Westminster Press.Strommen, M. P. (1971). Research on religious development. New York: Hawthorn Books.Strong, A. L. (1909). The psychology o f prayer. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Sunden, Hj. (1974). Religionspsykologi. Stockholm: Med bidrag av Gustaf Stahlberg. '

Eikind, D. (1971). The development of religious under- standing in children and adolescents. In M. P. Strom- men, (Ed.), Research on religious development (pp. 655-685). New York: Hawthorn Books.Elkins, D ., Anchor, K. N ., & Sandler, H. M. (1979). Relaxation training and prayer behavior as tension re- duction techniques. Behavioral Engineering, 5, 81-87.Ellens, J. H. (1977). Communication theory and peti- tionary prayer. Journal o f Psychology and Theology, 5, 48-54.Fingarette, H. (1958). The ego and mystic selflessness. Psychoanalytic Review, 45, 5-40.Freud, S. (1961). Civilization and its discontents. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), Standard edition o f the com- plete psychological works o f Sigmund Freud (Vol. 21 pp. 57-146. London: Hogarth. (Original work pub- lished 1929)Galton, F. (1872). Statistical inquiries into the efficacy of prayer. Fortnightly Review, 18(Vol. XII, No. LXVIII New Series), 125-135.Godin, A. (1971). Some developmental tasks in Chris- tian education. In M. P. Strommen (Ed.), Research on religious development (pp. 109-154). New York: Hawthorn Books.Godin, A. & Van Rooey, B. (1959). Immanent justice and divine protection. Lumen Vitae, 14, 129-148.Goldman, R. (1964). Religious thinking from childhood to adolescence. London: Routledge.Goleman, D. (1971). Meditation as meta-therapy: Hypotheses toward a proposed fifth state of conscious- ness. Journal o f Transpersonal Psychology, 3, 1-25.Gorsuch, R. (1983). Factor analysis (2nd ed.). Hills- dale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Heiler, F. (1958). Prayer (S. McComb, Ed. and Trans.). New York: Galaxy Books/Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1932)Hodge, A. (1931). Prayer and its psychology. London: Society for Promoting Christian Knowledge.Hood, Jr., R. W. (1977). Differential triggering of mys- tical experience as a function of self actualization. Re- view o f Religious Research, 18, 264-270.Hood, Jr., R. W., Hall, J. R., Watson, P. J., & Bider- man, M. (1979). Personality correlates of the report of mystical experience. Psychological Reports, 44, 804- 806.Horton, W. M. (1931). A psychological approach to theology. New York: Harper & Brothers.James, W. (1963). The varieties o f religious experience. New York: University Books. (Original work published 1902)Johnson, P.E. (1945). Psychology o f religion. New York: Abingdon-Cokesbury.Kris, E. (1936). The psychology of caricature. Inter- national Journal o f Psychoanalysis, 17, 285-303.Long, D ., Eikind, D ., & Spilka, B. (1967). The child’s conception of prayer. Journal for the Scientific Study o f Religion, 6, 101-109.

114

Page 12: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

J.R. FINNEY AND H.N. MALONY

Valentine, C. H. (1929). Modern psychology and the validity o f Christian experience. London: Society for Promoting Christian Knowledge.Welford, A. T. (1947). Is religious behavior dependent upon affect or frustration? Journal o f Abnormal andSocial Psychology, 42, 310-319.

A U T H O R SFINNEY, JOHN R. Address: 2261 N. Villa Heights, Pasadena, California 91107. Title: Doctoral Candidate. Degrees: BA, Wayne State University; MDiv. Du- buque Theological Seminary. Specializations: Inte- gration of psychology and theology, Christian mysti- cism, existential psychotherapy.

M ALO NY, H. NEW TO N. Address: Graduate School of Psychology, Fuller Theological Seminary, 177 N. Madison Avenue, Pasadena, California 91101. Title: Professor of Psychology. Degrees: MDiv, Yale Divinity School; PhD, George Peabody College. Specializations: Social issues, organizational psychol- ogy, psychology of religion.

Strunk, Jr., O. (1959). Readings in the psychology o f religion. New York: Abingdon Press.

Strunk, Jr., O. (1965). Mature religion: A psychological study. New York: Abingdon Press.

Surwillo, W. W., & Hobson, D. P. (1978). Brain elec- trical activity during prayer. Psychological Reports, 43, 135-143.

Thouless, R. H. (1956). An introduction to the psy- chology o f religion. Cambridge: The University Press. (Original work published 1923)Thouless, R. H., & Brown, L. B. (1964). Petitionary prayer: Belief in its appropriateness and causal efficacy among adolescent girls. In A. Godin (Ed.), From re- ligious experience to a religious attitude (pp. 123-136). Brussels: Lumen Vitae Press.

Tinsley, E. J. (1969). Mysticism. In A. Richardson (Ed.), Λ dictionary o f Christian theology (pp. 225-226). Philadelphia: Westminster Press.Tillich, P. (1967). Systematic theology (Three vol- umes in one). New York: University of Chicago Press/Harper & Row.

115

Page 13: Empirical Studies of Christian Prayer: A Review of the Literature · 2019-12-10 · intercession, thanksgiving, and adoration. Mystical prayer has come to be more com- monly known

Copyright and Use:

As an ATLAS user, you may print, download, or send articles for individual use according to fair use as defined by U.S. and international copyright law and as otherwise authorized under your respective ATLAS subscriber agreement.

No content may be copied or emailed to multiple sites or publicly posted without the copyright holder(sV express written permission. Any use, decompiling, reproduction, or distribution of this journal in excess of fair use provisions may be a violation of copyright law.

This journal is made available to you through the ATLAS collection with permission from the copyright holder( s). The copyright holder for an entire issue of ajournai typically is the journal owner, who also may own the copyright in each article. However, for certain articles, the author of the article may maintain the copyright in the article. Please contact the copyright holder(s) to request permission to use an article or specific work for any use not covered by the fair use provisions of the copyright laws or covered by your respective ATLAS subscriber agreement. For information regarding the copyright holder(s), please refer to the copyright information in the journal, if available, or contact ATLA to request contact information for the copyright holder(s).

About ATLAS:

The ATLA Serials (ATLAS®) collection contains electronic versions of previously published religion and theology journals reproduced with permission. The ATLAS collection is owned and managed by the American Theological Library Association (ATLA) and received initial funding from Lilly Endowment Inc.

The design and final form of this electronic document is the property of the American Theological Library Association.