54
Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder by Alexander R. Daros A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Department of Psychology University of Toronto © Copyright by Alexander R. Daros 2012

Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

  • Upload
    lytuong

  • View
    257

  • Download
    9

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder

by

Alexander R. Daros

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts

Department of Psychology University of Toronto

© Copyright by Alexander R. Daros 2012

Page 2: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

ii

Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder

Alexander R. Daros

Master of Arts

Department of Psychology University of Toronto

2012

Abstract

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a serious mental illness characterized by emotion

dysregulation. Symptoms related to emotion are thought to contribute to difficulties in perceiving

emotional expressions. Individuals with BPD and demographically matched healthy controls

completed a task assessing the recognition of happy, sad, and neutral facial expressions at two

intensities. Patients with BPD demonstrated comparable performance on the recognition of very

happy and very sad facial expression but were significantly less accurate on neutral expressions.

Patients with BPD were also significantly worse in recognizing mildly happy facial expressions,

however the severity of current depressive symptoms intervened this relationship. There was

evidence that perceptual biases within BPD are unique from mood-congruent biases typically

found in major depressive disorder. The findings advance research on the topic of emotion

perception in BPD and suggest important new lines of investigation that may be useful for

delineating the nature of emotion dysregulation in BPD.

Page 3: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

iii

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to many people for their assistance and support on this project. From

conception to completion, my graduate advisor and mentor, Dr. Anthony C. Ruocco supported

me throughout the long process. Anthony, thank you for keeping me on track, pushing me to

think critically, and helping me to become a better writer. My subsidiary advisor, Dr.

Konstantine Zakzanis helped shape this project for the better by encouraging me to quantitatively

review previous literature on the topic. In doing so, my understanding on the subject matter has

increased and new skills in summarizing and analyzing data were acquired. I am also gracious

for the helpful comments and support received by Dr. Amanda Uliaszek, my thesis reviewer.

This was a thesis committee so well grounded in clinical research that I have gained considerable

knowledge throughout the entire process.

This project relied on the teamwork of every single one of the members of the Clinical

Neurosciences Laboratory. One person cannot undertake the amount of work that goes into a

clinical research study. To the lot of you, I say thank you for your exceptionally hard work and

commitment to the projects in the lab. The attitude in the lab was always positive thanks to all of

your support and affinity for research.

Friends and family provided an astonishing amount of support and cathartic release

throughout the writing of this thesis. I really do appreciate all of the kind words, praise, and

constructive feedback that I have received during this year of intense research.

Page 4: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

iv

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments ........................................................................................................................ iii            Table of Contents...........................................................................................................................iv            List of Tables .................................................................................................................................vi            List of Figures................................................................................................................................vii            Chapter 1.........................................................................................................................................1            1 Introduction................................................................................................................................1           1.1   Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation......................................1               1.2   Measuring Emotion Perception Difficulties in BPD....................................................4             1.3   Emotion Recognition in BPD.......................................................................................7             1.4   Emotional Acuity in BPD ............................................................................................9         1.5   Limitations of Previous Research ..............................................................................10         1.6   The Present Study ......................................................................................................11        Chapter 2.......................................................................................................................................13            2        Method.....................................................................................................................................13               2.1 Participants ..................................................................................................................13               2.2        Measures......................................................................................................................14     2.2.1 Diagnostic Interviews………………………………………………………14 2.2.2 Self-Report Measures………………………………………………………15 2.2.3 Neuropsychological Measures……………………………………………..15 2.2.4 Penn Emotional Acuity Test……………………………………………….16     2.3   Statistical Plan…........................................................................................................17          Chapter 3.......................................................................................................................................19            

Page 5: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

v

3        Results......................................................................................................................................19           3.1          Demographic and Clinical Characteristics.................................................................19             3.2   PEAT Accuracy..........................................................................................................19             3.3   Analysis of Current Mood…………………………………………………………..20       3.4 Secondary Analyses………………………………………………………………...22 3.4.1 Reaction Time……………………………………………………………...22 3.4.2 Comorbidity and Psychotropic Medications……………………………….22      Chapter 4.......................................................................................................................................23         4          Discussion..............................................................................................................................23           4.1   Strengths and Limitations..........................................................................................28               4.2   Implications and Conclusions....................................................................................29          References.....................................................................................................................................31            Tables............................................................................................................................................40     Figures...........................................................................................................................................46  

Page 6: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

vi

List of Tables

Table 1. Description of Emotion Perception Tasks……………………………………………40

Table 2. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Patients with Borderline Personality

Disorder and Healthy Controls………………………………………….…………………….41

Table 3. Clinical Characteristics for Patients Diagnosed with BPD for the Present Study…..42

Table 4. Accuracy Results from the PEAT for BPD and HC…………………..………………43

Table 5. Response Patterns Based on PEAT Stimulus Classification……………………….…44

Table 6. Reaction Time (RT) Results from the PEAT for BPD and HC………………………..45

Page 7: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

vii

List of Figures

Figure 1. Response patterns on (A) mildly happy stimuli and (B) neutral stimuli of the PEAT for

BPD (n = 30) and HC (n = 27) participants.……………………………………………………46

Figure 2. Mediation analyses with BDI score as a mediator and (A) TC score, (B) NC score, (C)

MHC score as dependent variables……………………………………………..……………...47

Page 8: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

1

Chapter 1

Introduction 1

1.1 Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a serious mental illness that affects 1-2% of the general

population and is characterized by a pervasive pattern of emotional instability, unstable

interpersonal relationships, impulsive aggression, and suicidality (Lenzenweger & Pastore, 2007;

Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, 2001). Patients with BPD typically suffer from increased

economic hardship and comorbidity with Axis I disorders leading to a disproportionally higher

utilization of inpatient psychiatric services (Comtois et al., 2003; Grant et al., 2008). A

staggering two-thirds of patients with BPD present with three or more Axis I diagnoses, the most

common being major depressive disorder (MDD) and bipolar disorder (BD) (Zimmerman &

Mattia, 1999). Prevalence of BPD among psychiatric outpatients is approximately 15%

(Torgersen et al., 2001) and this illness is often overlooked and underdiagnosed in many patients

(Skodol et al., 2002). Additionally, BPD is associated with a high prevalence of self-injurious

behaviour (Clarkin, Widiger, Frances, Hurt, & Gilmore, 1983; McGlashan et al., 2005), suicide

attempts (Soloff, Lynch, & Kelly, 2002; Soloff, Lynch, Kelly, Malone, & Mann, 2000; Zisook,

Goff, Sledge, & Shuchter, 1994) and completed suicides (Frances, Fyer, & Clarkin, 1986; Paris,

Brown, & Nowlis, 1987).

Emotion dysregulation has been hypothesized as the core clinical feature of BPD (Linehan,

1993; Selby & Joiner, 2009). Difficulties with emotion regulation may, at least in part, account

for the substantial comorbidity of BPD with mood and other related disorders, as well as their

increased risk for suicide. Linehan’s (1993) biosocial developmental model remains a prominent

Page 9: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

2

theoretical framework for describing emotion dysregulation in BPD. Biological predispositions

such as a hyperbolic temperament and an emotional vulnerability are hypothesized to lead

individuals with BPD to experience a heightened sensitivity to emotional stimuli, experiences of

emotions as more intense than other individuals, and slow returns to baseline emotional arousal

(Zanarini & Frankenburg, 2007). In conjunction with vulnerabilities, BPD develops in the

context of an invalidating environment in which communication of emotional experience is met

by erratic, inappropriate, and extreme responses by others. The individual’s emotional behavior

is negated, rejected, or dismissed, even though it may be a valid response on the part of the

individual (Linehan, 1993). Emotion dysregulation is also theorized to culminate in the

development of behavioural dysregulation. In particular, acts that these individuals find difficult

to control but are utilized in situations where they seek to shift attention away from unpleasant

emotional states (e.g., self-injurious behaviour). Rumination also appears to play a role in BPD

and can lead to an amplification of negative affect (Donaldson & Lam, 2004; Lavender &

Watkins, 2004) suggesting a second possible link underlying the comorbidity of BPD with

MDD. In one study, patients with BPD reported higher levels of rumination compared to

individuals diagnosed with MDD (Abela, Payne, & Moussaly, 2003). This result was surprising

given the strong and largely specific connection between rumination and depression (Abela et

al., 2003; Just & Alloy, 1997).

Research on affective instability and emotional intensity in BPD has provided support for the

biosocial developmental model. Individuals with BPD have reported more mood variability

throughout the day compared to patients with MDD and healthy individuals (Cowdry, Gardner,

O'Leary, Leibenluft, & Rubinow, 1991). Patients with BPD also have shown greater emotional

lability with regard to anger, anxiety, and the oscillation between depression and anxiety

Page 10: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

3

compared to patients with MDD, Bipolar II disorder, and cyclothymia (Koenigsberg et al., 2002).

In one experience sampling study that took place over the duration of one month, patients with

BPD regularly experienced more extreme changes in hostile, fearful, and sad affect than

depressed patients (Trull et al., 2008). Research has also reported a positive correlation between

negative emotional intensity in BPD and intense negative emotions following social interactions

(Russell, Moskowitz, Zuroff, Sookman, & Paris, 2007). Patients with BPD report greater

subjective intensity of negative emotions as compared to healthy controls (Levine, Marziali, &

Hood, 1997), and the magnitude of this intensity is strongly associated with severity of BPD

symptoms (Cheavens et al., 2005; Rosenthal, Cheavens, Lejuez, & Lynch, 2005; Yen, Zlotnick,

& Costello, 2002).

Research from neuroimaging studies generally supports an underlying pathology for emotion

dysregulation in BPD. Decreased amygdala and hippocampal volumes have been reported in

structural neuroimaging studies of patients with BPD, regions thought to be involved in the

regulation of emotional experiences (Driessen et al., 2000; Ruocco, Amirthavasagam, &

Zakzanis, 2012; Schmahl, Vermetten, Elzinga, & Bremner, 2003; Tebartz van Elst et al., 2003).

Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated an altered baseline metabolism in limbic

and paralimbic regions linked to emotion generation in response to stressful challenges (Schmahl

et al., 2006). In addition, functional neuroimaging studies have found heightened reactivity to

negatively valenced pictures in the cingulate, insula, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients

with BPD when compared to healthy controls (see Ruocco, Amirthavasagam, Choi-Kain, &

McMain, 2012). Koenigsberg and colleagues (2009) noted that patients with BPD utilized

different brain areas, an action-prone system, while looking at negatively valenced images

compared to the less reactive system used by controls. These results led the authors to

Page 11: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

4

hypothesize that emotion dysregulation has downstream influences on emotion perception in

BPD, which may have implications for the appropriateness of social behavior and interpersonal

interactions in these patients. Perhaps most critical for this process is the role of the prefrontal

cortex in differentiating negative expressions of emotion (Adolphs, 2002; Hariri, Bookheimer, &

Mazziotta, 2000; Narumoto et al., 2000). Recent neuroimaging studies have provided evidence

of a dysfunction of prefrontal systems during negative emotion processing in BPD (Koenigsberg

et al., 2009; Ruocco, Medaglia, Ayaz, & Chute, 2010). Understanding how individuals with BPD

perceive emotions may provide new avenues of research that can implicate brain areas involved

in the neuropathology of BPD. To facilitate regulation of emotional responses to evocative

stimuli, the ability to control attention while in contact with these stimuli is necessary (von

Ceumern-Lindenstjerna et al., 2010). If emotionally valenced stimuli are perceived in a

hyperarousing or hypervigilant manner in BPD, disengaging one’s attention from emotional

stimuli becomes more difficult due to emotion dysregulation (Linehan, 1993). Whether the

recruitment of additional attentional resources leads to benefits or deficits in the perception of

emotionally valenced stimuli within BPD, however, remains to be fully clarified.

1.2 Measuring Emotion Perception Difficulties in BPD

Individuals with BPD have been described as highly vigilant to social stimuli, especially for

social cues that might signal social threat or rejection (Linehan, 1995). Patterns in visually

recognizing and identifying emotional facial expressions may hold clues as to whether

individuals with BPD receive added benefits or consequences from their heightened sensitivity to

such stimuli. Recognition of six basic emotional expressions (happiness, sadness, fear, disgust,

anger, surprise) has been argued to be universal across cultures because of the highly salient and

evolutionary significant information they contain (Ekman, 1992; Ekman, Levenson, & Friesen,

Page 12: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

5

1983). Many psychiatric conditions have been associated with deficits in emotion recognition,

including MDD, BD, schizophrenia, panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-

compulsive disorder, and social phobia, among others (Bolte & Poustka, 2003; Corcoran,

Woody, & Tolin, 2008; Derntl, Seidel, Kryspin-Exner, Hasmann, & Dobmeier, 2009; Feinberg,

Rifkin, Schaffer, & Walker, 1986; Kohler et al., 2003; Rector, Daros, Bradbury, & Richter,

2012). Accurate emotion recognition serves as a cognitive cornerstone for appropriate social

functioning and the capacity to recognize internal states from external cues may be important for

promoting empathy, trust, and prosocial behavior (Marsh, Kozak, & Ambady, 2007). In turn,

enduring misinterpretations due to inaccurate recognition are likely to result in emotional

disturbances, inadequate social behavior, and impaired social functioning in a variety of

psychiatric disorders.

While the literature on emotion perception research is vast, three main aspects of facial emotion

perception have frequently been studied in BPD (see Table 1). The first can be labeled emotion

recognition and is usually measured by presenting a series of single photographs of emotional

facial expressions and having the respondent ascribe a particular emotion to the stimulus in some

fashion. Photographs typically remain static rather than progressively morphed and they

primarily employ prototypic expressiveness for each emotion displayed (i.e., 100%

expressiveness). A second method used to measure emotion perception has frequently been

termed emotion discrimination in which respondents are asked to distinguish between two

emotions. One variation of this experimental design presents a single photo containing, for

example, 40% anger and 60% happiness, and then asks participants to state which emotion they

see (e.g., Domes et al., 2008). A further design presents two stimuli at the same time and asks the

participant to determine which face shows more emotion. This second design has more flexibility

Page 13: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

6

in that the participant can be presented with a combination of a neutral face and an emotional

face, two faces displaying negative emotions, or one face with a positive expression and the

other with a negative expression. Finally, a third test of emotional perception involves the acuity

of or threshold for perceiving emotions. These tasks typically present a neutral face first, which

is then followed by an incremental blend of a target emotional expression, which represents a

successive approximation of the target emotion from mild to more extreme representations of

this expression. The respondent is asked to stop the progression of morphs when they are certain

that they perceive an emotion in the face and can articulate that emotion. An alternate variant of

this task randomly presents individual stimuli at different levels of blending of emotions (e.g.,

50% neutral/50% target emotion; 10% neutral/90% target emotion).

A variety of facial emotion stimulus sets have been developed to study emotion recognition in

healthy individuals and patients with psychiatric illnesses. Most emotion recognition studies of

BPD utilized Ekman and Friesen’s (1976) Pictures of Facial Affect (POFA), which includes 14

actors (eight female and six male) and their portrayal of six basic emotions (happy, sad, angry,

fear, disgust, and surprise) and neutral facial expressions. Many researchers favour the POFA

because these photographs have been rated by large normative groups and can be reliably

identified in a number of cultures with agreement rates often close to 100% in Western societies

(Ekman, 1992). There are, however, several drawbacks to using the POFA, including a high

ceiling and a lack of ethnic diversity of the actors (all Caucasian), although a more diverse set of

faces has been developed (i.e., Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion, or

JACFEE; Ekman and Matsumoto, 1992). The JACFEE collection consists of 56 colour

photographs portraying each of the seven emotional categories as in the POFA. These stimuli

comprise an equal number of male and female actors as well as Caucasian and Japanese actors.

Page 14: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

7

An abbreviated recognition task is the Fear-Anger-Neutral (FAN) test, which was designed to

target the more difficult discrimination of negative and neutral facial expressions (Gur et al.,

2002; Schneider et al., 2006). A more recently developed set of standardized facial emotion

stimuli is contained within the Penn Emotion Recognition Task (ER40) (Gur et al., 2002; Kohler,

Turner, Gur, & Gur, 2004), which consists of 40 stimuli displaying five emotions: happy, sad,

anger, fear, and neutral (termed “no emotion”). Advantages of the ER40 are that it contains

ethnically diverse actors and exists in a completely standardized and computerized form. The

POFA and JACFEE, on the other hand, must be assembled into an experimental paradigm by

individual investigators, the specific parameters of which may differ across studies. Importantly,

these tests depict emotional stimuli at 100% expressivity (i.e., the prototypic expression) as

opposed to blending or morphing two emotions together into more emotionally ambiguous facial

expressions.

1.3 Emotion Recognition in BPD

Several studies have demonstrated that patients with BPD may be less accurate than non-

psychiatric comparison participants in their recognition of negative facial emotions, including

anger, fear, disgust, and sadness (Bland, Williams, Scharer, & Manning, 2004; Dyck et al., 2009;

Levine et al., 1997; Unoka, Fogd, Fuzy, & Csukly, 2011). These findings were initially thought

to reflect a more generalized deficit in the perception of negative emotions in BPD. Other

research, however, indicated that these patients may be less accurate in recognizing faces

showing no emotion (i.e., neutral) (Wagner & Linehan, 1999) as well as surprised facial

expressions (Lynch et al., 2006). These findings could not be explained by a speed-accuracy

tradeoff for patients with BPD (Dyck et al., 2009; Merkl et al., 2010; Minzenberg, Poole, &

Vinogradov, 2006). Contrary to these studies were a smaller number of investigations which

Page 15: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

8

suggested that patients with BPD may actually be more accurate in their recognition of fearful

(Merkl et al., 2010; Wagner & Linehan, 1999) and surprised facial expressions (Unoka et al.,

2011). Perhaps more consistent, however, is the finding of no significant emotion recognition

deficits in patients with this illness (Dyck et al., 2009; Lynch et al., 2006; Minzenberg et al.,

2006; Robin et al., 2012; von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna et al., 2007). Based on these results, it

remains unclear whether patients with BPD indeed show deficits in facial emotion recognition,

and if they do, whether these deficits might be limited to specific categories of emotion.

Diagnostic comorbidity with BPD may have a significant impact on facial emotion recognition

capacities. Given that mood disorders are prevalent in patients with BPD (Zimmerman & Mattia,

1999), the contribution of mood state to biases in emotion perception is important to consider in

this population. For example, patients with MDD show mood-congruent biases in facial emotion

recognition as well as in their memory for emotion words (Murphy et al., 1999; Watkins, Vache,

Verney, Muller, & Mathews, 1996). Studies of facial emotion recognition have reported no

differences in accuracy or reaction time (RT) for patients with BPD in regards to comorbid

psychiatric conditions (Bland et al., 2004; Domes et al., 2008; Dyck et al., 2009; Robin et al.,

2012; Unoka et al., 2011), while other investigators chose to exclude patients with current

depression to avoid this confound altogether (Dyck et al., 2009; Merkl et al., 2010; Minzenberg

et al., 2006). Unfortunately, the results of mood rating scales were not reported in most emotion

recognition studies of BPD, making it difficult to understand the contributions of patients’

current mood to emotion perception deficits in these studies.

To shed light on this inconsistency, Domes and colleagues (2009) provided a comprehensive

narrative review on the topic of facial emotion perception in BPD, concluding that these patients

may show subtle impairments in labeling emotional expressions along with a tendency to

Page 16: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

9

perceive emotionally ambiguous faces (i.e., neutral ones) more negatively. A negative perceptual

bias is consistent with previous research demonstrating a heightened negative perception of other

persons in BPD (Arntz & Veen, 2001; Wagner & Linehan, 1999) and in over-attributing anger to

ambiguous facial stimuli (Domes et al., 2008), which could reflect an anticipation of

interpersonal rejection or social threat. The results of a recent meta-analysis on emotion

recognition in BPD suggests that these individuals may have selective deficits in the recognition

of neutral (no emotion), disgust, and angry facial expressions, although there is some evidence to

suggest a more generalized difficulty recognizing negatively valenced emotions (Daros,

Zakzanis, & Ruocco, 2012).

1.4 Emotional Acuity in BPD

Neuroimaging research on individuals with BPD has found increased metabolic brain reactivity

to negatively valenced pictures accompanied by slower returns to baseline, suggesting a

hypervigilant response when compared with healthy controls (Herpertz et al., 2001; Koenigsberg

et al., 2009). If BPD patients are acutely sensitive (i.e., hypervigilant) to emotional stimuli they

should recognize emotions at lower thresholds when compared to healthy individuals. One way

of testing this hypothesis incorporates the use of morphing tasks, in which emotional stimuli are

morphed from neutral to full expressivity. Three studies used similar methodological designs to

test emotional acuity in BPD. Each presented a series of morphed stimuli in order from 0%

(neutral) to 100% (emotion) in 5% increments. Participants were instructed to press a stop button

when they felt they were aware of the emotion the face was beginning to show. This instruction

was intended to reduce false alarms. Stopping the sequence and then giving the correct answer

for the emotion provided a detection threshold based on the increment value. The increment

value was then averaged for multiple presentations of six basic emotions (happy, sad, anger,

Page 17: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

10

fear, disgust, and surprise) (Domes et al., 2008; Jovev et al., 2011; Lynch et al., 2006). Emotion

sensitivity was therefore operationally defined as the ability to recognize emotion at lower levels

of intensity and it was expected that individuals with BPD would exhibit this pattern of response.

In two studies of emotion sensitivity, a comparable detection threshold was found in patients

with BPD and healthy controls (Domes et al., 2008; Jovev et al., 2011), while a third study

demonstrated emotion sensitivity (i.e., lower detection threshold) in patients with BPD that was

consistent for each of the six basic emotions, including happiness (Lynch et al., 2006).

Furthermore, Lynch and colleagues (2006) found that this hypersensitivity to emotional

expressions was not associated with impulsivity and inaccurate responding in the BPD group.

Given that the stimuli were not randomly presented but morphed starting from neutral,

impulsiveness could still play a role in patients’ response styles on this task. Overall, the results

of these studies provide mixed findings for the nature of recognition at milder intensities of

emotion in BPD and how this is related to emotional sensitivity.

1.5 Limitations of Previous Research

There are several limitations to previous studies of emotion perception in BPD. First, most

patient samples contained high levels of diagnostic comorbidity, particularly with mood and

anxiety disorders. Very few of these studies incorporated symptom rating scales at the time of

testing to evaluate possible state-related biases in emotion perception. Second, given that many

studies used the POFA, it is important to consider ceiling effects associated with these stimuli

and the lack of ethnically diverse actors. Third, although patients with BPD have been shown to

be less accurate than healthy individuals in recognizing specific facial displays of emotion, most

patients were at or above 80% accuracy on these tasks. This suggests a subtle deficit as opposed

to a frank impairment in emotion recognition. Fourth, the instructional sets used for facial

Page 18: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

11

emotion recognition tasks are important to consider as they may impact the relative emphasis

given to accuracy versus speed, which could lead to systematic biases in test findings. Although

most emotion perception studies place an emphasis on both speed and accuracy, patients with

BPD might employ a strategy that maximizes the quickness of their responses while sacrificing

accuracy. The results of our recent meta-analysis on this topic, however, suggests that this is

likely not the case, as patients were found to be both slower to respond to stimuli and less

accurate in their performances (Daros et al., 2012). Nevertheless, future work should consider

the effects of emphasizing accuracy over speed, or allowing unlimited time to respond, to

determine whether patients with BPD show these subtle deficits in facial emotion perception

under different task conditions.

1.6 The Present Study

The current study seeks to clarify the manner in which individuals with BPD respond to positive,

neutral, and negative facial expressions by taking advantage of a computerized test that assesses

the recognition of emotional facial stimuli presented to participants. This test effectively

combines two areas of perception research (emotion recognition and acuity) because it

determines if participants can recognize and classify varying intensities of three facial

expressions (happy, neutral, and sad). Therefore, it will illuminate the ways in which patients

with BPD may interpret milder expressions of happiness and sadness, as well as examine full-

threshold stimuli. A significant limitation of previous research is that the error patterns on neutral

stimuli are not reported, making it difficult to determine whether BPD patients are misattributing

a positive or negative emotion to these faces. The current study will provide a detailed

assessment of these responses to determine if there are any systematic biases in BPD patients’

responses to these trials. Given that most research on emotion perception in BPD has not

Page 19: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

12

examined the relationship of mood state with performance on emotion tasks, the current study

will also examine the potential contribution of current depressive symptomatology on emotional

acuity in BPD.

The specific hypotheses for this study are as follows: (1) Patients with BPD will not differ from

healthy controls in the recognition of very expressive happy and sad faces, however, accuracy at

recognizing neutral expressions is expected to differ significantly, in concordance with earlier

meta-analytic findings. (2) Current mood state, particularly in the form of depressive symptoms,

is expected to be significantly associated with emotion recognition performance in patients with

BPD. This finding was expected to relate only to emotions of positive valence (i.e., very happy

and mild happy expressions) given that mood-congruent biases demonstrated by clinically and

non-clinically depressed samples typically involve decreased recognition accuracy on positive

emotions (Bourke, Douglas, & Porter, 2010; Gray et al., 2006; Surguladze et al., 2004). (3)

Finally, it was expected that BPD would report negative emotional expressions for faces

displaying no emotion (i.e., neutral faces) consistent with literature reporting a negative bias

upon viewing neutral facial stimuli (Dyck et al., 2009; Wagner & Linehan, 1999). In addition to

these specific hypotheses, exploration was planned to determine whether emotion sensitivity

could be demonstrated using the measures in the study.

Page 20: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

13

Chapter 2

Method 2

2.1 Participants

As part of a larger study of neurocognitive functioning in individuals with BPD, participants

were enrolled through the Clinical Neurosciences Laboratory at the University of Toronto

Scarborough. They were recruited from the Borderline Personality Disorder outpatient clinic at

the Center for Addiction and Mental Health as well as through online and print advertisements in

Toronto, Canada. Participants were selected for inclusion in this study if criteria were met for

BPD according to the Fourth Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Additional inclusion criteria for

BPD patients were as follows: at least 18 years old at the time of recruitment, English-speaking,

and able and willing to provide written informed consent to participate in this study. Exclusion

criteria included a history of schizophrenia or any psychotic disorder, bipolar I disorder, current

alcohol or non-alcohol substance use disorder, lifetime eating disorder requiring hospitalization,

mental retardation (IQ < 70), any neurological or severe somatic disorder, and significant head

trauma.

Healthy controls were matched to patients with BPD by age, gender, and education. They were

recruited from the community through print and online advertisements. Inclusion criteria for

healthy controls were the following: at least 18 years old at the time of recruitment, English-

speaking, and able and willing to provide written informed consent. Exclusion criteria included

any personal or family history (first- and second-degree relatives) of psychiatric illness, medical

and neurologic illness that could affect brain functioning (e.g., hypothyroidism, seizure disorder,

Page 21: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

14

dementia), significant head trauma, and a history of learning or developmental disorders (e.g.,

attention-deficit disorder, autism).

This study received approval from the Research Ethics Board at the Centre for Addiction and

Mental Health and the University of Toronto. Potential participants completed an initial phone

screen to assess the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Eligible individuals were invited to visit the

University of Toronto Scarborough where all procedures took place. After a complete

description of the study, individuals provided written informed consent to participate in the

research protocol. Participants were required to provide a negative urine toxicology screen on the

day of testing before completing the laboratory procedures. They were compensated up to $100

for their participation.

2.2 Measures

All participants completed semi-structured diagnostic interviews administered by bachelor- and

doctoral-level diagnostic interviewers trained to reliably administer these measures and were

directly supervised by a licensed clinical psychologist (ACR). Interviews were conducted

without knowledge of the status of the recruited individual (i.e., whether they were a patient or

healthy control). Narratives for each participant were prepared based on all of the available

information obtained during the interviews and discussed in a diagnostic consensus meeting with

multiple diagnostic interviewers in attendance.

2.2.1 Diagnostic Interviews

The Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P; First,

Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002) is a diagnostic interview to assess major psychiatric

disorders in terms of categorical constructs. Multiple studies have found the reliabilities of Axis I

Page 22: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

15

diagnoses to be good, although there are differences based on the psychiatric condition (e.g.,

Lobbestael, Leurgans, & Arntz, 2011; Skre, Onstad, Torgersen, & Kringlen, 1991). The SCID-

I/P has also demonstrated superior validity over other standard interviews at clinical intake sites

(e.g., Basco et al., 2000; Fennig, Naisberg-Fennig, Craig, Tanenberg-Karant, & Bromet, 1996).

The Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1997)

is a reliable, semi-structured interview designed to assess for DSM-IV Axis II disorders with

good interrater reliability and validity (Jane, Pagan, Turkheimer, Fiedler, & Oltmanns, 2006). In

accordance with the test manual, a personality disorder diagnosis was made when the diagnostic

criteria were met for at least the past five years and did not occur exclusively during the course

of an Axis I disorder.

2.2.2 Self-Report Measures

Severity of current depressive symptoms was assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-

II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996), a 21-item scale with ratings from zero to three. Responses are

summed to reflect the severity of depression over the past two weeks. The BDI-II has

demonstrated good reliability and validity in clinical samples (A. T. Beck, Steer, Ball, & Ranieri,

1996).

2.2.3 Neuropsychological Measures

The Victoria Symptom Validity Test (VSVT; Slick, Hopp, Strauss, & Thompson, 1997) is a test

used to measure the level of effort participants put forward during the testing session. Unusually

poor performance on this task may reflect poor effort, deliberate feigning, exaggeration of real

cognitive deficits, or any combination of the above. Participants were included only if their

Page 23: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

16

VSVT scores were within the valid range based on normative data.

To rule out any discrepancies based on intelligence that may arise on emotion perception tasks,

all participants completed the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, 2002),

designed as a tool for estimating intellectual functioning (IQ) of adults aged 16-89. The WTAR

is normed on a nationally representative, stratified American sample, has excellent test-retest

reliability, and extensive research supporting its validity.

The Benton Facial Recognition Test, Short Form (BFRT; Benton, Sivan, Hamsher, Varney, &

Spreen, 1983) was used to evaluate basic facial recognition capacities. The BFRT requires

participants to match a target face with up to three pictures of the same person in a six-stimuli

array of faces that vary in terms of angles and lighting. Short form scores were transformed into

full-scale age-corrected scores using normative data provided in Benton et al. (1983).

2.2.4 Penn Emotional Acuity Test

The Penn Emotional Acuity Test (PEAT) is a subtest of the University of Pennsylvania

Computerized Neurocognitive Test Battery (PennCNP) and is described as a “measurement of

emotional recognition and discrimination” (Erwin et al., 1992; Gur et al., 1992; Gur et al., 2002).

The PEAT presents 40 faces, one at a time, composed of variably happy, sad, and neutral faces

balanced for gender (Sachs, Steger-Wuchse, Kryspin-Exner, Gur, & Katschnig, 2004). The

presentation takes place in two randomly presented blocks, the first of which contains sad and

neutral faces (sad-neutral block) and the second contains happy and neutral faces (happy-neutral

block). The instructions are as follows: “In this test you will see some faces. Look carefully at

each face and decide if the face is happy, sad, or neutral. Use the seven-point scale you see

below to indicate your rating.” The seven-point Likert scale is forced-choice and consists of the

Page 24: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

17

following options: very happy, moderately happy, slightly happy, neutral, slightly sad,

moderately sad, and very sad. There is a brief practice session of five faces that follows the

instructions and precedes the forty trials. Outcome measures include the accuracy and RT for

overall performance and five collapsed emotion categories: very happy, mildly happy, neutral,

mildly sad, and very sad. In addition, responses per item were compiled to determine average

deviations from the correct score.

2.3 Statistical Plan

Differences between groups were assessed using Chi-square and independent samples t-tests. A

demographic matching process was used and therefore groups were not expected to significantly

differ in terms of age, gender, education, intelligence, or accuracy on the BFRT. Accuracies and

RTs were considered for six variables on the PEAT, the overall total and subcategories of very

happy, mild happy, neutral, mild sad, and very sad. An independent samples t-test was used to

examine performance differences for the overall PEAT statistics. A 2 X 5 MANOVA (group x

emotion categorization) was used along with independent samples t-tests to determine significant

differences between groups for subcategories of accuracy. Answers on the PEAT were then

arranged into a response matrix to determine whether the response patterns differed between

groups. The interpretation of significant group differences was aided by the calculation of

Cohen’s d, measured as the difference between the two raw means divided by the pooled

standard deviation (SD). Cohen (1988) provided guidance for interpreting effect size statistics

(d) as small (0.2), medium (0.5) and large (≥ 0.8).

Secondary analyses explored the relationship of current moods and performance on the PEAT.

Pearson’s and Spearman’s correlations were utilized to determine the relationships between

variables (e.g., the relationship between overall score on the PEAT and BDI scores). The

Page 25: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

18

contribution of Axis I comorbid diagnoses were examined using t-tests for within-group patient

comparisons. Multiple regressions were used to test whether mediation was occurring in the

relationship between BPD and emotion perception performance. Consideration to the

assumptions of normality and heterogeneity were undertaken using a variety of statistical tests.

Variables were analyzed for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Differences between groups

were tested under the assumption of non-normality using non-parametric tests such as the Mann-

Whitney U and Kruskal-Wallis in order to determine if the direction of significance would be

affected. Finally, Spearman’s correlations were used to determine if ranking the data would

significantly alter its association with other variables under an assumption of non-normality.

Page 26: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

19

Chapter 3

Results 3

3.1 Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Demographic information for the BPD patient sample and IQ- and demographically-matched

healthy controls is presented in Table 2. There were no differences between the groups in age,

gender, education or IQ. Patients with BPD and healthy controls performed within the average

range with regard to single-word reading (WTAR) and facial recognition (BFRT). With regard to

clinical characteristics, patients with BPD were more depressed than controls, reporting

moderate levels of depression on average (M = 27.03, SD = 9.76) on the BDI (Beck et al., 1996).

At the time of the study, 89.3% of the patient sample had a history of inpatient hospitalization.

Most patients (89.3%) had completed one or more outpatient treatment programs. Current rates

of psychotropic medication use among patients were as follows: sedatives (34.5%), stimulants

(13.8%), minor tranquilizers (6.9%), antidepressants (62.1%), neuroleptics (10.3%), and mood

stabilizers (20.7%). Table 3 lists the frequencies of Axis I comorbidities for individuals with

BPD. The most common comorbid condition was MDD, with a substantial proportion of patients

in a current episode (40.0%). Other remarkable diagnostic comorbidities included a history of

past substance dependence (33.3%), PTSD (30.0%), eating disorders (26.3%), and anxiety

disorders (23.3%).

3.2 PEAT Accuracy

The total correct (TC) score on the PEAT was calculated using individual answers collapsed into

five categories of response: very happy (VHC), mild happy (MHC), neutral (NC), mild sad

(MSC), and very sad (VSC). Following the scoring guidelines for this task, the two most extreme

Page 27: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

20

categories of emotion stimuli comprised two levels of intensity (“very” and “moderately”) that

were combined into one category (“very”). Responses to all stimuli corresponding to these two

intensities were scored as correct if either response was selected.

Overall, patients with BPD were less accurate than controls on the PEAT, t(55) = -2.92, p =

.005, d = .77. A MANOVA examining differences for patients and controls by emotional

intensity category (VHC, MHC, NC, MSC, VSC) was statistically significant, F(5,51) = 3.19, p

= .01. Post-hoc t-tests revealed comparable accuracy between patients and controls for the

categories VHC, MSC, and VSC (all p’s > .55; see Table 4). Patients did, however, score

significantly worse on NC (t(55) = -2.26, p = .03, d = .60), and MHC (t(55) = -2.38, p = .02, d

=.63) when compared to controls.

To explore the patterns of errors that patients with BPD made for each of the emotional intensity

categories, a classification matrix was generated that presents the frequency with which each

response was selected for each respective category of stimuli (see Table 5 and Figure 1). BPD

patients were more than three times more likely to perceive MHC stimuli as more extreme (i.e.,

selecting VHC) than healthy controls. For the NC category, patients appeared to perceive any

emotion (i.e., this finding was not valence-specific) by selecting either MHC or MSC for these

trials.

3.3 Analysis of Current Mood

BDI scores were moderately and significantly correlated with lower overall accuracy on the

PEAT (r = -0.43, p = .001), suggesting that more severe current depression is associated with

poorer recognition of emotional intensities, at least with regard to happy, neutral and sad facial

expressions. Given that BPD is also associated with increased depressive symptomatology,

Page 28: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

21

mediation analyses were explored to understand the role of current depression in possibly

mediating the relationship between group status (i.e., patients versus control) and overall

performance accuracy on the PEAT. Following the guidelines presented in Baron and Kenny

(1986), multiple regression analyses were performed using TC on the PEAT as the dependent

variable in relationship to group status and BDI score. The variables TC, group status, and BDI

total were significantly correlated with each other (Figure 2A). In Step 1 of the mediation model,

the regression of TC on BPD status, without including the possible mediator of current

depression, was significant, β = -2.67, t(55) = -2.92, p = .005. Step 2 demonstrated that the

regression of TC on current depression also was significant, β = -0.10, t(55) = -3.34, p = .002.

Step 3 of the mediation analysis demonstrated that current depression, controlling for BPD

status, was no longer statistically significant, β = -0.11, t(55) = -1.75, p = .09. The results of this

analysis did not satisfy the requirements of mediation and therefore the association between BPD

diagnosis and poorer overall recognition accuracy on the PEAT did not appear to be mediated by

current depression severity.

When the same process was followed above for NC as the dependent variable, mediation status

was not achieved (Figure 2B). On the other hand, depression was found to be a significant

mediator of the relationship between group status and poorer accuracy on MHC (see Figure 2C).

In Step 1 of the mediation model, the regression of BPD status on MHC status, without including

the possible mediator of current depression, was significant, β = -0.67, t(55) = -2.38, p = .02.

Step 2 demonstrated that the regression of current depression on MHC was also significant, β = -

5.07, t(55) = -3.06, p = .003. Step 3 of the mediation analysis demonstrated that current

depression, controlling for BPD status, was significant, β = -0.04, t(55) = -2.00, p = .05 and that

the effect of the original variable, BPD group status, was no longer significant (β = 0.34, t(55) =

Page 29: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

22

0.60, p = .55). Sobel’s test statistic confirmed the finding of mediation for MHC, t = 2.47 (p <

.01).

3.4 Secondary Analyses

3.4.1 Reaction Time

To achieve normality, RT data were transformed using a square root function of the raw data.

Additionally, three outliers were removed from the patient sample because their scores were

more than three standard deviations above the mean. Although there were no significant

differences with respect to RT, F(1,55) = 0.14, p = .71, analysis of standardized effect sizes

revealed that patients with BPD were faster, on average, across correct trials for all emotional

intensities and valences, t(52) = -0.46, p = .67, d = .12 (Table 6). The largest differences were

seen on VSC trials (d = .38) and MHC (d = .28).

3.4.2 Comorbidity and Psychotropic Medications

There were no differences in overall PEAT accuracy between patients with current MDD and

those without, t(28) = -0.07, p = .94. Similarly, neither a history of PTSD nor substance

dependence was associated with overall performance differences on the PEAT (all p’s > .16).

Patients currently using antidepressant medication were significantly less accurate in recognizing

VHC, t(27) = -2.43, p = .02, and VSC facial expressions, t(27) = -2.06, p = .05, as compared to

those not currently taking these medications.

Page 30: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

23

Chapter 4

Discussion 4The present study utilized an established test to determine how patients with BPD and healthy

individuals perceive emotions corresponding to three valences (happy, neutral, and sad) in both

mild and more extreme intensities. This study advanced previous research by accounting for

current mood state and comparing error patterns made by both BPD patients and healthy

controls. Groups were tightly matched on age, education, gender, and IQ. The patient sample

was clinically stable but reported more symptoms of depression than controls, which fell within

the moderate range of severity (Beck et al., 1996).

Several important findings were revealed with the PEAT, a measure of emotion recognition for

happy, neutral, and sad facial expressions at varying levels of emotional intensity. Overall

accuracy on the task differed significantly between the groups, with healthy controls performing

more accurately than patients with BPD. Further inspection of the emotional intensities revealed

that patients were less accurate in recognizing mildly happy and neutral stimuli. On the other

hand, patients with BPD showed comparable levels of recognition accuracy for the emotions at

100% intensity (i.e., very sad and very happy). These results are consistent with a recent meta-

analysis in which patients with BPD were not found to significantly differ from controls in the

recognition of happy and sad expressions (at 100% intensity), but a large effect size difference

for reduced accuracy in recognizing neutral faces (Daros et al., 2012). This difficulty in

recognizing neutral expressions in patients with BPD has now been replicated across multiple

studies using a variety of methodologies. Several studies on emotion recognition have found

decreased accuracy on neutral facial expressions specifically (Bland et al., 2004; Dyck et al.,

Page 31: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

24

2009; Levine et al., 1997; Wagner & Linehan, 1999). In addition, previous research suggests that

patients with BPD have more trouble understanding mixed valence feelings and have an inability

to synthesize both positive and negative feelings about themselves and others (Levine et al.,

1997). Individuals with BPD are also known to misinterpret ambiguous social cues more

negatively (Wagner & Linehan, 1999) and process neutral information in a negatively biased

way (Meyer, Pilkonis, & Beevers, 2004). Collectively, the findings of the present study support

the notion that patients with BPD misinterpret neutral stimuli and are more likely to ascribe a

particular valence to them.

This study is among the first to characterize the pattern of errors patients with BPD made on the

PEAT. For mildly happy stimuli, patients with BPD were more likely to select a response in the

direction of a higher intensity of happiness. Patients were more likely than controls to select

mildly happy or mildly sad responses to neutral faces as compared to healthy controls. This latter

finding was not specific to a particular valence, which was unexpected given that individuals

with BPD are thought to process neutral stimuli in a more negatively biased manner (Dyck et al.,

2009; Korfine & Hooley, 2000; Levine et al., 1997; Meyer et al., 2004; Wagner & Linehan,

1999). Specifically, two previous studies of emotion recognition in BPD found evidence for a

negative perceptual bias for neutral stimuli, although the response options did not include milder

intensities of emotions (Dyck et al., 2009; Levine et al., 1997). The misperception of neutral

stimuli as being more positive or negative suggests that patients with BPD may not necessarily

have a bias towards a specific emotional valence (i.e., they may show an affectively non-specific

perceptual aberration for neutral faces). This non-specificity with regards to selection of

emotional valence may reflect the increased tendency that individuals with BPD experience

emotional extremes of both valences (Buie & Adler, 1982). Moreover, the emotional sensitivity

Page 32: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

25

that underlies many behaviors in BPD may cause individuals with this illness to overreact to

even minor emotional events, and the task of being confronted with salient emotional (or non-

emotional) faces could lead them to become emotionally unstable and perceive emotions in faces

that healthy individuals do not (Lynch et al., 2006).

Prior research on the topic of emotion recognition in BPD is limited to the extent that current

mood state was not reported and could account for emotion perception in biases associated with

this disorder. As previously mentioned, analyses were carried out to explore whether current

depressive symptomatology might mediate the relationship between group status (i.e., BPD

versus healthy control) and performance indices on the PEAT. The results of these analyses

indicated that current depressive symptomatology mediated this relationship for mildly happy

facial expressions but not neutral faces or total accuracy on this task. The finding that depression

may account for BPD patients’ reduced accuracy in recognizing mild happy faces is in some

ways consistent with documented emotion recognition biases in patient with depression (Gray et

al., 2006; Surguladze et al., 2004). These results, however, also depart significantly from

research on depressed samples in two ways. First, research on depressed patients (with no

personality disorder) suggests that happy faces at 100% intensity would be recognized with less

accuracy; however, the moderately depressed BPD patients in this sample recognized these faces

with near perfect accuracy, and there was no difference in performance in this respect when we

compared currently depressed versus euthymic patients with BPD. Second, reviews on emotion

recognition in MDD have generally found a negative bias in depressed individuals where they

tend to rate all expressions as less positive or sadder (Bourke et al., 2010). Individuals with BPD

did not display the negative perceptual bias that would be expected based on research of

depressed individuals. Unexpectedly, these patients were more likely to perceive mild happy

Page 33: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

26

faces as more intensely happy (rather than less neutral or sad). Furthermore, they were equally

likely to select positive or negative responses for neutral faces. Consistent with these findings, a

previous study found that while patients with BPD showed moderate levels of depression, they

were actually more adept at recognizing happy facial expressions than healthy controls (Lynch et

al., 2006).

Also contrary to what would be expected in depression, patients with BPD in a previous study

were quicker at recognizing facial expressions more generally (across all valences), which could

suggest that emotional sensitivity in BPD may be independent of current depressive symptoms

(Lynch et al., 2006). In the present study, patients with BPD recorded faster RTs (albeit not

significantly) on correct trials regardless of emotional valence. Patients were also particularly

fast in recognizing very happy and very sad faces, the latter of which could be associated with a

mood-congruent bias for sad faces given that the sample of patients with BPD was moderately

depressed at the time of testing. Post-hoc comparison between individuals who were currently

depressed versus non-depressed, however, found that there was no significant difference between

groups on the RT for very sad stimuli. As further evidence that emotion sensitivity may be

uniquely contributing to the pattern of results obtained in the present study, Lynch and

colleagues (2006) argue that quick and accurate responding to emotional stimuli in patients with

BPD is unlikely to be due to higher depressive symptoms. Previous research has demonstrated

that clinically depressed individuals display longer reaction times compared with non-depressed

individuals on attention-dependent tasks (Farrin, Hull, Unwin, Wykes, & David, 2003; Thomas,

Goudemand, & Rousseaux, 1999). While RTs were not significantly different between groups in

the present study, it appears that there is some evidence for emotional sensitivity. Non-

significantly faster RTs were present for the BPD group, despite moderate levels of depression in

Page 34: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

27

the BPD group, which suggests the possible influence of emotion sensitivity on the results

obtained in the present study (i.e., patients with BPD are able to recognize and/or categorize

emotions faster on average).

Diagnostic comorbidity with respect to current MDD, PTSD, or past substance dependence did

not appear to account for the performance differences revealed in primary analyses for the

PEAT. Current antidepressant medication use, however, was associated with lower accuracy in

recognizing very happy and very sad facial expressions. Importantly, there was no difference in

severity of current depression for patients taking antidepressants versus those who were not.

Nevertheless, it should be noted that patients with BPD were commonly taking more than one

psychotropic medication; however, this was difficult to control in order to ensure stability in the

patient group during the testing procedures. Research on the effects of antidepressants on

emotion recognition is sparse; however, one study has suggested that early reversals in the

decreased recognition of happiness after two weeks of antidepressant treatment predicted

symptomatic improvement at six weeks in patients with MDD (Tranter et al., 2009). One further

study suggests that antidepressant use in patients with MDD may normalize emotion recognition

independently of mood state (Anderson et al., 2011). Therefore, patients with MDD may

subjectively report depressive symptoms while antidepressants manipulate affective processing

systems on a biological basis for improved recognition of emotional expressions (particularly

happiness). In the present study, approximately 60% of the patients with BPD were currently

taking antidepressants and performed well on the recognition of very happy stimuli.

Nevertheless, more research is required in order to understand the effects of antidepressant

medications on state-dependent emotion recognition in patients with MDD and BPD.

Page 35: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

28

4.1 Strengths and Limitations

A number of strengths and limitations should be noted as they pertain to the current study. First,

the PEAT is limited in that it provides only three emotional valences: happy, sad, and neutral. It

would be important to examine other emotions in mild and extreme intensities in a fashion

similar to the PEAT, especially given findings of reduced accuracy for facial displays of anger

and disgust in patients with BPD (Daros et al., 2012). Importantly, the paradigm employed by

the PEAT to measure acuity of emotion recognition provides important information that differs

from information obtained from emotion threshold paradigms. The latter puts patients in control

of when they subjectively perceive an emotion. This can create problems when recognition rates

for each emotion are compared across participants because some patients may have stopped the

progression through emotional intensities earlier while others may have stopped the sequence at

a higher intensity. The PEAT avoids this by using preset levels of intensity coupled with a

forced-choice selection approach rather than stopping a morphing sequence. Also, by virtue of

examining emotions at varying levels of intensity (and hence difficulty), the PEAT avoids the

ceiling effect common to POFA tasks; no one completed the task in the present study perfectly.

A further strength of the PEAT in comparison to emotion threshold paradigms is the use of

randomized presentations, which more stringently control for impulsivity. A second limitation of

PEAT is the limited number of stimuli in some emotional categories and the large number of

neutral stimuli included in this task. An improved design might incorporate more stimuli for the

mild and extreme expressions, which could improve the reliability of these more extreme ratings

as well as provide a more adequate measure of these emotional intensities for patients with BPD.

Third, a larger sample size might have allowed us to examine specific indices of the PEAT with

greater statistical power (i.e., RT indices). Fourth, these results do not speak to the specificity of

Page 36: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

29

our findings to BPD as compared to other related diagnostic entities, such as MDD. Comparisons

of these findings with individuals diagnosed with MDD may help to strengthen the specificity of

our findings regarding mood-state contributions for patients with BPD. Finally, psychotropic

medication use was not controlled in the present study and the patients were shown to have been

taking a wide variety of medications at the time of testing. It is therefore difficult to know how

pharmacologic treatments might impact the present findings.

4.2 Implications and Conclusions

Notwithstanding these limitations, this study provides novel information about emotion

perception abilities in patients with BPD. There are both diagnostic and treatment implications

based on the findings of this study. Diagnostically, tasks such as the PEAT may be better able to

discriminate between patients with BPD, MDD, and comorbid MDD-BPD. Patients with BPD

are specifically less accurate in recognizing neutral and patients with MDD appear to be less

accurate in recognizing happiness in addition to presenting with a negative bias. Taken together,

clinicians and researchers could differentiate between these conditions, in addition to the

comorbid diagnosis of MDD-BPD, with an experimentally constructed task rather than self-

report or interview measures. With regards to treatment implications, an important aspect of

treatment may include understanding and correcting misinterpretations of neutral stimuli. Similar

to Aaron T. Beck’s cognitive-behavioural therapy for depression (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery,

1987), individuals with BPD should explore with their therapist their cognitive appraisals

regarding neutral and negative information. The patient may also greatly benefit from group

discussion involving cognitive restructuring revolving around the interpretation of neutral

stimuli. Learning to understand and appropriately respond to neutral facial stimuli may help

patients with BPD better regulate their social behavior when in the presence of others.

Page 37: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

30

In summary, the findings of the present study indicated that individuals with BPD show

comparable performance with regard to accuracy in recognizing very happy and very sad facial

expressions (i.e., 100% intensity) but significantly lower accuracy on neutral expressions as

compared to healthy controls. Patients with BPD were also significantly worse in recognizing

mildly happy facial expressions and the severity of current depressive symptoms appeared to

mediate this relationship. There was no evidence, however, of a negative perceptual bias on the

PEAT. That is, BPD patients rated mildly happy stimuli as more intensely happy and neutral

stimuli as either mildly happy or sad, suggesting that their perceptual bias is unique from the

mood-congruent biases typical in depressed individuals. Finally, there was some evidence BPD

patients were faster in recognizing emotions across nearly all emotion categories of emotion and

intensity, perhaps providing preliminary evidence for increased emotional sensitivity in BPD.

Taken together, these findings advance prior research on the topic of emotion perception in BPD

and suggest important new lines of investigation that may be useful for delineating the nature of

emotion dysregulation in BPD.

Page 38: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

31

References

Abela, J. R., Payne, A. V., & Moussaly, N. (2003). Cognitive vulnerability to depression in individuals with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17(4), 319-329.

Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12(2), 169-177. doi: S095943880200301X

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th Edition. Text Revision. Washington, DC: Author.

Anderson, I. M., Shippen, C., Juhasz, G., Chase, D., Thomas, E., Downey, D., . . . Deakin, J. F. (2011). State-dependent alteration in face emotion recognition in depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 198(4), 302-308. doi: bjp.bp.110.078139

Arntz, A., & Veen, G. (2001). Evaluations of others by borderline patients. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189(8), 513-521.

Basco, R., Bostic, Q., Davies, D., Rush, J., Witte, B., Hendrickse, W., & Barnett, V. (2000). Methods to Improve Diagnostic Accuracy in a Community Mental Health Setting. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(10), 1599-1605. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1599

Beck, A.T., Rush, J., Shaw, B.F., & Emery, G. (1987). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. (1996). Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67(3), 588-597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13

Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G.K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Benton, A.L., Sivan, A.B., Hamsher, K.S., Varney, N.R., & Spreen, O. (1983). Contribution to Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.

Bland, A. R., Williams, C. A., Scharer, K., & Manning, S. (2004). Emotion processing in borderline personality disorders. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25(7), 655-672. doi: 10.1080/01612840490486692

Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2003). The recognition of facial affect in autistic and schizophrenic subjects and their first-degree relatives. Psychological Medicine, 33(5), 907-915.

Bourke, C., Douglas, K., & Porter, R. (2010). Processing of facial emotion expression in major depression: a review. The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44(8), 681-696. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.496359

Page 39: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

32

Buie, D.H., & Adler, G. (1982). The definitive treatment of borderline personality. International Journal of Psychoanalytical Psychotherapy, 9, 51-87.

Cheavens, J. S., Zachary Rosenthal, M., Daughters, S. B., Nowak, J., Kosson, D., Lynch, T. R., & Lejuez, C. W. (2005). An analogue investigation of the relationships among perceived parental criticism, negative affect, and borderline personality disorder features: the role of thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(2), 257-268. doi: S0005-7967(04)00050-6

Clarkin, J. F., Widiger, T. A., Frances, A., Hurt, S. W., & Gilmore, M. (1983). Prototypic typology and the borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92(3), 263-275.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Comtois, K. A., Russo, J., Snowden, M., Srebnik, D., Ries, R., & Roy-Byrne, P. (2003). Factors associated with high use of public mental health services by persons with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatric Services, 54(8), 1149-1154.

Corcoran, K. M., Woody, S. R., & Tolin, D. F. (2008). Recognition of facial expressions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(1), 56-66. doi: S0887-6185(07)00018-7

Cowdry, R. W., Gardner, D. L., O'Leary, K. M., Leibenluft, E., & Rubinow, D. R. (1991). Mood variability: a study of four groups. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(11), 1505-1511.

Daros, A. R., Zakzanis, K. K., & Ruocco, A. C. (2012). Facial emotion recognition in Borderline Personality Disorder. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Derntl, B., Seidel, E. M., Kryspin-Exner, I., Hasmann, A., & Dobmeier, M. (2009). Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48(4), 363-375. doi: bjcp682

Domes, G., Czieschnek, D., Weidler, F., Berger, C., Fast, K., & Herpertz, S. C. (2008). Recognition of facial affect in Borderline Personality Disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22(2), 135-147. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2008.22.2.135

Domes, G., Schulze, L., & Herpertz, S. C. (2009). Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder-a review of the literature. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(1), 6-19. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.6

Donaldson, C., & Lam, D. (2004). Rumination, mood and social problem-solving in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 34(7), 1309-1318.

Page 40: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

33

Driessen, M., Herrmann, J., Stahl, K., Zwaan, M., Meier, S., Hill, A., . . . Petersen, D. (2000). Magnetic resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(12), 1115-1122.

Dyck, M., Habel, U., Slodczyk, J., Schlummer, J., Backes, V., Schneider, F., & Reske, M. (2009). Negative bias in fast emotion discrimination in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine, 39(5), 855-864. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004273

Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological review, 99(3), 550-553.

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of Facial Affect: Available from http://www.paulekman.com/research_cds.php.

Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221(4616), 1208-1210.

Ekman, P., & Matsumoto, D. (1992). Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions of emotion and neutral faces. San Francisco, CA: Authors.

Erwin, R. J., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Skolnick, B., Mawhinney-Hee, M., & Smailis, J. (1992). Facial emotion discrimination: I. Task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects. Psychiatry Research, 42(3), 231-240.

Farrin, L., Hull, L., Unwin, C., Wykes, T., & David, A. (2003). Effects of depressed mood on objective and subjective measures of attention. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 15(1), 98-104.

Feinberg, T. E., Rifkin, A., Schaffer, C., & Walker, E. (1986). Facial discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43(3), 276-279.

Fennig, S., Naisberg-Fennig, S., Craig, T. J., Tanenberg-Karant, M., & Bromet, E. J. (1996). Comparison of clinical and research diagnoses of substance use disorders in a first-admission psychotic sample. American Journal of Addictions, 5(1), 40-48.

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders — Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, 11/2002 revision). New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Frances, A., Fyer, M., & Clarkin, J. (1986). Personality and suicide. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 487, 281-293.

Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Goldstein, R. B., Huang, B., Stinson, F. S., Saha, T. D., . . . Ruan, W. J. (2008). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(4), 533-545.

Page 41: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

34

Gray, J., Venn, H., Montagne, B., Murray, L., Burt, M., Frigerio, E., . . . Young, A. H. (2006). Bipolar patients show mood-congruent biases in sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion when exhibiting depressed symptoms, but not when exhibiting manic symptoms. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11(6), 505-520. doi: 769903065

Gur, R. C., Erwin, R. J., Gur, R. E., Zwil, A. S., Heimberg, C., & Kraemer, H. C. (1992). Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression. Psychiatry Research, 42(3), 241-251.

Gur, R. C., Sara, R., Hagendoorn, M., Marom, O., Hughett, P., Macy, L., . . . Gur, R. E. (2002). A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 115(2), 137-143. doi: S0165027002000067

Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport, 11(1), 43-48.

Herpertz, S. C., Dietrich, T. M., Wenning, B., Krings, T., Erberich, S. G., Willmes, K., . . . Sass, H. (2001). Evidence of abnormal amygdala functioning in borderline personality disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 50(4), 292-298.

Jane, J. S., Pagan, J. L., Turkheimer, E., Fiedler, E. R., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2006). The interrater reliability of the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47(5), 368-375. doi: S0010-440X(06)00023-X

Jovev, M., Chanen, A., Green, M., Cotton, S., Proffitt, T., Coltheart, M., & Jackson, H. (2011). Emotional sensitivity in youth with borderline personality pathology. Psychiatry Research, 187(1-2), 234-240. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.12.019

Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression: tests and an extension of the theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(2), 221-229.

Koenigsberg, H. W., Harvey, P. D., Mitropoulou, V., Schmeidler, J., New, A. S., Goodman, M., . . . Siever, L. J. (2002). Characterizing affective instability in borderline personality disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(5), 784-788.

Koenigsberg, H. W., Siever, L. J., Lee, H., Pizzarello, S., New, A. S., Goodman, M., . . . Prohovnik, I. (2009). Neural correlates of emotion processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 172(3), 192-199. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.07.010

Kohler, C. G., Turner, T. H., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C. M., Siegel, S. J., Kanes, S. J., . . . Gur, R. C. (2003). Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: intensity effects and error pattern. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(10), 1768-1774.

Kohler, C. G., Turner, T. H., Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (2004). Recognition of facial emotions in neuropsychiatric disorders. CNS Spectrums, 9(4), 267-274.

Page 42: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

35

Korfine, L., & Hooley, J. M. (2000). Directed forgetting of emotional stimuli in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 214-221.

Lavender, A., & Watkins, E. (2004). Rumination and future thinking in depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(2), 129-142. doi: 10.1348/014466504323088015

Lenzenweger, M. F., & Pastore, R. E. (2007). On determining sensitivity to pain in borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(6), 747-748; author reply 748-749. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.747

Levine, D., Marziali, E., & Hood, J. (1997). Emotion processing in borderline personality disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185(4), 240-246.

Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment for borderline personality disorder. . New York: Guilford Press.

Linehan, M. M. (1995). Understanding Borderline Personality Disorder: The Dialectical Approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Lobbestael, J., Leurgans, M., & Arntz, A. (2011). Inter-rater reliability of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID I) and Axis II Disorders (SCID II). Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18(1), 75-79.

Lynch, T. R., Rosenthal, M. Z., Kosson, D. S., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Blair, R. J. (2006). Heightened sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion in borderline personality disorder. Emotion, 6(4), 647-655. doi: 2006-21788-010

Marsh, A. A., Kozak, M. N., & Ambady, N. (2007). Accurate identification of fear facial expressions predicts prosocial behavior. Emotion, 7(2), 239-251. doi: 2007-06782-002

McGlashan, T. H., Grilo, C. M., Sanislow, C. A., Ralevski, E., Morey, L. C., Gunderson, J. G., . . . Pagano, M. (2005). Two-year prevalence and stability of individual DSM-IV criteria for schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, and obsessive-compulsive personality disorders: toward a hybrid model of axis II disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(5), 883-889. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.883

Merkl, A., Ammelburg, N., Aust, S., Roepke, S., Reinecker, H., Trahms, L., . . . Sander, T. (2010). Processing of visual stimuli in borderline personality disorder: a combined behavioural and magnetoencephalographic study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 78(3), 257-264. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.08.007

Meyer, B., Pilkonis, P. A., & Beevers, C. G. (2004). What's in a (neutral) face? Personality disorders, attachment styles, and the appraisal of ambiguous social cues. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(4), 320-336. doi: 10.1521/pedi.18.4.320.40344

Minzenberg, M. J., Poole, J. H., & Vinogradov, S. (2006). Social-emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47(6), 468-474. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.03.005

Page 43: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

36

Murphy, F. C., Sahakian, B. J., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W., & Paykel, E. S. (1999). Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychological Medicine, 29(6), 1307-1321.

Narumoto, J., Yamada, H., Iidaka, T., Sadato, N., Fukui, K., Itoh, H., & Yonekura, Y. (2000). Brain regions involved in verbal or non-verbal aspects of facial emotion recognition. Neuroreport, 11(11), 2571-2576.

Paris, J., Brown, R., & Nowlis, D. (1987). Long-term follow-up of borderline patients in a general hospital. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 28(6), 530-535.

Rector, N. A., Daros, A. R., Bradbury, C. L., & Richter, M. A. (2012). Disgust recognition in obsessive-compulsive disorder: diagnostic comparisons and posttreatment effects. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 57(3), 177-183.

Robin, M., Pham-Scottez, A., Curt, F., Dugre-Le Bigre, C., Speranza, M., Sapinho, D., . . . Kedia, G. (2012). Decreased sensitivity to facial emotions in adolescents with Borderline Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Research. doi: S0165-1781(12)00141-2

Rosenthal, M. Z., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2005). Thought suppression mediates the relationship between negative affect and borderline personality disorder symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(9), 1173-1185. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.006

Ruocco, A. C., Amirthavasagam, S., Choi-Kain, L. W., & McMain, S.F. (2012). Neural correlates of negative emotionality in borderline personality disorder: An activation-likelihood-estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry. Epub Aug 17. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.014

Ruocco, A. C., Amirthavasagam, S., & Zakzanis, K. K. (2012). Amygdala and hippocampal volume reductions as candidate endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Psychiatry Research, 201(3), 245-252. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.012

Ruocco, A. C., Medaglia, J. D., Ayaz, H., & Chute, D. L. (2010). Abnormal prefrontal cortical response during affective processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 182(2), 117-122. doi: S0925-4927(10)00036-3

Russell, J. J., Moskowitz, D. S., Zuroff, D. C., Sookman, D., & Paris, J. (2007). Stability and variability of affective experience and interpersonal behavior in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 578-588. doi: 2007-11737-014

Sachs, G., Steger-Wuchse, D., Kryspin-Exner, I., Gur, R. C., & Katschnig, H. (2004). Facial recognition deficits and cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 68(1), 27-35. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00131-2

Page 44: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

37

Schmahl, C., Bohus, M., Esposito, F., Treede, R. D., Di Salle, F., Greffrath, W., . . . Seifritz, E. (2006). Neural correlates of antinociception in borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 659-667. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.659

Schmahl, C. G., Vermetten, E., Elzinga, B. M., & Bremner, J.D. (2003). Magnetic resonance imaging of hippocampal and amygdala volume in women with childhood abuse and borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 122(3), 193-198.

Schneider, F., Gur, R. C., Koch, K., Backes, V., Amunts, K., Shah, N. J., . . . Habel, U. (2006). Impairment in the specificity of emotion processing in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(3), 442-447. doi: 163/3/442

Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2009). Cascades of Emotion: The Emergence of Borderline Personality Disorder from Emotional and Behavioral Dysregulation. Review of General Psychology, 13(3), 219. doi: 10.1037/a0015687

Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Pfohl, B., Widiger, T. A., Livesley, W. J., & Siever, L. J. (2002). The borderline diagnosis I: psychopathology, comorbidity, and personality structure. Biological Psychiatry, 51(12), 936-950.

Skre, I., Onstad, S., Torgersen, S., & Kringlen, E. (1991). High interrater reliability for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Axis I (SCID-I). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 84(2), 167-173.

Slick, D., Hopp, G., Strauss, E., & Thompson, G. (1997). The Victoria symptom validity test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Soloff, P. H., Lynch, K. G., & Kelly, T. M. (2002). Childhood abuse as a risk factor for suicidal behavior in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 16(3), 201-214.

Soloff, P. H., Lynch, K. G., Kelly, T. M., Malone, K. M., & Mann, J. J. (2000). Characteristics of suicide attempts of patients with major depressive episode and borderline personality disorder: a comparative study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(4), 601-608.

Surguladze, S. A., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brebion, G., Travis, M. J., & Phillips, M. L. (2004). Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression. Neuropsychology, 18(2), 212-218. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.212

Tebartz van Elst, L., Hesslinger, B., Thiel, T., Geiger, E., Haegele, K., Lemieux, L., . . . Ebert, D. (2003). Frontolimbic brain abnormalities in patients with borderline personality disorder: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 54(2), 163-171.

Thomas, P., Goudemand, M., & Rousseaux, M. (1999). Attentional resources in major depression. European Archlives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 249(2), 79-85.

Page 45: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

38

Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(6), 590-596.

Tranter, R., Bell, D., Gutting, P., Harmer, C., Healy, D., & Anderson, I. M. (2009). The effect of serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants on face emotion processing in depressed patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 118(1-3), 87-93. doi: S0165-0327(09)00050-0

Trull, T. J., Solhan, M. B., Tragesser, S. L., Jahng, S., Wood, P. K., Piasecki, T. M., & Watson, D. (2008). Affective instability: measuring a core feature of borderline personality disorder with ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(3), 647-661. doi: 2008-11014-013

Unoka, Z., Fogd, D., Fuzy, M., & Csukly, G. (2011). Misreading the facial signs: specific impairments and error patterns in recognition of facial emotions with negative valence in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 189(3), 419-425. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.010

von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna, I. A., Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Frey, M., Fiedler, P., & Resch, F. (2007). Wahrnehmung und Bewertung von emotionalen Gesichtsausdruken bei weiblichen Jugendlichen mit einer Borderline-Personlichkeitsstorung. Zeitschrift fur Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 35(5), 333-340. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917.35.5.333

von Ceumern-Lindenstjerna, I. A., Brunner, R., Parzer, P., Mundt, C., Fiedler, P., & Resch, F. (2010). Attentional bias in later stages of emotional information processing in female adolescents with borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 43(1), 25-32. doi: 000255960

Wagner, A. W., & Linehan, M. M. (1999). Facial expression recognition ability among women with borderline personality disorder: implications for emotion regulation? Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(4), 329-344.

Watkins, P. C., Vache, K., Verney, S. P., Muller, S., & Mathews, A. (1996). Unconscious mood-congruent memory bias in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(1), 34-41.

Wechsler, D. (2002). WTAR: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Yen, S., Zlotnick, C., & Costello, E. (2002). Affect regulation in women with borderline personality disorder traits. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(10), 693-696. doi: 10.1097/01.NMD.0000034744.11140.99

Zanarini, M. C., & Frankenburg, F. R. (2007). The essential nature of borderline psychopathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21(5), 518-535. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.5.518

Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (1999). Axis I diagnostic comorbidity and borderline personality disorder. Comprensive Psychiatry, 40, 245-252.

Page 46: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

39

Zisook, S., Goff, A., Sledge, P., & Shuchter, S. R. (1994). Reported suicidal behavior and current suicidal ideation in a psychiatric outpatient clinic. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 6(1), 27-31.

Page 47: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

40

Tables

Table 1

Description of Emotion Perception Tasks

Emotion Recognition Emotion Discrimination Emotional Acuity

Goal of Task Identification of an

emotion known from previous experience.

Understanding the difference between one emotion and another.

Determine the sharpness of recognition for emotional

intensity.

Most Frequent Presentations

(1) Participants are shown series of single

photographs of emotional facial

expressions and the participant is asked to label the emotion in some fashion. It is

important to note that photographs in the

typical emotion recognition task are not

morphed but remain static.

(1) Presentations with singular photos that blend

two emotions (e.g., containing 40% anger and 60% happiness) and asks participants to state which

emotion they see. (2) Presentation of two stimuli at the same time probing

discrimination between the two facial displays of

emotion in various ways. This design is more

flexible.

(1) Neutral faces are incrementally blended

towards a fully expressive target emotion. The

respondent is asked to stop the progression of morphs when they can articulate what emotion is being

displayed. (2) An alternate variant of this task randomly presents morphed stimuli at different levels of saturation, the saliency of the emotional

expression (e.g., 50% neutral, 50% emotion; 10%

neutral, 90% emotion).

Typical Instructions

“What emotion do you see? How does the

person feel?”

“What emotion do you see? Which face displays

an emotion? Which face is angrier? Which face

displays fear (vs. disgust)?”

“When can you tell me the specific emotion that is

forming? When can you start to see an emotion and tell me what emotion that is?”

Response Styles

Open-ended or forced-choice; paper-and-

pencil or computerized

Forced-choice

Computerized

Forced-choice

Computerized

Page 48: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

41

Table 2

Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with borderline personality disorder and healthy controls BPD Group

(n = 30) Healthy Controls

(n = 27)

Variable M SD M SD Statistic p

% Female 93.33 96.30 X2 = 0.25 .62

Age 29.00 (9.34) 27.19 (10.86) t = 0.67 .50

Mean yrs. education 13.83 (2.65) 14.93 (1.96) t = 1.75 .09

FSIQ (WTAR)1 108.55 (7.91) 110.19 (6.16) t = 0.86 .40

BFRT2 47.44 (4.10) 47.37 (5.10) t = 0.05 .96

BDI total 27.03 (9.76) 1.63 (2.12) t = 13.23 <.001 Notes: M = mean; SD = standard deviation.

1FSIQ = Full Scale IQ, based on the WTAR = Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. Standard scores

with M = 100, SD = 15.

2BFRT = Benton Facial Recognition Test. Short-form scores converted to age-corrected long-

form raw scores.

Page 49: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

42

Table 3

Clinical characteristics for patients diagnosed with BPD for the present study BPD N

% Medicated 75.86 29

% Antidepressants 62.10 29

% Mood Stabilizers 20.70 29

% Sedatives 34.48 29

% Stimulants 13.79 29

% Neuroleptics 10.34 29

% Minor Tranquilizers 6.90 29

% Major Depression, Past 40.00 30

% Major Depression, Current 40.00 30

% PTSD, Past 20.00 30

% PTSD, Current 10.00 30

% Substance Dependence, Past 33.33 30

% Dysthymia 13.33 30

% Any Anxiety Disorder, Current 23.33 30

% Eating Disorder, Past 26.33 30

Note: One patient’s data was incomplete regarding medication use.

Page 50: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

43

Table 4

Accuracy results from the PEAT for BPD and HC BPD (n = 30) HC (n = 27)

Variable M SD M SD t(55) p d

Accuracy

Very Happy (VHC) 4.17 1.32 4.15 1.17 0.06 .97 .02

Mild Happy (MHC) 2.37 1.22 3.04 0.85 -2.38 .02 -.63

Neutral (NC) 15.03 3.53 16.85 2.36 -2.26 .03 -.60

Mild Sad (MSC) 2.77 1.14 2.81 1.33 0.15 .88 .04

Very Sad (VSC) 4.93 1.20 5.11 0.97 0.61 .55 .16

Total Correct1 29.27 3.94 31.96 2.90 -2.92 .01 -.77 Notes: M = mean; SD = standard deviation. d = Cohen’s standardized effect size. 1The subcategories contained the following number of stimuli: VHC = 6, MHC = 4, NC = 20,

MSC = 4, VSC = 6. The total score was out of 40.

Page 51: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

44

Table 5

Response patterns based on PEAT stimulus classification

Participant Response

% Respondents VHC MHC NC MSC VSC

Answer 1-2 3 4 5 6-7

VHC BPD 69.44 26.67 3.89 -- --

HC 69.14 29.01 1.85 -- --

MHC BPD 29.17 56.67 14.17 -- --

HC 9.26 75.93 13.89 0.93 --

NC BPD 0.33 10.67 75.83 12.83 0.33

HC -- 6.67 84.26 8.89 0.19

MSC BPD -- 0.83 7.50 70.00 21.67

HC -- 0.93 4.63 70.37 24.07

VSC BPD 0.56 1.11 0.56 15.56 82.22

HC 0.62 -- -- 14.81 84.57

Page 52: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

45

Table 6 Reaction Time (RT) results from the PEAT for BPD and HC

BPD (n = 27) HC (n = 27)

M SD M SD t(52)1 p d

Median RT Correct

Very Happy (VHC) 1761.46 450.13 1819.80 393.37 -0.592 .58 -.16

Mild Happy (MHC) 1676.91 463.10 1809.52 495.52 -1.03 .31 -.28

Neutral (NC) 1765.31 530.27 1809.67 638.87 -0.22 .83 -.06

Mild Sad (MSC) 1881.50 560.08 1840.48 647.14 0.33 .74 .09

Very Sad (VSC) 2160.69 796.19 2450.33 794.81 -1.382 .17 -.38

Median RT Correct Total 1786.31 469.51 1830.57 414.47 -0.44 .67 -.12 Notes: M = mean; SD = standard deviation. d = Cohen’s standardized effect size.

1Square root transformation applied before using independent samples t-test.

2 In these cases, one patient in each of these instances got all VHC or VSC incorrect and there

was no correct RT values to be included, df = 51.

Page 53: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

46

A

B

Figure 1. Response patterns on (A) mildly happy stimuli and (B) neutral stimuli of the PEAT for

BPD (n = 30) and HC (n = 27) participants.

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

VHC MHC NC MSC VSC

Perc

ent o

f Res

pond

ents

HC BPD

0%

10%

20%

30%

40%

50%

60%

70%

80%

90%

VHC MHC NC MSC VSC

Perc

ent o

f Res

pond

ents

HC

BPD

Page 54: Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder · Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... Emotion Perception in Borderline Personality Disorder ... in the

47

Figure 2. Mediation analyses with BDI score as a mediator and (A) TC score, (B) NC score, (C)

MHC score as dependent variables. The simple correlation (and beta coefficient) for the

association between two variables is represented by r. β1 represents the association for the

mediator, controlling for group status. β2 represents the magnitude of the association between

group status and the dependent variable after the MR with the mediator has been run (* indicates

p < .05).

A

B

C

Sobel’s t: t = 2.47 p < .01