Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

  • Upload
    blessa

  • View
    220

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    1/30

    - PLEASE DO NOT QUOTE -

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades?

    Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students*

    W o r k i n g D r a f t : 1 0 / 1 2 / 1 0 : V e r s i o n : 3 . 0

    M a rt i n R ya n , University College Dublin

    Liam D el aney, University College DublinColm Harmon, University College Dublin, IZA

    Abstract

    This paper examines the relationship between lecture attendance and grades, presenting evidencefrom longitudinal tracking of Irish students. This is only the second study, which the authors are awareof, to examine the highereducation production function across multiple subject areas. Previous researchby the authors suggests that empirical models of highereducation production functions may be biasedif they do not include measures of non-cognitive ability and other individual differences. In addition, aclassical criticism about causality from within labour economics can be applied to emphasise thatwhile more motivated, dedicated and future-orientated students may be more likely to attend theirlectures, students with those same characteristics are also more likely to achieve higher grades.Therefore, an important contribution is the measurement and inclusion of the following constructs:willingness to take risks, consideration of future consequences and non-cognitive ability traits. Theauthors also control for the effects of additional study-hours, prior academic achievement and universityfixed effects. The data were collected through a web-survey that the authors designed. Preliminaryresults suggest that lecture attendance matters for grades, both before and after the inclusion of controlsfor potentially confounding factors.

    JEL: I21, J2, D90

    Keywords: higher education, education inputs, lecture attendance, hours of study, future-orientation, attitude to risk, non-cognitive ability, conscientiousness

    *Acknowledgements: Thanks to seminar participants at the UCD School of Economics and the Geary Institute forproviding comments; and to participants at the annual conference of the Irish Society of New Economists (Trinity CollegeDublin; September 2010).

    Corresponding author. Em ai l: [email protected]. Postal correspondence: Martin Ryan, Desk 7.1, 2nd Floor, Geary

    Institute, University College Dublin, Be lf ie ld , Dublin 4, Ireland. The corresponding author acknowledges financial supportfrom the Irish Research Council for the Humanities and Social Sciences (IRCHSS).

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    2/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    2

    I . I n t r o d u c t i o n

    There is evidence that lecture attendance is an important determinant of academic achievement, for

    example: Schmidt (1983); Romer (1993); Durden and Ellis (1995); Dolton, Marcenaro and Navarro

    (2003); Martins and Walker (2006) and Cohn and Johnson (2006). In fact, a large literature has grown

    around the question of whether (and to what extent) lecture attendance affects student achievement. A

    perennial motivation for research in this area is the issue of absenteeism amongst university students.

    Past estimates place the rate of student absenteeism as high as 40% (Romer, 1993), and even as high

    70% (Moore et al., 2003). In 1993, Romer published an article which ignited a lively debate about

    mandatory attendance policy. Of course, non-attendance at lectures bears a heavy economic cost. The

    National Centre for Educational Statistics in the United States estimates that American colleges anduniversities spend $32 billion yearly on student instruction. This is approximately $12,000 per full time

    student (Dobkin, Gil and Marion, 2007). Despite this, a substantial fraction of students fail to attend

    lectures, which are traditionally the primary means by which educational material is presented.1

    Crucially, it is empirically established that grades affect future earnings; see Wise (1975), Filer (1983),

    Jones and Jackson (1990), Loury and Garman (1995), McIntosh (2006) and Naylor, Smith and McKnight

    (2007). In addition, there are strong indications that there is a positive correlation between lecture

    attendance and exam performance (see section 3 of this paper), notwithstanding concerns about

    causation. Therefore, students who fail to attend their lectures are potentially disadvantaged in relation

    to their academic achievement and their subsequent earnings in the labour market.

    Unlike the United States, where annual tuition costs average $13,424 at four-year public institutions

    ($30,393 at four-year private institutions), there have been no tuition fees for higher education in

    Ireland since 1996.2 In addition, the tuition fees that were charged prior to 1996 were never

    comparable to the full economic cost of providing higher education in Irelands (mainly public)

    institutions. According to the OECD (2004), the annual tuition fees paid by Irish students before 1996

    covered approximately 30% of the operating costs of higher education institutions (HEIs) in Ireland.

    These fees averaged approximately 2,500 per annum and accounted for about one-third of the total

    1 However, 4.6 million students in the United States (1 out of every 4) took a college-level online course at the start of the2008/09 academic year. This was a 17% increase from the previous academic year, according to the seventh annual SloanSurvey of Online Learning (Allen and Seaman, 2009). A large majority of students about three million weresimultaneously enrolled in face-to-face courses.2 Ireland refers to the Republic of Irelandthroughout this paper. Figures for tuition costs in the United States are for theacademic year 2007/08 (National Centre for Educational Statistics).

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    3/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    3

    cost of attending higher education, the remainder being mainly housing and maintenance costs (OECD,

    2004). Despite students currently having to pay an annual registration fee of 1,500 to attend Irish

    institutions, tertiary education is predominantly subsidised in Ireland.3 1,346 million was allocated

    to current activities in Irelands HEIs in 2009 (Comptroller and Auditor General, 2010). 185,542

    students attended these institutions (both universities and institutes of technology) in the academic

    year 2008/09. This paper examines only the students attending Irelands seven universities; there were

    97,001 of these students in 2009, and the institutions they attended received 742 million in funding

    for current activities (Comptroller and Auditor General, 2010). A notable comparison with the United

    States is that there is a considerable subsidy for the provision of higher education in Ireland. Therefore,

    besides the potential for individual costs related to lower academic achievement and lower levels of

    earnings, the cost of poor student performance also falls to a large extent on society. Approximately

    8,244 per student (not including student contributions) is spent on current expenditure in Irishuniversities each year.

    Of course, establishing causality between lecture attendance and grades is difficult; however, this

    research paper benefits from the use of longitudinal data and the inclusion of the following constructs:

    willingness to take risks, consideration of future consequences and non-cognitive ability traits. Ryan,

    Delaney and Harmon (2010) suggest that empirical models of higher education production functions

    may be biased if they do not include measures of the aforementioned individual differences.4 In

    addition, in the literature on the micro-level returns to education, there is a concern that higher-ability

    individuals are more likely to attain higher levels of education as well as higher levels of earnings. In the

    area of research examined in this paper, there is aparallelconcern that more motivated, dedicated and

    future-orientated students are more likely to attend their lectures as well as achieve higher grades.

    There is evidence that unobserved heterogeneity amongst students explains more about student

    achievement than observable inputs such as lecture attendance (Martins and Walker, 2006). Also, it is

    3 Irish students that qualify for a higher education maintenance allowance, or the grant, as it is colloquially known, do nothave to pay the registration fee. To qualify for the full grant, the (pre-tax) family income of the student must be no more than41,110 (if the family has four or fewer children.) There are slightly higher thresholds for larger numbers of children. Inaddition, reduced grant payments are available up to a family income threshold of 51,380. The maintenance allowance isnever more than 3,342 and is often closer to 1,370, depending on how far the student lives away from college. The most

    recent figure for the average industrial wage in Ireland is from 2006: 29,910.4 The findings from Ryan, Delaney and Harmon (2010) suggest that non-cognitive abilities may be more important thanfinancial constraints in the determination of higher education inputs such as lecture attendance and additional study-hours. Infact, the impact of non-cognitive ability on the extent of highereducation inputs is often more significant than other variablessuch as course or institutional choice, or parental background. However, while non-cognitive attributes have exhibitedpredictive grade-related validities (e.g., Lievens, Coetsier, De Fruyt, & De Maeseneer, 2002; Robbins et al., 2004), they are not asgood predictors of college grades as the actual academic behaviours that they are thought to influence (Cred, Roch, andKieszczynka; 2010).

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    4/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    4

    generally accepted that more able (and motivated and hard-working) students are more likely both to

    attend and to score highly in their courses (Arulampalam, Naylor and Smith; 2009).

    In the analysis, the authors also control for the effects of additional study-hours, prior academic

    achievement and university fixed effects. There is evidence that additional study-hours are positively

    related to grades, for example: Martins and Walker (2006); Arulampalam, Naylor and Smith (2007) and

    Stinebrickner and Stinebrickner (2007). Bratti and Staffolani (2002) find that the effect of lecture

    attendance on performance is not robust to the inclusion of the number of hours of study. Other

    research suggests that ability (proxied by prior academic achievement) has a significant independent

    effect on grade, and in some studies it exceeds the effect of attendance (Park and Kerr, 1990). Finally,

    this is only the second study, which the authors are aware of, to examine a higher education production

    function across multiple subject areas.

    The remainder of the paper is organised as follows. The next section describes the theoretical

    framework: a higher education production function. The third section reviews the existing literature on

    the relationship between lecture attendance and academic performance. The fourth section presents

    the survey data; these were collected through a web-survey that the authors designed. The fifth section

    presents the method and results. Preliminary results suggest that lecture attendance matters for grades,

    both before and after the inclusion of controls for potentially confounding factors. The sixth section

    concludes with a discussion.

    I I. A H ighe rEducation Production Function

    A simple production model lies behind much of the analysis in the economics of education. Some

    common inputs are school resources, teacher quality, and family attributes, and the outcome is student

    achievement (Hanushek, 2007). Much of the work using this model of production has concentrated on the

    educational attainment of pupils in compulsory schooling, with less attention paid to higher education

    (Arulampalam, Naylor and Smith; 2009). However, there is a precedent for the theoretical consideration

    of highereducation production functions (Freire and Silva, 1975; Johnson, 1978; Hopkins, 1990; Douglas

    and Sulock, 1995). There is also a much wider empirical literature on higher education production

    functions, in which researchers give attention to student inputs, in particular: lecture attendance and

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    5/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    5

    additional hours of study.

    After enrolling in a course of higher education, students allocate their time between educational

    inputs (primarily lecture attendance and additional study-hours), and other activities, such as leisure

    and part-time work. Ryan, Delaney and Harmon (2010) ma p th e e du ca ti on a l i np u ts o f lecture

    attendance and additional study-hours into the Juster and Stafford (1991) model of inter-temporal

    time use. The theoretical framework used in this paper draws on the literature on highereducation

    production functions. Production functions in economics are meant to represent the process by

    which an institutionin this case a college or universitytransforms inputs into outputs (Hopkins,

    1990). In this case, the authors are particularly concerned with the role of student inputs in the

    production function. A linear higher education production function is presented in equation (1):

    (1)

    where Yis a measure of educational achievement for student i in universityjin year t, R is a vector

    for the lecture attendance of student i in universityjin year t,Xis a vector of observable student and

    family characteristics for student i in university j in year t, C is a vector of typically unobservable

    individual differences for student i attending university j in year t, and and are a set of fixed

    effects for university jand year t, respectively. Finally, is a stochastic error term for student i in

    university jin year t. The effect of R on Yis the focus of this study; direct measurement of C(over

    time) is also a major part of this papers contribution. represents a set of estimates for the effect of

    attendance on the educational outcome in question. In a difference formulation, any time invariant

    variables contained in Cdrop out, resulting in the estimating equation (2):

    (2)

    Equation 2 includes an individual fixed effect , for student i which allows one to analyse the

    effect on educational outcomes for an individual student moving from one level of lecture attendance

    to another as compared to the difference for students who do not change their lecture attendance

    during the time they are observed in the dataset. Finally, Cred, Roch, and Kieszczynka (2010)

    outline the possible structures which might explain the relationship between students individual

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    6/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    6

    differences, their lecture attendance and their academic achievement, as shown in Figure 1. We

    proceed with a unique effects model, as we want to estimate the effect of lecture attendance on grades

    while controlling for individual differences.

    Fig.1: Possible Structures between Individual Differences, Attendance and Grades

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    7/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    7

    I I I . E x i s t i n g L i t e r a t u r e

    In the international literature, many researchers have attempted to measure the impact of

    absenteeism on student performance (Anikeeff, 1954; Schmidt, 1983; Jones, 1984; Buckalew, et al.,

    1986; Brocato, 1989; Park and Kerr, 1990; Van Blerkom, 1992; Romer, 1993; Gunn, 1993; Durden and

    Ellis, 1995; Devadoss and Foltz, 1996; Marburger, 2001; Bratti and Staffolani, 2002; Dolton, et al.,

    2003; Kirby and McElroy, 2003; Rodgers, 2001; Rocca, 2003; Stanca, 2006; Lin and Chen, 2006;

    amongst others). In each study, the authors find a positive correlation between exam performance

    and attendance. While there is an older literature established by educational psychologists, the rest of

    this section is mainly focused on evidence produced by economists on whether lecture attendance

    matters for grades.

    In a paper examining student time allocation in a University of Wisconsin-Madison

    Macroeconomics Principles course (n = 216), Schmidt (1983) reports that hours spent attending

    lectures and class-discussions positively affects course grades, even after controlling for hours of

    study. Park and Kerr (1990) use a multinomial logit model in order to identify the determinants of

    academic performance in a Money and Banking course (n = 97). They find that higher attendance is

    associated with better performance, although students GPA and college entrance exam scores are

    more important factors overall. Romer (1993) surveys attendance at all undergraduate economics

    classes during one week at a large public institution, a medium-sized private university, and a small

    liberal arts college. He runs regressions of student performance on fraction of lectures attended, both

    excluding and including some proxies for motivation. The effect of class attendance is always positiveand significant; however, its magnitude is greatly reduced by the inclusion of proxies for motivation.

    In the light of this evidence Romer (1993) suggests that a policy of mandatory attendance might

    enhance student academic performance.

    Durden and Ellis (1995) use students self-reported number of absences in order to explore the

    relationship between absenteeism and academic achievement (n = 346) in a Principles of Economics

    course. Controlling for student differences in background, ability and motivation, they find a nonlinear

    effect of attendance on learning: while a few absences do not lead to worse grades, excessive

    absenteeism does. Marburger (2001) examines the effect of absenteeism on exam performance in aPrinciples of Microeconomics course (n = 60). Students absences records over the semester are

    matched with records of the class meetings when the material corresponding to each question of

    three multiple-choice exams was covered. Results show that missing class on a specific day

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    8/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    8

    significantly increase the likelihood to respond incorrectly to a multiple-choice question based on the

    material covered that day compared to students who were present. This finding suggests a negative

    relationship between absenteeism and academic performance.

    Rodgers (2001) finds a small but statistically significant impact of attendance on academic

    performance in a sample of students enrolled in her Introductory Statistics course (n = 167). Cohenand Johnson (2006) examine the relationship between class attendance and academic performance in

    a sample of 347 economics students. Their findings indicate a strong positive correlation between

    attendance and performance. Using data on a sample of approximately 400Agricultural Economics

    students at four large U.S. universities, Devadoss and Foltz (1996) find that, after taking into account

    motivational and aptitude differences across students, the difference in exam performance between a

    student with perfect attendance and a student attending only half of the classes is, on average, a full

    grade. Dolton, Marcenaro and Navarro (2001) use data from the University of Malaga drawn from a

    survey conducted in April 1999 on first and final year students. Their sample includes 3722

    observations taken from students from forty different subject areas. Dolton, Marcenaro and Navarro

    (2001) is the only study (that we are aware of) to examine a higher education production function

    across multiple subject areas. They find that lectures are four times more productive than self-study.

    Using a sample of (n = 371) first-year Economics students from Italy, Bratti and Staffolani (2002) find

    that, after controlling for the number of study hours, the positive and significant effect of class

    attendance on performance is not robust to the inclusion of self study.

    Kirby and McElroy (2003) base their analysis on a sample of first year Economics students in

    Ireland (n = 368). They find that class attendance is significantly affected by hours worked and travel

    time to university. On the other hand, tutorial attendance appears to enhance exam performance

    more than class attendance. Maloney and Lally (1998) find that both lecture attendance and previous

    results are positively and significantly related to examination results for second and third year

    economics students at the National University of Ireland, Galway. In the Maloney and Lally (1998)

    study, lecture attendance and previous results are more important in explaining the examination

    results of second year students than of third year students. Stanca (2006) uses a large panel data set

    collected from an Introductory Microeconomics course in an Italian university (n = 766). The data

    combine administrative and survey sources. However, a notable feature of the data is that attendance

    to classes and tutorials is self-reported by students. Applying three different econometric approaches(OLS-proxy regression, instrumental variables and panel estimators) to address the endogeneity of

    the attendance rate variable, Stanca (2006) concludes that attendance has an important independent

    effect on academic performance.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    9/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    9

    Marburger (2006) examines the introduction of a mandatory attendance policy; he focuses on two

    groups of students studying an introductory module in Microeconomics in two consecutive years at

    the same university. Students in one group (no-policy) were told that the university-wide attendance

    policy would not be applied to them; and students in the other groups (policy) were subject to the

    university-wide attendance policy. Marburger (2006) expresses concern that absenteeism may be

    endogenous to the day and timing of a particular class so he uses students from the same teaching slot

    in 2003 as a control group. Members of the no-policy group (n = 38) attended the module in 2002,

    while members of the policy group (n = 39) attended the same module in 2003. Marburgers (2006)

    findings concur with the previous result that lecture attendance matters for grades. In addition, (in

    the absence of any mandatory policy on attendance) absenteeism in the no-policy group increased

    throughout the semester.

    Chan et al. (1997) examines the relationship between class attendance and academic performance

    in a Principles of Finance course (n = 71). After correcting for selectivity bias (due to student

    withdrawals) by using Tobit and Heckman two-stage models, they find a positive effect of attendance

    on performance. They also find that a mandatory attendance policy would not significantly enhance

    course grades. Dobkin and Marion (2010) estimate the effect of class attendance on exam

    performance by implementing a policy in three large economics classes that required students scoring

    below the median on the midterm exam to attend class. This policy generated a large discontinuity in

    the rate of post-midterm attendance at the median of the midterm score. Dobkin and Marion (2010)

    estimate that near the policy threshold, the post-midterm attendance rate was 36 percentage points

    higher for those students facing compulsory attendance. They also estimate that a 10 percentage

    point increase in a student's overall attendance rate results in a 0.17 standard deviation increase in

    the final exam score.

    Research by Thomas and Webber (2001) emphasises the effects of peer groups on student choice,

    while Webber and Walton (2006) illustrate that peer groups can be gender-specific. Attendance at

    university seminars may be the result of ones friends attending either that seminar or another class

    on the same day, and not an independent decision made by the student. While the presence and

    significance of friendships are difficult to model, many freshmen place a high value on the chance to

    socialise while at university, such that attendance in class is often a by-product of this socialising with

    friends (Allen and Webber, 2010).

    Some studies have been undertaken that demonstrate no relationship between attendance and

    academic performance. Browne et al. (1991) finds that students who did not attend lectures did

    equally well in the Test of Understanding College Economics (TUCE), a standard American multiple-

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    10/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    10

    choice based test, than those who did attend lectures regularly. However, Browne et al. (1991) also

    find that students that attended lectures regularly performed better in an essay assessment. Rodgers

    (2002) implements an incentive scheme in an undergraduate introductory statistics module in an

    Australian university. The scheme was designed so that each students overall mark was reduced by 1

    per cent for every tutorial missed in excess of two. Students attendance and performance were

    compared with the performance of students who had undertaken the same module in the previous

    academic year, prior to the introduction of the incentive scheme. The results from Rodgers (2002)

    indicate that while attendance did improve, improved attendance did not translate into improved

    academic performance, even when the penalty points that had been deducted for non-attendance

    were added back on to the students overall marks.

    I V . D a t a

    The data that we use in this paper were collected through the longitudinal component of a web-

    survey that the authors designed: rounds 2 and 3 of the Irish University Study. Round 2 was conducted

    during spring 2009; Round 3 was conducted during spring 2010. Analysis is restricted to observations

    where students are enrolled in full-time courses; this is because part-time students are a

    characteristically different group. In addition, the sample is restricted to full-time undergraduates

    because post-graduates are also a characteristically different group. All of the students in the analytical

    sample are studying for honours bachelor degrees (n = 782). The Honours Bachelor Degree in Irish

    universities is normally awarded following completion of a programme of three or four years

    duration (180-240 ECTS credits), although there are examples of longer programmes in areas such as

    architecture, dentistry and medicine. Entry to a programme leading to an honours bachelor degree is

    determined by students performance in the Leaving Certificate (Leaving Cert.), which is the senior

    state examination at the end of secondary school in Ireland. Seven subjects are typically examined in

    the Leaving Cert., with students required to take compulsory courses in Maths, English and the Irish

    Language. They are free to choose additional courses from a range of language, science, business and

    humanities subjects, depending on availability within the secondary school. Exams can be sat at

    Ordinarylevel or Higherlevel; this distinction is important for the number of points awarded. Entry

    into Irish higher education is based on the points system" in which the more advanced papers get

    higher points.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    11/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    11

    Fig.2: Distribution of Leaving Cert. Points-Scores (Irish Universities Study)

    0

    .01

    .02

    .03

    .0

    4

    .05

    Percentageofstudentsineach

    bin

    200 300 400 500 600

    Leaving Cert. scores in bins of 10-points

    Points are awarded for the six examinations in which a student performs best.5 The distribution of

    Leaving Cert. points-scores for the students from the Irish Universities Study is shown in Fig. 2.

    Robustness checks between the data from the Irish Universities Study and data available for the

    population of Irish university students are presented in Appendix A (across gender, institution and area

    of study). Overall, on several observables, the sample is representative of its underlying population.

    Fig. 4 shows histograms of percentage average (university) grade-scores from the university

    sample. This is the dependent variable in the production function estimated in this paper. The

    variable comes from asking students the following question: On a scale of 0-100, what is your

    average grade-score at university? As this is a self-reported outcome, we make three data-

    comparisons which all show that the dependent variable is representative of population grade-scores.

    The first is a comparison to an alternative measure of grade-score: 1h1, 2h1, 2h2, Pass or A+, A, A-, B+,

    B, B-, C+, C, C-, D+, D, D- (whichever grading-scheme the student is actually marked on). This

    alternative measure of grade-score comes from a question at a different position in the survey-

    questionnaire. Table 1 (for Round 2 of the study) and Table 2 (for Round 3 of the study) show that

    students are answering questions about their grade-scores in a consistent manner.

    5 Entry is through a centralised application system - the Central Applications Offce (CAO). This office was established in 1976to streamline and co-ordinate student applications for university places. A total of ten higher education courses may bechosen in order of preference. Each applicant is given a place in the highest of his course preferences in which his meritrating will allow (Coolahan, 1991).

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    12/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    12

    Fig.4: Histograms of Per cent age Average (Univer sity) Gr ade

    0

    .02

    .04

    .06

    Density

    0 20 40 60 80 100percent last year

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = 1.86

    Round 2

    0

    .02

    .04

    .06

    Density

    40 60 80 100percent this year

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = 1.63

    Round 3

    Source: Irish University Study

    The second data-comparison is a test/re-test analysis on students percentage average grade-score

    from Round 2 and their reporting of their percentage average grade-score from last year, taken in

    Round 3. This comparison also shows that students are answering questions about their grade-scores

    in a consistent manner. Finally, the third data-comparison outlined in Table 3, shows students grade-

    scores (from Round 3) against grade-scores for the university population. Here, the measure 1h1,

    2h1, 2h2, Pass must be used; students who are marked by A+, A, A-, B+, B, B-, C+, C, C-, D+, D, D-

    have their grades re-coded to be comparable. Comparisons are available by gender and field of study.

    The self-reported grade-scores are representative across gender breakdowns, but are upward-biased

    across fields of study. Therefore, re-weighting grade-scores by field of study would be a useful

    robustness check.

    Another issue is salient when discussing the self-reported nature of the grade-data examined in

    this paper. It is possible that students may be over-stating their grades due to the Lake Wobegon

    effect. The Lake Wobegon effect, also known as illusory superiority or the above average effect, is a

    cognitive bias that causes individuals to overestimate their positive qualities and abilities and to

    underestimate their negative qualities, relative to others. This is evident in a variety of areas including

    intelligence, performance on tasks or tests and the possession of desirable characteristics or

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    13/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    13

    personality traits (Colvin et al., 1995; Kruger and Dunning, 1999).6 This concern can be easily

    circumvented if official records data are available; however, many researchers can only access

    student-reported data. In 1994, Maxwell and Lopus published an article about the Lake Wobegon

    Effect in student self-reported data; demonstrating biases stem from two sources. First, below-

    average students tend to inflate their academic achievements, and second, they often fail to report

    their inferior accomplishments.

    In a more recent paper, Haley, Johnson and McGee (2010) also examine whether using student-

    survey data in place of official records data biases regression estimates. They motivate their

    contribution by noting a useful statistical feature of over-reporting on bounded variables such as

    grade point average. "Specifically, the misreports will be negatively correlated with the true grade

    point average, yielding a form of non-classical measurement error that actually counteracts the bias."

    Haley et al. (2010) connect this observation to reliability ratios used in labour economics. In two

    applications, they find that it is unnecessary to correct for the bias from the Lake Wobegon effect

    because it is so small.

    Lecture attendance (the independent variable of particular interest) is measured as the self-reported

    percentage of university lectures attended by each student. The distribution of percentage lectures

    attended for Rounds 2 and 3 of the study is shown in Fig. 5. Approximately 12% of students claim to

    attend all of their lectures. 32% of students claim to attend 90% or more of their lectures. 47% of

    students claim to attend 80% or more of their lectures. 57% of students claim to attend 70% or more of

    their lectures. 67% of students claim to attend 50% or more of their lectures. Overall, the mean-level of

    percentage lectures attendedis 83% in Round 2 of the study, and 84% in Round 3 of the study. This is

    a self-reported behaviour, and one that is subject to much comparison of anecdote amongst academic

    instructors. Furthermore, there is reasonable ground to suspect self-reported lecture attendance to be

    over-stated due to the phenomenon of social desirability bias. Social desirability bias is a term used to

    describe the tendency of respondents to reply in a manner that will be viewed favourably by others;

    see Bound et al. (2001) on Measurement Error in Surveys for a discussion.

    While benchmarking against official data is difficult in the case of lecture attendance, there is some

    reassurance from mean-levels ofpercentage lecture attendance being similar across both Round 2 and

    Round 3 of the study. In addition, a data-comparison can be made with a comprehensive attendance

    survey (measured by head-count) that was conducted at one Irish university during the academic

    year 2008/09. Under the guidance of Gabrielle Kelly (2010), students in an undergraduate Survey

    Sampling class carried out a survey to estimate the attendance rate at lectures in science modules in

    6 The phraseology of the Lake Wobegon effect comes from Garrison Keillor's fictional town, Lake Wobegon, where "all the

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    14/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    14

    UCD. Only first-years in the UCD College of Engineering, Mathematical and Physical Sciences and the

    UCD College of Life Science were included (this was due to student drop-out in first-year being

    prevalent in these colleges). 7 The overall attendance rate was 47.3%. However, there is also a

    (statistically significant) decrease in attendance rate as class size increases. Figure 6 (taken from the

    report on the UCD Attendance Survey; 2009) shows a plot of attendance rate vs. enrolment. The

    Table 1: Percentage Average Grade vs. Ordered Grade-Score (Round 2)

    Grade Option 1 % Average Grade N

    1st 73 7 792:1 64 7 1632:2 59 7 81

    Pass 51 9 18Fail 50 0 1

    Grade Option 2 % Average Grade N

    A+ 81 11 5A 78 8 18A- 75 7 19B+ 70 10 41B 68 8 48B- 65 9 35C+ 60 6 30

    C 59 8 16

    C- 56 10 7

    D+ 56 7 10

    D 52 3 3

    Table 2: Percentage Average Grade vs. Ordered Grade-Score (Round 3)

    Grade Option 1 % Average Grade N

    1st 74 5 1592:1 66 6 2512:2 60 7 52

    Pass 54 5 4

    Grade Option 2 % Average Grade N

    A+ 80 9 5

    children are above average".7 Engineering modules were excluded. This was done because according to the authors of the UCD Attendance Survey, thesemodules are very practical-work orientated and so would have a higher attendance rate.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    15/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    15

    A 80 8 29A- 77 7 31B+ 73 6 60B 69 6 39B- 66 4 13

    C+ 63 5 18C 58 5 12

    C- 50 9 4D+ 52 4 2

    D 44 5 4

    Table 3: Population Grades (from HEA Data) vs. Survey Grades

    Gender/Field of Study 1h1 2h1 2h2 Other Pass

    Male: Population 44.0 35.5 38.5 44.0 40.0Female: Population 56.0 64.5 61.5 56.0 60.0

    Male: Survey 44.0 32.0 46.0 ////// 50.0Female: Survey 56.0 68.0 54.0 ////// 50.0

    Education: Population 9.2 54.0 28.5 5.8 2.5

    Education: Survey 44.0 56.0 ////// ////// //////

    Humanities and Arts: Population 9.3 43.0 26.4 19.9 1.4Humanities and Arts: Survey 28.7 61.74 9.7 ////// //////

    Social Science/Business: Population 13.6 46.4 19.6 19.3 1.2Social Science/Business: Survey 29.5 57.0 13.5 ////// //////

    Science: Population 21.4 42.0 22.4 11.7 2.5Science: Survey 45.0 45.0 8.0 2.0 //////

    Engineering/Construction: Population 26.1 35.2 28.2 8.1 2.3

    Engineering/Construction: Survey 36.0 44.0 20.0 ////// //////

    Health and Welfare: Population 13.5 37.1 21.6 13.2 14.6

    Health and Welfare: Survey 29.0 52.0 14.0 5.0 //////

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    16/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    16

    Fig. 5: Histograms of Per cent age Lectu res Att ended

    0

    .01

    .02

    .03

    .04

    Density

    0 20 40 60 80 100PERCENT LECTURES ATTEND

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = 3.56

    Round 2

    0

    .01

    .02

    .03

    .04

    Density

    0 20 40 60 80 100Lectures

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = 3.44

    Round 3

    Source: Irish University Study

    higher attendance rates between 70% and 80% occurred for smaller class sizes of between 30 and

    160, whereas the lower rates of 15% to 35% occurred for larger class sizes of between 185 and 485.

    The Irish Universities Study includes students from all courses but the UCD Attendance Survey

    only includes students enrolled in STEM (science, technology, engineering and maths) courses. These

    STEM courses have a greater amount of lectures to attend, which could possibly result in lower levels

    of attendance (compared to non-STEM courses). The above consideration would lead one to expect a

    higher level of percentage lectures attended in the Irish University Study, compared to the UCD

    Attendance Survey. It should also be noted that there is heterogeneity in students level ofpercentage

    lectures attendedacross the seven Irish universities; the range spans from 78% in one university to

    87% in another. In addition, students may be attending more of their lectures in the recession than

    they used to beforehand. University students in the UK study for two hours and 12 minutes more (per

    week) now than they did two years ago (in 2007), according to the Higher Education Policy Institute

    (2009).8

    Finally, there remains the possibility that the web-survey designed by the authors may have

    received an unrepresentative level of response from more conscientious (or diligent) students; and

    that this conscientiousness is correlated with higher levels of lecture attendance. As mentioned earlier

    8 Given lower levels of labour demand in the part-time jobs market, there is certainly less opportunity for students toallocate their time to work (that is, there is diminished opportunity-cost of study-time). In addition, the evolving crisis in thegraduate labour market may motivate students to be more patient; and achieve higher academic standards (1 in 3 menunder the age of 25 are currently unemployed in Ireland).

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    17/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    17

    in this section, robustness checks between the data from the Irish Universities Studyand data available

    for thepopulation of Irish university students are presented in Appendix A (across gender, institution

    and area of study). Overall, on several observables, the sample is representative of its underlying

    population. Of course, conscientiousness is a typically unobserved variable and therefore it is

    unavailable in the official data on the population of Irish university students. However,

    conscientiousness is measured in the Irish University Study, and Figure 7 shows that the sample

    examined in this paper score highly on that variable.

    The independent variables are grouped into four themes: (i) student characteristics (ii) family

    demographics (iii) individual differences, and (iv) fixed effects (based on controlling for university).

    Student characteristics include students age, students gender (whether the student is male), the year

    of the course that the student is studying in, additional study-hours and any financial supports that are

    received. Additional study-hours are measured using the following question: How many hours per week

    do you spend on average on personal study time? Students give their answer in a grid comprised of

    hours (per week), categorised as follows: 0, 1-5, 6-10, 11-15, 16-20, 21-30, 31-40, 41-50, 51-60, 60+.

    Fig. 6: Attendance Rate vs. Enrolment (UCD Attendance Survey; 2009)

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    18/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    18

    Fig. 7: Kernel Histogram of Students Conscientiousness

    0

    .05

    .1

    .15

    Density

    0 5 10 15conscie

    Financial supports include finance received from students parents, and finance received from the

    state. Finance received from students parents is the sum of direct transfers and indirect payments on

    the behalf of students. The family demographics are as follows: whether the students father has some

    higher education, whether the students father has a professional or lower professional occupation,

    and the family income of the student. Individual differences are measured by willingness to take risks

    (on a scale of 0-10), consideration of future consequences (on a scale of 4-25) and the personality traits:

    openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism. These personality traits are

    proxies for non-cognitive ability traits. The measurement of these variables relating to individualdifferences was discussed in detail in Ryan, Delaney and Harmon (2010).

    Table 4 (for Round 2 of the study) and Table 5 (for Round 3 of the study) show summary

    statistics for all of the data discussed in this section. The average percentage grade-score is 65-58, the

    average percentage of lectures attended is 83-84, the average year in university is 2-2.5, and the

    average students age is 21-22. About 36% of students are male, approximately 50% of students

    fathers have some higher education, and average family-income is in the range of 60,000-80,000.

    Students average points-score in the Leaving Certificate is 475; their average time spent studying per

    week is 15 hours. Table 6 shows the test/re-test reliability of the data discussed in this section. The

    top half of the table shows variables where variation is expected over time; the bottom half of the

    table shows variables where variation is not expected over time. Leaving Cert. points, students age

    and students gender are virtually identical, which gives the data string credibility. It is possible that

    the variable indicating whether fathers have some higher education could be changing over time; the

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    19/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    19

    correlation for this variable is 0.9. A correlation coefficient of 0.7 is viewed by most researchers as the

    threshold for stability. Theory suggests that students willingness to take risks and consideration of

    future consequences should change very slowly over time; the correlation for these variables are 0.59

    and 0.61, respectively. Figure 8 illustrates the willingness to take risks variable; Figure 9 illustrates

    the consideration of future consequences variable.

    Table 4: Summary Statistics: Round 2

    Variable Mean S.D. N

    Average grade 65.0 9.87 604Lecture attendance 83.4 15.7 741

    Year in university 2.00 0.97 801Age of student 21.2 5.07 803Student is male 0.35 0.47 803

    Students father has HE 0.50 0.50 752Family-income bracket 4.21 2.51 773Future-orientation 13.9 3.54 751

    Willing to take risks 6.33 1.66 755

    Leaving Cert. points 474 76.0 719

    Study-time interval 2.99 1.30 733

    Table 5: Summary Statistics: Round 3

    Variable Mean S.D. N

    Average grade 68.5 8.34 681Lecture attendance 84.3 18.2 683

    Year in university 2.64 0.99 799Age of student 21.9 5.08 803Student is male 0.35 0.48 803Students father has HE 0.50 0.50 756Family-income bracket 4.09 2.35 755Future-orientation 14.0 3.51 732

    Willing to take risks 5.96 1.88 745

    Leaving Cert. points 476 73.9 706

    Study-time interval 3.44 1.52 769

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    20/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    20

    Table 6: Test/Re-Test Reliability of the Data

    Variable R3-Mean R2-Mean Corr.

    Year in university 2.64 2.01 0.44Average grade 68.5 65.1 0.50

    Study-time interval 3.44 2.99 0.56

    Lecture attendance 84.4 83.4 0.65Family-income bracket 4.09 4.21 0.81

    Willing to take risks* 5.96 6.33 0.59Future-orientation* 14.1 13.9 0.61Students father has HE* 0.50 0.50 0.90

    Leaving Cert. points* 476 474 0.99

    Age of student* 22.0 21.2 0.99

    Student is male* 0.35 0.35 0.99

    *Do not expect variation across time

    V I . M e th o d a n d R e su l ts

    The determinants of students academic achievement (university grades) are estimated using the

    following econometric model:

    Fig. 8: Attitude to Risk over Time

    0

    .1

    .2

    .3

    Density

    0 2 4 6 8 10RISKS

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = .35

    Round 2

    Kernel density estimate

    0

    .05

    .1

    .15

    .2

    .25

    Density

    0 2 4 6 8 10Willing to take risks

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = .36

    Round 3

    Kernel density estimate

    Source: Irish University Study

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    21/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    21

    Fig. 9: Future Orientation over Time

    0

    .05

    .1

    .15

    Density

    5 10 15 20patience_2

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = .71

    Round 2

    Kernel density estimate

    0

    .05

    .1

    .15

    Density

    5 10 15 20patience

    kernel = epanechnikov, bandwidth = .85

    Round 3

    Kernel density estimate

    Source: Irish University Study

    where Y is grade-score for student i in university j in year t, R is a vector for the lecture attendance of

    student i in university j in year t, X is a vector of observable student and family characteristics for

    student i in university j in year t, C is a vector of typically unobservable individual differences for

    student i attending university j in year t, and and are a set of fixed effects for university j and year

    t, respectively. Finally, is a stochastic error term for student i in university j in year t. The effect of R

    on Y is the focus of this study; direct measurement of C (over time) is also a major part of the analysis.

    represents a set of estimates for the effect of attendance on the educational outcome in question.

    Academic achievement is modelled using ordinary least squares (OLS) regression on the same

    individuals over two periods in time: spring 2009 and spring 2010. The results are shown in Table 7.The first column is the baseline specification. The second column (Lagged) estimates the same

    model but adds lags of university grade-score, lecture attendance and hours of study. The third

    column (Replaced) substitutes Round 3 measures of future-orientation and attitude to risk with

    measures from Round 2. The fourth column (Big 5) adds the Big 5 personality traits. The fifth

    column (STEM) adds a binary indicator to control for whether a student is enrolled in a STEM course.

    The sixth column (FE) adds institutional fixed effects based on university attended. Where they apply,

    control variables for missing value adjustment, STEM-enrolment (statistically insignificant) and

    institutional fixed effects (all statistically insignificant) are not shown in the results. Outliers and

    missing values are adjusted only for independent variables. Making adjustments for outliers and

    missing values (in particular on the lecture attendance variable) does not affect the results.

    There is a clearly positive and very statistically significant relationship between lecture

    attendance and grades, throughout every specification in Table 7. A one percentage increase in

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    22/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    22

    lecture attendance predicts on average (across the six specifications) an extra 6% of onepercentage

    grade score. This means that a student could improve their grade-score by one per cent if they attend

    approximately 15% more of their lectures. The relationship between lecture attendance and grades is

    visualised by a local polynomial smooth plot in Figure 10 (grade-score is on the vertical axis; lecture

    attendance is on the horizontal axis). Round 2 is shown on the left; Round 3 on the right.

    Fig. 10: The Relationship between Lecture Attendance and Grades

    Table 7: Students Average Grade at University: Robust OLS Regression

    (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

    Baseline Lagged Replaced Big 5 STEM FE

    Ave. % of lectures attended 0.075*** 0.040* 0.089*** 0.073*** 0.075*** 0.088***

    (0.022) (0.023) (0.021) (0.021) (0.022) (0.022)

    Students year of course -0.284 0.004 -0.209 -0.286 -0.241 -0.055

    (0.323) (0.335) (0.325) (0.321) (0.335) (0.327)

    Age of the student 0.003 -0.074 0.001 -0.000 0.005 -0.040

    (0.149) (0.139) (0.151) (0.151) (0.149) (0.145)

    Whether the student is male 0.549 0.460 0.180 0.839 0.530 0.247

    (0.617) (0.560) (0.627) (0.636) (0.618) (0.617)

    Whether the father has HE -0.343 -0.716 -0.181 -0.231 -0.331 -0.032

    (0.722) (0.653) (0.688) (0.720) (0.721) (0.696)Family income in brackets 0.219 0.224* 0.219 0.249* 0.214 0.199

    (0.136) (0.130) (0.136) (0.137) (0.136) (0.133)

    Students future-orientation 0.620*** 0.486*** 0.572*** 0.624*** 0.600***

    (0.094) (0.089) (0.095) (0.095) (0.096)

    Students att. to take risks -0.055 0.099 -0.069 -0.054 -0.024

    (0.186) (0.172) (0.197) (0.186) (0.177)

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    23/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    23

    Students LC points-score 0.002 -0.008* -0.000 0.002 0.003 0.002

    (0.005) (0.005) (0.002) (0.005) (0.005) (0.005)

    Hours of study in brackets 0.275 0.051 0.285 0.266 0.272 0.337

    (0.208) (0.223) (0.214) (0.211) (0.208) (0.206)

    Students openness 0.291*

    (0.164)

    Students conscientiousness 0.184

    (0.146)

    Students extraversion -0.157

    (0.112)

    Students agreeableness 0.115

    (0.149)

    Students neuroticism 0.094

    (0.126)

    Lag of university grade 0.360***

    (0.054)

    Lag of lecture attendance 0.020

    (0.027)

    Lag of study-hours 0.014

    (0.303)

    Round 2 future-orientation 0.542***

    (0.104)

    Round 2 att. to take risks 0.258(0.199)

    Constant 51.581*** 36.761*** 50.580*** 46.883*** 51.325*** 51.824***

    (4.580) (4.899) (3.855) (5.573) (4.603) (5.262)

    Observations 655 581 655 655 655 655

    R-squared 0.139 0.322 0.123 0.149 0.140 0.197*** p

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    24/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    24

    to some previous findings in the literature.

    V I . C on cl us io n

    The key results from this paper show that lecture attendance is more important for academic

    achievement than scholars additional hours of study, what subject area they enroll in9, what university

    they study at, many of their personality traits and all of their family background, including parental

    education and family-income. Indeed, the only predictors of better academic performance across the

    six specifications in Table 7 are lecture attendance and future-orientation. Furthermore, these

    relationships are observed over a longitudinal setting. Strikingly, the relationship between lecture

    attendance and academic achievement is virtually the same after controlling for personality traits

    related to non-cognitive ability. This addresses the concern that more motivated, dedicated and future-

    orientated students are more likely to attend their lectures as well as achieve higher grades.Of course, the finding related to students lecture attendance is most central to institutional

    policy setting in higher education. Attendance is voluntary in many college classes, primarily because

    of the difficulty in taking attendance on a regular basis, but also because of the view that students should

    have some autonomy in determining the manner in which they engage with academic material

    (Stephenson, 1994).10 Mandatory attendance policy becomes more of an issue, however, where there is a

    professional element to a programme. In nursing, for example, there is a high minimum attendance

    stipulated by the Irish Nursing Board (2005): students must attend 80% of a minimum of 1,533 hours.

    Another argument in favour of requiring attendance is that it is needed to develop a strong sense of

    community in a classroom. Indeed, there may be outcomes other than academic achievement which have

    an important relationship with students lecture attendance. A recent report from the institutional

    research office at one Irish university has recommended that mandatory attendance policy could be

    one mechanism to reduce student drop-out (Blaney and Mulkeen, 2009).11 Attendance monitoring

    has been used as a trigger for student interventions at Newcastle University (Bevitt et al. 2009). The

    authors are currently working on a research note that will report on the relatively unexplored topic of

    students lecture attendance and their well-being.

    Future research should examine the impact of experimental designs related to enforcing

    and/or monitoring lecture attendance. There is much debate on what incentives or penalties are

    9 This is only the second study, which the authors are aware of, to examine the higher education production function acrossmultiple subject areas10 Analogies can be drawn here with libertarian paternalism debates in public behavioural economics.11 The report is entitled Student Retention in a Modular World - A Study of Retention of UCD Entrants: 1999-2007.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    25/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    25

    appropriate in this regard as penalising students for not showing up can be seen as double jeopardy:

    they would be punished by lower test scores in addition to a lower attendance score. While some

    instructors may dislike mandatory attendance policies because they can be a lot of work to enforce,

    there are recent technological advances such as dibbers (used at the Lancaster University

    geography department) or clickers (Hoekstra, 2008) which substantially ease the burden of

    collecting attendance data. Smart-card technology is available explicitly for the use of measuring

    student attendance;12 there are even new electronic systems which are being used to detect the ID

    cards students are carrying as they enter classrooms at Arizona University. Future research should

    also make comparisons between headcount data and self-reported lecture attendance. One of the

    authors is currently working on a small project which will compare self-reported grade-scores and

    administrative grade data for one Economics class at an Irish university.

    Finally, new innovations such as online learning also bear consideration. 4.6 million students

    in the United States (1 out of every 4) took a college-level online course at the start of the 2008/09

    academic year.13 Given the importance of face-to-face lectures for students academic achievement (as

    demonstrated in this study), it should be an immediate priority to establish how online lectures

    compare. Indeed, some amount of online learning may be inevitable in the future due to recent

    pressures on resources in higher education: both reduced levels of funding and higher levels of

    student enrolment. Another possibility is that the availability of online materials may discourage

    attendance. According to the results of a MIT survey, the penalty to not going to a lecture is reduced

    by the presence of online learning materials (Clay and Breslow, 2006).

    R e fe r e nce s

    An Bord Altranais (2005) Requirements and Standards for Nurse Registration Education Programmes, 3rdedition, Dublin: An Bord Altranais.

    Anderson, Gordon, Dwayne Benjamin, and Melvyn A. Fuss. 1994. The Determinants of Success inUniversity Introductory Economics Courses. Journal of Economic Education Spring: 99-119.

    Anikeeff, M. (1954), The Relationship Between Class Absences and College Grades, Journal of EducationalPsychology, 45, 244-249.

    Arulampalam, W., Naylor, R. and Smith, J. Am I Missing Something? The Effects of Absence from Class on

    12 The TDS Student Attendance Monitoring Solution is currently being used at one Irish university: DCU.13 This was a 17% increase from the previous academic year, according to the seventh annual Sloan Survey of OnlineLearning (Allen and Seaman, 2009). A large majority of students about three million were simultaneously enrolled inface-to-face courses.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    26/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    26

    Student Performance. IZA Discussion Papers 3749, 2008, Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA).

    Bandiera, O., Larcinese, V. and Rasul, I. (2008). Heterogeneous class size effects: new evidence from a panelof university students, LSE mimeo.

    Becker, William E. and John R. Powers. 2001. Student Performance, Attrition, and Class Size Given MissingStudent Data. Economics of Education Review 20: 377-388.

    Bonin, H., Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Hu ma n , D. and Sunde, U. Cross-sectional Earnings Risk and OccupationalSorting: The Role of Risk Attitudes. Labour Economics, 2007, 14(6), pp. 926-937.

    Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. Heckman, J.J. and ter Weel, B. The Economics and Psychology of PersonalityTraits. Journal of Human Resources, 2008, 43 (4).

    Brocato, J. (1989), How Much Does Coming to Class Matter? Some Evidence of Class Attendance and GradePerformance, Educational Research Quarterly, 13, 2-6.

    Browne, M. Neil and John H. Hoag. 1995. Can We (in Good Conscience) Require Attendance in OurClassroom? The Journal of Economics 21: 116-119.

    Buckalew, W., Daly, D., and K. Coffield (1986), Relationship of Initial Class Attendance and SeatingLocation to Academic Performance in Psychology Classes, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 24, 63-64.

    Chan, Kam C., Shum, Connie, Wright, David J. 1997. Class Attendance and Student Performance inPrinciples of Finance Financial Practice and Education 7: 58-65.

    Chen, J. and Lin, T-F. (2006). Class attendance and exam performance: A randomized experiment.

    Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=908923

    Cohn, E. and Johnson, E. (2006) Class attendance and performance in principles of economics, EducationEconomics 14 (2): 211233.

    Deere, Donald. 1994. Correspondence: Should Class Attendance be Mandatory? Journal of EconomicPerspectives 8 (3): 210-211.

    Devadoss, S. and Foltz, J. (1996). Evaluation of factors influencing student class attendance andperformance. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 78, 499-507.

    Dobkin, C., Gil, R., and Marion, J. (2007). Causes and consequences of skipping class in college. Mimeo, UCSanta Cruz.

    Dohmen, T. Falk, A., Hu ma n, D., Sunde, U., Schupp J. and Wagner, G.G. Individual Risk Attitudes: NewEvidence from a Large, Representative, Experimentally-Validated Survey. IZA Discussion Paper No. 1730,2005.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    27/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    27

    Dolton, P., Marcenaro, O. and Navarro, L. The Effective Use of Student Time: A Stochastic FrontierProduction Function Case Study. Economics of Education Review, 2003, 22(6), pp. 547-60.

    Douglas, S. and Sulock, J. (1995). Estimating educational production functions with correction for drops,Journal of Economic Education, 26, 101-112.

    Durden, G. C. and Ellis, L. V. (1995). The effect of attendance on student learning in Principles ofEconomics. American Economic Review, 85, 343-346.

    Ehrenberg, R. G., & Sherman, D. R. (1987). Employment while in college: Academic achievement andpostcollege outcomes. Journal of Human Resources, 22, 1-23.

    Ehrenberg, R. G., Brewer, D., Gamoran, A. and Willms, J. D. (2001). Class Size and Student Achievement.Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2 1-30.

    Eikebrokk, T. and Nyhus, E. The Consideration of Future Consequences Scale: An Assessment and Review.University of Agder Working Paper, 2008.

    Emerson, Tisha L. N. and Beck A. Taylor. 2004. Comparing Student Achievement Across Experimental andLecture-Oriented Sections of a Principles of Microeconomics Course. Southern Economic Journal, 2004 70(3): 672-693.

    Gosling, S., Rentfrow, P. and Swann Jr., W. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journalof Research in Personality, 2003, 37, pp. 504-528.

    Grabe, M. (2005) Voluntary use of online lecture notes: correlates of note use and note use as an alternativeto class attendance, Computers and Education 44: 409421.

    Gunn, P. (1993), A Correlation between Attendance and Grades in a First-year Psychology Course, CanadianPsychology, 34, 201-202.

    Hanushek, Eric A. 1979. "Conceptual and empirical issues in the estimation of educational productionfunctions." Journal of Human Resources 14, no.3 (Summer):351-388.

    Hanushek, E. A., Kain, J. F., Markman, J. M. and Rivkin, S. G. (2003). Does Peer Ability Affect StudentAchievement? Journal of Applied Econometrics, 18, 527-544.

    Hanushek, E. Educational Production Functions.Palgrave Encyclopedia, January 2007.

    Hopkins, D. S. P. (1990). The higher education production function: Theoretical foundations and empiricalfindings. In S. A. Hoenack & E. L. Collins (Eds.), The Economics of American Universities: Management,Operations, and Fiscal Environment (pp. 11-32). Albany: State University of New York Press.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    28/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    28

    Jaeger, D., Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Hu m a n , D. Sunde, W. and Bonin, H. Direct Evidence on Risk Attitudes andMi gr at i on . Th e Review of Economics and Statistics, August 2010, Vol. 92, No. 3, Pages 684-689.

    Joireman, J., Balliet, D., Sprott, D., Spangenberg, E., and Shultz, J. (2008). Consideration of future consequences,ego-depletion, and self-control: Support for distinguishing between CFC-immediate and CFC-future sub-scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 15-21.

    Jones, H. (1984), Interaction of Absences and Grades in a College Course, The Journal of Psychology, 116, 133-136.

    Kalenkoski, C.M., and Pabilonia, S.W. Parental Transfers, Student Achievement, and the Labour Supply of CollegeStudents. US Bureau of Labour Statistics (BLS) Working Papers, 2008, 401.

    Kennedy, Peter E. and John J. Siegfried,. 1997. Class Size and Achievement in Introductory Economics:Evidence from TUCE III Data. Economics of Education Review 16 (4) 385-394.

    Kirby, A. and McElroy, B. (2003). The effect of attendance on grade for first year Economics students in

    University College Cork. The Economic and Social Review, vol. 34(3), pp 311-326.

    Krohn, G. A. and OConnor, C. M. (2005). Student effort and performance over the semester, Journal ofEconomic Education, 36, pp. 3-28.

    Kyllonen, C. Enhancing Noncognitive Skills to Boost Academic Achievement. Educational Testing in America:State Assessments, Achievement Gaps, National Policy and Innovations. Princeton Educational TestingService, 2008.

    Lin, T. and J. Chen (2006), Cumulative Class Attendance and Exam Performance, Applied Economics Letters.

    Marburger DR (2001) ,Absenteeism and Economic Exam Performance, The Journal of Economic Education32:99-109.

    Marburger, D. R. (2006). Does mandatory attendance improve student performance, Journal of EconomicEducation, 37, 148-154.

    Martins, P. and Walker, I . Student Achievement and University Classes: Eects of Attendance, Size, Peers, andTeachers.IZA Discussion Papers 2490, 2006, Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA).

    Maxwell, Nan L. and Jane S. Lopus. 1994. The Lake Wobegon Effect in Student Self-Reported Data. AmericanEconomic Review Papers and Proceedings 84: 201-205.

    Neri, F. and Meloche, Y. (2007). The impact of lecture attendance on academic performance in a large firstyear Economics course. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=975573

    Oettinger, G. Parents Financial Support, Students Employment, and Academic Performance in College.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    29/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    29

    University of Texas Working Paper, 2005.

    Park, Kang H. and Peter Kerr. 1990. Determinants of Academic Performance: A Multinomial Logit Approach.Journal of Economic Education 21: 101-111.

    Plant EA et Al (2005) Why Study time does not predict grade point average amongacross college students:Implications of Deliberate practice for academic performanceContemporary Educational Psychology 30:96-116.

    Raimondo, Henry J, Louis Esposito, and Irving Gershenberg. 1990. Introductory Class Size and StudentPerformance in Intermediate Theory Courses. Journal of Economic Education, (Fall): 369-381.

    Rocca, A. K. (2003), Student Attendance: A Comprehensive Literature Review, Journal on Excellence inCollege Teaching, 14, 85-107.

    Rodgers, J.R. (2002) Encouraging tutorial attendance at university did not improve performance, AustralianEconomic Papers, September, 255266.

    Romer, D. Do Students Go to Class? Should T h e y? T h e Journal of Economic Perspectives, 1993, 7(3), pp. 167-74.

    Romer, D. (1994). Response in Correspondence, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 8, no.3, 214-215.

    Schmidt RM (1983), Who maximises what? A study of student time allocation, The American EconomicReview, 73:23-28.

    Stanca L (2006) , The effects of Attendance on Academic Performance: Panel Data Evidence for Introductory

    Microeconomics The Journal of Economic Education, 37:251-266.

    Stinebrickner, R. and Stinebrickner, T. R. The Causal Ef f e c t of Studying on Academic Performance. Journal ofLabour Economics, 2007.

    Stinebrickner, Ralph, and Todd R. Stinebrickner. (2004). Time-Use and College Outcomes. Journal ofEconometrics, 121 (1-2): 243-269.

    Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger D. and Edwards, C. The consideration of future consequences: weighingimmediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1994, 66 (4), pp.742-752.

    Van Blerkom, L. (1992), Class Attendance in an Undergraduate Course, Journal of Psychology, 126, 487-494.

    Ziegert, Andrea L. 2000. The Role of Personality Temperament and Student Learning in Principles ofEconomics: Further Evidence. Journal of Economic Education 31 (4): 307-322.

  • 7/23/2019 Effects of Orientation Program to Academic Programs

    30/30

    Does Lecture Attendance Matter for Grades? Evidence from Longitudinal Tracking of Irish Students

    Zimmerman, D. J. (2003). Peer effects in academic outcomes: Evidence from a natural experiment. TheReview of Economics and Statistics, 85(1), 9-23.

    Appendix A: IUS (Survey) Data versus HEA (Population) Data

    Gender IUS Data Set 2 (09/10) HEA 2009 IUS Data Set 1 (08/09) HEA 2008

    Male 37% 43% 36% 42%

    Female 63% 57% 64% 58%

    University

    DCU 6% 9% 6% 9%

    NUIG 12% 16% 16% 16%

    NUIM 12% 8% 10% 7%

    TCD 19% 15% 18% 15%

    UCC 20% 18% 17% 18%

    UCD 24% 23% 23% 23%

    UL 7% 12% 10% 11%

    SubjectEducation 2% 4% 3% 5%

    Humanities & Arts 23% 25% 24% 25%

    Social Science 11% 7% 10% 6%

    Business 11% 13% 12% 13%

    Law 4% 6% 5% 7%

    Science 16% 12% 15% 11%

    Maths 3% 1% 2% 1%

    Computing 3% 3% 3% 3%

    Engineering 7% 8% 8% 8%

    Agriculture 2% 2% 2% 2%

    Health 15% 18% 12% 18%

    Sport 0% 0% 1% 0%

    Other 3% 2% 4% 2%