4
CYBERPSYCHOLOGY & BEHAVIOR Volume 12, Number 1, 2009 © Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. DOI: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0107 Rapid Communication Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students’ Performance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task? Annie Beth Fox, B.A., Jonathan Rosen, B.A., and Mary Crawford, Ph.D. Abstract Instant messaging (IM) has become one of the most popular forms of computer-mediated communication (CMC) and is especially prevalent on college campuses. Previous research suggests that IM users often multitask while conversing online. To date, no one has yet examined the cognitive effect of concurrent IM use. Participants in the present study (N 69) completed a reading comprehension task uninterrupted or while concurrently hold- ing an IM conversation. Participants who IMed while performing the reading task took significantly longer to complete the task, indicating that concurrent IM use negatively affects efficiency. Concurrent IM use did not affect reading comprehension scores. Additional analyses revealed that the more time participants reported spending on IM, the lower their reading comprehension scores. Finally, we found that the more time partici- pants reported spending on IM, the lower their self-reported GPA. Implications and future directions are dis- cussed. 51 Introduction W ITH THE GROWTH IN POPULARITY of instant messaging (IM) programs, an increasing number of people are multitasking while communicating online. 1 It is possible that IM use may negatively impact performance on a concur- rently performed task. Research has demonstrated that the concurrent use of media (e.g., television) during an intellec- tual task (e.g., reading) negatively affects recall and recog- nition memory. 2 Most college students spend a significant amount of time engaged in intellectual activities like study- ing, and many students likely study while socializing over IM. College students who multitask in this way may be putting themselves at a disadvantage. Most research has used IM to deliver an interruption (thereby dividing attention or forcing a switch between tasks) rather than to examine how sustained concurrent use of IM affects memory or performance. An exception is a study by Cummings, 3 who found that concurrent use of IM during a cognitively demanding task led to a decrease in per- formance. Instead of focusing on directing and controlling missiles (the primary task), participants were focused on IM, through which they received messages and queries, which inhibited their performance on the primary task. These re- sults are consistent with the findings of a qualitative study of IM in the workplace. Workers reported that concurrent IM use can be interruptive and detrimental to their atten- tion. 4 Considering the limited research on the effect of IM on concurrent task performance, the purpose of the present study was to examine whether concurrent use of IM hinders students’ abilities to perform a learning task—reading prose for future recall. Participants were given an easy or difficult reading passage and then asked to answer multiple-choice and free-recall questions about the passage. Half of the par- ticipants held concurrent IM conversations while reading and answering questions. We made three predictions: H1: Participants who IM while reading the passage and an- swering questions should perform worse on recall and recognition tests than those who perform the task uninter- rupted. H2: Participants who read the high-difficulty passage should perform worse than those who read the low-diffi- culty passage. Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut.

Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students' Performance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?

  • Upload
    mary

  • View
    217

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students' Performance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?

CYBERPSYCHOLOGY & BEHAVIOR

Volume 12, Number 1, 2009© Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.DOI: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0107

Rapid Communication

Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students’ Performance on a Concurrent

Reading Comprehension Task?

Annie Beth Fox, B.A., Jonathan Rosen, B.A., and Mary Crawford, Ph.D.

Abstract

Instant messaging (IM) has become one of the most popular forms of computer-mediated communication (CMC)and is especially prevalent on college campuses. Previous research suggests that IM users often multitask whileconversing online. To date, no one has yet examined the cognitive effect of concurrent IM use. Participants inthe present study (N � 69) completed a reading comprehension task uninterrupted or while concurrently hold-ing an IM conversation. Participants who IMed while performing the reading task took significantly longer tocomplete the task, indicating that concurrent IM use negatively affects efficiency. Concurrent IM use did notaffect reading comprehension scores. Additional analyses revealed that the more time participants reportedspending on IM, the lower their reading comprehension scores. Finally, we found that the more time partici-pants reported spending on IM, the lower their self-reported GPA. Implications and future directions are dis-cussed.

51

Introduction

WITH THE GROWTH IN POPULARITY of instant messaging(IM) programs, an increasing number of people are

multitasking while communicating online.1 It is possible thatIM use may negatively impact performance on a concur-rently performed task. Research has demonstrated that theconcurrent use of media (e.g., television) during an intellec-tual task (e.g., reading) negatively affects recall and recog-nition memory.2 Most college students spend a significantamount of time engaged in intellectual activities like study-ing, and many students likely study while socializing overIM. College students who multitask in this way may beputting themselves at a disadvantage.

Most research has used IM to deliver an interruption(thereby dividing attention or forcing a switch betweentasks) rather than to examine how sustained concurrent useof IM affects memory or performance. An exception is astudy by Cummings,3 who found that concurrent use of IMduring a cognitively demanding task led to a decrease in per-formance. Instead of focusing on directing and controllingmissiles (the primary task), participants were focused on IM,

through which they received messages and queries, whichinhibited their performance on the primary task. These re-sults are consistent with the findings of a qualitative studyof IM in the workplace. Workers reported that concurrentIM use can be interruptive and detrimental to their atten-tion.4 Considering the limited research on the effect of IM onconcurrent task performance, the purpose of the presentstudy was to examine whether concurrent use of IM hindersstudents’ abilities to perform a learning task—reading prosefor future recall. Participants were given an easy or difficultreading passage and then asked to answer multiple-choiceand free-recall questions about the passage. Half of the par-ticipants held concurrent IM conversations while readingand answering questions. We made three predictions:

H1: Participants who IM while reading the passage and an-swering questions should perform worse on recall andrecognition tests than those who perform the task uninter-rupted.

H2: Participants who read the high-difficulty passageshould perform worse than those who read the low-diffi-culty passage.

Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut.

Page 2: Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students' Performance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?

H3: There will be an interaction between IM condition andtask difficulty such that participants who IM while readingthe difficult passage will perform worse on the task thanparticipants in any other condition.

Method

Sixty-nine undergraduate students participated for coursecredit in an introductory psychology course. All participantsused AOL Instant Messenger (AIM) to communicate anony-mously with a confederate partner. Two reading passages ofvarying difficulty were used: an SAT-level passage (low dif-ficulty) and a GRE passage (high difficulty). Passages weretaken from SAT and GRE practice exams. A 14-item readingcomprehension test containing seven multiple-choice ques-tions (to test recognition memory) and seven fill-in-the-blankquestions (to test free-recall memory) was created for eachof the passages. Presentation order of the tests was counter-balanced across conditions. A survey of computer and In-ternet use and attitudes was also administered.

A 2 � 2 (CMC condition: IM versus no IM � passage dif-ficulty: high versus low) factorial design was used. Partici-pants were randomly assigned to conditions upon arrival atthe lab, were assigned a (confederate) partner, and were toldto communicate with that partner over AIM for 5 minutesto get to know one another. After 5 minutes, participantswere given either the high- or low-difficulty reading passageand instructed to take as much time as they needed to readand understand the passage. A hard copy of the reading pas-sage was presented to resemble the type of offline readingin which participants may engage. Half the participantsended their IM conversation immediately before reading thepassage (no-IM condition); half continued IMing while read-ing the passage (IM condition). An instruction screen onthe computer served as a hidden timer. When participantsfinished reading, they completed the reading comprehen-sion test and then filled out the survey of computer use andattitudes.

Results

Participants were highly experienced and frequent usersof IM (M � 5.3 years experience; M � 1.76 hours a day spenton IM). A 2 � 2 (CMC condition: no IM versus IM � passagedifficulty: high versus low) ANOVA was performed on mul-tiple-choice score, free-recall score, total score, time to com-plete multiple-choice questions, time to complete free-recallquestions, and total time to complete all questions. We didnot find a significant main effect of CMC condition on free-recall, multiple-choice, or total test score, nor did we find theexpected interaction between CMC condition and difficulty.Thus, neither hypothesis 1 nor hypothesis 3 was supported.

A main effect of CMC condition emerged for time to re-spond to multiple-choice questions, time to complete the en-tire test, and time to read the passage. Participants in the IMcondition (M � 4.66, SD � 1.87) took significantly longer tocomplete the multiple-choice section than did those in theno-IM condition (M � 2.63, SD � 0.89), F(1, 66) � 31.60, p �0.001. Similarly, those in the IM condition (M � 12.56, SD �4.39) took significantly longer to complete the entire test thandid those in the no-IM condition (M � 8.23, SD � 2.48), F(1,66) � 24.36, p � 0.001. Participants in the IM condition alsotook longer to read the passage (M � 5.53, SD � 2.86) than

did those in the no-IM condition (M � 3.33, SD � 1.29), F(1,66) � 16.39, p � 0.001.

Hypothesis 2 was supported for free-recall and total scoresbut not for multiple-choice scores. Participants in the diffi-cult condition (M � 3.58, SD � 1.52) scored significantlyworse than those in the easy condition on the free-recall test(M � 5.23, SD � 1.03), F(1, 66) � 27.61, p � 0.001. Partici-pants in the difficult condition (M � 5.96, SD � 2.95) alsohad significantly lower total scores than did those in the easycondition (M � 8.11, SD � 1.64), F(1,66) � 13.96, p � 0.001.Additionally, those in the difficult condition (M � 2.23, SD �0.73) took significantly longer to answer the free-recall ques-tions than did participants who read the easy passage (M �1.77, SD � 1.16), F(1, 66) � 4.20, p � 0.044. These results val-idate our difficulty manipulation in that the GRE passagewas indeed more difficult than the SAT passage for partici-pants to complete. That an effect of difficulty was not foundfor multiple-choice questions is consistent with research sug-gesting that recognition is easier than recall. No other maineffects or interactions were found with respect to the read-ing comprehension data.

We also examined whether experience with IM predictedtest scores, holding IM and difficulty condition constant.Multiple regression analyses demonstrated that averagedaily use of IM (in minutes) was a significant predictor ofmultiple-choice (� � �0.30, t(65) � �2.56, p � 0.01), free-re-call (� � �0.20, t(65) � �1.96, p � 0.05), and total score onthe test (� � �0.30, t(65) � �2.82, p � 0.01). The more timeparticipants reported spending on IM, the lower their com-prehension scores.

Given this unexpected finding, that self-reported IM usewas negatively related to cognitive performance, we con-ducted an exploratory analysis using another measure ofcognitive performance: grade point average. Participantswho reported lower GPAs were more likely to also reportspending more time per day on IM (� � �0.23, t(68) ��1.97, p � 0.05). Thus, there was a negative association be-tween IM use and two independent self-report measures ofcognitive performance.

Discussion

Two of our three hypotheses were not supported. Theseresult are inconsistent with prior research indicating that in-terruptions negatively affect task performance.5 There maybe several explanations. First, because the participants werehighly experienced IM users, they may frequently conversewith more than one person while performing a concurrenttask. Conversing with one person may not have challengedthe participants’ multitasking abilities. Relatedly, readingand IMing at the same time may also not have stretched theirabilities as multitaskers. However, our data contradict thisexplanation. We found that average daily IM use was nega-tively related to performance on the reading comprehensiontest, indicating that expertise did not help participants suc-cessfully complete the task. In fact, expertise with IM pre-dicted lower scores on the comprehension test. Interestingly,GPA also was negatively related to time spent on IM. Ourresults suggest that poorer students spend more time in on-line communication with others, and the more time spentIMing, the lower the scores on the reading comprehensiontest. Future studies involving IM and reading comprehen-sion should include measures that assess these factors.

FOX ET AL.52

Page 3: Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students' Performance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?

Another potential explanation for not finding a detrimentaleffect of IM is that IM may be a type of negotiated interrup-tion in which a participant decides when to switch tasks ratherthan being forced to do so upon an intrusive interruption.6 IMmay be viewed as a form of negotiated interruption becauseparticipants were free to decide when to switch between tasksand were able to continue the primary activity (reading and/oranswering questions) until ready to switch tasks. Because par-ticipants were able to negotiate the interruption, they also likelytook preparatory action regarding the primary task (i.e., not-ing where they left off) that would allow them to transitionback to the original task once they completed the interruptiontask. Research has demonstrated that performance on a pri-mary task is disrupted less when participants are warned be-fore an interruption is to occur.7 The time between the warn-ing and the interruption allows the individual to makepreparations for returning to the primary task.

Where we did see an effect of IM was in the time to com-plete the multiple-choice questions. Participants who IMedtook significantly longer to complete the multiple-choice testas well as to complete the entire test. This is consistent withprevious research demonstrating that interruptions increasethe time required to complete a primary task.8 We did not,however, find a significant effect of IM on time to completethe free-recall questions. This is an anomalous finding thatneeds to be investigated further.

Limitations and future directions

The present study has several limitations, including a rel-atively small sample size and limited generalizibility of thereading comprehension task. The reading passages wereshort, and although they tapped an important study skill(comprehension), the task itself may not be generalizable tothe type of reading and writing college students may actu-ally do when concurrently IMing.

Despite the ubiquity of IM, little is known about its cog-nitive consequences. To our knowledge, the present study isthe first to specifically examine the cognitive consequencesof IM. We did not find a cognitive decrement in experiencedIM users. However, we did find that concurrent IM use de-creased efficiency, and more importantly, there was an as-sociation between self-reported IM use and poor perfor-mance on the cognitive test. Our results suggest that IMcould impact college students’ academic performance, al-though the extent of that impact needs further examination.Additional research is needed in this area because IM and

similar CMC technologies that foster rapid processing andmultitasking have become an important part of the culturallandscape.

Disclosure Statement

The authors have no conflict of interest.

References

1. Shiu L, Lenhart A. (2004) How Americans use instant mes-saging. Pew Internet & American Life Project. www.pewin-ternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Instantmessage_Report.pdf.

2. Armstrong GB, Chung L. Background television and readingmemory in context. Communication Research 2000; 27:327–52.

3. Cummings ML. The need for command and control instantmessage adaptive interfaces: lessons learned from tacticalTomahawk human-in-the-loop simulations. CyberPsychol-ogy & Behavior 2004; 7:653–61.

4. Cameron AF, Webster J. Unintended consequences of emerg-ing communication technologies: instant messaging in theworkplace. Computers in Human Behavior 2005; 21:85–103.

5. Gillie T, Broadbent D. What makes interruptions disruptive?A study of length, similarity, and complexity. PsychologicalResearch 1989; 50:243–50.

6. McFarlane D. (1999) Coordinating the interruption of peoplein human–computer interaction. In Sasse A, Johnson C, eds.Proceedings of Human–Computer Interaction (INTERACT’99),Amsterdam: IOS Press, pp. 295-303. http://interruptions.net/literature/McFarlane Interact99-Coordinating.pdf.

7. Trafton JG, Altman EM, Brock DP, et al. Preparing to resumean interrupted task: effects of prospective goal encoding andretrospective rehearsal. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 2003; 58:583–603.

8. Adamczyk PD, Bailey BP. (2004) If not now, when? The ef-fects of interruption at different moments within task execu-tion. In Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors inComputing Systems, CHI 2004, pp. 271-8. http://orchid.cs.uiuc.edu/publications/acm-chi-2004-adamczyk.pdf.

Address reprint requests to:Annie Beth Fox

Department of PsychologyUniversity of Connecticut

406 Babbidge Road, Unit 1020Storrs, CT 06269

E-mail: [email protected]

IM AND CONCURRENT TASK PERFORMANCE 53

Page 4: Distractions, Distractions: Does Instant Messaging Affect College Students' Performance on a Concurrent Reading Comprehension Task?

This article has been cited by:

1. Laura E. Levine, Bradley M. Waite, Laura L. Bowman. 2014. Mobile Media Use, Multitasking and Distractibility. InternationalJournal of Cyber Behavior, Psychology and Learning 2:3, 15-29. [CrossRef]

2. Yuan-Hsuan Lee, Jiun-Yu Wu. 2013. The indirect effects of online social entertainment and information seeking activities onreading literacy. Computers & Education 67, 168-177. [CrossRef]

3. Joanne Meredith, Jonathan PotterConversation Analysis and Electronic Interactions 370-393. [CrossRef]4. Laura E. Levine, Bradley M. Waite, Laura L. Bowman. Use of Instant Messaging Predicts Self-Report but Not Performance

Measures of Inattention, Impulsiveness, and Distractibility. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, ahead of print.[Abstract] [Full Text HTML] [Full Text PDF] [Full Text PDF with Links]

5. Paul A. Kirschner, Jeroen J.G. van Merriënboer. 2013. Do Learners Really Know Best? Urban Legends in Education. EducationalPsychologist 48:3, 169-183. [CrossRef]

6. Aryn C. Karpinski, Paul A. Kirschner, Ipek Ozer, Jennifer A. Mellott, Pius Ochwo. 2013. An exploration of social networkingsite use, multitasking, and academic performance among United States and European university students. Computers in HumanBehavior 29:3, 1182-1192. [CrossRef]

7. Aditya Johri, Hon Jie Teo, Jenny Lo, Monique Dufour, Asta Schram. 2013. Millennial engineers: Digital media and informationecology of engineering students. Computers in Human Behavior . [CrossRef]

8. Harold Pashler, Sean H. K. Kang, Renita Y. Ip. 2013. Does Multitasking Impair Studying? Depends on Timing. Applied CognitivePsychology n/a-n/a. [CrossRef]

9. Brett R.C. Molesworth, Marion Burgess, Daniel Kwon. 2013. The use of noise cancelling headphones to improve concurrent taskperformance in a noisy environment. Applied Acoustics 74:1, 110-115. [CrossRef]

10. Anne Mangen, Bente R. Walgermo, Kolbjørn Brønnick. 2013. Reading linear texts on paper versus computer screen: Effects onreading comprehension. International Journal of Educational Research 58, 61-68. [CrossRef]

11. E. Pintor, P. Gargantilla, M. Rubio, B. Herreros. 2012. Aparatos electrónicos utilizados por los alumnos de medicina en las clasesteóricas: ¿herramientas docentes o fuentes de distracción?. Revista Clínica Española 212:9, 469-470. [CrossRef]

12. Rachel J. Katz-Sidlow, Allison Ludwig, Scott Miller, Robert Sidlow. 2012. Smartphone use during inpatient attending rounds:Prevalence, patterns and potential for distraction. Journal of Hospital Medicine 7:8, 595-599. [CrossRef]

13. Kelvin F. H. Lui, Alan C.-N. Wong. 2012. Does media multitasking always hurt? A positive correlation between multitaskingand multisensory integration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 19:4, 647-653. [CrossRef]

14. Laura E. Levine, Bradley M. Waite, Laura L. BowmanCybermedia Use, Multitasking, and Academic Distractibility 342-353.[CrossRef]

15. Baoxiang Song, Wei Wang. 2011. Instant messaging continuance: A media choice theory perspective. Frontiers of Business Researchin China 5:4, 537-558. [CrossRef]

16. Jennifer Lee, Lin Lin, Tip Robertson. 2011. The impact of media multitasking on learning. Learning, Media and Technology1-11. [CrossRef]

17. S. Adam Brasel, James Gips. 2011. Media Multitasking Behavior: Concurrent Television and Computer Usage. Cyberpsychology,Behavior, and Social Networking 14:9, 527-534. [Abstract] [Full Text HTML] [Full Text PDF] [Full Text PDF with Links][Supplemental Material]

18. Wade C. Jacobsen, Renata Forste. 2011. The Wired Generation: Academic and Social Outcomes of Electronic Media Use AmongUniversity Students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 14:5, 275-280. [Abstract] [Full Text HTML] [Full TextPDF] [Full Text PDF with Links]

19. Paul A. Kirschner, Aryn C. Karpinski. 2010. Facebook® and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior 26:6,1237-1245. [CrossRef]

20. Laura L. Bowman, Laura E. Levine, Bradley M. Waite, Michael Gendron. 2010. Can students really multitask? An experimentalstudy of instant messaging while reading. Computers & Education 54:4, 927-931. [CrossRef]