80
opmaak en foto van cover: www.hansvandijk.nl Marjolein Deunk Simone Doolaard Annemieke Smale-Jacobse Roel J. Bosker Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of studies into the cognitive effects of differentiation practices

Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

  • Upload
    lyque

  • View
    215

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

opmaak en foto van cover: www.hansvandijk.nl

Marjolein Deunk

Simone Doolaard

Annemieke Smale-Jacobse

Roel J. Bosker

Differentiation within and across classrooms:A systematic review of studies into the cognitive effects of differentiation practices

Page 2: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms:

A systematic review of studies into the cognitive effects of

differentiation practices

Marjolein Deunk

Simone Doolaard

Annemieke Smale-Jacobse

Roel J. Bosker

Page 3: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

ISBN 978-90-6690-586-3

© Maart 2015. GION onderwijs/onderzoek

Rijksuniversiteit, Grote Rozenstraat 3, 9712 TG Groningen

Niets uit deze uitgave mag worden verveelvoudigd en/of openbaar gemaakt door middel van

druk, fotokopie, microfilm of op welke andere wijze dan ook zonder voorafgaande

schriftelijke toestemming van de directeur van het instituut.

No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photo print, microfilm or any

other means without written permission of the director of the institute.

Page 4: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of
Page 5: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of
Page 6: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

1. Introduction ............................................................................................................................ 5

2. Theoretical framework: Situation up to 1995 ......................................................................... 9

2.1. Tracking or whole class ability grouping ........................................................................ 9

2.2. Setting ............................................................................................................................ 10

2.3. Within-class ability grouping for specific subjects ........................................................ 10

2.4. Grouping and adaptive teaching .................................................................................... 12

2.5. Evidence from previous meta-analyses ......................................................................... 13

3. Method .................................................................................................................................. 14

3.1. Literature search procedures .......................................................................................... 14

3.2. Inclusion criteria ............................................................................................................ 15

3.3. Additional relevant sources ........................................................................................... 16

3.4. Computation of effect sizes ........................................................................................... 16

3.5. Meta-analysis ................................................................................................................. 16

4. Results .................................................................................................................................. 19

4.1. General results of the literature search .......................................................................... 19

4.2. Effects of differentiation in ECE and Kindergarten (2;6-6 years) ................................. 19

4.2.1. Overview of differentiation in ECE and Kindergarten ........................................... 19

4.2.2. Selected studies ....................................................................................................... 20

4.2.3. Literature synthesis ................................................................................................. 21

General overview ........................................................................................................... 21

Results of the included studies ...................................................................................... 22

4.2.4. An example of an effective comprehensive program: EMERGE ........................... 25

4.3. Effects of differentiation in Primary Education (6-12 years) ........................................ 26

4.3.1. Overview of differentiation in Primary Education .................................................. 26

4.3.2. Selected studies ....................................................................................................... 27

4.3.3. Literature synthesis ................................................................................................. 27

Results of an intervention study on within-class ability grouping ................................ 27

Results of studies on naturally occurring ability grouping practices............................. 27

Results of studies on differentiation based on computerized systems .......................... 31

Results of studies on differentiation as part of a broader school reform program ........ 34

4.3.4. An example of an effective comprehensive program: Success for All ................... 37

4.4. Effects of differentiation in Early Secondary Education (12-14 years) ......................... 38

4.4.1. Overview of differentiation in Early Secondary Education .................................... 38

4.4.2. Selected studies ....................................................................................................... 39

Page 7: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

4.4.3. Literature synthesis .................................................................................................. 39

General overview ........................................................................................................... 39

Results of the included studies ....................................................................................... 39

4.4.4 An example of an effective comprehensive program: IMPROVE ........................... 41

4.5. Reflection on the included studies ................................................................................. 42

5. Conclusion and discussion .................................................................................................... 47

5.1. Early Childhood Education and Kindergarten ............................................................... 47

5.2. Primary Education .......................................................................................................... 49

5.3. Early Secondary Education ............................................................................................ 50

5.4. Recommendations for research and practice .................................................................. 52

References ................................................................................................................................. 55

Appendix 1: Included studies ECE and Kindergarten .............................................................. 61

Appendix 2: Included studies Primary Education .................................................................... 65

Appendix 2a: An intervention study on ability grouping ...................................................... 65

Appendix 2b: Ability grouping studies ................................................................................. 66

Appendix 2c: Studies on computerized systems ................................................................... 72

Appendix 2d: Studies on differentiation as part of a broader program ................................. 74

Appendix 3: Included studies Early Secondary Education ....................................................... 76

Page 8: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

5

1. Introduction

The quality of schools is for an important part determined by the way teachers deal with

cognitive differences between students and adapt their instruction to individual needs. In order

to achieve this, teachers need advanced professional skills to deal with these differences, apart

from basic skills of classroom management and general didactics. They need to have insight in

(differentiated) performance goals, be able to interpret students’ current levels based on

classwork and test scores, decide what students of different levels need to learn, and they need

to know how to teach these students with varying cognitive abilities. Furthermore, teachers

need to be aware of school wide decisions about the aim of providing adaptive instruction and

the effect of different classroom practices aimed at low, average or high performing students.

The combination of these attitudes, knowledge and practices is called differentiation.

There are different teaching strategies that can be used to differentiate in classes and in

schools. Schools can create heterogeneous classes or - based on general ability of the students

– homogenous classes. Homogeneous classes are generally applied in secondary education

(e.g. Ireson, Hallam, & Plewis, 2001), while heterogeneous classes are the standard in early

childhood education and primary education. Within heterogeneous classes, teachers can make

use of homogeneous grouping (also referred to as ability grouping) or heterogeneous grouping

(e.g. Lou et al., 1996; Slavin, 1987a). Furthermore, in heterogeneous classrooms, teachers

may provide adapted instruction and offer adapted learning content, in which the lower ability

students may receive more time to master the core learning content (e.g. Anderson &

Algozzine, 2007; de Koning, 1973; George, 2005; Reezigt, 1993).

Which teaching strategies teachers choose to use seems to relate to the implicit or

explicit learning goals they have for their classroom as a whole. From a ‘theoretical’ point of

view teachers can strive for convergence or divergence (Blok, 2004; Bosker, 2005). Teachers

aiming at convergence are mainly focusing on reaching a minimum performance level with all

of their students, which implies they might have to dedicate additional time and effort to the

low achieving children in order for them to reach that minimum performance level, even when

this goes at the expense of the high ability children, who by consequence receive less

attention. Teachers aiming at divergence mainly focus on helping all children to reach their

highest potential, equally dividing attention between students with lower and higher ability.

Their use of ability-appropriate performance goals for (groups of) students of different ability

levels, may lead to a widening of the gap between lower and higher ability students. In

practice though, most teachers will combine convergent and divergent goals and will try to

reach a minimum performance level with the low ability students, while also offering high

ability children the opportunity to extend their knowledge without proceeding (too much)

ahead of their peers in the classroom. The achievement distributions resulting from convergent

and divergent differentiation are depicted in Figure 1, including the regression lines indicating

the relation between post- and pre-test. In the figure on the left hand side, the lines A and B

Page 9: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

6

are initially further apart but approach each other in time, indicating the relative better

progress of the initially lower achieving students. In the figure on the right hand side the

difference between lines A and C widens over time, indicating the relative better progress of

the initially higher achieving students.

Figure 1: Convergent (left) and divergent (right) differentiation compared with respect to the

effects on the distribution for initially low and high achieving students

Broadly speaking, there are three problems related to using differentiation in education:

1. teachers are not always fully aware which differentiation goal they (should) strive for

(de Koning, 1973),

2. the potential convergent or divergent effects of varying differentiation strategies are

not fully clear, as research shows mixed results, and

3. therefore it is difficult for teachers to make explicit decisions on when to use which

differentiation strategy, for what goal.

Ability grouping, as a form of differentiation, has been studied extensively. Five key meta-

analyses of studies on ability grouping until 1995 are conducted by Kulik and colleagues

(1982; 1984), Lou and colleagues (1996) and Slavin (1987a; 1987b; Slavin, 1990). Kulik and

colleagues focused on homogeneous ability grouping in primary (1982) and secondary

education (1984), Lou and colleagues (1996) focused on homogeneous and heterogeneous

grouping in primary and (post)secondary education, and Slavin focused on homogeneous

ability grouping in primary (1987a) and secondary education (1990) and on mastery learning

in primary and secondary education (1987b). The findings of these key studies will be

described in the theoretical framework in chapter 2.

A difficulty in summarizing the effects of studies on ability grouping is that ability

grouping is operationalized in different ways and these differences are likely to influence the

outcome of the study. Slavin (1987a) pointed to the different ways grouping can be organized,

for example temporarily within classes, between classes or between grades (for example

Joplin Plan), special classes for high or low achievers or within-class homogeneous ability

grouping for specific subjects. This last form of grouping is most common in elementary

classrooms. Teachers may assign students to reading or math groups of different achievement

Page 10: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Introduction

7

levels or may start with whole-group instruction and offer remediation or enrichment

afterwards, while the other students work independently. Many modern learning materials

provide content based on ability, with basic content for the whole group, followed by

rehearsal or enrichment material, depending on the level of mastery of individual students.

This helps teachers in offering differentiated learning content to the students in the classroom.

Partly due to the mixed research results, the use and effects of ability grouping are

much debated. Arguments in favor of working with small homogeneous groups are that

instruction, learning pace and learning materials can be better adjusted to the needs of the

students, which will enhance their learning. Arguments against working with small group

homogeneous groups are that students have less interaction with the teacher, who has to divide

his/her attention between multiple groups. Most concerns are related to the learning

opportunities of low ability students in small homogeneous groups: within these groups, they

cannot profit from the input of higher ability peers or from the role models that high ability

students can be. Furthermore, teacher expectations of low ability students may be lower,

leading students in low ability groups to have less opportunity-to-learn. Finally, students in

lower ability groups may experience difficulty in moving upwards to higher ability groups,

especially when the gap between lower and higher ability students increases. The variety of

research results suggest that children with different ability levels may profit from being part of

either homogeneous or heterogeneous groups, but, in general, early selection in which

children are placed in low ability homogeneous classes for longer times at a young age will

put them at a disadvantage. This is especially relevant for children from impoverished

backgrounds and/or minority groups, who might be labeled as being of ‘low ability’ before

they had been able to show their potential. When these children are placed in a low ability

class too soon – based on general estimates or even prejudices, rather than on actual

performance level - they might encounter low expectations, less demanding teaching and

unequal opportunities. Or, according to Slavin: “ability grouping [for a prolonged period, at a

young age, SD] goes against our democratic ideals by creating academic elites (…) the use of

ability grouping may serve to increase divisions along class, race, and ethnic group lines.”

(Slavin, 1987a, p.297).

The aim of the current review is to analyze existing research on differentiation from 1995

onwards and add to the insights in how differentiation practices can positively affect the

language and math performance of low, average and high ability students. Because of the

specific characteristics of different educational age groups, the review will separately focus on

early childhood education and kindergarten (2;6 to 6 year olds), primary education (6 to 12

year olds) and early secondary education (12 to 14 year olds)1. The review does not focus on

grouping only, although many studies may focus on grouping practices without specifying

1 When interventions were conducted in overlapping age groups, the studies were presented in both sections. In

case of follow-up measures, the study is described in the section where the intervention is conducted only.

Originally, we intended to include studies from 2;6 to 16 years old, but finally we decided to limit the upper age

to 14 years.

Page 11: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

8

whether or not ability grouping creates a context for differentiating in for example learning

time, learning content, learning materials, adaptive testing or adaptive instruction. One-to-one

tutoring is excluded, since this educational practice is focused on some individuals instead of

the performance of the entire class. Studies focusing exclusively on tutoring are excluded as

well, although peer tutoring, as such or as an element cooperative learning, can be part of

working in differentiated groups. Furthermore, all the different ways in which teachers may

take into account performance differences of students are considered in this review.

Page 12: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

9

2. Theoretical framework: Situation up to 1995

2.1. Tracking or whole class ability grouping

Kulik and Kulik (1984) conducted a meta-analysis on ability grouping in primary education.

They focused on whole class ability grouping, in which students are assigned to classrooms

based on their ability. Overall, students in homogeneously grouped classrooms had better

achievement than students in heterogeneous classrooms, although the effect size (ES)2 is small

(ES=+0.19). However, these effects can be explained by studies focusing only on special

classes for gifted students. Studies focusing on the entire population of low, average and high

achievers show much smaller effects of homogeneous grouping (ES=+0.07). Also Slavin

(1987a) described the effects of whole class ability grouping in primary education. He only

included programs targeting students from low, average and high ability (thus rejecting whole

class grouping for gifted students) and found no overall effect of this type of grouping (effect

sizes range from ES=-0.15 to +0.15, with a median of 0.00).

The authors referred to above conducted studies on whole class ability grouping in

secondary education as well. Results from the study of Kulik and Kulik (1982) were that

performance of students in homogeneous classrooms was higher than performance of students

in heterogeneous classrooms. The general effect size was small (ES=+0.10), although the

range of effect sizes found in different studies is large, from ES=-1.00 to ES=+1.25. Just like

in their study of 1984, effects disappear when only studies are included focusing on the entire

population of high, average and low performing students (ES=+0.02). Similar to his study on

primary education, Slavin (1990) only included studies that focused on the entire population

of low, average and high performing students in his meta-analysis on whole class ability

grouping. Overall, he found no effect of grouping, just like in his study on primary education

(ES=-0.02).

Regarding differential effects for low, average and high ability students, Slavin

(1987a) found inconsistent results for students of different ability levels: some studies

included in the review found negative effects for low ability students and positive effects for

high ability students, but others found the opposite pattern or no differential effects at all.

Effect sizes of individual studies ranged for low achievers from -0.46 to +0.64, for average

achievers from -0.11 to +0.22 and for high achievers from -0.24 to +0.54. Kulik and Kulik

(1982; 1984) did not report differential effects for whole class ability grouping or tracking in

primary or secondary education. They only looked at the effects of grouping programs

targeted specifically at gifted or impaired students. Whole class ability grouping for gifted

students had positive effects on these gifted students in primary education (ES=+0.49) and in

2 In this chapter we refer to effect sizes with ES, indicating that these were reported effect sizes. In the chapter

where we present the results of our review we will use d, since we recalculated all the research results ourselves,

and expressed and summarized them as the effect size d, being the standardized mean difference between a

treated and an untreated group.

Page 13: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

10

secondary education (ES=+0.33), but effects of this ‘extraction’ of high performing students

on the performance of average and low ability students that remain in the regular classrooms

were not reported. Slavin (1990) looked at differential effects of ability grouping in secondary

education. He found virtually no differential effects for high (ES=+0.01), average (ES=-0.08)

and low achievers (ES=-0.02).

2.2. Setting

Setting is between-class ability grouping for specific subjects. It can be organized with

parallel classrooms of the same grade level or across grade levels. The regrouping is (in

theory) done on the basis of actual performance in the specific subjects, instead of more

general intelligence or ability measures.

Slavin (1987a) describes the effect of regrouping for reading and/or mathematics

between classrooms, but within grades, which is of course only feasible in larger schools.

According to Slavin, the studies that qualified for his best evidence synthesis did not provide

conclusive evidence on the effectiveness of grouping for specific subjects compared to

ordinary heterogeneous classrooms. He considered the quality and quantity of the eligible

studies to be insufficient to draw conclusions on the overall effects. The total effect sizes of

regrouping for specific subjects compared to heterogeneous classrooms of the individual

studies range from -0.28 to +0.43.

Slavin (1987a) also studied the effect of regrouping for specific subjects across

grades. In this arrangement, students are temporarily regrouped based on performance level,

irrespective of grade level, meaning for example that high performing grade 2 students can be

placed together with low performing grade 3 students. The studies in Slavin’s review show

positive effects of between-class grouping across grades (ES=+0.45).

Because Slavin (1987a) considered the studies in his best-evidence synthesis on setting

not strong enough to draw firm conclusions of general effects, no overall differential effects

are reported either. Individual studies indicate more positive effects for high ability than low

ability students though. Effects for high achieving students range from ES=-0.25 to ES=+0.79,

for average achieving students from ES=-0.33 to ES=+0.22 and for low achieving students

from ES=-0.41 to E.S.=+0.32. Slavin reported no overall significant differential effects for

between-class grouping across grades. He stated: “In no case did one subgroup gain at the

expense of another; either all ability levels gained more than their control counterparts or (…)

none did.” (Slavin, 1987a, p.317).

2.3. Within-class ability grouping for specific subjects

Slavin (1987a) described the effect of within-class ability grouping in primary education, a

common and relatively easy way of organizing grouping in primary education. According to

Slavin, studies regarding this type of grouping are most likely to use random assignment, thus

potentially leading to more valid research results in terms of causal attribution. Almost all

Page 14: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Theoretical framework: Situation up to 1995

11

eligible studies in Slavin’s review, concern within-class ability grouping for mathematics.

Generally, the studies show positive effects for homogeneous within-class ability grouping

compared to no grouping (randomized studies: ES=+0.32; nonrandomized studies:

ES=+0.36). In his study on grouping in secondary education, Slavin (1990) described the few

available studies on within-class grouping in secondary education and found no effects (ES=-

0.02), contrary to the findings in primary education.

Homogeneous ability grouping is not the only way of handling differences in the

classroom. One may also use heterogeneous grouping and let students of different abilities

engage in cooperative learning. Lou and colleagues (1996) conducted a meta-analysis of

studies on within-class grouping in elementary, secondary and post-secondary education in the

period 1965 to 1995 and analyzed the effects of grouping versus whole class activities as well

as the effects of homogeneous versus heterogeneous within-class ability grouping. They found

a small overall effect of small group instruction, either homogeneous or heterogeneous, over

whole class instruction (ES=+0.17). Like in the other reviews, there were substantial

differences within individual studies, some favoring small group instruction, some favoring

whole class instruction. This was however not caused by the combined analysis of

homogeneous and heterogeneous ability grouping, since both had similar positive effects

compared to whole class instruction (respectively ES=+0.16 and ES=+0.19). When

homogeneous and heterogeneous ability grouping were directly compared, an overall

advantage of homogeneous ability grouping was found (ES=+0.12).

Mastery learning can be seen as a special form of within-class ability grouping.

Classrooms using mastery learning use regular progress assessment to check whether students

reach certain ability levels. The group that does not perform well enough, receives additional

instruction inside or outside the classroom. The group that does, may receive advanced

materials for enrichment. Every thematic unit starts with whole class instruction; ability

groups are created based on students’ actual performance. Slavin’s (1987b) meta-analysis of

studies on mastery learning, in which the control group was provided equal learning time and

in which effects were measured using standardized tests, showed a small median effect size

(ES=+0.04). Studies which used tests developed by the researchers showed a larger median

effect (ES=+0.26). Four other studies compared classrooms with mastery learning with

additional instruction time with control classes that did not receive additional time. These

studies had a median effect size of +0.31, although Slavin argues that a median effect size is

difficult to interpret because the four studies differ too much from each other. Taken together,

Slavin concluded that mastery learning is not more effective than traditional instruction, when

equal amounts of learning time are provided. But it does seem to help teachers to focus on

instructional objectives, as is indicated by the results of studies using researcher developed

tests, that resemble the content taught more closely than standardized tests.

Slavin (1987a) cautiously described that within-class homogeneous ability grouping is

especially beneficial to low achievers (ES=+0.65), followed by high achievers (ES=+0.41),

followed by average achievers (ES=+0.27). Lou and colleagues (1996) found a different

Page 15: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

12

pattern for homogeneous ability grouping within the classroom. They found that only medium

ability students benefit from learning in small homogeneous groups (ES=+0.51).

Homogeneous within-class grouping had negative effects on low ability students, compared to

heterogeneous within-class grouping (ES=-0.60). For high ability students it made no

difference whether they were placed in small homogeneous or heterogeneous groups. Lou and

colleagues found that grouping in general was beneficial to students of all ability levels, when

compared to whole class instruction. They showed that low ability students profited most of

small grouping (ES=+0.37), followed by high ability students (ES=+0.28), followed by

medium ability students (ES=+0.19).

2.4. Grouping and adaptive teaching

The mixed results of the studies in the meta-analyses indicate that more factors play a role in

the effectiveness of ability grouping. Lou and colleagues (1996) and Slavin (1987a)

emphasized the important role of adapting instruction to the needs of the group. Lou and

colleagues state that “Overall, it appears that the positive effects of within-class [both

homogeneous and heterogeneous, MD] grouping are maximized when the physical placement

of students into groups for learning is accompanied by modifications to teaching methods and

instructional materials. Merely placing students together is not sufficient for promoting

substantive gains in achievement.” (Lou et al., 1996, p. 448). Also Slavin notices that, for

grouping arrangements to have an effect, learning materials and instruction should be adapted:

“regrouping for reading and/or mathematics can be effective if instructional pace and

materials are adapted to students' needs, whereas simply regrouping without extensively

adapting materials or regrouping in all academic subjects is ineffective.” (Slavin, 1987a,

p.311). Unfortunately, as Slavin notes, many studies do not provide specified information on

the instructional practices used in interaction with small ability groups. Lou and colleagues

(1996) analyzed the results of studies that did provide (some) information on teacher

practices. They found larger effects for within-class grouping when teachers adapted their

instruction when teaching to small groups (ES=+0.25) compared to teachers who provided

‘whole class instruction’ to small groups (ES=+0.02).

From his best evidence synthesis, Slavin (1987a) extracted some criteria that are likely

to influence the effect of ability grouping focused on convergent differentiation. The first

criterion is that the grouping must lead to homogeneous groups in the skill being taught.

Groups based on more general performance may actually not be very homogeneous regarding

the skill being taught, leading to poorly formed ability groups. The second criterion is that

groups must be flexible. Students assigned to tracked classrooms are likely to remain in the

classroom for a long period, while students grouped within or between classrooms only for

specific subjects may be reassigned to groups of different levels more easily. The third

criterion is that teachers adapt their teaching to the needs of the different ability groups. There

appear to be quality differences in the appropriateness of the instruction, learning materials

and learning content different ability groups receive. Frequent formative assessment seems to

Page 16: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Theoretical framework: Situation up to 1995

13

be necessary to be able to adapt to the students’ needs. Another important aspect is the

instruction time that students receive. The more ability groups a teacher creates, the less time

there is available for each group and the more time students have to spend working

independently. The use of three ability groups is most common, but whether this is more

effective than for example two or four ability groups remains unclear.

2.5. Evidence from previous meta-analyses

Considering the results from meta-analyses on differentiation up to 1995, several conclusions

can be drawn. First of all, whole class ability grouping or tracking seem to have no effects

when the entire population of low, average and high performing students is taken into account.

Differential effects of tracking are inconclusive. Tracking, or between-class ability grouping

may have positive effects, especially when grouping is done across grades. Again, differential

effects are inconclusive, although across grade grouping seems to be beneficial for all ability

groups. Within-class ability grouping also seems to have positive effects, although effect sizes

of this type of grouping are smaller than the effect sizes of between-class grouping. Within-

class grouping seems to be beneficial due to the combination of small group instruction and

homogeneous grouping. Differential effects however are inconclusive: in the review of Slavin

(1987a), within-class ability grouping is most beneficial to low achievers. In contrast, Lou and

colleagues (1996) reported that low achievers indeed benefit from grouping, but not from

homogeneous grouping. Within-class heterogeneous grouping may be more beneficial for low

ability students, according to Lou and colleagues. Slavin as well as Lou and colleagues

emphasize the importance of adapted instruction and learning materials in combination with

grouping: grouping alone is not enough, it is merely a context for the teacher to apply

adequate teaching practices, adapted to the needs of different students. This is confirmed by

Slavin (1987b) who suggests that the lack of effects of mastery learning may have to do with

insufficient quality and quantity of corrective instruction.

Based on the previous research no general effects are expected for whole class ability

grouping or tracking, unless within-class grouping is used within the tracked classrooms or

other adaptive high quality teaching methods are used (Slavin, 1990). When differential

effects are found, it is expected that whole class homogeneous grouping has negative effects

on low ability students, since it is less likely that students are then instructed in smaller groups

and since this configuration excludes the possibility to work in heterogeneous ability groups

for part of the time. Positive differential effects for streaming and within-class homogeneous

grouping are expected, provided that high quality adaptive instruction is offered to the

different ability groups. These effects are expected to be positive for low, average and high

ability students.

Page 17: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

14

3. Method

The effectiveness of different differentiation practices are studied by applying a best evidence

synthesis, which is a meta-analysis extended with additional contextual information on the

selected studies, with an emphasis on studies that are particularly relevant to the topic under

study (Slavin, Lake, Chambers, Cheung, & Davis, 2009). In an attempt to perform the most

comprehensive literature search, both an electronic database search and a cited-references

search is conducted. In order to find as many relevant sources as possible, the electronic

database search starts broadly and the number of results is narrowed down by manually

applying additional selection criteria. Effect sizes are calculated for each eligible study.

Content coding is performed in order to create an overview of the different types of studies

and the different elements of differentiation studied. This information is used to provide

context to the effect size data of the meta-analysis.

3.1. Literature search procedures

An extensive literature search was conducted in the educational databases ERIC, psychINFO

and SSCI. The databases were searched by making use of 10 keywords, which were used

twice: once in combination with the keyword achiev* and once in combination with the

keyword effect*. The ten keywords are: “ability group*”, “adapt* instruct*”, “adapt*

teach*”, “aptitude treatment”, differentiat*, grouping*, “individuali* instruct*”,

“individuali* teach*”, “mastery learning” and streaming. Papers in which these keywords

are mentioned in the abstract were included in the initial selection, provided they were:

articles published in peer-reviewed journals, published between 1995 and 2012, written in

English and aimed at the age-category 2-16 years (i.e. preschool – secondary education).

In addition to the database search, a ‘cited references’ search was conducted. Eleven

key publications on differentiation were selected, namely Blok (2004), Borman et al. (2005),

de Koning (1973), Gamoran and Weinstein (1998), Irseon and Hallam (2001), Kulik and

Kulik (1982), Lou et al. (1996), Reezigt (1993), and Slavin (1987a; 1987b; 1990). Three of

the key publications (Blok, 2004; de Koning, 1973; Reezigt, 1993) are based on the

educational context in the Netherlands. Using the SSCI database, all papers published from

1995 onwards, that made reference to one of these eleven key publications were collected.

These two broad search methods led to a large amount of references, which was

narrowed down by manually applying selection criteria. The first broad selection criterion was

whether the study was on language or math or not. Language in this case encompasses

reading, writing, vocabulary, grammar etc. in the native language of the country under study

(i.e. no foreign language studies). The selection was based on title, abstract and keywords. In

case of doubt, the paper remained included in the selection. Abstracts which indicated that

studies did not focus on students up to 16 years of age, were not linked to education, did not

include effects on language- or math performance, were case studies, or did not make use of

Page 18: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Method

15

empirical research methods, were rejected. Applying these criteria narrowed down the number

of references. Of this narrowed down selection, the full text papers were collected.

3.2. Inclusion criteria

A set of 8 final criteria for inclusion was applied to the selection of full text papers. The first

criterion focused on the content of the study, the second was practical and the third to eighth

focused on the quality of the study. The criteria were based on those used in the best evidence

syntheses conducted by Slavin and colleagues (1987a; 2008; 2009).

1. The study addresses effects of differentiation on language or math performance of all

students or groups of students in a classroom. The intervention takes place ‘inside’ the

classroom (i.e. no out-of-class tutoring), during the regular school day.

2. The study could have taken place in any country, but the report had to be available in

English.

3. The intervention has a minimum duration of 12 weeks, measured from beginning of

treatment to posttest.

4. Each treatment group consists of at least 15 students and of at least two teachers that

are involved in the study.

5. The study compares children taught in classes using a given intervention to those in

control classes using another intervention or standard teaching practice (“business as

usual”). Or the study uses secondary data analysis on existing databases in order to

compare groups of classes.

6. The study uses random assignments or matching or conditioning with appropriate

adjustments for any pretest differences (e.g. ANCOVA). Studies without control

groups are excluded.

7. The study provides pretest data, unless the study uses random assignments of at least

30 units (students, classes or schools) and there are no indications of initial inequality.

Studies with pretest differences of more than 0.50 of a standard deviation are excluded.

8. The dependent measures include quantitative measures of performance, such as

standardized reading measures. Experimenter-made measures were accepted if they

were comprehensive measures that would be fair to the control group, but measures

inherent to the experimental program were excluded.

From the included papers3, relevant data was selected to calculate effect sizes. In addition,

these studies were coded for content. The content coding included: grade under study, type of

differentiation, country (and state) in which the intervention is conducted, sample size,

duration of intervention, dependent variables and instrumentation and external variables and

covariates (if applicable). In addition, a short summary is made of the study, its effects,

drawbacks and strong points, and its relevance for the best evidence synthesis.

3 A full list of all the references found is available upon request.

Page 19: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

16

3.3. Additional relevant sources

Relevant studies on (aspects of) differentiation could also be found in other sources than

papers published in academic, peer-reviewed journals. Therefore, an additional electronic

search was performed in the databases ERIC and psychINFO. The search criteria were similar

to the search of the journal articles, except for publication type, which could be books,

dissertations and theses or reports. The references that were found in this search were checked

against the selection criteria applied to the abstracts as described above. Subsequently, the

most relevant sources were selected and used for contextual information on differentiation in

the different age groups.

3.4. Computation of effect sizes

To be able to compare the effects of the different studies, all results are converted to Cohen’s

d, which is the standardized mean difference between groups. The ways of calculating d when

using different types of data stemming from various research designs are described in

Borenstein et al. (2009). When correlations between pretest and posttest were needed for

calculating d, but were not provided in the study at hand, a pre-post correlation of 0.70 was

assumed. Next to d estimates for its 95% confidence interval are presented. If the reader is

interested in either more conservative or more liberal intervals, these can be simply derived

from the estimates presented.

For every study a general d is calculated. When multiple outcome measures are used,

they are labeled as either measures of math, vocabulary, reading or reading comprehension,

since this is more informative than the names of individual tests, which vary between studies.

If possible, differential effect sizes for high, average and low performing students are

provided. The effect of differentiation is considered to be divergent when the effect size d is

largest for high ability students and convergent when the effect size d is largest for low ability

students.

3.5. Meta-analysis

In some specific instances it is possible to combine results of different studies into one

summary effect size (c.f. Borenstein et al., 2009). These instance are:

1. The studies have the same topic (e.g. within-class ability grouping);

2. The studies are conducted in the same stage of the education system (ECE and

kindergarten; primary; early secondary);

3. The studies focus on the same subject domain (either reading or mathematics).

In a statistical meta-analysis the crucial information (an effect size and a standard error

suffice) is summarized as a weighted average, with weights being inversely proportional to the

magnitude of the standard errors. And the standard error for this summary effect size is

derived from the standard errors of the individual studies. A quite surprising result may be that

the summary effect size may have a standard error so small that the resulting confidence

Page 20: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Method

17

interval for the effect size estimate does not contain zero, whereas none of the individual

studies had produced a significant effect. The reason of course is that in the summary effect

size and its standard error all the samples from the different studies are more or less combined

into one very big sample. The meta-analyses were conducted using the CMA-software

developed by Borenstein et al. (2009). In meta-analyses in which multiple outcomes from the

same study are used, the results are adjusted and the adjustment factor is presented in a note to

the table.

Page 21: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

18

Page 22: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

19

4. Results

4.1. General results of the literature search

The broad database search in ERIC, psychINFO and SSCI, using the 10 keywords related to

differentiation, led to 2,478 unique references4. In addition, a cited reference search was

conducted based on the 11 key publications. This led to an additional 262 new references,

adding up to 2740 potentially interesting references. Of these, about 500 seemed relevant at

first sight, mostly studies regarding primary education5. Careful reading of the abstracts led to

a selection of approximately 200 papers eligible for further analysis based on their full text

versions. The final 8 inclusion criteria (see paragraph 3.2) were applied to the full text papers.

A total number of 266 journal articles met de inclusion criteria and were used in the meta-

analysis.

In addition, potentially interesting books, reports and theses were searched using the

same key words used in the general database search. This resulted in 828 publications, of

which 97 seemed appropriate, based on the general inclusion criteria. Of this set of books,

reports and theses, the 10 most relevant sources were selected manually. They were not

included in the meta-analysis, but used for gathering theoretical background information.

4.2. Effects of differentiation in Early Childhood Education and Kindergarten

(2;6-6 years)

4.2.1. Overview of differentiation in ECE and Kindergarten

Early childhood education (ECE) is designed to stimulate children in their development,

reduce and prevent learning- and language delays and to prepare children for formal

education. Preschool and kindergarten teachers have to deal with children from very different

(language) backgrounds and different starting levels and aim to help them all to acquire the

minimum level needed to enter first grade. The goal of differentiation in early childhood

education is thus mainly convergent.

Most studies on differentiation activities in early childhood education focus on

(emergent) literacy and early reading. This is not surprising, as language and literacy

development is one of the core tasks of ECE, especially when it is aimed at second language

learners and/or children from impoverished backgrounds with limited language input at home.

The type of differentiation that is typically used is within-class homogeneous ability grouping:

4 Three of the searches in SSCI resulted in over 1000 hits (differentiat* & achiev*; differentiat* & effect*;

grouping* & effect*). These are narrowed down by selecting the “web of science categories”: education,

educational research and psychology educational. 5 With the distribution ECE and kindergarten : primary education : secondary education being 1 : 2 : 4

6 The article of Tach and Farkas (2006) is used twice.

Page 23: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

20

the classroom is divided into small groups of children of similar proficiency levels, who

receive specific, proficiency level appropriate instruction in literacy or early reading skills.

Ability grouping in preschool and Kindergarten appears to be not as straightforward as

depicted above. Ongoing assessment and frequent re-grouping is considered to be important

(Slavin, 1987a), but details on how this is applied are often not reported in research.

Furthermore, ability groups are not always as homogeneous as they are supposed to be, for

example because proficiency is not measured well enough or because other student features

are emphasized as well, such as student interest, learning style and gender as a base for

grouping. Also secondary goals of grouping play are role, like stimulating self-regulated

learning, enhancing student ownership in learning and maintaining a positive classroom

atmosphere. These secondary goals are advocated by Tomlinson (2000), a scholar who is

specialized in differentiation and writes primarily for an audience of practitioners. Also

Howard Gardner’s (1984) work on multiple intelligences and variation in learning style is

mentioned in this respect. Other factors than ability alone thus seem to play a role in ability

grouping.

The problem with this ‘broad view’ on differentiation is that the more student features

are taken into account, the more difficult it becomes to create homogeneous groups. In theory,

teachers could first group students based on performance and then make smaller subgroups

based on for example learning style, as described by Neel (2008) in her study on reading in

first grade. However, this would only be feasible when working with a larger group of

students/classrooms in a school. Another problem of grouping based on multiple student

features is that it further decreases the transparency of the educational practice of

differentiation. In many studies it is not clear on what basis ability groups are formed, how

teachers designed their instruction plans focusing on different groups of students and how

(well) they implemented them.

A complicating factor in the interpretation of the effects and meaning of grouping in

early childhood education is discussed by McCoach (2003), who suggests that grouping for

reading in 1st grade reflects a traditional teaching approach, while traditional Kindergarten

teachers would probably not use achievement grouping. On the contrary, the Kindergarten

teachers who use achievement grouping may be innovative in their teaching and more focused

on academic results, according to McCoach. The effects of grouping may thus be confounded

by teacher characteristics that are associated with a tendency to use grouping and this may

especially be the case in ECE and kindergarten classrooms. This illustrates again that results

on grouping are difficult to interpret without detailed information on how teachers create and

treat these groups.

4.2.2. Selected studies

In the initial database search, approximately 50 papers focusing on education in preschool or

kindergarten were found. Approximately 15 papers were selected for further inspection based

on their full text versions. Of these, seven papers met the inclusion criteria, described in the

Page 24: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

21

general method section (paragraph 3.2). These selected papers are alphabetically listed and

summarized in appendix 1.

Of the seven selected studies on differentiated instruction in early childhood education,

six are based on ECLS-K data. This data originates from the Early Childhood Longitudinal

Study (ECLS), in which development, school readiness and school experiences are

investigated in three large groups of children. The first group is followed from birth to

kindergarten (ECLS-B), the second group is followed from kindergarten (entry in 1998-1999)

to 8th

grade (ECLS-K) and the third group will be followed from kindergarten (entry in 2010-

2011) to 5th

grade (ECLS-K: 2011). The study is conducted in the United States by the

Institute of Education Sciences and the National Center for Education Statistics. The studies in

the current review are based on the first cohort of kindergartners (ECLS-K). The ECLS

database is for the most part publically available to researchers. A wide range of child-

assessments are used in the ECLS-K: reading, mathematics, general knowledge, social-

emotional and physical development. However, most of the studies included in the current

review only make use of the reading/literacy measures, and one focuses on math growth.

4.2.3. Literature synthesis

General overview

Ability grouping is measured in different ways in the selected studies, sometimes very broad

and sometimes in more detail, ranging from whether grouping is used at all (Adelson &

Carpenter, 2011) to how often it is used per week (D. B. McCoach, O'Connell, & Levitt,

2006), to how many time a day is spent on grouping (Chang, 2008; Hong & Hong, 2009;

Hong, Corter, Hong, & Pelletier, 2012). In general, ability grouping in early childhood

education seems to have a positive effect. Most studies report positive effect sizes for

grouping, for students of all levels (d ranges from +0.068 to +1.276). Due to too big

differences between the studies in terms of operationalization of differentiation, it was not

possible to perform meta-analyses on the studied included.

Only two studies look into differential effects for low, average and high performing

students (namely Gettinger & Stoiber, 2012; Hong et al., 2012). The effects of the studies

seem contradictive and have to do with the amount of instruction time students receive when

grouped. Hong and colleagues (2012) conclude that if relatively little time is spent on reading,

intensive grouping, compared to whole class instruction, is not beneficial to students of all

ability levels. Gettinger and Stoiber (2012) describe an intervention of ability grouping with

an emphasis on adaptive education and high quality instruction and found this to be beneficial

for all students, including low performing students. Ability grouping under these conditions is

most beneficial to average ability students, followed by low ability students, followed by high

ability students (Gettinger & Stoiber, 2012). The effect of differentiation in this study is thus

neither divergent nor convergent, as the gap between high ability students and their classmates

does not enlarge, but the low ability students do not approach their average performing peers

either.

Page 25: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

22

Results of the included studies

The only study in the selection using a randomized controlled trial is the one of Gettinger and

Stoiber (2012). They studied the effect of an early literacy intervention based on close

monitoring and assessment of students’ progress and adjusting instruction based on the

monitoring results (for key features and estimated effects, see appendix 1). The progress

monitoring is used for providing additional small-group instruction to low performing

students, adjusting the general whole class instruction and providing additional challenge to

the group of high performing students. The way teachers were supposed to monitor

performance and adjust their teaching- and lesson plans for different groups of students is

described in detail, which is an exception in empirical studies on differentiation in early

childhood education and kindergarten. More details on the content of the program are

described below in paragraph 4.2.4. A total of 124 3- and 4-year olds in 15 classrooms were

included in the study. Eight classrooms (62 preschoolers) were randomly assigned to the

intervention condition, which lasted for 4 months. A drawback of the design is that students in

the experimental condition received more practice with the content and format of the effect

measures due to the monthly progress monitoring and may therefore have been better prepared

for the posttests. Overall results were that students from the intervention group scored better

on all five literacy measures than matched control students (effect sizes ranging from

d=+0.388 to d=+0.911). Positive effects were found for all three achievement levels. On two

measures, significant effects are found for all three ability groups: on the reading tasks

measuring upper case letter naming and on the reading/reading comprehension task which

measured both knowledge of book and print concepts and story comprehension. Average

ability students gained most on both measures (respectively d=+1.276 and d=+0.999),

followed by low ability students (d=+1.015 and d=+0.876) followed by high ability students

(d=+0.675 and d=+0.696).

The other studies described in this section (for key features and estimated effects, see

appendix 1) are all based on ECLS-K data, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study starting in

Kindergarten. A drawback is that this database lacks detailed information on the grouping

practices of the teachers. Teacher’s self-reported use of ability grouping and time spend on

language/reading or mathematics is measured with Likert scales. No information is available

on the flexibility of groups, the basis on which groups are formed and the way learning

content is (differentially) conveyed. This makes interpretation of the results more complex.

Nevertheless, the size and the representativeness of the ECLS-K dataset make the studies

important for collecting empirical evidence on the effects of grouping for young children.

Hong and colleagues performed two related studies on the relationship between

homogeneous grouping, instruction time and reading growth (Hong & Hong, 2009; Hong et

al., 2012). They created six categories of educational practice based on instruction time (high

or low) and homogeneous grouping (high intensive, low intensive or none). Teachers who

reported to spend more than 1 hour a day on literacy instruction were classified as providing

‘high’ amounts of instruction time. Teachers who reported to spend more than 40% of the

Page 26: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

23

literacy time on instruction to homogeneous groups were classified as using ‘high intensive’

grouping. “No grouping” means that only whole class instruction was provided. Hong and

colleagues used these categorizations in both studies, but in 2009 (10,189 students, 1,858

classrooms, 740 schools) they presented among others the main effects and focused on general

reading growth and in 2012 (8,668 students, 1,697 classrooms, 665 schools), they presented

differential effects and focused on effects for low, average and high performing students.

Results from the 2009 study were that when teachers provide 1 hour or more literacy

instruction a day, it is beneficial to use homogeneous grouping compared to whole class

instruction. This counted both for high intensity grouping, when students spent 40% or more

of the time spend on literacy instruction in homogeneous groups (d=+0.198), and for low

intensity grouping, when students spent less time in homogeneous groups (d=+0.164). When

teachers provided less than 1 hour a day of literacy instruction, no significant effects of

grouping over whole class instruction were found. In this context of low instruction time, high

intensity grouping seemed to be less beneficial than whole class instruction, but effects were

not significant. In spite of these non-significant results, the authors concluded that the

combination of low instruction time with high intensity grouping appeared to have an adverse

effect.

Hong and colleagues (2012) therefore studied whether this negative effect of low

instruction time in combination with high intensity grouping holds for groups of students of

different ability levels. First the effect of grouping was studied for different groups given that

instruction time is low. Differential effects only reached significance for the low ability group.

For these students, whole group instruction was more beneficial than intensive grouping,

when instruction time was low (effect sizes for the 5 different literacy measures ranged from

d=+0.181 to d=+0.328). The authors also studied whether the effect of intensive grouping was

influenced by the amount of time spent on instruction. For all ability groups, intensive

grouping was more beneficial when high instruction time was provided than when low

instruction time was provided. For high ability students significant effects of high instruction

time were found for two of the reading measures (effect sizes d=+0.267 and d=+0.284). For

average ability students positive effects were found on all four reading measures (effect sizes

range from d=+0.145 to d=+0.174), but not on the measure of reading comprehension. For

low ability students positive effects were found on three of the reading measures and the

reading comprehension measure (effect sizes range from d=+0.208 to d=+0.268).

The study of Chang (2008) is the only one in the collection of selected papers that

focusses on early mathematical development. The longitudinal study of ECLS-K data focuses

among others on the effects of grouping on the performance of different groups of minority

students, learning English as a second language. Since the current review does not focus on

second language learners, only the data of the Caucasian group and the African-American

group with English as (only) mother tongue is used here7 (respectively 5,863 and 1,151

7 The groups of English only speaking students from the Hispanic and Asian group were small and therefore not

used here.

Page 27: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

24

students). Chang studied the relation between the frequency of 4 types of classroom practices

and mathematics achievement. The four types of classroom practice were: teacher-directed

whole class activity; teacher-directed small-group activity8; teacher-directed individual

activity; and student-selected individual activity. Teachers indicated the frequency in which

they used every type of classroom practice on a 5-point scale, ranging from no use to more

than 3 hours a day. Results were that more teacher-directed whole class instruction was

significantly related to more math improvement for Caucasian and African-American English-

only speakers (d=+0.152 and d=+0.134 respectively). The other effects were smaller,

inconsistent, or not significant: more time spent in teacher-directed small group settings had a

negative or no significant effect on math improvement (d=-0.045 and d=+0.002, 95% CI

contains 0); more teacher-directed individual activity had a small positive or negative effect

(d=+0.008 and d=-0.069); more child-selected individual activity had a small positive effect

(d=+0.012 and d=+0.020). In theory, high time can be spent on multiple practices and it is not

a case of either one classroom practice or the other. For example, a combination of intensive

whole class instruction and intensive child-initiated individual activity may be effective, but

this is not tested here.

McCoach and colleagues (2006) studied, among others, the effects of homogeneous

grouping on reading growth based on ECLS-K data. They based their analyses on the data of

10,191 students of 620 schools. The amount of time spent on ability grouping was measured

on a 5 point scale, as reported by the teacher. This measure is a rough indication of frequency

of grouping: from never to daily. Results were that higher frequencies of ability grouping were

related to more reading growth (d=+0.127).

Adelson and Carpenter (2011) studied ECLS-K data of over 9,000 students, from

almost 1,700 classrooms and 580 schools. They compared, among others, the effect of whole

class education with homogeneous grouping on reading growth from fall to spring in

Kindergarten K2. The use of ability grouping for reading was measured with a dichotomous

question to the teacher (yes/no). Results were that classrooms in which homogeneous

grouping took place, students showed more reading growth (d=+0.068). Unfortunately, there

was no additional information on the grouping practice, for example on frequency of grouping

or time spent in the groups.

Tach and Farkas (2006) used ECLS-K data as well to study the effects of

homogeneous grouping. They analyzed among others whether students in Kindergarten

classrooms using ability grouping had better reading achievement at the end of the school

year9. They included almost 12,000 students from over 2,400 classrooms in their analyses and

found the use of ability groups in Kindergarten had a positive effect on reading achievement

(d=+0.346).

8 Though not explicitly mentioned, this seems to refer to small homogeneous ability groups.

9 Tach and Farkas also studied the effects of grouping at the end of first grade. These results are described in the

section on primary education.

Page 28: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

25

4.2.4. An example of an effective comprehensive program: EMERGE

Because of the importance of implementing high quality, adaptive instruction in order to make

differentiation practices like ability grouping effective, an example will be given of a

comprehensive program aimed at development in early childhood education which has a clear

component of differentiation based on cognitive ability. The EMERGE program, as studied by

Gettinger and Stoiber (2012) and which is included in the literature synthesis in paragraph

4.2.3, will be described.

EMERGE is based on the Response-to-Intervention (RTI) approach, which includes

screening students, providing differentiated instruction, continuous monitoring and adapting

instruction based on the monitoring results. It is in other words a form of differentiation based

on actual performance in which ability grouping is used for (part of the) instruction and in

which instruction is adapted to the needs of the students. Chambers and colleagues (2010)

describe EMERGE in a best evidence synthesis on ECE programs and conclude there is

limited evidence of the effectiveness of the program, due to insufficient numbers in the study.

However Chambers and colleagues based their conclusion on an older study (Gettinger &

Stoiber, 2007) and did not consider their paper from 2012. Due to the strong emphasis on

implementation and the connection between grouping and instruction, the program is

described here nevertheless.

Gettinger and Stoiber (2012) acknowledge that systematic progress monitoring alone

is not sufficient to improve student performance. Teachers should know how to use this

monitoring data to adapt their instruction. Therefore, professional development and coaching

is part of the intervention. A problem with frequent (monthly) progress monitoring is that it is

difficult to find measures sensitive to short-term growth in literacy development in preschool

and Kindergarten. The authors therefore aim at developing assessments that are directly linked

to the instruction received. Accompanying advantage is that this helps teachers to adapt their

instruction to the needs of students, because it is directly clear which elements of the learning

content are not well understood. Trained examiners conducted the monthly assessment battery

for progress monitoring. The assessments were planned after each thematic unit and measured

letter recognition, vocabulary (explicitly taught in the previous thematic unit) and book

recognition and book comprehension (of books read in the previous thematic unit). The

assessments were administered to all the children in the classroom individually in 10 minutes

per child and took place during learning center time. The assessment data was used in

instruction, which was divided into two phases: first core literacy instruction and then small

group differentiated instruction, based on the progress data.

The core literacy instruction consisted of three elements. The first element is shared

book reading, with dialogic reading and a special focus on print. Teachers received detailed

cues in order to enhance the quality of the shared book reading and a literacy coach modeled

one whole-group reading session a week. Twelve books were used per monthly thematic unit.

The second element is explicit vocabulary instruction. Each monthly unit, sixteen words,

extracted from the books read in classroom, were discussed. Vocabulary was instructed by

Page 29: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

26

explaining word meanings, as well as providing contexts in which the word is used and

stimulating students to provide their own examples. The third element is explicit focus on

letters and sounds during book reading and small group instruction. Letters and sounds are not

treated in isolation, but embedded in other engaging activities. The literacy coach provides

demonstration and feedback on all instructional activities within the core instruction.

In addition to the core instruction, daily 30 minutes small group instruction was

provided. Three ability groups were created based on the progress monitoring data. Groups

consisted of 4 to 6 children who needed additional instruction and practice though repeated

shared book reading and accompanying focus on vocabulary and letter and sound knowledge.

High ability students were engaged in additional, more challenging discussions and tasks. A

special 5-step plan provided teachers guidance in translating progress data (which they

received from the researchers) into differentiated lesson plans. All in all, EMERGE combines

ability grouping with frequent progress monitoring and intensive coaching of teachers in how

to translate assessment data into differentiated lesson plans and how to provide high quality

instruction.

4.3. Effects of differentiation in Primary Education (6-12 years)

4.3.1. Overview of differentiation in Primary Education

In primary education, differentiation is a topic of great concern to teachers. They have to deal

with groups of students with a large variation in abilities, which may amount to students

within the same class differing four years in didactical age. The desire to fit their instruction to

the needs of individual students has led to some widely adopted grouping practices in primary

education. One of the most common practices is within-class ability grouping (Kulik & Kulik,

1984; Slavin, 1987a). In this case, teachers form homogeneous groups within the classroom

based on students’ prior performance and provide instruction in these small homogeneous

groups. For instance, in reading instruction, a survey in the United States shows that about two

third of the teachers in the first grade of primary education use some type of within-class

ability grouping (Chorzempa & Graham, 2006). The within-class ability grouping procedures

are typically organized by teachers. Additionally, some articles have addressed using ICT as a

tool to facilitate teachers in their within-class ability grouping procedures. ICT programs can

be used as a tool to allocate students to groups based on their prior performance and can also

be used to facilitate the choice of suitable learning materials for different students.

Another practice used in primary education is setting students in separate

homogeneous classes based on their abilities for specific subjects such as reading or

mathematics. Setting or regrouping is used frequently in some countries such as the United

Kingdom and Australia. This is mostly true in the upper primary school grades. For instance,

almost 40 percent of grade 5 and 6 teachers in the United Kingdom use setting for

mathematics instruction (Hallam, Ireson, Lister, Chaudhury, & Davies, 2003). The expected

Page 30: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

27

benefit of setting is that teachers can fit whole-group instruction to the needs of the group

more easily when the group is quite homogeneous.

4.3.2. Selected studies

Approximately 200 references to studies focusing on differentiation in primary education were

found using the database search. Of these, approximately 90 were selected for further

inspection based on their full text versions. After applying the 8 final inclusion criteria (see

paragraph 3.2), 16 papers remained and were included in the current review.

These 16 articles were divided into four categories: one article described an

intervention study on within-class ability grouping; five articles describe natural occurring

ability grouping practices; in five articles the effects of computerized testing systems with

clues about differentiated instruction for the teacher were described, and in five studies

differentiation was part of a broader program. Some of the articles are based on ECLS-K data

(see 4.2.2.). In the closing paragraph of this section an exemplary comprehensive program that

includes differentiation practices next to all sorts of other educational interventions will be

described.

4.3.3. Literature synthesis

Results of an intervention study on within-class ability grouping

Of the included studies in primary education, one study was on an intervention using different

types of within-class ability grouping (see appendix 2a). This study of Leonard (2001)

comprises two consecutive years. In each year performance on the Maryland Functional

Mathematics Test is monitored in a grade six cohort from three classrooms. In the first year of

the study, all grade six students in cohort 1 were seated in small heterogeneous groups during

mathematics lessons. In the consecutive year, all grade six students in cohort 2 were seated in

small homogeneous ability groups. The grouping intervention was executed by clustering

students’ tables in small groups of three to four based on students’ pretest performance and

grades. During the year, students of both cohorts collaborated on thematic mathematical

activities. The article does not clarify how instruction by the teacher was provided. The effects

of homogeneous table grouping compared to heterogeneous table grouping were negative and

non-significant (d=-0.250). The intervention does not support the hypothesis that

homogeneous grouping has a different effect on students’ performance than heterogeneous

grouping. Based on qualitative analyses of students group interactions, the author concluded

that the way the group collaborated may have been more determinative for achievement than

the clustering of students in table groups based on ability level.

Results of studies on naturally occurring ability grouping practices

The second category of studies does not describe intervention programs, but rather analyzes

the effects of naturally occurring differentiation practices in education. In these studies,

teacher questionnaires or administrative information was used to assess ongoing

differentiation practices in classes or schools. In turn, this information was related to student

Page 31: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

28

performance measures for reading and literacy, writing, or math using quantitative analytical

procedures. In the studies on the effects of naturally occurring differentiation practices, two

types of differentiation were found. The first is within-class ability grouping. The effects of

this type of differentiation were assessed in three studies (Condron, 2008; Nomi, 2010; Tach

& Farkas, 2006). Another type of differentiation found in the literature on primary education

was between-class homogeneous ability grouping (or setting). This type of differentiation was

addressed in two studies (Macqueen, 2012; Whitburn, 2001). The key features and findings of

these studies are summarized in appendix 2b.

Within-class ability grouping

The articles on the effectiveness of naturally occurring within-class ability grouping are all

based on longitudinal data from the ECLS-K cohort, which already was described in

paragraph 4.2.2. In the ECLS-K dataset teachers provided information about their grouping

procedures. Student performance data is gathered in kindergarten and at the end of first grade.

One study also adds third grade performance data to assess the effect of grouping from first to

third grade (Condron, 2008). The selected articles in primary education using the ECLS-K

data assess the effect of within-class ability grouping on students’ reading performance.

In the article of Condron (2008), effects are presented of placing students in reading

groups based on their reading performance from kindergarten to first grade and from first to

third grade. Using the propensity score matching technique, the author compared the scores of

students in a low, middle or high level reading group to scores of non-grouped students with a

similar likelihood of being placed in one of these groups. For both first and third grade,

placement in a high ability group led to higher gains in reading performance (first grade:

d=+0.207; third grade: d=+0.177). Placement in an average level reading group did not have a

significant effect on reading performance (first grade: d=-0.043; third grade: d=+0.046), and

placement in a low-level group had a significant negative effect on reading performance in

both first and third grade (first grade: d=-0.288; third grade: d=-0.245). This shows that

within-class ability grouping may lead to divergent differentiation effects.

The articles of Nomi (2010) and Tach and Farkas (2006) both analyze the effect of

grouping practices in first grade on first grade spring reading performance. These studies

show that within-class ability grouping is frequently used in primary education; in the ECLS-

K dataset ability grouping occurs in about 70 percent of the first grade classrooms. Tach and

Farkas (2006) used multilevel modeling to estimate effects of grouping on reading

performance students in first grade. In this study, the occurrence of ability groups in first

grade had a significant negative effect on students’ reading performance (d=-0.191). However,

additional results show that being in a high ability group positively affected performance. This

effect is more profound for African-American or Hispanic students, suggesting that student

race interacts with grouping effects. Nomi (2010) used propensity score matching to examine

the effects of ability grouping on reading achievement. The reading scores of 8785 students in

total were used to analyze the effects of school grouping policies. The author found that on

Page 32: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

29

average, schools using ability grouping served a relatively heterogeneous student population.

This confirms the notion that ability grouping is often used as a tool for schools to deal with

student diversity. However, in the study of Nomi, no evidence was found of benefits of ability

grouping over whole class instruction (d=-0.010).

Summarizing the effects found in the ECLS-K studies (Nomi, 2010; Tach & Farkas,

2006), the meta-analyses presented in Table 1 show that overall within-class ability grouping

had a small negative effect on students’ reading performance (d=-0.070). Meta-analyses of the

effect of within-class ability grouping for students of differential ability (Condron, 2008;

Nomi, 2010) show that in the ECLS-K dataset within-class ability grouping had a small

negative effect on the reading performance of low ability students (d=-0.232), no effect for

students of average ability (d=0.000) and a small positive effect on the reading performance of

high ability students (d=+0.155). Notice furthermore that the confidence intervals for the

effect sizes d for the three ability types of students do not overlap, indicating significant

differential effects in favor of the more able students. Stated otherwise: the results support a

divergent pattern. However, caution should be exercised with generalization, since all findings

were based on the same dataset.

Table 1: Meta-analyses: naturally occurring ability grouping practices within classes in primary education;

general and differential effects

Included papers School subject Grade Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence interval

Nomi, 2010; Tach &

Farkas, 2006

Reading and literacy K-1 -0.070* -0.110; -0.029**

Condron, 2008;

Nomi, 2010

Reading and literacy K-3 Low ability

-0.232*

Average ability

0.000

High ability

+0.155*

-0.270; -0.195**

-0.032; +0.031**

+0.124; +0.186**

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

** The standard errors are multiplied with a factor √2 to account for the fact that the same data is used

Between-class setting

A second type of differentiation we found in studies on naturally occurring practices in

primary education is setting students in between-class ability classes for specific topics such

as reading or mathematics. Two selected articles discuss the effects of between-class ability

grouping on student performance (Macqueen, 2012; Whitburn, 2001). In the article of

Macqueen (2012) the gains in performance of students grouped in between-class ability

groups were compared to the performance gains of students in heterogeneous classes. Students

in both conditions were grouped in heterogeneous home classrooms for most school subjects.

However, students in the between-class setting group were allocated to smaller, homogeneous

classes for specific school subjects based on their performance on mathematics and literacy.

Students in the non-grouping condition remained in their heterogeneous home classrooms

throughout the school year. The performance gains between grade three and five for

Page 33: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

30

mathematics, literacy and writing of students in regrouped classes were compared to the gain

scores of students in heterogeneous classes. In general, small and non-significant effects of

regrouping students based on their literacy abilities on student performance in literacy and

writing were found compared to learning in mixed ability classrooms (literacy: d=+0.196;

writing: d=-0.082). Regrouping students based on their mathematical abilities had a small

negative and non-significant effect on students’ mathematics performance (math: d=-0.125).

Analysis of differential effects for high, middle and low groups based on mathematical ability

or literacy ability also did not show any significant differences between the two conditions

(see appendix 2b).

Whitburn (2001) compared mathematics performance between students grouped in

homogeneous classes based on their prior mathematics achievement to the performance of

students taught in mixed ability classes. Both groups of students were taught using the same

interactive, whole class teaching method, which was part of a larger intervention study.

Within this intervention, teachers initiated the two different grouping procedures.

Mathematical performance in this project was regularly monitored using short written tests

about previously taught mathematical topics. These tests were used to analyze grouping

effects on student performance in grades three and four. In the article, results are presented of

three consecutive cohorts of students. In these three cohorts, approximately 200 students were

taught in ability grouped classes and about 1000 students were taught in mixed ability classes.

The first cohort had been grouped for 21 months, the second cohort had been grouped for 15

months and the third cohort had been grouped for about 3 months. The analyses in the first

cohort show small and non-significant effects of between-class ability grouped students’

performance compared to the performance of students in heterogeneous groups (cohort 1

grade 3: d=-0.030; cohort 1 grade 4: d=-0.270). This finding is replicated in the second cohort

(cohort 2 grade 3: d=-0.030; cohort 2 grade 4: d=-0.130). In the third cohort a significant

small negative effect of grouping students in homogeneous classes over heterogeneous classes

was found (cohort 3 grade 3: d=-0.110; cohort 3 grade 4: d=-0.290).

Meta-analyzing the effects of between-class grouping (see Table 2) shows that in the

studies of Macqueen (2012) and Whitburn (2001), between-class setting based on

mathematical ability has a significant negative effect on students’ mathematics performance

(d=-0.142*). The effect of setting is negative and significant for both low ability students (d=-

0.224*), average ability students (d=-0.437*), and high ability students (d=-0.162*).

Furthermore, the confidence intervals for the effect sizes d for the various ability groups do

show quite some overlap, which indicates the absence of differential effects, be it divergent or

convergent.

Page 34: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

31

Table 2: Meta-analyses: naturally occurring ability grouping practices between classes in primary education;

general and differential effects (compared to heterogeneous classes)

Included papers School subject Grade Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence interval

Macqueen, 2012;

Whitburn, 2001

Mathematics 3-6 Overall

-0.142*

Low ability

-0.224*

Average ability

-0.437*

High ability

-0.162*

-0.245; -0.038

-0.382; -0.065

-0.593; -0.282

-0.314; -0.009

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Summarizing, in the studies on naturally occurring differentiation practices, we found that the

effects of within-class grouping vary depending on students’ ability. Small positive effects of

grouping for high ability students were found, but overall within-class ability grouping had a

negative effect on early elementary students’ reading performance. For setting, or regrouping,

a meta-analysis of two studies shows a negative effect on students’ mathematics performance.

However, there are some concerns about the generalizability of these findings. One

methodological concern for the within-class grouping analyses is that they were all based on

the same dataset. And for the analyses of the effects of setting only two studies met the

inclusion-criteria. Moreover, a major drawback of the articles about naturally occurring

practices is that they often do not give insight in the instruction teachers provide. Thus, it is

unclear whether and how instruction within these ability groups was tailored to the needs of

students.

Results of studies on differentiation based on computerized systems

The third category of studies concerns differentiation guided by computer systems. In most

educational settings, ability grouping practices are based on teacher-directed allocation of

students based on students’ prior performance. However, recent developments show that

computer technology can also be used as a tool to support differentiation in primary education.

Computer algorithms may be used to give suggestions on homogeneous grouping procedures

based on students’ prior performance. They can also be used to determine which type of

instruction is most suitable for students’ needs based on analyses of their prior performance.

Using computer technology to support differentiation in such a manner is described in the

articles of Connor and colleagues (Connor, Morrison, Fishman, Schatschneider, &

Underwood, 2007; Connor, Morrison et al., 2011a; Connor, Morrison et al., 2011b) and

Ysseldyke and colleagues (Ysseldyke et al., 2003; Ysseldyke & Bolt, 2007). An overview of

these studies can be found in appendix 2c.

Connor and colleagues published several articles on the effects of individualizing

student instruction (ISI) using A2i software (Assessment-to-Instruction). The ISI intervention

is designed to support teachers in their efforts to provide optimally effective reading

instruction for all students. The computerized system advices the teacher about the amount of

Page 35: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

32

teacher- or student-managed instruction suitable for the specific child based on students’ prior

performance. Additionally, the program provides teachers with suggestions about the content

of the instruction regarding whether the reading instruction should be more code focused or

meaning focused. Based on the suggestions made by the computer program, teachers can

provide reading instruction to small homogeneous groups of students . In the review, three

articles of Connor and colleagues were included which used a student-level cognitive output

measure (Connor et al., 2007; 2011a; 2011b).

In the article of 2007, the authors report on the effectiveness of the ISI treatment on

student language and literacy outcomes. The growth of first grade students from schools in

which teachers used the ISI program to differentiate their reading instruction was compared to

students’ growth in reading performance in matched control schools. Teachers using the ISI

intervention received the program and a professional development course in the use of

differentiated reading instruction. Control group teachers did not receive any professional

development nor did they use the computer program. Results show that the individualized

instruction had a small but significant positive effect on students’ reading achievement on a

standardized test (d=+0.183). Although these results were presumably affected by teachers’

professional development in the experimental group, the authors show that students’ growth in

the experimental group was related to the amount of time spent on the intervention in the

classroom, suggesting that the intervention in itself was also related to students’ reading

outcomes.

Connor et al. (2011a) replicated the first grade results in their study. They analyzed the

effectiveness of the ISI-intervention on students’ word reading skills in comparison to a

business as usual control group. Teachers in the experimental group used the suggestions by

the computer program to form ability groups and to choose the content of their instruction

based on students’ needs. They were supported in the use of the ISI intervention by

professional development instruction and coaching. In the control group, teachers spent an

equal amount of time on small group reading instruction, but did not have access to the

computer program. Classroom observations showed that teachers in the ISI-condition were

better able to fit the content instruction to student-needs based on prior performance than

teachers in the control condition. Matching the instruction to recommendations of the

computerized algorithm strongly predicted students’ reading outcomes. Multilevel analyses

show that the ISI-intervention had a significant positive effect (d=+0.249) on students’ word

readings scores on a standardized test collected in spring of the school year. The authors argue

that the effectiveness of the treatment had increased compared to the study in 2007 since they

made the computer program more user-friendly and the professional development program for

teachers was improved.

Another study on the effectiveness of the ISI-treatment reports treatment effects on

student results in third grade (Connor et al., 2011b). In this study, effects on students’ reading

performance of the intervention were compared to an alternative intervention based on

vocabulary instruction. In the ISI-treatment condition, teachers assessed students’

Page 36: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

33

performance three times a year, used the computerized instructions to determine the focus and

content of their instruction. Teachers also received a professional development training on

implementation of the treatment. In the vocabulary treatment condition, teachers received a

professional development training in which they read and discussed instruction principles

from a vocabulary handbook and designed and evaluated their lessons collaboratively with a

focus group of other teachers. Classroom observations during the school year showed that

teachers in both conditions did not differ in the amount of individualized instruction, in their

organization and planning activities, in the use of strategies and in classroom-management

styles. However, teachers from the ISI group did match their instruction more closely to the

content suggested by the computer algorithm. Multilevel analyses of student results show that

the ISI-training had a small significant positive effect on students’ reading comprehension (d

=+0.191) and on vocabulary performance (d=+0.033) in comparison to the vocabulary

intervention.

Ysseldyke and colleagues (Ysseldyke et al., 2003; Ysseldyke & Bolt, 2007) used a

computer program to support differentiated mathematics instruction. The program they used is

called Accelerated Math. In the article of 2003, the effectiveness of the program on student

results in third, fourth, and fifth grade was assessed. Accelerated Math generates mathematics

exercises for students of different levels of proficiency. After completing the exercises,

students scan their work and the computer provides them with immediate feedback. Also, the

program provides teachers with suggestions about content and grouping practices based on

each student’s individual performance. In this study, teachers from four schools volunteered to

use the computer program during mathematics instruction. Of all classrooms, teachers in ten

classrooms fully implemented the program. Scores of students from classrooms in which

teachers used Accelerated Math were compared to students from other classrooms in these

schools and a random group of students from the district’s testing database. Within schools,

significant small to medium positive effects were found of using the program on a

standardized math test (d=+0.189) and on a computerized adaptive math test (d =+0.268). In

the study published in 2007, Ysseldyke and Bolt investigated the effect of the same system in

both primary and secondary schools. Classrooms were randomly assigned to within-school

experimental and control groups. Again it turned out that when teachers implemented the

continuous progress monitoring system as intended, their students gained significantly more

than (Terra Nova test: d=+0.469; STAR Math test: d=+0.458).

A meta-analysis on the effect estimates from the studies on the computer-based

differentiation interventions shows that both in math and in reading, computer algorithms

fostering differentiation can positively affect student performance (see Table 3). The meta-

analysis of the articles of Connor and colleagues (2007; 2011ab; 2011ba) shows a significant

small positive effect of the computer intervention on students’ reading performance

(d=+0.204). A meta-analysis of the two articles of Ysseldyke (2003; 2007) shows a significant

medium positive effect of the computerized differentiation intervention on students’ math

performance (d=+0.345). Although the number of articles included in this meta-analysis is

Page 37: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

34

small, the cumulated effects show that a computer supported approach to differentiation in

which both grouping and instructional content is addressed can be beneficial for students’

performance in primary education.

Table 3: Meta-analyses; differentiation based on computerized systems in primary education; effects for reading

and mathematics

Included papers School subject Grade Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence interval

Connor et al., 2007;

2011a; 2011b

reading 1-3 +0.204* +0.104; +0.303

Ysseldyke et al.2003;

Ysseldyke & Bolt,

2007

mathematics 2-6 +0.345*

+0.232; +0.458

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Results of studies on differentiation as part of a broader school reform program

The fourth category of articles describes differentiation in the context of a broader program.

Implementing differentiation practices cannot be done in isolation, and moreover synergetic

effects can be expected when differentiation is one of the many elements of a well-designed

comprehensive program. This paragraph looks into studies on the effects of such programs,

although one has to bear in mind that effects (or absence of effects) cannot – by definition –

be solely attributed to the differentiation component of such a program. The key features and

summary estimated effects for the various studies are presented in appendix 2d .

The most well known and most researched program is Success for All. Success for All

aims at comprehensive school reform to ensure that all children can read. For reading

instruction pupils are regrouped across grades according to specific performance levels (i.e.

setting). Every nine weeks pupils are assessed and regrouped when necessary. Pupils that need

additional help receive one-to-one-tutoring to get them back on track so as to achieve

convergent differentiation. The article of Borman, Slavin, Cheung, Chamberlain, Madden and

Chambers (2007) reports final literacy outcomes for a 3-year longitudinal sample of pupils

from 35 schools who participated in an effect study of Success for All (cluster randomized

controlled design) from kindergarten to second grade. The significant effects of the treatment

were as large as one third of a standard deviation on all three outcome measures (Word

Identification: d=+0.220, Word Attack: d=+0.330, Passage Comprehension: d=+0.210).

The second article that matches the criteria for inclusion is an article of Stevens and

Slavin (1995) in which achievement (among other measures) of grade two to six students of

two cooperative elementary schools were compared to the achievement of comparable

students in three control schools. Being a cooperative school implied several elements: using

cooperative learning across a variety of content areas, full-scale mainstreaming of

academically handicapped students, teachers using peer coaching, teachers planning

cooperatively, and parent involvement in school. For the present study, teachers were trained

to work with two comprehensive programs designed to accommodate student diversity: CIRC

(Cooperative Integrated Reading and Composition) and TAI (Team Assisted

Page 38: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

35

Individualization-Mathematics). In both programs students worked in heterogeneous learning

teams but received instruction in relatively homogeneous teaching groups, elements that are

also included in Success for All. Students’ achievement was tested in reading, language and

mathematics after one and after two years. During the first years the two schools were

implementing the program and students’ achievement only differed – in favor of the

cooperative schools – on reading vocabulary (d=+0.170). After two years, students of the

cooperative schools also performed better at reading comprehension (d=+0.280), language

expression (d=+0.210), and math computation (d=+0.290). In language mechanics and math

application treatment and control schools do not differ.

Because the programs (cooperative school, CIRC and TAI) had so many components it

is difficult to ascribe the outcomes to any single element. However, according to the authors,

the results of the study support the hypothesis that cooperative learning can be effective in

producing higher student achievement. In terms of differentiation, this finding supports the

effectiveness of working in heterogeneous learning teams - which involves group goals based

on group members’ individual learning performance - and homogeneous teaching groups.

Reis, McCoach, Coyne, Schreiber, Eckert and Gubbins (2007) combined their School-

wide Enrichment Model in Reading Framework (SEM-R) with Success for All. This article

discusses an experiment executed in two primary schools serving a primarily culturally

diverse, high poverty group of students. The schools participating in the study were required

to give reading instruction each afternoon in addition to the Success for All program which

they used in the morning. In the experiment, effectiveness of two types of reading instruction

is evaluated by randomly assigning teachers and students to two conditions. Teachers were

frequently coached and observed during the experiment. Students in the control condition

received twelve weeks of literacy instruction based on whole group instruction with

workbook-materials and test-preparation assignments. Students in the experimental condition

used the School-wide Enrichment Model in Reading Framework (SEM-R) for twelve weeks.

In SEM-R teachers first read aloud and use higher order questioning and thinking-skills

instruction. Then, students were encouraged to select books suitable for their ability level.

During this phase, teachers gave individualized support and differentiated instruction about

reading strategies. In the third phase, students chose between different literacy-related

activities with varying complexity. Posttest results showed a positive effect of SEM-R on

students reading fluency (d=+0.299), but no significant effects on students reading

comprehension (d=+0.220).

After this experiment Reis, McCoach, Little, Muller and Kaniskan (2011)

implemented SEM-R without Success for All in five primary schools serving a primarily

culturally diverse, high poverty group of students. This article discusses a cluster-randomized

experiment in which teachers were randomly assigned to a control or treatment condition. In

both conditions teachers had a two-hour block of reading and arts instruction every day for

five months. In the control condition, the full two hours were devoted to the regular reading

and language arts program. This program was mostly teacher led and consisted of silent

Page 39: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

36

reading activities, test preparation activities, workbook exercises and some small group or

individual instruction (21% of the time). The teachers assigned to the experimental condition

used the same program for the first hour and used SEM-R during the second hour. Matching

students on individual performance, teachers provided students with feedback and individual

instructions. In the third phase, students chose between different literacy-related activities with

varying complexity. Posttest results for reading fluency and reading comprehension were

mixed. Students in the control and the experimental group both increased their performance

after the intervention. In two schools students receiving SEM-R outperformed control

students, but in the other three schools no apparent differences were found. The authors

suggest that the SEM-R approach may be especially suitable for (sub)urban schools.

Nevertheless, the overall effects were non-significant (Fluency: d=+0.254, Comprehension:

d=+0.145).

In the Netherlands, Houtveen and van de Grift (2012) reported on the effects of the

Reading Acceleration Programme (RAP). The program aims at reducing the percentage of

struggling readers in the first year of formal schooling. A quasi-experimental study was

carried out. The teachers in the experimental group had been trained to improve their core

instruction (Tier 1), to broaden their instruction for struggling readers (Tier 2) and to

implement special measures for pupils who did not respond sufficiently to the interventions

(Tier 3). The aim of Tier 2 and 3 is to make it possible for the students to attend the whole

group instruction successfully (convergent differentiation). After correcting for pre-test, age,

intelligence, socioeconomic status and ethnic minority a significant difference on reading was

found in favor of the pupils in the experimental group (Word Decoding: d=+0.280, Fluency:

d=+0.620).

A meta-analysis on the effects presented in the articles of Stevens and Slavin, Borman

et al. and Reis shows a small significant positive effect of the programs on reading

comprehension (d=+0.231); see Table 4. The meta-analysis of the effects from the studies of

Borman et al., Houtveen and van de Grift and Reis shows a significant medium positive effect

of the programs on basic reading (d=+0.375). Mathematics and language were only covered

by the study of Stevens and Slavin. These effects are non-significant or very small.

The main drawback of these programs in terms of this best-evidence review on

differentiation is the fact that it is unclear which part of the program causes the effect.

Probably all aspects ‘work’ together, which leads to higher achievement of students.

Page 40: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

37

Table 4: Meta-analyses: differentiation as part of comprehensive programs in primary education; effects on

basic reading and reading comprehension

Included papers School subject Grade Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence interval

Borman et al., 2007;

Reis et al, 2007; Reis

et al., 2011; Stevens &

Slavin, 1995

reading

comprehension

Grades 2 to 6 +0.231* +0.128; +0.333

Borman et al., 2007;

Houtveen et al., 2012;

Reis et al, 2007; Reis

et al., 2011

basic reading Grades 2 to 6 +0.375*

+0.279; +0.471

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

4.3.4. An example of an effective comprehensive program: Success for All

SfA - its effects were presented in the previous paragraph - is a school wide program for

students in grades pre-K to 6 which organizes resources to ensure that virtually every student

will reach the third grade on time with adequate basic skills and build on this basis throughout

the elementary grades. The main element is the reading program. In grades K-1 (in

Kindergarten: Stepping Stones and KinderRoots incorporated in KinderCorner, in grade 1:

Reading Roots containing FastTrack Phonics, Shared Stories, Story Telling and Retelling

(STAR) and Language Links) it emphasizes language and comprehension skills, phonics,

sound blending and use of shared stories that students read to one another in pairs. The stories

combine teacher-read material with phonetically regular student material to teach decoding

and comprehension in the context of meaningful, engaging stories. In grades two to six

(Reading Wings, an adaptation of Cooperative Integrated Reading and Composition - CIRC)

students use “real” novels and books but not workbooks. The program emphasizes cooperative

learning and partner reading activities, comprehension strategies such as summarization and

clarification built around narrative and expository texts, writing and direct instruction in

reading comprehension skills.

During daily 90-minute reading periods, students from all heterogeneous ‘home room’

classes (grade 1 to 6) are regrouped across age lines so that each reading class contains

students all at one reading level. Use of tutors as reading teachers during reading time reduces

the size of most reading classes to about twenty students. Students in first to sixth grade are

assessed every trimester to determine whether they are making adequate progress in reading.

This information is used to suggest alternate teaching strategies in the regular classroom,

changes in reading group placement and provision of tutoring services. Specially trained

teachers and paraprofessionals offer tutorial services in grade one to three to students who are

failing to keep up with their classmates in reading. Tutorial instruction is closely coordinated

with regular classroom instruction. It takes place in one-to-one settings, twenty minutes daily

during times other than reading periods.

The instruction process is based on research-proven practices combined in the model

of instructional effectiveness called QAIT, quality, adaptation (to the level and pace of each

student), incentive (strategies to increase students’ motivation to learn) and time. Cooperative

Page 41: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

38

learning is a central feature in SfA: groups can earn recognition only if all team members have

learned, so they encourage and help each other to master academic content.

SfA further consists of comprehensive, theme-based preschool (Curiosity Corner) and

Kindergarten (KinderCorner) programs, a professional development program, a school

facilitator and a solutions team in each school to plan school wide strategies for parental and

community involvement, attendance and school climate.

4.4. Effects of differentiation in Early Secondary Education (12-14 years)

4.4.1. Overview of differentiation in Early Secondary Education

While primary education is generally a heterogeneous environment, secondary education

tends to be more homogeneous, due to external differentiation or tracking. Students in

secondary education are generally assigned to educational tracks or grouped for specific

subjects, mostly language and math (setting). Tracking and setting are based on student’s

cognitive abilities, leading to homogeneous classes or courses. In the first one or two years of

secondary education, a mitigated form of external differentiation may be used, with students

with adjacent educational levels grouped together. Students are provided with differentiated

assignments and tests, with additional work or test items for the more able students. This way,

the most appropriate level for every student should emerge during the early secondary school

years. After the first basic years of secondary education, students choose vocational tracks or

curricular profiles based on their own interests.

Grouping in secondary education leads to divergent differentiation in the student

population as a whole, although within classrooms or curricular subjects convergent

differentiation is pursued. Within tracked classrooms, although the groups are homogeneous

based on general levels of ability, large individual differences between students may still exist,

which requires within-class differentiation. However, differentiation is not an educational

practice that teachers in secondary schools tend to apply, especially in the higher pre-

academic tracks (Inspectorate of Education, 2013).

Countries differ in the way secondary education is organized: the degree to which

external differentiation is implemented and the age at which students are tracked differs. This

international variation in educational systems makes it difficult to study the effects of external

differentiation. Most studies make use of cross-sectional international assessments of IEA-

TIMSS or OECD-PISA, and thus are suffering from all sorts of methodological flaws that

hinder causal conclusions to be made about the relation between differentiation and student

achievement. The most obvious problem is that students are selected into tracks at an early

age, so one never knows whether the student achievement differences between integrated and

differentiated educational systems – say at the age of 15 - are the result of the system

differences or differences already present at an earlier age – say the age of 12. Clever

solutions have been tried to circumvent this problem, like naturally occurring experiments in

Great-Britain and Sweden where integrated and differentiated systems co-existed for a while

Page 42: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

39

(c.f. Luyten, 2008), Difference-in-Difference models in which many countries with and

without early tracking were compared with respect to the within-country differences between

secondary and primary school performance (Hanushek & Woessmann, 2006), or propensity

score matching techniques in which students from the integrated Polish System were matched

to similar students from the tracked system before the education reform (Jakubowski,

Patrinos, Porta, & Wisniewski, 2010). The results are not very clear-cut, but at least seem to

indicate that integrated systems in general do not perform worse than differentiated systems.

And moreover, as was described in the theoretical framework, no effects of tracking or setting

are found when the results of students of lower, average and higher ability are taken into

account simultaneously. Below we will concentrate on reviewing systematically studies that

where conducted within one country with a direct comparison of differently differentiated

groups of students.

4.4.2. Selected studies

In the initial database search, approximately 100 papers focusing on early secondary education

(12-16 years) were found. Of these, approximately 40 were selected for further inspection

based on their full text versions. In order to maintain the focus on early secondary education

and/or middle school, the general age criteria were sharpened and restricted to the first two

years of secondary education (grades 7 and 8; approximately 12-14 years of age). Four of the

obtained papers met these new age criteria and the 8 final inclusion criteria (paragraph 3.2).

These selected papers are alphabetically listed and summarized in appendix 3.

4.4.3. Literature synthesis

General overview

The studies selected for this review all focused on differentiation practices for mathematics

only. Two studies from the same authors (Burris et al., 2006; 2008) are on the effects of an

accelerated math curriculum in heterogeneous classrooms. The study by Barrow c.s. (2009)

focuses on computer assisted mathematics instruction according to general principles of

mastery learning. And a study by Linchevski and Kutscher (1998) focuses on the question

whether small heterogeneous groups have different effects on mathematics achievement that

homogeneous groups. Key features and summary of estimated effects for each of the studies

are presented in appendix 3. Due to large differences between the studies in terms of

operationalization of differentiation and/or the criterion variables used, it was not possible to

perform meta-analyses on the studied included.

Results of the included studies

Barrow, Markman and Rouse (2009) conducted a randomized controlled trial on the use of

individualized computerized (pre-)algebra instruction. Within schools, grade 8 classrooms

were randomly assigned to the experimental condition using computerized instruction, or to

the control condition using traditional forms of instruction. Each computerized mathematics

lesson consisted of a pretest, a review of prerequisite knowledge, the subject content, a review

Page 43: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

40

and a comprehensive test. Students repeat the lesson until they reach sufficient mastery. The

teacher receives progress reports and provides individualized instruction to students who need

it. Use of the computerized instruction positively influenced algebra achievement of the

students (d=+0.416).

Burris, Heubert, and Levin (2006) studied the effect of offering an accelerated math

curriculum in heterogeneous classrooms in middle school on students’ math achievement and

completion of advanced courses. They studied whether more students would take and pass

advanced math classes in high school when heterogeneous, advanced math classes were

offered to all students in middle school and whether providing heterogeneous math classes to

students of all ability levels would influence the performance of initial high achievers. The

study focusses on cohorts of students before and after a curriculum change, in which

accelerated mathematics was implemented in middle school. The accelerated mathematics

included offering the regular 3-year math curriculum for grades 6, 7 and 8 of middle school in

2 years, creating time to offer a more advanced algebra course in 8th

grade. Originally, only

selected students took part in the accelerated program, but after a while schools were

mandated to offer accelerated mathematics for all students, in heterogeneous classrooms.

Additional math support was available for students struggling with the advanced curriculum.

Results showed that opening up the curriculum for all students in heterogeneous classrooms

led to more students successfully completing two of the three advanced mathematics courses

that increase in difficulty (d=+1.450 and d=+1.511).

In a later study, Burris and colleagues (2008) again studied the effect of offering an

accelerated math curriculum to all students, making use of the system change in a New York

state school district. This time, instead of studying the relationship between detracking and

completing mathematics courses, they looked at the relationship between detracking and

receiving diplomas tied to state-wide or international standards. These diplomas are additional

to local school diplomas and reflect rigorous achievement requirements. Results show that

detracked students had a greater chance of receiving a state diploma than tracked students

(d=+3.187)10

No significant differences between detracked and tracked students were found

for receiving the prestigious international baccalaureate diploma.

Linchevski and Kutscher (1998) studied the effect of teaching mathematics in

heterogeneous groups. The schools participating in the study had heterogeneous classrooms in

which students worked sometimes in whole class settings, small heterogeneous groups, small

homogeneous groups and large homogeneous groups. Large (whole) group learning was

mainly teacher driven, while small group learning was fostered by cooperative learning. After

one school year, heterogeneous classrooms (with cooperative learning and instruction in

homogeneous groups when needed) had a significant small positive effect on math

performance compared to the performance that was expected when students would have been

10

This somewhat unusual large effect size is calculated by transforming the LogOdds-Ratio of 5.78 into the

effect size d applying the equality d=LogOddsRatio x (√3)/ 𝜋 (Borenstein et al., 2009, p. 47).

Page 44: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

41

homogeneously grouped throughout the year (d=+0.112). Retention effects for a small group

of schools at the end of 8th

grade were not significant.

4.4.4 An example of an effective comprehensive program: IMPROVE

Comprehensive programs of which differentiation is an integral part do exist, but solid proof

that such programs are effective only exists in the domain of mathematics (Slavin, Lake, &

Groff, 2009) and not for reading and/or science. The Best Evidence Encyclopedia11

only

mentions two, namely STAD and IMPROVE. The reason why these were not initially

included in the meta-analysis were that no references were found in our search to STAD, due

to the fact that the key element of this program is cooperative learning, rather than

differentiation. The literature search did result in references to IMPROVE , but these were

rejected as the quintessential element of the program is metacognitive instruction rather than

differentiation. However, IMPROVE and STAD do contain differentiation as an element,

albeit less pronounced than other elements. Therefore, therefore IMPROVE be described here

as an example of a successful comprehensive program for early secondary education.

IMPROVE (Mevarech & Kramarski, 1997) is developed as an alternative to streaming or

setting, and was evaluated in Israeli schools. The acronym stands for: Introducing new

(mathematical) concepts, Metacognitive questioning, Practicing, Reviewing and reducing

difficulties, Obtaining mastery, Verification, and Enrichment. Important elements are that

within the heterogeneous groups students question each other metacognitively (which implies

cooperative learning based on peer interaction), continue learning for mastery up till 80%

correct, and based on this criterion students either continue for enrichment or individualized

corrective instruction. The evaluation studies are relevant because IMPROVE is compared to

business as usual in ability tracked classrooms. All in all, students in the IMPROVE condition

outperform the control students, but the results are somewhat mixed. In a first study the main

effect of IMPROVE for algebra is d=+0.301, and there are some indications for treatment x

aptitude interactions, meaning that IMPROVE is effective for low, middle, and high ability

students, but especially for the latter two groups. A second study produced similar main

effects, and also the treatment x aptitude interactions seemed to indicate that IMPROVE was

somewhat more effective for middle and high ability students than that it was for low ability

students. Stated somewhat conservative: IMPROVE is effective, but there are no indications

that it leads to convergent differentiation. The authors indicate that “It is possible that lower

achieving students need additional support in order to further enhance their achievement”

(Mevarech & Kramarski, 1997, p. 385). Although the effects are positive, once again one has

to bear in mind, that it is the synergetic effect of various elements (a.o. metacognitive

strategies, cooperative learning, regular assessments, learning for mastery, corrective

instruction) that is probably generating the effects and not differentiation as such.

11

Retrieved from http://www.bestevidence.org/math/mhs/top.htm at November 14, 2014.

Page 45: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

42

4.5. Reflection on the included studies

Having presented and discussed the many findings from the 26 studies, we have to consider

the possibility that the results may suffer from selection problems. Although our literature

search initially resulted in almost 2,500 references, our very strict substantive and rigorous

methodological inclusion criteria ruled out the vast majority of these references. Valuable as

many of these references may have been from a conceptual, theoretical, and/or practical point

of view, or as a rich qualitative description of occurring differentiation practices, for this

review we were solely interested in studies that could shed light on the association between

differentiation practices and students’ cognitive outcomes. This type of selection was thus

intended. Another kind of selection, however, could not be controlled by us, and that is that

valuable studies may not have found their way to scientific journals since the results were

viewed as disappointing or not ground breaking enough. Such selection often starts with

researchers who themselves may not find it worthwhile to put effort in trying to get non-

significant effects published. And, in second instance, journal editors and reviewers may be

biased towards accepting manuscripts that contain statistically significant effects. To gain

insight in the prevalence of this second type of selection within our dataset we assume the

following model underlying publication bias Studies that do not have much statistical power

as a result of small samples, only get published if they produce large effects that

counterbalance the large standard errors. Studies that produce smaller effects find their way

only to journals if they have (considerably) more statistical power (resulting from a big

sample with consequently small standard errors). If this model is true, then the distribution of

reported effect sizes is strongly biased (normally positively biased, but that of course depends

on the phenomenon of interest and the scaling of the variables) as a function of an increasing

standard error. A visual inspection of the relation between effect size and confidence interval

may help us to sort this out. For that purpose we selected one finding for mathematics and

language respectively per study (in case there were multiple cohorts we treated each cohort as

a separate study), discarding the studies of Burris that focused on other outcomes (taking an

advanced course or getting a diploma). See Figure 2.

Page 46: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

43

Figure 2: Forest plot for the studies (one finding per subject per study selected) in the review

There is a slight tendency that the studies with the smaller effect sizes also have the smaller

confidence intervals, and at least for language in early childhood education, kindergarten and

primary education the larger effect sizes are accompanied with wider confidence intervals. A

second aid to detect potential publication bias may be of further help, and this is to be found in

Figure 3.

Page 47: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

44

Figure 3: Funnel plot to inspect publication bias from the studies reviewed

The vertical line in the middle represents the average effect in a meta-analysis using a random

effects model12

. The picture shows that all the effect sizes are evenly distributed to the left and

the right of the line. Assuming the correctness of our model of publication bias, our results

thus do not seem to be overwhelmingly plagued with this phenomenon.

Finally, we can analyze whether differences in effect sizes found are related to the

sector studied (Early Childhood, primary or secondary education), the type of differentiation

(ability grouping (either within or across classes) or otherwise), whether it is computer

supported or not and if differentiation was studied as being an element of a broader program.

Table 5 contains the regression coefficients from a meta-regression model in which the effect

sizes were regressed on these study characteristics. The meta-regression analyses were

conducted using HLM software (Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, Congdon, & du Toit, 2011).

Table 5: Meta-regression results (standard errors in brackets) from regressing effect sizes on study

characteristics

Regression coefficient 95% confidence

interval

t-ratio p-value

Intercept

primary vs ECE

secondary vs ECE

ability grouping vs otherwise

computer supported or not

part of broader program or not

+0.176 (0.054)

-0.293 (0.083)

-0.227 (0.127)

-0.011 (0.089)

+0.401 (0.088)

+0.428 (0.085)

+0.070; +0.282

-0.456; -0.130

-0.476; +0.022

-0.185; +0.163

+0.229; +0.573

+0.261; +0.595

+3.354

-3.548

-1.793

-0.122

+4.566

+5.024

.004

.002

.086

.905

<.001

<.001

12

The difference between a fixed and random effect model is, that in the first we assume that in all the studies

the true effect size is the same, whereas in the latter we do not.

Page 48: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Results

45

The meta-regression results for the selected findings indicate that differentiation practices in

primary are less effective than those in Early Childhood Education; that differentiation

practices in secondary education are almost even effective as those in primary education; that

using computer supported differentiation is more effective than other differentiation practices;

and that broader programs of which differentiation is one of many key elements are the most

effective. Ability grouping, either within or across classes, is not less effective than other

differentiation practices given the other study characteristics13

. The meta-regression results

may be of help in finding some structure amidst all the associations reported.

13

Not reported here are the results of an additional meta-regression analysis, in which also a dummy for subject

domain (mathematics versus language) was included. This model produced similar results and there appear to be

no differences between the two subject domains.

Page 49: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

46

Page 50: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

47

5. Conclusion and discussion

Students differ, and they may differ quite a lot even if they are in the same classroom.

Didactical age differences between children in the same class may amount to 4 years,

implying that, for instance in a grade 4 class of a primary school, some students perform at the

average level of grade 2, whereas others have already advanced up till the average level of

grade 6. Differentiation and adaptive instruction together are seen as a way to address these

differences, but how these practices can be implemented well in the classroom is less clear.

Differentiation is essential, but there are many forms. Grouping may be one, allowing time

differences for mastering curricular subjects another. What are proven effective practices?

In this systematic review we summarized the results of studies into the effects of

differentiation practices along three stages in the education system: early childhood education

and kindergarten (2;6 to 6 years), primary education (6 to 12 years), and early secondary

education (12-14 years). We also described exemplary effective comprehensive programs, in

which differentiation was one of many elements, for each stage. From the almost 2,500

references related to differentiation found in the literature search around 1% met the inclusion

criteria set for this review.

5.1. Early Childhood Education and Kindergarten

Early Childhood Education and Kindergarten was not part of the reviews on studies on

differentiation up to 1995 (Kulik & Kulik, 1984; Lou et al., 1996; Slavin, 1987a; Slavin,

1987b). These reviews include studies with grade 1 as the youngest age group and therefore

do not provide information on differentiation at earlier ages. Only the study of Lou and

colleagues might be informative in this respect. They compared the effects of within-class

homogeneous grouping between early and late elementary grades (respectively grades 1-3 and

grades 4-6) and found that the effects in the earlier grades were much smaller (d=+0.08, 95%

CI [+0.02;+0.14]) than the effects in the later grades (d=+0.29, 95% CI [+0.24;+0.35]). One

may infer from this finding that homogeneous ability grouping is less effective at lower

grades, and therefore as well in pre-K and K. On the other hand, since language and literacy

development is a main goal of Early Childhood Education, especially for second language

learners and children with limited language input at home, (convergent) differentiation

practices are probably applied. In order to gather empirical evidence on this matter, in the

current review studies on differentiation practices in Early Childhood Education and

Kindergarten are taken into account.

The general result from the systematic review is that within-class homogeneous ability

grouping has a moderate positive effect on the language performance of the classroom, with

effect sizes for undifferentiated effects ranging from d=+0.068 to d=+0.911. The existence

and direction of differential effects for differentiation on language growth are studied less and

are inconclusive. It is therefore difficult to draw conclusions about the convergent or divergent

Page 51: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

48

effect of differentiation practices in Early Childhood Education. Mathematical performance

was only addressed in one study (Chang, 2008), in which spending relatively large amounts of

time in small groups had no or negative effects. There are several factors that should be taken

into account when interpreting these findings.

Important to note is that only seven studies on differentiation in ECE and Kindergarten

met de inclusion criteria, of which six were based on data from the same longitudinal study,

ECLS-K. This means only a fraction of the studies on teaching practices and child

development in ECE and Kindergarten was selected for the current review and results may

therefore be hard to generalize. Perhaps studies in this field generally do not explicitly focus

on achievement in relation to grouping or other differentiation practices and/or do not describe

these practices in terms of ‘differentiation’. In order to get a better view on differentiation at

these younger ages, in a future study, it may be worthwhile to look in more detail at the jargon

used for describing differentiation practices in ECE and Kindergarten and to include studies

using other, more descriptive, research methods as well.

The differentiation practice used in the selected studies is ‘within-class homogeneous

ability grouping’. Due to different combinations of variables from the ECLS-K database, these

studies vary in their operationalization of ‘homogeneous grouping’, from broad dichotomous

grouping/no grouping to combinations of intensity of grouping and intensity of instruction.

The studies based on ECLS-K data do not specify how the ability groups are formed and on

what information they are based. Furthermore, they do not specify the type and quality of the

instruction and materials provided to these ability groups. The importance of this information

is illustrated with the study of Hong and Hong (2009), who found that homogeneous ability

grouping, of either high or low intensity, had positive effects on reading growth only if

students receive at least one hour of reading instruction a day. When students received less

instruction, grouping did not make a difference compared to whole class activities. This

emphasizes that the effects of grouping as such are difficult to interpret as long as it is

unknown what the teacher does with these groups. This is in line with the conclusion Lou and

colleagues drew from their review: “It appears that the positive effects of within-class

grouping are maximized when the physical placement of students into groups for learning is

accompanied by modifications to teaching methods and instructional materials. Merely

placing students together is not sufficient for promoting substantive gains in achievement.”

(Lou et al., 1996, p.448). Making use of existing databases, like ECLS-K, implies having to

work with available data and therefore not being able to gather additional information on

differentiation practices, unfortunately.

One study included in the current literature review does provide more information of

the implementation of differentiation, namely the study on the effect of the comprehensive

literacy program EMERGE (Gettinger & Stoiber, 2012). Within EMERGE, within-class

ability grouping is part of a broader package of frequent process monitoring, enriched literacy

content, and intensive teacher coaching. What is relevant is not the amount of time students

spend in homogeneous ability groups (which is 30 minutes daily), but the fact that groups are

Page 52: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Conclusion and discussion

49

created based on recent performance data and that teachers are guided towards offering

students of different performance levels appropriate, differentiated instruction and activities.

This approach is fundamentally different compared to the ECLS-K studies, which only look at

intensity or frequency of grouping.

5.2. Primary Education

Overall, based on reviews summarizing studies on differentiation up to 1995, previous studies

did not report clear effects of between-class homogeneous ability grouping in primary

education, but they did report some positive effects of providing students with instruction in

small (homogeneous) ability groups within the classroom. Furthermore, both Slavin (1978a)

and Lou and colleagues (1996) argue that the key of successful differentiation may not be

merely placing students in groups, but actually adapting the teaching to the needs of different

ability groups. Aim of this review was to replicate and extend the knowledge on the effects of

differentiation practices. In the current systematic review, we included sixteen articles dealing

with differentiation practices in primary education. Within these articles, we discerned four

types of studies: studies of an intervention using ability grouping , studies analyzing the

effects of naturally occurring grouping practices, studies on differentiation practices supported

by computer systems, and studies in which differentiation was part of a broader school reform.

In the studies on naturally occurring practices, we found two types of differentiation

practices which were also described in previous studies: within-class homogeneous ability

grouping and between-class homogeneous grouping (also called setting). The two between-

class ability grouping studies in our sample were on the effects of regrouping students for

specific subjects or tracking students in homogeneous classes. Summarizing the effects of the

two studies, a small negative effect was found of streaming or tracking on students’

mathematics performance in homogeneous ability grouped classes compared to heterogeneous

classes, especially for average ability students. This in contrast to previous reviews (Lou et

al., 1996; Slavin, 1987a), in which no clear differential effects were found.

Another two studies of naturally occurring practices in primary education compared

within-class ability grouping to not grouping students. Here, effects of near zero were found.

However, the two studies providing insight in differential effects, show that homogeneous

ability grouping overall had a small positive effect on high ability students’ reading

performance and a small negative effect on low ability students’ performance. In this respect,

within-class ability grouping could have a divergent effect, widening the gap between high

and low ability students’ performance. Only one study in our sample evaluated the

effectiveness of an intervention which was specifically aimed at grouping students in either

homogeneous versus heterogeneous ability groups within the classroom. In this study, a

negative and non-significant effect of homogenous grouping was found compared to

heterogeneous grouping. The finding from the meta-analysis of Lou et al. (1996) in which

heterogeneous grouping was more beneficial for low ability students could not be replicated.

Page 53: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

50

One reason why our findings on the effects of within-class ability grouping were not in

line with previous positive findings on within-class ability grouping (Lou et al., 1996; Slavin,

1987a) may be that the studies on natural occurring grouping practices only gave insight in

whether teachers used grouping or not, but not in how the grouping was actually used to

provide adapted instruction. As noted previously, grouping may only be effective in cases in

which instruction is also adapted to students’ specific academic needs. The fact that ability

grouping should be combined with instructional practices is illustrated by our review of the

effectiveness of the use of adaptive computer systems for students’ performance in reading

and mathematics. In these studies, the computer adaptive system evaluated students’ prior

performance and used this to provide suggestions on the instructional content that students

needed, which in turn influenced the grouping practices. Our meta-analyses of the findings of

the studies using such a combination of adaptive testing, feedback and differentiated

instruction show that this type of within-class differentiation can positively affect students’

performance. Such computerized aids for supporting differentiation practices seem to be an

interesting addition to the literature on differentiation from 1995 onwards.

Lastly, the effects of school reform programs in which differentiation was a prominent

part of the program were evaluated. These comprehensive school reform programs such as

Success for All, SEM-R and the Reading Acceleration Program overall had small to medium

positive effects on students’ reading performance. Again, it seems that the positive effect is

magnified by combining different grouping practices with a varied offer of instructional

content and school wide reform. For instance, in the Success for All program, students are

regrouped across classes for daily reading periods. In the small reading classes, students’

progress is frequently monitored and powerful instructional strategies aimed at increasing

achievement and motivation are applied by well-trained tutors. Also, in the program, students

work in cooperative groups frequently. This is another way to flexibly group students

according to their instructional needs.

5.3. Early Secondary Education

The big differentiation question in secondary education can be framed as: “To track or not to

track?” International debates about comprehensive or differentiated systems are heated, but

the problem is that decisive scientific information can hardly be found since comparing the

performance of national education systems mostly is based on international cross-sectional

assessment studies like OECD-PISA or IEA-TIMSS. Another problem is that it is hard to

ascribe differences between students to the effects tracking, since these may be due to existing

differences that led them to be placed in a certain track in the first place. The results of studies

on this topic are not very clear-cut, but at least seem to indicate that integrated systems in

general do not perform worse than differentiated systems. And moreover, as was described in

the theoretical framework, no effects of tracking or setting are found when the results of

students of lower, average and higher ability are taken into account simultaneously.

Page 54: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Conclusion and discussion

51

The early review studies of Kulik and Kulik (1982) and Slavin (1990) on

differentiation in secondary education concern the effects of ability grouping practices. A

rigorous approach to assessing effects of ability grouping practices is to consider the whole

population of students and not a selected subpopulation (e.g. gifted students or low ability

students). Unfortunately, many studies do not address the effects ability grouping practices

may have for the students not included. Studies on ability grouping practices for high ability

students, for example, often fail to study the effects that separating high from average and low

ability students may have on the performance of these latter two groups. In the end we only

found four studies that both met are substantive and methodological inclusion criteria and

studied the whole range of students varying in abilities.

The studies differ quite a bit. One study focused on computer aided mastery learning in

the domain of mathematics, provided individualized instruction to students. Moreover, using

progress reports from the computer system teachers provided additional individual support to

students who need this. The effects of this approach were near medium (d=+0.416), and in

line with findings reported for similar differentiation practices in primary education.

Two studies by Burris and colleagues (2006, 2008) looked into the effects of an

accelerated math curriculum - the same curricular content was offered in two rather than the

usual three years - that was taught in heterogeneous ability classes (rather than in the usual

homogeneous ability classes), with additional instructional help for struggling learners. Unlike

the other studies in our review the effects studied where not the cognitive math effects, but

whether or not students opted for advanced math subjects after those two years and/or

received a prestigious diploma afterwards. The results of these studies indicated that this was

indeed the case, leading the authors to the conclusion that detracking can be done

successfully.

Linchevski and Kutscher (1998) also looked for the effects of detracking grouping

strategies in the mathematics domain. They studied an intervention that consisted of a mix of

either heterogeneous or homogenous grouping after whole class instruction, with small group

learning being fostered by cooperative learning. Heterogeneous grouping had a slight

advantage over homogeneous grouping (d=+0.112), but retention effects could not be

established.

Integrating differentiation practices in comprehensive programs that includes many

more elements seems very promising. Once again, however, successful studies only have been

conducted in the domain of mathematics. Similar studies on comprehensive programs for

language were either designed with less rigor or produced less promising findings. We

discussed the IMPROVE program, as an example of a proven effective broad program

(d=+0.301). Important elements are that within the heterogeneous groups students question

each other metacognitively (which implies cooperative learning based on peer interaction),

continue learning for mastery up till 80% correct, and based on this criterion students either

continue for enrichment or individualized corrective instruction.

Page 55: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

52

5.4. Recommendations for research and practice

When trying to understand the effects of differentiation, it is important to use an ecologically

valid operationalization of differentiation. Differentiation is more than within-class

homogeneous ability grouping, and within-class homogeneous ability grouping is more than

placing students together at a table for a certain amount of time. The real question is how

teachers take into account differences between students in daily classroom practice and how

they can be supported in doing so. Sensible ability grouping (both homogeneous and

heterogeneous) and sensible application of other differentiation practices, like adaptive

questioning during whole class activities, assume two things: teachers need to have an

accurate view of students’ level of understanding and teachers need to know which instruction

and learning activity is appropriate for children at different levels, given the goals they strive

for. Therefore, differentiation might be best applied within the context of comprehensive

programs aimed at supporting teachers to adapt their teaching towards the needs of students.

Most research on comprehensive programs we found focuses on reading and literacy.

Differentiation in the domain of mathematics is often approached by using computer software.

Software, either aiming at the domain of mathematics or language, can take part of the

assessment and diagnosing out of the hand of the teacher and may provide instructional

suggestions. Computer supported differentiation practices open the gates for completely

individualized learning and instruction routes. Although computerized programs can be a

helpful tool, it is the teacher who implements the differentiation practices and using

differentiation software is not a guarantee for actual differentiation in the classroom.

For future research into differentiation practices our recommendations are the following:

1. Differentiation is not a concept that is used much in studies in Early Childhood

Education. However, it is likely to be part of ECE classrooms with their child-

following perspective of ECE, emphasis on play and on “naturally occurring” learning

and instruction. It is therefore worthwhile to study the differentiation practices and

their potential beneficial effects within the context of rich educational programs in

more detail.

2. Computer supported differentiation practices seem promising. In our description of

these practices we encountered elements such as assessment, using data for diagnosis,

suggesting individual learning routes and indicating the need of supplementary

support, etc. Comprehensive computerized programs may thus support teachers in

implementing differentiation. Further research on how these programs influence

teaching practices will help to understand how to use software as an effective teaching

tool.

3. The most promising route for differentiation seems to be to embed it in a broader

structure, either within a computerized system or a comprehensive educational

program, which includes, for instance, meta-cognitive learning strategies, cooperative

learning, regular assessment, remedial instruction, and flexible grouping. Studying the

Page 56: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Conclusion and discussion

53

effect of differentiation within such a broader structure is complicated, since all

elements intertwine. Nevertheless, it seems important to further study the effects of

differentiation when it is combined with other support systems, in order to determine

how differentiation practices can be embedded within the classroom and the school.

Page 57: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

54

Page 58: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

55

References

* Adelson, J. L., & Carpenter, B. D. (2011). Grouping for achievement gains: for whom does

achievement grouping increase kindergarten reading growth? Gifted Child Quarterly,

55(4), 265-278.

Anderson, K. M., & Algozzine, B. (2007). Tips for teaching: Differentiating instruction to

include all students. Preventing School Failure, 51(3), 49-54.

* Barrow, L., Markman, L., & Rouse, C. E. (2009). Technology's edge: The educational

benefits of computer-aided instruction. American Economic Journal-Economic Policy,

1(1), 52-74.

Blok, H. (2004). Adaptief onderwijs: betekenis en effectiviteit. Pedagogische Studiën, 81(1),

5-27.

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to

meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.

Borman, G. D., Slavin, R. E., Cheung, A. C. K., Chamberlain, A. M., Madden, N. A., & Cha,

b., B. (2005). The national randomized field trial of Success for All: second-year

outcomes. American Educational Research Journal, 42(4), 673-696.

* Borman, G. D., Slavin, R. E., Cheung, A. C. K., Chamberlain, A. M., Madden, N. A., &

Chambers, B. (2007). Final reading outcomes of the national randomized field trial of

success for all. American Educational Research Journal, 44(3), 701-731.

Bosker, R. J. (2005). De grenzen van gedifferentieerd onderwijs (inaugurele rede).

Groningen: Rijksuniversiteit Groningen.

* Burris, C. C., Wiley, E., Welner, K. G., & Murphy, J. (2008). Accountability, rigor, and

detracking: Achievement effects of embracing a challenging curriculum as a universal

good for all students. Teachers College Record, 110(3), 571-607.

* Burris, C. C., Heubert, J. P., & Levin, H. M. (2006). Accelerating mathematics achievement

using heterogeneous grouping. American Educational Research Journal, 43(1), 105-136.

* References with an * are included in the meta-analysis.

Page 59: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

56

Chambers, B., Cheung, A., Slavin, R. E., Smith, D., & Laurenzano, M. (2010). Effective early

childhood education programs: a systematic review. Downloaded from:

www.bestevidence.org: Best Evidence Encyclopedia.

* Chang, M. (2008). Teacher instructional practices and language minority students: A

longitudinal model. Journal of Educational Research, 102(2-), 83-97.

Chorzempa, B. F., & Graham, S. (2006). Primary-grade teachers' use of within-class ability

grouping in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 529-541.

* Condron, D. J. (2008). An early start: Skill grouping and unequal reading gains in the

elementary years. Sociological Quarterly, 49(2), 363-394.

* Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B. J., Schatschneider, C., & Underwood, P.

(2007). Algorithm-guided individualized reading instruction. Science, 315(5811), 464-

465.

* Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Schatschneider, C., Toste, J. R., Lundblom, E., Crowe, E.

C., & Fishman, B. (2011a). Effective classroom instruction: Implications of child

characteristics by reading instruction interactions on first graders' word reading

achievement. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 4(3), 173-207.

* Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., Fishman, B., Giuliani, S., Luck, M., Underwood, P. S., . . .

Schatschneider, C. (2011b). Testing the impact of child characteristics x instruction

interactions on third graders' reading comprehension by differentiating literacy

instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(3), 189-221.

De Koning, P. (1973). Interne differentiatie. Amsterdam: APS/RITP.

Gamoran, A., & Weinstein, M. (1998). Differentiation and opportunity in restructured

schools. American Journal of Education, 106(3), 385-415.

Gardner, H. (1984). Frames of mind: the theory of multiple intelligences. London:

Heinemann.

George, P. S. (2005). A rationale for differentiating instruction in the regular classroom.

Theory into Practice, 44(3), 185-193.

Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. (2007). Applying a response-to-intervention model for early

literacy development in low-income children. Topics in Early Childhood Special

Education, 27(4), 198-213.

Page 60: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

References

57

* Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. C. (2012). Curriculum-based early literacy assessment and

differentiated instruction with high-risk preschoolers. Reading Psychology, 33(1-2), 11-

46.

Hallam, S., Ireson, J., Lister, V., Chaudhury, I. A., & Davies, J. (2003). Ability grouping

practices in the primary school: A survey. Educational Studies, 29(1), 69-83.

Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2006). Does educational tracking affect performance and

inequality? Differences-in-differences evidence across countries. The Economic Journal,

116(510), 63-76.

* Hong, G., Corter, C., Hong, Y., & Pelletier, J. (2012). Differential effects of literacy

instruction time and homogeneous ability grouping in kindergarten classrooms: Who will

benefit? Who will suffer? Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 34(1), 69-88.

* Hong, G., & Hong, Y. (2009). Reading instruction time and homogeneous grouping in

kindergarten: an application of marginal mean weighting through stratification.

Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 31(1), 54-81.

Houtveen, T., & van de Grift, W. (2012). Improving reading achievements of struggling

learners. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 23(1), 71-93.

Inspectorate of Education. (2013). De staat van het onderwijs. Onderwijsverslag 2011/2012.

Utrecht: Inspectie van het Onderwijs.

Ireson, J., & Hallam, S. (2001). Ability grouping in education. London: Paul Chapman

Publishing.

Ireson, J., Hallam, S., & Plewis, I. (2001). Ability grouping in secondary schools: Effects on

pupils' self-concepts. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(2), 315-326.

Jakubowski, M., Patrinos, H. A., Porta, E. E., & Wisniewski, J. (2010). The impact of the

1999 education reform in Poland. World Bank: Policy Research Working paper 5263.

Kulik, C. C., & Kulik, J. A. (1982). Effects of ability grouping on secondary school students:

a meta-analysis of evaluation findings. American Educational Research Journal, 19(3),

415-428.

Kulik, C. C., & Kulik, J. A. (1984). Effects of ability grouping on elementary school pupils: a

meta-analysis (Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological

Association ed.)

Page 61: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

58

* Leonard, J. (2001). How group composition influenced the achievement of sixth-grade

mathematics students. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 3(2-3), 175-200.

* Linchevski, L., & Kutscher, B. (1998). Tell me with whom you're learning, and I'll tell you

how much you've learned: Mixed-ability versus same-ability grouping in mathematics.

Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(5), 533-554.

Lou, Y., Abrami, P. C., Spence, J. C., Poulsen, C., Chambers, B., & d'Appolonia, S. (1996).

Within-class grouping: a meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 66(4), 423-458.

Luyten, H. (2008). Empirische evidentie voor effecten van vroegtijdige selectie in het

onderwijs, literatuurstudie in opdracht van het Ministerie van OCW. Enschede:

Universiteit Twente.

* Macqueen, S. (2012). Academic outcomes from between-class achievement grouping: the

Australian primary context. Australian Educational Researcher, 39(1), 59-73.

* McCoach, D. B., O'Connell, A. A., & Levitt, H. (2006). Ability grouping across

kindergarten using an early childhood longitudinal study. Journal of Educational

Research, 99(6), 339-346.

McCoach, D. E. (2003). Does grouping matter? A cross-classified random effects model of

children's reading growth during the first two years of school. ProQuest, Dissertation

Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 64(5). (2003-95021-

019).

Mevarech, Z. R., & Kramarski, B. (1997). IMPROVE: A multidimensional method for

teaching mathematics in heterogeneous classrooms. American Educational Research

Journal, 34, 365-394.

Neel, J. L. (2008). The effects of differentiated developmentally appropriate instruction of

first grade learners. ProQuest, Dissertation Abstracts International Section A:

Humanities and Social Sciences, 68(7). (2008-99011-064).

* Nomi, T. (2010). The effects of within-class ability grouping on academic achievement in

early elementary years. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 3(1), 56-92.

Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R. T., & du Toit, M. (2011). HLM7

- Hierarchical Linear and Nonlinear Modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software

International.

Reezigt, G. J. (1993). Effecten van differentiatie op de basisschool. Groningen: RION.

Page 62: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

References

59

* Reis, S. M., McCoach, D. B., Coyne, M., Schreiber, F. J., Eckert, R. D., & Gubbins, E. J.

(2007). Using planned enrichment strategies with direct instruction to improve reading

fluency, comprehension, and attitude toward reading: An evidence-based study.

Elementary School Journal, 108(1), 3-24.

* Reis, S. M., McCoach, D. B., Little, C. A., Muller, L. M., & Kaniskan, R. B. (2011). The

effects of differentiated instruction and enrichment pedagogy on reading achievement in

five elementary schools. American Educational Research Journal, 48(2), 462-501.

Slavin, R. E., Lake, C., Chambers, B., Cheung, A., & Davis, S. (2009). Effective beginning

reading programs: A best-evidence synthesis. Baltimore: Best Evidence Encyclopedia.

Slavin, R. E., Lake, C., & Groff, C. (2009). Effective programs in middle and high school

mathematics: A best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 78, 427-515.

Slavin, R. E. (1987a). Ability grouping and student achievement in elementary schools: A

best-evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 57(3), 293-336.

Slavin, R. E. (1987b). Mastery learning reconsidered. Review of Educational Research, 57(2),

175-213.

Slavin, R. E. (1990). Achievement effects of ability grouping in secondary schools: A best-

evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 60(3), 471-499.

Slavin, R. E., & Lake, C. (2008). Effective programs in elementary mathematics: A best-

evidence synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 78(3), 427-515.

* Stevens, R. J., & Slavin, R. E. (1995). The cooperative elementary-school - effects on

students achievement, attitudes, and social-relations. American Educational Research

Journal, 32(2), 321-351.

* Tach, L. M., & Farkas, G. (2006). Learning-related behaviors, cognitive skills, and ability

grouping when schooling begins. Social Science Research, 35(4), 1048-1079.

Tomlinson, C. A. (2000). Differentiation of instruction in the elementary grades. Champaign,

IL: ERIC Digest.

* Whitburn, J. (2001). Effective classroom organisation in primary schools: mathematics.

Oxford Review of Education, 27(3), 411-428.

* Ysseldyke, J., & Bolt, D. M. (2007). Effect of technology-enhanced continuous progress

monitoring on math achievement. School Psychology Review, 36(3), 453-467.

Page 63: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

Differentiation within and across classrooms

60

* Ysseldyke, J., Spicuzza, R., Kosciolek, S., Teelucksingh, E., Boys, C., & Lemkuil, A.

(2003). Using a curriculum-based instructional management system to enhance math

achievement in urban schools. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk, 8(2),

247-265.

Page 64: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

61

Appendix 1: Included studies ECE and Kindergarten

Article Type of

differentiation

Location Sample size Duration Grouping

criteria

Design Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence

interval

Adelson &

Carpenter,

2011

homogeneous

ability

grouping for

reading

USA

(ECLS-K)

580

schools,

1690

classrooms,

9340

students

fall-

spring

K2

achievement Relationship achievement

grouping for reading

(yes/no, as indicated by

teacher) and reading growth

+0.068* +0.028; +0.109

Chang, 2008 grouping*acti

vity for math

USA

(ECLS-K)

5863

Caucasian

English

only

speaking

students;

1151

African-

American

English

only

speaking

students

spring

K2, with

follow

ups to

spring

grade 5

achievement

and interest

Relationship time spent on

different classroom practices

(as indicated by teacher on a

5-point scale ranging from 0

to 3+ hours a day) and math

growth

Caucasian

whole cl. +0.152*

small gr. -0.045*

indiv. +0.008*

child sel. +0.012*

Afr.-Am.

whole cl. +0.134*

small gr. +0.002

indiv. -0.069*

child sel. +0.020*

+0.151; +0.153

-0.047; -0.044

+0.007; +0.009

+0.011; +0.013

+0.128; +0.141

-0.005; +0.008

-0.076; -0.063

+0.013; +0.027

Gettinger &

Stoiber, 2012

progress

monitoring

and adjusted

instruction for

reading

USA,

large

urban

metropolis

in the

Midwest

15

classrooms,

124

students

4 months achievement classrooms randomly

assigned to intervention

condition with close

monitoring/ formative

assessment and adapted

instruction for low, general,

and high performing

students.

overall

V +0.837*

R1 +0.388*

R2 +0.574*

R3 +0.911*

R/RC +0.572*

High ability

V +0.243

R1 +0.474

R2 +0.468

+0.470; +1.204

+0.033; +0.743

+0.215; +0.933

+0.542; +1,281

+0.213; +0.931

-0.357; +0.843

-0.133; +1.080

-0.138; +1.074

Page 65: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

62

V=vocabulary

R1=rhyme awareness and

alphabet knowledge

R2=print knowledge and

phonological awareness

R3=upper case letter naming

R=reading

RC=reading comprehension

R3 +0.675*

R/RC +0.696*

Average ability

V +0.465

R1 +0.500

R2 +0.845*

R3 +1.276*

R/RC +0.999*

Low ability

V +0.337

R1 +0.594

R2 +0.500

R3 +1.015*

R/RC +0.876*

+0.060; +1.289

+0.081; +1.312

-0.121; +1.050

-0.087; +1.087

+0.241; +1.448

+0.642; +1.910

+0.386; +1.612

-0.331; +1.004

-0.083; +1.271

-0.173; +1.173

+0.311; +1.719

+0.182; +1.570

Hong &

Hong, 2009

homogeneous

ability

grouping for

reading

USA

(ECLS-K)

740

schools,

1858

classrooms,

10189

students

fall-

spring

achievement Relationship between

instruction time (high or

low) * grouping (G - high,

low or no) and reading

growth.

nb no grouping = whole

class

Grouping under

low instr. time

low G +0.036

high G -0.040

Grouping under

high instr. time

low G +0.164*

high G +0.198*

-0.094; +0.165

-0.173; +0.094

+0.047; +0.281

+0.051; +0.346

Hong et al.,

2012

homogeneous

ability

grouping for

reading

USA

(ECLS-K)

665

schools,

1697

classrooms,

8668

students

fall-

spring

achievement Relationship between

instruction time (high or

low) * grouping (G high,

low or no) and reading

growth for 3 groups of

students (high, medium, low

ability)

R1=letter recognition

R2= beginning sounds

R3=ending sounds

Whole class vs

intensive

grouping under

low instr. time

high ability

R1 -0.064

R2 +0.083

R3 +0.088

R4 +0.184

RC +0.142

-0.281; +0.152

-0.134; +0.299

-0.128; +0.305

-0.033; +0.401

-0.074; +0.359

Page 66: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

63

R4=sight words

RC=reading comprehension

Average ability

R1 +0.031

R2 +0.048

R3 +0.038

R4 +0.072

RC +0.023

Low ability

R1 +0.236*

R2 +0.181*

R3 +0.220*

R4 +0.325*

RC +0.328*

High vs low

instruction time

under intensive

grouping

High ability

R1 +0.073

R2 +0.267*

R3 +0.175

R4 +0.284*

RC +0.255

Average ability

R1 +0.158*

R2 +0.145*

R3 +0.152*

R4 +0.174*

RC +0.118

Low ability

R1 +0.236*

R2 +0.170

R3 +0.234*

R4 +0.268*

RC +0.208*

-0.070; +0.131

-0.052; +0.148

-0.062; +0.139

-0.029; +0.127

-0.077; +0.124

+0.070; +0.402

+0.015; +0.346

+0.054; +0.386

+0.159; +0.491

+0.162; +0.494

-0.181; +0.327

+0.011; +0.522

-0.079; +0.430

+0.029; +0.539

0.000; +0.510

+0.040; +0.277

+0.027; +0.263

+0.034; +0.270

+0.055 - +0.292

0.000; +0.236

+0.045; +0.427

-0.020; +0.360

+0.043; +0.424

+0.077; +0.459

+0.018; +0.398

Page 67: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

64

D.B.

McCoach et

al., 2006

homogeneous

ability

grouping for

reading

USA

(ECLS-K)

620

schools,

10191

students

fall-

spring

achievement Relationship between

frequency of ability

grouping per week (as

indicated by teacher on a 5

point scale ranging from

never to daily) and reading

growth

+0.127* +0.068; +0.186

Tach &

Farkas, 2006

Homogeneous

ability

grouping for

reading

USA

(ECLS-K)

Kindergarte

n sample:

2420

classrooms,

11769

students

fall-

spring K

achievement Multi-level analysis studying

the relationship between

ability grouping in

Kindergarten and reading

achievement at the end of

the school year

+0.346* +0.265; +0.427

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Page 68: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

65

Appendix 2: Included studies Primary Education

Appendix 2a: An intervention study on ability grouping

Article Type of

differentiation

Location Sample size Duration Grouping

criteria

Design Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence

interval

Leonard, J.,

2001

Within-class

heterogeneous

small groups

versus within-

class

homogeneous

small groups

USA 177

students

from 3

classes:

88 students

heterogeneo

us cohort

(16 low, 34

average, 43

high); 89

students

homogeneo

us cohort

(37 low, 29

average, 28

high)

One

school

year (fall

– spring)

Achievement Comparison of students’

mathematics achievement

in the homogeneously

grouped cohort versus the

heterogeneously grouped

cohort

Overall

-0.250

Low ability

-0.397

Average ability

-0.133

High ability

-0.185

-0.546; +0.046

-1.006; +0.213

-0.644; +0.379

-0.675; +0.305

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Page 69: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

66

Appendix 2b: Ability grouping studies

Article Type of

differentiation

Location Sample size Duration Grouping

criteria

Design Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence

interval

Condron,

2008

Within-class

ability

grouping

USA K – 1:

13,625

students

(ungrouped:

4718

students,

low group:

2219,

average

group:

3380, high

group:

3308)

Grade 1 –

3:

13.010

students

(ungrouped:

6873, low

group:

1436,

middle

group:

2067, high

group:

2634)

Growth

from

kindergar

ten to the

end of

grade

one and

from

grade

one to

the end

of grade

three

Achievement Propensity score matching is

used to estimate the effect of

placement in a high, average

or low ability group in

comparison to non-grouped

instruction.

We report the general effects

cumulated over the various

strata

K – grade 1

Low ability

-0.288*

Average ability

-0.043

High ability

+0.207*

Grade 1 - 3

Low ability

-0.245*

Average ability

+0.046

High ability

+0.177*

-0.343; -0.233

-0.088; +0.002

+0.158; +0.256

-0.305; -0.185

-0.005; +0.097

+0.129; +0.225

Macqueen,

2012

Between-class

ability

grouping

Australia 8 schools.

Literacy:

regrouping

Growth

from

grade

Achievement Comparison of growth

scores of students in

between-class ability

Overall Literacy

+0.196

-0.170; +0.561

Page 70: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

67

(setting) 50 students,

heterogeneo

us 68

students

Writing:

regrouping

29 students,

heterogeneo

us 47

students

Math:

regrouping

51 students,

heterogeneo

us 69

students

three and

five

grouped classes versus

students in heterogeneous

classes in the areas of

literacy, writing and

mathematics.

Low lit group: Low level

literacy group versus

heterogeneous

Average lit group: Average

level literacy group versus

heterogeneous

High lit group: High level

literacy group versus

heterogeneous

Low math group: Low level

math group versus

heterogeneous

Average math group:

Average level math group

versus heterogeneous

High math group: High level

math group versus

heterogeneous

Overall Writing

-0.082

Overall Math

-0.125

Low lit group:

Literacy

-0.379

Average lit

group: Literacy

+0.275

High lit group:

Literacy

+0.218

Low lit group:

Writing

+0.038

Average lit

group: Writing

-0.023

High lit group:

Writing

+0.196

Low math

group: Math

-0.776

Average math

group: Math

-0.061

High math

group: Math

+0.171

-0.545; +0.381

-0.488; +0.237

-1.290; +0.532

-0.286; +0.836

-0.243; +0.678

-1.130; +1.206

-0.738; +0.691

-0.463; +0.855

-1.620; +0.067

-0.605; +0.483

-0.294; +0.636

Page 71: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

68

Nomi, 2010 Within-class

ability

grouping

USA 13512

schools

with 13512

students:

3922

schools

with 2043

students

ungrouped,

9590

schools

with 6742

students

ability-

grouped ;

Achieve

ment

from

kindergar

ten to the

end of

first

grade

Achievement Propensity score matching is

used to estimate the effect

on reading scores of

placement in a high, average

or low ability group in

comparison to a non-

grouped classroom.

Overall

-0.010

Low ability

-0.030

Average ability

0.021

High ability

-0.059

-0.060; +0.039

-0.126; +0.066

-0.063; +0.105

-0.141; +0.023

Tach &

Farkas, 2006

Within-class

ability

grouping

USA First grade

sample:

3133

classes with

13.010

students

(ability

grouped

classes:

2256)

Achieve

ment

from

kindergar

ten to the

end of

first

grade

The authors

analyze which

variables

affect

teachers’

grouping

practices.

Students’ prior

achievement

has the

strongest

effect on

grouping.

Also, grouping

effects were

found for

students’

learning

Multilevel analyses are used

to determine the effect of

having ability groups present

in the classroom on students’

reading performance

-0.191*

-0.261; -0.120

Page 72: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

69

behavior, SES,

age, and

classroom-

level variables

related to

average

performance,

ethnicity, SES

and age.

Whitburn,

2001

Between-class

ability

grouping

(setting)

United

Kingdom

1200

students

(200

students in

homogeneo

us

classrooms

and 1000 in

heterogeneo

us

classrooms)

Cohort 1:

21

months

Cohort 2:

15

months

Cohort 3:

3 months

Achievement Comparison of mathematics

performance between

students taught in

homogeneous (set) classes

and students in mixed

ability classes.

First Cohort

Total grade 3

-0.030

Total grade 4

-0.270*

Low ability

grade 3

+0.040

Low ability

grade 4

-0.340

Average ability

grade 3

-0.670*

Average ability

grade 4

-0.690*

High ability

grade 3

+0.080

High ability

grade 4

-0.090

-0.292; +0.232

-0.533; -0.007

-0.353; +0.433

-0.735; +0.055

-1.071; -0.269

-1.091; -0.289

-0.313; +0.473

-0.483; +0.303

Page 73: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

70

Second Cohort

Total grade 3

-0.030

Total grade 4

-0.130

Low ability

grade 3

+0.060

Low ability

grade 4

-0.060

Average ability

grade 3

-0.210

Average ability

grade 4

-0.340

High ability

grade 3

-0.070

High ability

grade 4

-0.630*

Third Cohort

Total grade 3

-0.110

Total grade 4

-0.290*

Low ability

grade 3

-0.450*

Low ability

-0.292; +0.232

-0.393; +0.133

-0.333; +0.453

-0.453; +0.333

-0.604; +0.184

-0.735; +0.055

-0.463; +0.323

-1.030; -0.230

-0.373; +0.153

-0.553; -0.027

-0.847; -0.053

Page 74: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

71

grade 4

-0.480*

Average ability

grade 3

-0.450*

Average ability

grade 4

-0.480*

High ability

grade 3

-0.040

High ability

grade 4

-0.480*

-0.877; -0.083

-0.847; -0.053

-0.877; -0.083

-0.433; +0.353

-0.877; -0.083

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Page 75: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

72

Appendix 2c: Studies on computerized systems

Article Type of

differentiation

Location Sample size Duration Grouping

criteria

Design Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence

interval

Connor et

al., 2007

Within-class

differentiated

instruction

USA 10 schools,

47 classes

(treatment

22 classes,

control 25

classes),

616 students

fall-

spring

performan

ce

A cluster-randomized field

trail is used in which

effects of differentiated

instruction using the

computer program are

compared to students

results in matched control

schools on a language and

literacy outcome measure.

+0.183* +0.025; +0.342

Connor et

al., 2011a

Within-class

differentiated

instruction

USA 7 schools, 33

classes (16

treatment, 17

control), 464

students

(experimenta

l group: 219

students,

control

group: 229

students)

fall-

spring

performan

ce

Multilevel modeling is

used to analyze the effects

of differentiated

instruction using the

computer program in

comparison to a

vocabulary instruction

intervention on reading

comprehension an

vocabulary outcome

measures.

Reading

comprehension

+0.191*

Vocabulary

+0.033

+0.005; +0.377

-0.153; +0.219

Connor et

al., 2011b

Within-class

differentiated

instruction

USA 7 schools, 25

classes, 396

students ((16

treatment, 17

control), 464

students

(experimenta

l group: 3

fall-

spring

performan

ce

Multilevel modeling is

used to analyze the effects

of differentiated

instruction using the

computer program in

comparison to a control

group on a language and

literacy outcome measure.

+0.249* +0.050; +0.448

Page 76: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

73

schools, 14

classes, 222

students;

control

group: 4

schools, 11

classes, 174

students ))

Ysseldyke et

al., 2003

Within-class

differentiated

instruction

USA Experimenta

l group: 18

classes, 397

students.

Within-

school

control

group: 484

students

Septembe

r - June

performan

ce

An analysis of variance of

the mean scores on two

mathematics tests (NALT

and STAR Math) of the

experimental and the

control group

NALT

+0.189*

STAR Math

+0.268*

+0.030; +0.349

+0.109; +0.428

Ysseldyke et

al., 2007

Within-class

differentiated

instruction

USA Experimenta

l condition:

8 schools, 41

classrooms;

Control

condition: 8

schools, 39

classrooms

October -

May

performan

ce

An analysis of variance of

the mean scores on two

mathematics tests (NALT

and STAR Math) of the

experimental and the

control group in primary

education

Terra Nova

+0.469*

STAR Math

+0.458*

+0.312; +0.626

+0.294; +0.622

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Page 77: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

74

Appendix 2d: Studies on differentiation as part of a broader program

Article Type of differentiation

Location Sample size Duration Grouping

criteria

Design Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence

interval

Borman, et

al., 2007

Ability grouping

across grades for

reading, as a part of a

whole school

comprehensive reform

USA 35 schools: 1445

students in Grade

2 (longitudinal

sample of students

that started in K)

3 years,

from

kindergart

en to

second

grade

Achieveme

nt,

measured

every 9

weeks

Cluster

randomiz

ed design

Word Identification:

+0.220*2

Word Attack:

+0.330*2

Passage Comprehension:

+0.210*1

+0.024; +0.416

+0.114; +0.546

+0.034; +0.386

Houtveen

& van de

Grift, 2012

Direct instruction in

heterogeneous group,

and intensive small

group instruction,

aimed at convergent

differentiation

The

Netherla

nds

37 schools; 21

treatment schools,

16 control

schools, 1021

students

December

-May

Achieveme

nt

Quasi-

experime

ntal

Word Decoding:

+0.280*2

Fluency: +0.620*2

+0.156; +0.404

+0.494; +0.746

Stevens &

Slavin,

1995

Students work in

heterogeneous learning

teams but receive

instruction in relatively

homogeneous teaching

groups, as part of a

whole school reform

program

USA 5 schools: 2

treatment schools,

3 control schools,

grade 2 – 6

After 1

and 2

years

Achieveme

nt

Quasi-

experime

ntal

After 1 year:

Read voc: +0.170*

Read comp: +0.130

Lang mech: -0.010

Lang expr: +0.080

Math comp: +0.120

Math appl: -0.050

After 2 years:

Read voc: +0.210*

Read comp: +0.280*1

Lang mech: +0.100

Lang Expr: +0.210*

Math comp: +0.290

Math appl: +0.100

+0.014; +0.326

-0.026; +0.286

-0.164; +0.144

-0.074; +0.234

-0.056; +0.296

-0.204; +0.104

+0.075; +0.345

+0.128; +0.432

-0.069; +0.269

+0.069; +0.351

+0.139; +0.441

-0.058; +0.258

Reis et al.,

2007

SEM-R (School-wide

Enrichment Model in

Reading Framework):

differentiated,

USA 2 schools, 14 (7

treatment, 7

control) teachers,

226 students,

12 weeks Teacher’s

judgment

Randomiz

ed design

Fluency: +0.299*2

Comprehension: +0.2201

+0.005; +0.594

-0.529; +0.970

Page 78: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

75

individual reading

instruction among

other things (all

students participate in

SfA in the morning)

grade 3 – 6,

teachers and

students were

randomly

assigned to

treatment or

control group

Reis et al.,

2011

SEM-R (School-wide

Enrichment Model in

Reading Framework):

differentiated,

individual reading

instruction among

other things

USA 5 schools, 63

teachers, 1192

students (grade 2,

3, 4, 5),

teachers/classes

were randomly

assigned to

control or

treatment groups

24 weeks Teacher’s

judgment

Cluster-

randomiz

ed design

Fluency: +0.2542

Comprehension: +0.1451

-0.063; +0.571

-0.096; +0.386

1) Effects included in the meta-analysis of comprehensive reading

2) Effects included in the meta-analysis of basic reading, correction is made for including two measures from 1 study by multiplying the standard error with √2.

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0

Page 79: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

76

Appendix 3: Included studies Early Secondary Education

Article Type of

differentiation

Location Sample

size

Duration Grouping

criteria

Design Effect sizes (d) 95% confidence

interval

Barrow et al.,

2009

computer aided

individualized

instruction

USA, 3

large

urban

districts

in

Northeas

t,

Midwest

and

South

1605

students,

59

teachers,

146

classrooms

, 17

schools

1 school-

year

n/a random

assignment

Within school random

assignment of classrooms

+0.416* +0.261 - +0.571

Burris et al.,

2006

heterogeneous

ability grouping

with advanced

courses for all and

remediation if

necessary

USA,

suburban

area

6 cohorts,

3 before

(477

students)

and 3 after

(508

students)

accelerated

math

curriculum

for all

2 years Heterogeneou

s grouping; all

students

included

(achievement)

Quasi-experimental

longitudinal cohort study

M1=sequential maths

M2=calculus

M3=advanced place calc

M1: +1.450*

M2: +1.511*

M3: +1.097

+0.062 - +2.838

+0.204 - +2.817

-0.171 - +0.2365

Burris et al.,

2008

heterogeneous

ability grouping

with advanced

courses for all and

remediation if

necessary

USA 6 cohorts,

1300

students

2 years Heterogeneou

s grouping; all

students

included

(achievement)

Quasi-experimental cohort

study

state=diploma tied to state-

wide standards

int.= diploma tied to

international standards

state: +3.187*

int.: +0.965

+1.700; +4.673

-0.302; +2.232

Linchevski &

Kutscher,

Effect of mixed

ability grouping

Israel 1629

students,

1 school-

year (7th

Achievement regression-discontinuity

design. Study 1: analysis

gr 7: +0.112*

gr.8: +0.164

+0.018; +0.207

-0.037; +0.364

Page 80: Differentiation within and across classrooms: A systematic review of

77

1998 on math 12 schools,

40

classrooms

/groups

grade) on school level. For 4

schools (12 groups)

retention effects at the end

of 8th

grade.

* 95% confidence interval of effect size does not contain 0