21
Current Biology 25, 656–662, March 2, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.022 Report Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on Sensory Experience Mark S. Blumberg, 1,2,3, * Cassandra M. Coleman, 1 Greta Sokoloff, 1,3 Joshua A. Weiner, 2,3 Bernd Fritzsch, 2,3 and Bob McMurray 1,3,4 1 Department of Psychology, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA 2 Department of Biology, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA 3 Delta Center, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA 4 Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA Summary Myoclonic twitches are jerky movements that occur exclu- sively and abundantly during active (or REM) sleep in mam- mals, especially in early development [1–4]. In rat pups, limb twitches exhibit a complex spatiotemporal structure that changes across early development [5]. However, it is not known whether this developmental change is influenced by sensory experience, which is a prerequisite to the notion that sensory feedback from twitches not only activates sensorimotor circuits but modifies them [4]. Here, we inves- tigated the contributions of proprioception to twitching in newborn ErbB2 conditional knockout mice that lack muscle spindles and grow up to exhibit dysfunctional proprio- ception [6–8]. High-speed videography of forelimb twitches unexpectedly revealed a category of reflex-like twitching— comprising an agonist twitch followed immediately by an antagonist twitch—that developed postnatally in wild- types/heterozygotes, but not in knockouts. Contrary to evi- dence from adults that spinal reflexes are inhibited during twitching [9–11], this finding suggests that twitches trigger the monosynaptic stretch reflex and, by doing so, contribute to its activity-dependent development [12–14]. Next, we as- sessed developmental changes in the frequency and organi- zation (i.e., entropy) of more-complex, multi-joint patterns of twitching; again, wild-types/heterozygotes exhibited devel- opmental changes in twitch patterning that were not seen in knockouts. Thus, targeted deletion of a peripheral sensor alters the normal development of local and global features of twitching, demonstrating that twitching is shaped by sen- sory experience. These results also highlight the potential use of twitching as a uniquely informative diagnostic tool for assessing the functional status of spinal and supraspinal circuits. Results and Discussion Contrary to the longstanding view of twitching as an aimless, disjointed, and uncoordinated by-product of a dreaming brain [15], it is in fact a complex behavior with its own developmental dynamics [5]. This new perspective raises the question of whether the development of twitching is shaped by sensory experience, particularly in early infancy when active sleep and twitching are prominently expressed [1, 2, 16]. Muscle- specific ErbB2 knockouts provide a unique opportunity to address this question: few if any muscle spindles are present at birth and they are completely lacking by postnatal day (P) 9[6]. This phenotype arises because normal spindle deve- lopment requires activation of ErbB2 receptors, located on spindles, by neuregulin 1 (Nrg1) released from Ia afferents [6, 7, 17]. Muscle-specific ErbB2 knockouts survive to adult- hood [6], at which time they exhibit abnormal limb extension reflexes and ataxia [6–8]. However, motor outflow is spared in these knockouts, so twitching continues to be expressed. Thus, by comparing twitching in wild-types (WTs) and hetero- zygotes (Hets) with that of knockouts (KOs) across the early postnatal period, the developmental contributions of muscle spindles to twitching can be revealed. To confirm the phenotypes of WTs, Hets, and KOs, we analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see the Supplemental Information). As expected [6], whereas spin- dles were readily observed in the WTs and Hets, none were observed in the KOs. Next, we used high-speed videography and 3D motion tracking to assess twitching in P4 and P10 mice. At P4, 9 WTs, 11 Hets, and 7 KOs yielded 37–54 video segments containing 4,020–6,985 twitches per genotype. At P10, seven WTs, eight Hets, and eight KOs yielded 40–47 video segments containing 1,824–2,176 twitches per genotype. The analyses below focus exclusively on shoulder (adduc- tion and abduction) and elbow (flexion and extension) twitches in the left and right forelimbs. We first perform pairwise ana- lyses of twitch movements, followed by an analysis of more complex patterns of multi-joint twitching. Absence of Muscle Spindles Differentially Affects Agonist-Antagonist Twitches We first assessed genotype- and age-related differences in the organization of twitching by computing inter-twitch intervals (ITIs) and plotting their frequency distributions. After initial re- view of the data, we focused our attention on ITIs for four cat- egories of twitch pairs. Each pair was composed of an initial ‘‘agonist’’ twitch and (1) a succeeding twitch that occurred within the same joint but in the opposite direction (‘‘agonist- antagonist twitches’’; e.g., right elbow flexion / right elbow extension; Movie S1), (2) a succeeding twitch that was a repeat of the first (‘‘agonist-agonist twitches’’; e.g., right elbow flexion / right elbow flexion), (3) a succeeding twitch that occurred within the same limb but at the other joint (e.g., right elbow flexion / right shoulder abduction), and (4) a succeeding twitch that occurred in the other limb in a homologous fashion (e.g., right elbow flexion / left elbow flexion). The ITI distribu- tions for the last two twitch pairs exhibited negligible geno- typic differences at P4 and P10 and so are not discussed further (Figure S1). For agonist-antagonist twitches, the three genotypes segre- gate substantially between P4 and P10, with the P10 WTs and Hets—but not the KOs—exhibiting a pronounced develop- mental increase in frequency at short ITIs (Figure 1A). These differences were assessed statistically by performing two-fac- tor (age and genotype) between-subject ANOVA on mean twitch latencies computed over a 150-ms window. There was *Correspondence: [email protected]

Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Current Biology 25, 656–662, March 2, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.022

ReportDevelopment of Twitchingin Sleeping Infant MiceDepends on Sensory Experience

Mark S. Blumberg,1,2,3,* Cassandra M. Coleman,1

Greta Sokoloff,1,3 Joshua A. Weiner,2,3 Bernd Fritzsch,2,3

and Bob McMurray1,3,41Department of Psychology, The University of Iowa, Iowa City,IA 52242, USA2Department of Biology, The University of Iowa, Iowa City,IA 52242, USA3Delta Center, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA4Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, TheUniversity of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA

Summary

Myoclonic twitches are jerky movements that occur exclu-sively and abundantly during active (or REM) sleep in mam-mals, especially in early development [1–4]. In rat pups, limbtwitches exhibit a complex spatiotemporal structure thatchanges across early development [5]. However, it is notknown whether this developmental change is influencedby sensory experience, which is a prerequisite to the notionthat sensory feedback from twitches not only activatessensorimotor circuits but modifies them [4]. Here, we inves-tigated the contributions of proprioception to twitching innewborn ErbB2 conditional knockout mice that lack musclespindles and grow up to exhibit dysfunctional proprio-ception [6–8]. High-speed videography of forelimb twitchesunexpectedly revealed a category of reflex-like twitching—comprising an agonist twitch followed immediately by anantagonist twitch—that developed postnatally in wild-types/heterozygotes, but not in knockouts. Contrary to evi-dence from adults that spinal reflexes are inhibited duringtwitching [9–11], this finding suggests that twitches triggerthemonosynaptic stretch reflex and, by doing so, contributeto its activity-dependent development [12–14]. Next, we as-sessed developmental changes in the frequency and organi-zation (i.e., entropy) of more-complex, multi-joint patterns oftwitching; again, wild-types/heterozygotes exhibited devel-opmental changes in twitch patterning that were not seenin knockouts. Thus, targeted deletion of a peripheral sensoralters the normal development of local and global features oftwitching, demonstrating that twitching is shaped by sen-sory experience. These results also highlight the potentialuse of twitching as a uniquely informative diagnostic toolfor assessing the functional status of spinal and supraspinalcircuits.

Results and Discussion

Contrary to the longstanding view of twitching as an aimless,disjointed, and uncoordinated by-product of a dreaming brain[15], it is in fact a complex behavior with its own developmentaldynamics [5]. This new perspective raises the question ofwhether the development of twitching is shaped by sensoryexperience, particularly in early infancy when active sleep

and twitching are prominently expressed [1, 2, 16]. Muscle-specific ErbB2 knockouts provide a unique opportunity toaddress this question: few if any muscle spindles are presentat birth and they are completely lacking by postnatal day (P)9 [6]. This phenotype arises because normal spindle deve-lopment requires activation of ErbB2 receptors, located onspindles, by neuregulin 1 (Nrg1) released from Ia afferents[6, 7, 17]. Muscle-specific ErbB2 knockouts survive to adult-hood [6], at which time they exhibit abnormal limb extensionreflexes and ataxia [6–8]. However, motor outflow is sparedin these knockouts, so twitching continues to be expressed.Thus, by comparing twitching in wild-types (WTs) and hetero-zygotes (Hets) with that of knockouts (KOs) across the earlypostnatal period, the developmental contributions of musclespindles to twitching can be revealed.To confirm the phenotypes of WTs, Hets, and KOs, we

analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (seethe Supplemental Information). As expected [6], whereas spin-dles were readily observed in the WTs and Hets, none wereobserved in the KOs. Next, we used high-speed videographyand 3D motion tracking to assess twitching in P4 and P10mice. At P4, 9 WTs, 11 Hets, and 7 KOs yielded 37–54 videosegments containing 4,020–6,985 twitches per genotype. AtP10, sevenWTs, eight Hets, and eight KOs yielded 40–47 videosegments containing 1,824–2,176 twitches per genotype.The analyses below focus exclusively on shoulder (adduc-

tion and abduction) and elbow (flexion and extension) twitchesin the left and right forelimbs. We first perform pairwise ana-lyses of twitch movements, followed by an analysis of morecomplex patterns of multi-joint twitching.

Absence of Muscle Spindles Differentially AffectsAgonist-Antagonist TwitchesWefirst assessed genotype- and age-related differences in theorganization of twitching by computing inter-twitch intervals(ITIs) and plotting their frequency distributions. After initial re-view of the data, we focused our attention on ITIs for four cat-egories of twitch pairs. Each pair was composed of an initial‘‘agonist’’ twitch and (1) a succeeding twitch that occurredwithin the same joint but in the opposite direction (‘‘agonist-antagonist twitches’’; e.g., right elbow flexion / right elbowextension; Movie S1), (2) a succeeding twitch that was a repeatof the first (‘‘agonist-agonist twitches’’; e.g., right elbow flexion/ right elbow flexion), (3) a succeeding twitch that occurredwithin the same limb but at the other joint (e.g., right elbowflexion / right shoulder abduction), and (4) a succeedingtwitch that occurred in the other limb in a homologous fashion(e.g., right elbow flexion/ left elbow flexion). The ITI distribu-tions for the last two twitch pairs exhibited negligible geno-typic differences at P4 and P10 and so are not discussedfurther (Figure S1).For agonist-antagonist twitches, the three genotypes segre-

gate substantially between P4 and P10, with the P10 WTs andHets—but not the KOs—exhibiting a pronounced develop-mental increase in frequency at short ITIs (Figure 1A). Thesedifferences were assessed statistically by performing two-fac-tor (age and genotype) between-subject ANOVA on meantwitch latencies computed over a 150-ms window. There was*Correspondence: [email protected]

Page 2: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

a marked decrease in latency at P10 but only in the WTs andHets (Figure 2A, left). The ANOVA revealed significant main ef-fects of age (F [1,44] = 27.2; p < 0.001) and genotype (F [2,44] =14.2; p < 0.001) and a significant age 3 genotype interaction(F [2,44] = 5.2; p < 0.01). Next, we tested the mean likelihoodthat an antagonist twitch occurred within 50 ms of the agonist(Figure 2A, right). We found genotypic differences at bothages; by P10, agonist-antagonist twitches were 43more likelyin the WTs and Hets than in the KOs. ANOVA revealed a signif-icant main effect of genotype (F [2,44] = 16.6; p < 0.001).

For the second category of twitch pairs—the agonist-agonist twitches—the genotypic differences were small, espe-cially at ITIs less than 50 ms (Figure 1B). KOs exhibited slightlylonger mean latencies than the WTs and Hets at both ages,with latencies in all three genotypes decreasing slightly withage (Figure 2B, left); ANOVA revealed significant main effectsof age (F [1,44] = 29.9; p < 0.001) and genotype (F [2,44] =17.9; p < 0.001). Conversely, the mean likelihood that anagonist-agonist twitch occurred within 50 ms was lower forthe KOs at both ages, with likelihoods in all three genotypesincreasing with age (Figure 2B, right); again, ANOVA revealedsignificant main effects of age (F [1,44] = 21.2; p < 0.001) andgenotype (F [2,44] = 14.6; p < 0.001).

To better visualize differences in agonist-antagonist andagonist-agonist twitching across age and genotype, we con-structed perievent histograms from data pooled across sub-jects (Figure 3). As shown previously [5], strongly coupledtwitch pairs exhibit pronounced peaks on either side of the‘‘trigger’’ twitch at 0 ms (preceding and following peaks occurdepending on whether the trigger twitch is the second or firsttwitch in the pair, respectively). For the agonist-antagonistpairs in the WTs and Hets, such peaks were weak at P4 andpronounced by P10; note the clustering of the peaks within50ms of the trigger and the narrow peri-trigger gaps. However,this developmental progression was not seen in the KOs—peaks were weak or absent at both ages in this genotype. Incontrast, perievent histograms of agonist-agonist twitches ex-hibited weak or absent peaks and wide peri-trigger gaps in allthree genotypes at both ages. The contrast between the two

0

8

16

24

0 50 100 150

Nor

mal

ized

Fre

quen

cy (

%)

0 50 100 150

0

8

16

24

0 50 100 150

Nor

mal

ized

Fre

quen

cy (

%)

0 50 100 150

Inter-Twitch Interval (ms) Inter-Twitch Interval (ms)

Agonist-Antagonist

Agonist-Agonist

4-Day-Olds 10-Day-OldsWild type

Heterozygote

Knockout

A

B

Figure 1. Frequency Distribution of Inter-twitchIntervals for Wild-Type, Heterozygous, andKnockout Mice at P4 and P10

Inter-twitch intervals were restricted to one oftwo movement categories. For each category,the first twitch of a pair could occur at any joint,whereas the second immediately succeedingtwitch was (A) an antagonist twitch (e.g., rightelbow flexion / right elbow extension) or (B) arepeat of the first twitch (e.g., right elbow flexion/ right elbow flexion). Frequencies were normal-ized over the 150-mswindow. P4: n = 3,995–6,931ITIs; P10: n = 1,784–2,131 inter-twitch intervals.See also Figures S1 and S2.

types of twitch pairs is striking because,at the completion of each initial agonisttwitch, it is theoretically equiprobablethat the second twitch will be an antag-onist or an agonist. That they are not,in fact, equiprobable leads us to con-clude that the early postnatal develop-ment of agonist-antagonist twitchesuniquely depends upon the presenceof muscle spindles.

In trying to account for the spatiotemporal structure oftwitching in newborn rats in a previous report [5], we dis-counted the possibility that sensory feedback triggers reflex-mediated activation of twitches. This view derived, in part,from evidence in adult cats that monosynaptic and polysyn-aptic spinal reflexes are powerfully inhibited during twitching[9–11] (see also [18]). The present results led us to reconsidera role for reflexes during twitching.When a skeletal muscle is stretched andmuscle spindles are

activated, spinally projecting Ia afferents activate motoneu-rons projecting back to that same muscle (or a related one).In this way, the monosynaptic stretch reflex counteracts thestretch on the muscle and controls muscle length. But, dolimb twitches, by contracting one muscle and stretching anopposing one, trigger spinally mediated reflexive reactions toresult in opposite-going twitches? There are several reasonsto believe that they might. First, agonist-antagonist twitchesdeveloped substantially in the WTs and Hets between P4and P10, consistent with evidence that the monosynapticreflex circuit strengthens over the early postnatal period[13, 14, 19]. Second, adult ErbB2 KOs exhibit deficient hin-dlimb extension reflexes as well as a near-complete absenceof Ia afferent connections with spinal motoneurons [6, 8];accordingly, if the stretch reflex were indeed triggered duringtwitching, one would expect that, of all four twitch pairs, theagonist-antagonist twitches would be most affected by theabsence of muscle spindles, which was clearly the case. Third,the peak latency from the first (agonist) to the second (antag-onist) twitch was approximately 30 ms (Figure 1A), which isboth shorter than the latency for agonist-agonist twitches (Fig-ure 1B) and longer than the latencies for the two other twitchpairs (Figure S1); in other words, the timing of agonist-antago-nist twitches is unique in relation to the other twitch pairs.Given this evidence of reflexive twitches in theWTs andHets,

we returned to an earlier data set comprising typically devel-oping rats [1] to determinewhether they exhibit similar patternsof twitching across the first postnatal week (although it shouldbenoted that P2 andP8 ratsmaybemore functionally similar toP0 and P7 mice, respectively; see http://www.translatingtime.

657

Page 3: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

net/translate). Figure S2 compares the ITIs of agonist-antago-nist and agonist-agonist twitches at P2 and P8. At both ages,the rats exhibit agonist-antagonist twitches that peak atshorter ITIs than do the agonist-agonist twitches. Thus, thesedata in rats parallel those reported here in mice (see Figures1 and 3). It also appears that agonist-antagonist twitching ismore developed at P2 in rats than at P4 in mice. In fact, thepeak latency of approximately 50 ms for agonist-antagonisttwitches in the P2 rats is close to the reported latency of 69.76 9.2 ms for the monosynaptic stretch reflex in P0 to P1 rats[20], thus providing additional evidence that agonist-antago-nist twitches are produced by a reflex mechanism.

Latent Class Analysis of Multi-Joint Twitch PatternsWhereas the analyses thus far have focused on pairwisemovements, twitching also comprises complex multi-jointcombinations of actions [1]. Moreover, these complex combi-nations exhibit large-scale developmental changes that areconsistent with a selectionist scheme: complex twitches thatare frequently expressed early in development become betterorganized later in development, and twitches that are betterorganized early in development become more frequent laterin development. Our question here is whether these develop-mental changes are altered in ErbB2 KOs due to diminishedproprioceptive feedback.

Using latent class analysis (LCA), we first identified andcharacterized the complex twitch patterns across age and ge-notype. To do this, we conducted six separate LCA analyseson the WT, Het, and KO mice at each age. These analyseswere conducted on windowed data sets, in which, for each

P4 P100

2

4

6

8

10

Mea

n Li

kelih

ood

(%)

P4 P10-0

25

50

75

100

Mea

n La

tenc

y (m

s)

Wild type

Heterozygote

Knockout

Agonist-Antagonist

P4 P100

2

4

6

8

10

Mea

n Li

kelih

ood

(%)

P4 P10-0

25

50

75

100

Mea

n La

tenc

y (m

s)

Agonist-Agonist

*

**

* *

*†

A

B

Figure 2. Mean Latencies and Likelihoods forAgonist-Antagonist and Agonist-Agonist TwitchPairs in Wild-Type, Heterozygous, and KnockoutMice at P4 and P10

Thesemovement categories correspond to thosein Figure 1. Mean latencies between twitch pairswere calculated over a 150-ms window (andthus are longer than the peak latencies observedin Figure 1), and mean likelihoods of each pairwere calculated over a 0- to 50-ms window.Means were calculated across individual pups(n = 7–11 pups per group). *, significant differencefrom both WT and Het; y, significant differencefrom WT only. Mean + SEM.

50-ms increment, we designatedwhether each of the eight possibletwitch movements (two movements 3two joints 3 two limbs) did or did notoccur. The resulting vectors werepooled across all subjects within a givenage and genotype (litter was used as arandom effect). From this, LCA yieldeda set of clusters, each representing adistinct pattern of twitch movements.Moreover, each cluster was associatedwith a frequency of occurrence and avector of the likelihoods that each jointmovement belonged to the cluster (seeFigure S3). Table S1 provides a sum-mary description of the clusters. Theclusters identified by LCA generally rep-resented motor patterns comprising

more than two twitches. Averaged across all six groups, theclusters included 2.28 joint movements that were stronglyassociated (probability > 0.5) with a given cluster and an addi-tional 1.41 joint movements that were moderately associated(probability = 0.1–0.5).We examined how these clusters changed over develop-

ment. To do this, we matched clusters across P4 and P10within each genotype using a procedure similar to thatdescribed previously [5]. Table S1 shows that, across the threegenotypes, 14–18 clusters were identified as matching, thusleaving 11–15 clusters at P4 that did not have a match at P10(they were ‘‘lost’’) and two clusters at P10 that did not have amatch at P4 (they ‘‘appeared’’). Next, for each genotype, weconducted regression analyses on these clusters to examinethe patterns of change across age and genotype. These ana-lyses considered two factors within and across age: the fre-quency of occurrence of a cluster and its entropy (computedusing Shannon’s information over the profile probabilities ofeach cluster). Here, entropy is a measure of the organizationof joints within a cluster: Higher-entropy clusters can beseen as having more-random activity across all joints, andlower-entropy clusters as having a few strongly associatedjoints and little ‘‘noise’’ in the weakly associated ones. Asshown in Figure 4, all three genotypes at P4 showed strongand significant negative correlations between frequency andentropy (WT: r =20.50; Het: r =20.55; KO: r =20.47): clustersthat were less structured (high entropy) were also expressedless frequently. By P10, however, these correlations were nolonger significant for any genotype (WT: r = 20.09; Het: r =20.30; KO: r = 20.33).

658

Page 4: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Both the WTs and Hets showed strong developmental con-tinuity in that clusters that were highly frequent at P4 tended toremain highly frequent at P10 (Figures 4A and 4B; upper hori-zontal connections:WT: r = 0.53; Het: r = 0.57), and the clustersthat were highly structured at P4 tended to remain highly struc-tured at P10 (lower horizontal paths:WT: r = 0.57; Het: r = 0.65).This continuity was markedly different in the KOs: neither fre-quency (r = 0.19) nor entropy (r = 0.39) was significantly corre-lated across age in these mice (Figure 4C).

Analyses of the cross-correlations across genotypes pre-sent a mixed picture. Specifically, frequency at P4 was signif-icantly and negatively correlated with entropy at P10 in theWTs and Hets (WT: r = 20.51; Het: r = 20.59). This suggeststhat the higher-frequency twitch patterns at P4 tended tobecome more organized (lower entropy) at P10 as minor com-ponents of the twitch patterns were pruned. However, this wasnot observed in the KOs (KO: r = 20.31), suggesting musclespindles may be required for this within-cluster pruning. Incontrast, entropy at P4 was significantly and negatively corre-lated with frequency at P10 in all three genotypes (WT: r =20.41; Het: r = 20.48; KO: r = 20.55). Here, the better-orga-nized (lower entropy) twitch patterns tended to becomemore frequent later in development.

These results reaffirm in newborn mice a major finding fromour earlier report in newborn rats [5]: clusters of twitchingdevelop such that more-frequent clusters become more orga-nized (in the WTs and Hets, but not the KOs) and more-orga-nized clusters become more frequent (in all three genotypes).These relationships are illustrated by the diagonal pathways in

Wild type

Heterozygote

Knockout

4-Day-Olds 10-Day-Olds

-250 -200 -150 -100 -50 0 50 100 150 200 2500

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

-250 -200 -150 -100 -50 0 50 100 150 200 2500

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

-250 -200 -150 -100 -50 0 50 100 150 200 2500

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

-250 -200 -150 -100 -50 0 50 100 150 200 250

Mea

n no

rmal

ized

cou

nt (

%)

Mea

n no

rmal

ized

cou

nt (

%)

Mea

n no

rmal

ized

cou

nt (

%)

Time in relation to trigger twitch (ms) Time in relation to trigger twitch (ms)

-250 -200 -150 -100 -50 0 50 100 150 200 250

-250 -200 -150 -100 -50 0 50 100 150 200 250

Agonist-Antagonist

Agonist-Agonist

Figure 3. Perievent Histograms Comparing Tem-poral Pairwise Relationships for Agonist-Antago-nist and Agonist-Agonist Twitches for Wild-Type,Heterozygous, and Knockout Mice at P4 and P10

Perievent histograms were computed from datapooled across subjects. Significant bins for theagonist-antagonist plots are indicated by upperand lower confidence bands (solid and dashedblack lines; p < 0.01). The agonist-agonist perie-vent histograms exhibit mirror imaging becausethey are auto-correlations. See also Figure S2and the Supplemental Experimental Proceduresfor details.

Figures 4A and 4B. What is novel here isthe use of ErbB2 KOs to assess the rolethat proprioception plays in this devel-opmental process. If we focus on thedownward-going diagonal path, musclespindles appear necessary for highlyfrequent (‘‘well-practiced’’) twitches tobecome more organized across devel-opment; we might think of this develop-mental process as ‘‘pruning’’ of irrele-vant features of clusters such that theybecome more refined. In contrast, if wefocus on the upward-going diagonalpath, muscle spindles appear unneces-sary for highly organized twitches tobecome more frequent across develop-ment; we might think of this process as‘‘amplification.’’ Whereas ‘‘pruning’’ en-tails changes within a cluster, ‘‘amplifi-cation’’ entails changes to clusters as a

whole. That only the latter process was intact in the KOssuggests that ‘‘pruning’’ and ‘‘amplification’’ are governed bydistinct mechanisms acting over developmental time.There was a strong propensity for highly frequent and orga-

nized clusters at P4 to remain highly frequent and organized byP10; however, this was true only for the WTs and Hets. That is,focusing on the horizontal paths in Figure 4, muscle spindlesappear necessary to maintain the frequency and structure ofclusters across development. In other words, this continuityacross age may not come for free but rather must be activelymaintained through proprioception.

ConclusionsHere, we have presented evidence for the first time that thedevelopment of twitching is shaped by sensory experience.We obtained this evidence by comparing the spatiotemporalstructure of twitching at two postnatal ages in mice with andwithout normal proprioception. Critically, had we not obtainedsuch evidence, it would be difficult to sustain the theory thatanimals use twitches to build, refine, and maintain sensori-motor maps [4].Sensory feedback from a twitching limb may modulate sub-

sequent twitching in several non-mutually exclusive ways [5].First, feedback may activate local spinal circuits—including,but not limited to, reflex-mediated activation of twitches—thereby triggering cascades of subsequent twitches in one orboth limbs; this process could contribute to the moment-to-moment spatiotemporal structure of twitching. Second, sen-sory feedbackmay exert long-term influences on sensorimotor

659

Page 5: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

integration, thereby modifying the future production oftwitches (as well as wake movements). The present findingsprovide support for both mechanisms.The specific engagement by twitching of the monosynaptic

reflex circuit raises the question of whether twitches con-tribute to its activity-dependent development. In fact, it hasbeen suggested that the activity-dependent release of neuro-trophin 3 by muscle spindles strengthens Ia afferent connec-tions on motoneurons during the 2nd postnatal week [13, 14].Thus, in light of the early postnatal functional development ofmuscle spindles [21] and the monosynaptic stretch reflex[12–14], we propose twitching as a source of discrete activityfor developing spinal circuits.It is possible that other proprioceptors (i.e., Golgi tendon or-

gans) and tactile receptors contribute to aspects of twitchingthat were not affected in the ErbB2 KOs. In fact, twitchinghas been implicated in the self-organization of the withdrawalreflex, which involves the integration of tactile and propriocep-tive information [22]. Moreover, a computational model oftwitching suggests that different types of peripheral sensorscontribute differentially to the development of spinal circuits[4, 23]. It is not known whether and how these various sensorsinteract through development to shape the patterning oftwitching.It is possible that twitching plays no causal role in sensori-

motor development and that the effects observed here ontwitching are mediated entirely by wake movements; in thatcase, developmental changes in twitching would merelycome along for the ride. However, evidence is accumulatingin favor of a causal role for twitching. This claim is based, inpart, on overwhelming evidence that the nervous systempays close attention to sensory feedback arising from twitches[3, 24–29] and the recent demonstration that sensory feedbackfrom twitching limbs is processed very differently from feed-back arising from wake movements [29]. Moreover, and ofparticular relevance to the present report, twitch-dependentneural activity in week-old ErbB2 KOs is substantially disrup-ted in the cerebellum (B.D. Uitermarkt et al., 2013, Soc. Neuro-sci., conference) and sensorimotor cortex (A. Tadjalli et al.,2013, Soc. Neurosci., conference). Nonetheless, selective ma-nipulations of sleep and wake movements may be needed toestablish the precise functional contributions of twitchingacross early development.There are tangible benefits to taking more seriously the

notion that themotor systemduringwake functions very differ-ently than it does during sleep [29–31]. For example, just asREM behavior disorder—a movement disorder expressedexclusively during REM sleep—is predictive of the later onsetof Parkinson’s disease [32, 33], twitching in early infancy mayprove useful for diagnosing neurodevelopmental disordersbefore they can be detected using standard neurological as-sessments, which are routinely performed only during wake-fulness. The present results underscore this unrecognizedvalue of twitching as a sensitive indicator of the functionalstatus of spinal and supraspinal circuits. Ultimately, thesefindings should be extended to other peripheral sensory re-ceptors, at later ages, and to humans—in healthy and disor-dered populations—if we wish to grasp the full potential oftwitching as both a diagnostic and explanatory tool.

Experimental Procedures

All experiments were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and UseCommittee of The University of Iowa. Subjects were male and female

Wild type

P4 P10

freq freq

entropy

r = .53

r =-.09

r = -.

50

r = .57

r = -.51

r = -.59

entropy

Heterozygote

r = -.4

1

P4 P10

freq freq

entropy

r = .57

r = .65

r = -.30

r = -.

.55

entropy

r = -.4

8

Knockout

P4 P10

freq freq

entropy

r = .19

r = -.33

r = -.

47

r = .39

r = -.31

entropy

r = -.5

5

A

B

C

Figure 4. Regression Analyses of LCA Cluster Frequency and Entropy forWild-Type, Heterozygous, and Knockout Mice at P4 and P10

Correlation coefficients (r) in red are statistically significant (p < 0.05), andthe width of each connecting line is scaled to the magnitude of r. See alsoFigure S3.

660

Page 6: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

ErbB2+/+ (WT), ErbB2+/2 (heterozygous), and ErbB22/2 (KO) mice. Asdescribed previously [5], small dots of UV fluorescent paint were appliedon the two forelimbs and chest at strategic locations for identifying indi-vidual joint movements. Pups were secured in a supine position incustom-made silicone molds appropriate to their age and size. Twohigh-speed (250 frames/s) digital video cameras (Integrated DesignTools) with 105-mm micro-Nikkor lenses (Nikon) were used to recordtwitches. Recordings began when the pup was acclimated in an incu-bator (35!C) and cycling between sleep and wakefulness. Under UV illu-mination, multiple 20-s recordings were acquired. For each subject, aminimum of three and a maximum of eight videos were acquired. Asdescribed previously [5], automatic motion tracking of the joints was per-formed using ProAnalyst (Xcitex). 3D reconstruction of the pups’ limbmovements in space was done with a calibration fixture with an accuracyof approximately 0.1 mm. Data were imported into Spike2 (CambridgeElectronic Design) as eight continuous waveforms representing the fourjoints across the two forelimbs. For pup-specific parametric analyses,ANOVA was performed using SPSS (IBM). When appropriate, follow-upANOVAs and post hoc tests (LSD) were used. For all inferential statistics,alpha was set at 0.05. Perievent histograms were used to assess pairwisewithin-joint relationships using the ‘‘event correlation’’ function in Spike2.As described previously [29], statistical significance was assessed usinga jitter protocol implemented in MATLAB (MathWorks) [34]. LCA (LatentGOLD software; Statistical Innovations) and follow-up analyses were per-formed as described previously [5], with some modifications (see theSupplemental Experimental Procedures). All means are presented withtheir SE.

Supplemental Information

Supplemental Information includes Supplemental Experimental Proce-dures, three figures, one table, and one movie and can be found with thisarticle online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.022.

Acknowledgments

We thank Hugo Gravato Marques, Alexandre Tiriac, and Carlos Del Rio-Ber-mudez for helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript. This workwas supported by a grant from the NIH (NS73869) to M.S.B.

Received: November 5, 2014Revised: December 23, 2014Accepted: January 7, 2015Published: February 19, 2015

References

1. Gramsbergen, A., Schwartze, P., and Prechtl, H.F.R. (1970). The post-natal development of behavioral states in the rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 3,267–280.

2. Jouvet-Mounier, D., Astic, L., and Lacote, D. (1970). Ontogenesis of thestates of sleep in rat, cat, and guinea pig during the first postnatalmonth. Dev. Psychobiol. 2, 216–239.

3. Tiriac, A., Uitermarkt, B.D., Fanning, A.S., Sokoloff, G., and Blumberg,M.S. (2012). Rapid whisker movements in sleeping newborn rats.Curr. Biol. 22, 2075–2080.

4. Blumberg, M.S., Marques, H.G., and Iida, F. (2013). Twitching in senso-rimotor development from sleeping rats to robots. Curr. Biol. 23, R532–R537.

5. Blumberg, M.S., Coleman, C.M., Gerth, A.I., and McMurray, B.(2013). Spatiotemporal structure of REM sleep twitching revealsdevelopmental origins of motor synergies. Curr. Biol. 23, 2100–2109.

6. Leu, M., Bellmunt, E., Schwander, M., Farinas, I., Brenner, H.R., andMuller, U. (2003). Erbb2 regulates neuromuscular synapse formationand is essential for muscle spindle development. Development 130,2291–2301.

7. Andrechek, E.R., Hardy, W.R., Girgis-Gabardo, A.A., Perry, R.L., Butler,R., Graham, F.L., Kahn, R.C., Rudnicki, M.A., and Muller, W.J. (2002).ErbB2 is required for muscle spindle and myoblast cell survival. Mol.Cell. Biol. 22, 4714–4722.

8. Wang, Z., Li, L., and Frank, E. (2012). The role of muscle spindles in thedevelopment of the monosynaptic stretch reflex. J. Neurophysiol. 108,83–90.

9. Baldissera, F., Broggi, G., and Mancia, M. (1966). Monosynaptic andpolysynaptic spinal reflexes during physiological sleep and wakeful-ness. Arch. Ital. Biol. 104, 112–133.

10. Kubota, K., and Tanaka, R. (1968). Fusimotor unit activities and naturalsleep in the cat. Jpn. J. Physiol. 18, 43–58.

11. Gassel, M.M., Marchiafava, P.L., and Pompeiano, O. (1964). Tonic andphasic inhibition of spinal reflexes during deep, desynchronized sleepin unrestrained cats. Arch. Ital. Biol. 102, 471–479.

12. Mendell, L.M., Munson, J.B., and Arvanian, V.L. (2001). Neurotrophinsand synaptic plasticity in the mammalian spinal cord. J. Physiol. 533,91–97.

13. Chen, H.-H., Hippenmeyer, S., Arber, S., and Frank, E. (2003).Development of the monosynaptic stretch reflex circuit. Curr. Opin.Neurobiol. 13, 96–102.

14. Mentis, G.Z., Alvarez, F.J., Shneider, N.A., Siembab, V.C., andO’Donovan, M.J. (2010). Mechanisms regulating the specificity andstrength of muscle afferent inputs in the spinal cord. Ann. N Y Acad.Sci. 1198, 220–230.

15. Blumberg, M.S. (2010). Beyond dreams: do sleep-related movementscontribute to brain development? Front. Neurol. 1, 140.

16. Roffwarg, H.P., Muzio, J.N., and Dement, W.C. (1966). Ontogeneticdevelopment of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science 152, 604–619.

17. Hippenmeyer, S., Shneider, N.A., Birchmeier, C., Burden, S.J., Jessell,T.M., and Arber, S. (2002). A role for neuregulin1 signaling in musclespindle differentiation. Neuron 36, 1035–1049.

18. Waldenstrom, A., Christensson, M., and Schouenborg, J. (2009).Spontaneous movements: Effect of denervation and relation to theadaptation of nociceptive withdrawal reflexes in the rat. Physiol.Behav. 98, 532–536.

19. Seebach, B.S., andMendell, L.M. (1996). Maturation in properties ofmo-toneurons and their segmental input in the neonatal rat. J. Neurophysiol.76, 3875–3885.

20. Kudo, N., and Yamada, T. (1985). Development of the monosynapticstretch reflex in the rat: an in vitro study. J. Physiol. 369, 127–144.

21. Vejsada, R., Hnık, P., Payne, R., Ujec, E., and Palecek, J. (1985). Thepostnatal functional development of muscle stretch receptors in therat. Somatosens. Res. 2, 205–222.

22. Petersson, P., Waldenstrom, A., Fahraeus, C., and Schouenborg, J.(2003). Spontaneous muscle twitches during sleep guide spinal self-or-ganization. Nature 424, 72–75.

23. Marques, H.G., Bharadwaj, A., and Iida, F. (2014). From spontaneousmotor activity to coordinated behaviour: a developmental model.PLoS Comput. Biol. 10, e1003653.

24. Khazipov, R., Sirota, A., Leinekugel, X., Holmes, G.L., Ben-Ari, Y., andBuzsaki, G. (2004). Early motor activity drives spindle bursts in thedeveloping somatosensory cortex. Nature 432, 758–761.

25. Sokoloff, G., Uitermarkt, B.D., and Blumberg, M.S. (2014). REM sleeptwitches rouse nascent cerebellar circuits: Implications for sensori-motor development. Dev. Neurobiol. Published online March 28, 2014.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22177.

26. Mohns, E.J., and Blumberg, M.S. (2010). Neocortical activation ofthe hippocampus during sleep in infant rats. J. Neurosci. 30,3438–3449.

27. Mohns, E.J., and Blumberg, M.S. (2008). Synchronous bursts ofneuronal activity in the developing hippocampus: modulation by activesleep and association with emerging gamma and theta rhythms.J. Neurosci. 28, 10134–10144.

28. McVea, D.A., Mohajerani, M.H., and Murphy, T.H. (2012). Voltage-sensi-tive dye imaging reveals dynamic spatiotemporal properties of corticalactivity after spontaneous muscle twitches in the newborn rat.J. Neurosci. 32, 10982–10994.

29. Tiriac, A., Del Rio-Bermudez, C., and Blumberg, M.S. (2014). Self-gener-ated movements with ‘‘unexpected’’ sensory consequences. Curr. Biol.24, 2136–2141.

30. De Cock, V.C., Vidailhet, M., Leu, S., Texeira, A., Apartis, E., Elbaz, A.,Roze, E., Willer, J.C., Derenne, J.P., Agid, Y., and Arnulf, I. (2007).Restoration of normal motor control in Parkinson’s disease duringREM sleep. Brain 130, 450–456.

31. Bliwise, D.L., Trotti, L.M., Greer, S.A., Juncos, J.J., and Rye, D.B. (2010).Phasic muscle activity in sleep and clinical features of Parkinson dis-ease. Ann. Neurol. 68, 353–359.

32. Schenck, C.H., Bundlie, S.R., and Mahowald, M.W. (1996). Delayedemergence of a parkinsonian disorder in 38% of 29 older men initially

661

Page 7: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

diagnosed with idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disor-der. Neurology 46, 388–393.

33. Claassen, D.O., Josephs, K.A., Ahlskog, J.E., Silber, M.H., Tippmann-Peikert, M., and Boeve, B.F. (2010). REM sleep behavior disorder pre-ceding other aspects of synucleinopathies by up to half a century.Neurology 75, 494–499.

34. Amarasingham, A., Harrison, M.T., Hatsopoulos, N.G., and Geman, S.(2012). Conditional modeling and the jitter method of spike resampling.J. Neurophysiol. 107, 517–531.

662

Page 8: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Current Biology Supplemental Information

Development  of  Twitching in  Sleeping  Infant  Mice Depends  on  Sensory  Experience Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Greta Sokoloff, Joshua A. Weiner, Bernd Fritzsch, and Bob McMurray

Page 9: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Supplemental Figures

Figure S1, related to Figure 1. Frequency distributions of inter-twitch intervals for

wild type, heterozygous, and knockout mice at 4 and 10 days of age. Inter-twitch

intervals were restricted to one of two movement categories. For each category, the first

twitch of a pair could occur at any joint, whereas the second twitch was (A) a

succeeding twitch in the same limb but at another joint (e.g., right elbow flexion ! right

shoulder abduction) or (B) a succeeding homologous twitch in the other limb (e.g., right

elbow flexion ! left elbow flexion). Frequencies were normalized over the 150-ms

window. Ns at P4: 3995-6931; Ns at P10: 1784-2131.

0

8

16

24

32

0 50 100 150

Nor

mal

ized

Freq

uenc

y (%

)

0 50 100 150

0

8

16

24

32

0 50 100 150

Nor

mal

ized

Freq

uenc

y (%

)

Inter-Twitch Interval (ms)0 50 100 150Inter-Twitch Interval (ms)

A. Same limb, other joint

B. Other limb, homologous

4-Day-Olds 10-Day-OldsWild type

Heterozygote

Knockout

Page 10: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Figure S2, related to Figures 1 and 3. Frequency distributions of inter-twitch

intervals for agonist-antagonist and agonist-agonist twitches in rat pups at 2 (P2)

and 8 (P8) days of age. These plots were produced using the same methods as those

used for Figure 1. Data are from [S1].

0

5

10

15

20

0 50 100 150

Nor

mal

ized

Fre

quen

cy (%

)

Inter-Twitch Interval (ms)

P2 Agonist-Antagonist

P8 Agonist-Antagonist

P2 Agonist-Agonist

P8 Agonist-Agonist

Page 11: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Figure S3, related to Figure 4. Example profile plots of multi-joint patterns of twitching identified using latent class analysis for wild type and knockout mice at 4 and 10 days of age. Each plot can be interpreted as the likelihood that, given the existence of a cluster, a particular joint movement would be included within it. For each genotype at each age, a low- and high-entropy (E) example is provided. Abbreviations: ShAd, shoulder adduction; ElbFlx, elbow flexion; ShAb, shoulder abduction; ElbExt, elbow extension.

E=1.64

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Prob

ability

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Prob

ability

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Prob

ability

ShAd

ElbF

lxSh

AbElbE

xtSh

AdElbF

lxSh

AbElbE

xt

ShAd

ElbF

lxSh

AbElbE

xtSh

AdElbF

lxSh

AbElbE

xt

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Prob

ability

Left Right

Wild type4-Day-Olds 10-Day-Olds

Knockout

E=2.29 E=2.39

E=1.23

E=1.26

E=2.31E=2.18

E=1.34

Left Right

Page 12: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Supplemental Table

Table S1. Descriptive data for the clusters, identified by latent class analysis, at 4 (P4) and 10 (P10) days of age and for each of the three genotypes. Wild type Heterozygote Knockout

P4 P10 P4 P10 P4 P10 No. of clusters 29 16 29 20 21 15 No. of joint movements strongly associated with cluster (p > 0.5)

2.38 2.32 2.37 2.32 2.28 2.0

No. of joint movements moderately associated with cluster (0.1 < p < 0.5)

1.31 1.5 1.31 1.5 1.05 1.8

P4 clusters not retained over development 15 — 11 — 11 —

P4 clusters matched to P10 clusters 14 18 14

New clusters at P10 — 2 — 2 — 2

Page 13: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Supplemental Experimental Procedures

Experiments were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide

for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH Publication No. 80-23) and were

approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Iowa.

Subjects

Subjects were male and female ErbB2+/+ (wild type, WT), ErbB2+/- (heterozygous, Het),

and ErbB2-/- (knockout, KO) mice. When littermates were tested, they were always of

different genotypes. For this study, a total of 27 P4-5 mice (hereafter, P4; WT: n=9; Het:

n= 11; KO: n=7) from 22 litters (body weight: WT: 3.2 + 0.2 g; Het: 2.9 + 0.2 g; KO: 2.4

+ 0.1 g) and 23 P10-11 mice (hereafter P10; WT: n=7; Het: n= 8; KO: n=8) from 20

litters (body weight: WT: 6.2 + 0.6 g; Het: 6.5 + 0.4 g; KO: 5.1 + 0.5 g) were used.

Breeding pairs and their litters were housed and raised in laboratory cages (28 x 18 x

14 cm; Thoren Caging Systems, Hazleton, PA) in the animal colony at the University of

Iowa. Food and water were available ad libitum. All animals were maintained on a 12-h

light-dark schedule with lights on at 0600 h. All testing was done during the lights-on

period.

We obtained ErbB2flox/flox human skeletal α-actin (HSA)-Cre mice (see [S2]) and

established a breeding colony of ErbB2+/- HSA-Cre-positive pairs. Offspring were

genotyped by P2 from tail snips. Tissue was digested in 50 mM NaOH for 60 min and

10 µl of Tris HCL (pH 8) was added. Samples were spun down for 6 min.

Primers used are as follows: ErbB2loxP-F: GCTCAGCCCTCTTGTTCTACTTCT

G 3’; ErbB2 Exon 1-R: 5’ TCAGGCAGACACTTCCACGCTAG 3’; Cre1: 5’ CCTGTTTT-

Page 14: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

GCACGTTCACCG 3’; Cre3: 5’ ATGCTTCTGTCCGTTTGCCG 3’; IMR42: 5’

CTAGGCCACAGAATTGAAAGATCT 3’; IMR43: 5’ GTAGGTGGAAATTCTAG-

CATCATCC 3’.

Subjects were individually marked with non-toxic marker for identification until

testing at P4 or P10. After testing, toe tattoos were used for permanent identification.

To verify the conditional knockout of muscle spindles, we performed histology on

forelimb and hindlimb muscles of WT (n=2), Het (n=2), and KO (n=4) mice at P6-10.

After euthanasia, limbs were removed and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde before

switching to 1 M phosphate-buffered sucrose. Muscle tissue was sectioned transversely

at a thickness of 20-25 µm on a cryostat (Leica Microsystems, Buffalo Grove, IL).

Sections were incubated on gelatin-subbed slides for 24 h at -4˚C in universal blocking

serum (3% Bovine serum albumin, 0.1% Triton, and 0.02% Sodium Azide in PBS) with

anti-vesicular glutamate transporter 1 (1:1000; AB5905; Millipore, Temecula, CA) and

anti-parvalbumin (1:200; AB11427; Abcam Inc., Cambridge, MA). After incubation with

primary antibodies, sections were washed 3 X 10 min in PBS and then incubated with

fluorescent secondary antibodies at room temperature for 90 min (Alexa Fluor® 488 and

Alexa Fluor® 568; Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY). Sections were washed 2 X 10

min in PBS. A final 10-min wash in PBS included 15 µl of DAPI (Life Technologies,

Grand Island, NY) to counterstain all cell nuclei. Sections were rinsed with Barnstead

water and coverslipped with Fluoro-Gel (Electron Microscopy Sciences, Hatfield, PA).

Labeling was visualized at 10–20X magnification using a Leica fluorescent microscope

(Leica Microsystems).

Page 15: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Preparation of subjects for videorecording

As described previously [S1], on the day of testing, a pup with a visible milk band was

anesthetized with isoflurane and secured in a supine position in a custom-made silicone

mold appropriate to the age and size of the pup. To keep the pup in place, light

restraints were placed over the abdomen and neck. Small dots of ultraviolet (UV)

fluorescent paint were applied on the two forelimbs and chest at strategic locations for

identifying individual joint movements. After approximately 10 min when this set-up

procedure was complete, the pup was transferred to a humidified incubator maintained

at thermoneutrality (P4: 36.5°C; P10: 34°C).

Data acquisition

Two high-speed (250 frames/s) digital video cameras (Integrated Design Tools,

Tallahassee, FL) with 105 mm micro-Nikkor lenses (Nikon, Melville, NY) were used.

These cameras record directly to digital at 1280 x 1024 pixels. Motion Studio software

(Integrated Design Tools) was used to synchronize the cameras and record videos.

Recordings began when the pup was acclimated in the incubator and cycling

between sleep and wakefulness. Under UV illumination, multiple 20-s recordings were

acquired. During each recording, the subject was closely monitored for behavioral state.

Videos were not saved if wake behaviors or startles were detected. When data were

saved, approximately 35 min were needed to download the data from the two cameras,

after which the next recording began. At the completion of the recording session, each

pup was returned to its home cage and the cameras were calibrated for 3-D motion

tracking using a calibration fixture and ProAnalyst software (Xcitex, Boston, MA).

Page 16: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

For each subject, a minimum of three and a maximum of eight videos were

acquired. A total of 135 P4 videos (WT: 44; Het: 54; KO: 37) and 133 P10 videos (WT:

40; Het: 46; KO: 47) were acquired and used for analysis.

Data reduction

As described previously [S1], automatic motion tracking of the joints was performed

using ProAnalyst 3-D software. Frame-by-frame confirmation and manual correction

was used when necessary. Three-dimensional reconstruction of the pups’ limb

movements in space was done with a calibration fixture with an accuracy of

approximately 0.1 mm.

Based on our previous study, we identified four joint angles that reliably identified

shoulder abduction and adduction and elbow extension and flexion for each of the two

forelimbs. These angles were computed using the ProAnalyst software for each of the

5000 video frames from each camera for a given 20-s recording. Next, the data were

imported into Spike2 (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) as eight

continuous waveforms representing the four joints across the two forelimbs. Slow

oscillations were filtered from the waveforms (time constant = 0.4 s). Mean baseline

quiescent activity for each waveform was calculated from four 1-s time-points across the

20-s recording. The threshold for estimating the onset time for each twitch event was

the standard deviation of this mean multiplied by 10. Waveforms were randomly

selected to cross-check event onset times against video records.

Page 17: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Data analysis

A single datafile was created with onset times for each specific joint movement; breaks

between 20-s recordings were marked to prevent inappropriate analyses across

sessions. Inter-twitch intervals were computed from these records and counts were

normalized over a 150-ms window. Because each onset time was linked with a

particular pup, it was possible to perform pup-specific as well as pooled analyses.

For pup-specific analyses of twitch pairs, mean latencies from the first to second

twitch were calculated over a 150-ms window. The mean likelihood that a twitch pair

occurred over a 0-50-ms window was also calculated; this likelihood represents the

probability that both joints in a pair twitched at least once in a given window. Probability

values were empirical logit transformed prior to analysis. For each movement category,

two-factor (age, genotype) between-subject analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were

performed separately for mean latency and mean likelihood using SPSS (IBM, Armonk,

NY). When appropriate, follow-up ANOVAs were performed within each age to assess

genotype differences; for post hoc tests, the Least Significant Difference (LSD) test was

used. For all inferential statistics, alpha was set at 0.05. All means are presented with

their standard error.

Perievent histograms were used to assess within-joint relationships for agonist-

antagonist and agonist-agonist pairs. They were computed using the “event correlation”

function in Spike2 on pooled data at each age and genotype. For agonist-antagonist

pairs, four separate event correlations were computed for each of the four joints. For

each joint, one movement (e.g., left elbow flexion) was designated the “target” and the

other (e.g., left elbow extension) was designated the “trigger” (the choice was arbitrary).

Page 18: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Each of the four event correlations yielded the total number of target events for each 10-

ms bin surrounding the trigger; counts were then summed within each bin and

normalized in relation to the total number of target twitches across the 500-ms window

(i.e., 250 ms before and after the trigger). Perievent histograms for agonist-agonist pairs

were constructed identically except that eight separate event correlations were

computed for each of the eight possible joint movements. We tested statistical

significance for the agonist-antagonist event correlations using a jitter protocol [S3,

S4] implemented in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA). We corrected for multiple

comparisons using the method of Amarasingham et al. [S5]; this method produces

upper and lower confidence bands for each event correlation (p < 0.01).

Latent class analysis (LCA) was performed as described previously [S1]. We first

created a “windowed” dataset by stepping through the raw data in 50 ms increments

and identifying those twitches that occurred within each window. Only windows with at

least two twitches were included. Next, Latent GOLD software (Statistical Innovations,

Belmont, MA) was used to perform the LCA on the windowed dataset. The data at each

age and genotype were analyzed separately and litter was used as a random effect.

Cluster convergence occurred for all datasets and the determination of the best model

fit was made by minimizing the value of the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). We

evaluated model fit by examining the bivariate residuals for each joint, a measure of the

degree to which the clusters failed to predict the pairwise co-occurrence of each joint.

None of these residuals exceeded a value of 1.0, suggesting that LCA had determined

the best model fit.

As described previously [S1], P4 and P10 clusters for each genotype were

Page 19: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

matched for similarity to examine developmental changes using the eight probability

values that comprise each profile plot. Two similarity rules were used: Euclidean

distance and a probabilistic rule (where the probability of the same twitch being present

in each cluster was added to the probability that the same twitch was absent from both.

A P4 and P10 cluster were deemed a match only if they were the best matches on both

similarity rules exclusively and reciprocally (i.e., the P4 cluster’s closest match was the

P10 cluster, and vice versa).

Regression analyses of the LCA clusters were performed using SPSS, as

described previously [S1]. We computed Shannon’s Entropy, E, for each cluster by first

normalizing the probabilities associated with each twitch (for a given cluster) and then

dividing each probability by the sum of the probabilities. A cluster with low entropy is

considered highly organized. Cluster frequency was obtained from Latent Gold LCA

software and was log-transformed prior to analysis. The regressions examined the

various relationships between entropy and frequency at P4 and P10.

Because different numbers of clusters were identified at each age and because

not every cluster matched across ages, different numbers of clusters contributed to

each path of the regression analyses. For example, at P4 the frequency-entropy

correlation included 19 clusters, whereas at P10 it only included 16. For some analyses,

we maximized the amount of data available by assuming that the frequency of a non-

matching cluster (e.g., a cluster at P4 that did not occur at P10) was 0 at that age (since

that cluster did not occur). We could not assign such values for entropy, so those

analyses have smaller sample sizes.

Page 20: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

Relationship between the number of twitches in the sample and LCA measures.

The LCA reported fewer clusters at P10 than P4 for all genotypes (see Table S1). The

likely cause of this is that there were substantially fewer twitches overall in the P10

sample (1824-2176 twitches/genotype) than the P4 sample (4020-6985

twitches/genotype), as the LCA’s implicit threshold for including a cluster in the model is

based on the frequency of that pattern in the data. If the smaller number of clusters in

the older animals was due to the smaller number of twitches, we were concerned that

this could also systematically bias the frequency or entropy estimates on which our

regressions were based.

To rule this out, we conducted an additional LCA analysis using only half of the

WT P4 data (either the first or second half), and compared the resulting clusters to

those extracted from the full dataset. As expected, the truncated dataset yielded fewer

clusters (1st half: 18 clusters; 2nd half: 21 clusters; full dataset: 29 clusters). We

compared the entropy of these clusters using unpaired t tests and found no significant

differences in mean entropy between either half and the full dataset (Mfull=1.74, SD=.43,

M1st=1.77, SD=.40, t45=.31, NS; M2nd=1.73, SD=.40, t45=.007, NS). When we matched

clusters from the truncated and full analyses, entropy was highly correlated (1st half vs.

full dataset: r=.83, 2nd half vs. full dataset: r=.77). To examine frequency, we multiplied

the frequency of the clusters from the truncated dataset by 2 (since there were half as

many twitches). Considering the matched clusters, we found no significant differences

in mean frequency between either half and the full dataset (Mfull=112.5, SD=87,

M1st=131.1, SD=80, t16=1.49, NS; M2nd=1.67, SD=.39, t17=1.56, NS), and the

frequencies were highly correlated (1st half vs. full dataset: r=.81, 2nd half vs. full

Page 21: Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on …€¦ · analyzed skeletal muscles for the presence of spindles (see theSupplementalInformation).Asexpected[6],whereasspin-dles

dataset: r=.95). Thus, although a dataset with fewer twitches leads to fewer clusters,

this does not appear to systematically bias either cluster entropy or frequency

estimates.

Supplemental References

S1. Blumberg, M. S., Coleman, C. M., Gerth, A. I., and McMurray, B. (2013). Spatiotemporal structure of REM sleep twitching reveals developmental origins of motor synergies. Curr Biol 23, 2100–2109.

S2. Leu, M., Bellmunt, E., Schwander, M., Farinas, I., Brenner, H., and Muller, U. (2003). Erbb2 regulates neuromuscular synapse formation and is essential for muscle spindle development. Development 130, 2291–2301.

S3. Harrison, M. T., and Geman, S. (2009). A rate and history-preserving resampling algorithm for neural spike trains. Neural Comput 21, 1244–1258.

S4. Amarasingham, A., Harrison, M. T., Hatsopoulos, N. G., and Geman, S. (2012). Conditional modeling and the jitter method of spike resampling. J Neurophysiol 107, 517–531.

S5. Amarasingham, A., Harrison, M. T., and Hatsopoulos, N. G. (2011). Conditional modeling and the jitter method of spike re-sampling: Supplement. arXiv. Available at: http://arxiv.org/abs/1111.4296 [Accessed June 9, 2014].