7
 Child Deve lopmen t, Septe mber/ Octob er 2008, Volume 79, Number 5, Pages 1230 1236 Development of Three-Dimensional Object Completion in Infancy Kasey C. Soska New York University Scott P. Johnson University of California, Los Angeles Three-dimensional (3D) object completion was investigated by habituating 4- and 6-month-old infants ( n 5 24 total) with a computer-generated wedge stimulus that pivoted 15 , providing only a limited view. Two displays, rotating 360°, were then shown: a complete, solid volume and an incomplete, hollow form composed only of the sides seen during habituation. There were no reliable preferences for either test display by 4-month-olds. At 6 mon ths , inf ant s showeda rel iable nov elt y pre fer enc e forthe inc omplet e test dis pla y . Inf ant s in a contro l gro up ( n 5 24) not habituated to the limited-view wedge preferred neither test display. By 6 months, infants may represent simple obj ects as comple te in 3D space despi te a limite d perspect ive. Possib le mechan isms of develop ment of 3D object completion are discussed. Piag et (195 4) pion eere d thestud y of infants’ per cept ual and cog niti ve abil iti es with his foun dat iona l work on objec t kno wl edge and spa tial cog ni ti on. Since these original studies, a wealth of information on visual- cognitive abilities has accrued in such areas as object permanence (e.g., Baillargeon, 1987; Diamond, 1990), spat ial rea soning (e.g ., Bre mner, 197 8; Rie ser , 1979 ), and perceptual completion (e.g., Johnson, Bremner, et al., 2003; Kellman & Spelke, 1983). Departures from the age and mot or constr aint s Pia get suggest ed ori ginall y hav e bee n cla imed , spa rki ng pol emic arg ume nts on the ‘‘true’’ capacities of young infants (Spelke, 1998). Modern rationalist views of infants’ understand- ing of the physic al world have contended that infant s are more sophisticated in their representations of the visual w or ld t ha n Pia ge t s ur mi se d (S pe lke, Breinl inger, Macomber , & Jacob son, 1992) . Studi es examining infants’ understandings of object continu- ity and soli dit y , for exa mple , hav e sugges ted tha t infants as young as 2.5 months may have a rudimen- tary understanding of object permanence (Aguiar & Baillargeon, 1999). In general, those arguing in favor of the precocious nature of infants’ object perception and physical understanding maintain that develop- mental processes are more related to infants’ sensi- tivity to relevant features of the objects used in the di splays rather than changes in the under lying mec h- anisms of information- proce ssing and object knowl- edge (e.g., Baillargeon, 2004). Oth ers hav e arg ued for a more gra du al eme rg ence of obje ct kn owle dg e ov er th e 1st ye ar af te r bi rt h— wit h inf ant s’ und ers tan din g of the phy sic al wor ld bui lt upon succe ssivel y more sophi sticat ed atte ntion al skills , an increasing ability to integrate multiple sources of information in the visual array, and emerging manual and other exploratory skills to facilitate the acquisition of physical knowledge (e.g., Colombo, 2001; Gibson, 1988; Johnson, 2003). Work on infants’ perception of causality (e.g., Cohen & Oakes, 1993), perception of obj ect uni ty (e. g., Johnson, 200 4), and obj ect rec ogn iti on (e. g., Kra ebe l & Ger har dstein, 200 6) has pro vided evi den ce for inf ant s’ gra dua lly emerging obj ect kno wl- edge. The present study, therefore, was motivated by this growing awareness of the importance of under- sta ndi ng dev elo pme nta l processes in pr ovi din g a mor e acc urate acc oun t of inf ant s’ phy sic al wor ld tha n is offered by rationalist approaches. We examined infants’ perception of the full, volu- metric form of three-dimensional (3D) objects when provided only a limited view, asking if infants repre- sent objects seen from a limited vantage as coherent volumes or inst ead as consi st ing solel y of the surfa ces tha t are vis ibl e from the curren t vie wpoi nt. Thi s question maps onto work investigating developmen- tal processes of object and spatial knowledge and rel ates to work on per ceptua l complet ion in genera l in infancy. This resea rch was supp orted by the Nati onal Insti tute s of Healt h Grant R01-HD40432. Portions of this work were presented at the 2006 meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Sarasota, FL, and the 2006 International Conference on Infant Studies, Kyoto, Japan. We grat efull y ackn owle dge the infa nts and pare nts who part icipa ted in the studies. We also thank Juliet Davidow, Melissa Rozon, and Lauren Clepper for their help in recruiting infant participants, and we thank Holly Hamilton and Alissa Gaughan for their assistance with reliability coding. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kase y C. Soska , Depa rtmen t of Psych olog y , New Y ork Universit y , 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. # 2008, Copyright the Author(s)  Journal Compilation #2008, Soci ety forResearchin ChildDevelo pment , Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2008/7905 -0002

Development of Three-Dimensional Object Completion in Infancy - Soska & Johnson (2008).pdf

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • Child Development, September/October 2008, Volume 79, Number 5, Pages 1230 1236

    Development of Three-Dimensional Object Completion in Infancy

    Kasey C. SoskaNew York University

    Scott P. JohnsonUniversity of California, Los Angeles

    Three-dimensional (3D) object completion was investigated by habituating 4- and 6-month-old infants (n 5 24total) with a computer-generated wedge stimulus that pivoted 15, providing only a limited view. Two displays,rotating 360, were then shown: a complete, solid volume and an incomplete, hollow form composed only of thesides seen during habituation. There were no reliable preferences for either test display by 4-month-olds. At 6months, infants showed a reliable novelty preference for the incomplete test display. Infants in a control group (n524) not habituated to the limited-view wedge preferred neither test display. By 6 months, infants may representsimple objects as complete in 3D space despite a limited perspective. Possible mechanisms of development of 3Dobject completion are discussed.

    Piaget (1954)pioneered the studyof infants perceptualand cognitive abilities with his foundational workon object knowledge and spatial cognition. Since theseoriginal studies, a wealth of information on visual-cognitive abilities has accrued in such areas as objectpermanence (e.g., Baillargeon, 1987; Diamond, 1990),spatial reasoning (e.g., Bremner, 1978;Rieser, 1979), andperceptual completion (e.g., Johnson, Bremner, et al.,2003; Kellman & Spelke, 1983). Departures from theage andmotor constraints Piaget suggested originallyhave been claimed, sparking polemic argumentson the true capacities of young infants (Spelke,1998).

    Modern rationalist views of infants understand-ing of the physical world have contended that infantsare more sophisticated in their representations of thevisual world than Piaget surmised (Spelke,Breinlinger, Macomber, & Jacobson, 1992). Studiesexamining infants understandings of object continu-ity and solidity, for example, have suggested thatinfants as young as 2.5 months may have a rudimen-tary understanding of object permanence (Aguiar &Baillargeon, 1999). In general, those arguing in favorof the precocious nature of infants object perceptionand physical understanding maintain that develop-

    mental processes are more related to infants sensi-tivity to relevant features of the objects used in thedisplays rather than changes in the underlying mech-anisms of information-processing and object knowl-edge (e.g., Baillargeon, 2004).

    Others have argued for amore gradual emergence ofobject knowledge over the 1st year after birthwith infants understanding of the physical world builtupon successivelymore sophisticated attentional skills,an increasing ability to integrate multiple sources ofinformation in the visual array, and emerging manualand other exploratory skills to facilitate the acquisitionof physical knowledge (e.g., Colombo, 2001; Gibson,1988; Johnson, 2003). Work on infants perception ofcausality (e.g., Cohen & Oakes, 1993), perception ofobject unity (e.g., Johnson, 2004), and object recognition(e.g., Kraebel & Gerhardstein, 2006) has providedevidence for infants gradually emerging object knowl-edge. The present study, therefore, was motivated bythis growing awareness of the importance of under-standing developmental processes in providing amoreaccurate account of infants physical world than isoffered by rationalist approaches.

    We examined infants perception of the full, volu-metric form of three-dimensional (3D) objects whenprovided only a limited view, asking if infants repre-sent objects seen from a limited vantage as coherentvolumes or instead as consisting solely of the surfacesthat are visible from the current viewpoint. Thisquestion maps onto work investigating developmen-tal processes of object and spatial knowledge andrelates towork on perceptual completion in general ininfancy.

    This research was supported by the National Institutes of HealthGrant R01-HD40432. Portions of this work were presented at the2006 meeting of the Vision Sciences Society, Sarasota, FL, and the2006 International Conference on Infant Studies, Kyoto, Japan. Wegratefully acknowledge the infants and parentswho participated inthe studies. We also thank Juliet Davidow, Melissa Rozon, andLauren Clepper for their help in recruiting infant participants, andwe thank Holly Hamilton and Alissa Gaughan for their assistancewith reliability coding.Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to

    Kasey C. Soska, Department of Psychology, New York University, 6Washington Place, New York, NY 10003. Electronic mail may besent to [email protected].

    # 2008, Copyright the Author(s)Journal Compilation# 2008, Society for Research inChildDevelopment, Inc.All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2008/7905-0002

  • The 3D nature of our world may pose a challengeto the immature visual system. Distant objects areoften occluded by nearer objects, and recognition ofthe partially occluded object requires synthesis ofthe visible portions into a coherent percept. Objectscome in and out of existence as they move orobservers move around them, and the far sides ofopaque objects are blocked from sight by their visibleportionsthe problem of self-occlusion. Despite thefragmented nature of the visual world, adult observ-ers are adept at discriminating and recognizingcomplete forms through spatial and spatiotemporalocclusion (Kellman & Shipley, 1991; Marr, 1982) andthrough self-occlusion (Tse, 2002). Thus, our visualexperience is not composed of disjoint surfaces,fleeting percepts of moving objects, and incomplete,hollow volumes; rather, our visual system fills inthemissing parts, resulting in a richer experience thanwhat is visible directly.

    The visual system of the adult is able to accomplishperceptual completion under many circumstanceswithout apparent effort, but the visual experience ofvery young infants is substantially different. Four-month-olds perceive a moving, center-occluded rodas a unified object rather than two disjoint segments(Kellman & Spelke, 1983), but newborn infants per-ceive this display as consisting of unconnected pieces(Slater, Johnson, Brown, & Badenoch, 1996). In theseinvestigations, infants were habituated to a display inwhich a rodmoved laterally behind an occluder. Afterhabituation, infants were shown two test displays,both without the occluding box: one depicting a com-plete, unbroken rod and in the other, two broken rodpieces of the same size as the visible portions seenpreviously. Consistently longer looking at eithertest display is interpreted as a novelty response;therefore, a posthabituation preference for the brokenrodpieceswould imply unity perception during habit-uation. Newborns preferred the unbroken rod at test,whereas older infants preferred the broken rod parts.

    Other experiments examined spatiotemporal com-pletion: the perception of occluded trajectories ofmoving objects.When shown adisplay inwhich a ballmoves back and forth, the center portion of itstrajectory occluded by a box, infants at 6 monthsappeared to perceive the ball continuing behind theoccluder, both in an habituation experiment (Johnson,Bremner, et al., 2003) and in an oculomotor anticipa-tion paradigm (Johnson, Amso, & Slemmer, 2003), butyounger infants provided evidence of perceiving thevisible parts of the trajectory only, failing to completeit, in parallel with experiments on perception of objectunity described previously. Infants visual cognitionthus undergoes substantial development during the

    first few months after birth toward an experience ofa more holistic percept of the visual environment.

    These and other experiments have characterizedinfants perception of spatial and spatiotemporalunity. In contrast, little is known about how infantscome to understand self-occlusion and perceivevolumetric unity. Work on the development of 3Dobject completion and infants understanding of self-occlusion may help elucidate how the infants visualsystem overcomes the problems imposed on it by thecluttered 3Dworld as well as further general learningprinciples.

    A subsidiary goal of the present work is to under-stand better the nature and limits of 3D formperception in young infants. Early research showedthat 4-month-olds could discriminate familiar objectsin novel axes of rotation from novel rotating objects(Kellman, 1984). Infants in these experiments, how-ever, could not generalize 3D form across viewswithin static displays and required either object orobserver movement to discriminate novel and famil-iar objects (Kellman & Short, 1987). Infants as youngas 2months are adept at picking up 3D structure fromkinetic information (Arterberry & Yonas, 2000), andby 4 months, infants are sensitive to some pictorialcues that signify 3D structure (Bhatt & Bertin, 2001;Bhatt & Waters, 1998; Shuwairi, Albert, & Johnson,2007). Infants at 2 and 4 months will complete a 3Dshape when occluded in displays depicting fullyrotating 3D forms (Johnson, Cohen, Marks, &Johnson, 2003). Recentwork from an operant learningparadigm provides evidence that at 3 months, infantsmay achieve view-invariant 3D object recognitionwhen given the opportunity to cause the movementof various 3D forms (Kraebel & Gerhardstein, 2006).

    Together, this work on perceptual completion and3D object perception shows that young infants by 4months show some nascent understanding that ob-jects are continuous in space and that most objects are3D. Infants appear to continually build upon theirexisting knowledge and attentional skills to visuallycomplete more complex displays and veridicallyperceive more complex objects. Still, it is not knownwhen and how infants come to perceive a complete3D volume through self-occlusion.

    In the present experiment, we showed infantscomputer-generated displays depicting objects rotat-ing in depth around the vertical axis (see Figure 1)either pivoting back and forth through 15 (limitedview) or rotating through a full 360 ( full view). Infantswere shown a limited-view display during habitua-tion, providing the opportunity to see only a few facesof the object. Once infants met the habituation crite-rion, they were shown two new full-view displays.

    Development of 3D Object Completion 1231

  • One display depicted a complete solid object, and theotherdepicted an incomplete hollowobject composedof only the surfaces seen during habituation. All thedisplays contained rich information for 3D formincluding kinetic cues (e.g., relative motion, contourdeformation) and pictorial cues (e.g., shading, linejunctions). We reasoned that if an infant perceived thelimited-view object as complete during habituation,then during test he or she should respond to theincomplete display as novel and thus look longer at itthan at the complete display. If, however, the infantshowed no reliable preference for either displayfollowing habituation, then we reasoned that infantlikely did not perceive the self-occluded surfaces ofthe limited-view object.

    We tested infants at 4 and 6 months to exploredevelopmental differences in performance. To assesswhether infants in these age groups show any spon-taneous preferences for incomplete or complete 3Dobjects, infants in a control group were shown com-plete and incomplete test displays without first beinghabituated to the limited-view object.

    Method

    Participants

    The final sample consisted of twenty-four 4-month-olds (M age 5 118.4 days, SD 5 12.8) with 11males and 13 females and twenty-four 6-month-olds(M age 5 179.3 days, SD 5 9.4) with 12 males and12 females. Additional infants were observed butexcluded from the analyses due to fussiness (9 in-

    fants), consistent inattention toward the screen (6infants), parental interference (2 infants), or experi-menter error (2 infants). Participants were recruitedvia letter and phone using a marketing databasecontaining the names of parents with young children.The majority were from Caucasian, middle-classfamilies in the New York City metropolitan area. Allparents were fully debriefed about the experimentalprocedure and informed consent was obtained beforebeginning the session.

    Apparatus and Stimuli

    Infants were tested individually and seated ontheir parents lap 120 cm from a 76-cm monitor ina darkened room. An experimenter was hiddenbehind a divider and operated aMacintosh computerthat performed several functions. The computer pre-sented the stimulus displays, stored each infantsdata, calculated the habituation criterion for eachinfant, and changed displays after the criterion wasmet. The computer also recorded how long eachinfant looked at each display, according to the experi-menters judgments.

    The displays depicted a computer-generatedwedge (Figure 1). Each shape subtended a maximumvisual angle of 8.5 (height) and 11.5 (width). Duringhabituation, the object rotated 15 back and fortharound the vertical axis. It took 4 s to complete a pivotin both directions and return to center. At test,following habituation, the complete object was thesimplest interpretation of the limited-view object:a symmetrical mirror of the faces seen originally.

    Figure 1. The visual stimulus set.Note. Infants were habituated to a wedge-shaped object that rotated back and forth 15 around the vertical axis. During test, infants vieweda complete and an incomplete version of the object on alternating trials, both rotating through 360.

    1232 Soska and Johnson

  • The incomplete object was composed of only the facesseen during habituation. In test displays, it took 10 sfor the object to make a full rotation. The objects werecovered with a brown, wood-like surface and pre-sented against a background consisting of a 12 20grid of white dots, which covered the entire monitor.Informal pilot testing revealed that all the displayswere interpreted as rotating 3D objects by adultobservers.

    Procedure

    Each trial began with the presentation of anattention-getter (a looming and contracting ballaccompanied by a repetitive sequence of tones) toattract the infants attention to the screen and centerthe point of gaze. Once the experimenter determinedthat the infant had fixated the screen, the attention-getter was replaced with the experimental stimulusand timing of each trial began. A trial ended whenthe infant looked away for 2 s or until 60 s of totallooking had accumulated; the attention-getter wasthen shown again, immediately followed by a newtrial. The habituation display (the limited-viewobject) was presented until the infant met a habitua-tion criterion: a decline in looking time during fourconsecutive trials adding up to less than half the totallooking time during the first four trials. Upon meet-ing the criterion, the infant was shown the testdisplays starting on the next trial. Consequently,there was a minimum of five habituation trials, andthe maximum number of trials was set to be 12. Thetwo test displays were seen in alternation three timeseachfor a total of six posthabituation trials. In thecontrol task, the procedure was the same exceptinfants were not habituated to the limited-view objectdisplay first. Instead, infants only saw the two testdisplays in alternation three times each. Infants wererandomly assigned to one of the two test displayorders (incomplete first or complete first) and to thehabituation task or no-habituation control.

    To assure reliability of the experimenters judg-ments, an independent observer coded looking timesoff-line for 25%of the sample. The Pearson correlationbetween the experimenters and the coders judg-ments of the total looking time on each trial was .93,indicating strong agreement between the on-line andoff-line coding.

    Results

    Data consisted of looking times toward the two dis-plays at test. Before analysis, the data were logarith-

    mically transformed due to excessive skew in somecells that violated the assumptions of the analysis ofvariance (ANOVA)data in the figures are based onnontransformed scores. Preliminary analyses re-vealed there were no notable differences in lookingtimes based on sex of infant or order of test displaypresentation, so these factors were not included infurther analyses.

    Figures 2 and 3 show looking-time results of 4- and6-month-olds, respectively. A 2 (age: 4 or 6 months)2 (condition: habituation or no-habituation control)2 (test display: complete or incomplete object) 3 (testtrial block: first, second, or third) mixed-design AN-OVA was performedwith age and conditionas between-participant factors and test displayand test trial block as within-participant factors. Theanalysis revealed a significant main effect of condi-tion, F(1, 44)5 16.58, p, .001, partialg25 .274, due togreater looking overall by infants in the controlcondition; a significant main effect of test display,

    Figure 2. Looking-time results of 4-month-olds.Note. Error bars represent standard error of the means.

    Figure 3. Looking-time results of 6-month-olds.Note. Error bars represent standard error of the means.

    Development of 3D Object Completion 1233

  • F(1, 44) 5 25.11, p , .001, partial g2 5 .363, due togreater looking overall at the incomplete object; anda significant main effect of test trial block, F(2, 88) 526.89, p , .001, partial g2 5 .379, due to an overalldecline in looking across test trials. This latter effectwas mediated by a reliable Test Trial Block Condi-tion interaction, F(2, 88) 5 3.83, p , .05, partial g2 5.08, stemming from a greater decline in looking acrosstrials by infants in the control condition.

    Most important, there was a reliable three-wayinteraction of age, condition, and test display, F(1, 44)5 5.11, p , .05, partial g2 5 .104. Further analysesindicated that this three-way interaction was dueto the presence of a significant Condition TestDisplay interaction for the 6-month-old infants, F(1,22) 5 12.00, p , .01, partial g2 5 .353, but not forthe 4-month-old infants, F(1, 22) 5 .035, ns, partialg2 5 .002. Separate pairwise comparisons in thehabituation and control conditions revealed that4-months-olds showed no reliable preference foreither test display, t(11) , 2.2, p . .05 (see Figure 2).Infants at 6 months of age in the control task showedno preference for either test display, t(11) 5 0.836, ns,though 6-month-old infants in the habituation condi-tion reliably preferred the incomplete display, t(11)56.16, p , .001 (see Figure 3).

    We conclude that infants at 4 months do notperceive completion of the self-occluded surfaces inlimited-view objects. Although previous work hasindicated that 4-month-olds may be sensitive to bothkinetic and static cues to depth (Arterberry & Yonas,2000; Bhatt & Bertin, 2001; Kellman, 1984; Shuwairiet al., 2007), infants at this age provide no evidence ofsensitivity to the full, volumetric forms of limited-view 3D objects under the conditions we provided.Results of 6-month-olds, in contrast, provide positiveevidence for 3D object completion in this age group.

    Discussion

    We investigated developments in 3D object comple-tion: perception of objects as volumetric and completein 3D space when only a limited vantage point isavailable. We habituated infants at 4 and 6 months todisplays depicting objects rotating through 15. Dur-ing test, infants were shown alternating displaysdepicting objects, either complete volumes or hollowfacades, rotating through 360. If infants perceiveda complete volume with self-occluded surfaces whileviewing the limited-vantage object, we reasoned thatthe incomplete test display should have been experi-enced as novel and therefore recruit increased lookingtimes relative to the complete display. The 4-month-

    olds preferred neither test display, providing noevidence for 3D object completion at this age, but6-month-olds showed reliably longer looking times tothe incomplete test display. These older infants pro-vided evidence for perceptual completion of thelimited-view stimulus, inferring the existence ofself-occluded surfaces. Infants at both 4 and 6monthsof age who were not first habituated to the limited-view object showed no reliable preferences for eitherdisplayimplying that intrinsic preferences forincomplete objects can be excluded as an alternativeexplanation of infants performance during the habit-uation tasks.

    Thus, we have evidence for a developmental pro-gression of infants 3D object completion abilities,strengthening theview that object knowledge emergesgradually over the 1st year after birth. Between 4 and 6months of age, infants seem to become increasinglyproficient at forming percepts of complete volumes.The period around 6 months appears to representa transitional period in the development of this visual-cognitive skill, as infants nascent volumetric comple-tion abilities are first becoming evident. Although it isclear that the rudiments of 3D object completionemerge around6months of age, furtherdevelopmentsin this ability may relate to the complexity of the 3Dforms used during testing.

    These findings provide evidence regarding thetime course of infants perception of complete formsthrough self-occlusion to accompany the sizable lit-erature on infants and adults perceptual completionabilities (e.g., Johnson, 2004; Kellman & Shipley, 1991;Tse, 2002). More than simply perceiving and register-ing the visible elements in a scene, perceptual com-pletion through various types of occlusion (Johnson,Bremner, et al., 2003; Kellman & Spelke, 1983) in-volves the workings of sophisticated and complexmechanisms that fit together the visible parts of theworld to form sensible holistic forms. 3D objectcompletion is a perceptual inference that requiresinfants to recognize the unified and reversible aspectsof an object in space.

    Understanding the development of 3D object com-pletion in infancy shapes a framework of how objectknowledge and perception emerge over the 1st yearafter birth. Insights into the general processes ofdevelopment can be gained, as this collective workon visual completion can help inform theories ofcognitive development. For example, previous workon infants visual completion provides strong supportfor a constructivist view of perceptual development(Johnson, 2003, 2004). Continually more complexforms are derived from the processing of smallerunits that are assembled into a fuller percept. The

    1234 Soska and Johnson

  • experiments presented here add to this body ofknowledge, by showing that 3D object completionperception may be built out of lower perceptualknowledgekinetic and pictorial information forthree dimensionality. Interestingly, infants at around6 months of age also begin to use similarity of form toorganize abstract visual patterns (Quinn, Bhatt,Brush, Grimes, & Sharpnack, 2002). Perhaps bottom-up configural processing, such as grouping by form, isdeveloping in tandem with experiential components,supportingmore sophisticated object perception suchas volumetric completion.

    One possibility is that infants around 6 months,who show the first evidence of 3D object completion,may also be starting to tune into the relevant featuresof 3D objects during normal visual experience. Anobject recognition system emerging during the mid-dle of the 1st year (Colombo, 2001) may guide infantsinformation-processing and attentional allocation.Another possibility builds on Piagets (1954) proposalthat object knowledge is constructed in tandem withthe increasing coordination of action systems. Thepresent work on 3D object completion has discovereda perceptual ability whose onset is commensuratewith the 5- to 6-month age range during whichcoordinated visual-manual exploration of objects isknown to arise (Eppler, 1995; Rochat, 1989). As theyplay, infants may discover that objects have hiddensurfaces, revealed via rotation, and build up volu-metric completion from these explorations.

    References

    Aguiar, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1999). 2.5-month-old infantsreasoning about when objects should and should not beoccluded. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 116 157.

    Arterberry, M. E., & Yonas, A. (2000). Perception of three-dimensional shape specified by optic flow by 8-week-old infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 550 556.

    Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3.5 and4.5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23,655 664.

    Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants physical world. CurrentDirections in Psychological Science, 13, 89 94.

    Bhatt, R. S., & Bertin, E. (2001). Pictorial cues and three-dimensional information processing in early infancy.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 315 332.

    Bhatt, R. S., & Waters, S. E. (1998). Perception of three-dimensional cues in early infancy. Journal of ExperimentalChild Psychology, 70, 207 224.

    Bremner, J. G. (1978). Egocentric versus allocentric spatialcoding in nine-month-old infants: Factors influencingthe choice of code. Developmental Psychology, 14,346 355.

    Cohen, L. B., & Oakes, L. M. (1993). How infants perceivea simple causal event.Developmental Psychology, 29, 421433.

    Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention ininfancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 337 367.

    Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural basisof memory functions as indexed by the AB and delayedresponse tasks in human infants and adult monkeys.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 267 317.

    Eppler, M. A. (1995). Development of manipulatory skillsand the deployment of attention. Infant Behavior andDevelopment, 18, 391 405.

    Gibson, E. J. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the develop-ment of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowl-edge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 1 41.

    Johnson, S. P. (2003). The nature of cognitive development.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 102 104.

    Johnson, S. P. (2004). Development of perceptual comple-tion in infancy. Psychological Science, 15, 769 775.

    Johnson, S. P., Amso, D., & Slemmer, J. A. (2003). Devel-opment of object concepts in infancy: Evidence for earlylearning in an eye tracking paradigm. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences (USA), 100, 10568 10573.

    Johnson, S. P., Bremner, J. G., Slater, A., Mason, U.,Foster, K., & Cheshire, A. (2003). Infants perception ofobject trajectories. Child Development, 74, 94 108.

    Johnson, S. P., Cohen, L. B., Marks, K. H., & Johnson, K. L.(2003). Young infants perception of object unity inrotation displays. Infancy, 4, 285 295.

    Kellman, P. J. (1984). Perception of three-dimensional formby human infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 36, 353 358.

    Kellman, P. J., & Shipley, T. F. (1991). A theory of visualinterpolation in object perception. Cognitive Psychology,23, 141 221.

    Kellman, P. J., & Short, K. R. (1987). Development of three-dimensional form perception Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 545 557.

    Kellman, P. J., & Spelke, E. S. (1983). Perception of partlyoccluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15,483 524.

    Kraebel, K. S., & Gerhardstein, P. C. (2006). Three-month-old infants object recognition across changes inviewpoint using an operant learning procedure. InfantBehavior and Development, 29, 11 23.

    Marr, D. (1982). Vision. New York: Freeman.Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. NewYork: Basic Books.

    Quinn, P. C., Bhatt, R. S., Brush, D., Grimes, A., &Sharpnack, H. (2002). Development of form similarityas a Gestalt grouping principle in infancy. PsychologicalScience, 13, 320 328.

    Rieser, J. J. (1979). Spatial orientation of six-month-oldinfants. Child Development, 50, 1078 1087.

    Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 871884.

    Shuwairi, S. M., Albert, M. K., & Johnson, S. P. (2007).Discrimination of possible and impossible objects ininfancy. Psychological Science, 18, 303 307.

    Development of 3D Object Completion 1235

  • Slater, A., Johnson, S. P., Brown, E., & Badenoch, M. (1996).Newborn infants perception of partly occluded objects.Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 145 148.

    Spelke, E. S. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and the originsof knowledge. Infant Behavior and Development, 21,181 200.

    Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K.(1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99,605632.

    Tse, P. U. (2002). A contour propagation approach tosurface filling-in and volume formation. PsychologicalReview, 109, 91 115.

    1236 Soska and Johnson