63
Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Development of Perceptual Organization

Ruth Kimchi

University of Haifa

Leuven, Belgium 2014

Page 2: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Perceptual Organization

The processes structuring the bits and pieces of visual information into coherent units that eventually we experience as environmental objects

Page 3: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Gestalt Psychology

• Organization is achieved by innate grouping and segregation principles

GroupingClassical: proximity, good continuation, similarity, common fate,

closure (Wertheimer, 1923/1955)

New: common region (Palmer, 1992); connectedness (Palmer & Rock, 1994)

Figure-ground segregation

Classical: relative size, contrast, convexity, symmetry (Rubin,1915/1958)

New: familiarity (Peterson & Gibson, 1994), lower region (Vecera et al., 2002), spatial frequency (Klymenko & Weisstein, 1986), top/bottom polarity (base width; Hulleman & Humphreys, 2004), extremal edges (Palmer & Ghose, 2008)

Page 4: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014
Page 5: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Modern theories of visual perception have treated PO as:

• A unitary phenomenon

• Operates at a single, early, preattentive stage

• In a bottom-up fashion

• Provides the substrate on which higher-level perceptual processes operate

(e.g., Julesz, 1981; Marr, 1982; Neisser, 1967; Treisman, 1982, 1988).

Page 6: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

PO: Not a monolithic entity but a multiplicity of processes (Behrmann & Kimchi, 2003; Kimchi, 2003)

• Time course(e.g., Han et al., 2002; Hadad & Kimchi, 2008; Kurylo, 1997; Kimchi, 1998, 2000;

2012; Razpurker-Apfeld & Kimchi, 2007)

• Developmental trajectory(e.g., Hadad & Kimchi, 2006; Hadad et al., 2010; Hayden et al., 2009; Kimchi,

2012; Kimchi et al., 2005; Kovacs, 2000; Scherf et al., 2009; Quinn & Bhatt, 2006)

• Multifaceted relationship with attention(e.g., Ben Av & Sagi, 1995; Freeman et al., 2001; Kimchi & Razpurker-Apfeld,

2004; Kimchi & Peterson, 2008; Kimchi et al., 2007; Moore et al., 2003; Russell &

Driver, 2005; Shomstein et al., 2010 )

• Influenced by experience and familiarity(e.g., Kimchi & Hadad, 2002; Peterson & Gibson, 1994; Vickery & Jiang, 2009;

Zemel et al., 2002)

Page 7: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Ontogenesis of Perceptual Organization: Theoretical Approaches

• Gestalt (Koffka, 1935; Kohler, 1929; Wertheimer, 1923/1958): – most, if not all, of the basic organizational processes

operate from birth.

• Hebb (1949): – perceptual organization is a learned process in which eye

movements that are used to scan images generate the internal representation of objects.

• Contemporary (e.g., Baillargeon et al. 2009; Bhatt & Quinn, 2011; Johnson, 2009; Spelke, 1982): – A combination of innate and learning contributions.

Page 8: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Ontogenesis of Perceptual Organization

• Functional onset

• Rate of development

• The age at which ultimate functioning is attained

Page 9: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Infants Research: • 3-month-olds are capable of grouping visual

elements into unitary structures in accord with both classical and modern organizational principles:

Common motion (Kellman et al., 1987; Kellman & Spelke, 1983; Kellman et al., 1986)

Good continuation (Quinn & Bhatt, 2005a) Proximity (Quinn et al., 2008) Connectedness (Hayden et al., 2006)

Common region (Bhatt et al., 2007)

Lightness similarity (Quinn, et al., 1993) even newborns (Farroni et al., 2000), and 2-month-olds (Farran et al., 2008).

Page 10: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014
Page 11: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Sensitivity to global and local structures3- to 4-month-olds sensitive to both structures,

with a greater sensitivity to global (Freeseman, et al., 1993; Frick, et al., 2000; Ghim & Eimas, 1988; Quinn, et al., 1993; Quinn & Eimas, 1986)

• The ability to perceive the unity of partly occluded objects emerges at 2 months.

• Sensitivity to subjective contours 3- to 4 months.

These completion abilities continue to develop during the first year of life (e.g., Craton, 1996; Csibra, 2001; Eizenman & Bertenthal, 1998; Ghim, 1990; Johnson & Aslin, 1995, 1996; Kavsek, 2002)

Page 12: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Some organizational principles have a later functional onset than other 3-month-olds are insensitive to closure

(Gerhardstein et al, 2004)

Grouping by form similarity only in 6- to 7-month-olds (Quinn & Bhatt, 2006)

Page 13: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

6- to 7-month-olds, but not 3- to 4-month olds (Quinn et al. 2002)

3- to 4-month-olds can use form similarity to organize elements if they are provided with varied examples with which to abstract the invariant arrangement of the pattern (Quinn and Bhatt, 2005).

Grouping by shape similarity

Page 14: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Studies beyond the 1-2 years:

• Protracted developmental trajectory for some perceptual organization abilities, even those that appear to emerge during infancy:

Visual spatial integration (e.g., Hadad & Kimchi, 2006; Hadad et al., 2010; Kaldy & Kovacs, 2003; Kovacs, 2000; Kovacs et al., 1999)

Subjective contours (e.g., Abravanel ,1982; Hadad et al., 2010)

Grouping multiple elements into a global shape (Burack et al., 2000; Enns et al., 2000; Kimchi et al., 2005; Mondloch et al., 2003; Scherf et al., 2009)

Page 15: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Although many perceptual organization abilities emerge early in life, organizational abilities vary in their rate of development and some reach the ultimate level of functioning only in late childhood or even in adolescence.

Page 16: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Two series of studies:

Grouping and individuation of multiple elements in the organization of hierarchical stimuli

Grouping of shape by perceptual closure

Page 17: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Infants studies: Greater sensitivity to global than to local structures (e.g., Ghim & Eimas, 1988).

• Sensitivity to the global structure in hierarchical visual stimuli continues to develop into late childhood (Burack et al., 2000; Enns et al., 2000; Harrison & Stiles, 2009; Porporino et al., 2004;

Scherf et al., 2009).

• Longer developmental progression for processing local structure (Mondloch et al., 2003).

Development of the Perceptual Organization of Hierarchical Stimuli

Page 18: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Performance of 5- 10- and 14-year-olds and young adults on few- and many-element hierarchical displays, was compared in two tasks:

* Visual search

* Speeded classification

Development of the Perceptual Organization of Hierarchical Stimuli

Page 19: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Visual Search

• Task: search for a diamond target among square distractors.• Display size: 2, 6, or 10.• Dependent variables:

Baseline RT (display size=2; measures response speed independent of search rate).Search rate - The slop of the RT function over display size.

Kimchi et al., 2005

Page 20: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014
Page 21: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Accuracy: 5-year olds: 96%10-year olds: 98%14-year olds: 97%Adults: 94%

Results

Page 22: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

0

200

400

600

800

1000

1200

1400

1600

1800

5 10 14 23Age (years)

Res

pons

e Ti

me

(ms)

Few-Global Few-Local

Many-Global Many-Local

Baseline RT (Display Size = 2, Target-present Trials)

• RT improved with age.

• Global advantage in the many-element stimuli.

• No age related changes in target-distractor discriminability.

Kimchi et al., 2005

Page 23: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

-10

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

5 10 14 23Age (years)

RT

Slo

pe

(ms/

item

)Few-Global Few-Local

Many-Global Many-Local • Few-element: Search rates for global target improved with age; the efficient search for local target did not vary with age.

• Many-element: Search rates for local target improved with age; the efficient search for global target did not vary with age.

• Significant improvement between 5 to 10.

RT Slopes (Target-present Trials)

Kimchi et al., 2005

Page 24: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Classify the central stimuli with the stimuli to the right or left side:

• global classification: based on similarity in global configuration

• local classification: based on similarity in local elements

Speeded Classification

Page 25: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

0

5

10

15

5 10 14 22

Err

or

Rat

e (%

)

Age (years)

Few-Global Few-Local

Many-Global Many-Local

• Few-element: Accuracy of global classification improved with age; no age related changes for local classification.

• Many-element: Accuracy of local classification improved with age; no age related changes for global classification.

• Significant improvement between age 5 – 10.

Page 26: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Summary of Results • Search rates for global targets and accuracy of

global classification improved with age for the few-element patterns

did not change with age for the many-element patterns.

• Search rates for local targets and accuracy of local classification improved with age for the many-element patternsno age-related changes for the few-element patterns.

• Improvement mainly for the transition from ages 5 to 10.

Page 27: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Longer developmental progression for grouping a few large elements than many small elements

• Longer developmental progression for individuating many small elements than few large elements.

Page 28: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

How processing might change developmentally to produce this pattern of results?

• Individuating elements within many-element patterns requires attentional change (e.g., narrowing the spatial focus or refocusing on a different detail level). The ability to flexibly deploy attention improves

with age (e.g., Enns & Girgus, 1985; Plude, Enns, & Brodeur, 1994).

• The organization of the individuated few large elements into a global configuration requires apprehending the spatial relations among them. spatial abilities improves with age (e.g., Stiles,

2001).

Page 29: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Enns et al. (2000) used few-element hierarchical stimuli, and therefore concluded that grouping develops with age.

• Mondloch et al. (2003) used many-element stimuli, and therefore concluded that local processing develops with age.

Local Target Global Target

Congruent

Incongruent

Page 30: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• In contrast to the early maturation of grouping many small elements

• Scherf et al. (2009), using primed matching, showed age-related improvement in the ability to encode the global shape of the many-element patterns at the short prime durations, which continued through adolescence.

• Clear early advantage for encoding the global shape was observed only in adults; children were biased to encode the local elements, and adolescents began to demonstrate the beginning of early global advantage.

Page 31: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Scherf, Behrmann, Kimchi, & Luna, 2009

Page 32: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Different tasks may require different representations for successful performance, crude versus more refined, which depend on relatively rudimentary ability to group elements into a shape versus more mature ability, respectively.

Page 33: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• A coarse representation of the global configuration may suffice to support performance in visual search and speeded classification tasks (Kimchi et al., 2005).

• Primed matching task (Scherf et al., 2009) requires a more precise, integrated representation of the global shape of the prime for facilitating responses to test figures similar to the prime in the global shape and/or interfering with responses to the test figures dissimilar in global shape to the prime.

Page 34: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Summary• Children and adolescents are capable of grouping

many small elements to a certain degree, which may support some global information and figural perception, but the full process of integrating local elements into coherent shapes to the extent of facilitating shape identification appears to develop late into adolescence.

• This long developmental trajectory coincides with what is known about the structural and functional development of the ventral visual pathway (Bachevalier, Hagger, & Mishkin, 1991; Gogtay et al., 2004).

Page 35: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Grouping of Shape by Closure  

Page 36: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

The Role of Closure in Perceptual Organization

The Gestalt psychologists noted that perceptual closure plays a crucial role in perceptual organization, in particular, in determining the shape of an object:

“If a line forms a closed, or almost closed, figure, we see no longer merely a line on a homogeneous background, but a surface figure bounded by the line” (Koffka, 1935).

Page 37: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Several psychophysical studies with adults documented the role of closure in perceptual organization (e.g., Elder & Zucker, 1993, 1994, 1998, Hadad & Kimchi, 2008; Kimchi, 2000; Kovacs & Julesz, 1993; Saarinen & Levi,

1999).

• Greater contour detection sensitivity for closed fragmented contours than for open contours (Kovacs & Juleasz, 1993).

• Shape discrimination is more precise for closed contours than for non-closed contours (Saarinen & Levi, 1999).

• Search for a concave target among convex distractors is efficient for closed stimuli but inefficient for open stimuli (Elder & Zucker, 1993).

Page 38: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• In natural scenes, closed connected contours often appear in the image as fragmented, due, for example, to occlusion.

• The perceptual system must group the image fragments and uncover the shape of the object

Page 39: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Research with adults:

The efficiency of the grouping depends on

• The size of the gaps between the closure-inducing fragments (Elder & Zucker, 1993), and on

• The distribution of the gaps along the contour -- whether the gaps occur at points of change in contour direction or at straight contour segments (Kimchi, 2000; Spehar, 2002).

Page 40: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Adults utilize closure and its combination with collinearity and proximity to organize fragmented image contours into shapes.

What is the developmental course of this ability?

Page 41: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Developmental Research

• 3-month-olds are able to rely on good continuation of contour elements, though their ability is far from adult-like, but they appear to be completely insensitive to closure (Gerhardstein, Kovacs, Ditre, & Feher, 2004).

• Kindergartners may be sensitive to closure (Enns & Girgus, 1985).

• We do not know of any study that directly examined the development of the ability to utilize closure for the perceptual grouping of shape in younger and older children.

Page 42: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Comparing the performance of 5- and 10-year-old children and young adults in a visual search task (similar to the one used by Elder and Zucker, 1993)

Task: search for a concave target among convex distractors.

Page 43: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Concave Convex

Basic Stimuli (similar to the ones used by Elder & Zucker, 1993):

The line segments are the same for the concave and convex stimuli but their placement relative to each other differs, bending inward for the concave and outward for the convex.

Therefore, the discrimination between the two stimuli requires grouping of the contour segments into coherent two-dimensional shapes.

Page 44: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Open vs. Closed Stimuli

• Task: search for a concave target among convex distractors.• Display size: 2, 6, or 10.• Dependent variables:

Baseline RT (display size=2; measures response speed independent of search rate).Search rate - The slop of the RT function over display size.

Page 45: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Accuracy:

5-year olds: 95.3%10-year olds: 98.2%Adults: 97.5%

Results

Page 46: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Baseline RT (Display Size = 2, Target-present Trials)

• RT improved with age.• Faster RTs for closed

than for open stimuli.• Larger improvement

with age for the open stimuli, indicating age-related change in target-distractor discriminability.

Hadad & Kimchi, 2006

Page 47: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

RT Slopes (Target-present trials)

• Closed stimuli: Equally efficient search for all age groups.

• Open Stimuli: Inefficient search. Significant improvement with age.

Hadad & Kimchi, in press

Page 48: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Children, like adults, are able to derive the shape of a closed figure, while encountering difficulty when closure is absent.

• The age-related improvement for the

open stimuli: improvement in spatial abilities

Summary of Results

Page 49: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Noncollinear vs. Collinear Stimuli

Noncollinear Collinear

Page 50: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Accuracy:

5-year olds: 96.7%

10-year olds: 98.2%

Adults: 97.7%

Results

Page 51: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Baseline RT (Display Size = 2, Target-present Trials)

500

700

900

1100

1300

1500

1700

1900

5 10 23

Rea

ctio

n T

ime

(ms)

Age (years)

Noncollinear-LG

Noncollinear-SG

Collinear-LG

Collinear-SG

• RT improved with age.• Faster RTs for collinear

stimuli than for noncollinear stimuli.

• No age related changes in target-distractor discriminability for the different stimuli.

Hadad & Kimchi, 2006

Page 52: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

RT Slopes (Target-present trials)

-15

-5

5

15

25

35

45

55

5 10 23

Rea

ctio

n T

ime

Slo

pe

(ms/

item

)

Age (years)

Noncollinear-LG

Noncollinear-SG

Collinear-LG

Collinear-SG

• Noncollinear: Efficient search for small gap and inefficient search for large gap for all age groups.

• Collinear: Efficient search for small gap for all age groups.Significant improvement from age 5 to 10 for large gap.

Hadad & Kimchi, 2006

Page 53: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

10-year-olds and adults:• Spatial proximity between the closure-

inducing line segments influenced grouping into a shape for the non-collinear stimuli, but not for the collinear stimuli

5-year-olds:• Search depended critically on the spatial

proximity between the closure-inducing line segments: efficient when the lines were close, inefficient when the lines were spatially distant, regardless of presence or absence of collinearity.

Page 54: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Young children can utilize closure as efficiently as adults for the perceptual grouping of shape for closed or nearly closed stimuli.

• When the closure-inducing fragments are spatially distant, older children and adults, but not 5-year-olds, can utilize collinearity to enhance closure for the perceptual grouping of shape.

Summary of Results

Page 55: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• The significant improvement in the ability to utilize collinearity for spatially distant fragments between age 5 and 10 suggests a longer developmental progression in the ability to employ long-range contour interpolation (see also, Kovacs, 2000, Kovacs et al., 1999).

• It has been suggested (Kovacs, 2000) that cortical connectivity in layers 2/3 of V1 that seem to be immature even at 5 years of age (Burkhalter, Bernardo, & Charles, 1993), on the one hand, and a delayed development of feedback connections between V1 and V2 (Burkhalter, 1993), on the other, may underlie the lower contour integration ability in young children.

Page 56: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Conclusions

Organizational processes may vary in their developmental trajectory:• Infant research: organizational principles

vary in their functional onset some are functional as early as 3 to 4 months

of age (e.g., common region, connectedness, good continuation, lightness similarity, and proximity

others are not functional until 6 to 7 months of age (e.g., shape similarity).

Page 57: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Studies with older children:• Some organizational processes mature relatively

early: Individuation of a few large elements (Kimchi

et al., 2005)

Grouping spatially close fragments into a shape (Hadad & Kimchi, 2006)

• Other organizational processes develop with age and reach adult-like levels only in late childhood Grouping multiple elements into a global

shape (Kimchi et al., 2005, Scherf et al., 2009) Grouping spatially distant fragments by

collinearity into a shape (Hadad & Kimchi, 2006)

Page 58: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Even processes that appear to emerge early in life may have a long developmental course:

Infant are sensitive to the global configuration of hierarchical stimuli (e.g., Ghim & Eimas, 1988)

The ability to group multiple elements into a global shape continues to develop, and adult-like performance is not observed before the age of 10.

Infant are sensitive to good continuation (e.g., Quinn & Bhatt, 2005)

The ability to group fragments by collinearity matures between ages 5 and 10.

Page 59: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Infants’ behavior reflects some rudimentary organization skills but these skills are not fully mature.

• Grouping involves two distinct processes: – unit formation or clustering determines which elements

belong together and are segregated from other elements

– shape formation or configuring determines how the grouped elements appear as a whole based on the interrelations of the elements (Kimchi & Razpurker-Apfeld,

2004; Koffka, 1935; Rock, 1986; Trick & Enns, 1997).

• Infants may show an early ability to determine what elements cluster together but are much less skilled at organizing such clusters into integrated, distinct shapes.

Page 60: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

The developmental changes in perceptual organization abilities

• May depend on maturation of the visual system

• May be acquired through learning and experience

• May be a function of developmental improvements in other processes, such as flexibility of attention

Page 61: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Organizational abilities are present, at various levels of functioning, across the developmental course, and can support perceptual performance to a certain degree.

Implies that different tasks may tap into different levels of the organizational abilities.

Consequently, depending on the task, adult-like performance may be observed at different ages, sometimes leading to a wrong conclusion regarding the age at which the ability reaches ultimate, adult-like functioning.

Page 62: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

• Batsheva Hadad• Suzy Scherf• Marlene Behrmann• Steve Palmer

• Allegra Dan• Gilad Goldstein

• Israel Science Foundation (ISF)• Israel-US Binational Foundation• Max Wertheimer Minerva Center for Cognitive

Processes and Human Performance

Page 63: Development of Perceptual Organization Ruth Kimchi University of Haifa Leuven, Belgium 2014

Thank You