1
Crossmodal attention alters auditory contrast sensitivity for amplitude and frequency modulated auditory information via a mechanism of contrast gain Vivian M. Ciaramitaro, Hiu Mei Chow & Luke G. Eglington Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston BACKGROUND CONCLUSIONS METHODS Amplitude Modulation (AM) Frequency Modulation (FM) single subject REFERENCES Stimuli Analysis Contrast Gain vs Response Gain Behavioral Measures: Obtain auditory contrast thresholds for both visual attention conditions, the less demanding and more demanding. Fit psychometric functions (Psignitfit 4 toolbox and Matalb) for each condition with a Weibull to determine threshold: the stimulus supporting threshold performance (75% correct), slope: the rate of change to get to threshold, and asymptote: the plateau in performance. If varying attentional load of the concurrent visual task influences auditory detectability, auditory thresholds should differ between the low load (color) and high load (number) visual tasks. RESULTS Auditory stimuli were presented binaurally via headphones in each interval. Only one interval contained a modulated sound: either a sinusoidally amplitude modulated white noise or a frequency modulated 1000Hz tone at 5 or 10Hz. Visual stimuli, a series of 5 letters, were presented concurrently in an RSVP stream at central fixation in each interval. Low Load Visual Color Task: judge which interval has white letters High Load Visual Number Task: judge which interval has more ‘A’s. Auditory Task: judge which interval has modulated sound. Feedback: red (incorrect) green (correct) blue (too slow) Threshold Visual Slope Lapse Auditory across subjects across subjects Percent correct Log (threshold) (mean +/- sem) -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0 Color Number Color Number slope (mean +/- sem) 2 4 6 0 lapse (mean +/- sem) 0 0.05 0.01 0.015 0.2 40 60 80 100 0.075 0.1313 0.2297 0.402 0.7034 0.075 0.1313 0.2297 0.402 0.7034 s4 s4 Percent correct (mean +/- sem) 5Hz 10Hz 50 70 90 Color Number Color Number Frequency modulation index (Auditory Contrast) Percent Correct Log (threshold) (mean +/- sem) 0.2 0.4 0.8 1.6 3.2 40 60 80 100 Color Number s5 s5 Color Number Color Number slope (mean +/- sem) 2 4 6 0 -0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2 0 lapse (mean +/- sem) 0 0.05 0.01 0.015 0.2 0.2 0.4 0.8 1.6 3.2 Percent correct (mean +/- sem) 50 70 90 Color Number Color Number 5Hz 10Hz s1 s2 s3 s4 s5 s6 s7 Mean Amplitude modulation index (Auditory Contrast) Auditory thresholds were lower for the color task (low visual load) versus number task (high visual load) for both AM and FM sounds (p=.006). There was no main effect of visual load or auditory modulation frequency on estimates of lapse rate for either AM or FM sounds. For FM sounds lapse rate tended to be higher for the high vs low visual task (p=.05). Auditory slopes (at threshold) were steeper for the color (low load) versus number (high load) task (p=.046) and steeper for the 10Hz vs 5Hz auditory modulation (p=.03) for FM sounds, with no significant effects for AM sounds. Visual percent correct performance was higher for the color task (low visual load) versus number task (high visual load) for both AM (p=.001) and FM (p=.002) sounds with no main effect of auditory modulation frequency or auditory contrast. W fixate RSVP 500ms 700ms 0.5 1 percent correct auditory amplitude modulation (auditory contrast) threshold (~75% correct) 0. 02 0. 04 0. 06 0. 08 1 0.53 0.68 from Carrasco, Progress in Brain Research (2006) A ISI RSVP visual response visual feedback auditory response auditory feedback 500ms 600ms 1000ms 1000ms W RSVP ISI 500ms 500ms 600ms LOW LOAD VISUAL TASK TIME 100ms 100ms low load visual high load visual A HIGH LOAD VISUAL TASK RSVP To determine if varying attentional load can influence the detectability of an auditory stimulus, we used a two interval forced choice paradigm (2IFC) to obtain auditory contrast detection thresholds for ampltiude as well as frequency modulated sounds under two attention conditions. We varied attentional load across blocks of trials by varying the difficulty of a simultaneous visual task. Either the visual task was less demanding or more demanding. Subjects completed 10 blocks (1125 trials) of each of two attention conditions over several days, within a 2 week window. Each session lasting 1-2 hours. Attending to a visual stimulus can increase perceived contrast and alter the visual contrast response function via a mechanism of increasing contrast gain, as found for sustained visual attention, or via a combination of response gain and contrast gain, as found for transient visual attention (see Carrasco 2006 for a review). It is unclear if sensitivity to auditory contrast can be altered via attentional mechanisms found to be important for visual processing. Here we investigate covert, sustained, crossmodal attention and ask: (1) How does crossmodal attention, varying task difficulty of a concurrent visual task, or visual attentional load, alter auditory performance? (2) Are changes in auditory contrast sensitivity seen with crossmodal attention similar to the effects of visual attention on visual contrast, namely, changes in response gain vs contrast gain? Subjects required a greater degree of modulation in the auditory stimulus, more auditory contrast, to achieve the same level of auditory performance when simultaneously performing the more demanding number task (high visual load) versus the less demanding color task (low visual load). This attention effect was seen in both maniipulations of auditory contrast: amplitude (AM) and frequency (FM) modulated sounds. Our results suggest that (1) Attention can be a limited resource across modalities, such that devoting more attention to a concurrent visual task can result in a deterioration in auditory processing. (2) Attention effects in the auditory domain can be similar to effects found in the visual domain. Our covert crossmodal sustained attention manipulation suggests that attention acts on auditory contrast via a mechanism of contrast gain, shifting the contrast response to the left when the visual task is more demanding, with no significant change in lapse rate. The effects of visual task demand on FM contrast detection is driven by changes in auditory threshold and slope but not lapse rate. The effects of visual task demand on AM contrast detection is driven by changes in auditory threshold but not slope nor lapse rate, Bonnel, A-M, Hafter, ER (1998) Divided attention between simultaneous auditory and visual signals. Perception amd Psychophysics, 17-190. Carrasco, M (2006) Covert atention increases contrast sensitivity: psychophysical, neuro- physiological and imaging studies. Progress in Brain Research, 33-70. McDonald, JS, Lavie, N (2011) Visual attentional load induces inattentional deafness. Atten- tion, Perception and Psychophysics, 1780-9. Ruthruff, E, Van Selst, M, Johnston, JC, Remington, RW (2006) How does practice reduce dual task interference: Integration, automatization or simply stage-shortening? Psychologi- cal Research,125-142. This work was supported in part by a UMass Boston Healey grant to vmc.

Crossmodal attention alters auditory contrast sensitivity ... · Vivian M. Ciaramitaro, Hiu Mei Chow & Luke G. Eglington Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Crossmodal attention alters auditory contrast sensitivity ... · Vivian M. Ciaramitaro, Hiu Mei Chow & Luke G. Eglington Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston

Crossmodal attention alters auditory contrast sensitivity for amplitude and frequency modulated auditory information via a mechanism of contrast gain

Vivian M. Ciaramitaro, Hiu Mei Chow & Luke G. EglingtonDepartment of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston

BACKGROUND

CONCLUSIONSMETHODS

Amplitude Modulation (AM) Frequency Modulation (FM)

single subject

REFERENCES

Stimuli

Analysis

Contrast Gain vs Response Gain

Behavioral Measures: Obtain auditory contrast thresholds for both visual attention conditions, the less demanding and more demanding. Fit psychometric functions (Psignitfit 4 toolbox and Matalb) for each condition with a Weibull to determine threshold: the stimulus supporting threshold performance (75% correct), slope: the rate of change to get to threshold, and asymptote: the plateau in performance.

If varying attentional load of the concurrent visual task influences auditory detectability, auditory thresholds should differ between the low load (color) and high load (number) visual tasks.

RESULTS

Auditory stimuli were presented binaurally via headphones in each interval. Only one interval contained a modulated sound: either a sinusoidally amplitude modulated white noise or a frequency modulated 1000Hz tone at 5 or 10Hz. Visual stimuli, a series of 5 letters, were presented concurrently in an RSVP stream at central fixation in each interval. Low Load Visual Color Task: judge which interval has white letters

High Load Visual Number Task: judge which interval has more ‘A’s.

Auditory Task: judge which interval has modulated sound.

Feedback: red (incorrect) green (correct) blue (too slow)

Thre

shol

d

Visu

al

Sl

ope

Laps

e

Au

dito

ry

across subjects

across subjects

Perc

ent c

orre

ctLo

g (th

resh

old)

(mea

n +/

- sem

)

-0.8

-0.6

-0.4

-0.2

0

Color Number Color Number

slop

e(m

ean

+/- s

em)

2

4

6

0

laps

e(m

ean

+/- s

em)

0

0.05

0.01

0.015

0.2

40

60

80

100

0.075 0.1313 0.2297 0.402 0.7034 0.075 0.1313 0.2297 0.402 0.7034

s4 s4Pe

rcen

t cor

rect

(mea

n +/

- sem

)

5Hz 10Hz

50

70

90

Color Number Color Number

Frequency modulation index (Auditory Contrast)

Per

cent

Cor

rect

Log

(thr

esho

ld)

(mea

n +/

- sem

)

0.2 0.4 0.8 1.6 3.2 40

60

80

100Color Number s5 s5

Color Number Color Number

slo

pe(m

ean

+/- s

em)

2

4

6

0

-0.8

-0.6

-0.4

-0.2

0

laps

e(m

ean

+/- s

em)

0

0.05

0.01

0.015

0.2

0.2 0.4 0.8 1.6 3.2

Perc

ent c

orre

ct(m

ean

+/- s

em)

50

70

90

Color Number Color Number

5Hz 10Hzs1s2s3s4s5s6s7Mean

Amplitude modulation index (Auditory Contrast)

Auditory thresholds were lower for the color task (low visual load) versus number task (high visual load) for both AM and FM sounds (p=.006).

There was no main effect of visual load or auditory modulation frequency on estimates of lapse rate for either AM or FM sounds. For FM sounds lapse rate tended to be higher for the high vs low visual task (p=.05).

Auditory slopes (at threshold) were steeper for the color (low load) versus number (high load) task (p=.046) and steeper for the 10Hz vs 5Hz auditory modulation (p=.03) for FM sounds, with no significant effects for AM sounds.

Visual percent correct performance was higher for the color task (low visual load) versus number task (high visual load) for both AM (p=.001) and FM (p=.002) sounds with no main effect of auditory modulation frequency or auditory contrast.

W

�xate

RSVP

500ms

700ms

0.5

1

perc

ent c

orre

ct

auditory amplitude modulation (auditory contrast)

threshold (~75% correct)

0.02 0.04 0.06 0.08 1

0.53 0.68

from Carrasco, Progress in Brain Research (2006)

A

ISI

RSVP

visualresponse

visualfeedback

auditoryresponse

auditoryfeedback

500ms

600ms

1000ms

1000ms

W

RSVP

ISI

500ms

500ms

600ms

LOW LOAD VISUAL TASKTIME

100ms

100ms

low loadvisual high load

visual

A

HIGH LOAD VISUAL TASKRSVP

To determine if varying attentional load can influence the detectability of an auditory stimulus, we used a two interval forced choice paradigm (2IFC) to obtain auditory contrast detection thresholds for ampltiude as well as frequency modulated sounds under two attention conditions. We varied attentional load across blocks of trials by varying the difficulty of a simultaneous visual task. Either the visual task was less demanding or more demanding.

Subjects completed 10 blocks (1125 trials) of each of two attention conditions over several days, within a 2 week window. Each session lasting 1-2 hours.

Attending to a visual stimulus can increase perceived contrast and alter the visual contrast response function via a mechanism of increasing contrast gain, as found for sustained visual attention, or via a combination of response gain and contrast gain, as found for transient visual attention (see Carrasco 2006 for a review). It is unclear if sensitivity to auditory contrast can be altered via attentional mechanisms found to be important for visual processing.

Here we investigate covert, sustained, crossmodal attention and ask:(1) How does crossmodal attention, varying task difficulty of a concurrent visual task, or visual attentional load, alter auditory performance?

(2) Are changes in auditory contrast sensitivity seen with crossmodal attention similar to the effects of visual attention on visual contrast, namely, changes in response gain vs contrast gain?

Subjects required a greater degree of modulation in the auditory stimulus, more auditory contrast, to achieve the same level of auditory performance when simultaneously performing the more demanding number task (high visual load) versus the less demanding color task (low visual load).

This attention effect was seen in both maniipulations of auditory contrast: amplitude (AM) and frequency (FM) modulated sounds.

Our results suggest that(1) Attention can be a limited resource across modalities, such that devoting more attention to a concurrent visual task can result in a deterioration in auditory processing.

(2) Attention effects in the auditory domain can be similar to effects found in the visual domain. Our covert crossmodal sustained attention manipulation suggests that attention acts on auditory contrast via a mechanism of contrast gain, shifting the contrast response to the left when the visual task is more demanding, with no significant change in lapse rate.

The effects of visual task demand on FM contrast detection is driven by changes in auditory threshold and slope but not lapse rate.

The effects of visual task demand on AM contrast detection is driven by changes in auditory threshold but not slope nor lapse rate,

Bonnel, A-M, Hafter, ER (1998) Divided attention between simultaneous auditory and visual signals. Perception amd Psychophysics, 17-190.

Carrasco, M (2006) Covert atention increases contrast sensitivity: psychophysical, neuro-physiological and imaging studies. Progress in Brain Research, 33-70.

McDonald, JS, Lavie, N (2011) Visual attentional load induces inattentional deafness. Atten-tion, Perception and Psychophysics, 1780-9.

Ruthruff, E, Van Selst, M, Johnston, JC, Remington, RW (2006) How does practice reduce dual task interference: Integration, automatization or simply stage-shortening? Psychologi-cal Research,125-142.

This work was supported in part by a UMass Boston Healey grant to vmc.