2
Bulletin of the Psy chonomic Societ y 1975, Vol. 5 (4). 331-332 Conditioned aversion to morphine with lithium chloride in morphine-dependent rats B. FERNANDEZ and J. W. TERNES Caribbean Primate Research Center, University of Puerto Rico Sabana Seca, Puerto Rico 00794 Two groups of IBO-day-old male Wistar rats which had learned to drink morphine solution in order,to avoid withdrawal stress received two separate pairings of morphine taste with either IP injection of Li'Cl or physiological saline. A two-bottle extinction test between morphine solution and tap water was given on the day separating the two conditioning trials and for 4 days following the second conditioning trial. An ANOVA indicated no significant main effects; however, the Groups by Days interaction was significant. Tests of simple main effects of groups were significant on the days following each conditioning trial. Thus, long-term familiarity with the test substance did not block conditioning. The results are discussed as an example of prepared learning. Bolles (1972) has suggested the existence of an innate recognition mechanism which precluded the necessity of learning a preference for special food flavors that satisfy most naturally occurring need states. However, Parker, Failor, and Weidman (1973) demonstrated acquired preferences for a novel flavor associated with beneficial p ostin gestional consequences when morphine withdrawal was the relevant need state. They suggest that, with unnatural need states, there exists no such innate recognition mechanism for repletion. A striking example of this can be seen in the work of Stolerrnan and Kumar (1 970). TIley repeatedly gave both morphine and quinine solutions to thirsty rats. Initial aversions for bitt er morphine were converted into preferences while consumption of bitter quinine was unaffected. The authors suggested that relief from thirst, a natural need state, did not accout for the acquired preference for morphine, whereas, relief from withdrawal stress, an unnatural need state, was apparently responsible. Ternes (in press) demonstrated that rats forced to drink weak solutions of morphine during a 12-week period will subsequently self-administer morphine by drinking it in preference to water when both are made concurrently available. This acquired preference for morphine has been interpreted as evidence of morphine dependency. The long induction period suggests that rats are relat ively unpr epared to leam to associate the bitter taste of morphine with the beneficial postingestional consequences of relief from withdrawal stress. The purpose of the present experiment was to test the hypothesis that conditioned aversion (Garcia, Kimeldorf, & Koelling, 1955) can be used as a method for weaning morphine dependent rats from their drug. We here present data suggesting that short-term aversions to morphine taste were produced in dependent rats which had previously learned to drink weak morphinized This research is based on a Master's thesis submitted to the Un iversity of Puerto Rico by the first author under the direction of the second author. Reprints may be obtained from J. W. Ternes, Department of Psychology, Rio Piedras, Puerto Rico. solutions in order to avoid withdrawal stress. METHOD Subjects Subje cts were 10 male Wistar rat s which were morphine dependent. Throughout the experiment, the subjects were housed in standard Hoeltge wire rat cages in an open-air animal shelter at the Caribbean Primate Research Center, Sabana Seca, Puerto Rico. Procedure The dependency induction and preference test procedure has been described elsewhere (Terne s, in press). Basically, the induct ion procedure involved providing subjects with a solution of.3 mg morphine sulphate per milliliter of tap water as the only source of liquid for 84 days. Preference for morphine solution over water in an 8-day two-bottle preference test was interpreted as evidence that morphine dependence had been acquired. Immediately following th e preference test subjects were randomly assigned to two groups of five rats each. The first day of the experiment subjects were weighed and deprived of food and liquid for 8-h. Subsequently a .3 mg/rnl morphinized solut ion was placed on each cage. Two hours later the experimental subjects were injected intraperiotoneally (lP) with a .15-M solutio n of lithium chloride (LiCl) equivalent to 2% of subject's body weight. Control subjects were injected with equivalent doses of phy siological saline. One hour after injection the morphine solution was removed from the cages. Subject s were deprived until the followin g mornin g when they were provided with food and liquid for l -h, Subsequently they were deprived until 4:00 p.m. that day when a two-bottle extinction test between .3 mg/rnl morphiniz ed solution and wat er was initiated. The following day, the same deprivation, conditioning deprivation sequence was repeated. Following the second conditioning trial, extinction testing was continued for 4 consecutive days. Thus, there was a total of 5 days of extinction testing, with the first day separating the 2 conditioning trial days, Data from the five postconditioning extin ction tests were converted into daily morph ine preferen ce scores by determining the p er cen tage of the t ot al l iqu id co nsumption (morphine + water) that was morphin e during each 24-h period. The data from the first extinction test, i.e. the test given on the day following the first conditioning trial, was included in the analysis. 331

Conditioned aversion to morphine with lithium chloride in morphine-dependent rats

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Conditioned aversion to morphine with lithium chloride in morphine-dependent rats

Bulletin of the Psy chonomic Society1975, Vol. 5 (4) . 331-332

Conditioned aversion to morphinewith lithium chloride in morphine-dependent rats

B. FERNANDEZ and J. W. TERNESCaribbean Primate Research Center, University ofPuerto Rico

Sabana Seca, Puerto Rico 00794

Two groups of IBO-day-old male Wistar rats which had learned to drink morphine solution in order,toavoid withdrawal stress received two separate pairings of morphine taste with either IP injection of Li'Clor physiological saline. A two-bottle extinction test between morphine solution and tap water was givenon the day separating the two conditioning trials and for 4 days following the second conditioning trial.An ANOVA indicated no significant main effects; however, the Groups by Days interaction wassignificant. Tests of simple main effects of groups were significant on the days following eachconditioning trial. Thus, long-term familiarity with the test substance did not block conditioning. Theresults are discussed as an example of prepared learning.

Bolles (1972) has suggested the existence of an innaterecognition mechanism which precluded the necessity oflearning a preference for special food flavors that satisfymost naturally occurring need states. However, Parker,Failor, and Weidman (1973) demonstrated acquiredpreferences for a novel flavor associated with beneficialpostingestional consequences when morphinewithdrawal was the relevant need state . They suggestthat, with unnatural need states, there exists no suchinnate recognition mechanism for repletion. A strikingexample of this can be seen in the work of Stolerrnanand Kumar (1970). TIley repeatedly gave both morphineand qu inine solutions to thirsty rats. Initial aversions forbitt er morphine were converted into preferences whileconsumption of bitter quinine was unaffected . Theauthors suggested that relief from thirst , a natural needstate , did not accout for the acquired preference formorphine, whereas, relief from withdrawal stress , anunn atural need state, was apparently responsible .

Ternes (in press) demonstrated that rats forced todrink weak solutions of morphine during a 12-weekperiod will subsequently self-administer morphine bydrinking it in preference to water when both are madeconcurrently available. This acquired preference formorphine has been interpreted as evidence of morphinedependency . The long induction period suggests that ratsare relat ively unpr epared to leam to associate the bittertaste of morphine with the beneficial postingestionalconsequences of relief from withdrawal stress.

The purpose of the present experimen t was to test thehypothesis that conditioned aversion (Garcia, Kimeldorf,& Koell ing, 1955) can be used as a method for weaningmorphine dependent rats from their drug. We herepresent data suggesting that short-term aversions tomorphine taste were produced in dependent rats whichhad previously learned to drink weak morphinized

This research is based on a Master's thesis submitted to theUniversity of Puerto Rico by the first author under the directionof the second author. Reprints may be obtained from J. W.Ternes, Department of Psychology, Rio Piedras, Puerto Rico.

solutions in order to avoid withdrawal stress .

METHOD

Subjects

Subje cts were 10 male Wistar rat s which were morphinedependent. Throughout the experiment, the subjects werehoused in standard Hoeltge wire rat cages in an open-air animalshelter at the Caribbean Primate Research Center, Sabana Seca,Puerto Rico.

Procedure

The dependency indu ction and preference test procedure hasbeen described elsewhere (Terne s, in press). Basically, theinduct ion procedure involved providing subjects with a solutionof.3 mg morphine sulphate per milliliter of tap water as the onlysource of liquid for 84 days. Preference for morphine solutionover water in an 8-day two-bottle preference test was interpretedas evidence that morphine dependence had been acquired.

Immediately following th e preference test subjects wererandomly assigned to two groups of five rats each . The first dayof the experiment subjects were weighed and deprived of foodand liquid for 8-h. Subsequently a .3 mg/rnl morphinizedsolut ion was placed on each cage. Two hours later theexperimental subjects were injected intraperiotoneally (lP) witha .15-M solutio n of lithium chloride (LiCl) equivalent to 2% ofsubject's body weight. Control subjects were injected withequivalent doses of phy siological saline. One hour after injectionthe morphine solution was removed from the cages. Subject swere deprived until the following morning when they wereprovided with food and liquid for l -h, Subsequently they weredeprived until 4 :00 p.m. that day when a two-bottle extinctiontest between .3 mg/rnl morphinized solution and water wasinitiated. The following day, the same deprivation, conditioningdepr ivation sequence was repeated. Following the secondconditioning trial, extinction testing was continued for 4consecutive days. Thu s, there was a total of 5 days of extinctiontesting, with the first day separating the 2 conditioning tr ialdays, Data from the five postcond itioning extin ction tests wereconverted into daily morph ine preferen ce scores by determiningthe percen tage o f the total l iqu id co nsumpt ion(morphine + water) that was morphin e during each 24-h period .The data from the first extinction test , i.e. the test given on theday following the first conditioning trial, was included in theanalysis.

331

Page 2: Conditioned aversion to morphine with lithium chloride in morphine-dependent rats

332 FER.'\.-\:\DEZ A.'\D TERT\ES

RESULTS

A 2 by 8 analysis of variance (A.\TOVA) with repeatedmeasures on the second factor (Winer, 1962) indicatedth at there were no significant differences between thetwo groups prior to condi tioning. Figure 1 (le ft panel)shows the profile of the preconditioning group morphinescores .

A separate 2 by 5 ANOVA with repeated measures ont h e second factor was perfo rmed on thepost conditioning data . TIle main effec ts of groups (F =2.26, df = 1/8 , r > .17) and of days (F =1.9, df =4/32 ,p > .13) were not significant. The Groups by Daysinteraction was significant (F = 3.276 , df = 4/32,p < .024) . Figure 1 (right panel) shows the profile of thepostconditioning group morphine scores . The liCl groupis consistently lower than the saline group , with thelargest differences occurring on the days following thetwo conditioning trials . In tests of simple main effects,groups were significant only on Day 1 (p < .01) andDay 2 (p < .05).

DISCUSSION

The significant Groups by. Days, interaction indicates atransient conditioning effect whi ch extinguished within 4 days.Figure I , the profile of th e daily gro up means , shows tha t theaverison was obse rvab le on ly on the day aft er each of the twoco ndi tio ning tr ials.

Typicall y. gustato ry conditio ning produces aversions to noveltaste stirn uli. Familiarization with the tes t substa nce is usuall ythought to attenuat e the aversion . Elk ins (1973) for example ,man ipulated flavor availability prior to conditioning andobtained a grad ed effect upon bo th acq uisition and resistance toextinct ion with :20 days of precondi tion ing expos ure beingsufficien t to co mpletely blo ck aversio n formation. In the presentexperiment , subjec ts had daily experience with the test so lutionover a 3-month period and, because of their dependency uponthis solut ion for the maintenance of their habit, ingestedsignific ant quan tities of it every da y. Th us, the ·co nditio ning ofan aver sion af ter such long fami liarization with the test flavorsuggests that rat s are highly prepared to make the associat ion ofbitter morphine taste with illness , the attenuating effe ct s offamiliar ity no t withstan ding.

The dire consequence wh ich morphine abs tine nce represent sto the dependent organism wou ld fu rthe r ex plain the transitoryna ture of the aversion in the pre sen t study. Seldom ha s a

w 100uz~ 80

~wb ' ti 'e::

60Q.. f,/)

~~-u 40iff,/)

~ 20 - U CI EXPERIMENTAl

Z ..... SAliNE CONTROL<:w~

1234 51 234 5PRECONDITIONING POSTCQt\[)lTIOOING

DAYS

Figure 1. Group mean preferences for morphine prior toconditioning (left panel) and following conditioning (rightpanel).

conditioned aversion been placed under such extreme pre ssure toextinguish in a two-bottle preferen ce test . According to Wikler(1953) maximum withdrawal stress occ urs be tween 48- h and96-h after the last dose of morphine . Since extreme withdrawalstress is proba bly more aversive than co ndi tioned LiCi illness . itis not sur prising that the aversions were co mpletely ext inguishedin all subjec ts within 4 days of the final conditio ning tri al.

REFERENCES

Bolles. R. C. Reinforcement, e xp ectancy, and learning.psychological R eview, 1972. 79, 394-410.

Elkins, R. L . Attenuation of drug-induced bait sh yness to apalatable solution as an increasing function of its av ailab ilityprior to conditioning. Journal of Beh avio ral Biology, 19 73, 9 ,221·226.

Garcia, J ., Kimeldorf, D. J ., & Koelling, R. A. Conditionedaversion to saccharin result ing from exposure to gammairradiation . Science, 1955. 122, 157-158 .

Park er, L ., Failor, A.. & Weidman, K. Conditioned preferences inthe rat with an unnatural n eed stat e : Morphine withdrawal .J ournal of Comparative and Ph vsiol og tcal Ps ycholog y. 1973.82. 294-300.

Stolerman, 1. P.• & Kumar, R. Preferences for morphine in rats:Validation of an experimental model of dependence.Psv ehopharmacologia, 1970, 17. 137-150 .

Ternes, J . W. Induced preference for morphine in rats. Th eBulletin of the Psychonomic Society , 1975 , in press.

Wikler, A. Opiate addiction. Springfield , Ill: Thomas, 1953 .Winer, B. J . Stat istical principles in experimen tal desig n. Ne w

York: McGraw·Hill , 1962.

(Received for publication November 6.1974.)