9
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 425 Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna, August 22-26 2006 Conceptions of musical ability Susan Hallam Institute of Education, University of London, UK. [email protected] ABSTRACT Historically, musical ability has been conceptualized in relation to aural abilities. Recently, this view has been challenged. Musical ability is now viewed by a number of authors as a social construction acquiring different meaning in different cultures, sub-groups within cultures and at the individual level. This study aimed to explore conceptions of musical ability in the United Kingdom. An inventory adopting a five point rating scale was developed to assess individuals’ perceptions of musical ability. The inventory included 77 statements derived from previous qualitative research categorized into 21 themes (Hallam and Prince, 2003). The inventory was completed by 660 individuals. Musical ability was most strongly perceived as relating to a sense of rhythm, followed by the ability to understand and interpret the music, express thoughts and feelings through sound, being able to communicate through sound, motivation to engage with music, personal commitment to music, and being able to successfully engage musically with others. Least important were having technical skills, being able to compose or improvise, being able to read music, and understanding musical concepts and musical structures. Factor analysis revealed 6 factors. The first was concerned with being able to read music and play an instrument or sing. The second focused on musical communication, the third valuing, appreciating and responding to music, and the fourth to composition, improvisation and the skills required for undertaking these. The fifth factor related to personal commitment to music, motivation, discipline and organisation, and the sixth to rhythmic and aural skills. The findings suggest that the construct of musical ability is perceived broadly in the general population. The high proportion of participants stressing the importance of having a sense of rhythm may reflect the characteristics of popular music where ‘the beat’ is central. The stress on motivation and commitment also suggests an awareness of the time required to successfully develop musical skills. INTRODUCTION The concept of musical ability has a long history. The development of tests to assess ‘musical ability’ paralleled that of intelligence testing. In the early and mid-twentieth century, there was an assumption that individuals were endowed with different levels of ‘intelligence’ that were genetically based, relatively immutable and unchanging. Such measures of intelligence have continued to be used to identify individuals with learning difficulties and sometimes in situations where it is necessary to select individuals for limited educational or employment opportunities. In parallel with intelligence tests, musical ability tests were first developed to assist music teachers in the selection of those pupils most likely to benefit from music tuition. Testing began in 1883 when Carl Stumpf suggested a number of simple aural tests which music teachers might undertake to select pupils. Subsequently, a range of tests have been developed which can be administered to groups of children of different ages. The content of the tests varies although they all focus on aural skills (for reviews see Shuter-Dyson, 1999; Hallam, 2006). The most comprehensive set of measures is that of Gordon (1965, 1979, 1982, 1989a, 1989b) who has devised tests to be used with pre-school children through to adults, taking account of prevailing cultural norms based on tonal imagery, rhythm imagery and musical sensitivity. Recent testing procedures reflect technological advances. Individualised computer based In: M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (2006) Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition (ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 22-26 2006.©2006 The Society for Music Perception & Cognition (SMPC) and European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Copyright of the content of an individual paper is held by the primary (first-named) author of that paper. All rights reserved. No paper from this proceedings may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval systems, without permission in writing from the paper's primary author. No other part of this proceedings may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval system, without permission in writing from SMPC and ESCOM.

Conception of Musical Ability

  • Upload
    sijon

  • View
    32

  • Download
    3

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Conception of Musical Ability

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 425

Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna, August 22-26 2006

Conceptions of musical ability

Susan Hallam Institute of Education, University of London, UK. [email protected]

ABSTRACT

Historically, musical ability has been conceptualized in

relation to aural abilities. Recently, this view has been

challenged. Musical ability is now viewed by a number of

authors as a social construction acquiring different meaning

in different cultures, sub-groups within cultures and at the

individual level. This study aimed to explore conceptions

of musical ability in the United Kingdom. An inventory

adopting a five point rating scale was developed to assess

individuals’ perceptions of musical ability. The inventory

included 77 statements derived from previous qualitative

research categorized into 21 themes (Hallam and Prince,

2003). The inventory was completed by 660 individuals.

Musical ability was most strongly perceived as relating to a

sense of rhythm, followed by the ability to understand and

interpret the music, express thoughts and feelings through

sound, being able to communicate through sound,

motivation to engage with music, personal commitment to

music, and being able to successfully engage musically

with others. Least important were having technical skills,

being able to compose or improvise, being able to read

music, and understanding musical concepts and musical

structures. Factor analysis revealed 6 factors. The first was

concerned with being able to read music and play an

instrument or sing. The second focused on musical

communication, the third valuing, appreciating and

responding to music, and the fourth to composition,

improvisation and the skills required for undertaking these.

The fifth factor related to personal commitment to music,

motivation, discipline and organisation, and the sixth to

rhythmic and aural skills. The findings suggest that the

construct of musical ability is perceived broadly in the

general population. The high proportion of participants

stressing the importance of having a sense of rhythm may

reflect the characteristics of popular music where ‘the beat’

is central. The stress on motivation and commitment also

suggests an awareness of the time required to successfully

develop musical skills.

INTRODUCTION

The concept of musical ability has a long history. The

development of tests to assess ‘musical ability’ paralleled

that of intelligence testing. In the early and mid-twentieth

century, there was an assumption that individuals were

endowed with different levels of ‘intelligence’ that were

genetically based, relatively immutable and unchanging. Such

measures of intelligence have continued to be used to identify

individuals with learning difficulties and sometimes in

situations where it is necessary to select individuals for

limited educational or employment opportunities. In parallel

with intelligence tests, musical ability tests were first

developed to assist music teachers in the selection of those

pupils most likely to benefit from music tuition. Testing

began in 1883 when Carl Stumpf suggested a number of

simple aural tests which music teachers might undertake to

select pupils. Subsequently, a range of tests have been

developed which can be administered to groups of children

of different ages. The content of the tests varies although

they all focus on aural skills (for reviews see Shuter-Dyson,

1999; Hallam, 2006). The most comprehensive set of

measures is that of Gordon (1965, 1979, 1982, 1989a,

1989b) who has devised tests to be used with pre-school

children through to adults, taking account of prevailing

cultural norms based on tonal imagery, rhythm imagery

and musical sensitivity. Recent testing procedures reflect

technological advances. Individualised computer based

In: M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (2006) Proceedings

of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition

(ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 22-26 2006.©2006 The Society for

Music Perception & Cognition (SMPC) and European Society for the

Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Copyright of the content of an

individual paper is held by the primary (first-named) author of that

paper. All rights reserved. No paper from this proceedings may be

reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or

mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information

retrieval systems, without permission in writing from the paper's

primary author. No other part of this proceedings may be reproduced or

transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical,

including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval

system, without permission in writing from SMPC and ESCOM.

Page 2: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 426

systems can assess the recognition of change in

synthesiser-produced melodies and allow for individual

speed of responding increasing validity and reliability

(Vispoel, 1993; Vispoel & Coffman, 1992) through

minimising the reliance on the general cognitive processing

skills needed to perform well on earlier tests (Doxey &

Wright, 1990). What these various tests have in common is

that they assess the ability to discriminate sounds that vary

in subtle ways.

The devisers of the various musical ability measures held

different beliefs about the nature of musical ability. Revesz

(1953) adopted the term ‘musicality’ to denote the ‘ability

to enjoy music aesthetically’ which was assessed by

establishing the depth to which a person could listen to and

comprehend the artistic structure of a composition.

Seashore (Seashore et al, 1960) believed that musical

ability was a set of loosely related basic sensory

discrimination skills, which had a genetic basis and would

not change over time except for variation due to lapses of

concentration or other environmental changes. He did not

believe that subtest scores should be combined to obtain a

single score, but rather that a profile should be obtained

which could be divided into a number of clearly defined

characteristics which were unrelated to each other (pitch,

loudness, rhythm, time, timbre, tonal memory). In contrast,

Wing (1981) believed in a general ability to perceive and

appreciate music rather than a profile. He believed that the

elements in his battery of tests should be related to each

other and an overall score should be reported. Gordon

(1979) viewed musical ability as consisting of three parts,

tonal imagery (melody and harmony), rhythm imagery

(tempo and metre) and musical sensitivity (phrasing,

balance and style). His tests contrasted with earlier work in

that musical ability was viewed in part as sensitivity to the

prevailing musical cultural norms.

The concept of musical ability has been severely criticised

in recent years. Focussing on the importance of effort,

some have proposed that it is time spent practising which

underpins the development of expert performance, not

inherited ability. Ericsson and colleagues (1993) suggested

a monotonic relationship between ‘deliberate practice’ and

an individual’s acquired performance, a relationship

supported by Sloboda and colleagues (1996). Interviews

with the parents of the children in Sloboda et al’s study

revealed that singing by the child at an early age was the

only sign that distinguished those children who later

succeeded in being accepted by a high status music school

reinforcing the practice explanation (Howe, Davidson,

Moore & Sloboda, 1995). However, not all the evidence is

supportive. Sloboda and Howe (1991) found that students

identified as having greater ability by their teachers had

undertaken less practice in their main instrument, their

practice time having been spread more equally across three

instruments. Wagner (1975) found that increased practice

did not lead to any greater improvement in performance

over an eight-week period and Zurcher (1972) found no

relationship between total practice time and performance

achievement. Reported correlations between achievement

and time spent practising also vary considerably and are

only moderate (Sloboda et al., 1996). In addition, skills can

be developed through playful practice and playing in

groups, not only through deliberate practice. Social factors

such as parental support, teacher’s personality, and peer

interactions have also been shown to be more important

than amount of practice time in achieving a high level of

musical performance (Moore et al., 2003).

There has also been a tendency in much of this research to

neglect the issue of drop-outs – those who may have

undertaken extensive practice, been unsuccessful and

dropped out. While Sloboda and colleagues (1996)

demonstrated that those who had dropped out had

undertaken less practice and achieved less than those who

continued, in much of the research on drop-outs no single

explanatory factor has emerged. Rather a number of

factors, including socio-economic status, self-concept in

music, reading achievement, scholastic ability, measured

musical ability, math achievement, and motivation are all

valid predictors of continuing to play a musical instrument

(Mawbey, 1973; McCarthy, 1980; Klinedinst, 1991,

Hallam, 1998a). Hurley (1995) found that students who

dropped out viewed continuing to play as demanding too

great a time cost for the relatively small rewards it offered.

When the quality of performance has been considered

rather than the level of expertise attained, the amount of

practice undertaken is not a good predictor (Hallam, 1998a;

2004; Williamon & Valentine, 2000). A further issue is that

measures of time spent practising do not take account of

the effectiveness of the practice undertaken. There are

certainly differences in the practising strategies adopted by

students and their metacognitive skills, although their

development seems to be inextricably intertwined with the

acquisition of knowledge (Hallam, 2001a; 2001b).

Increasingly it has been recognised that aural skill is only

one of many skills necessary for the development of

musical expertise. In 1979, Gilbert devised tests of motor

skills, performance on which was highly correlated with

musical attainment (Gilbert, 1981). The importance of

creativity in music has been acknowledged and ways of

assessing it devised (Vaughan, 1977; Webster, 1988), the

evidence suggesting that generally, musical creativity

factors seemed to be discrete from those assessed by

musical ability tests (Swanner, 1985). In relation to

instrumental playing, McPherson (1995/6), identified five

distinct skills: sight reading; performing rehearsed music;

playing from memory; playing by ear, and improvising,

while Hallam (1998b) suggested that ‘musical skills’

included aural, cognitive, technical, musicianship,

performance and learning skills. There has also been an

increasing acknowledgement that individual musicians

Page 3: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 427

have differing strengths and weaknesses within their profile

of musical skills.

There has been little research addressing the ways in which

individuals within society as a whole conceive of musical

ability. Sloboda et al. (1994) proposed the existence of a

folk psychology of talent held by non-academics which

postulated innately determined differences between

individuals in their capacity for musical accomplishment.

This was supported by the findings from a survey that

indicated that more than 75% of a sample of educational

professionals believed that playing an instrument, singing

and composing required a special gift or natural talent

(Davis, 1994). A number of researchers have explored the

conceptualisation of musical ability by different groups in

society. Haroutounian (2000) analysed the level of

importance attached to particular criteria in identifying

musically able children. General behaviours of ‘sustained

interest’ and ‘self-discipline’ received higher mean

responses than music-specific characteristics indicative of

music aptitude. A performance assessment scale showed

note and rhythmic accuracy rated highest in importance

followed by steady rhythmic performance, dynamic

contrasts, and technical fluency. Originality received the

lowest rating. However, interviews with experts across the

musical fields of research, performance, psychology, and

education, teachers involved in gifted education programs

and others regularly involved in the identification of gifted

children revealed categories of perceptual awareness and

discrimination; meta-perception; creative interpretation;

behaviour/performance; and motivation. The most decisive

factor perceived to determine musical potential in children

rested on criteria related to the child’s creative expressive

involvement in musical activities. This contrasted with the

questionnaire survey which found that creativity was found

to be an inadequate measure reinforcing the complexity and

difficulty of defining and identifying musical potential.

Hallam and Prince (2003) explored the qualitatively

different ways in which groups of people with differing

levels of involvement in active music making

conceptualised ‘musical ability’. Individuals (129

musicians; 80 non-music educators; 112 adults in other

occupations; 60 students involved in extra-curricular

music; 14 not involved in extra-curricular music) were

asked to complete in writing the statement ‘Music ability

is:’ The statements were analysed using an iterative

process of categorisation. Musical ability was

conceptualised in relation to: receptive activities;

generative activities; the integration of a range of skills; the

extent to which it is learned; metacognition; and

motivation. Overall, 28% of the sample mentioned aural

skills as indicative of musical ability, 32% included

listening and understanding, 24% having an appreciation of

music, and 15% being responsive to music. By far the

largest response in any category was that musical ability

was being able to play a musical instrument or sing (cited

by 71% of the sample). This response was highest in

children who did not take part in extra-curricular music

(86%), and adults not involved in education (83%). The

integration of a range of skills was cited by 9% of

respondents. Personal qualities including motivation,

personal expression, immersion in music, total

commitment and metacognition (being able to learn to

learn) were cited most by musicians. The findings did not

indicate a general conception of musical ability as

genetically determined. In addition, the concept of musical

ability was constructed in different ways by each group of

participants. The greater the active involvement with music

making the more detailed and complex the constructions

became. This qualitative research, relying as it did on

individuals spontaneously generating their own

conceptualisations of musical ability, did not take account

of non-articulated beliefs. The purpose of this study, using

the categorisations derived from the qualitative study by

Hallam and Prince (2003) is to explore current conceptions

of the nature of musical ability.

METHOD

The present study is an extension of the research carried

out by Hallam and Prince (2003), which used qualitative

methods to determine how participants perceived the

construct ‘musical ability’. In the qualitative study,

respondents completed the statement ‘Musical ability is’.

The constructs derived from the first study were Musical

Ear; Rhythmic Ability; Listening and Understanding;

Response to Music; Appreciation of Music; Knowledge

about Music; Evaluative Activities; Performing; Reading

Music; Technical Skills; Emotional Sensitivity;

Communication and Interpretation; Performing in a Group;

Composition/ Improvisation; Organisation of Sound;

Creativity; Integration of Skills; Metacognition;

Motivation; Personal expression; and the Origins of

Musical Ability, i.e. whether it was learned or innate.

In the current study each of these categories was

represented by several statements derived from the

qualitative study. For instance, the statements relating to

having a musical ear were: Musical ability depends on

having perfect pitch; Musical ability is being able to play

by ear; Musical ability is being able to internalise sound. A

total of 77statements were included in the questionnaire.

Statements were responded to through levels of agreement

on a 5 point rating scale. Statements relating to the origins

of musical ability were not included in the analyses

described here. Respondents were also asked to provide

information regarding age, gender, occupation and musical

experience. The questionnaire was self-administered.

The sample was an opportunity sample which consisted of

650 individuals aged 14 to 90. There were 212 males and

447 females, one participant did not indicate their gender.

The sample was balanced between several different groups:

Page 4: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 428

102 musicians, 95 educators who were not musicians, 132

adults who were actively engaged in music making in an

amateur capacity, 60 adults who were not actively engaged

in making music, 193 children actively engaged in making

music in addition to their engagement with the school

curriculum and 71 children with no engagement with music

outside of the school curriculum.

FINDINGS

Table 1 and Figure 1 set out the mean responses in each

category. Having a sense of rhythm was the most highly

supported conception of musical ability. Other highly rated

categories related to being able to express oneself through

sound, being able to understand and interpret music and

being able to communicate through music. A range of

personal factors including motivation, personal

characteristics and being able to work in a group followed.

Having a musical ear received relatively low ratings given

its high rating in musical ability tests. The lowest ratings

were for reading music and knowledge about music.

Table 1: Mean responses to each category

Musical ability is N Mean SD

Having a sense of rhythm 645 3.84 .69

Expressing thoughts and

feelings through sound 475 3.75 .62

Being able to understand

and interpret the music 644 3.74 .81

Communication through

music 640 3.66 .75

Motivation 627 3.56 .88

Personal characteristics 635 3.48 .73

Group performance 639 3.44 .82

Integration of skills 626 3.39 .75

Responding to music 648 3.37 .75

Metacognition 639 3.34 .81

Playing or singing 641 3.32 .88

Having a musical ear 637 3.29 .74

Listening and

understanding 643 3.27 .71

Appreciation of music 644 3.13 .87

Creativity 641 3.10 .79

Evaluation skills 644 3.04 .87

Technical skills 647 3.03 .86

Composing or

improvising 638 2.99 .82

Reading music 642 2.77 .99

Knowledge about music 648 2.68 .86

Figure 1: Mean responses in each category

sense of rhythm

to express thoughts and feelings through sound

to understand and interpret the music

comm

unication

motivation

personal characteristics

group performance

integration of skills

responding

metacognition

playing or singing

musical ear

listening and understanding

appreciation

creativity

evaluation skills

technical skills

composing or im

provising

reading music

knowledge

0.00

1.00

2.00

3.00

4.00

Mean

To further explore the nature of conceptions of musical

ability a factor analysis was conducted. Principal

component analysis was used followed by a Varimax

rotation with Kaiser Normalization. Six factors were

identified which had eigenvalues of above 2. The factors

were: being able to read music and play an instrument or

sing (Eigenvalue 15.5); musical communication (5.01);

valuing, appreciating and responding to music (3.1);

composition, improvisation and the skills needed to

undertake them (2.5); commitment, motivation, personal

discipline and organisation (2.4); and rhythmic ability,

pitch skills, and understanding (2.3). Factor 1 included high

ratings for statements relating to reading music, being able

to play an instrument or sing, having appropriate technical

and physical skills, critically evaluating and analyzing

performance, and understanding the music. In short, all of

the skills required to play an instrument or sing well (see

Table 2). Only weightings above .3 are included.

Table 2: Factor 1- Being able to read music and play an

Instrument

Factor 1

To transfer what is written on a score to an

instrument .747

Being able to play an instrument well .740

Being able to sight read .696

Having the technical skills to play an

instrument .669

Being able to play an instrument/sing .663

Being able to read music .655

Generating music .641

To master technique .632

Understanding musical concepts .554

Knowing about musical form .546

Page 5: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 429

Reproduce a melody or rhythm on an

instrument .528

To play an instrument, sing, read music .477

Judge what is musical good or bad .410

Analyse a piece of music .404

Good overall physical coordination .384

To convey the emotions intended by the

composer .382

Is complex and requires being able to do

many things .379

Understanding, knowledge and a flair to be

creative in music .363

To value music by taking part in making it .354

To critically evaluate musical performances .331

Factor 2 focused on those issues relating to musical

communication including conveying emotions and moods

to an audience, playing and performing with feeling and

emotion, interpreting the music, and making decisions

about performance. There were also weightings related to

being sensitive to other musicians within the group and

inspiring group performance. High weightings were also in

evidence in relation to making sense of the world through

music, being inspired by music and taking risks (see Table

3).

Factor 3 took into account those aspects of engagement

with music which focus on listening and appreciation

including responding to music, valuing music through

listening, hearing and understanding music, and being able

to describe music in words and gestures (see Table 4).

Table 3: Factor 2 - Musical communication

Factor 2

To communicate moods and emotions

through music .696

To perform showing understanding of

expression .686

To play with feeling .653

To interpret the feelings of music .651

To express thoughts and feelings

through music .637

To convey your interpretations to an

audience .602

To be sensitive to others within an

ensemble .559

Expression through sound .539

To unite, inspire group performance .516

To convey the emotions intended by the

composer .511

To use music as a source of inspiration .496

To play as part of a group .472

Understanding, knowledge and a flair to

be creative in music .448

A relationship between music and your

life .429

To make decisions about performance

and compositions .414

Is complex and requires being able to

do many things .413

The integration of different distinct

skills .399

To help others enjoy or play music .397

To perceive what is musically beautiful .363

To use music to express one's

personality .340

To make sense of the world through

musical stimuli .315

Risk taking .307

Table 4: Factor 3 - Valuing, appreciating and

responding to music

Factor 3

Being able to value music by listening

to it

.696

Being able to enjoy music .637

Being able to appreciate music .596

Respond to the mood of the music .577

Respond creatively to music .569

To perceive what is musically beautiful .519

To value music by taking part in making

it

.488

Judge what is musical good or bad .472

Being able to describe music in words

and gestures .463

Respond to a musical stimuli .431

To hear and understand music .378

To critically evaluate musical

performances .372

Move in time with a rhythm .360

The focus of Factor 4 was composition, improvisation and

the skills needed to undertake them (see Table 5). This

included making decisions, integrating different distinct

skills, taking risks, being able to read music, and play by

ear.

Table 5: Factor 4 - Composition, improvisation and the

skills needed to undertaken them

Factor 4

Compose using new styles .691

To compose .646

To improvise .637

Page 6: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 430

To integrate listening, performing and

composing .617

To organise sound .552

To play an instrument, sing, read music .508

To make decisions about performance and

compositions .472

Risk taking .388

The integration of different distinct skills .384

Being able to describe music in words and

gestures .348

Analyse a piece of music .340

Being able to read music .324

Able to play by ear .318

Factor 5 focused on personal commitment, motivation and

organisation – all of the elements which enable an

individual to develop high level skills in music.

Commitment to practice loaded highly on this factor, as did

motivation to succeed, setting and attaining goals and

personal organisation and discipline. Immersion in music,

using it as a source of inspiration and as a means of

expressing oneself were also important as was being self-

critical.

Table 6: Factor 5 -Commitment, motivation, personal

discipline and organisation

Factor 5

The commitment to practice .771

The motivation to succeed .763

Working towards set goals .718

Personal organisation and discipline .666

Showing in interest or desire to make music .604

To be self critical in performances .494

To immerse yourself in music .402

A relationship between music and your life .392

To use music as a source of inspiration .391

To use music to express one's personality .384

Factor 6 loaded on those elements which have traditionally

been considered in musical ability tests, for instance,

rhythmic ability, being able to recognise tone/pitch and

internalise sound, and analyse music (see Table 7).

Table 7: Factor 6- Rhythmic ability, pitch skills, and

understanding

Factor 6

Perceive a rhythmical progression .707

Sing in time .682

A good sense of rhythm .635

Able to recognise tone/pitch .617

Internalise sound .519

Able to play by ear .495

Move in time with a rhythm .384

Analyse a piece of music .346

To hear and understand music .330

DISCUSSION

The findings described above indicate that conceptions of

musical ability in the general population are much broader

than those identified by traditional tests of musical ability.

The high proportion of participants stressing the

importance of having a sense of rhythm may reflect the

characteristics of popular music where ‘the beat’ is central.

The stress on motivation and commitment also suggests an

awareness of the time required to successfully develop

musical skills. There was also considerable emphasis on

being able to work well with other musicians in a group.

Of the factors that emerged only one reflected traditional

conceptions relating to aural abilities (rhythm and pitch),

the remaining factors focused on other elements which

contribute towards expert musical behaviour in its various

forms. Factor 1 focused on being able to read music and

sing or play an instrument along with all the skills required

to do this effectively including technical skills, having the

appropriate physical characteristics to play an instrument,

being able to evaluate performance, understand music and

be creative.

The second factor encapsulated elements relating to

musical communication. These not only included

communicating with the audience, both emotions and

specific interpretations, but also communication with other

performers reflecting the recent interest in research in these

areas, for instance, research on group rehearsal (e.g.

Davidson and King, 2004; Goodman, 2000; 2002), the role

of movement in musical communication (e.g. Davidson,

1993), the role of playing from memory in communication

(e.g. Williamon, 1999), and research on music and emotion

(Juslin and Sloboda, 2001).

The third factor related to valuing, appreciating and

responding to music. Some of the earlier tests of musical

ability acknowledged the importance of musical

appreciation. Revesz (1953) considered that ‘musicality’

included the ‘ability to enjoy music aesthetically’, Wing

(1981) described a general ability to perceive and

appreciate music and Gordon’s (1979) conceptualisation

included musical sensitivity. The findings from this

research reinforce the importance of these elements.

The fourth factor centred around the skills required for

composition, improvisation and the skills required for

undertaking these. The highest weighting was on

composing using new styles - a form of creativity. Other

elements were those which might be considered to be

necessary in order to compose and improvise - including

integrating listening, performing and composing, reading

Page 7: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 431

music, playing by ear, making decisions, analysis,

description, and risk taking. Traditional tests of musical

ability have tended to neglect creative musical outcomes,

although as we saw earlier some authors have recognized

its importance and devised ways of assessing it (Vaughan,

1977; Webster, 1988).

The fifth factor related to personal commitment to music,

motivation, discipline and organisation, and acknowledged

the importance of these in developing musical expertise

reflecting research findings referred to earlier which

suggest that time spent practising underpins the

development of expert performance, not inherited ability

(Ericsson et al., 1993; Sloboda et al., 1996). However, the

range of other elements identified as contributing to

musical ability support findings which contest a simple

relationship between amount of practice and attainment

(Sloboda and Howe, 1991; Wagner, 1975; Zurcher, 1972;

Hallam, 1998a; 2004).

The sixth factor focused on rhythmic and aural skills

indicating that these are still perceived as constituting an

important element of musical ability albeit with a greater

focus on rhythm than may have previously been the case.

The emphasis on rhythm in much contemporary popular

music may explain why it is perceived as an important

element of musical ability.

Overall, the research suggests that in a Western musical

culture at this time, musical ability is perceived to be

exemplified by actual musical skills in performing,

composition and improvisation, through listening, valuing

and appreciating music and through being able to

communicate through music. While aural skills play a part

they are perceived to be less important than generative

skills. The acquisition of these skills is perceived to require

high levels of commitment and motivation, a way of life of

which music is a crucial part. In the long term, the truly

defining element of musical ability may come to be

perceived as interest and commitment as the means to

create and perform music become increasingly accessible

to everyone through more advanced computer technology.

What are the implications of the findings for education?

Where a process of selection for playing an instrument is

necessary because resources are limited it may be better to

take account of a wider range of factors than those assessed

in traditional tests with a particular emphasis on

motivation. While aural skills may be important, without

sufficient effort and commitment on the part of the learner

little will be achieved.

REFERENCES

Davidson, J.W. (1993) ‘Visual perception of performance

manner in the movement of solo musicians’. Psychology of

Music, 21(2), 103-113.

Davidson, J. & King, E.C. (2004) ‘Strategies for ensemble

practice’. In A. Williamon (ed.) Musical Excellence. (pp

105-122) Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Davis, M. (1994) Folk music psychology. The

Psychologist, 7(12), 537.

Doxey, C. & Wright, C. (1990) ‘An exploratory study of

children’s music ability’. Early Childhood Research

Quarterly, 5, 425-440.

Ericsson, K.A. , Krampe, R.T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993)

‘The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert

performance’. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363-406.

Gilbert, J.P. (1981) ‘Motoric music skill development in

young children: A longitudinal investigation’. Psychology

of Music, 9 (1), 21-25.

Goodman, E. (2000) ‘Analysing the ensemble in Music

Rehearsal and Performance: The Nature and Effects of

Interaction in Cello-Piano Duos’. Unpublished Doctoral

Dissertation, University of London.

Goodman, E. (2002) ‘Ensemble performance’. In J. Rink

(ed) Musical Performance: A Guide to Understanding (pp.

153-167). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gordon, E.E. (1965). Musical Aptitude Profile manual.

Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Gordon, E. E. (1979). Primary measures of music

audiation. Chicago: GIA

Gordon, E.E. (1982). Intermediate measures of music

audiation. Chicago: GIA.

Gordon, E.E. (1989a). Advanced measures of music

audiation. Chicago: GIA.

Gordon, E.E. (1989b). Audie: A game for understanding

and analysing your child’s musical potential. Chicago:

GIA

Hallam, S. (1998a) ‘Predictors of Achievement and Drop

Out in Instrumental Tuition’. Psychology of Music, 26(2),

116-132.

Hallam, S. (1998b) Instrumental Teaching: A practical

guide to better teaching and learning. Oxford: Heinemann

Hallam, S. (2001a) ‘The development of metacognition in

musicians: Implications for education’. The British Journal

of Music Education, 18 (1), 27-39.

Page 8: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 432

Hallam, S. (2001b) ‘The Development of Expertise in

Young Musicians: Strategy Use, Knowledge Acquisition

and Individual Diversity’. Music Education Research,

3(1), 7-23.

Hallam, S. (2004) ‘How important is practicing as a

predictor of learning outcomes in instrumental music?’ In

S.D. Lipscomb, R. Ashley, R.O. Gjerdingen & P. Webster.

Proceedings of the 8th

International Conference on Music

Perception and Cognition, August 3-7th 2004,

Northwestern University, Evanston, USA (pp 165-168)

Hallam, S. (2006) ‘Musicality’. In G. McPherson (Ed) The

child as musician: A handbook of musical development.

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hallam, S. & Prince, V. (2003) ‘Conceptions of Musical

Ability’. Research Studies in Music Education, 20, 2-22.

Haroutounian, J. (2000). Perspectives of musical talent: a

study of identification criteria and procedures. High Ability

Studies, 11 (2), 137-160.

Howe, M.J.A., Davidson, J.W., Moore, D.M. & Sloboda,

J.A. (1995) ‘Are there early childhood signs of musical

ability?’ Psychology of Music, 23, 162-76.

Hurley, C.G. (1995) ‘Student motivations for beginning

and continuing/discontinuing string music tuition’. The

Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 6(1),

44-55.

Juslin, P., & Sloboda, J. (2001) Music and emotion: theory

and research. Oxford: Oxford University Press

Klinedinst, R.E. (1991) ‘Predicting performance

achievement and retention of fifth-grade instrumental

students’. Journal of Research in Music Education, 39(3),

225-238.

Mawbey, W.E. (1973) ‘Wastage from instrumental classes

in schools’. Psychology of Music, 1, 33-43.

McCarthy, J.F. (1980) ‘Individualised instruction, student

achievement and drop out in an urban elementary

instrumental music program’. Journal of Research in Music

Education, 28, 59-69.

McPherson, G.E. (1995/6) ‘Five aspects of musical

performance and their correlates’. Bulletin of the Council

for Research in Music Education, Special Issue, the 15th

International Society for Music Education, University of

Miami, Florida, 9-15 July, 1994.

Moore, D., Burland, K., & Davidson, J. (2003) The social

context of music success: A developmental account. British

Journal of Psychology, 94, 529-549.

Revesz, G. (1953) Introduction to the Psychology of Music,

London: Longmans

Seashore, C.E., Lewis, L., & Saetveit, J.G. (1960)

Seashore measures of musical talents. New York: The

Psychological Corporation.

Shuter-Dyson, R. (1999). Musical Ability. In D. Deutsch

(Ed.), The Psychology of Music, (pp 627-651). New York,

Harcourt Brace and Company

Sloboda, J. A., Davidson, J. & Howe, M. J. A. (1994) ‘Is

everyone musical?’ The Psychologist, 7(8), 349-354.

Sloboda, J.A., Davidson, J.W., Howe, M.J.A. & Moore,

D.G, (1996) ‘The role of practice in the development of

performing musicians’. British Journal of Psychology, 87,

287-309.

Sloboda, J.A. & Howe, M.J.A. (1991) ‘Biographical

precursors of musical excellence: An interview study’.

Psychology of Music, 19, 3-21.

Stumpf, C. (1883) Tonpsychologie. Leipzig: Hirzel.

Swanner, D.L. (1985) ‘Relationships between musical

creativity and selected factors, including personality,

motivation, musical aptitude, and cognitive intelligence as

measured in third grade children’. Unpublished

dissertation, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,

Ohio.

Vaughan, M.M. (1977) ‘Measuring creativity: Its

cultivation and measurement’. Bulletin of the Council for

Research in Music Education, 50, 72-77.

Vispoel, W.P. (1993) ‘The development and evaluation of

a computerized adaptive test of tonal memory’. Journal of

Research in Music Education, 41, 111-136.

Vispoel, W.P. & Coffman, D.D. (1992) ‘Computerized-

adaptive and self-adapted tests of music listening skills:

Psychometric features and motivational benefits’. Applied

measurement in Education, 7, 25-51.

Wagner, M.J. (1975) ‘The effect of a practice report on

practice time and musical performance’. In C.K. Madsen,

R.D. Greer, & C.H. Madsen, Jr., (Eds), Research in Music

Behaviour, New York: Teachers College Press.

Webster, P.R. (1988) ‘New perspectives on music aptitude

and achievement’. Psychomusicology, 7(2), 177-194.

Williamon, A. (1999) ‘The value of performing from

memory’. Psychology of Music, 27, 84-95.

Page 9: Conception of Musical Ability

ICMPC9 Proceedings

ISBN 88-7395-155-4 © 2006 ICMPC 433

Williamon, A. and Valentine, E. (2000) ‘Quantity and

quality of musical practice as predictors of performance

quality’. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 353-376.

Wing, H.D. (1981) ‘Standardised Tests of Musical

Intelligence’. Windsor, England: National Foundation for

Educational Research

Zurcher, W.Z. (1972) ‘The effects of model-supportive

practice on beginning brass instrumentalists’.

Unpublished, EdD dissertation, Teachers College,

Columbia University.